Top Banner
A PROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF DEPERSONALIZATION TYPES John R.Jacobs, Ph.D. Gregory Bovasso, Ph.D. John R. Jacobs, Ph.D., is Associate Professor in the Department of Psycholob'Y at Southern Connecticut Stale University, in New I-I;wen, Connecticul. Gregory Bavassa, Ph.D. is a Post-cloCloral Fellow in the Department afMental Hygiene atJohns Hopkins University ill Baltimore, Mal)·land. For reprints write Gregory Bavassa, Ph.D., P05t-doctoral Fellow, Department of Mental Hygiene, School of Hygiene and Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, 624 N. Broadway, Baltimore, MD 2J205-1999. ABSTRACf FiVt! types of deperJOtwliullion experiences baset! Ort swks developed b)'Jacobs and Bovasso (/992) were used 10 cluster subjects into six groIlJ)s. Four relatively small groups which had regular depersoT/al- iUllion experiences were ideNtified: the Derealiud, the Self-negaling, tIll! B{J(ly-detor.hed, and the Profoundly Depersonalized. The fifth grou!), the Fleetingly Depersonaliud, and the sixth group, the Non- dejJmOtwliuxl, constituled 25 % and 50% ofthe !JOpuwtioll, respec- tivPly. A !,rofile rmal)'sis indicated qua!ilatilie differences between the six groups in their pathological traits, which fell along a con- ti/UIlIIIl of pallwlof:,rical sroerity. nit: results support the validity of (I mullitlimensionol de!IerSOIwliullion constmct which may clarify some of lhe contr(/(/ictions and inconsistencies in the literature on depersonalization. Further, the results tn<l)'facilitate clinicians' dif feren liation oftheir !mtients olonga conti nuulll ofl)(jlhological sever- ity based on lhe lype and frequenC)' of depersonaliwlio1l experiences which thl:)' rejNrl. The concept of depersonalization has been widely spec- ulated upon by clinicians, but has remained LHlder- researched despite the surge of attention to various forms of dissocialion, such as multiple personality disorder (Spiegel, 1993; Singer &SincolT, 1990). The definition ofthe conSU'uCl of depersonaliztltion and the con'eel idemification of the of depersonalization have been a source of con- trovcrs}' in psychiatry (Le\)' & Wachtel, 1978; Mellor, 1988). In the last decade a number of measures (Bernstein & PUlnam, 1986; Frischholz, el al. 1990; Frischholz, Braun, & Sahes, 1991; Kirby, 1990; Sanders, 1986; Steinberg, 1991) studying diverse forms of dissociation such as psychogenic fugue, amnesia, auditory hallucinalions, and multiple per- sonality disorder have been established. The absence of cmpiricallysound instruments that mcasure different forms of depersonalization may account for inconsistent findings regarding its symptoms, incidence and prevalence, and its association with other forms of psychopathology. Jacobs and Bovasso (1992) found empirical evidence to support a multidimensional construct of depersonalization dilTerenlialcd by mild self-ob5Clyd\ion on one end ofthc con- tinuum and psychotic states on the other. Their findings sug- gest that an array of symptoms has been attributed to deper- sonalization because the constrUCt is lTlultidimensional (Mellor, 1988). These depersonalization symptoms have been attributed to disorders such as depression (Tucker, Harrow, & Quinlan, 1973) and anxiety (Oberdorf, 1950), as well as to non-pathological phenomena, such as therapeulic change (Kelly, 1955), and adjuslment to new social roles (Levy & Wachtel, 1976). The empirical dnelopmctll of a multidi- mensional construct may resolve the ambiguity surrounding the construcl of depersonalization and ilS confusion with otller constructs. Tllis rnultidimensiOilal construct involves an expansion of the slandard ps}'chiatric concept of deper- sonalization. In the lttultidilllcnsior131 model, thc principal form of depersonalization, II/oulhenticity, involves a loss of a sense of genuineness abom one's behavior reflected in the need to cOTltinuously remind oneself of onc's actions. A second SCt of s}'mptoms which had been long regarded as a form of depersonalization, DerealiUltiol/, involves a loss offamiliariry with friends or surroundings. A third typc of dcpersonal- izatioll, Bod)' detachment, involves pcrceptions of lhe body as distorted or delached, and is commonly rcported in psy- chiatric populations. A fourth t}l)C, Self-negation involvcs thc reluclance 10 acknowledge that oneselfis imulved in orcxpe- riencing a particular situation, emOlion or cognition. The fifth, Self-objectification, involves a gross disorientation in thc external world and the expericnce of the selfas numb, dead or inanimate. Thc lncasuremcnt of thesc fivc depcrsonalization dimen- sions facilitates the developme11l of a typology of deperson- alization experiences which may be used to classify ual cascs. Certain individuals may experience one or more forms of depersonalization while they do nOI, or less fre- quently experience other forms. Further, individuals c1assi- 169 D1SS00ATlO;.<, VoL IX. :-;0. 3, Septemberl996
7

APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

Apr 30, 2022

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

APROFILEANALYSIS OF

PSYCHOPATHOLOGYIN CLUSTERS OF

DEPERSONALIZATIONTYPES

John R.Jacobs, Ph.D.Gregory Bovasso, Ph.D.

John R. Jacobs, Ph.D., is Associate Professor in theDepartment of Psycholob'Y at Southern Connecticut StaleUniversity, in New I-I;wen, Connecticul. Gregory Bavassa,Ph.D. is a Post-cloCloral Fellow in the Department afMentalHygiene atJohns Hopkins University ill Baltimore, Mal)·land.

For reprints write Gregory Bavassa, Ph.D., P05t-doctoralFellow, Department of Mental Hygiene, School of Hygieneand Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, 624 N.Broadway, Baltimore, MD 2J205-1999.

ABSTRACf

FiVt! types ofdeperJOtwliullion experiences baset! Ort swks developedb)'Jacobs and Bovasso (/992) were used 10 cluster subjects into sixgroIlJ)s. Four relatively small groups which had regular depersoT/al­iUllion experiences were ideNtified: the Derealiud, the Self-negaling,tIll! B{J(ly-detor.hed, and the Profoundly Depersonalized. The fifthgrou!), the Fleetingly Depersonaliud, and the sixth group, the Non­dejJmOtwliuxl, constituled 25 %and 50% ofthe!JOpuwtioll, respec­tivPly. A !,rofile rmal)'sis indicated qua!ilatilie differences betweenthe six groups in their pathological traits, which fell along a con­ti/UIlIIIl ofpallwlof:,rical sroerity. nit: results support the validity of(I mullitlimensionol de!IerSOIwliullion constmct which may clarifysome of lhe contr(/(/ictions and inconsistencies in the literature ondepersonalization. Further, the results tn<l)'facilitate clinicians' differen liation oftheir!mtients olonga continuulll ofl)(jlhological sever­ity based on lhe lype and frequenC)' ofdepersonaliwlio1l experienceswhich thl:)' rejNrl.

The concept ofdepersonalization has been widely spec­ulated upon by clinicians, but has remained LHlder­researched despite the surge of attention to various formsof dissocialion, such as multiple personality disorder (Spiegel,1993; Singer &SincolT, 1990). The definition ofthe conSU'uClof depersonaliztltion and the con'eel idemification of the~ymptomsof depersonalization have been a source of con­trovcrs}' in psychiatry (Le\)' & Wachtel, 1978; Mellor, 1988).In the last decade a number of measures (Bernstein &PUlnam, 1986; Frischholz, el al. 1990; Frischholz, Braun, &Sahes, 1991; Kirby, 1990; Sanders, 1986; Steinberg, 1991)studying diverse forms of dissociation such as psychogenicfugue, amnesia, auditory hallucinalions, and multiple per-

sonality disorder have been established. The absence ofcmpiricallysound instruments that mcasure different formsof depersonalization may account for inconsistent findingsregarding its symptoms, incidence and prevalence, and itsassociation with other forms of psychopathology.

Jacobs and Bovasso (1992) found empirical evidence tosupport a multidimensional construct of depersonalizationdilTerenlialcd by mild self-ob5Clyd\ion on one end ofthc con­tinuum and psychotic states on the other. Their findings sug­gest that an array ofsymptoms has been attributed to deper­sonalization because the constrUCt is lTlultidimensional(Mellor, 1988). These depersonalization symptoms have beenattributed to disorders such as depression (Tucker, Harrow,& Quinlan, 1973) and anxiety (Oberdorf, 1950), as well asto non-pathological phenomena, such as therapeulic change(Kelly, 1955), and adjuslment to new social roles (Levy &Wachtel, 1976). The empirical dnelopmctll of a multidi­mensional construct may resolve the ambiguity surroundingthe construcl of depersonalization and ilS confusion withotller constructs. Tllis rnultidimensiOilal construct involvesan expansion of the slandard ps}'chiatric concept of deper­sonalization.

In the lttultidilllcnsior131 model, thc principal form ofdepersonalization, II/oulhenticity, involves a loss of a sense ofgenuineness abom one's behavior reflected in the need tocOTltinuously remind oneself of onc's actions. A second SCtof s}'mptoms which had been long regarded as a form ofdepersonalization, DerealiUltiol/, involves a loss offamiliarirywith friends or surroundings. A third typc of dcpersonal­izatioll, Bod)' detachment, involves pcrceptions of lhe body asdistorted or delached, and is commonly rcported in psy­chiatric populations. A fourth t}l)C, Self-negation involvcs thcreluclance 10 acknowledge that oneselfis imulved in orcxpe­riencing a particular situation, emOlion or cognition. Thefifth, Self-objectification, involves a gross disorientation in thcexternal world and the expericnce of the selfas numb, deador inanimate.

Thc lncasuremcntof thesc fivc depcrsonalization dimen­sions facilitates the developme11l of a typology of deperson­alization experiences which may be used to classify individ~

ual cascs. Certain individuals may experience one or moreforms of depersonalization while they do nOI, or less fre­quently experience other forms. Further, individuals c1assi-

169D1SS00ATlO;.<, VoL IX. :-;0. 3, Septemberl996

Page 2: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

DEPERSONALIZATION TYPES

Mean Depersonalization Scale Score*

MeasuresThe five depersonalization scales (Jacobs & Bovasso,

1992) each consisted of five items. Subjects rated the fre­quency of the occurrence of the expelience expressed in eachitem, as follows: 0) never, I) yearly, at least once a year, 2)monthly, at least once a montll, 3) weekly, at least once aweek, or 4) daily, at least once a day. Data from 11 of the 232subjects who responded to a Depersonalization item that mea­sured careless or random responses were not used in tlle anal­ysis. The Depersonalization scale was group-administered;the researcher read instructions to the subjects and remainedin the room to answer any questions about the form.

Ten scales from the Differential Personality Inventory,or DPI (Jackson & Messick, 1973) were used to assess patho­logical traits associated with depersonalization. The DPl hasinternal consistency and convergent and discriminant valid­ity (Jackson & Carlson, 1973), and has also been validatedagainst the BriefPsychiatric Rating Scale (Auld & Noel, 1984).

.67 100 **

%

50

25

9

9

5

4

5

.27

.90

.69

1.29

1.30

2.30

METHOD

SubjectsThe subjects were 232 students from a large nortlleast­

ern university. They were approximately 75% women, witha median age of lwen ty-two.

the literature as combining a loss ofauthenticity and self-negation (Myers& Grant, 1972; Torch, 1978). Theseindividuals have difficulties inacknowledging and experiencingemotions and cognition which violatetheir self-expectations. A fourth typeofdepersonalized individual is evere­ly dissociated, and therefore reportshigh levels of several dimensions ofdepersonalization, particularly Self­objectification, which is the mostpathological depersonalization expe­rience. Self-objectification is experi­enced in only a small proportion of thepopulation and is associated wi thsevere personality disorders (Munich,1978). The authors also expect twoadditional types ofdepersonalization:the Fleetingly Depersonalized and theNon-depersonalized. Individuals whoonly fleetingly experience deperson­alization have been frequently notedin the research literature (Eliot,Rosenberg, & Wagner, 1984), and asubstantial body of the general popu­lation reports no experiences of

depersonalization (Nemiah, 1976).

TABLE 1Depersonaliztion Groups Resulting from Cluster Analysis

Cluster 1 2 3 4

on-depersonalized .19 .59 .15 .29

Fleetingly Depersonalization .88 1.08 .54 .88

Derealized .47 2.16 .42 .87

Self-negating 1.56 2.04 .59 2.04

Body Detachment 2.50 2.04 1.22 .98

Profoundly Depersonalized 2.47 2.50 2.20 2.70

All Subjects .70 1.10 .44 .75

* 1) Body Detachment; 2) Derealization; 3) Self-objectification;4) Self-negation; 5) Inauthenticity.

** Percentages do not sum to 100 due to munding.

fied Witll different types of depersonalization may differ inthe severity of more general patllOlogical traits. The authorshypothesize that six depersonalization groups will bestdescribe the population sampled. These groups will expeli­ence qualitatively different depersonalization experienceswhich will be also differentiated by levels of general psy­chopathology.

Derealization is commonly found in mildly and severe­ly dissociated individuals in both clinical and non·dinical pop­ulations (Eliot, Rosenberg, & Wagner 1984; Ross, Joshi, &Currie, 1990; Sanders, McRoberts, & Tollefson, 1989;Trueman, 1984). Although derealization and depersonal­ization are currently regarded as independent, derealizationexperience are frequently presented by individuals suffer­ing depersonalization disorder. 1 onetheless, tlle indepen­del1Ce of the depersonalization and derealization constructshas been supported in previous research (Fleiss, Gurland,& Goldberg, 1975). Thus, the autllOrs expect to find a clus­ter of individuals who expelience derealization exclusively,as well as clusters tllat experience bOtll derealization anddepersonalization. The literature (Jacobson, 1971; Nueller,1982; Tucker, Harrow, & Quinlan, 1973) also suggests anotll­er distinct type of depersonalization experience involvingBody-<letachment. This type of individual frequently expe­riences estrangement from the body, as well as general dere­alization.

A third type of depersonalized individual is depicted in

170DlSSOCIATIOK, Vol. IX, :0;0. 3, Seplember1996

Page 3: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

JACOBS/BOVASSO

o O••oellll:r.IUon

Fleeting Detached Negating Derealized ProfoundTYPE OF DEPERSONALIZATION CLUSTER

.L

Fleeting Detached Negating Derealized ProfoundTYPE OF DEPERSONALIZATION CLUSTER

FiGURE 2

None

OPI SCALES:

_ Feeling, of Unrealll _ Mood Fluetullion r Neurotic Oilorglni:r.

_ Perceplull Oillorl _ Self Depree,tlon

OPt SCALES:

_ Broodlne.. .. O.orl..lon

_ Thoughl Oilorglnl:r. _ Flmlllil Dileord

FIGURE I

STANDARD SCORES (Z)

2

- 1

1.5

_S~TA~N~D:::A:::R:::D:...::S-=C-=O:::R-=E-=S-,(::Z),- _2.5r

0.5

o

-o.sL-­None

RESULTS

The DPI measures the same generaldomain of ps)'chopatholob'1' as theMinneapolis Multiphasic Pcrsonalil)'Invelltor)'. or MMPI Uad.son &HolTman. 1987). The I)PI \\~.tS select­ed for the presellt simi)' because irs<;cales specificall)' measure phenome­na most cOllllHanl)' reponed to beassociated with dcpcrsonaliz:llion. par­ticularly general feelings of unreality.

The ten OPt scalessclcclt..'<1 for thestudy measured Broodiness. Dcpres­..ion. Desocialinlion. Feelings of

nrealily. Mood Fluctuation.:-.ieufOlic Disorbranil-;Itioll. ThoughlDisorg-.tni1.alion, Perceptual DistOr­tion. Self Depredation. and Shallaw­!leSSOrArre(;l. Forcach subject. a lotal«,core on each DI'I scale ....-as calculat­ed b.~scd on [me/false responses toeach item. In addition, the IWI in­frequency and Defensiveness scaleswere used to check the validit), of lheresponses. Dcfensi'"CllcsS meaSllresthe tendency nOllO endorse items lhat~He low in social desirabilit),. In­frequency measures random or care­less respondillg, Onl)' 15 subjecl.Sendorsed one of the 1I\"c DPI In­frequenc), scale itcl11s, which wascommon in 50% or fcwcr of the sub­jt'CL" in the Ol'l's normativc sample"None of the sllbjccL" here Clldorsedmore lhan unc of thc Infrequenc)'itt.:ms. TIlliS, lhe 01'1 responses wcre\~llid, and no subjects wcrc eliminat­ed from Llle analysis.

Using Ward's method of hier~u·­

chical clusler analysis, subjects werecategorized into six groups based 011lheir responses 10 the five depcrson­aliL<llion scales (Sec Table I). The six­du<;ter solution was chosen on an a pri­ori basis. A I>osl-hoc examination ofallSOlutions resulting in fewer than sixCIU<;lcrsconlirmcd tll;lIlhe six cluster'Kllution llIaximilcd qualitati\'e differ­("Ilces in depersonalization among lhedusters.

iiIDI5SOCLUlO\. \01. IX. \0.1~I~

Page 4: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

The first depersonalization group contained the Derealized,who experienced Derealization on a monthly basis, but noother form ofdepersonalization. The second group consistedof the Self-negating, who regularly experience both Self-nega­tion and Derealization, and to a lesser extent Body-detach­ment. The third group consisted of the Body-detached, whoregularly experienced Body Detachment and Derealization,but only infrequently experienced the other types of deper­sonalization. The fourth group consisted of the ProfoundlyDepersonalized, who regularly experienced all forms ofdeper­sonalization. They were the only group to experience regu­lar Inauthenticityand Self-objectification, the latter ofwhichis the most pathological form of depersonalization. TheFleetingly Depersonalized and Non-depersonalized groups alsoemerged as predicted. These latter two groups consisted of25% and 50% of the sample, respectively. The existence andprevalence of the Fleetingly Depersonalized and a Non­depersonalized group was expected in a non-clinical popu­lation and is consistent with the literature (Nemiah, 1976).

A profile analysis was conducted to test whether tlle clus­ters differed in their profiles on more general traits associ­ated with pathology, as measured by the DP! scales. To testtllis hypothesis, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANO­VA) was performed using the DP! scales as repeated measuresof the within-subjects factor, general pathology, and thedepersonalization clusters as levels of the between-subjectsfactor, depersonalization. As expected a significant multi­variate interaction was found between depersonalization andgeneral pathology, £(45, 1182)~3.1,12<.0001. All univariatetests of interaction effects were also significant, except fortwo. The depersonalization clusters failed to significantly dif­fer in the degree to which their members reported a) unequallevels ofNeurotic Disorganization and Perceptual Distortionand b) unequal levels of Depression and Desocialization.

Overall, the subjects in the depersonalization clusters hadsignificantly different profiles on the DP! scales (See Figures1 and 2). The Non-depersonalized subjects had DP! scoresbelow average on all DP! scales, except for Familial Discord,which was average. The Fleetingly Depersonalized had DP!

scores which were in tlle average range, except for Feelingsof Unreality, Mood Fluctuation, and Perceptual Distortion,which were slightly above average. The Derealized hadslight elevations on the DP! scales, except for Desocializationand Familial Discord, which were slightly below average, andFeelings of nrealityand Mood Fluctuation scales which weresomewhat above average. The Self-negating scored aboveaverage on the Feelings of Unreality scale, and to a lesserexten t scored above average on the Broodines.s andDesocialization scales. Otherwise, the Self-ne~atinghad DP!scores which were only somewhat above average, and in arange similar to the scores by the Derealized and FleetinglyDepersonalized. In contrast, the Profoundly Depersonalizedhad exceptionally high elevations on nearly all the DP! scales,except DesociaIization and Familial Discord. Similarly, the

172

Body-detached had substantial elevations on most of the DP!scales, including Desocialization, but not Familial Discord.

DISCUSSION

Five of the six groups matched the authors' predictionsregarding tlle clustering of depersonalization symptoms,whereas expectations for the Self-negating group were onlypartially confirmed. The Self-negating group was expectedto report regular experiences of Inauthenticity, which wasnot the case. Inauthenticity experiences were not regularlyexperienced by any depersonalization group, except tlleProfoundly Depersonalized. Inauthenticity, which pertainsto experiences of the self as not genuine, may be associated,,~th pathological experiences, but only in a small portionof the population. Although occasional loss ofgenuinenessmay be common, persistent experiences of this type appearto be associated with relatively severe character pathologies.

Derealization is common to several groups regularlyexperiencing various forms of depersonalization, and is themost commonly expel;enced form ofdepersonalization, andpossibly an early symptom of the dissociation process.Individuals in the Derealized group, who only experienceDerealization, experience low levels of dissociation, as mea­sured by tlle DP! Feelings ofUnreality scale, whereas the Body­detached and the Self-negating report symptoms of deper­sonalization which reflect moderate levels ofdissociation. TheDerealized do not regularly experience symptoms associat­ed with the moderately dissociated groups, the Self-negat­ing and the Body-detached. These two moderately dissoci­ated groups have qualitatively distinct depersonalizationexperiences from each oilier. The Body-detached experiencetlleir physique as unfamiliar, detached or not belonging toiliem. The Self-negating experience alienation from emo­tions, ilioughts or situations which they recognize but try notto acknowledge because they are ego-dystonic. Thus, theBody-detached group's distress is caused by a diminished orlost relation to their body, whereas the Self-negating group'sdistress is caused by a lost recognition ofcertain experiences.

The more general traits of the Body-detached and Self­negating clusters also differ. Although similar in their brood­iness, desocialization and sense of unreality, the Body­detached tend to be more depressed, more disorganized intheir thoughts and feelings, and more given to perceptualdistortions and self-deprecation than the Self-negating.Aliliough the self-negated are moderately disturbed, tlleirdissociation stems largely from not wanting to acknowledgeego-dystonic events in the external world. The bodydetached's depersonalization is internalized wherein fun­damental aspects of themselves (i.e., their body) are expe­rienced as unreal. Body-detached experiences have long beenassociated wiili strong mood disorders, particularly depres­sive disorders Uacobson, 1959) whereas Self-negating expe­riences are associated wiili youiliful expectations ofilie world

DISSOCLmO\ Vol. IX. :\0. 3, Septemberl996

Page 5: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

which have gOlle unfilled (W<lgner & Tnu::man, 1984). TheBody-<Ictached's depression might account for their c1c\<tt­cd scores on self-depreciation and thought disorganization.Deprt:s.sion has long been associated \\;th negath'e and palho­logical self-images as well as diflicuhy in ororanizing think­ing and acting cfTecti\·e1y.

The Profoundly Depersonalized have the highest Ic\'cl.~

of dissociation, ;Ind experience all forms of depersonaliza­tion, most notabl)' Self-objectilicatioll which docs not occurregularly in any ofl..he other t}l>es. These individuals mal' beO\'erwhelmed b)' thcirdissociativeexpcriencesand ma)' ha\'clost familiarity \,·jlh theil' bodies. cognition. elllotions andthe cXlcmal world. This impairmclll of reality testing is afundamental feaUlre ofllorderline PersonalilY Disorder, andthe symptoms as.~ociated with Profound Depcrsonali7.:11ionhave bcen related to Borderline Personality Disorder(Chopra & BcaLWn, 1986; Gunderson. Kolb, & Austin. 1981,Munich, 1978). The rrofoundly Depersonali7.crl are rel:nivel}'high in thought disorganization, as \\'ell as neurotic disor­g.:miz:lIion, the tendency to be inefficient and indTcctivc inthe completion of rominc tasks. These traiL~,which arc char­acteristics of the Profoundly Dcpcrsonalized, and to a less­erextent the Bod}'-dctached, ma)' be severe enough to ilnpairthe ordinary functioning which most indi\'iduals take forgr.tnted. In the Profoundly Depersonalized and the Body­detached, social and occupational competence may be low­ered. These indh'iduals rna)' have dinicult)' al.lending t,o rel­evantdctails, and theirelnotionsoftcn ovcrpower thcirabilityto think and act cOcnivcly. Tucker et a1. (1973) noted thatsevere depersonalization was associated with high levels ofdisorg;:mi'led thinking, but that moderate and mild deper­!tOnalization ....<15 not necessarily associated with disorganil.edthinking. Thus, the perceptions of the ProfoundlyDepcrsonalized indh'idllal may nOI bc reliable and he or shelIlay be grosslydisorientcd in the external world. The break­down of allention and reasoning capacities has been asso­ciated with an implosion ofaversivc emotions. This gross dis­orientation allows lhe indh'idual to doubt the dislurbingreality and in turn, 10 defend againsl the avcrsive emotionsassocialCd with il (r-,'Iunich, 1978; Nores & Klelli, 1977).

Depression, which has been consisl,cntly associated withdepcrsonalization Uacobsoll. 1964; Neullcr, 1982; Tuckerel al., 1973) characterizes only the Profollndly Depersonal­ized and the BodY"detached, arguing thai only severely deper­sonalized individuals manifest persislent depressi\·e cogni­tion and affcct. TIle consistency with which dcpression anddissociation are associated in the literalUre raises the possi­bility that their presence is interactive; one may intensify theother. The distorted cognition and feelings which are char­acteristicofdepression may result in perceptionsof the world.the self, and the body asstr.l.nge and unfamiliar. In turn, theconsistent presence of depersonalizalion is likcl}' to makethe individual morc disu·csscd and depressed.

Similarly, self-deprcciation also dislinguishes lhe

JACOBS/BOVASSO

Profollndly Depcrsonalized and the Body-detached from theFleetingly Depersonalized, Derealized and Self-negatingI)'I>CS. TheJackson and Messick (1972) scale for Sclf-dcpre­dation consists of apprais..'tls of the self as worthless, unlov­able, and dcscl"\'ing ofrejcction. This self-cffaccment 1»' theProfoundly Depersonalized indhiduals adds support to theinferencc that these individuals are defending Ihemselvesagainstlnore illlensc Ih reats 10 identity Ihan the other dcper­sonalized types. Se\'Cre depersonalizalion has been associ­atcd ,\'ith the dC"c1opmenlal impainnem ofidentit)' and grossidentity diffusion characteristic of Borderline PersonalityDisorder. Other phenomena associalCd \\'ith acute deper­sonalizalion, such as life threatening trauma and sexualabuse, obviously I hreaten self:Concept and llsllally have neg­ative affcclS on sell~\'dlualiorls.

The faCior which mOSI distinguishes the Body-detachedfrom the Pl'Ofoundly Depersonalized is Desocialization,prob3blydue to the profoundly depersonalized being so cog­niti\'e1y disorganized that their social competcnce is ncga­tively affected. Thc)' pl'Obably lack both confidence and cog­nitivc skills to pcrform well in social siUlations. This groupis clearly the most pathological of the depersonalized types.The high scores of the Profoundly Depersonalized onthoughtclisorganiz.'ttion suggcslthat this group has the great­est difficulty with organizing and acting upon information.This is reflected in profound states of depersonalizationwherc thl.'y blur such fundamental perceplions ofselfas beingalive or distinguishing the self from the extcrnal world.

Broodinessgcllcr:lll)'distinguishes the three mostdeper­sonalized clusters from the threc least depersonalized elliS­

ters.Jackson and Messick (1972) define their Broodinessscaleas measuring an intense suspicion ofothers' llJoti\'ations, cau­tion about making personal disclosure and a tendcncyto\\~.trd paranoid idcation. These individuals search realil)'for information to just ify their persecutory ideation. althoughthey probably havc only vague ideas of others' motivations.Secondly, their constant and intense examination of themoti\'csorothers might make it more difficult to experienceothers as genuine or situations as rdatively straightfom-ardand not deceptive. For the brood)' indi\'idual, depersonal­ization may be fadlitat,ed by selectively perceiving informa­tion which docs not confirm their vague suspicions as unn,:­al. Other information supporting their \'iew of the world ashostile and persecutory is probably 50 .wersivc that it isexpe­riencecl as unreal. These indi\'idllals ,Ire in a double-bind;non-threatening perceptions \'iolatc their suspicions andseem unreal while threats to self and idelltity becomeunreal becallsc thcy are frighlening.

[n the groups displaring mild and moderate levels ofdel>crsonalizatioll, intact intellectual perceptions may lackaccompanying emotions. These thrcegrollps, the FleetinglyDepersonalized, Dcrealizcd and Self-negating may beemplo}ing depcrsonaliz.'ttion to defend against rdatively lessthrealening stimuli than individuals classified in the more

173

Page 6: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

DEPERSONALIZATION TYPES

severely depersonalized clusters. Eliot et al. (1984) note thatthe Fleetingly Depersonalized were defending against vio­lated self-expectations. Levy and Wachtel (1978) attributedthe anxiety of these individuals to role strain and Roberts(1960) and Torch (1978) attributed it to changes in famil­iar objects. These experiences violate expectations, but arenot severe enough to override intellectual functions and per­ceptions. Torch (1978) and Levy and Wachtel (1976) notethat certain derealized subjects may over-intellectualize andbe hypervigilant toward reality, becoming emotionallydetached from jarring events. In contrast, the reactions ofProfoundly Depersonalized individuals have been associat­ed with life-threatening trauma (Kletti, 1976), sexual abuse(Steinberg, 1991), suicidal impulses (Munich, 1978), and ina diffusion or loss offundamental aspects of identity (Chopra& Beatson, 1986; Gunderson et aI., 1981).

Although these results should be approached with cau­tion, a vivid pattern emerges which suggests that deperson­alization may be indicative of overall level of psychopathol­ogy. Individuals who report certain types ofdepersonalizationhave higher levels of pathology than individuals who reportother types of depersonalization. The failure to differenti­ate these distinct types ofdepersonalization may result in themisclassification of individuals with varying levels of overallpsychopathology. In assessing individuals for psychopathol­ogy, consideration of the distinct types of depersonalizationreported by an individual may provide an expedient indexof their overall level of pathology.•

REFERE CES

Auld, F., & Noel, M.C. (1984). Further evidence of the validity ofthe Differential Personality Inventory, Academic Psychology Bulletin,6, I, 11-19

Bernstein, E.M., & Purnam, F.W. (1986). Development, reliability,and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of NtmJous and MentalDiseases, 174,727-735.

Chopra, H., & Beatson,]. (1986). Psychotic symptoms in border­line personality disorder. AmericanJournal ofPsychiatry, 143, 1605­1607.

Eliot, G., Rosenberg, M., & Wagner, M. (1984). Transient deper­sonalization in youth. Social Psychology Quarterly, 74,115-129.

Frischholz, E]., Braun, B.G., & Sachs, R.G. (1991). Construct valid­ity of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES). DJSSOCIA TION, 4,

185-188.

Frischholz, E]., Braun, B.G., Sachs, R.G., Hopkins, L., Sc~aeffer,

D.M., Le,vis,]., Leavitt, F., Pasquotto, M.A., & Schwartz, M.(1990).The Dissociative Experiences Scale: Further replication and vali­dation. DISSOCIATION, 3, 151-153.

174

Gunderson,]., Kolb,]., & Austin, V. (1981). The diagnostic inter­view for borderline patients. AmericanJournal ofPsychiatry, 138, 896­903.

jackson, D.N., & Garlson, R (1973). Convergent and discriminantvalidation of the Differential Personality Inventory. Journal ofClinical Psychology, 29, 214-219.

jackson, D.N., & Messick, S. (1972). Differential Personality Inventory.Cleveland Ohio: Psychological Corporation.

jackson, D.N., & Hoffman, H. (1987) Common dimensions ofpsy­chopathology from the MMPI and the Basic Personality Inventory.Journal ofClinical Psychology, 43(6), 661-669.

jacobs,]., & Bovasso, G. (1992). Toward a clarification of the con­struct of depersonalization and its association vvith affective andcognitive dysfunctions. Journal ofPersonality Assessment, 59(2), 352­365.

jacobson, E. (1964). Theselfand oilject world. New York InternationalUniversity Press.

Kelly, G. (1955). Psychology ofpersonal constructs. New York: Norton.

'emiah (1976). Depersonalization neurosis. In The comprehensivetextbook ofpsychiatry (pp. 1268-73). New York: Basic Books.

Noyes, R, & Kletti, R (1977). Depersonalization in response to life­threatening danger. Comparative Psychiatry, 18,375-384.

Levy,]., & Wachtel, P. (1978). Depersonalization: An effort atclarification. AmericanJournal ofPsychoanalysis, 38, 291-300.

Mellor, C. (1988). Depersonalization and self-perception. British

Journal ofPsychiatry, 153, 15-19.

Munich, R (1978). Depersonalization in a female adolescent.InternationalJournal ofPsychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 6, 187-199.

Myers, c., & Grant, C. (1972). A study of depersonalization in stu­dents. BritishJournal ofPsychiatry, 121,59-65.

ueller, YL. (1982). Depersonalization: Symptoms, meanings, ther­apy. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 66, 451-458.

Oberdorf, C.P. (1950). The role of anxiety in depersonalization.InternationalJournal ofPsychoanal)'sis, 31, 1-5.

Roberts, W. (1960). ormal and abnormal depersonalization.Journal ofMental Science, 101,478-493.

Ross, C.A.,joshi, S., & Currie, R (1990). Dissociative experiencesin the general population. AmericanJournalofPsychiatry, 147, 1547­1552.

Sanders, B., McRoberts, G., & Tollefson, C. (1989). Childhood stressand dissociation in a college population. DISSOCIA TION, 2, 17-23. .

DIssoumox, 1'01. IX. :\0. 3, Septembe.I996

Page 7: APROFILE ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY IN CLUSTERS OF ...

Sanders. S. (1986). The Perceplual A]lcr:uion Seait': A scale mea­suring diswc::i;uioll. ,lmmwlljQuma/Q/Qlllical Hypnosis, 29.95-102.

Singer. J.L, & Sincoff, Jlt (1990). Bcrond repression and thedefenses. In Sillgcr.J.L (cd.). RtpmsiQfl alld dissociation: Impficatio1U

fqr P"'W,udlty Ihi:01J. pSJrhopathMl1ffJ and hnlilh. Chicago: UllivcrsilrorChkago PreMo

Spiegel. O. (Ed.). (1993). I)wOOa/ilW diJOYtUrs: A clinical rnnnu.LUlhenille, 1\10: Sidra Prc~.

Steinberg. M. (1991). Depersonalization: AssessmclII and treatment.RLWw of/~(hitlITJ, 10.223-2<17,

Torch, [.1\1. (1978). Rl.."\icwohhe relationship between o~ssion

and depersonalil.;uion. Acfa PJJduutrica Scondimnlla, 58. 191·198.

Tmemen. D. (1984). DepcrsonaliLation in a college sample.JounUlIof Gron-alITjchoklgy. /4, 980-989.

Tucker.GJ..I"arrow.~I.. &Quilllan. D. (1973). Oepersonalil.alion.d)'sphoria, and lhough. dismrb,.1nce. AWU'ricanJournal ofP5JcJliatry,IJO, i02-706.

DISSOClUlO'\. \'ollX. \0.1~btrJ~

JACOBS/BOVASSO

175