Top Banner
The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use Richard Joiner a, * , Mark Brosnan a , Jill Duffield b , Jeff Gavin a , Pam Maras c a Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK b School of Psychology, University of the West of England, Bristol, UK c Department of Psychology and Counselling, University of Greenwich, London, UK Available online 20 April 2005 Abstract This paper reports a study investigating the relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use. The participants were 446 students (319 females and 127 males) from two universities in the UK and one university in Australia. Measures of Internet identification and Internet anxiety were developed. The majority of participants were NOT anxious about using the Internet, although there were approximately 8% who showed evidence of Internet anxiety. There was a significant and negative relationship between Internet anxiety and Internet use. Those who were more anxious about using the Internet used the Internet less, although the magnitude of effect was small. There was a positive and significant relationship between Internet use and Internet identification. Those who scored high on the measure of Internet identification used the Internet more than those who did not. There was also a signif- icant and negative relationship between Internet anxiety and Internet identification. Finally, males had a significantly higher Internet identification score than females. Implications of these findings are discussed. Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Internet anxiety; Internet identification; Internet use 0747-5632/$ - see front matter Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2005.03.002 * Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 1225 384373; fax: +44 1225 386752. E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Joiner). Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Computers in Human Behavior
13

The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

Apr 30, 2023

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

omputers in

C

Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Human Behavior

The relationship between Internet identification,Internet anxiety and Internet use

Richard Joiner a,*, Mark Brosnan a, Jill Duffield b,Jeff Gavin a, Pam Maras c

a Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UKb School of Psychology, University of the West of England, Bristol, UK

c Department of Psychology and Counselling, University of Greenwich, London, UK

Available online 20 April 2005

Abstract

This paper reports a study investigating the relationship between Internet identification,

Internet anxiety and Internet use. The participants were 446 students (319 females and 127

males) from two universities in the UK and one university in Australia. Measures of Internet

identification and Internet anxiety were developed. The majority of participants were NOT

anxious about using the Internet, although there were approximately 8% who showed evidence

of Internet anxiety. There was a significant and negative relationship between Internet anxiety

and Internet use. Those who were more anxious about using the Internet used the Internet less,

although the magnitude of effect was small. There was a positive and significant relationship

between Internet use and Internet identification. Those who scored high on the measure of

Internet identification used the Internet more than those who did not. There was also a signif-

icant and negative relationship between Internet anxiety and Internet identification. Finally,

males had a significantly higher Internet identification score than females. Implications of

these findings are discussed.

� 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Internet anxiety; Internet identification; Internet use

0747-5632/$ - see front matter � 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.chb.2005.03.002

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 1225 384373; fax: +44 1225 386752.

E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Joiner).

Page 2: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1409

1. Introduction

The Internet has become part of everyday life (Jackson, 1999; Jackson, Ervin,

Gardner, & Schmitt, 2001a; US Department of Commerce, NTIA, 1995, 1997,

1999, 2002). Changes in education are likely to mean that information communica-tion technology (ICT) skills will become indispensable for modern life and a tool

for lifelong learning (Russell & Stafford, 2002). However, research has shown cer-

tain social groups are under-represented on the Internet (Hoffman & Novak, 1998;

Jackson, 1999; Sax, Ceja, & Teranishi, 2001; Schofield, 1997), not simply due to a

lack of access, but more because of cognitive, motivational and affective factors

(Jackson et al., 2001a). Therefore, as Jackson, Ervin, Gardner, and Schmitt

(2001b) argued, �Psychology has an important role in advancing understanding of

why people choose to use or not use the Internet� (p. 15). Cooper and Weaver(2003) have developed a model of computer use, which provides a useful heuristic

�road map� to study this question. The main focus of Cooper and Weaver�s model is

the individual, and there are four inter-related factors that influence an individual�suses of, and attitudes towards, computers. These factors are computer identifica-

tion, computer anxiety, performance attributions, and expectations. While these

factors are related to each other in several ways, Cooper and Weaver recommend

thinking of the processes operating within the individual as somewhat distinct, with

each contributing to attitudes towards and uses of computers and technology indirect and distinct ways.

The crux of this model is that anything that increases a person�s computer anxiety

leads them to doubt their ability or causes them to disidentify with computers will

lead the individual to form negative attitudes about computers and technology. Neg-

ative attitudes, in turn, will mean that the individual is more likely to avoid activities,

contexts and careers involving computers.

The current study extends this model of computer use to explore attitudes towards

and uses of the Internet. We focus specifically on Internet anxiety and Internet iden-tification, and the relationships between them.

1.1. Computer and Internet anxiety

An important factor in Cooper and Weaver�s (2003) model is computer anxiety.

Computer anxiety has been defined as �an irrational anticipation of fear evoked by

the thought of using (or actually using) computers, the effects of which result in

avoiding, or minimising, computer usage� (Brosnan, 1998a, p. 17). Cooper andWeaver predict that computer anxiety is inversely related to computer experience.

This prediction is based on research conducted over the last 20 years. Chua, Chen,

and Wong (1999), for example, conducted a meta-analysis of 19 studies published

between 1990 and 1996. These studies reported that computer anxiety was inversely

related to computer experience. Recently, the UCLA Internet Project (2003) sur-

veyed 2000 households across the United States and compiled the responses of

Internet users and non-users. They reported that, in 2002, 30.3% of new Internet

Page 3: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

1410 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

users and 10.8% of experienced Internet users (users with more than six years of

experience) were moderately to highly technophobic. Thus, whilst computer anxi-

ety consistently has been found to be inversely related to computer experience

for the majority of individuals, this is confounded by a small proportion that re-

main highly anxious (Rosen & Maguire, 1990).There have been a number of studies using a broad range of samples that have

explored the relationship between computer anxiety and Internet use. Jackson

et al. (2001a, 2001b) conducted a study on a group of Anglo-American undergrad-

uates. She found that computer anxiety was negatively related to students� use of

the Internet. Durndell and Haag (2002) surveyed a group of Romanian university

students and reported that computer anxiety was inversely proportional to the stu-

dents� self-reported use of the Internet. Barbiete and Weiss (2004), using an online

sample, found that the number of years spent using the Internet was lower for stu-dents who scored high on computer anxiety than for students who scored low on

the same measure. Cody, Dunn, Hoppin, and Wendt (1999) surveyed a group of

older adults (average age 80) and found that computer anxiety predicted time

on-line.

There have also been a small number of studies that have investigated the

relationship between Internet anxiety and Internet use. Chou (2003) developed

a new scale of Internet anxiety and found that it was related to self-reported

use of the Internet. This raises the question of whether Internet anxiety is thesame as computer anxiety. Chou (2003) and Presno (1998) argue that

although Internet anxiety shares some of the same concepts as computer anx-

iety, it has a number of constructs that are unique to it. Presno (1998) out-

lines four anxieties people have with the Internet: Internet terminology

anxiety; Internet search anxiety; Internet time delay anxiety; and general fear

of Internet failure.

The current study similarly focuses on Internet-related anxiety, conceptualised in

more generic terms as measured by Brosnan and Thorpe (2001; Thorpe and Bros-nan, 2001).

1.2. Internet identification

Identity is an important factor in computer use and experience (Cooper & Wea-

ver, 2003; Facer, Furlong, Furlong, & Sutherland, 2003; Holloway & Valentine,

2003). Research in this area, however, conceptualises identity in a number of ways.

Facer et al. (2003), for example, conceptualised it in terms of consumption (Gid-dens, 1991), while Holloway and Valentine focused on social identity. Following

Cooper and Weaver�s (2003) model of computer use, we do not focus on identity

per se, but instead focus on domain identification. Derived from the research of

Claude Steele and colleagues (Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999; Steele, 1997; Steele

& Aronson, 1995), this approach provides a link between social identity and the

performance of some skill or ability. It is therefore highly suited to the study of

Internet use.

Page 4: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1411

A domain is defined as a performance context (Aronson et al., 1999). It is a con-

text in which one can put a skill or a set of skills into practice. To identify with a

domain is to have one�s self-esteem bound up with one�s ability to perform success-

fully in that domain (Aronson et al., 1999; Cooper & Weaver, 2003). Examples of

domains include mathematics (Aronson et al., 1999; Spencer et al., 1999; Steele,1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995), sports (Stone, Perry, & Darley, 1997; Stone, Lynch,

Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999), and computers and technology (Cooper & Weaver,

2003). We use the term �Internet identification� to refer to the extent to which an indi-

vidual�s self-concept is bound up with his or her perceived ability to use the Internet.

One of the aims of the current study is to examine the relationship between Internet

identification and actual Internet use.

Identification with a domain such as the Internet serves a variety of functions

(Cooper & Weaver, 2003). First, it provides a means of self-definition or self-es-teem, making the person feel better about him- or herself. Second, it is a means

of identifying with others who share the same values and goals, providing a refer-

ence group orientation and shared activities. Third, it serves as a means of defining

oneself in contrast to others who identify with other domains. Finally, domain

identification has implications for behaviour. The higher one�s identification with

a particular domain, the more likely one is to participate within this domain. Re-

cent research has shown that identification has a strong influence on behaviour in a

variety of domains (Ethier & Deaux, 1994; Ethier, 1995; Reicher, 1986); the stron-ger the identification, the stronger the influence. Conversely, as Cooper and Wea-

ver suggest, those who disidentify with a particular domain are less likely to

participate in that domain.

If we apply this to the Internet, an individual with a high degree of Internet

identification would need to be able to use the Internet effectively to maintain their

sense of self-worth. Consequently, he or she would be likely to have a high degree

of experience using the Internet; would have positive attitudes towards the Inter-

net; would be motivated to spend time learning how to use it; and might takecourses on using the Internet. If he or she performs badly using the Internet this

would be likely to make him or her feel anxious because it would threatens a sense

of self-worth.

Finally, Cooper and Weaver (2003) speculated that identification with the com-

puter would be inversely related to computer anxiety. If computers are not rele-

vant to a person�s self-worth, then that person�s actual or expected experiences in

this domain should relate to lower levels of anxiety than a person whose self-

worth is relevant to performance in this domain. Applied to the Internet, themodel would predict that Internet identification is inversely related to Internet

anxiety.

The aim of this paper is to apply Cooper and Weaver�s (2003) model of com-

puter use to Internet use. Focusing on two of the factors in this model, this

study has three hypotheses. First, Internet anxiety will be inversely related to

Internet use. Second, identification with the Internet will be positively related

to Internet use. Finally, Internet identification will be inversely related to Internet

anxiety.

Page 5: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

1412 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The participants were 446 undergraduate students from the University of Bathand University of Greenwich in the UK, and Macquarie University in Australia.

There were 319 females and 127 males and the average age was 22 years (age range

was 18–48 years).

2.2. Procedure

The questionnaires were handed out at the end of a lecture and the students were

asked to complete them before they left. The questionnaire contained a section onthe students� ownership of a computer; how old they were when they first used the

Internet; who showed them how to use it and four purposely designed scales: two

scales that measured Internet use, an Internet anxiety scale and an Internet identifi-

cation scale. The students were also asked how many hours they spend on the Inter-

net in a week and how many hours they spent on the Internet per week for studying.

2.3. Measures

To assess the students� Internet use we used three measures. The students were

first asked how many hours a week they spent on the Internet. They answered using

a eight-point scale (0 = 0 h, 1 = 1–5 h, 2 = 6–10 h, 3 = 11–15 h, 4 = 16–20 h, 5 = 21–

25 h, 6 = 26–30 h and 7 = over 30 h). Two scales were developed to measure stu-

dents� use of the Internet. The first was a measure of students� use of the Internet gen-

erally, in which seven items identified use of e-mail, chat, newsgroups, online games,

sex sites, shopping and downloading/listening to music. They answered using a five-

point scale (0 = never, 1 = rarely, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often and 4 = very often). Reli-ability was adequate (Cronbach a = 0.77). The final question was an open-ended

question that asked students to specify any other uses they made of the Internet.

The second measure of use was designed to measure how often students used the

Internet for university coursework. The students answered six items concerning how

often they searched the library website for references, contacted university staff via

e-mail for information, contacted external experts via e-mail for information, down-

loaded relevant material from course web pages, used WWW for searching for rele-

vant material and posted questions to newsgroups and message boards. Theyanswered using the same five-point scale used in the general Internet use scale

(0 = never, 1 = rarely, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often and 4 = very often). Reliability was

adequate (Cronbach a = 0.77).

A scale was developed to measure Internet anxiety and was based on a scale de-

signed to measure computer anxiety (Brosnan & Thorpe, 2001; Thorpe & Brosnan,

2001). The students were asked six questions (shown below) and answered using a

five-point Likert Scale (Cronbach a = 0.77).

Page 6: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1413

1. I always feel anxious when using the Internet.

2. I go out of my way to avoid using the Internet.

3. It is easy for me to use the Internet.

4. It is important for me to be able to use the Internet.

5. My anxiety about using the Internet bothers me.6. I am more anxious about using the Internet than I should be.

An Internet identification scale was developed from an identity scale used for

measuring school, peer and family identity in relation to students� motivation, learn-

ing and behaviour in school (Maras, 2002).The students responded to 10 statements

(shown below) using a five-point Likert type scale (Cronbach a = 0.76). These items

were based on previous work (Maras, 2002) and used items typically found in iden-

tity scales, such as similarity and affect relative to the target identification (Tajfel,1979).

1. I would describe myself as an Internet user.

2. I am very similar to other Internet users.

3. Other Internet users are very like me.

4. I am very different from Internet users.

5. I feel very emotionally attached to other Internet users in general.

6. I feel a part of an Internet users community.7. When there is an opportunity I always get involved in using the Internet.

8. Whenever I can, I tell people I am an Internet user.

9. I like people who use the Internet frequently.

10. Using the Internet is a very important aspect of being a student.

3. Results

Descriptive analysis of the data revealed that the measures were all highly and

positively skewed. Therefore, we employed non-parametric statistics throughout

the statistical analysis. Most students (42%) used the Internet between 1 and

5 h a week and 18% used it between 6 and 10 h a week. Two students did not

use the Internet and 31 students used it for over 30 h a week. The descriptive sta-

tistics for the students� general use of the Internet are shown in Table 1. Interest-

ingly the activities that most students used regularly were e-mail and shopping.Most students did not use the Internet for any other purposes. These findings

were indicated by the fact that for all questions, except email use, the modal re-

sponse was 0 (i.e., never), whilst e-mail use has a modal score of 4 (i.e., very

often).

Table 2 shows the students� use of the Internet for coursework. The most frequent

use of the Internet for coursework was to download course material, searching the

library website and searching the Internet for course materials. Most students did

not report contacting external experts via e-mail or contacting newsgroups.

Page 7: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

Table 1

General Internet use

Mean Mode (%N) SD

E-mail use 3.4 4.0 60 0.9

Chatroom use 1.3 0.0 46 1.5

Newsgroup use 0.8 0.0 55 1.1

Online game use 0.7 0.0 62 1.8

Pornography use 0.6 0.0 74 1.2

Shopping on the WWW 1.2 0.0 36 1.1

Download music 1.6 0.0 31 1.3

Table 2

Experience using the Internet for coursework

Mean Mode %N SD

Searching library website 2.7 4.0 32 1.3

Contact staff via e-mail 1.8 2.0 38 1.1

Contact external expert via e-mail 0.7 0.0 54 0.9

Downloading material 3.3 4.0 47 0.9

Searching WWW for material 3.0 3.0 38 1.0

Contacting news group 0.6 0.0 62 1.0

1414 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

The mean score for Internet anxiety was 13.2 and SD = 3.3. Using a one sample

Wilcoxan Signed Rank Test with a predicted mean = 18, the obtained mean was sig-

nificantly lower than the predicted mean (Wilcoxan Signed Rank Test, z = 16.7,p < 0.05). Therefore, the students were not anxious about using the Internet.

Thirty-four students had scores of above 18. The anxiety scores ranged from 8 to

27 with 92% scoring 18 or less and 8% scoring more than 18. The mean score for

Internet identification was 29.3 and SD = 5.1. Using a one sample Wilcoxan Signed

Rank Test with a predicted mean = 30, the obtained mean was significantly lower

than the predicted mean (Wilcoxan Signed Rank Test, z = 3.0, p < 0.01). The Inter-

net identification scores ranged from 13 to 45 with 60% scoring 30 or lower and 40%

scoring above 30.Table 3 shows the correlations between Internet identification, Internet anxiety

and the various measures of Internet use. Non-significant correlations are not

Table 3

Correlation matrix for Internet identification, Internet anxiety, and Internet use

1 2 3 4 5

1 Hours per week 0.73** 0.48** �0.13**

2 General Internet use 0.47** �0.17**

3 Internet use for coursework 0.24**

4 Identification

5 Anxiety �0.14**

** Correlation is significant at the 0.001 level (two-tailed).

Page 8: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1415

shown. We found that Internet identification was positively related to hours per

week using the Internet (q = 0.52, p < 0.01), general Internet use (q = 0.49,

p < 0.01) and using the Internet for coursework (q = 0.20, p < 0.01). There was a sig-

nificant and negative relationship between Internet identification and Internet anxi-

ety (q = �0.14, p < 0.01), Internet anxiety was negatively related to general Internetuse (q = �0.17, p < 0.05) and Internet anxiety was negatively related to hours per

week using the Internet (q = �0.13, p < 0.01).

Factor analysis (principal components analysis, varimax rotation with Kaiser nor-

malisation) was used to examine the relationships between the items in the Internet

identification scale. The rotated component matrix for the three factors is presented

in Table 4. Coefficients less than 0.5 are not shown. A three-factor solution emerged

after five iterations and explained 64% of the variance. The first factor explained

34% of the variance (eigenvalue = 3.4) and loaded with the students� awareness of beingpart of an Internet community. The second factor explained 20% of the variance (eigen-

value = 2.0) and loaded on the questions concerned with similarity to other Internet

users. The final factor explained 10% of the variance (eigenvalue 1.0) and was one ques-

tion concerned with the importance of using the Internet in being a student.

Table 5 shows the correlations between the three different factors of Internet iden-

tification, Internet anxiety, and the three measures of Internet use. Non-significant

Table 4

Internet identification factor matrix

Components

1 2 3

I would describe myself as an Internet user

I am very similar to other Internet users 0.89

Other Internet users are very like me 0.89

I am very different from Internet users �0.73

I feel very emotionally attached to other Internet users in general 0.78

I feel a part of an Internet users community 0.84

When there is an opportunity I always get involved in using the Internet 0.73

Whenever I can, I tell people I am an Internet user 0.72

I like people who use the Internet frequently 0.56

Using the Internet is a very important aspect of being a student 0.93

Table 5

Correlation matrix for Internet identification factors, Internet anxiety, and Internet use

Identification components

1 2 3

Hours per week 0.48** 0.19**

General Internet use 0.50** 0.16**

Internet use for coursework 0.19** 0.18**

Internet anxiety �0.15**

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).

Page 9: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

Table 6

Gender differences and Internet identification

Male Female

M SD M SD

Factor 1 12.7 3.8 11.4 3.5*

Factor 2 9.4 2.3 9.4 1.8

Factor 3 3.8 0.9 3.9 0.9

Identity total 30.3 5.2 28.8 5.1*

Anxiety 12.4 3.0 13.6 3.3*

* p < 0.01.

1416 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

statistical correlations are not shown. Factor 1 was significantly correlated with

hours per week using the Internet (q = 0.48, p < 0.01), general Internet use

(q = 0.50, p < 0.01) and using the Internet for coursework (q = 0.19, p < 0.01). Fac-

tor 2 was significantly correlated with hours per week using the Internet (q = 0.19,

p < 0.01) and general Internet use (q = 0.16, p < 0.01). Factor 3 was significantly cor-

related with using the Internet for coursework (q = 0.18, p < 0.01). Factor 3 was sig-

nificantly and negatively related to Internet anxiety (q = �0.15, p < 0.01). It is

important to note that principal components analysis was used to prevent the needfor unnecessarily repetitive statistical analysis, and it is not believed that either factor

represents a �stable� underlying dimension.

Comparison of males and females in terms of Internet identification revealed a

number of significant gender differences (see Table 6). Males� identification with

the Internet was significantly higher than females (Mann–Whitney, z = 2.3,

p < 0.01). In particular, males were significantly more likely to feel part of an Inter-

net community than females (Mann–Whitney, z = 3.1, p < 0.01). Furthermore, males

were significantly less anxious about the Internet than females (Mann–Whitney,z = 3.6, p < 0.01).

4. Discussion

The overall aim of this study was to apply two of the factors in Cooper and Wea-

ver�s (2003) model of computer use to Internet use. All three hypotheses based on

this model were supported. We found a positive relationship between Internet iden-tification and Internet use, a negative relationship between Internet identification

and Internet anxiety and a negative relationship between Internet anxiety and Inter-

net use.

On the whole, students were not anxious about using the Internet, although

approximately 9% did show evidence of Internet anxiety. This result is consistent

with the findings reported by the UCLA Internet Project (2003). We also found that

there was a significant and negative relationship between Internet anxiety and Inter-

net use. Those who were more anxious about using the Internet used the Internet

Page 10: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1417

less. This finding is consistent with Cooper and Weaver�s (2003) model and previous

research (Barbiete & Weiss, 2004; Chou, 2003; Cody et al., 1999; Durndell & Haag,

2002; Jackson et al., 2001a).

In terms of Internet identification, participants in this study did not identify with

the Internet. However, there was a positive and significant relationship between allthree measures of Internet use and Internet identification. Those who scored high

on Internet identification used the Internet more than those who did not. This find-

ing supports Cooper and Weaver�s (2003) model and is consistent with research con-

cerning the strength of identification and behaviour. (Ethier & Deaux, 1994; Ethier,

1995). We extended this research by looking at some of the relationships between

Internet use and the three aspects of identification with the Internet: awareness of

group membership, similarity with other Internet users and the importance of using

the Internet for being a student. Awareness of group membership was significantlyand positively related to all three measures of Internet use, that is, hours per week

spent using the Internet, general use of the Internet and use of the Internet for

coursework. Similarity to other Internet users was positively and significantly related

to hours per week spent using the Internet, and general use of the Internet, but not to

the use of the Internet for coursework. Finally, the sole question of the identity ques-

tionnaire relating to the importance of the Internet for being a student was as pre-

dicted correlated with use of the Internet for coursework. Consistent with Cooper

and Weaver�s (2003) model, we found a small but significant negative relationshipbetween Internet anxiety and identification with the Internet. Those with high Inter-

net identification experience less anxiety in using the Internet than those with low

Internet identification.

Overall, most participants in this study did not identify with the Internet, and

this raises a number of interesting questions. For example, what do people think

identifies Internet users? What characteristics do they possess and why did the par-

ticipants in this study not identify with them? We could make a number of specu-

lations in answer to these questions. It is probable that the participants see Internetusers as male, white and �geeky� (see Duffield, Gavin, & Joiner, 2003). Holloway

and Valentine (2003) identified groups of boys they labelled �techno boys�. These

boys were highly technologically literate, but were labelled by their other classmates

as �boffins�, �computer freaks� and �nerds�. They were seen as socially inadequate,

unmasculine and were generally marginalised. Thus, it is no surprise that our sam-

ple did not identify with Internet users. That males reported higher levels of Inter-

net identification than females, and that in particular males identify more than

females with an Internet community, is consistent with research and theory suggest-ing that the Internet is a masculinised domain (Bimber, 2000; Morehan-Martin,

1998), and the generally low levels of Internet identification by both males and fe-

males may reflect the varied and competing images of Internet users of both sexes

(Duffield et al., 2003).

One problem with this study is its correlational nature and the inherent difficulty

of drawing causal links between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Inter-

net-related behaviour. It is possible that a person�s strong identification with the

Internet caused them to use the Internet more. However, it is equally likely that

Page 11: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

1418 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

the students may have identified more with the Internet because of their experiences

with the Internet. Similarly, it is difficult to know whether Internet anxiety deter-

mines the pattern of Internet use or vice versa. Research on computer anxiety shows

that it is a strong predictor of computer achievement (Marcoulides, 1988) and gen-

eralising this finding to the Internet would suggest that Internet anxiety is predictiveof Internet use. Also Jackson et al. (2003) carried out a study which found that a per-

son�s attitudes towards the Internet at time one was predictive of their actual use at

time two. This indicates that self-reported measures of Internet attitudes can be pre-

dictive of future and actual behaviour. Further longitudinal research is needed to

investigate whether Internet identification and Internet anxiety are predictive of

Internet use.

In Section 1, we reported that, �Psychology has an important role in advancing

understanding of why people choose to use or not use the Internet� (Jacksonet al., 2001b, argued p. 15). This research has shown that two psychological factors,

namely Internet Identification and Internet anxiety, are both related to use of the

Internet.

References

Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men

can�t do math. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29–46.

Barbiete, F. G., & Weiss, E. M. (2004). Computer self efficacy and anxiety scales for an Internet sample:

testing measurement equivalence of existing measures and development of new scales. Computers in

Human Behaviour, 20, 1–15.

Bimber, B. (2000). Measuring the gender gap on the Internet. Social Science Quarterly, 81(3), 868–876.

Brosnan, M. (1998a). Technophobia: The psychological impact of information technology. London:

Routledge.

Brosnan, M., & Thorpe, S. (2001). Does Technophobia conform to DSM-IV criteria for a specific phobia

(300.29)? Paper presented at the 22nd international conference of STAR, the International Society for

Stress and Anxiety Research, Palma de Mallorca, 12–14, July.

Chou, C. (2003). Incidence and correlates of Internet anxiety among high school teachers in Taiwan.

Computers in Human Behaviour, 19, 731–749.

Cody, M. J., Dunn, D., Hoppin, S., & Wendt, P. (1999). Silver surfers: training and evaluating Internet use

among older adult learners. Communication Education, 48(4), 269–286.

Cooper, J., & Weaver, K. D. (2003). Gender and computers: Understanding the digital divide. Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Chua, S. L., Chen, D., & Wong, A. F. L. (1999). Computer anxiety and its correlates: a meta-analysis.

Computers in Human Behaviour, 15, 609–623.

Duffield, J., Gavin, J., & Joiner, R. (2003). Drawing the Net. Paper presented at the annual conference of

The British Psychological Society, 10th–14th March.

Durndell, A., & Haag, Z. (2002). Computer self efficacy, computer anxiety, attitudes towards the Internet

and reported experience with the Internet, by gender, in an East European sample. Computers in

Human Behaviours, 18(4), 521–535.

Ethier, K. A., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity in a changing context: maintaining

identification and responding to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 83–87.

Ethier, K. A. (1995). Becoming a mother: identity acquisition during the transition to motherhood.

Unpublished Doctorial Thesis, City University of New York.

Facer, K., Furlong, J., Furlong, R., & Sutherland, R. (2003). ScreenPlay: children and computing in the

home. London: RoutledgeFalmer.

Page 12: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420 1419

Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Cambridge: Polity

Press.

Hoffman, D. L., & Novak, T. P. (1998). Bridging the racial divide on the Internet. Science, 280, 390–391.

Holloway, W., & Valentine, G. (2003). Cyberkids: Children in the information age. London:

RoutledgeFalmer.

Jackson, L. A. (1999). Social psychology and the digital divide. Paper presented at the symposium entitled

�The Internet: a place for social psychology� The 1999 conference of the Society for Experimental Social

Psychology, St Louis.

Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N. (2001a). Gender and the Internet: women

communicating and men searching. Sex Roles, 44(5/6), 363–379.

Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N. (2001b). The racial digital divide:

motivational, affective and cognitive correlates of Internet use. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,

31, 2019–2046.

Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Barbatsis, G., Biocca, F., Zhao, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2003). Internet

attitudes and Internet use: some surprising findings from the HomeNet Too Project. International

Journal of Human Computer Studies, 59(3), 355–382.

Maras, P. (2002). Identity, social perception and motivation: interdependent or autonomous factors? In

British journal of educational psychology cutting edge conference, The Lake District, UK.

Marcoulides, G. A. (1988). The relationship between computer anxiety and computer achievement.

Journal of Educational Computer Research, 4(2), 151–158.

Morehan-Martin, J. (1998). Males, females and the Internet. In J. Gackenbach (Ed.), Psychology and the

Internet (pp. 169–197). San Diego: Academic Press.

Presno, C. (1998). Taking the byte out of Internet anxiety: instructional techniques that reduce computer/

Internet anxiety in the classroom. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 18(2), 147–161.

Reicher, S. (1986). Social influence in the crowd: attitudinal and behavioural effects of deindividuation in

conditions of high and low group salience. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 341–350.

Rosen, L., & Maguire, P. (1990). Myths and Realities of computer phobia: a meta-analysis. Anxiety

Research, 3, 175–179.

Russell, N., & Stafford, N. (2002). Trends in ICT access and use. Department for Education and Skills,

Research Report RR358.

Sax, L. J., Ceja, M., & Teranishi, R. T. (2001). Technological preparedness among entering freshmen: the

role of race, class and gender. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 24(4), 363–383.

Schofield, J. W. (1997). Psychology. Computers and classroom social processes: a review of the literature.

Social Science Computer Review, 15, 27–39.

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women�s math performance.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.

Stone, J., Perry, Z. W., & Darley, J. M. (1997). ‘‘White men can�t jump’’: evidence for the perceptual

confirmation of racial stereotypes following a basketball game. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,

19(3), 291–306.

Stone, J., Lynch, C., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on Black and White

athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1213–1227.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: how stereotypes shape the intellectual identities and performance

of women and African Americans. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.

Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-

Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

Tajfel, H. (1979). Individuals and social groups in social psychology. British Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 18, 139–190.

Thorpe, S., & Brosnan, M. (2001). A single treatment for technophobia. Paper presented at the 22nd

international conference of STAR, the International Society for Stress and Anxiety Research, Palma de

Mallorca, 12–14 July.

Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the screen. London: Penguin.

UCLA Internet Project (2000, 2001, 2003). Surveying the digital future. UCLA Centre for communication

Policy, University of California, Los Angeles, CA. Available from www.ccp.ucla.edu.

Page 13: The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use

1420 R. Joiner et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 23 (2007) 1408–1420

US Department of Commerce, National Telecommunication and Information Administration (NTIA).

(1995). Falling through the Net: A survey of the ‘‘Have Nots’’ in Rural and Urban America. Washington,

DC: Available from http://www.ntia.doc.gov/ntiahome/fallingthru.html.

US Department of Commerce, National Telecommunication and Information Administration (NTIA).

(1997). Racial divide continues to grow. Washington, DC. Available from http://www.ntia.doc.gov/

ntiahome/net2/.

US Department of Commerce, National Telecommunication and Information Administration (NTIA).

(1999). Falling through the Net: defining the digital divide. Washington, DC. Available from http://

www.ntia.doc.gov/ntiahome/fttn99/contents.html.

US Department of Commerce, National Telecommunication and Information Administration (NTIA).

(2002). A nation online: Internet use in America. Washington, DC. Available from http://

www.ntia.doc.gov/opadhome/digitalnation/index.html.