Top Banner
1 Accepted for publication in The Mental Lexicon. Authors copy. Spelling errors in English derivational suxes reflect morphological boundary strength: A case study Susanne Gahl 1 , Ingo Plag 2 1 University of California at Berkeley, USA 2 Heinrich-Heine Universität Düsseldorf, Germany Address for correspondence: Susanne Gahl, Department of Linguistics, UC Berkeley, 1203 Dwinelle Hall, Berkeley, CA 94720-2650. Phone: (510) 643-7621. Fax: (510) 643-5688. [email protected]
67

ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

Dec 03, 2021

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

1

Accepted for publication in The Mental Lexicon.

Author’s copy.

Spelling errors in English derivational suffixes reflect morphological boundary strength:

A case study

Susanne Gahl1, Ingo Plag

2

1 University of California at Berkeley, USA

2Heinrich-Heine Universität Düsseldorf, Germany

Address for correspondence:

Susanne Gahl, Department of Linguistics, UC Berkeley, 1203 Dwinelle Hall, Berkeley, CA

94720-2650. Phone: (510) 643-7621. Fax: (510) 643-5688.

[email protected]

Page 2: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

2

Do speakers decompose morphologically complex words, such as segmentable, into their

morphological constituents? In this article, we argue that spelling errors in English affixes reflect

morphological boundary strength and degrees of segmentability. In support of this argument, we

present a case study examining the spelling of the suffixes –able/-ible, -ence/-ance, and -ment in

an online resource (Tweets), in forms such as <availible>, <invisable>, <eloquance>, and

<bettermint>. Based on previous research on morphological productivity and boundary strength

(Hay, 2002; Hay & Baayen, 2002, 2005), we hypothesized that morphological segmentability

should affect the choice between <able> vs. <ible>, <ance> vs. <ence>, and <ment> vs. <-mint>.

An analysis of roughly 37,000 non-standard spellings is consistent with that hypothesis,

underscoring the usefulness of spelling variation as a source of evidence for morphological

segmentability and for the role of morphological representations in language production and

comprehension. The present study contributes to the growing number of studies of misspellings

as a source of evidence in psycholinguistics generally and morphology in particular.

Page 3: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

3

Pronunciation variation has established itself as a source of evidence on a range of questions

in psycholinguistics, including the processing of morphologically complex words (Cohen 2014;

Hay 2003, 2007; Hay and Baayen 2005; Kemps, Ernestus, Schreuder, and Baayen 2005; Plag

2014; Seyfarth, Garellek, Gillingham, Ackerman, and Malouf 2017, among many others). For

example, Hay (2003, 2007); Plag and Ben Hedia (2018); Smith, Baker, and Hawkins (2012)

found acoustic reduction of affixes correlating with morphological categories. Along similar

lines, Sproat and Fujimura (1993) and Lee-Kim, Davidson, and Hwang (2013) show that the

duration and degree of velarization of /l/ at morpheme boundaries in English depends on the

strength of the boundary. A smaller literature (e.g. Assink, 1985; Fayol, Largy, & Lemaire, 1994;

Largy, 1996; Schmitz, Chamalaun, & Ernestus, 2018) has mined spelling variability for

psycholinguistic evidence. In this paper, we argue that misspellings such as <availible> and

<invisable> also reflect the presence and strength of morphological boundaries, along with

several other lexical properties.

Research on pronunciation variation does not characterize variants as ’mispronounced’.

Therefore, misspellings might seem less analogous to pronunciation variation and more like

’slips of the pen’, i.e. a written analog to speech errors (see e.g. Cutler, 2011; Dell, 1986).

However, a characteristic property of speech errors is that talkers, once they become aware of

what they have said, consider themselves to have made an error. By contrast, a spelling like

<availible> may well represent what an individual believes to be the correct spelling of a word.

As Sandra (2010, p. 425) point out "it might be argued that the incorrect orthographic

representations are also stored in the mental lexicon". Nonstandard spellings may therefore have

more in common with pronunciation variants than with speech errors.

Orthographically licensed variation in spelling has already been recognized as reflecting

morphological structure, specifically in the case of doublets, specifically the use of spaces and

hyphens in English compounds. Kuperman and Bertram (2013) show that the alternation

Page 4: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

4

between concatenated (e.g. <postcard>), hyphenated (<word-play>), and spaced (<trash can>)

compounds in English reflects aspects of morphological processing and in turn affects processing

during reading (Falkauskas & Kuperman, 2015; Rahmanian & Kuperman, 2019). Specifically,

Kuperman and Bertram (2013) argue that variability in compound spelling correlates with

differences in morphological boundary strength (Hay & Baayen, 2002, 2005).

Based on the research on compound spelling variability, and the research on keystroke

variability in compounds and derived words, we hypothesized that spelling mistakes in English

derivational affixes likewise reflect the segmentability of words, i.e. the presence and the strength

of morphological boundaries, among other factors.

Following Hay and Baayen (2002, 2005), we consider morphological boundary strength to be

a gradient property of morphological boundaries reflecting their salience and the degree of

segmentability of complex forms (Hay & Baayen, 2002, 2005). On this view, boundary strength

is influenced by multiple parameters such as morphological productivity, semantic transparency,

and the relative frequency of base and derived word (Blumenthal-Dramé et al., 2017; Hay &

Baayen, 2002; Vannest, Newport, Newman, & Bavelier, 2011). The gradience of morphological

boundaries, and the consequences of segmentability for the representation of words and

morphemes, are a matter of active debate (Baayen, 2014; Hay & Baayen, 2005). In a recent

contribution to that debate, Schmitz et al. (2018) argue that misspellings of Dutch verb forms

reflect holistic representations vs. on-the-fly generation of inflectional endings. Effects of

morphological boundary strength on spelling behavior thus bear upon key issues in research on

lexical processing.

The empirical starting point for our hypothesis about spelling was the observation that pairs of

English derivational affixes such as -able and -ible are often confused with one another. For

example, at the time of writing, the Wikipedia page listing frequent misspellings in Wikipedia

listed 4277 misspellings of 3077 word forms, many of them morphologically complex

Page 5: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

5

(Wikipedia, 2017). Of the 3077 word forms in the list, 2876 appear in the English Lexicon

Project (ELP, Balota et al., 2007). Of these 2876 forms, 2152 were multimorphemic, according

to the ELP. Evidently, morphologically complex words give rise to many spelling errors.

Intriguingly, the errors seem to reflect morphological structure, rather than simple letter

confusions: For example, the Wikipedia list includes 60 misspellings of words standardly ending

in <able> or <ible>. (In what follows, we use italics to represent morphemes, preceded by a

hyphen for suffixes (e.g. -ible), and angle brackets to represent strings of letters (e.g. <ible>.) In

25 of these 60 cases, the misspelling differs from the standard spelling only in that <able> is

replaced with <ible> or vice versa (e.g. <acceptible>, <capible>, <formidible>, <hospitible>,

<inevitible>, <liible>, <unavailible>; and <accessable>, <compatable>, <eligable>, <feasable>,

<incorruptable>, <incredable>, <infallable>, <irresistable>, <permissable>, <plausable>,

<possable>). These examples suggest that writers appear to exchange <ible> and <able>, rather

than spelling these endings in some other way, e.g. as <eble> or <ibble>, even though all of these

variants would result in forms with identical or nearly identical pronunciations.

We hypothesized that such orthographic morpheme-exchanges reflected morphological

boundary strength: Boundaries before -able tend to be stronger than those before -ible,

suggesting that words in -able tend to be more segmentable than words in -ible. (We discuss this

point in greater detail below.) We hypothesized that the degree of segmentability should affect

spelling behavior. Against the backdrop of that general hypothesis, we evaluated two specific

hypotheses leading to overlapping, but different, testable predictions: The first, which we call the

’segmentability’ hypothesis, holds that high segmentability promotes correct affixal spelling.

Some evidence supporting that hypothesis comes from case studies of individuals with acquired

dysgraphia who produced more correct spellings for multimorphemic forms than

monomorphemic ones (see Rapp & Fischer-Baum, 2014, for an overview). The second, which

we call the ’typicality’ hypothesis, holds that typical instances of affixes should be easier to spell

Page 6: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

6

than atypical ones. For example, the word washable has several properties typical of words with

strong morphological boundaries, making it a typical instance of a word containing -able:

Among other things, washable is semantically transparent and less frequent than its base, wash.

By contrast, available and laudable are atypical words with -able, in that they contain weak

morphological boundaries: The derived forms are more frequent than their bases (avail- and

laud-), and the semantic relationship between the derived forms and the bases is fairly opaque.

The segmentability hypothesis and the typicality hypothesis make identical predictions about

the difficulty of washable, laudable and available, though for different reasons. Both predict

washable to be an easier spelling target than laudable and available. The segmentability

hypothesis predicts this pattern because washable contains the strongest morphological boundary

of these three words. The typicality hypothesis predicts the same pattern because the properties

of washable match those of its suffix (-able), whereas laudable and available are atypical

environments for -able and invite the non-standard spellings <laudible> and <availible>.

The predictions of the two hypotheses diverge in the case of -ible-words with strong

boundaries. For example, the word accessible is semantically transparent and is less frequent

than its base access (for example based on the SUBTLEXus database, Brysbaert and New 2009).

The segmentability hypothesis predicts accessible to be a relatively easy spelling target - the

word is highly segmentable - whereas the typicality hypothesis predicts it to be a difficult

spelling target - the word contains an atypical instance of -ible, making <accessable> the

expected spelling. More generally, the segmentability hypothesis predicts affixes in highly

segmentable words to be easy spelling targets, regardless of the identity of the affix. The

predictions of the typicality hypothesis depend on the identity of the affix, more specifically on

the match or mismatch between properties of affixes and words.

We tested these hypotheses by examining three pairs of competing spellings, representing

three constellations of morphological boundary strength: <able>/<ible>, <ence>/<ance>, and

Page 7: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

7

<ment>/<mint>. The suffixes -able/-ible differ in boundary strength, while -ance and -ence do

not (the reader is referred to “Target Suffixes” for more detail). The suffix ment tends to form

salient boundaries. While <ment> lacks a suffixal twin in standard orthography, it competes with

a non-standard affixal spelling <mint>. The pairs -ance/-ence and the singleton -ment thus

function as a control condition. If spelling behavior reflects the boundary strength associated

with each affix (as opposed to the salience of the boundary in a given word, i.e. a stem+suffix

combination), then boundary strength should affect the spelling of -ible/-able, i.e. the suffixes

that differ in boundary strength, but not -ence/-ance or -ment, i.e. the suffixes that did not differ

in the boundary strength, and the suffix without a competitor.

Our data come from a large unmoderated source of written productions: Tweets, i.e. short

messages posted on the internet by means of a messaging and social networking service, Twitter

(Twitter, 2006, http://www.twitter.com). Related research also using Tweets is reported in

Schmitz et al. (2018). We analyze the distribution of spelling variants by means of logistic

regression models taking into account a range of morphological, orthographic and lexical

predictors of spelling errors.

Morphological boundary strength and word segmentability have been found to be reflected in

a number of behavioral and distributional phenomena. Accordingly, a range of variables have

served as measures of boundary strength. We therefore begin our discussion by summarizing

prior research on measures of morphological boundary strength generally and the boundary-

relevant properties of our target suffixes in particular. We then turn to other factors besides

boundary strength that may affect the spelling of our target words. A general assumption

underlying the current study is that one and the same affix may occur in words that are more or

less segmentable. In considering the role of boundary strength in spelling behavior, it is

important, therefore, to consider the properties of bases and derived words, as well as those of

affixes. We do so in the opening section of the Results.

Page 8: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

8

Background

Morphological Boundary Strength

At least three types of converging measures of boundary strength can be regarded as

established: semantic transparency, base type (a categorical measure), and gradient

distributional properties of morphemes and phonological segments. In this study, we

concentrate on base type and distributional measures.

Undoubtedly the most widely discussed measure of boundary strength in English is a binary

distinction between ’weak’ boundaries (often represented with a plus sign) and ’strong’ ones

(often represented with a hash mark) (see e.g. Chomsky & Halle, 1968; Dressler, 1985;

Kiparsky, 1982; Siegel, 1979). In this categorical scheme, boundaries between bound roots and

affixes are held to be ‘weak’, while boundaries at the edges of free bases are held to be ‘strong’.

Similarly, English derivational affixes have been analyzed as falling into classes differing in

boundary strength, on the basis of phonological (e.g. stress-shifting) and semantic criteria. Also

related to this binary disctinction is the fact that compounds, as combinations of morphologically

free constituents, are held to contain strong internal boundaries.

More recently, Hay (2003); Hay and Baayen (2005) proposed gradient measures of boundary

strength. One such measure, sometimes referred to as ‘base-to-derived’ or ’relative’ frequency, is

estimated by calculating the ratio of the frequencies of the base and the whole-word frequency,

i.e. the derived form. The higher the ratio of base frequency and whole-word frequency, the

stronger the morphological boundary and the more segmentable the word. For instance,

government is far more frequent than its base govern and is therefore less easily segmented than,

for example, enjoyment, whose base is far more frequent than its base. Two additional gradient

lexical properties correlating with boundary strength are semantic transparency and

morphological productivity (Hay & Baayen, 2002): More semantically transparent formations

such as shoeless are argued to contain stronger boundaries than semantically more opaque ones

Page 9: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

9

such as regardless, and highly productive affixes, such as -ness, tend to be associated with

stronger boundaries than less productive ones such as -th. Finally, boundary strength also

manifests itself in affix ordering (Hay & Plag, 2004; Plag & Baayen, 2009; Zirkel, 2010): Weak

morphological boundaries tend to occur ’inside’ of strong boundaries, rather than ’outside’, a

pattern termed ’complexity-based ordering’ (see Hay and Baayen 2002 for discussion and

illustration).

Morphological boundaries and typing speed

Temporal properties of written language production have already been shown to be affected

by the presence and strength of morphological boundaries. Gagné and Spalding (2014); Libben,

Weber, and Miwa (2012) and Libben and Weber (2014), for example, investigated typing

latencies for English compounds without spaces (e.g. strawberry) and found that inter-keystroke

intervals were significantly elevated at the boundary between the stems. Similarly, Gagné and

Spalding (2016a) found differences in typing speed between monomorphemic words and

compounds. Similar results were obtained by Libben, Jarema, Luke, and Bork (September 25 –

28, 2018) for other kinds of complex words in English and French, i.e. stem-stem combinations,

as in xylo-phone, prefix-stem combinations, as in im-plant, and stem-suffix combinations as in

form-ation). In all conditions apart from French prefix-stem words the letter transition across the

morpheme boundary showed longer keystroke intervals than the preceding and following letter

transitions. Boundary strength has likewise been shown to affect typing behavior: Gagné and

Spalding (2014, 2016a, 2016b); Libben and Weber (2014) found that compounds with

semantically transparent constituents showed different inter-keystroke intervals compared to

those with non-transparent constituents. Testing compound frequency and head frequency in

addition to semantic transparency, Sahel, Nottbusch, Grimm, and Weingarten (2008) likewise

Page 10: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

10

found that keystroke intervals varied with the strength of the compound-internal boundary. These

findings lend general support to the idea that boundary strength can affect written language

production.

The variables relating to boundary strength in the statistical models in the current study were

informed by three measures: a binary distinction between free bases and bound roots, and two

gradient measures (relative frequency and bigram probabilities). We describe these measures in

detail in our Methods section. The variables are grounded in previous treatments of our target

suffixes, to which we turn next, before discussing additional factors that we had reason to believe

would affect the spelling of our target words.

Target Suffixes

To understand the effects of boundary strength on spelling behavior, we examined three pairs

of spellings, each of which appeared to be tricky spelling targets, based on informal

observations: <able>/<ible>, <ence>/<ance>, and <ment>/<mint>. While <ment> lacks a

suffixal twin in standard orthography, it competes with a non-standard affixal spelling <mint>.

We are not aware of any systematic studies of the non-standard spelling <mint>. The

segmentability of words with the affixes -able/-ible,-ence/-ance, and -ment, however, has been

studied in a fair amount of detail.

able/ible. Many sources treat <ible> and <able> as two orthographic variants or two

allomorphs of one and the same suffix (see e.g. Plag, 2003, 95) . Bauer, Lieber, and Plag (2013,

307) explicitly label the two items ‘allomorphs’, on the grounds that they do not appear to differ

in meaning. And yet, previous research also provides evidence for differences between -ible and

-able, summarized in Table 1, which is partly based on Table 14.1 in Bauer et al. (2013, 290ff.).

It will be observed that the evidence suggests that -able tends to be associated with stronger

Page 11: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

11

morphological boundaries than -ible, in that it attaches to a wider range of bases than -ible, is

considered highly productive, and does not typically induce stress shifts or other

morphophonological alternations in the bases it combines with (with few exceptions, such as

admirable).

=========== Place Table 1 About Here ==========================

-ance/-ence. Like -able and -ible, the pair -ance and -ence are treated as allomorphs of one

and the same affix by Bauer et al. (2013, section 10.2.1), on the grounds that we find the same

semantics for a whole set of phonologically related formatives: -ance, -ence, -ce, -cy. As Bauer

et al. point out, -ance, -ence, -ce, -cy have several puzzling properties, two of which may give

rise to spelling uncertainty. First, almost every derivative in -ance or -ence has a corresponding

adjectival derivative ending in -ant or -ent, justifying two morpheme-based parses: Xent + ce or

X + ence and thus making the location of the boundary unclear. Second, the distribution of the

<a> vs. the <e> is not well predictable on the basis of the base, which might further increase the

chances of misspelling. Bauer et al. (2013) also mention several facts suggesting that -ance may

be associated with slightly stronger boundaries than -ence: There are a few cases of -ance

attaching to non-latinate bases (believance, coming outtance), but none for -ence, suggesting a

wider range of bases for -ance compared to -ence. There also appears to be a greater number of

word types with -ance than with -ence. In all other respects, the two endings appear to behave

similarly.

-ment. The suffix -ment derives event nominalizations with a wide range of possible readings,

depending on the base and the context (see e.g. Kawaletz & Plag, 2015). -ment is most often

found with verbal bases, though other bases are also found. Many words with -ment contain

bound roots, but many others contain free bases. The suffix was highly productive in the 19th

century, and it is still moderately productive today. Given its productivity, and given that -ment

Page 12: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

12

is the only consonant-initial suffix in the current study, a property associated with perceptually

salient boundaries (Hay, 2003), we expect it to be associated with strong morphological

boundaries, compared to the other target suffixes.

Other Factors Likely To Affect Spelling Difficulty

Among other factors likely to affect spelling behavior, perhaps the most prominent one is

lexical frequency. Other things being equal, one might expect highly frequent (and therefore

familiar) words to be easier to spell than rare ones (see e.g. Assink, 1985; Fayol et al., 1994;

Largy, 1996, for discussion). On the other hand, high usage frequency also entails frequent

opportunities for misspelling a word, and for finding it in a corpus. We return to this point in the

description of our data and sampling methods.

In the current study, we in addition considered the segmentability of the base. We

suspected that morphological complexity of bases might affect word segmentability and hence

spelling behavior: Words in which our target suffixes follow morphologically complex material

(e.g. indescribable, uncombable or imperturbable) may themselves be more readily segmented

than words with morphologically simple bases. Although we are not aware of studies testing this

intuition directly, we reasoned that salient boundaries within (complex) bases might promote

segmentability of the form as a whole and therefore included the presence of a boundary within

bases as a predictor in our model. We return to this issue in the General Discussion.

Base segmentability may also shape the behavior of another variable that has been extensively

studied in research on lexical processing, which is word length. The expected effects of word

length in letters on suffix spelling may not be obvious: Although long words offer more

opportunities for error, that fact need not result in greater numbers of errors on a particular letter

Page 13: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

13

in an affix, such as the <a> or <i> in <able> or <ible>. Long words need not be difficult to spell,

especially if they saliently contain components that are relatively easy spelling targets. Indeed,

long words are more likely to be morphologically complex than short ones. If it is indeed the

case, as we hypothesized, that high segmentability promotes correct spelling, then word length

might be positively associated with spelling accuracy.

We are aware that word length (in letters) has been found to be negatively associated with

accuracy, both in individuals with disgraphia (Caramazza, Miceli, Villa, and Romani , 1987) and

in healthy individuals (Bloomer, 1956; Cahen, Craun, and Johnson, 1971; Carlisle, 1988;

Spencer, 2007). However, as noted in Spencer (2007), among others, apparent effects of word

length may reflect other factors, such as the number of letters per grapheme, as well as lexical

frequency.

Several additional visual and phonological factors may affect spelling behavior. For

example, misspellings resulting in the letter sequence <ii>, such as amiible or

remediible ’look wrong’, presumably partly because the sequence <ii> is extremely rare

in English (although it does occur, e.g. in the spelling ‘Hawaii’, i.e. ’Hawai’i’ without

the okina). In our analyses, we take effects of the rarity of specific letter combinations

into account by considering bigram probabilities.

Another fact related to specific letter combinations that may well affect spelling concerns

pronunciation. Presumably, many instances of spelling variability are due to what might be

called ’pronunciation spelling’, i.e. plausible spellings, given the pronunciation of the target. The

situation is the inverse of what is known as ’spelling pronunciation’: ‘Spelling pronunciations’

are cases in which speakers unfamiliar with the standard pronunciation of a word pronounce it in

a way that is plausible, given its spelling, e.g. pronouncing <awry> [ɔɹɪ]. By ‘pronunciation

Page 14: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

14

spelling’, conversely, we mean cases in which the speaker is familiar with the pronunciation of a

word, but not with its standard spelling. In most of our target pairs pronunciation offers little help

in deciding between the two spellings: For example, <available> and <availible> are both

plausible spellings of [əveɪləbl]. However, in one class of target words, pronunciation does

provide strong spelling clues: The pronunciation of <g> and <c> after [i] vs. [a] may make

misspellings like <allocible> for allocable or <legable> for legible far easier to avoid and detect

than forms like invisable. Therefore, we expect such misspellings to be relatively rare. We return

to this issue in the description of our variables and in the discussion of the results.

Methods

Target Words

The target words for the analysis were all words ending in <able>, <ible>, <ance>,

<ence>, and <ment> in the CELEX lexical database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Gulikers, 1995),

with the following exceptions:

Doublets Pairs such as discernable/discernible, indispensable/indispensible,

collectable/collectible, valance/valence), where both spellings were listed in the English

Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007).

Pseudoaffixes Words ending in the strings <ance>, <ence>, <ment>, <able>, and

<ible> where these endings represented stems, such as (un)able, dement or parts of

stems, such as freelance, bible, table, foible, advance, askance.

Pseudobases Words like cement, whose base is not attested in other words in CELEX

(in transparently related meanings). Such items warrant future investigation, particularly as

informal web searches revealed that spellings such as cemint are indeed attested.

Page 15: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

15

Hyphens and whitespaces Items containing hyphens or white spaces, such as enfant

terrible.

We excluded words like crucible and thurible, in which <ible> is historically derived from

Latin -(i)bulum rather than -a/ibilis. We considered excluding words in which the target endings

optionally bore main stress, e.g. category-ambiguous words like torment and certain

unambiguous forms, such as refinance and dalliance. In the case of dalliance, some speakers not

only stress the final syllable in the words, but also nasalize the vowel in the final syllable. We

decided to include this word on the grounds that (unlike thurible or crucible) it contains the target

suffix -ance. Our list of spelling variants initially included the form <ambiance>, which we later

identified as an officially recognized orthographic doublet. The pair <ambience, ambiance> was

therefore also excluded.

Data Collection

The data were collected using the searchTwitter function in the twitteR library (Gentry, 2015)

in R(R Development Core Team, 2008) in five separate data collection sessions between

December 2016 and October 2017. The searchTwitter function returns tweets posted during the

previous seven days (Gentry, 2015). Each target spelling was included in two data collection

sessions. At the time when these searches were carried out, the number of hits for a given target

in a given session was capped at 2000. The data collection sessions were spaced several months

apart. Our analyses are based on the total number of hits, i.e. the sum of the number of hits in the

two sessions for each target. The search targets were the misspelled versions (’spelling variants’)

of our target words, which were created by changing <able>, <ible>, <ance>, <ence>, and

<ment> in the standard spelling to <ible>, <able>, <ence>, <ance>, and <mint>, respectively.

The resulting corpus consisted of 22857 tweets. The counts of word types and mispelled tokens

Page 16: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

16

for each target suffix are listed in Table 2.

=========== Place Table 2 About Here ==========================

Statistical modeling strategies

We used binary logistic regression to fit two sets of models. In one set, the outcome variable

coded whether a given spelling variant (e.g. <edable>) was attested at all in our corpus. In the

second set, we set a higher threshold, with the outcome variable indicating whether a given

spelling variant was attested at least six times. As Table 3 shows, the proportion of variant word

types attested at least six times ranged from 0.09 for -ment to 0.51 for -ence, -ance. While the

higher threshold reduces the size of the data set, it also potentially reduces the amount of noise

and the possibility of overestimating the occurrences of misspellings. A similar strategy, of using

binary logistic regression models with varying thresholds for modeling the probability of spelling

errors was implemented in Bar-On and Kuperman (2018, in press). An alternative strategy would

have been to include both correct and incorrect spellings in our corpus, and then to use a variable

coding whether a form was spelled correctly as the outcome variable. We would have liked to

use that strategy, but decided against it, due to the fact that, as already mentioned, the number of

hits per session was capped at the time the corpus was put together. This cap would have

inevitably led to a distorted picture of the relative frequency of correct and incorrrect spellings,

particularly in the case of high-frequency words: Most of the target words would have produced

exactly 2000 hits - and any word whose spelling variant was also attested 2000 times would have

come to look as though it got misspelled 50% of the time.

=========== Place Table 3 About Here ==========================

We used backwards elimination, i.e. initially including the full set of predictors and

Page 17: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

17

subsequently eliminating non-significant predictors, beginning with non-significant interactions,

then progressing to non-significant simple effects. Model improvement was determined based on

change in the AIC. The order of removal of non-significant effects was based on the degree to

which a given effect was established in previous literature, with well-established effects being

retained longer than effects for which, to our knowledge, little or no previous empirical support

was available. All continuous variables were centered and log-transformed. Separate models

were fitted for each (pair of) target suffixes. For each pair, we report the model using the

outcome variable coding whether a given spelling variant (e.g. edable) was attested at all in our

corpus. Where the results differed for the models using the more stringent threshold, i.e.

modeling whether a given spelling variant was attested at least six times, we note that fact.

In order to test the predictions of the ’typicality hypothesis’ introduced above, we included the

interactions of Suffix with each of the other variables, except in cases where too few target

words were available for a given combination of Suffix with the factor in question (for factorial

variables), or where target words for a given suffix were too sparsely distributed (in the case of

continuous variables). We also included interactions of Target frequency with the variables

intended to capture segmentability effects in our initial models, to explore the effects of the

segmentability variables at different frequency bands.

We also considered alternative outcome measures, such as the ratio of the frequency of the

spelling variants and the standard spellings, or the entropy when selecting one of the available

variants given their probability distribution (see, for example, Bar-On and Kuperman (2018, in

press) for discussion). However, for our data set we were not able to compute these measures

meaningfully because, as mentioned before, at the time when data collection was performed, the

searchTwitteR capped the frequency counts for each target at 2000. This constraint would have

resulted in identical frequency counts (of 2000) for many of our target words, including words

Page 18: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

18

that clearly differ in frequency, based on SUBTLX or CELEX. None of the spelling variants

occurred frequently enough for the cap to be a problem.

As an alternative to logistic regression we also explored models of count data

(negative binomial regression and hurdle regression), but the distribution of the counts

of misspellings violated the pertinent model assumptions. We therefore used logistic

regression with two different thresholds, as described above.

Descriptions of Variables

Our aim was to take into account factors known to affect the processing of derivationally

complex words, including morphological complexity and boundary strength, base

segmentability, lexical frequency, and word length. In addition, we wished to take into account

visual factors. In exploratory analyses, we determined a set of variables indexing these factors:

Base type A binary factor distinguishing two types of morphological bases: Roots (e.g.

applic-able) vs. word (e.g. govern-able).

Base complexity A binary factor distinguishing words with morphologically simple bases

(e.g. washable) vs. words in which the material preceding the target suffix contains internal

morphological boundaries, such as complex bases (e.g. act-ionable, ex-changeable), prefixes

before the morphological base the target suffix combines with (e.g. non-flammable), and

morphologically ambiguous forms (e.g. re-solvable). The term ’base’ is used somewhat loosely

here, to refer to the string preceding the target suffix; we do not intend any claim as to the

morphological base of the word or the combinatory properties of the suffix.

Length The target word’s length in letters.

Page 19: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

19

Target bigram The transitional probability of the initial letter of the target, given the

base-final letter (i.e. the forward bigram probability). We estimated this probability based on the

number of distinct words the bigram occurs in (termed the ’non-positional versatility’) as

reported in Solso and Juel (1980, 298). The Target bigram variable represents the number of

word types in which a given base-final letter is followed by a given suffix-initial letter, divided

by the total number of word types with a given base-final letter.

Variant bigram The transitional probability of the initial letter of the spelling variant (e.g.

<availible>), given the final letter of the base, estimated in the same way as for the Target bigram

variable. The intuition behind this variable is that spelling variants ’look odd’ to varying degrees:

For example, consecutive tokens of <i> (as in insatiible) seem highly noticeable because of the

rarity of <ii> in correct spellings of English words (as mentioned above). More generally, low-

probability letter sequences may make misspellings easier to spot and hence more likely to be

corrected. We discuss this variable further in the Discussion below.

Target frequency As estimates of the frequency of the target word, we used the lemma

frequencies in the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA Davies, 2013).

Base frequency As a gradient measure of boundary strength, we used the frequency of the

orthographic base to which the target suffix is attached. The orthographic base was the letter

string preceding the target suffix, i.e. the word minus the suffix (e.g. <avail-able>, <comprehens-

ible>. Higher values of this measure indicate greater segmentability.

Our measure of base frequency calls for further explanation. Measures of base frequency, and

relative (base-to-derived) were originally developed in investigations of words containing free

bases, i.e. forms that have lexical frequencies of their own. Estimating the frequency of bound

roots raises methodological difficulties, as bound roots by definition do not occur independently.

Page 20: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

20

Two main strategies have been employed to circumvent this problem. Some studies have

excluded words with bound roots from consideration altogether (e.g. Hay 2001), others have

assigned bound roots a frequency of zero and added a small constant to all frequencies, to enable

logarithmic tranformations of items with a raw frequency of zero (e.g. Baayen, Feldman, and

Schreuder 2006, 294). However, this solution is somewhat unsatisfactory, in that many bound

roots appear in multiple words, whose frequency may well affect the target word’s

decomposability.

For the current study, we developed a string-based measure of base frequency usable for

bound roots and free bases equally. The string-based base frequency is the cumulative frequency

of all words containing the sequence of letters formed by removing the target suffix from a target

word, e.g. in the case of possible or readable the frequency of all words beginning with poss or

read, respectively. The resulting estimates of base frequency are by necessity noisy, due to

accidental overlap in letter strings. For example, the base frequency of possible includes the

frequency of possum by this metric. We accept the noisiness of the string-based measure of

relative frequency, considering it an empirical issue whether it would converge with other

variables targeting boundary strength. To see whether this was the case, we checked the

relationship between base frequency and base type: If the measure succeeded in recovering

effects of boundary strength, then estimated base frequencies should be higher for bases that are

words (e.g. read) than for bases that are bound roots (e.g. poss-), other things being equal. That

expectation was partially supported: For the words with -ible, -able, base frequency was

significantly higher for free bases than bound roots (W = 4.42175 × 104 , p < .0001). For words

with -ment, however, base frequencies of words with free bases vs. bound roots did not differ

significantly. We did not perform a similar check for the words with -ance and -ence because

free bases were almost entirely absent from that set.

Page 21: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

21

We entered Base frequency and Target frequency as separate predictors in our models, instead

of a variable coding the relative (base-to-derive) frequencies. One reason for this decision was

that we did not expect an additional effect of relative frequency on top of base frequency and

target frequency (Plag & Baayen, 2009). A second reason is that relative frequency is computed

as the quotient of base frequency and whole-word frequency and is therefore not independent of

either, which renders using relative frequency together with base frequency or word frequency

(or both) in one regression model problematic. A third reason is conceptual: Whereas we were

interested in Base frequency as a measure of boundary strength, we expected that Target

frequency would play a role not only because of its relationship to boundary strength, but also

because of its role in determining sample size: Misspellings of words with high frequency are

ipso facto more likely to be attested in our sample, regardless of boundary strength or any other

morphological property. We therefore wished to treat Base frequency and Target frequency as

two separate variables in our models, so as to examine the sampling effect and any effects of

morphological boundary strength separately.

Results

Properties of the Target Suffixes and Target Words

We selected the affixes discussed here in the expectation that -ible vs. -able differed in

boundary strength, that -ence vs. -ance did not, and that -ment was associated with fairly strong

boundaries. We therefore begin our analyses by determining whether that expectation was borne

out in our sample.

With respect to the distribution of base types, i.e. words whose bases are roots vs. free bases,

our target suffixes patterned as expected, as shown in Table 4: Bound roots account for only

about one fifth (111 out of 500) of the words with -able, but about one half (46 out of 96) of the

Page 22: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

22

words with -ible, in line with the expectation that -able words should be more likely to contain

strong boundaries than -ible words. Also in line with our expectations, -ance and -ence did not

differ with respect to base type: both attach almost exclusively to bound roots. Finally, only one

seventh (44 out of 254) of the words -ment contained bound roots, confirming that this suffix

tends to be associated with strong morphological boundaries.

=========== Place Table 4 About Here ==========================

We also compared the relative (i.e. base-to-derived) frequencies associated with these

suffixes: If our string-based measure of base frequency is valid, one would expect the relative

(i.e. base-to-derived) frequency for -able to be higher than for -ible. That was indeed the case

(t(140) = 2.928, p = 0.002), adding support to the assumption that -able was indeed associated

with stronger morphological boundaries than -ible. The suffixes -ance and ence, on the other

hand, did not differ in base-to-derived frequency ((t(66.0374) = -0.316, p = 0.6237)), consistent

with our assumption.

Descriptive statistics for the continuous variables in our models are shown in Table 5. Tables

6 through 8 show the pairwise (Spearman) correlations among the numeric variables in our

models.

=========== Place Table 5 About Here ==========================

=========== Place Table 6 About Here ==========================

=========== Place Table 7 About Here ==========================

=========== Place Table 8 About Here ==========================

Page 23: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

23

Modeling Results

-ible/-able.

The model for -able/-ible after stepwise elimination of non-significant predictors is

summarized in Table 9. There was a significant effect of Suffix, reflecting the fact that words

with -ible were more liable to be misspelled than words with -able (β = 3.892, p < .0001). There

was also a significant effect of Base complexity, indicating that words with complex bases were

less likely to be misspelled (β = 0.003, p < .0001). The effect of Variant bigram indicates that

misspellings were increasingly likely to be found with increasing probability of the bigram at the

suffix boundary (β = 11.133, p < .0001). Increasing target word frequency was associated with

greater likelihood of the spelling variant being attested (β = 0.664, p < .0001), and so was

increasing base frequency (β = -0.151, p = 0.0017). Finally, there was a significant interaction of

Base frequency with Target frequency (β = 0.081, p = 0.0013). None of the interactions of Suffix

with any other variable reached significance.

=========== Place Table 9 About Here ==========================

There were 111 word types with -ible/-able with at least 6 spelling variants in our sample. The

pattern of results in the model using that higher threshold as the outcome variable (summarized

in Table 10) was similar to the previous model, with two differences: First, in the model with the

higher threshold, there was a marginally significant interaction of Suffix with Base frequency,

indicating that words with -ible were less likely to be misspelled with increasing base frequency.

Secondly, the interaction of Base frequency with Target frequency was marginally significant (β

= 0.054, p = 0.0643), and the simple effect of Base frequency was non-significant.

Page 24: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

24

=========== Place Table 10 About Here ==========================

The interaction of Base frequency with Target frequency is plotted in Figure 1, which shows

predictions and confidence bands of Base frequency for ten percentile ranges of Target

frequency. The effect of Base frequency (plotted along the x-axis) and the outcome variable (i.e.

presence of at least one misspelling) varied across frequency bands: For target words of low to

medium frequency, increasing base frequency was associated with fewer errors, consistent with

the idea that higher segmentability was associated with fewer misspellings. The effect of Base

frequency was attenuated with increasing Target frequency and was reversed in the two highest

percentile ranges of Target frequency, in which increasing Base frequency was associated with

greater numbers of misspellings.

=========== Place Figure 1 About Here ==========================

Finally, as a test of the validity of Base frequency, we compared the behavior of this variable

to that of a traditional estimate of base frequency in models of the subset of target words with

free bases. Recall that traditional estimates only apply to free bases, not to bound roots. If the

two variables tap into the same underlying property, they should have similar effects when

applied to words with free bases. This was the case. We interpret this as a strong indication that

our string-based measure of base frequency taps into the same underlying property as traditional

measures applicable only to free bases.

-ance/-ence.

The set of words with -ance/-ence included very few words with complex bases (only 7 for

the suffix -ence), so we refrained from entering Base complexity in the model. Including Length

also turned out to be problematic because of the presence of five very long words (of 13 or more

Page 25: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

25

letters). In a preliminary model, there was a significant effect of Length, which disappeared after

exclusion of the extremely long words. Here, we document the models without Length as a

predictor, but including the five long words.

=========== Place Table 11 About Here ==========================

The model for -ance/-ence, after backward elimination, is summarized in Table 11. As was

the case for -able/-ible, there was a significant effect of Variant bigram (β = 12.02, p = 0.0221),

indicating that misspellings were increasingly likely to be found with increasing probability of

the bigram straddling the suffix boundary. There was also a significant effect of Target frequency

(β = 0.69, p = 0), indicating that misspellings were increasingly likely to be found with

increasing target word frequency. Suffix (-ance vs.-ence) did not yield a significant main effect,

nor did it participate in any significant interactions. In the model using the higher threshold, i.e.

modeling the probability of a spelling variant being found more than five times, Target frequency

was the only significant predictor; as in all other models, increasing target frequency was

associated with an increasing probability of variants being attested the required number of times.

-ment.

The model for -ment is summarized in Table 12. After backwards elimination, there was a

significant effect of Target frequency (β = 0.646, p = 0), indicating that variants were more likely

to be attested with increasing target word frequency. There was also a marginally significant

effect of Base type (β = -0.761, p = 0.064), suggesting that target words with free bases were less

likely to be misspelled. However, the effect of Base type was non-significant in the model using

the higher threshold (not shown here), where only the effect of Target frequency was signicant (β

= 0.462, p < .001). At neither threshold was there an effect of Variant bigram or Base frequency

Page 26: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

26

or an interaction of Target frequency with Base frequency.

=========== Place Table 12 About Here ==========================

Summary of Results

The pattern of results is summarized in Table 13: For all three pairs we considered,

higher lexical frequency of the target word was associated with an increased probability

of the spelling variant being attested, as one would expect for any form: The more

frequently an item occurs, the more likely it is to be attested in any given corpus.

=========== Place Table 13 About Here ==========================

The other variables that were predictive of whether a given target word was misspelled

differed for the three pairs we considered. For the pairs -able, -ible and -ance, -ence, i.e. the

suffixes in which target and variant differed in the initial segment, there was a positive effect of

Variant bigram, such that the higher the variant bigram probability, the higher the probability of

the spelling variant being attested. No other variables besides Target frequency and Variant

bigram were significant for -ence/-ance. In particular, there was no significant effect of Suffix,

meaning that -ence vs. -ance appeared to be about equally likely to be misspelled.

Spelling variants for -ible/-able were less likely to be attested for words with complex bases

and higher base frequency. There was an interaction of Base frequency and Target frequency,

such that increasing Base frequency was associated with decreasing probability of misspelling

for words up to about the 80th percentile for Target frequency. For targets with high lexical

frequency, there was no effect of Base frequency. The suffixes -ible and -able differed from one

Page 27: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

27

another in that misspellings were more likely to be attested for -ible compared to -able, even

after controlling for lexical frequency. There were no significant interactions with Suffix,

meaning that the effects of the predictors did not appear to differ for -ible vs. -able.

For -ment, Target frequency was the only variable that was predictive of whether the spelling

variant with <mint> would be attested at least six times. When the outcome variable reflected

whether a <mint>-variant was attested at all, there was a marginally significant effect of Base

type, i.e. of whether the word contained a bound root vs. a free base, with free bases being

associated with a lower incidence of <mint>-variants.

Discussion

We explored predictors of misspellings such as <comprehensable>, and <avoidence>, in

which the standard spelling of a target suffix is replaced by that of a similar-sounding suffix. Our

general hypothesis was that these spelling variants would show systematic patterns, rather than

occurring at random, and that they would reflect morphological boundary strength, among other

factors. To explore that hypothesis, we examined spelling errors in one pair of suffixes differing

in boundary strength, -ible /able, and one pair of roughly equal boundary strength, -ance/-ence.

We also included the spelling <mint> of the suffix -ment (as in <statemint>) in our analysis, to

ask whether occurrences of <mint> likewise reflected boundary strength. We evaluated two

specific hypotheses, which we termed ’segmentability’ and ’typicality’, about the potential role

of morphological boundary strength in spelling variation. In order to be able to include words

with bound roots in the scope of our investigation, we developed a new, string-based, measure of

base frequency. We validated the measure by comparing its behavior for words with free bases to

more conventional estimates. There were several clear patterns, indicating that these spellings

indeed did not occur randomly, as well as some evidence consistent with the hypothesis that they

reflected morphological boundary strength, among other factors.

Page 28: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

28

Crucial to evaluating our hypothesis were the presence of an interaction between Target

Frequency and Base Frequency for -ible /able, and the absence of such an interaction for -ance/-

ence, which are not thought to differ in boundary strength, and ment, which lacks a competitor in

standard spelling. That interaction is only one of several variables one might wish to consider in

an investigation of morphological boundary strength. We refrained from attempting to include

additional variables reflecting boundary strength: Such additional variables can be expect to

correlate with the base-to-derived frequency, precisely to the extent that they all correlate with -

or reflect, or indeed themselves determine - morphological boundary strength.

Importantly, the overall pattern of (morphological and other) effects does not appear to be the

result of an across-the-board, ’morphology-blind’ default to the most frequent spelling in case of

uncertainty. In the case of -able / -ible, defaulting to <able> would be a reasonable strategy, as

word types with -able far outnumber word types with -ible. Consistent with the ’reasonable

default’ strategy, -ible was more likely to be spelled <able> than the other way around when

lexical frequency was controlled. However, no such default strategy seemed to be at work in the

case of -ance / -ence: Even though there were twice as many word types with -ence as with -ance

in our data set, words with -ance were no more likely to be misspelled than those with -ence

when lexical frequency was controlled. As for -ment, there are of course words ending in <mint>

in standard orthography (e.g. <mint>, <spearmint>, and <varmint> (as a regional variant of

<vermin>), but <mint> in these words does not represent the suffix -ment. Therefore, defaulting

to <ment> offers a perfectly safe strategy - provided writers recognize the ending in question as

representing a suffix at all. The pattern we observed with -ment is consistent with <ment> being

a default spelling – but in a manner that reflects the recognition of morphological structure.

Taken together, our models suggest that writers do not simply default to whichever ending they

have encountered more commonly.

Page 29: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

29

Interestingly, spellings like <spearment> and <pepperment> are by no means rare in print,

judging by an informal search on Google Books (https://books.google.com/). Such spellings may

reflect a kind of ’folk morphology’, with speakers treating spearmint as though it contained a

suffix. Conversely, forms like <governmint, adjournmint, ailmint>, and <settlemint> might

reflect a ’folk suffix’ -mint competing with the suffix spelled <ment> in standard orthography.

On that reading, -mint might be considered a ’weak boundary counterpart’ of -ment, analogous to

the relationship between -ible and -able. In any case, the case of -ment strongly suggests that

suffixal spellings are not due to a simple surface default strategy, but reflect morphological

structure. Our regression models take into account several factors that appear to be at play in

suffixal spelling variation.

Typicality vs. Segmentability

The modeling results allow us to evaluate two specific hypotheses about the role of

morphological boundaries in spelling variation. On the first, the Typicality hypothesis, spelling

variants should be more likely whenever the variant is expected, given the properties of the target

word and suffix. For example, Typicality would favor spellings like <availible> and

<suggestable>: The word available is more frequent than its base avail, which is typical for

words with -ible, and suggestible is less frequent than its base, which is typical for words with

able. The standard spellings <available> and <suggestible> are therefore somewhat unexpected.

On the Typicality hypothesis, one would expect, among other things, that words with free bases

should be less likely to be misspelled if the standard spelling is <able> vs. <ible>, but more

likely to be misspelled if the standard spelling is <ible>. More generally, one would expect

interactions of Suffix with other predictors in our model of -ible/-able. That was not the case,

meaning that there was no support for the Typicality hypothesis in our models.

Page 30: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

30

On the Segmentability hypothesis, on the other hand, there should be fewer misspellings with

increasing strength of morphological boundaries, regardless of the target suffix. There was partial

support for this hypothesis: Higher Base frequency andBase type (free as against bound roots)

were associated with decreased probability of attested spelling variants for -ible/-able and -ment,

respectively. In the case of -ance/-ence, we did not observe effects of Base frequency or Base

type. The fact that the dataset for -ance/-ence was smaller than for -ible/-able, may explain the

absence of significant effects, but there are also several other complicating factors.

Before considering these additional factors more closely, we note that the Segmentability

hypothesis is consistent with evidence from several strands of previous research: For example,

the ability to identify derivational morphemes is associated with better spelling performance in

children (see e.g. Carlisle, 1988; Singson, Mahony, & Mann, 2000), as well as high school and

college-age students (Mahony, 1994). In addition, complex words have sometimes been found to

be better preserved than morphologically simple ones in individuals with neuropsychological

impairments (Rapp & Fischer-Baum, 2014). Badecker, Hillis, and Caramazza (1990), for

example, found individuals with dysgraphia to be more successful at producing word-final letters

immediately preceded by a morpheme boundary than those not immediately preceded by a

morpheme boundary. These observations suggest that, sometimes, morphologically complex

words have a processing advantage over morphologically simple ones. That processing

advantage may in turn help explain the ’spelling advantage’ of complex words, i.e. the

segmentability effect.

On the other hand, there is also evidence that the presence of morpheme boundaries may

make spelling errors more, not less, likely in some cases: In a study of a frequent spelling error in

Hebrew, involving the insertion of a character representing a vowel in certain types of nouns,

Bar-On and Kuperman (2018, in press) found the locus of the insertion to be sensitive to

Page 31: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

31

morphological structure. Among other patterns, Bar-On and Kuperman (2018, in press) found

insertions to have a strong tendency to occur immediately preceding a suffix. While this result,

like the previous ones, shows that morphology influences spelling variation, the presence of a

morphological boundary in the Hebrew case is associated with an increased chance of error,

unlike what we saw in the present study. It is difficult to know whether this difference is due to

the non-concatenative nature of Hebrew morphology, different phoneme-grapheme mapping for

vowels vs. consonants, or some other difference either in the structure of Hebrew vs. German and

English, or in the tasks and methods employed.

Our results might also appear to run counter to another published finding. Schmitz et al.

(2018, p.111) report that there were "fewer errors for more frequent word forms" in a corpus of

17,432 tweets containing 1,185 misspelled forms. However, the frequency in question, according

to the discussion of the regression models in Schmitz et al. (2018), pertains to the relative

frequency of two homophonous forms, not of the absolute frequency of either form.

The observed direction of the effects of segmentability on spelling is far from inevitable, even

in a language like English. We also considered the alternative possibility that high segmentability

should be associated with increased spelling difficulty, due to a paradigmatic consequence of

segmentability: Recognizing the morpheme boundary in a word like available makes that word

both easier and more difficult to spell. It makes it easier in that it privileges two options

(<availible> and <available>) from the much larger set of possibilities that includes

<availabble>, <availeble>, and <availibbel>. On the other hand, writers must now make a choice

between <able> and <ible>, both of which represent common affixes. Paradigmatic competition

has been demonstrated to affect pronunciation variation (Cohen, 2014; Kuperman, Pluymaekers,

Ernestus, & Baayen, 2007), and it seems plausible that it might also affect spelling variation.

Specifically, competition might be particularly strong in highly segmentable words and might

Page 32: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

32

make such words difficult spelling targets. That is the opposite of what we observed here.

However, competition as discussed in (Cohen, 2014; Kuperman et al., 2007) depends on several

other factors (such as morphological family size). We consider the relationship between

segmentability and competition to be an avenue worth exploring in future research.

Several patterns in our models, and several other variables that may have affected our results,

merit closer inspection. In particular, we discuss here the effects of Base complexity, and Target

bigram, i.e. the probability of the initial letter of the target suffix, given the final letter of the

base, a variable that we believe reflects several distinct properties of letters, words, and sounds.

Base Complexity

We suspected that morphological complexity of the base might affect spelling behavior, such

that forms in which the target suffix follows a morphologically complex form might be easier to

spell than forms in which the suffix attaches to a monomorphemic base. It will be observed that

we are using the term ’base’ somewhat loosely here: We suspected that complexity would play a

role even in words like un-seasonable or un-sinkable, i.e. words in which the material preceding

the suffix would not be considered the morphological base of the target suffix. There was partial

support for this idea. There was no effect of base complexity for -ence, -ance or -ment, possibly

because Base complexity is not independent of other variables (a fact that informed variable

selection for each set of models): For -ment, for example, only six target words with bound roots

contained complex bases. In the model of -ible and -able, however, complex bases were

associated with fewer errors. Recall that -ible vs. -able did not differ from one another with

respect to base complexity. Therefore, it appears unlikely that the effect of Base complexity was

actually an effect of Suffix in disguise. Instead, we believe that base complexity promotes

segmentability.

Page 33: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

33

Why should base complexity be associated with higher segmentability? Recall that in

multiply affixed words weak morphological boundaries tend to occur inside of strong boundaries

(Hay & Baayen, 2002; Hay & Plag, 2004; Plag & Baayen, 2009; Zirkel, 2010). Our target words

fall into two classes: Those in which another suffix precedes the target suffix (e.g. real-ize-able,

diagonal-ize-able, class-ify-able) and those in which the target suffix is the only suffix, but which

contain prefixes. In the former case, our target suffix has a stronger boundary than the preceding

suffix. We are not aware of any studies that have tested the segmentability of prefix-suffix

combinations. A priori, however, we note that there are two possible bracketings [Prefix-Base]-

Suffix, and

Prefix-[Base-Suffix] (though in many words, the bracketing is ambiguous). Given the parse

[Prefix-Base]-Suffix, the same reasoning applies as with doubly suffixed bases: the target suffix

has the strongest boundary that is present in the word. Only with the parse Prefix-[Base-Suffix]

would the target suffix have a weaker boundary than the other affix present in the word. Thus,

based on considerations of complexity-based ordering, in two out of three affix-configurations

we would expect an enhanced tendency for segmentation for the target suffix.

To our knowledge, previous literature has been silent on effects of base complexity on

behavioral measures such as lexical decision or reading times: There is a copious literature on

affix ordering and other combinatorial properties of affixes, but far less information seems to be

available on the effects of multiple affixation on recognition, reading, or writing. Processing

effects of morphological boundaries have so far primarily been studied in words containing only

one derivational affix. We believe that the effect of Base complexity underscores the need to

study how multiply affixed words are processed.

Variant Bigram Probability

Page 34: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

34

Turning now to the variant bigram probability, we take the effects of this variable to reflect at

least three sets of factors: The first is that high-bigram-probability errors may ‘look right’,

making errors harder to detect. The second is that the process of typing (or thumbing) may be

routinized to a higher degree for high-probability bigrams than low-probability ones, making

errors harder to avoid. The third set of factors concerns pronunciation: Some spelling variants,

e.g. <legable>, <allocible>, and <diligance>, invite pronunciations that differ from those of their

intended targets, e.g. <legible>, <revocable>, and <diligence>. In fact, we believe that many

misspellings represent what one might term ‘pronunciation spellings’, a converse of ‘spelling

pronunciations’. While the latter is an accepted term for non-standard pronunciations based on

standard spellings (e.g. [maIzld] for <misled>), ‘pronunciation spellings’ are non-standard

spellings based on (standard or non-standard) pronunciation. We avoid the more familiar term

‘phonetic spelling’ here, as that term is typically applied in discussion of learning and

development. The Tweets analyzed here do not generally give us the impression that their

authors were in the process of learning to write - or to spell, for that matter, despite the

occasional non-standard spelling. The properties of letter combinations like <gi>, <ge> or <ci>

vs. <ga> or <ca>, and their different pronunciations, serve as a reminder that the bigram

probabilities in our data are not truly gradient - particularly because the set of base-final letters

that occur with a given suffix is quite restricted in some cases. Before considering this point

further, we wish to draw attention to a related issue, concerning the bigrams present in the

standard spelling.

Target Bigram Probability

It is tempting to think that bigram probability of the target may index boundary strength

associated with our target suffixes, consistent with previous work on transitional probabilities in

speech perception (Hay, 2002, and references therein), but we do not believe that the effects we

Page 35: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

35

observed should be attributed directly to these distributional properties. Instead, effects of target

bigram probability likely arise for reasons that are analogous to those mentioned in connection

with variant bigram probabilities. An additional complicating factors in the interpretation of

target bigram probability is that the range of letters that may precede the standard spelling of a

given suffix may be quite restricted: For example, <ible> only follows 9 distinct letters in our

dataset, whereas <able> follows 22 distinct letters. We leave it to future research to determine

the extent to which such regularities affect spelling in cases where a writer is uncertain of the

standard spellling.

Setting aside the underlying mechanisms, the effect of Variant bigram may informally be

described as reflecting whether spelling variants ‘look wrong’. The question then arises how the

variants compare to their orthographically correct cousins in this regard: If standard and variant

both ‘look right’ (or wrong), that fact might increase spelling uncertainty. Put differently, the

question is whether using a non-standard spelling, makes things better or worse. To address that

question, we added Target bigram as a predictor to the final model of -ible,-able spelling, along

with an interaction of Target bigram and Variant bigram. The strong correlation between the two

bigram variables (Spearman’s rho = 0.31, p < .001) means that the model estimates should be

taken with some caution. We nevertheless included this model, as a preliminary check of the

possibility just mentioned, of the effect of variant bigram being modulated by that of the target

bigram.

=========== Place Table 14 About Here ==========================

We fitted the model following the same procedure as before, i.e. starting with a model

containing all predictors and using stepwise backward elimination. The resulting model is

summarized in Table 14. Collinearity within the model appeared to be acceptably low: The

Page 36: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

36

highest (generalized) variance inflation factor, of 1.9556271, was associated with the variant

bigram probability, but was sufficiently low so as to not cause concern. There was a significant

interaction of the two bigram probabilities (β = -184.525, p = 0.001). The interaction is

visualized in Figure 2 for three sections of target bigram probability. As can be seen in the plot,

the positive effect of variant bigram probability was strongest for words with low target bigram

probabilities, attenuated for words with medium-range target bigram probabilities, and possibly

reversed for high target bigram probabilities; the large confidence interval in the highest

frequency range renders that last point inconclusive. This pattern suggests that high variant

bigram probability can interfere with correct spelling, except in words with high target bigram

probability. Stated informally, when standard spellings ‘look right’ to begin with, writers are less

likely to deviate from the standard spelling. We further asked whether low (target) bigram

probabilities tended to occur in infrequent words; if so, then the vulnerable state of low-

probability target spellings might be a word frequency effect in disguise. That was not the case

(Spearman’s rho = -0.01, n.s.). We refrained from entering both bigram probabilities into a

model of -ance/-ence: Given the small number of word types for each bigram, such a model

would almost certainly be overfitted.

=========== Place Figure 2 About Here ==========================

The Role of Boundary Strength in Spelling Variation

Our general hypothesis was that not only the presence, but also the strength of morphological

boundaries would affect misspellings. One piece of evidence for this that we have not yet

discussed concerns the differences among the spelling targets considered here: Not only are the

suffixes -ible and -able difficult spelling targets, they also differ from one another in difficulty,

unlike -ence/-ance. The direction of difference is consistent with the notion that stronger

Page 37: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

37

morphological boundaries facilitate standard spellings. By contrast, we did not find any evidence

for suffix-specific effects in words with -ence vs. -ance, which are also difficult spelling targets,

but do not differ in boundary strength. We interpret this pattern as an indication that the

differences in the behavior of -able/ible vs. -ence/-ance are indeed related to the difference in

boundary strength in -able/ible , and the absence of such a difference in -ence/-ance. It remains

to be seen whether this interpretation is correct: Our set of words with -ance/-ence was far

smaller than the set of words with ible/able (n = 118 vs. n = 596, respectively), which may

explain the absence of significant effects. The marginally significant effect of Base Type on the

spelling of -ment may be another instance of an effect of boundary strength.

Our findings tie in with several strands of previous research on boundary strength. Nottbusch,

Grimm, Weingarten, and Will (2005); Sahel et al. (2008) and Weingarten, Nottbusch, and Will

(2004), for example, found that inter-keystroke intervals in typed productions of German noun

compounds reflected lexical frequency, head frequency, semantic transparency, and relative

(base-to-derived) frequency, i.e. variables that are associated with gradient morphological

boundaries. The more general finding of misspellings reflecting morphological properties of

words meshes well with research on inflectional affixes, specifically the work of Sandra (2010);

Sandra and Fayol (2003); Sandra, Frisson, and Daems (1999, 2004); Schmitz et al. (2018) on

patterns of misspellings in homophonous Dutch inflectional suffixes.

Limitations

We wished to focus specifically on effects of segmentability on legitimate suffixes, i.e.

spellings representing suffixes in standard orthography. A corollary of our hypothesis that the

lower segmentability of words in -ible vs. -able should make non-suffixal spellings of -ible

words (e.g. <legibbel> or <plauzebble>) more likely than those of -able words (e.g.

Page 38: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

38

<washebble> or <portebell>. Broadening the current line of investigation to other spelling

variants may also help clarify the extent to which if misspellings of suffixed words are due to

morphological structure vs. factors like keyboard layout or screen responsiveness. The current

study presents a case study, comparing a single pair of suffixes differing in boundary strength to

a single other pair that does not, and to a single other suffix without a competitor. To properly

evaluate the hypotheses we considered, the investigation has to extend to more affixes.

Another set of limitations has to do with the way Tweets are produced. The main advantage of

Twitter as a source of data lies in the diversity of topics covered and in the fact that tweets are

not generally subject to editorial review. However, Twitter data have several drawbacks: For

example, some Twitter users may be using auto-complete editors or spell checkers, both of

which may filter out many patterns one might observe in uncorrected spelling behavior.

Secondly, the mechanics of typing or swiping words differ for different input devices, meaning

that different letters are adjacent to one another or conveniently reached on keyboards. Thirdly,

Twitter users include native speakers of such as French or German, where cognates of English -

ible/-able are phonetically clearly distinct from one another, considerably reducing the risk of

substituting their written forms for one another.

Conclusion

Misspellings might not seem to be of particular interest to research on the mental lexicon

because many orthographic errors are no doubt attributable to the physical environments of

typing and hand-writing, such as keyboard layout, touchscreen responsiveness, tactile properties

of keyboards, touchscreens, and pens (Crump & Logan, 2010; Deorowicz & Ciura, 2005).

However, there is a substantial body of research demonstrating effects of typing and handwriting

on stages of language production that precede planning and execution of motor movements

Page 39: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

39

(Delattre, Bonin, & Barry, 2006; Lambert, Kandel, Fayol, & Espéret, 2008; Roux, McKeeff,

Grosjacques, Afonso, & Kandel, 2013; Scaltritti, Arfé, Torrance, & Peressotti, 2016).

Morphological boundaries - and syllable boundaries - in particular have been the focus of several

studies demonstrating that such boundaries affect hand writing and typing (Baus, Strijkers, &

Costa, 2013; Bertram, Tønnessen, Strömqvist, Hyönä, & Niemi, 2015; Nottbusch et al., 2005;

Roux et al., 2013; Sahel et al., 2008; Weingarten et al., 2004). There is also previous research

arguing for misspellings of inflected forms as reflecting morphological representations (e.g.

Sandra & Fayol, 2003; Sandra et al., 2004). We have argued that misspellings in English

derivational suffixes similarly reflect lexical structure, in addition to the mechanics of keyboards

or pens, in much the same way that pronunciation variation reflects lexical processing along with

articulatory and acoustic aspects of speech production. In light of previous findings on the effects

of spelling variability (both orthographically licensed and nonstandard variation) on reading

(Falkauskas & Kuperman, 2015; Rahmanian & Kuperman, 2019), the case studies we presented

here underscore the value of misspellings as a tool for understanding the processes underlying

both writing and reading.

Page 40: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

40

References

Adams, V. (2001). Complex words in English. Longman.

Assink, E. M. (1985). Assessing spelling strategies for the orthography of dutch verbs.

British Journal of Psychology, 76(3), 353–363.

Baayen, R. H. (2014). Experimental and psycholinguistic approaches to studying

derivation. Handbook of derivational morphology, 95–117.

Baayen, R. H., Feldman, L. B., & Schreuder, R. (2006). Morphological influences on the

recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and

Language, 55(2), 290–313.

Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The celex lexical database

(release 2). Distributed by the Linguistic Data Consortium, University of

Pennsylvania.

Badecker, W., Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1990). Lexical morphology and its role in

the writing process: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition,

35(3), 205–243.

Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., . . .

Treiman, R. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods,

39(3), 445–459. doi: 10.3758/bf03193014

Bar-On, A., & Kuperman, V. (2018, in press). Spelling errors respect morphology: a

corpus study of Hebrew orthography. Reading and Writing.

Bauer, L., Lieber, R., & Plag, I. (2013). The Oxford reference guide to English

morphology. Oxford University Press. Retrieved from

Page 41: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

41

https://doi.org/10.1093%2Facprof%3Aoso%2F9780198747062.001.0001 doi:

10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198747062.001.0001

Baus, C., Strijkers, K., & Costa, A. (2013). When does word frequency influence written

production? Frontiers in Psychology, 4. Retrieved from

https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2013.00963 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00963

Bertram, R., Tønnessen, F. E., Strömqvist, S., Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P. (2015). Cascaded

processing in written compound word production. Frontiers in H uman

Neuroscience, 9 , 207.

Bloomer, R. H. (1956). Word length and complexity variables in spelling difficulty. The

Journal of Educational Research, 49 (7), 531–536.

Blumenthal-Dramé, A., Glauche, V., Bormann, T., Weiller, C., Musso, M., & Kortmann,

B. (2017). Frequency and chunking in derived words: a parametric fmri study.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond kučera and francis: A critical

evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and

improved word frequency measure for american english. Behavior Research

Methods, 41 (4), 977–990.

Cahen, L. S., Craun, M. J., & Johnson, S. K. (1971). Spelling difficultyâĂŤa survey of

the research. Review of Educational Research, 41 (4), 281–301.

Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. (1987). The role of the graphemic

buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 26 (1),

59–85.

Page 42: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

42

Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in

fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9 (3), 247âĂŞ266.

doi: 10.1017/S0142716400007839

Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English.

Cohen, C. (2014). Probabilistic reduction and probabilistic enhancement. Morphology,

24 (4), 291–323. doi: 10.1007/s11525-014-9243-y

Crump, M. J. C., & Logan, G. D. (2010). Warning: This keyboard will deconstruct— the

role of the keyboard in skilled typewriting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17

(3), 394–399. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fpbr.17.3.394 doi:

10.3758/pbr.17.3.394

Cutler, A. (2011). Slips of the tongue and language production. Walter de Gruyter.

Davies, M. (2013). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (full text on

CD): 440 million words, 1990-2012.

Delattre, M., Bonin, P., & Barry, C. (2006). Written spelling to dictation:

Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1330.

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production.

Psychological review, 93(3), 283.

Deorowicz, S., & Ciura, M. G. (2005). Correcting spelling errors by modelling their

causes. International Journal of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, 15,

275–285.

Dressler, W. (1985). Morphonology. Ann Arbor: Karoma.

Page 43: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

43

Falkauskas, K., & Kuperman, V. (2015). When experience meets language statistics:

Individual variability in processing english compound words. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1607.

Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P. (1994). Cognitive overload and orthographic errors:

When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors. a study in

french written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,

47(2), 437–464.

Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2014). Typing time as an index of morphological and

semantic effects during english compound processing. Lingue e linguaggio, 13(2),

241–262.

Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2016a). Effects of morphology and semantic

transparency on typing latencies in english compound and pseudocompound

words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and

Cognition, 42(9), 1489.

Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2016b). Written production of english compounds:

effects of morphology and semantic transparency. Morphology, 26(2), 133–155.

Gentry, J. (2015). twitter: R based twitter client [Computer software manual].

Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=twitteR (R package

version 1.1.9)

Hay, J. (2001). Lexical frequency in morphology: is everything relative? Linguistics, 39

(6), 1041–1070.

Hay, J. (2002). From speech perception to morphology: Affix ordering revisited.

Language, 78 (3), 527–555. doi: 10.1353/lan.2002.0159

Page 44: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

44

Hay, J. (2003). Causes and Consequences of Word Structure. New York: Routledge.

Retrieved from \url{https://doi.org/10.4324%2F9780203495131} doi:

10.4324/9780203495131

Hay, J. (2007). The phonetics of ‘un’. Lexical creativity, texts and contexts, 39–57.

Hay, J., & Baayen, H. (2002). Parsing and productivity. In Yearbook of Morphology

(pp. 203–235). Springer Netherlands. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-3726-5_8

Hay, J., & Baayen, H. (2005). Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (7), 342–348. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.002

Hay, J., & Plag, I. (2004). What constrains possible suffix combinations? On the

interaction of grammatical and processing restrictions in derivational morphology.

Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 22 (3), 565–596.

Kawaletz, L., & Plag, I. (2015). Predicting the semantics of English nominalizations: a

frame-based analysis of -ment suffixation. In L. Bauer, P. Stekauer, & L.

Kortvelyessy (Eds.), Semantics of Complex Words (pp. 289–319). Dordrecht:

Springer.

Kemps, R. J. J. K., Ernestus, M., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Prosodic

cues for morphological complexity: The case of Dutch plural nouns. Memory

& Cognition, 33 (3), 430–446. doi: 10.3758/bf03193061

Kiparsky, P. (1982). Lexical morphology and phonology. In I.-S. Yang (Ed.), Linguistics

in the Morning Calm: Selected Papers from SICOL (pp. 3–91). Seoul: Hanshin.

Kuperman, V., & Bertram, R. (2013). Moving spaces: Spelling alternation in english

noun-noun compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28 (7), 939–966.

Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01690965.2012.701757 doi:

Page 45: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

45

10.1080/01690965.2012.701757

Kuperman, V., Pluymaekers, M., Ernestus, M., & Baayen, H. (2007). Morphological

predictability and acoustic duration of interfixes in dutch compounds. The Journal

of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(4), 2261–2271.

Lambert, E., Kandel, S., Fayol, M., & Espéret, E. (2008). The effect of the number of

syllables on handwriting production. Reading and Writing, 21(9), 859–883.

Largy, P. (1996). The homophone effect in written french: The case of verb-noun

inflection errors. Language and cognitive processes, 11(3), 217–256.

Lee-Kim, S.-I., Davidson, L., & Hwang, S. (2013). Morphological effects on the darkness

of English intervocalic /l/. Laboratory Phonology, 4(2), 475–511.

Libben, G., Jarema, G., Luke, J., & Bork, P. (September 25 – 28, 2018). Same words,

different languages: Examining English-French word recognition and production.

Edmonton.

Libben, G., & Weber, S. (2014). Semantic transparency, compounding, and the nature

of independent variables. In F. Rainer, F. Gardani, H. C. Luschützky, & W. U.

Dressler (Eds.), Morphology and Meaning (pp. 205–221). Amsterdam /

Philadelphia: Benjamins.

Libben, G., Weber, S., & Miwa, K. (2012). P3: A technique for the study of perception,

production, and participant properties. The Mental Lexicon, 7(2), 237–248.

Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high

school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 6(1), 19–44.

Marchand, H. (1969). The categories and types of present-day English word-formation

Page 46: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

46

(2nd ed.). München: Verlag C. H. Beck.

Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., Weingarten, R., & Will, U. (2005). Syllabic sructures in

typing: Evidence from deaf writers. Reading and Writing, 18(6), 497–526.

Plag, I. (2003). Word-formation in english. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved

from https://doi.org/10.1017%2Fcbo9780511841323 doi:

10.1017/cbo9780511841323

Plag, I. (2014). Phonological and phonetic variability in complex words: An uncharted

territory. Italian Journal of Linguistics/Rivista di Linguistica, 26(2), 209–228.

Plag, I., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Suffix ordering and morphological processing.

Language, 85 , 106–149.

Plag, I., & Ben Hedia, S. (2018). The phonetics of newly derived words: Testing the

effect of morphological segmentability on affix duration. In S. Arndt-Lappe, A.

Braun, C. Moulin, & E. Winter-Froemel (Eds.), Expanding the Lexicon:

Linguistic Innovation, Morphological Productivity, and the Role of

Discourse-related Factors (pp. 93–116). Berlin, New York: de Gruyter Mouton.

R Development Core Team. (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical

computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from

https://www.R-project.org/ (ISBN 3-900051-07-0)

Rahmanian, S., & Kuperman, V. (2019). Spelling errors impede recognition of

correctly spelled word forms. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23 (1), 24–36.

Rapp, B., & Fischer-Baum, S. (2014). Representation of orthographic knowledge. The

Oxford handbook of language production, 338.

Page 47: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

47

Roux, S., McKeeff, T. J., Grosjacques, G., Afonso, O., & Kandel, S. (2013). The

interaction between central and peripheral processes in handwriting production.

Cognition, 127 (2), 235–241.

Sahel, S., Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., & Weingarten, R. (2008). Written production of

german compounds: Effects of lexical frequency and semantic transparency.

Written Language & Literacy, 11 (2), 211–227.

Sandra, D. (2010). Homophone dominance at the whole-word and sub-word levels:

Spelling errors suggest full-form storage of regularly inflected verb forms.

Language and speech, 53 (3), 405–444.

Sandra, D., & Fayol, M. (2003). Spelling errors with a view on the mental lexicon:

Frequency and proximity effects in misspelling homophonous regular verb forms in

dutch and french. TRENDS IN LINGUISTICS STUDIES AND MONOGRAPHS,

151 , 485–514.

Sandra, D., Frisson, S., & Daems, F. (1999). Why simple verb forms can be so difficult

to spell: The influence of homophone frequency and distance in dutch. Brain

and language, 68 (1-2), 277–283.

Sandra, D., Frisson, S., & Daems, F. (2004). Still errors after all those years...: Limited

attentional resources and homophone frequency account for spelling errors on

silent verb suffixes in dutch. Written Language & Literacy, 7 (1), 61–77.

Scaltritti, M., Arfé, B., Torrance, M., & Peressotti, F. (2016). Typing pictures:

Linguistic processing cascades into finger movements. Cognition, 156 , 16–29.

Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2016.07.006

doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.006

Page 48: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

48

Schmitz, T., Chamalaun, R., & Ernestus, M. (2018). The dutch verb-spelling paradox

in social media. Linguistics in the Netherlands, 35 (1), 111–124.

Seyfarth, S., Garellek, M., Gillingham, G., Ackerman, F., & Malouf, R. (2017).

Acoustic differences in morphologically-distinct homophones. Language,

Cognition and Neuroscience, 1–18.

Siegel, D. (1979). Topics in english morphology. Garland.

Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and

morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and writing, 12

(3), 219–252.

Smith, R., Baker, R., & Hawkins, S. (2012). Phonetic detail that distinguishes prefixed

from pseudo-prefixed words. Journal of Phonetics, 40 (5), 689–705. Retrieved from

\url{http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/

S0095447012000356} doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2012.04.002

Solso, R. L., & Juel, C. L. (1980). Positional frequency and versatility of bigrams for

two-through nine-letter english words. Behavior Research Methods, 12 (3), 297–343.

Spencer, K. (2007). Predicting children's word‐spelling difficulty for common English words

from measures of orthographic transparency, phonemic and graphemic length and word

frequency. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 305-338.

Sproat, R., & Fujimura, O. (1993). Allophonic variation in English /l/ and its

implications for phonetic implementation. Journal of Phonetics, 21 , 291–

311.Twitter. (2006). Twitter. Retrieved from \url{https://twitter.com}

Vannest, J., Newport, E. L., Newman, A. J., & Bavelier, D. (2011). Interplay between

morphology and frequency in lexical access: The case of the base frequency effect.

Page 49: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

49

Brain Research, 1373, 144–159.

Weingarten, R., Nottbusch, G., & Will, U. (2004). Morphemes, syllables and graphemes

in written word production. In T. Pechmann & C. Habel (Eds.), Multidisciplinary

approaches to language production (pp. 529–572). Mouton de Gruyter. doi:

10.1515/9783110894028.529

Wikipedia. (2017). Wikipedia:lists of common misspellings — Wikipedia, the free

encyclopedia. Retrieved from \url{https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:

Lists_of_common_misspellings} ([Online; accessed 04 September 2017])

Zirkel, L. (2010). Prefix combinations in English: Structural and processing factors.

Morphology, 20(1), 239–266.

Page 50: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

50

Author note

We are grateful to Victor Kuperman and Sandra Dominiek for their insightful, stimulating, and

constructive comments, as well as to audiences at Berkeley, and at the Spoken Morphology workshops 2016

and 2017 (DFG Research Unit FOR2373). We are very grateful to the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft

for funding parts of this research (Grants PL151/8-1 and PL151/8-2 'Morpho-phonetic Variation in English'

and PL151/7-1 and PL151/7-2 'FOR 2737 Spoken Morphology: Central Project' awarded to Ingo Plag).

Page 51: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

51

Table 1

Characteristics of -able and -ible, based on Adams (2001); Bauer et al. (2013);

Marchand (1969).

Characteristic -able -ible

Base category verbs, phrasal verbs, nouns,

bound roots, compounds

(non-phrasal) verbs,

bound roots

Stratum of bases native, non-native non-native

Stress shifting rare rare

Base allomorphy rare frequent

Productivity high limited

Page 52: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

52

Table 2

Number of distinct word types and total count of misspelled tokens for each of five

target suffixes.

Suffix Types Misspelled tokens

-able 500 4055

-ance 41 1868

-ence 77 5753

-ible 96 10022

-ment 254 1159

Page 53: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

53

Table 3

Number and proportion of misspellings found at least once or 6 times for each of five

suffixes. target

n > 0 n > 5 prop above 0 prop above 5

-ible, -able 194 111 .33 .19

-ment 62 22 .24 .09

-ence, -ance 90 60 .76 .51

Page 54: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

54

Table 4

Categorical properties of target suffixes.

Suffix Word types Bound

roots

Complex bases Misspelled tokens

-able 500 111 189 4055

-ible 96 46 38 10022

-ance 41 39 7 1868

-ence 77 74 17 5753

-ment 254 44 83 1159

Page 55: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

55

Table 5

Median values of numerical properties of target words by suffix.

Suffix Length Target

frequency

Base

frequency

Target

bigram

Variant

bigram

-able 10.00 5.23 8.82 .09 .14

-ance 10.00 6.18 8.04 .09 .20

-ence 10.00 6.15 7.96 .20 .07

-ible 11.00 5.89 8.42 .11 .09

-ment 10.00 6.50 8.73 .03 .03

Page 56: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

56

Table 6

Pairwise (Spearman) correlations of gradient variables for -ible/-able.

Length Target

frequency

Base

frequency

Variant

bigram

Length 1.00 -.08 -.02 -.03

Target frequency -.08 1.00 .02 -.01

Base frequency -.02 .02 1.00 -.03

Variant bigram -.03 -.01 -.03 1.00

Page 57: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

57

Table 7

Pairwise (Spearman) correlations of gradient variables for -ence/-ance.

Length Target

frequency

Base

frequency

Variant

bigram

Length 1.00 -.29 -.11 .16

Target frequency -.29 1.00 .14 -.14

Base frequency -.11 .14 1.00 .05

Variant bigram .16 -.14 .05 1.00

Page 58: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

58

Table 8

Pairwise (Spearman) correlations of gradient variables for -ment.

Length Target

frequency

Base

frequency

Variant

bigram

Length 1.00 .00 -.19 -.03

Target frequency .00 1.00 .13 -.04

Base frequency -.19 .13 1.00 .05

Variant bigram -.03 -.04 .05 1.00

Page 59: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

59

Table 9

Logistic regression model of -ible/-able misspellings.

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) -1.5371 0.1834 -8.38 .0000

Suffix 3.8922 0.3906 9.96 .0000

Base complexity -0.7497 0.2528 -2.97 .0030

Variant bigram 11.1330 1.8481 6.02 .0000

Target frequency 0.6640 0.0758 8.76 .0000

Base frequency -0.1514 0.0483 -3.13 .0017

Target frequency:Base

frequency

0.0812 0.0253 3.21 .0013

Page 60: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

60

Table 10 Logistic regression model of -ible/-able misspellings attested 6 times or more.

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) -2.6852 0.2563 -10.48 .0000

Suffix 3.7902 0.4077 9.30 .0000

Base complexity -0.8909 0.3058 -2.91 .0036

Variant bigram 11.7904 2.5304 4.66 .0000

Target frequency 0.5770 0.0809 7.13 .0000

Base frequency 0.0115 0.0729 0.16 .8747

Target frequency:Base

frequency

0.0539 0.0291 1.85 .0643

Suffix:Base frequency -0.2248 0.1189 -1.89 .0588

Page 61: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

61

Table 11

Logistic regression model of -ence/-ance misspellings.

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) 1.7583 0.3341 5.26 .0000

Variant bigram 12.0204 5.2503 2.29 .0221

Target frequency 0.6904 0.1569 4.40 .0000

Page 62: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

62

Table 12

Logistic regression model of ment misspellings.

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) -0.9480 0.3665 -2.59 .0097

Base type -0.7608 0.4108 -1.85 .0640

Target frequency 0.6459 0.1011 6.39 .0000

Page 63: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

63

Table 13

Summary of results (threshold > 1). ‘yes’ represents a significant effect (p < 0.05, or

smaller), ‘(yes)’ represents a marginally significant effect.

type of effect variable -able/-ible -ance/-ence -ment/-mint

morphology Suffix yes

Base type (yes)

Base

complexity

yes

Base frequency yes

sampling Target

frequency

yes yes yes

orthography Variant bigram yes yes

(not applicable) Length

Page 64: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

64

Table 14 Logistic regression model of –ible/-able misspellings, taking into account target and

variant bigram probabilities.

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) -1.3510 0.1941 -6.96 .0000

Suffix 3.3789 0.4462 7.57 .0000

Base complexity -0.7832 0.2571 -3.05 .0023

Target bigram -3.0493 2.8305 -1.08 .2813

Variant bigram 6.6203 2.3937 2.77 .0057

Target frequency 0.6806 0.0781 8.71 .0000

Base frequency -0.1767 0.0502 -3.52 .0004

Target bigram:Variant bigram -184.5247 56.8764 -3.24 .0012

Target frequency:Base

frequency

0.0819 0.0263 3.12 .018

Page 65: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength

Figure 1. The interaction of Base frequency and Target frequency: Effect of Base frequency

on <ible>/<able> variation for ten quantiles of target frequency.

Figure 2. Effect of variant bigram probability (see text) on spelling variation in -ible/-able

words for three target bigram probability bands, from lowest (leftmost panel) to highest

(rightmost).

Page 66: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength
Page 67: ffixes reflect morphological boundary strength