Top Banner
Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A Fineberg* ,1,2,3 , Marc N Potenza 4 , Samuel R Chamberlain 1,3 , Heather A Berlin 5 , Lara Menzies 3 , Antoine Bechara 6 , Barbara J Sahakian 3 , Trevor W Robbins 7 , Edward T Bullmore 3,8 and Eric Hollander 9 1 Department of Psychiatry, National OCDs Specialist Centre, Hertfordshire Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital, Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire, UK; 2 Postgraduate Medical School, University of Hertfordshire, College Lane, Hatfield, UK; 3 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK; 4 Departments of Psychiatry and Child Study Center, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA; 5 Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy Place, New York, NY, USA; 6 Psychology Department and Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA; 7 Department of Experimental Psychology and Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK; 8 Addenbrooke’s Centre for Clinical Investigations, Clinical Unit Cambridge, Drug Discovery, GlaxoSmith-Kline R&D, Addenbrooke’s Center for Clinical Investigations, Cambridge, UK; 9 Montefiore Medical Center, University Hospital of Albert Einstein College of Medicine, NY, USA Failures in cortical control of fronto-striatal neural circuits may underpin impulsive and compulsive acts. In this narrative review, we explore these behaviors from the perspective of neural processes and consider how these behaviors and neural processes contribute to mental disorders such as obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), obsessive–compulsive personality disorder, and impulse-control disorders such as trichotillomania and pathological gambling. We present findings from a broad range of data, comprising translational and human endophenotypes research and clinical treatment trials, focussing on the parallel, functionally segregated, cortico-striatal neural projections, from orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) to medial striatum (caudate nucleus), proposed to drive compulsive activity, and from the anterior cingulate/ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens shell), proposed to drive impulsive activity, and the interaction between them. We suggest that impulsivity and compulsivity each seem to be multidimensional. Impulsive or compulsive behaviors are mediated by overlapping as well as distinct neural substrates. Trichotillomania may stand apart as a disorder of motor-impulse control, whereas pathological gambling involves abnormal ventral reward circuitry that identifies it more closely with substance addiction. OCD shows motor impulsivity and compulsivity, probably mediated through disruption of OFC-caudate circuitry, as well as other frontal, cingulate, and parietal connections. Serotonin and dopamine interact across these circuits to modulate aspects of both impulsive and compulsive responding and as yet unidentified brain-based systems may also have important functions. Targeted application of neurocognitive tasks, receptor-specific neurochemical probes, and brain systems neuroimaging techniques have potential for future research in this field. Neuropsychopharmacology advance online publication, 25 November 2009; doi:10.1038/npp.2009.185 Keywords: impulsive; compulsive; endophenotypes; serotonin; dopamine INTRODUCTION Whereas acts with impulsive or compulsive features may contribute specifically to creativity and endurance and generally to adaptive human behavior, disordered regulation of impulsive or compulsive behavior may be associated with adverse consequences and have a function in the develop- ment of mental disorder. Impulsivity may be defined as ‘a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli with diminished regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individual or to others’ (Chamberlain and Sahakian, 2007; Potenza, 2007b). In contrast, compulsivity represents a tendency to perform unpleasantly repetitive acts in a habitual or stereotyped manner to prevent perceived negative conse- quences, leading to functional impairment (WHO, 1992; Hollander and Cohen, 1996; Chamberlain et al, 2006b). These two constructs may be viewed as diametrically opposed, or alternatively, as similar, in that each implies a dysfunction of impulse control (Stein and Hollander, 1995). Each potentially involves alteration within a wide range of neural processes, including attention, perception, and coordination of motor or cognitive responses. Received 7 July 2009; revised 5 September 2009; accepted 17 September 2009 *Correspondence: Dr NA Fineberg, Department of Psychiatry, National OCDs Specialist Centre, Hertfordshire Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital, Howlands, Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire AL7 4HQ, UK, Tel: + 44 170 736 5085, E-mail: [email protected] Neuropsychopharmacology (2009), 1–14 & 2009 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/09 $32.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org
14

Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

May 27, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

Review

Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from AnimalModels to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review

Naomi A Fineberg*,1,2,3, Marc N Potenza4, Samuel R Chamberlain1,3, Heather A Berlin5, Lara Menzies3,Antoine Bechara6, Barbara J Sahakian3, Trevor W Robbins7, Edward T Bullmore3,8 and Eric Hollander9

1Department of Psychiatry, National OCDs Specialist Centre, Hertfordshire Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital,

Welwyn Garden City, Hertfordshire, UK; 2Postgraduate Medical School, University of Hertfordshire, College Lane, Hatfield, UK; 3Department of

Psychiatry and Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK; 4Departments of Psychiatry and Child

Study Center, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA; 5Department of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy

Place, New York, NY, USA; 6Psychology Department and Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA;7Department of Experimental Psychology and Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK;8Addenbrooke’s Centre for Clinical Investigations, Clinical Unit Cambridge, Drug Discovery, GlaxoSmith-Kline R&D, Addenbrooke’s Center for

Clinical Investigations, Cambridge, UK; 9Montefiore Medical Center, University Hospital of Albert Einstein College of Medicine, NY, USA

Failures in cortical control of fronto-striatal neural circuits may underpin impulsive and compulsive acts. In this narrative review, we

explore these behaviors from the perspective of neural processes and consider how these behaviors and neural processes contribute to

mental disorders such as obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), obsessive–compulsive personality disorder, and impulse-control

disorders such as trichotillomania and pathological gambling. We present findings from a broad range of data, comprising translational and

human endophenotypes research and clinical treatment trials, focussing on the parallel, functionally segregated, cortico-striatal neural

projections, from orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) to medial striatum (caudate nucleus), proposed to drive compulsive activity, and from the

anterior cingulate/ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens shell), proposed to drive impulsive activity,

and the interaction between them. We suggest that impulsivity and compulsivity each seem to be multidimensional. Impulsive or

compulsive behaviors are mediated by overlapping as well as distinct neural substrates. Trichotillomania may stand apart as a disorder of

motor-impulse control, whereas pathological gambling involves abnormal ventral reward circuitry that identifies it more closely with

substance addiction. OCD shows motor impulsivity and compulsivity, probably mediated through disruption of OFC-caudate circuitry, as

well as other frontal, cingulate, and parietal connections. Serotonin and dopamine interact across these circuits to modulate aspects of

both impulsive and compulsive responding and as yet unidentified brain-based systems may also have important functions. Targeted

application of neurocognitive tasks, receptor-specific neurochemical probes, and brain systems neuroimaging techniques have potential

for future research in this field.

Neuropsychopharmacology advance online publication, 25 November 2009; doi:10.1038/npp.2009.185

Keywords: impulsive; compulsive; endophenotypes; serotonin; dopamine

�������������������������������������������������������������������

INTRODUCTION

Whereas acts with impulsive or compulsive features maycontribute specifically to creativity and endurance andgenerally to adaptive human behavior, disordered regulationof impulsive or compulsive behavior may be associated withadverse consequences and have a function in the develop-ment of mental disorder. Impulsivity may be defined as ‘a

predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internalor external stimuli with diminished regard to the negativeconsequences of these reactions to the impulsive individualor to others’ (Chamberlain and Sahakian, 2007; Potenza,2007b). In contrast, compulsivity represents a tendency toperform unpleasantly repetitive acts in a habitual orstereotyped manner to prevent perceived negative conse-quences, leading to functional impairment (WHO, 1992;Hollander and Cohen, 1996; Chamberlain et al, 2006b). Thesetwo constructs may be viewed as diametrically opposed, oralternatively, as similar, in that each implies a dysfunction ofimpulse control (Stein and Hollander, 1995). Each potentiallyinvolves alteration within a wide range of neural processes,including attention, perception, and coordination of motoror cognitive responses.

Received 7 July 2009; revised 5 September 2009; accepted 17September 2009

*Correspondence: Dr NA Fineberg, Department of Psychiatry,National OCDs Specialist Centre, Hertfordshire Partnership NHSFoundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital, Howlands, WelwynGarden City, Hertfordshire AL7 4HQ, UK, Tel: + 44 170 736 5085,E-mail: [email protected]

Neuropsychopharmacology (2009), 1–14& 2009 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/09 $32.00

www.neuropsychopharmacology.org

Page 2: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

Neuroanatomical models posit the existence of separatebut intercommunicating ‘compulsive’ and ‘impulsive’ corti-co-striatal circuits, differentially modulated by neurotrans-mitters (Robbins, 2007; Brewer and Potenza, 2008). In thecompulsive circuit, a striatal component (caudate nucleus)may drive compulsive behaviors and a prefrontal compo-nent (orbitofrontal cortex, OFC) may exert inhibitorycontrol over them. Similarly, in the impulsive circuit, astriatal component (ventral striatum/nucleus accumbensshell) may drive impulsive behaviors and a prefrontalcomponent (anterior cingulate/ventromedial prefrontalcortex, VMPFC) may exert inhibitory control. Thus, in thismodel, there exist at least two striatal neural circuitries (onecompulsive and one impulsive) that drive these behaviors,and two corresponding prefrontal circuitries that restrainthese behaviors. Hyperactivity within the striatal compo-nents or abnormalities (presumably hypoactivity) in theprefrontal components may thus result in an increasedautomatic tendency for executing impulsive or compulsivebehaviors, depending on the sub-component afflicted.Other possible abnormalities within cortico-striatal circuits(eg related to diminished striatal activation to rewards) mayalso contribute to seemingly impulsive or compulsivebehaviors during engagement in reward-related behaviors.These pathologies can be explored using tasks of cognitiveperformance that tap into these specific functions and/or byfunctional imaging studies that measure activity withinthese neural systems. Overlap between these functionalsystems, so that what starts out as a problem in theimpulsive circuit may end up as a problem in thecompulsive circuit and vice versa, may contribute towardthe impulsive–compulsive diathesis model proposed byHollander and Wong (1995) (Brewer and Potenza, 2008).

There exist certain mental disorders for which impulsiveand compulsive behaviors seem, at least on phenotypicgrounds, to be the core and most damaging ingredient.These often highly heritable disorders, currently classifiedacross several DSM-IV-TR (APA) diagnostic categories,include obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), body dys-morphic disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, trichotillomania,attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), pathologi-cal gambling, and substance addictions (SAs). Of interest,autism is characterized by both compulsive behavior (asone of the three core symptom domains) as well asimpulsive behavior (as one of the associated symptomdomains).

Traditionally, compulsive and impulsive disorders havebeen viewed at opposite ends of a single dimension; theformer driven by a desire to avoid harm and the latter byreward-seeking behavior. However, convergent evidencefrom translational studies suggests that a shared tendencytoward behavioral disinhibition, presumably resulting fromfailures in ‘top–down’ cortical control of fronto-striatalcircuits, or alternatively from overactivity within striatalcircuitry, may crucially underpin both impulsive andcompulsive disorders. Thus, rather than polar opposites,compulsivity and impulsivity may represent key orthogonalfactors that each contribute to varying degrees across thesedisorders.

Many of these disorders tend to occur together, eitherwithin the same individual or clustering within families,implying the possibility of shared pathophysiological

mechanisms (Hollander et al, 2007b). Moreover, there isevidence of overlap in the treatment-response across somedisorders. OCD typically responds to serotonin reuptakeinhibitors (SRIs; clomipramine and selective SRIs, SSRIs)and to SSRIs combined with antipsychotic agents (Fineberget al, 2005). Antipsychotics represent first-line treatment forTourette’s syndrome, and it is, therefore, interesting thattheir combination with SSRIs shows greater efficacy in tic-related OCD (Bloch et al, 2006). Compulsions associatedwith autistic disorders may also respond to low-dose SSRIand to antipsychotics (Kolevzon et al, 2006). Trichotillo-mania may respond to SRIs and to antipsychotics, thoughconfirmation in controlled studies is required (Chamberlainet al, 2007d). ADHD, on the other hand, responds tonoradrenergic reuptake inhibitors as well as dopaminergicagents (eg amphetamine), pathological gambling, andsubstance abuse disorders may also share a therapeuticresponse to opiate antagonists (Brewer et al, 2008).

Attribution of cause and effect, using clinical data alone,may be confounded by the multiplicity of associatedsymptom domains that occur within complex mentaldisorders. Indeed, this group of disorders is characterizedby considerable phenotypic heterogeneity and overlap. Forexample, some cases with autism show no symptoms ofADHD or compulsive behavior, others show ADHD, othersOCD, and yet others show repetitive motor behaviors thatdo not resemble OCD. Translational research investigatesfrom the perspective of underlying mechanisms, and maythus be capable of pinpointing neural contributions drivingspecific aspects of mental disorder. Endophenotypes aremeasurable, heritable traits, theoretically situated in anintermediate position between the clinical phenotype andthe disease-susceptibility genotype. Such ‘intermediatephenotypes’ are hypothesized to be more directly relatedto genetic risk for polygenic mental disorders than clini-cally expressed behaviors (Gottesman and Gould, 2003;Chamberlain and Menzies, 2009). Endophenotypic modelsof disease may be helpful for clarifying our understandingof the genetic basis of complex brain disorders and thus forinforming diagnostic classification. Currently, impulsiveand compulsive disorders are classified within disparateDSM-IV categories. As the American Psychiatric Associa-tion considers the re-classification of OCD, anxietydisorders and impulse-control disorders (ICDs) for theforthcoming DSM-V revision (Fineberg et al, 2007a), it istimely to review the underpinning mechanisms of thesedisorders.

In this narrative review, we consider the neural andneuropsychological mechanisms associated with impulsiveand compulsive acts and their contribution toward exam-ples of impulsive and compulsive disorders. We assemblerelevant findings from a broad range of complementarydata, comprising recently published and as yet unpublishedtranslational studies, human endophenotypic research, andclinical treatment trials, including ongoing work from ourown units in the United Kingdom and the United States.Our analysis focusses on probing the parallel, functionallysegregated, cortico-striatal neural projections from OFC tomedial striatum (caudate nucleus), proposed to drivecompulsive activity, and from the anterior cingulate/VMPFC to the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens shell),proposed to drive impulsive activity, and the cross-talk

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

2

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 3: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

between them (Robbins, 2007; Brewer and Potenza, 2008)(Figure 1).

Using these data, we attempt to address key questionsincluding: (i) how much do compulsivity and impulsivitycontribute to these disorders, (ii) to what extent do theydepend on shared or separate neural circuitry, (iii) what arethe mediating monoaminergic mechanisms, (iv) do im-pulsive or compulsive behavioral components have anyprognostic value related to clinical treatment, and (v) isthere a unifying-dimensional model that fully accommo-

dates these data? We also draw attention to prospectsfor future research we believe may most fruitfully advancethe field.

TRANSLATIONAL MODELS OF IMPULSIVITY ANDCOMPULSIVITY

Objective neurocognitive tests hold potential for elucidatingthe mechanisms by which pharmacological agents exert

Relative functional overactivity

Relative functional underactivity

Disorder OCD ObsessiveCompulsivePersonalityDisorder

Trichotilloman-ia

PathologicalGambling

DSM-IVclassification

AnxietyDisorder

Axis IICluster C

Impulse ControlDisorder notelsewherespecified

Impulse ControlDisorder notelsewherespecified

ObservedBehavior

Obsessions,Compulsions,Rigid

Rigid Repetitivebody-focussedacts

Repetitivereward-focussedacts

Compulsivityand/orImpulsivity

MotorImpulsivity andCompulsivity

Compulsivity MotorImpulsivity

Reward-SeekingImpulsivity andSpecificCompulsivity

CandidateFronto-striatalCircuitry

OFC,plusVLPFC,RIFC,ACC

Caudate /Putamen

VLPFC RIFC

OFCplusVMPFC,ventralACC

Caudate(?) Putamen

VentralStriatum/NA

Figure 1 Compulsivity and impulsivity: candidate neural processes contributing to mental disorders. Although impulsive and compulsive disorders can bethought of as polar opposites, failures in cortical control of fronto-striatal neural circuits may underpin both compulsivity (orbitofrontal cortex(OFC)Fcaudate) and impulsivity (right inferior frontal cortex (RIFC)Fglobus pallidus and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)/ventromedial prefrontal cortex(VMPFC)Fventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (NA) shell), and contribute to these disorders.

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

3

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 4: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

their beneficial clinical effects and for predicting clinicaloutcomes (Chamberlain et al, 2007e; Brewer and Potenza,2008). Using sensitive and domain-specific neurocognitivetasks, impulsivity and compulsivity may be fractionatedinto separate and quantifiable neurobiologically specificdomains in human beings and experimental animals, withspecific aspects involving dissociable components of fronto-striatal circuitry (Winstanley et al, 2006).

Data indicate that impulsivity may derive from one ormore distinct neurocognitive mechanisms. These include atendency to pre-potent motor disinhibition, measured bythe stop signal reaction time (SSRT) task (Aron andPoldrack, 2005), mediated in human beings throughactivation of right inferior frontal (RIF) cortex and itssubcortical connections (Rubia et al, 2003) and modulatedin rats and human beings by norepinephrine (Chamberlainet al, 2006c, 2007a; Cottrell et al, 2008), but not serotonin(Clark et al, 2005; Chamberlain et al, 2006d). Another aspectinvolves difficulty in delaying gratification and choosingimmediate small rewards despite negative long-term con-sequences, measured by decision making or gambling taskssuch as the Cambridge Gambling Task (CANTAB),mediated through orbitofrontal and related cortical circui-try under probable serotonergic modulation (Rogers et al,1999b), and subcortical circuitry under joint dopaminergicand serotonergic control (Winstanley et al, 2006). A thirdcomponent comprises insufficient information samplingbefore making a choice, measured by information samplingtasks such as the Reflection Task (Clark et al, 2006) andpossibly the 5-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task (5-CSRTT)(Robbins, 2002) (Table 1).

Compulsivity is, perhaps, less well understood. Failures in(i) reversal learning (ie the ability to adapt behavior afternegative feedback, measured by specific reversal learningtasks) and (ii) extra-dimensional (ED) attentional set-shifting, may each contribute toward its expression (Diaset al, 1996; Clarke et al, 2005). Both deficits constitutemeasures of cognitive inflexibility, but each seems sub-served by separate neural circuitry.

Reversal learning is impaired by lesions to the OFC (butnot dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DLPFC) across species(Dias et al, 1996; Berlin et al, 2004; Hornak et al, 2004;Boulougouris et al, 2007). In human beings, the OFCactivates selectively during reversal learning (Hampshireand Owen, 2006). In contrast, lesions to the lateral PFCimpair ED set-shifting in primates (Dias et al, 1996), and inhuman beings performance of the task is associated withselective activation of the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontalcortex (VLPFC) (Hampshire and Owen, 2006) (Table 1).

There is now considerable evidence linking reversallearning with 5-HT mechanisms, including in rodents(Masaki et al, 2006; Boulougouris et al, 2008; Lapiz-Bluhmet al, 2009), non-human primates (Clarke et al, 2004, 2005;Walker et al, 2009), and human beings (Park et al, 1994;Rogers et al, 1999a; Evers et al, 2005) based on pharma-cological, neurochemical and dietary manipulations, andevidence of genetic polymorphisms in rhesus monkeys(Izquierdo et al, 2007). Generally, reducing brain serotonin,especially in specific regions such as the OFC (eg Clarkeet al, 2004), impairs reversal learning. Systemic adminis-tration of a 5-HT-2A receptor antagonist has also beenshown to impair spatial reversal learning (Boulougouriset al, 2008). A 5-HT6 receptor antagonist has also beenshown to enhance both reversal learning and attentionalshifting in rats (Hatcher et al, 2005). However, there havebeen some failures to find effects on reversal learning, oftenafter tryptophan depletion, in human beings (Talbot et al,2006) and rats (van der Plasse and Feenstra, 2008), andserotonin transporter deficiency in rats also does not seemto affect simple spatial reversal (Homberg et al, 2007).

5-HT2 RECEPTOR SUBTYPES MAY UNDERPINCOMPULSIVE BEHAVIORS

A multiplicity of 5-HT receptors has been identified forwhich specific ligands are under development. Preliminaryevidence from animal and human studies suggests a

Table 1 Subdividing Impulsivity and Compulsivity According to Neurocognitive Domains: Tasks and Neural/Neurochemical Correlates

Neurocognitive domain Definition Task Neural system Neurochemistry

Impulsivity

Motor impulsivity Prepotent motor disinhibition Stop signal reaction time task(SSRT)

Right inferior frontal cortexand subcortical connections

Norepinephrine

Decision-making impulsivity Difficulty in delayinggratification and choosingimmediate small rewardsdespite negative long-termconsequences

Decision making or gamblingtasks (eg Cambridge GamblingTask (CANTAB), Iowa gambletask)

Orbitofrontal cortex andsubcortical connections

CortexFserotoninSubcortical circuitry-serotonin/dopamine

Reflection impulsivity Insufficient informationsampling before making achoice

Reflection task, 5-CSRTT Not known Not known

Compulsivity

Cognitive inflexibility:reversal learning

Inability to adapt behaviorafter negative feedback

Reversal learning tasks Orbitofrontal cortex andsubcortical connections

Serotonin

Cognitive inflexibility:attentional set-shifting

Inability to switch attentionbetween stimuli

Extra-dimensional attentionalset-shifting (CANTAB)

Ventrolateral PFCFhumans.Lateral PFCFprimates andsubcortical connections

Dopamine

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

4

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 5: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

function for 5-HT2 receptors in compulsive behaviors.Transgenic mice lacking 5-HT2C receptors develop com-pulsive behavior patterns that constitute a plausible modelfor OCD (Chou-Green et al, 2003). However, there is anapparent mismatch of data obtained from this geneticpreparation with other data, possibly because of unspecifieddeveloped compensatory processes in the transgenic pre-paration, as recent pharmacological data indicate theopposite finding that 5-HT2C receptor activation isassociated with increased compulsivity. Thus, in a rewardedT-maze alternation rat model of OCD, Tsaltas et al (2005)found that administration of m-chlorophenylpiperazine(mCPP), a mixed serotonin agonist with potent 5-HT2Cagonist effects, increased persistence or compulsivity ofresponding, whereas chronic pretreatment with an SSRI(fluoxetine), but not a benzodiazepine or desipramine,abolished the effects of mCPP. Challenge with the 5-HT1Breceptor agonist naratriptan had no effect on compulsivitywithin this model, suggesting a specific function for the5-HT2C receptor, which may be down-regulated by chronicSSRI treatment. In OCD patients, acute pharmacologicalchallenge with mCPP exacerbated OCD symptomatology(Hollander et al, 1991b). This effect was also attenuated bypretreatment with fluoxetine (Hollander et al, 1991a) andclomipramine (Zohar et al, 1988). Moreover, consistent withthese findings, Boulougouris et al (2008) found that a5-HT2C receptor antagonist improved reversal learning. Onthe other hand, activation of prefrontal 5-HT2A receptorshas been proposed to underpin the anticompulsive effect ofSSRIs (Westenberg et al, 2007). Second generation anti-psychotics may exacerbate compulsive behaviors in patientswith schizophrenia, and it has been proposed that thisoccurs through potent 5-HT2A antagonism (Poyurovskyet al, 2008), though dopamine (DA) receptor antagonismrepresents another possible mechanism. Moreover, secondand first generation antipsychotics show clinical efficacywhen combined with SSRIs in OCD (Fineberg and Gale,2005), perhaps by increasing DA activity within the frontalcortex (Denys et al, 2004).

PHARMACOLOGICAL DIFFERENTIATION OFIMPULSIVITY AND COMPULSIVITY; RECEPTORLIGANDS

In animal models, an intriguing dissociation between theeffects of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists onmeasures of impulsivity and compulsivity has beenobserved. On the 5-CSRTT, systemic administration of a5-HT2C receptor antagonist (SB24284) exacerbated theenhanced impulsivity normally observed after global 5-HTdepletion produced by intracerebroventricular administra-tion of 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine; a similar SB24284-relatedenhancement in impulsivity was seen in sham-operated rats(Winstanley et al, 2004). In contrast, systemic administra-tion of a selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonist (M100907)had opposite actions, remediating impulsivity in bothsham-operated and 5-HT-depleted rats. These contrastinginfluences of the 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonistswere mimicked by infusions of the drugs into the nucleusaccumbens, but not the mPFC, in intact animals (Cottrellet al, 2008). However, in variations of the 5-CSRTT, it was

possible to detect significant reductions in impulsivity afterintra-mPFC infusion of the 5-HT2A receptor antagonist.The latter findings were consistent with observations that,in a population of Lister hooded rats, it was generally themost impulsive animals that had the greatest concentrationsof 5-HT in the mPFC, indicating that individual differencesand regional specificity are important considerations inunderstanding the relationship between 5-HT and beha-vioral disinhibition.

The effects of central 5-HT manipulations on impulsivitystand in some contrast to their actions on attentionalfunction per se in the 5-CSRTT. Several papers haveobserved either no effects or actual enhancement ofattentional accuracy when impulsive behavior is enhanced(Harrison et al, 1997) or after treatment with systemic orintra-PFC 5-HT2A receptor antagonists such as ketanserinor M100907 (Passetti et al, 2003; Winstanley et al, 2003) aswell as the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OHDPAT (Winstan-ley et al, 2003). These findings are compatible with thehypothesis that inhibitory control over impulsive behaviorand attentional function are only loosely coupled in this testsituation and suggests that there will be no simplerelationship between the two in such syndromes as ADHD.

An additional element of complexity is introduced whenconsidering the influences of these same drugs on measuresof compulsivity. Using a simple serial spatial reversal testthat is sensitive to lesions of the OFC (Boulougouris et al,2007), it was found that 5-HT2C receptor antagonism(produced by systemic administration) facilitated reversallearning. M1000907 had the opposite effect of impairing it(Tsaltas et al, 2005). Note that in terms of remediation, thisis opposite to what that was found for measures ofimpulsivity. Similar enhancements of reversal learning aftertreatment with the 5-HT2C antagonist were also found afterinfusion into the OFC (Boulougouris, Glennon, Robbins,unpublished results) (Table 2).

Regardless of precise elucidation of mechanism, thesedata pharmacologically dissociate these forms of impulsiv-ity and compulsivity, suggesting that they cannot hinge on acommon process of behavioral inhibition. The dissociationcannot easily be explained in terms of differences in species,drug, or dose of receptor antagonist used or the form ofmotivation used; they must be task–dependentFas bothtasks require response inhibition for efficient performance.Thus, we conclude that there is some other aspect of theprocesses engaged by the task, which differentiates them.These results also imply that impulsivity and compulsivity

Table 2 Differential Effects of 5-HT2C and 5-HT2A ReceptorAntagonists on Rat Models of Impulsivity and Compulsivity

Compulsivity(reversal learning task) Impulsivity (5CSRTT)

5-HT2C antagonist(SB24284)

Reduced Increased

5-HT2A antagonist(M100907)

Increased Reduced

Hypothesized-mediatingneuroanatomy

Neural projectionsfrom OFC to the caudatenucleus (dorsomedialstriatum in the rat)

Neural projections fromVMPFC (area 25) to theshell of the nucleusaccumbens

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

5

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 6: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

are functionally separate and reciprocally yoked, lendingsupport to the impulsive–compulsive diathesis model(Hollander and Wong, 1995). They also suggest thatimpulsivity and compulsivity can be dissociated by selective5-HT2 receptor ligands and hint at new clinical applicationsfor such agents. However, it will be important to resolvehow these data fit with the consistent finding that 5-HTdepletion in the OFC impairs visual object reversal learningin marmoset monkeys (Clarke et al, 2004, 2005; Yucel et al,2007). In addition, it would seem likely that these seeminglyopposed effects are mediated through separate neuralpathways: in the case of impulsivity, through projectionsfrom the infralimbic VMPFC (area 25), an area richlyinnervated by 5-HT2A receptors and strongly implicated inaffective regulation, toward the shell of the nucleusaccumbens (Vertes, 2004) and, in the case of compulsivity,in connections between the OFC and the caudatenucleus (or the dorsomedial striatum in the rat) (Schilmanet al, 2008).

DISSOCIATING IMPULSIVE AND COMPULSIVEDISORDERS USING NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TASKS

Impulsive and compulsive disorders usually involve arelatively diminished ability to delay or inhibit repetitivethoughts or behaviors. Thus, problems suppressing orinhibiting inappropriate behavior could underpin bothimpulsive and compulsive symptomatology (Chamberlainet al, 2005; Stein et al, 2006). ADHD is a disorder of earlyonset characterized by poorly conceived, impulsive actionsand robust impairment in motor inhibition as measured ontasks such as the SSRT (Aron et al, 2003; Lijffijt et al, 2005).Administration of cognition-enhancing agents such asatomoxetine and methylphenidate improves symptomsand ameliorates SSRT deficits in adults with ADHD,presumably acting through increased noradrenergic (orpossibly dopaminergic) neurotransmission (Chamberlainet al, 2007a).

Studies in OCD patients have revealed SSRT impairmentand poor performance on ED-shifting tasks (Chamberlainet al, 2006a, 2007c; Menzies et al, 2007a), implying bothimpulsive and compulsive contributions to the disorder.Unaffected first-degree relatives of OCD probands sharesimilar impairment on SSRT and ED-shifting tasks (Cham-berlain et al, 2007c) and thus seem to exhibit similar levelsof motor impulsivity and cognitive inflexibility, despite alack of OCD symptoms. In contrast to OCD, application of asimilar neurocognitive test battery to individuals withtrichotillomania showed a more focal and selective impair-ment in motor inhibition, consistent with its DSM-IVclassification as an ICD (Chamberlain et al, 2006b, 2007b).Whole-brain MRI in unmedicated trichotillomania identi-fied increased gray-matter density in the left putamen andmultiple cortical regions (Chamberlain et al, 2008b).Increased gray matter in striatal regions has also beenreported in studies of Tourette syndrome (Bohlhalter et al,2006; Garraux et al, 2006) and OCD (Menzies et al, 2008a).On the other hand, patients with Tourette’s syndrome werefound to share cognitive inflexibility and to be significantlymore impaired than OCD patients on decision-makingtasks, but less impaired on a task of motor inhibition

(Watkins et al, 2005), though another study investigatingadolescents with Tourette’s did not find evidence ofimpaired reward learning compared with controls on agamble task (Crawford et al, 2005). Li et al (2006) failed toshow performance deficits compared with controls on theSSRT in 30 children with Tourette’s syndrome.

The overlap of compulsive and impulsive respondingwithin OCD raises the question whether impulsivitynormally drives compulsivity, and thus whether it ispossible to show pathological compulsivity without motorimpulsivity. If so, which disorders might show ‘pure’compulsivity? Individuals with obsessive–compulsive per-sonality disorder comorbid with OCD showed increasedimpairment specifically in the domain of ED shifting. Thisfinding is consistent with the clinical presentation ofobsessive–compulsive personality disorder, which is char-acterized by excessive cognitive and behavioral inflexibility,but does not involve repetitive behaviors (ie obsessions orcompulsions). Thus, obsessive–compulsive personality dis-order may constitute a prototypic-compulsive disorder(Fineberg et al, 2007b). Confirmatory studies usingindividuals with non-comorbid OCPD would be welcomed.

NEUROCOGNITIVE ENDOPHENOTYPES, OCD, ANDBEYOND

Whereas neurocognitive tasks may be used to identify fairlyspecific neuropsychological domains, complementary neu-roimaging may be used to visualize the anatomicalsubstrates and neural circuits underlying genetic risk for adisorder. By integrating neurocognitive and structural MRIparameters, using a whole-brain multivariate analysis(technique of partial least squares, McIntosh and Lobaugh,2004) and a novel permutation test, Menzies et al (2007a)identified familial effects on performance on a motor-inhibition task (the SSRT) that were associated withvariation in multiple anatomical sites. Both OCD patientsand their unaffected first-degree relatives exhibited im-paired motor inhibitory control, indexed by prolongedlatency of the SSRT and longer latency was associated withboth decreased gray-matter volume in the OFC and RIFcortex (areas conventionally associated with OCD and SSRTactivation, respectively) and increased gray-matter volumein areas of the striatum, cingulate, and parietal cortex.These results argue for the first structural MRI endophe-notype-mediating familial, and possibly genetic, risk forOCD-related impulsivity. Future studies might profitablytest for specific genetic effects on variability in suchintermediate phenotypes, as an alternative to classicalassociation designs, for discovery of susceptibility alleles.

The findings with the SSRT, a relatively disease-non-specific task of motor impulsivity, raise the possibility thatsuch an endophenotype may not be restricted to OCD, butin addition relate to other disorders within, and perhapsoutside, the impulsive–compulsive disorders spectrum. Forexample, individuals with ADHD and their relatives seemimpaired on motor-inhibition tasks (Crosbie and Schachar,2001), but it is not yet clear whether the anatomicalcorrelates of impairment for those with familial risk forADHD are the same or differ from people with familial riskfor OCD.

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

6

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 7: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

The within-subject correlation between decreased gray-matter volumes within frontal areas of cortex and increasedvolumes in the striatum resonates with empirical OCDmodels derived from early functional imaging studies(Baxter et al, 1987) and later structural and functionalMRI studies (for review see, Menzies et al, 2008a).Preliminary findings from a subsequent study usingdiffusion tensor imaging within OCD family members(Menzies et al, 2008b) identified evidence of white-matterabnormalities in complementary brain areas including theright medial frontal (adjacent to anterior cingulate cortex,ACC) and right inferior parietal (adjacent to parietal cortex)zones, compatible with results from a prior study involvingOCD patients (Szeszko et al, 2005). However, by extendingthis study to include unaffected OCD family members, wehave proposed these findings as possible white-matterendophenotypes for OCD (Menzies et al, 2008b).

In addition to structural brain abnormalities in patientswith OCD and their relatives, research has started to probefunctional integrity of fronto-striatal circuitry using fMRIparadigms adapted for this purpose. Using an fMRIcognitive flexibility paradigm, it was shown that patientswith OCD and their unaffected first-degree relativesexhibited under-activation of the bilateral lateral OFCduring reversal of responses; they also tended to under-activate lateral aspects of the PFC during ED shifting attrend levels (Chamberlain et al, 2008a).

Together, these findings indicate that neuroimagingtechniques can provide a rich source of candidateendophenotypes for OCD. The results are compatible withtheories implicating failure of top–down cortical inhibitionof striatally mediated behaviors. They suggest that theidiosyncratic obsessive ruminations and compulsive ritualsthat characterize OCD are accompanied by more generalpropensities toward rigid and disinhibited behavior that areshared among non-affected family members. Thus, diffi-culties in ‘cognitive inhibition and flexibility’ may causallycontribute to the development of symptoms of OCD. Futurework should examine whether this approach can besuccessfully generalized to other disorders on the impul-sive–compulsive spectrum. The clinical relevance of puta-tive endophenotypes requires additional investigation todetermine whether (and how) unaffected relatives whoshare trait markers with OCD probands might be differ-entiated from non-OCD-related controls. An improvedunderstanding is needed of mechanisms by which environ-mental factors might elicit OCD in genetically vulnerableindividuals, and whether or how treatments could helpmodify disease onset.

ICDs AND MODELS OF REWARD

In contrast to compulsive disorders such as OCD, someICDs, such as pathological gambling, are characterized bychoosing short-term gratification irrespective of negativelong-term consequences. Berlin et al (2008) comparedindividuals with and without pathological gambling on aselected neuropsychological battery (Berlin et al, 2008).Individuals with pathological gambling who scored morehighly on self-reported measures of impulsivity such as theBarratt Impulsivity Scale had on average a faster subjective

sense of time (overestimated time) compared with controlsand showed deficits measured by a frontal behaviorquestionnaire considered to reflect prefrontal-corticaldysfunction. Subjects with pathological gambling alsoshowed disadvantageous decision making on the IowaGamble Task (Bechara et al, 1994) and executive planningdeficits (eg on Spatial Planning and Stockings of Cambridgesubtests of CANTAB), implicating prefrontal circuitryincluding the OFC/VMPFC region. In contrast to OCD(Watkins et al, 2005; Chamberlain et al, 2006b), set-shiftingwas unimpaired in pathological gambling. However, otherstudies indicate that individuals with pathological gamblingscore highly on specific measures of compulsivity or harmavoidance, and that measures of impulsivity and compul-sivity may change over time (eg, during the course oftreatment (Potenza, 2007a; Blanco et al, 2009). Thesefindings suggest that impulsivity and compulsivity are notdiametrically opposed and share a complex, orthogonalrelationship, with specific disorders showing a predomi-nance of one construct over the other that may shift in atemporally dynamic manner.

Hollander et al (2007a) compared three groups of age-and gender-matched individuals, comprising pathologicalgambling (predominantly impulsive) and OCD and autism(predominantly compulsive) disorders, using a battery ofclinical, cognitive, and functional imaging tasks. Duringexecution of response-inhibition tasks (go/no-go) thatnormally activate fronto-striatal circuitry, all three spec-trum-disorder groups showed abnormal fMRI activation indorsal (cognitive) and ventral (emotional) regions of theACC compared with healthy controls. There were nosignificant performance differences between the fourgroups. However, between-group analyses showed de-creased dorsal ACC activation in all three patient groupsrelative to healthy controls. Thus, during response inhibi-tion, both compulsive and impulsive disorders werecharacterized by diminished dorsal ACC activation, whichmay contribute toward failure to properly inhibit motoricbehaviors across these disorders.

When individual activation patterns of the ventral ACCwere correlated with measures of impulsivity or compulsiv-ity, disorder-specific between-group differences emerged.Within the pathological gambling group, increased ventralACC/ventral striatum activation correlated positively withclinical measures of increased impulsive reward-seekingbehavior (as measured by TCI Impulsiveness and TotalHarm Avoidance, NEO-FFI Extraversion, Total TimeEstimation, and the Iowa Gambling Task). Furthermore,gamblers with increased activation in the ventral ACC (area25) showed lower compulsivity scores on tasks of cognitiveset-shifting (ID/ED stages completed). In contrast, in theautistic (compulsive) group, increased ventral ACC/ventralstriatum activity correlated with increased severity ofcompulsive distress-relieving (reinforcing) habits, andincreased activation within the same areas of the ventralACC (area 25) correlated with increased compulsivity (ID/ED shift total errors adjusted) and decreased impulsivity onthe Time Estimation task.

This ‘double-dissociation’ suggests that in pathologicalgambling and autism, prevailing differences in neuromo-dulation impact on ventral corticostriatal pathways duringbehavioral inhibition, which in pathological gambling may

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

7

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 8: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

primarily drive impulsivity and in autism drive compulsiv-ity. It is also reminiscent of data from rats describedelsewhere here showing opposing effects of 5-HT2C and5-HT2A receptor antagonists on impulsivity in the 5-CSRTTand compulsivity (spatial serial reversal learning) (Tsaltaset al, 2005; Boulougouris et al, 2007)Fand also of thedoubly dissociable findings of Carli et alFthat infusions ofthe 5-HT1A agonist into the infralimbic region reducedperseverative behavior (on the 5-CSRTT) without influen-cing impulsive responding, with a 5-HT2A receptorantagonist having the opposite effect (Chambers et al,2004). Together, these findings suggest that the same neuralcircuitry may drive impulsive or compulsive aspects ofhuman behavior and that 5-HT subtypes in VMPFC(5-HT2A) and OFC (5-HT2C), and dorsal ACC deficits,may have a function in the failure of response inhibition inpredominantly impulsive (pathological gambling) andcompulsive (OCD, autism) disorders.

REWARD, REINFORCEMENT, AND DA

DA pathways in the mesolimbic system have an importantfunction in reward and reinforcement (Wise, 2002). Indisorders of impulse control, increased ventral ACCactivation during response inhibition may be related toincreased reward-seeking behavior. Preliminary resultssuggest that pathological gamblers are less sensitive toreward on the TCI reward dependence inventory thanhealthy controls and seek higher levels of stimulation(novelty seeking) (Berlin et al, 2008). However, otherstudies of subjects with pathological gambling have foundrelatively diminished activation of ACC, particularly in itsventral component, during appetitive states and cognitive-control experiments (Potenza et al, 2003a, b). With respectto compulsive disorders, the positive correlation betweenincreased ventral ACC activation during response-inhibi-tion tasks and increased compulsivity on ID/ED stages andtotal errors adjusted may reflect increased dopaminergicactivity from a relative deficit, in line with a mesolimbic DAmodel of OCD (Joel, 2006).

Hypothetically, intermittent and repeated stimulation ofmesolimbic DA pathways may ‘sensitize’ the reward systemand lead to escalation in reward seeking (Robinson andBerridge, 1993), which, if combined with poor prefrontal-cortex-mediated inhibitory control, may facilitate DArelated and seemingly impulsive-motivated behaviors.Excessive DA release and stimulation may deplete DAstores and lead to anhedonia and depression (Koob andLe Moal, 1997). Indeed, in substance abusers, decreasedactivity of the mesolimbic/mesocortical DA system, asmeasured by electrophysiological recordings and in vivomicrodialysis, intensifies after escalations in drug intake.This may generate an urge (compulsion) to seek strongerrewards to ‘replenish’ the DA deficiency. The demonstrationof decreased striatal D2-like receptors in chronic cocaineusers, by PET imaging (Volkow et al, 1999), suggests down-regulation in response to persistently elevated postsynapticDA concentrations, consistent with the hypothesis of adysregulated DA system after repeated stimulation of DArelease. Thus, what starts as increased DA release leading toincreased ventral ACC activity and increased reward

seeking (Wise, 2002) may end as a compulsive drive towardincreased levels of reward stimulation to restore a resultantDA deficiency. This compulsive drive may be exacerbatedby deficient impulse control and decision making, linked tothe orbitofrontal, ventromedial prefrontal, and ACC (Adin-off, 2004). However, the extent to which this hypothesisrelates to specific ICDs requires direct investigation.

INTEGRATING MECHANISMS OF INHIBITORYCONTROL, REWARD, AND DA

Models of compulsivity and impulsivity posit a balancebetween 5-HT (2A, 2C) receptor activity in VMPFC/OFCregions regulating aspects of response inhibition, and DAtone in the ventral loops linking ventral ACC with ventralstriatum/nucleus accumbens regulating reward and rein-forcement behavior. DA neurotransmission, particularlyphasic release, in the nucleus accumbens has beenassociated with reward seeking and reinforcement (Schultz,2002). Unexpected punishment (monetary loss) has beenproposed to result in a dip in central dopaminergic activity,reversal learning, and diminished reward seeking (Franket al, 2007). Pro-dopamanergic drugs, including levo-dopaand pramipexole (a D2-like DA receptor agonist), have beenassociated with altering reversal learning to unexpectedpunishment and ICDs in patients with Parkinson’s disease(Cools, 2006; Cools et al, 2006). Pramipexole has also beenassociated with impaired acquisition of reward-relatedbehavior in healthy participants, consistent with datasuggesting that phasic DA signaling is relevant to reinfor-cing actions leading to reward (Pizzagalli et al, 2008).However, other data indicate that pramipexole, whenadministered to healthy adults, does not alter behavioralimpulsivity, compulsivity, or related constructs includingdelay-discounting, risk-taking, response inhibition, orperserveration (Hamidovic et al, 2008). Furthermore,olanzapine, a drug with antagonist properties at theD2-like receptor family of DA receptors, has not showedsuperiority to placebo in two controlled trials involvingsubjects with pathological gambling (Fong et al, 2008;McElroy et al, 2008), and another D2-like antagonist,haloperidol, has been found to increase gambling-relatedmotivations and behaviors in individuals with pathologicalgambling (Zack and Poulos, 2007). Radioligand studies areimportant to clarify potential functions for D3 and D2receptors in the pathophysiology of pathological gambling,and such studies are complicated by these receptors sharingaffinities for existing radioligands.

Taking these findings into account, more research isneeded to better understand the relationship betweenimpulsivity, compulsivity, and DA function as they relateto specific psychiatric disorders such as pathologicalgambling. Impulsive or compulsive disorders may poten-tially derive from a mesolimbic DA deficiency. However,D2-like antagonists have shown a therapeutic benefit insome (OCD), but not other (pathological gambling)disorders characterized by impulsive and/or compulsivefeatures. Probing both the ventral and dorsal striatalcircuitry in human subjects with specific impulsive andcompulsive disorders using receptor-specific serotonergicand dopaminergic ligands would be an important next step

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

8

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 9: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

in understanding these conditions. It may be of particularinterest to explore the effects of 5-HT2A and 2C antagonistson DA transmission in this circuitry. These investigationscould provide additional insight into aspects such asdiminished ventral striatal and VMPFC activation seenacross studies involving disorders sharing impulsive andcompulsive features, such as pathological gambling and SAs(Reuter et al, 2005; Potenza, 2007a).

Our earlier definition of compulsivity (a tendency toperform repetitive acts in a habitual/stereotyped manner toattempt to prevent adverse consequences) and the currentdefinition (the alleviation of an aversive contingency suchas withdrawal) are conceptually related. For example,responding habitually to drug cues may be construed as amechanism to automatically anticipate a potentially aver-sive withdrawal syndrome and avert it before it actuallyhappens. Data link these habitual learning mechanisms (orcompulsivity) to parts of the dorsal striatum (the caudatefor instance), as reviewed earlier. More recent evidence nowlinks the dorsal striatum (its posterior part) to aversivemotivational learning (Seymour et al, 2007). Hence, from aneural perspective, evidence supports an overlap betweenthese two concepts of compulsivity.

IMPULSIVITY AND ‘BEHAVIORAL’ ADDICTIONS

Pathological gambling and SAs share many features. Thedisorders frequently co-occur and show similarities withrespect to symptom profiles, gender differences, naturalhistories, and familial propensities (Grant and Potenza,2006). Pathological gambling and SA show high levels ofimpulsivity on reward-discounting tasks, which correlatewith poor measures of functioning (Bechara, 2003) andpoor treatment outcome (Krishnan-Sarin et al, 2007) forindividuals with SAs and thus may have prognostic valuefor pathological gambling and other ICDs. Neurocognitiveand fMRI data suggest pathological gambling and SAs sharesimilar mediating neurocircuitry, in which, as comparedwith control subjects, relatively diminished activation of theventral striatum and VMPFC has been observed in rewardprocessing and other paradigms (Potenza et al, 2003a, b).Abnormal fMRI activation of the ventral striatum duringreward processing has been identified in the families ofindividuals with SA and may represent a candidatefunctional endophenotype for addictive disorders, althoughthis hypothesis requires direct examination in unaffectedrelatives of pathological gambling probands.

Over time, impulsive habitual responding in pathologicalgambling and SA may shift toward a more compulsivepattern of behavior, and it has been hypothesized thatprogressive recruitment of neighboring parallel and in-creasingly dorsal, cortico-striatal loops occurs in a spiralingmanner (Brewer and Potenza, 2008) reminiscent ofelaborately spiraling striato-nigrostriatal circuitry identifiedin primate (Lynd-Balta and Haber, 1994) and rodent (Belinet al, 2008) models of motivated behaviors mappingtransitional processes from ventral to dorsal striatum.Prospective, longitudinal studies after these changes withinindividuals over time will be informative and clinicallyrelevant. Promising research from treating individuals withpathological gambling with opioid antagonists (Brewer et al,

2008) not only discriminate pathological gambling fromOCD, in which opioid antagonists such as naloxone havebeen shown to make OCD worse (Insel and Pickar, 1983),but also suggest a therapeutic function for opioid antago-nists in other related ICDs (Grant et al, 2007).

NEW NEURAL TARGETS

To fully understand the neurobiology of impulsivity andcompulsivity and the potential for developing new treat-ments, we may need to explore beyond the neural circuitriesdiscussed in this article to include other neural structures,such as the insula. Data suggest that the insula is importantin coordinating ‘conscious’ urges. Lesions of the insula, forinstance after stroke, have been associated with rapidsmoking cessation (Naqvi et al, 2007). Exposure to cues inthe environment, or homeostatic states such as withdrawal,stress, or anxiety, may evoke ‘interoceptive’ representationsin the insula that translate into consciously perceived‘urges’. The insula is anatomically and functionally con-nected to the aforementioned neural systems implicated inimpulsivity, compulsivity, and inhibitory control. Concei-vably, the insula interacts with mechanisms of impulsivityand compulsivity by relaying signals (from the environmentor the viscera) to 5-HT 2C vs 5-HT 2A receptors in theprefrontal cortex. Thus, interoceptive signals mediatedthrough the insula may, on the one hand, sensitize theneural circuits driving impulsivity or compulsivity. On theother hand, insula activity may ‘hijack’ the inhibitory-control mechanisms of the prefrontal cortex and sub-vert attention, reasoning, planning, and decision-makingprocesses away from foreseeing the negative consequencesof a given action, and toward formulating plans to seekand procure rewarding stimuli such as drugs (Naqvi et al,2007).

CONCLUSION

Returning, then, to our motivating questions: (i) how muchdo compulsivity and impulsivity contribute to thesedisorders, (ii) to what extent do they depend on shared orseparate neural circuitry, (iii) what are the mediatingmonoaminergic mechanisms, (iv) do impulsive or compul-sive behavioral components have any prognostic valuerelated to treatment, and (v) is there a unifying-dimensionalmodel that fits the data? According to the availableevidence, impulsivity, and compulsivity, each seem to bemultidimensional and underpin at least some of theimpulsive and compulsive disorders, although the disordersshow overlapping, but also distinct profiles. Thus, over-arching failures within cortico-striatal neurocircuitry reg-ulating aspects of inhibitory control have been observed incognitive and imaging studies of all the disorders underreview, though for some disorders the data remainstantalizingly incomplete. Trichotillomania may stand apartas a disorder of motor-impulse control and dysfunctionwithin the RIF cortex and its cortico-subcortical connec-tions, whereas pathological gambling has been associatedwith impulsivity linked to poor decision making andabnormal ventral cortico-striatal circuitry, particularlyinvolving the VMPFC and ventral striatum, that identifies

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

9

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 10: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

it more closely with SAs. High levels of reward-relatedimpulsivity correlate with poor treatment outcome for SAsand may have prognostic significance for pathologicalgambling and other ICDs. Compulsive behaviors occurringwith autism are associated with similar abnormalities inventral reward circuitry. OCD, on the other hand, showsmotor impulsivity and compulsivity, presumably mediatedthrough disruption of OFC-caudate circuitry, as well asVLPFC, RIF cortex, cingulate, and parietal connections. Forthese disorders, inter-relating serotonin, DA, and noradre-naline projections are likely to have important modulatingfunctions, as well as other systems as yet incompletelycharacterized. Over time, impulsivity may evolve intocompulsivity and vice versa.

Thus, the picture seems far from a simple linear diathesiswith impulsivity and compulsivity occupying oppositepoles, and the ‘model’ probably involves a complicatedinteraction of multiple, orthogonally related diatheses,variably expressed across these circuits and disorders.Impulsive and compulsive disorders are conspicuouslyheterogeneous, sharing aspects of impulsivity and compul-sivity, and become even more complex and thus moredifficult to disentangle over time. For example, forimpulsive and addictive disorders, tolerance to rewardmay develop and the behaviors may persist as a method ofreducing discomfort (ie they become more compulsive). Forcompulsive disorders, it is possible that the performance ofthe repetitive behaviors themselves becomes reinforcingover time, despite their adverse long-term consequences (iethey become more impulsively driven). Mapping thesedisorders using an agreed-on battery of candidate endo-phenotypic markers may further clarify their relationshipwith each other, and future collaborative research enter-prises across centers with complementary expertise shouldbe encouraged. Novel approaches may be needed toinvestigate adequately through ‘triangulating’ approachessuch as complex interactions. In this respect, techniques foridentifying brain functional systems in neuroimaging data,such as the method of partial least squares (which alsoallows exploration of multiple behavioral and imagingvariables), may have significant potential as procedures forthe future in this field. We may also make further progressin dissecting the receptor mechanisms implicated incontrolling compulsive and impulsive behavior by use oftransgenic mouse preparations in the same tasks devised asfor rats (eg 5-CSRTT and reversal learning) and explorationof the full range of 5-HT receptors using new pharmaco-logical ligands.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Dr Fineberg has consulted for Lundbeck, Glaxo-SmithKline, Servier, and Bristol Myers Squibb; has receivedresearch support from Lundbeck, Glaxo-SmithKline, AstraZeneca, Wellcome; has received honoraria and support tolecture at scientific meetings from Janssen, Jazz, Lundbeck,Servier, Astra Zeneca, Wyeth. Dr Potenza consults for andhas advised to Boehringer Ingelheim; has consulted for andhas financial interests in Somaxon; has received researchsupport from the National Institutes of Health, Veteran’sAdministration, Mohegan Sun Casino, the National Center

for Responsible Gambling and the Institute for Research onGambling Disorders, and Glaxo-SmithKline, Forest Labora-tories, Ortho-McNeil and Oy-Control/Biotie pharmaceuti-cals; has participated in surveys, mailings, or telephoneconsultations related to drug addiction, ICDs or otherhealth topics; has consulted for law offices and the federalpublic defender’s office in issues related to ICDs and drugaddiction; has performed grant reviews for the NationalInstitutes of Health and other agencies; has given academiclectures in grand rounds, CME events, and other clinical orscientific venues; has guest-edited sections of journals; hasgenerated books or book chapters for publishers of mentalhealth texts; and provides clinical care in the ConnecticutDepartment of Mental Health and Addiction ServicesProblem Gambling Services Program. Dr Chamberlainconsults for Cambridge Cognition, Shire, and P1Vital. DrMenzies has received financial compensation resulting fromthe transfer of a technology not relating to the subjectmatter of this article between Cambridge EnterpriseLimited, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK, andCypress Bioscience, Inc, San Diego. Dr Bechara receivesroyalties from PAR, Inc. Dr Sahakian holds shares in CeNeS;has consulted for Cambridge Cognition, Novartis, Shire,GlaxoSmithKline, and Lilly; and has received honoraria forgrand rounds in psychiatry at Massachusetts GeneralHospital (CME credits) and for speaking at the Interna-tional Conference on Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophre-nia and Mood Disorders (2007). Dr Robbins consults forCambridge Cognition, E. Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, and AllonTherapeutics. Dr Bullmore is an employee of GlaxoSmithK-line (50%) and the University of Cambridge (50%) and ashareholder in GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Bullmore has receivedfinancial compensation resulting from the transfer of atechnology not relating to the subject matter of this articlebetween Cambridge Enterprise Limited, University of Cam-bridge, Cambridge, UK, and Cypress Bioscience, Inc, SanDiego. Dr Hollander has consulted to Somaxon, Neuropharm,Transcept, and Nastech. Dr Hollander has consulted to lawoffices and testified in the Mirapex Product Liabilitycase. He has received research support fromthe National Institutes of Health, Orphan Products Divisionof the Food and Drug Administration, National Alliancefor Research in Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders,Autism Speaks, the Seaver Foundation, and Solvay, OyContral, and Somaxon. This work was supported in part bya Wellcome Trust Programme Grant (076274/Z/04/Z) toDr Robbins, Dr Sahakian, BJ Everitt, and AC Roberts. TheBehavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute is sup-ported by a joint award from the Medical Research Council(MRC) and Wellcome Trust (G001354). Supported by theNational Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia andDepression (RG37920 Distinguished Investigator Award toDr Bullmore), the Harnett Fund and James Baird Fund(University of Cambridge) and the University of CambridgeSchool of Clinical Medicine, (MB/PhD studentship toDr Menzies), and the Medical Research Council (MB/PhDstudentship to Dr Chamberlain). Dr Bechara receivesgrant support from the National Institutes on Health (NIDAR01 DA023051, DA11779, DA12487, and DA1670), (NINDSP01 NS019632), and the National Science Foundation (NSFIIS 04-42586). Dr Potenza receives grant support fromthe National Institutes of Health (R01 s DA019039,

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

10

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 11: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

DA020908, DA015757, DA020709; R37 DA15969; RL1AA017539; P50 s DA09241, AA12870, AA015632), the VA(VISN1 MIRECC and REAP), and Women’s Health Re-search at Yale. Dr Robbins consults for pfizer, Dr Menzieshas received honoraria for presenting at the 8th Annualconference on Research of psychopathology and for workon the UK Government Foresight Project on mental capitaland wellbeing.

DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

REFERENCES

Adinoff B (2004). Neurobiologic processes in drug reward andaddiction. Harv Rev Psychiatry 12: 305–320.

Aron AR, Dowson JH, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2003).Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults withattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 54:1465–1468.

Aron AR, Poldrack RA (2005). The cognitive neuroscience ofresponse inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57: 1285–1292.

Baxter Jr LR, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Schwartz JM,Selin CE (1987). Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates inobsessive-compulsive disorder. A comparison with rates inunipolar depression and in normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry44: 211–218.

Bechara A (2003). Risky business: emotion, decision-making, andaddiction. J Gambl Stud 19: 23–51.

Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994).Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to humanprefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15.

Belin D, Mar AC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2008). Highimpulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking.Science 320: 1352–1355.

Berlin HA, Hamilton H, Hollander E (2008). Neurocognition andTemperament in Pathological Gambling. American PsychiatricAssociation, conference poster: Washington DC.

Berlin HA, Rolls ET, Kischka U (2004). Impulsivity, timeperception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patientswith orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain 127(Pt 5): 1108–1126.

Blanco C, Potenza MN, Kim SW, Ibanez A, Zaninelli R, Saiz-Ruiz Jet al (2009). A pilot study of impulsivity and compulsivity inpathological gambling. Psychiatry Res 167: 161–168.

Bloch MH, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Kelmendi B, Coric V,Bracken MB, Leckman JF (2006). A systematic review: anti-psychotic augmentation with treatment refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 11: 622–632.

Bohlhalter S, Goldfine A, Matteson S, Garraux G, Hanakawa T,Kansaku K et al (2006). Neural correlates of tic generation inTourette syndrome: an event-related functional MRI study.Brain 129(Pt 8): 2029–2037.

Boulougouris V, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2007). Effects oforbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serialspatial reversal learning in the rat. Behav Brain Res 179: 219–228.

Boulougouris V, Glennon JC, Robbins TW (2008). Dissociableeffects of selective 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists onserial spatial reversal learning in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology33: 2007–2019.

Brewer JA, Grant JE, Potenza MN (2008). The treatment ofpathological gambling. Addict Disord Treat 7: 1–14.

Brewer JA, Potenza MN (2008). The neurobiology and genetics ofimpulse control disorders: relationships to drug addictions.Biochem Pharmacol 75: 63–75.

Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW,Sahakian BJ (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compul-sive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive andbehavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 399–419.

Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW,Sahakian BJ (2006a). Strategy implementation in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Psychol Med 36: 91–97.

Chamberlain SR, Del Campo N, Dowson J, Muller U, Clark L,Robbins TW et al (2007a). Atomoxetine improved responseinhibition in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Biol Psychiatry 62: 977–984.

Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Clark L, RobbinsTW, Sahakian BJ (2007b). A neuropsychological comparison ofobsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Neuropsy-chologia 45: 654–662.

Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW,Sahakian BJ (2006b). Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibilityin obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Am JPsychiatry 163: 1282–1284.

Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD,Bullmore ET, Robbins TW et al (2007c). Impaired cognitiveflexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degreerelatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am JPsychiatry 164: 335–338.

Chamberlain SR, Menzies L (2009). Endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder: rationale, evidence and future potential.Expert Rev Neurother 9: 1133–1146.

Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, FinebergNA, del Campo N et al (2008a). Orbitofrontal dysfunction inpatients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffectedrelatives. Science 321: 421–422.

Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Sahakian BJ, Fineberg NA (2007d).Lifting the veil on trichotillomania. Am J Psychiatry 164: 568–574.

Chamberlain SR, Menzies LA, Fineberg NA, Del Campo N,Suckling J, Craig K et al (2008b). Grey matter abnormalities intrichotillomania: morphometric magnetic resonance imagingstudy. Br J Psychiatry 193: 216–221.

Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW,Sahakian BJ (2006c). Neurochemical modulation of responseinhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science 311:861–863.

Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Deakin JB, Corlett PR, Dowson J,Cardinal R et al (2006d). Lack of deleterious effects of buspironeon cognition in healthy male volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 21:210–215.

Chamberlain SR, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2007e). Theneurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. BiolPsychiatry 61: 1317–1319.

Chamberlain SR, Sahakian BJ (2007). The neuropsychiatry ofimpulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry 20: 255–261.

Chambers MS, Atack JR, Carling RW, Collinson N, Cook SM,Dawson GR et al (2004). An orally bioavailable, functionallyselective inverse agonist at the benzodiazepine site of GABAAalpha5 receptors with cognition enhancing properties. J MedChem 47: 5829–5832.

Chou-Green JM, Holscher TD, Dallman MF, Akana SF (2003).Compulsive behavior in the 5-HT2C receptor knockout mouse.Physiol Behav 78: 641–649.

Clark L, Robbins TW, Ersche KD, Sahakian BJ (2006). Reflectionimpulsivity in current and former substance users. BiolPsychiatry 60: 515–522.

Clark L, Roiser JP, Cools R, Rubinsztein DC, Sahakian BJ,Robbins TW (2005). Stop signal response inhibition isnot modulated by tryptophan depletion or the serotonintransporter polymorphism in healthy volunteers: implicationsfor the 5-HT theory of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl)182: 570–578.

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

11

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 12: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2004).Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion.Science 304: 878–880.

Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins TW,Roberts AC (2005). Prefrontal serotonin depletion affectsreversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J Neurosci 25:532–538.

Clarke HF, Walker SC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2007).Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion isbehaviorally and neurochemically specific. Cereb Cortex 17: 18–27.

Cools R (2006). Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for L-DOPA treatment in Parkinson’s disease.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30: 1–23.

Cools R, Altamirano L, D’Esposito M (2006). Reversal learning inParkinson’s disease depends on medication status and outcomevalence. Neuropsychologia 44: 1663–1673.

Cottrell S, Tilden D, Robinson P, Bae J, Arellano J, Edgell E et al(2008). A modeled economic evaluation comparing atomoxetinewith stimulant therapy in the treatment of children withattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United Kingdom.Value Health 11: 376–388.

Crawford S, Channon S, Robertson MM (2005). Tourette’s syndrome:performance on tests of behavioural inhibition, working memoryand gambling. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46: 1327–1336.

Crosbie J, Schachar R (2001). Deficient inhibition as a marker forfamilial ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 158: 1884–1890.

Denys D, Zohar J, Westenberg HG (2004). The role of dopamine inobsessive-compulsive disorder: preclinical and clinical evidence.J Clin Psychiatry 65(Suppl 14): 11–17.

Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (1996). Dissociation in prefrontalcortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature 380: 69–72.

Evers EA, Cools R, Clark L, van der Veen FM, Jolles J, Sahakian BJet al (2005). Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal cortex duringnegative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning. Neuropsy-chopharmacology 30: 1138–1147.

Fineberg NA, Gale TM (2005). Evidence-based pharmacotherapyof obsessivecompulsive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8:107–129.

Fineberg NA, Saxena S, Zohar J, Craig KJ (2007a). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: boundary issues. CNS Spectr 12: 359–364,367–375.

Fineberg NA, Sharma P, Sivakumaran T, Sahakian B, ChamberlainSR (2007b). Does obsessive-compulsive personality disorderbelong within the obsessive-compulsive spectrum? CNS Spectr12: 467–482.

Fineberg NA, Sivakumaran T, Roberts A, Gale T (2005). Addingquetiapine to SRI in treatment-resistant obsessive-compulsivedisorder: a randomized controlled treatment study. Int ClinPsychopharmacol 20: 223–226.

Fong T, Kalechstein A, Bernhard B, Rosenthal R, Rugle L (2008). Adouble-blind, placebo-controlled trial of olanzapine for thetreatment of video poker pathological gamblers. PharmacolBiochem Behav 89: 298–303.

Frank MJ, Moustafa AA, Haughey HM, Curran T, Hutchison KE(2007). Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles fordopamine in reinforcement learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA104: 16311–16316.

Garraux G, Goldfine A, Bohlhalter S, Lerner A, Hanakawa T,Hallett M (2006). Increased midbrain gray matter in Tourette’ssyndrome. Ann Neurol 59: 381–385.

Gottesman II, Gould TD (2003). The endophenotype concept inpsychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry160: 636–645.

Grant JE, Odlaug BL, Potenza MN (2007). Addicted to hair pulling?How an alternate model of trichotillomania may improvetreatment outcome. Harv Rev Psychiatry 15: 80–85.

Grant JE, Potenza MN (2006). Compulsive aspects of impulse-control disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am 29: 539–551.

Hamidovic A, Kang UJ, de Wit H (2008). Effects of low to moderateacute doses of pramipexole on impulsivity and cognition inhealthy volunteers. J Clin Psychopharmacol 28: 45–51.

Hampshire A, Owen AM (2006). Fractionating attentional controlusing event-related fMRI. Cereb Cortex 16: 1679–1689.

Harrison AA, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (1997). Central 5-HTdepletion enhances impulsive responding without affecting theaccuracy of attentional performance: interactions with dopami-nergic mechanisms. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 133: 329–342.

Hatcher PD, Brown VJ, Tait DS, Bate S, Overend P, Hagan JJ et al(2005). 5-HT6 receptor antagonists improve performance in anattentional set shifting task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl)181: 253–259.

Hollander E, Berlin HA, Bartz J, Anagnostou E, Pallanti S, Simeon Det al (2007a). The impulsive-compulsive spectrum: neurocogni-tive, functional imaging and treatment findings inform thephenotype. ACNP presentation. Scientific Abstracts ACNP 2007Annual Meeting, p 50.

Hollander E, Cohen LJ (1996). Impulsivity and Compulsivity.American Psychiatric Press Inc, Washington DC.

Hollander E, DeCaria C, Gully R, Nitescu A, Suckow RF, Gorman JMet al (1991a). Effects of chronic fluoxetine treatment on behavioraland neuroendocrine responses to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine inobsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 36: 1–17.

Hollander E, DeCaria C, Nitescu A, Cooper T, Stover B, Gully Ret al (1991b). Noradrenergic function in obsessive-compulsivedisorder: behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to clonidineand comparison to healthy controls. Psychiatry Res 37: 161–177.

Hollander E, Kim S, Khanna S, Pallanti S (2007b). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders:diagnostic and dimensional issues. CNS Spectr 12(2 Suppl 3): 5–13.

Hollander E, Wong CM (1995). Obsessive-compulsive spectrumdisorders. J Clin Psychiatry 56(Suppl 4): 3–6; discussion 53–5.

Homberg JR, Pattij T, Janssen MC, Ronken E, De Boer SF,Schoffelmeer AN et al (2007). Serotonin transporter deficiencyin rats improves inhibitory control but not behaviouralflexibility. Eur J Neurosci 26: 2066–2073.

Hornak J, O’Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, BullockPR et al (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgicalexcisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex inhumans. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 463–478.

Insel TR, Pickar D (1983). Naloxone administration in obsessive-compulsive disorder: report of two cases. Am J Psychiatry 140:1219–1220.

Izquierdo A, Newman TK, Higley JD, Murray EA (2007). Geneticmodulation of cognitive flexibility and socioemotional behaviorin rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 14128–14133.

Joel D (2006). Current animal models of obsessive compulsivedisorder: a critical review. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol BiolPsychiatry 30: 374–388.

Kolevzon A, Mathewson KA, Hollander E (2006). Selectiveserotonin reuptake inhibitors in autism: a review of efficacyand tolerability. J Clin Psychiatry 67: 407–414.

Koob GF, Le Moal M (1997). Drug abuse: hedonic homeostaticdysregulation. Science 278: 52–58.

Krishnan-Sarin S, Reynolds B, Duhig AM, Smith A, Liss T,McFetridge A et al (2007). Behavioral impulsivity predictstreatment outcome in a smoking cessation program foradolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend 88: 79–82.

Lapiz-Bluhm MD, Soto-Pina AE, Hensler JG, Morilak DA (2009).Chronic intermittent cold stress and serotonin depletion inducedeficits of reversal learning in an attentional set-shifting test inrats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 202: 329–341.

Li CS, Chang HL, Hsu YP, Wang HS, Ko NC (2006). Motorresponse inhibition in children with Tourette’s disorder.J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 18: 417–419.

Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H (2005). Ameta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

12

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 13: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor con-trol? J Abnorm Psychol 114: 216–222.

Lynd-Balta E, Haber SN (1994). The organization of midbrainprojections to the ventral striatum in the primate. Neuroscience59: 609–623.

Masaki D, Yokoyama C, Kinoshita S, Tsuchida H, Nakatomi Y,Yoshimoto K et al (2006). Relationship between limbic andcortical 5-HT neurotransmission and acquisition and reversallearning in a go/no-go task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl)189: 249–258.

McElroy SL, Nelson EB, Welge JA, Kaehler L, Keck Jr PE (2008).Olanzapine in the treatment of pathological gambling: a negativerandomized placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry 69: 433–440.

McIntosh AR, Lobaugh NJ (2004). Partial least squares analysis ofneuroimaging data: applications and advances. Neuroimage23(Suppl 1): S250–S263.

Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH,del Campo N et al (2007a). Neurocognitive endophenotypesof obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 130(Pt 12): 3223–3236.

Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ,Bullmore ET (2008a). Integrating evidence from neuroimagingand neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive dis-order: The orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. NeurosciBiobehav Rev 32: 525–549.

Menzies L, Williams GB, Chamberlain SR, Ooi C, Fineberg N,Suckling J et al (2008b). White matter abnormalities in patientswith obsessive-compulsive disorder and their first-degreerelatives. Am J Psychiatry 165: 1308–1315.

Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A (2007). Damage tothe insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science 315:531–534.

Park SB, Coull JT, McShane RH, Young AH, Sahakian BJ, RobbinsTW et al (1994). Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteersproduces selective impairments in learning and memory.Neuropharmacology 33: 575–588.

Passetti F, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2003). Double dissociation ofserotonergic and dopaminergic mechanisms on attentionalperformance using a rodent five-choice reaction time task.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 165: 136–145.

Pizzagalli DA, Evins AE, Schetter EC, Frank MJ, Pajtas PE, SantessoDL et al (2008). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairsreinforcement learning in humans: behavioral evidence from alaboratory-based measure of reward responsiveness. Psycho-pharmacology (Berl) 196: 221–232.

Potenza MN (2007a). Impulsivity and compulsivity in pathologicalgambling and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Rev Bras Psiquiatr29: 105–106.

Potenza MN (2007b). To do or not to do? The complexities ofaddiction, motivation, self-control, and impulsivity. AmJ Psychiatry 164: 4–6.

Potenza MN, Leung HC, Blumberg HP, Peterson BS, Fulbright RK,Lacadie CM et al (2003a). An FMRI Stroop task study ofventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gam-blers. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1990–1994.

Potenza MN, Steinberg MA, Skudlarski P, Fulbright RK, LacadieCM, Wilber MK et al (2003b). Gambling urges in pathologicalgambling: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. ArchGen Psychiatry 60: 828–836.

Poyurovsky M, Faragian S, Shabeta A, Kosov A (2008).Comparison of clinical characteristics, co-morbidity and pharma-cotherapy in adolescent schizophrenia patients with and withoutobsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 159: 133–139.

Reuter J, Raedler T, Rose M, Hand I, Glascher J, Buchel C (2005).Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of themesolimbic reward system. Nat Neurosci 8: 147–148.

Robbins TW (2002). The 5-choice serial reaction time task:behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163: 362–380.

Robbins TW (2007). Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatalsubstrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362: 917–932.

Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993). The neural basis of drugcraving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain ResBrain Res Rev 18: 247–291.

Rogers RD, Blackshaw AJ, Middleton HC, Matthews K, Hawtin K,Crowley C et al (1999a). Tryptophan depletion impairs stimulus-reward learning while methylphenidate disrupts attentionalcontrol in healthy young adults: implications for the mono-aminergic basis of impulsive behaviour. Psychopharmacology(Berl) 146: 482–491.

Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R,Wynne K et al (1999b). Dissociable deficits in thedecision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers,opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontalcortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidencefor monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20:322–339.

Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Taylor E (2003). Right inferiorprefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesialprefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage20: 351–358.

Schilman EA, Uylings HB, Galis-de Graaf Y, Joel D, GroenewegenHJ (2008). The orbital cortex in rats topographically projects tocentral parts of the caudate-putamen complex. Neurosci Lett 432:40–45.

Schultz W (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward.Neuron 36: 241–263.

Seymour B, Daw N, Dayan P, Singer T, Dolan R (2007). Differentialencoding of losses and gains in the human striatum. J Neurosci27: 4826–4831.

Stein DJ, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N (2006). An A-B-C model ofhabit disorders: hair-pulling, skin-picking, and other stereotypicconditions. CNS Spectr 11: 824–827.

Stein DJ, Hollander E (1995). Obsessive-compulsive spectrumdisorders. J Clin Psychiatry 56: 265–266.

Szeszko PR, Ardekani BA, Ashtari M, Malhotra AK, Robinson DG,Bilder RM et al (2005). White matter abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Arch GenPsychiatry 62: 782–790.

Talbot PS, Watson DR, Barrett SL, Cooper SJ (2006). Rapidtryptophan depletion improves decision-making cognition inhealthy humans without affecting reversal learning or setshifting. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 1519–1525.

Tsaltas E, Kontis D, Chrysikakou S, Giannou H, Biba A, Pallidi Set al (2005). Reinforced spatial alternation as an animal model ofobsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): investigation of 5-HT2Cand 5-HT1D receptor involvement in OCD pathophysiology. BiolPsychiatry 57: 1176–1185.

van der Plasse G, Feenstra MG (2008). Serial reversallearning and acute tryptophan depletion. Behav Brain Res 186:23–31.

Vertes RP (2004). Differential projections of the infralimbic andprelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse 51: 32–58.

Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ (1999). Imaging studies on therole of dopamine in cocaine reinforcement and addiction inhumans. J Psychopharmacol 13: 337–345.

Walker SC, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2009). Differentialcontributions of dopamine and serotonin to orbitofrontal cortexfunction in the marmoset. Cereb Cortex 19: 889–898.

Watkins LH, Sahakian BJ, Robertson MM, Veale DM, Rogers RD,Pickard KM et al (2005). Executive function in Tourette’ssyndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med 35:571–582.

Westenberg HG, Fineberg NA, Denys D (2007). Neurobiology ofobsessive-compulsive disorder: serotonin and beyond. CNSSpectr 12(2 Suppl 3): 14–27.

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

13

Neuropsychopharmacology

Page 14: Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal ... · Review Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review Naomi A

Winstanley CA, Chudasama Y, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, GlennonJC, Robbins TW (2003). Intra-prefrontal 8-OH-DPAT andM100907 improve visuospatial attention and decrease impulsiv-ity on the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 167: 304–314.

Winstanley CA, Eagle DM, Robbins TW (2006). Behavioral modelsof impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinicaland preclinical studies. Clin Psychol Rev 26: 379–395.

Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Glennon JC, Robbins TW(2004). 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists have opposingeffects on a measure of impulsivity: interactions with global5-HT depletion. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 176: 376–385.

Wise RA (2002). Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensedincentives. Neuron 36: 229–240.

World Health Organisation (1992). International Classi-fication of Diseases, 10th edition (ICD-10). World HealthOrganisation, Geneva.

Yucel M, Harrison BJ, Wood SJ, Fornito A, Wellard RM, Pujol Jet al (2007). Functional and biochemical alterations of themedial frontal cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. ArchGen Psychiatry 64: 946–955.

Zack M, Poulos CX (2007). A D2 antagonist enhances the rewardingand priming effects of a gambling episode in pathologicalgamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology 32: 1678–1686.

Zohar J, Insel TR, Zohar-Kadouch RC, Hill JL, Murphy DL (1988).Serotonergic responsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder.Effects of chronic clomipramine treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry45: 167–172.

Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviorsNA Fineberg et al

14

Neuropsychopharmacology