Top Banner

of 20

Wilson & Bai (2010)

Apr 03, 2018

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    1/20

    The relationships and impact of teachers metacognitive

    knowledge and pedagogical understandingsof metacognition

    Nance S. Wilson & Haiyan Bai

    Received: 18 December 2008 / Accepted: 28 September 2010# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

    Abstract We know that metacognitive students are successful in school (Sternberg

    Instructional Science 26:127140, 1998). However, despite the recognition of the role of

    metacognition in student success, limited research has been done to explore teachers

    explicit awareness of their metacognition and their ability to think about, talk about, and

    write about their thinking (Zohar Teaching and Teacher Education 15:413-429, 1999).

    Therefore, the current study investigates teachers understanding of metacognition and their

    pedagogical understanding of metacognition, and the nature of what it means to teachstudents to be metacognitive. One hundred-five graduate students in education participated

    in this study. The data analysis results, using mixed research method, suggest that the

    participants metacognitive knowledge had a significant impact on his/her pedagogical

    understanding of metacognition. The results revealed that teachers who have a rich

    understanding of metacognition report that teaching students to be metacognitive requires a

    complex understanding of both the concept of metacognition and metacognitive thinking

    strategies.

    Keywords Metacognition . Metacognitive thinking . Teachers . Instruction . Learning .

    Pedagogical understanding

    The demands of the twenty-first century require students to know more than content

    knowledge; they must know how to learn. Learning is an active process that requires

    students to think about their thinking, or be metacognitive. Metacognition is a persons

    knowledge about the cognitive processes necessary for understanding and learning (Flavell,

    Metacognition Learning

    DOI 10.1007/s11409-010-9062-4

    N. S. Wilson (*)

    School of Teaching, Learning and Leadership, University of Central Florida, 4000 Central Florida Blvd.,P. O. Box 161250, Orlando, FL 32816-1250, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

    H. Bai

    Department of Educational and Human Sciences, University of Central Florida, 4000 Central Florida Blvd.,

    P. O. Box 161250, Orlando, FL 32816-1250, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    2/20

    1976). It is not only about the strategies that students use, but also about students knowing

    when and how to use them. When a person is metacognitive, he/she demonstrates an

    awareness and regulation of his/her mental processes (Griffith & Ruan, 2005). A person

    who is metacognitive knows how to learn because he/she is aware of what he/she knows

    and what he/she must do in order to gain new knowledge.Metacognitive people exhibit the qualities of good readers (Griffith & Ruan, 2005;

    Randi, Grigorenko & Sternberg, 2005) and are successful in school (Sternberg, 1998).

    However, despite the recognition of the role of metacognition in student success, limited

    research has been done to explore teachers explicit awareness of their metacognition and

    their ability to think about, talk about, and write about their thinking (Zohar, 1999). Therefore,

    it is important to study teachers understandings of the act of teaching metacognition, the

    challenges they face in doing so, and the relationships between their metacognitive

    knowledge and pedagogical understanding of metacognition. Gaining the knowledge could

    improve our ability to plan staff development and teacher education programs.

    A necessary condition for teaching students to be metacognitive is a pedagogical

    understanding of metacognition. Pedagogical understanding refers to teachers knowledge

    regarding effective instruction for helping students achieve a goal, in this case becoming

    metacognitive. The present study investigates participants pedagogical understandings of

    metacognition, the nature of what it means to teach metacognition, and the relationships

    between the participants knowledge and pedagogical understandings of metacognition.

    The following sections begin with a discussion on metacognition in general. Next, is a

    discussion on pedagogical understandings of metacognition. The discussion that follows

    addresses why and how we evaluate teachers knowledge. The methods used for this study

    and the findings of the research are presented in the following section. Finally, teachers

    pedagogical understandings of metacognition from the perspective of this study and

    limitations of current study are discussed.

    Theoretical framework

    Metacognition

    The concept of metacognition gained prominence in the 1970s with Flavell (1976). It has been

    defined as having knowledge (cognition) and having understanding, control over, andappropriate use of that knowledge (Tei & Stewart, 1985, p. 47) and involves both the

    conscious awareness and the conscious regulation of ones learning. A metacognitive person

    checks for understanding and regulates his/her understanding by using a metacognitive strategy.

    In reading, metacognition has been the focus of many research studies (Carrell,

    Gajduske, & Wise, 1998; Wenden, 1998) because reading comprehension is a complex

    cognitive process that requires thinking about what the reader knows and the application of

    the reading processes (Baker, 2002; Zhang, 2001, 2008). That is, metacognition refers

    specifically to the knowledge that readers have regarding the specific tasks for

    comprehension. Metacognitive readers monitor their understanding of text and controltheir understanding through the application of reading strategies and the evaluation of the

    effectiveness of the strategies. Research on reading strategies has focused on both specific

    strategies and the collection of strategies that readers use for comprehension. Research

    studies have recognized the importance of metacognition in differentiating between skilled

    and unskilled readers (Mokhtari & Reichard, 2002). According to Snow, Burns & Griffin

    (1998), skilled readers use their knowledge of the world to comprehend text literally as

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    3/20

    well as to draw valid inferences from texts, in their comprehension of words, and in their

    use of comprehension monitoring and repair strategies (p. 62). Their success most likely

    stems from their ability to recognize when comprehension breaks down and their ability to

    use strategies and/or techniques to improve/repair comprehension.

    Since metacognition is the key to comprehension, it must be a valued component inliteracy instruction. Such instruction must address student background knowledge,

    knowledge and practice of metacognitive strategies, and knowledge regarding implemen-

    tation of the strategies (Griffith & Ruan, 2005). Such instruction includes long term direct

    explanation and modeling of strategies followed by guided student practice with the

    strategies (Pressley, 2002). Students must understand the what, how, and when of the

    strategies to be applied (Paris, Lipson & Wixson, 1994). In other words, students need to

    know what the strategies are, how to implement them, and under what conditions to

    implement them. For instance, a reader who comes upon a confusing phrase needs to do

    something. If he/she simply keeps on reading despite a breakdown in understanding, he is

    not being metacognitive; if the student stops to ask a question and/or reread, he/she is

    applying a particular strategy for a particular purpose when it is needed.

    Despite the case for metacognitive literacy instruction, research over the last three

    decades has not demonstrated wide spread application (Pressley, 2002). This kind of

    instruction is complex and requires that teachers make metacognition the goal for literacy

    instruction (Baker, 2002). Contrary to this knowledge, Pressley, Wharton-McDonald,

    Mistretta-Hampston & Echebarria (1998) and Durkin (1978-1979) found that teachers are

    more likely to test comprehension than teach comprehension. These tests require that

    students process text after reading rather than being metacognitive during reading. The

    assumption of the teachers seemed to be that if their students simply read, read, and read,and then were tested, tested, and tested, they would become good comprehendersthey

    would become self-regulated readers who used comprehension strategies (Pressley, 2002,

    p. 303). The disconnection between the research which clearly indentifies the instructional

    models that make students metacognitive and the instruction in classrooms led us to wonder

    if teachers understood the pedagogical issues surrounding the teaching of metacognition.

    Teachers pedagogical understandings of metacognition

    The pedagogical understanding of metacognition refers to teachers understanding of what

    is necessary for the teaching of metacognition. Pedagogical understandings in general referto the teaching strategies and/or instructional techniques that will be implemented in

    particular situations to achieve a teaching goal. Successful metacognitive literacy

    instruction addresses student schema, knowledge of strategies, and knowledge of the

    conditions for implementing strategies (Gourgey, 1999; Griffith & Ruan, 2005). A teachers

    pedagogical understanding of metacognition requires that they understand what is needed to

    successfully teach students to be metacognitive.

    The literacy research regarding metacognition clearly delineates the structure of

    instruction. Students need models of strategies in action, guided practice as they implement

    those models, and independent practice with the strategies (Clark & Graves, 2005). Inaddition, students need to see that strategies are flexible and that good readers implement

    different strategies depending on the purpose of the reading and the demands of the text

    (Pressley, 2002). The explicit instruction described above is the part of teachers

    pedagogical understandings of metacognition that includes instructional strategies. Yet,

    the implementation of those instructional strategies requires that teachers create a learning

    environment in which students are (1) explicitly required to apply metacognitive activities

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    4/20

    and (2) asked to reflect on their thinking processes (Leat & Lin, 2007). The explicit application

    means that students need time during reading to apply strategies. In addition, students need

    time to reflect on their thinking to determine the appropriateness of the strategy application.

    Instructional strategies that help teachers implement explicit instruction and provide students

    time for reflecting on the processes include: think alouds (Isreal & Massey, 2005),opportunities to practice thinking strategies (Schreiber, 2005), active discussions (Zohar,

    2006), and the use the language of thinking (Tishman, Perkins & Jay, 1995). However,

    despite the strong theoretical studies, in practice many teachers lack sufficient knowledge

    about metacognition (Veenman, van Hout-Wolters & Afflerbach, 2006).

    Measuring teachers metacognitive knowledge and pedagogical understandings

    of metacognition

    Teachers understanding of what is necessary for teaching and learning has a strong impact

    on their practice (Aguirre & Speer, 1999; Borko & Putnam, 1996; Zohar, 2006). This

    impact affects students learning. Metacognition is not just a skill to be taught, but a

    disposition of what it means to think and learn (Harpaz, 2007). Thus, an analysis of

    teachers understandings of how to guide students in being metacognitive and the

    relationships between teachers pedagogical understandings of metacognition and their

    knowledge of metacognition could inform professional development. However, a robust

    review of the literature could not find an instrument designed to assess teachers

    metacognitive knowledge and the instructional strategies teachers valued in guiding their

    students to be metacognitive. Therefore, the authors created a Teacher Metacognition

    survey which is designed to assess a participants perception of his/her knowledge of

    metacognition, his/her pedagogical knowledge of metacognition, and his/her beliefs about

    practices that encourage students metacognition.

    The instrument was created based on the three components of the metacognition

    knowledge: declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge (Schraw, 2001) and

    pedagogical approach for metacognitive strategies (Baylor, 2002). A teachers instruction

    of metacognition may be influenced by his/her individual understandings of what it means

    to teach metacognition (Baylor, 2002). This includes the use of reflection or debriefing

    techniques, think alouds, problem-solving activities, small and whole group discussions

    about process and explicit strategy instruction. A detailed study of teachers pedagogical

    understandings of metacognition requires that teachers have declarative, procedural, andconditional knowledge. Declarative knowledge is a teachers knowledge of what they

    should teach. Procedural knowledge is knowledge of how a teacher teaches something.

    Conditional knowledge is the understanding that the teaching of metacognitive strategies is

    dependent on the situation and that particular situations require the use of particular

    strategies.

    The research questions under investigation for this study are as follows:

    1. How are the participants understandings of metacognition related to his/her

    perceptions of the instructional strategies that assist students in becoming

    metacognitive?

    2. What instructional routines do participants value in teaching students to be

    metacognitive?

    3. What are the causal relationships among participants conditional knowledge,

    declarative knowledge, and perceptions of procedural knowledge as parts of their

    pedagogical knowledge of the metacognition?

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    5/20

    Methods

    This study examined what can be inferred regarding participants pedagogical under-

    standings of metacognition in terms of reading instruction and how that relates to

    metacognitive theory. The study utilized mixed method, which involved both quantitativeand qualitative techniques to explore the answers to our research questions. Mixed method

    research is particularly useful for gaining a better and complex understanding of this

    particular topic. Qualitative methodology is utilized to explore the themes raised from the

    survey data and investigate how these themes influenced each other to answer the research

    questions from qualitative perspectives. Quantitative research is employed to discover the

    relationships among the factors revealed by the qualitative study and using statistical

    analysis to assess the direct and indirect effects of teachers metacognitive knowledge on

    their pedagogical understanding of metacognition; thus responding to our third research

    question.

    Participants

    The participants were 105 graduate students who were K-12 teachers majoring in

    different areas in education in the College of Education in a large southeastern

    university in the United States. Ninety-eight percent of the participants were female,

    which reflects the current gender composition of K-12 teachers across the nation. The

    participants had a range of teaching experiences, with 56% of participants having

    taught fewer than three years, 22% of participants having taught four to six years, and

    22% of participants having taught more than six years. 13.3% of the participants had amasters degree, 34.4% had a bachelors degree, and 13.3% had certificates in teaching

    or other degrees. Fifty percent of the participants were elementary education graduates

    with English as a Second Language Endorsement. This training included two courses in

    ESOL training and field work with students for whom English is not their first

    language. Secondary education majors from a variety of disciplines made up 22% of

    the study sample, including 15% English Language Arts majors. Three percent of the

    participants had received Bachelors degrees in Exceptional Education. Twenty-five

    percent of the participants had undergraduate degrees in areas other than education. The

    demographic mix of the participants generally mirrored that of the general population in

    the public school district in the southeast area of the United States. The majority of theparticipants were under the age of 35, 81% of the total sample. Seventy-three percent

    of the participating teachers were students in the M.Ed. in Reading program for K-12

    (Table 1).

    Table 1 Participant demographics

    Area of undergraduate studies Area of master degree Age of the participants

    Elementary Education 50% Reading Education 73% Under the age of 35 85%

    Secondary Education 22% Educational Leadership 10% Between 3545 13%

    Exceptional Education 3% Mathematics Education 7% Over the age of 45 2%

    A degree in an area other than education 25% Science Education 5%

    Social Science

    Education

    5%

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    6/20

    Instrumentation

    The data for this study were collected using the researcher-created instrument. The survey

    includes two parts. Part One contains demographic questions followed by the two open-

    ended questions. Part Two is the Teachers

    Metacognition Scale (TMS) with 20 Likert-Scale questions. In order to assess the participants general understanding of the construct

    of metacognition, the participants were asked two open-ended questions before beginning

    the Likert scale section. The open-ended questions begin the section with the directions

    This section of the questionnaire is designed to help us gain a better understanding of the

    kinds of things that you do to teach students thinking skills and strategies. The questions

    included:

    1. What is metacognition?

    2. What are metacognitive thinking strategies?

    The questions served two purposes. One assures that teachers who participated in the

    study had a declarative understanding of metacognition. The first question, What is

    metacognition? determined if the participant had enough familiarity with the concept to

    define it. If the participants could identify a definition of metacognition derived from ideas

    in educational theory and research, they were included in the study. Ten cases were

    excluded from study because they left the question blank, wrote, I dont know, or

    responded to the question with a comment such as something a teacher does. The second

    open-ended question asked the participants What are metacognitive thinking strategies?

    This question gathered more detailed declarative knowledge from the participants regarding

    his/her understanding of metacognition in teaching. Five more participants were excludedfrom the study because they left the question blank or wrote, I dont know. Thus, the

    short answer questions led to the disqualification of fifteen participants. We also used these

    questions to attain a deeper understanding of the participants interpretations, thus allowing

    for some qualitative data analysis of the participants beliefs regarding the key concepts

    studied with the survey.

    TMS is designed to assess the participants self-perceptions regarding his/her

    understanding of metacognition and their pedagogical knowledge of the metacognition.

    In this section, each question asked participants about his/her modeling/demonstration of

    thinking processes, opportunities for practicing thinking processes, students sharing

    thinking processes, questioning strategies, providing feedback/debriefing practices, group-ing practices, and the use of active discussions (See Table 2). The questions in this section

    deal with classroom structure and teaching actions. The questions use knowledge gained

    from Leat and Lins (2007) analysis of how teachers might encourage metacognition and

    transfer, Clark and Graves (2005) literature review of effective metacognitive literacy

    instruction, Torff and Warburtons (2005) and Zohars (1999) research on teachers use of

    higher order thinking strategies during instruction, Desautels (2009) perspectives on a

    thinking classroom, and Cummins, Steward and Blocks (2005) description of teaching

    strategies that help students use multiple strategies during reading.

    Questions for participants to evaluate students

    metacognitive processing are also includedin TMS. The questions ask participants to rate the level of metacognitive thinking if students

    described actions during learning, planned a project, or wrote an essay (see Table 2). This

    section also addresses the types of teaching activities that as teachers, the participants believed

    required students to think about their thinking. The evaluation of activities is important in

    establishing the participants beliefs regarding the types of activities necessary for transfer of

    metacognition from explicit instruction activities to general use. Teachers will not take up

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    7/20

    research-based ideas, such as those regarding the teaching of metacognition, if they are

    presented as general principals that require them to turn them into specific practice (Black &

    Wiliam, 1998); thus this section addresses specific practices in which the participant believes

    he/she is requiring students to be metacognitive.

    Table 2 Likert-scale items

    Items Questions

    Item 1 You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive thinking

    if they spent most of their time planning the logistics of their final presentations before fullydeveloping their models.

    Item 2 A teacher is modeling for students. Rate the modeling script regarding its helpfulness in guiding

    students metacognitive thinking. Watch me think out loud while I try to predict what this story

    is going to be about. The title is Twisted, by Laurie H. Anderson. There is a picture of one of

    those flexible pretzel pencils on the cover. The pencil makes me think that this book will take

    place mostly in a school because pencils are used in schools.

    Item 3 You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive thinking if

    they are able to describe how and why they plan to use each of the six simple machines to

    create a roller coaster.

    Item 4 You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive thinking

    if they are aware of the reasoning involved in completing a Venn Diagram.

    Item 5 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should recognize that strategies

    are general and thus do not worry about the task for which we implement the strategies.

    Item 6 You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive thinking if

    they are asked to complete an essay that describes the events of Sherman s March on Atlanta

    including who, what, where, when and why.

    Item 7 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies the problem-solving activities are

    more important than time for students to talk about the activities.

    Item 8 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies the teacher should spend most of

    her time telling students how to fill out the strategy worksheet.

    Item 9 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should ask inferential

    questions and check the accuracy of student answers.Item 10 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should explain the

    mental processes used to answer inferential questions.

    Item 11 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should increase their

    awareness of the strategy and understanding of its power by relating it to specific task objectives.

    Item 12 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should debrief them

    after a lesson to review the thinking processes that helped students learn the content.

    Item 13 You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive thinking if

    they were able to describe their actions as being able to explain what was learned.

    Item 14 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should provide time for

    students to talk about how they solved problem-solving activities.

    Item 15 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should allow students toshare their thinking.

    Item 16 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should facilitate

    discussions on how problems are solved.

    Item 17 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should model her

    thinking processes.

    Item 18 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should allow students to

    generate questions regarding content.

    Item 19 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should provide

    problem-solving activities for students.

    Item 20 When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should ask students to

    explain how they came up with their answers.

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    8/20

    The original survey questionnaire had 27 items on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1

    (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Through a pilot study and expert examination,

    seven items were found to measure student knowledge of the metacognition instead of

    measuring teachers knowledge of metacognition or their pedagogical knowledge related to

    the metacognition; therefore, the seven items were deleted from the survey. The finalversion of the questionnaire is a 20-item survey. The instrument was evaluated for both

    validity and reliability.

    Validity of the instrument The content validity of the instrument was examined by experts

    in metacognitive theory and the literature review; therefore, the face validity of TMS was

    first achieved. To examine the construct validity, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was

    conducted. According to the metacognitive and pedagogical theory, the instrument was

    designed to measure four hypothetical constructs: declarative knowledge, conditional

    knowledge, procedural knowledge, and pedagogical knowledge. To confirm the construct

    validity measured by TMS, CFA was used by loading the twenty items on the four factors

    identified by the literature, namely, Conditional (conditional knowledge), Declarative

    (declarative knowledge), Procedural (procedure knowledge), and Pedagogical (pedagogical

    knowledge) (see Table 3). Table 3 revealed that the factor loadings for the four factors

    ranged from.24 to.54 for the factor of pedagogical, from.26 to.77 for the factor of

    conditional, from.34 to.56 for the factor of declarative, from.58 to.71 the factor of for

    procedural, and (see Table 3).

    Table 3 Factor loadings of TMS using principal components (n=105)

    Items Pedagogical Conditional Declarative Procedural

    Item 1 .540*** -0.504 0.419 0.044

    Item 2 .528*** -0.052 0.484 0.427

    Item 3 .507*** 0.19 0.288 0.417

    Item 4 .439*** 0.075 0.286 0.213

    Item 5 .240** 0.063 0.096 0.139

    Item 6 0.202 .770*** 0.002 0.08

    Item 7 -0.543 .494*** 0.143 0.18Item 8 -0.179 .412*** 0.156 0.186

    Item 9 -0.266 .255** 0.271 0.288

    Item 10 0.513 -0.245 .564*** -0.112

    Item 11 0.269 0.038 .455*** 0.099

    Item 12 0.337 -0.379 .454*** 0.363

    Item 13 -0.057 0.041 .336*** 0.302

    Item 14 0.087 -0.128 -0.156 .714***

    Item 15 -0.17 -0.124 -0.072 .665***

    Item 16 0.04 -0.045 -0.04 .622***

    Item 17 -0.318 0.011 -0.16 .651***

    Item 18 -0.345 -0.168 0.064 .608***

    Item 19 0.063 -0.024 -0.103 .580***

    Item 20 -0.401 -0.311 -0.121 .576***

    **p

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    9/20

    The loadings were significant with all p-values larger than.05. Sixty-one percent of the

    total variance was explained by the measurement model. This process confirmed the four

    hypothetical constructs measured by the 20-item TMS and provided strong evidence of the

    construct validity for TMS.

    Reliability of the instrument The internal subscale reliabilities were Cronbachs Alpha

    value of.76 for the pedagogical knowledge,.74 for declarative knowledge,.75 for the

    conditional knowledge, and.76 for the procedural knowledge. The internal reliabilities of

    the subscales were acceptable. The internal consistency of TMS was Cronbachs Alpha

    value of.75. Table 4 illustrates four factors and their coefficient alphas.

    Data collection procedures

    Data for this study were collected through online survey questionnaires using web-based media

    (Zoomerrang). Demographic data and two open-ended questions for collecting qualitative data

    concerning teachers deep understanding of their pedagogical and metacognition knowledge

    were administered as Part One of the online survey. The quantitative data were collected using

    TMS as Part Two of the survey. All teachers participated in the study voluntarily with the

    understanding that they were given extra credit as an incentive. The survey questionnaire was

    designed to measure K-12 teachers self-perceptions of their metacognitive knowledge and their

    perceptions of their pedagogical knowledge of metacognition. All participants were asked to

    complete the survey during anytime in the Fall Semester 2008.

    Data analysis

    Qualitative analysis Data analysis occurred in multiple phases. First, the qualitative data

    was analyzed using an iterative approach. It was a recursive process in which the data were

    reviewed to determine the major themes in the written responses. The analysis involved

    discovering patterns, themes, and categories in the data (Creswell, 1994; Patton, 2002).

    Therefore, open coding of the participants responses to the open-ended questions was the

    first step to identifying themes and patterns in the data. We used Teschs (1990) systematic

    process of analyzing textual data by reading all textual data by (1) identifying topics; (2)

    clustering together similar topics; (3) abbreviating topics as codes; (4) developingcategories; (5) looking for overlaps and interrelationship of topics; (6) assembling data in

    each category; (7) performing preliminary analysis of findings; and (8) confirming findings.

    First participants who did not answer the questions or who provided incomplete responses

    were excluded from the study. Then the open-ended questions were coded for patterns in

    responses. The responses were placed in a number of themes developed from the words of

    the participants including but not limited to thinking, awareness, strategies, and

    Table 4 An illustration of four factors and coefficient alpha

    Factor # Variables Alpha

    Factor 1: Pedagogical Item 1, Item 2, Item3, Item 4, Item 5 .76

    Factor 2: Conditional Item 6, Item 7, Item 8, Item 9 .74

    Factor 3: Declarative Item 10, Item 11, Item 12, Item 13 .75

    Factor 4: Procedural Item 14, Item 15, Item 16, Item 17, Item 18, Item 19, Item 20 .76

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    10/20

    comprehension. These themes were then analyzed by the researchers to determine more

    global patterns. The global patterns represented two large themes (1) metacognition is a

    construct that must be taught including declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge

    with pedagogical knowledge and (2) metacognition is something for which teachers make

    students aware focusing on declarative knowledge as part of their pedagogicalunderstanding of metacognition. Once themes were established, the data was sent to five

    teachers who had detailed knowledge regarding metacognition and metacognitive thinking

    strategies. The teachers were given the two themes as well as the open-ended responses for

    them to categorize the responses into the two large themes. The teachers were asked to

    apply a label of one of the two themes to each participants open-ended response. They

    coded the data independently and returned the information to the researchers through email.

    When the independently coded data was received from the teachers, the researchers used

    the information to determine inter-rater reliability for the open-ended responses. The

    agreement of the teachers assigning the responses to each theme was calculated using a

    mean score to find the inter-rater reliability as.89, which was the average value of

    agreement from each pair of raters.

    The final stage of qualitative analysis contextualized the results of the quantitative

    analysis of the responses on the Likert Scale questions. Using the factors established by the

    quantitative analysis the data was first grouped under the two global themes. Then using the

    factors as determined by the quantitative analysis explanations were reached using content

    analysis of the data focusing on the questions in each factor using the wording of the

    questions as well as participant responses; thereby contextualizing the quantitative analysis.

    Quantitative analysis The quantitative data were collected from the Likert scalequestionnaire of TMS. The data were coded and entered by two trained research assistants

    into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences V16.0 (SPSS) (SPSS Inc., 2007).

    The current research study used a structural equation modeling (SEM) with quantitative

    measurement to avoid misleading of regression estimates and secure correct conclusion about the

    relationship between the explored factors. SEM is employed to take measurement error into

    consideration. According to the theory and qualitative analysis results, the hypothetical structure

    equation model was constructed to explore the causal relationships among the hypothetical

    constructs in this study: Declarative (declarative knowledge), Conditional (conditional

    knowledge), Procedural (procedural knowledge), and Pedagogical (pedagogical knowledge).

    According to the literature on metacognition theory (Hartman, 2001) teachersunderstanding of metacognition is complex and depends on an interaction between

    declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge. The metacognition theory assumes that

    teachers conditional knowledge influences procedural knowledge, which in turn also

    affects declarative knowledge, and further alters their pedagogical knowledge in

    metacognition. Procedural knowledge directly affects declarative knowledge and pedagog-

    ical knowledge. Declarative knowledge builds upon conditional and procedural knowledge

    and directly influences pedagogical knowledge. Based on the literature and theory, the

    Hypothetical Structural Model of Metacognition (See Fig. 1) was constructed.

    The four variables measured by TMS involved in this study were defined as follows:

    & Pedagogical: participants pedagogical understanding of metacognition included his/her

    understanding of what it means to teach students to integrate declarative and procedural

    knowledge in solving problems of learning.

    & Conditional: participants understanding regarding the conditions under which certain

    strategies are implemented

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    11/20

    & Declarative: participants declarative knowledge of metacognition reflect an under-

    standing of the definitions of metacognitive strategies or making students aware of

    metacognitive strategies

    & Procedural: participants providing assignments that require students to apply particular

    metacognitive strategies

    Two stages of the SEM model were analyzed. For the first stage, data were analyzed to fit the

    Hypothetical Structure Equation Model of Metacognition to fit the quantitative data collectedby the TMS. T-test, and ANOVA were conducted to test for independent variables. Because

    no significant group differences were found in terms of gender, teaching experience, or the

    educational degrees of teachers, these variables were excluded in the hypothetical model. The

    analyses were conducted using the Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS) (Arbuckle,

    2007). The second stage was to explore the best fitted SEM model using the quantitative data

    to study the relationships between the identified latent variables, Declarative, Conditional,

    Procedural, and Pedagogical to answer the third research question.

    Analysis results

    The analysis sought to gather an understanding of the participants pedagogical

    understanding of metacognition; or what her/she believed was necessary for teaching

    students to become metacognitive. There were two themes uncovered by the analysis of the

    data qualitatively and quantitatively. The themes were informed by the qualitative analysis

    Declarative

    Conditional Procedural

    Pedagogical

    Item 4

    Item 3

    Item 6

    Item 8

    Item 9

    Item 7

    Item 17

    Item 20

    Item 18

    Item 11

    Item 12

    Item 16

    Item 15

    Item 19

    Item 13

    Item 5

    Item 1

    Item 2

    Item 10

    Item 14

    Fig. 1 Hypothetical structural model of metacognition

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    12/20

    whose results were consistent with the previous research on metacognition and learning.

    The first theme demonstrated an understanding that teaching metacognition was an active

    process of engaging students in sharing thinking processes through the teaching of

    declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge while making students accountable for

    using metacognitive skills. The second theme was the awareness of metacognition.

    Theme one: an active process requiring engagement

    Theme one was included two significant factors concerning when to teach students

    metacognitive thinking strategies. The theme was aligned with the qualitative data in which

    the participants described metacognition as thinking about their thinking and knowing how

    to think about solving a problem. This theme was matched with qualitative responses that

    involved active learning, such as teaching students to think about how you think/learn,

    teaching students strategies to improve understanding and task completion, and teaching

    students how to control ones cognitive processes. The common thread in this theme was

    teaching students that metacognition was an active process of thinking to solve a problem

    or learn something. The specific factors that compose this theme included: (1) teaching

    metacognition to students requires visible problem solving and (2) teaching conditional

    knowledge is key to teaching students to be metacognitive.

    Teaching students to be metacognitive requires visible problem solving (Pedagogical)

    Metacognition requires that the teacher guide students to help them become metacognitive

    while providing them with time to share their own thinking processes. Teachers

    participating in this survey identified a factor of pedagogical understandings ofmetacognition in which the teacher was key in providing a classroom where thinking

    about how you solve problems was visible. Participants demonstrated that when instructing

    students on the declarative and procedural aspects of metacognition they must be explicit

    and that students were held accountable for using metacognitive skills. In this theme, talk

    was central to learning. Participants indicated that in a classroom that encouraged students

    to use metacognitive thinking strategies; thinking was visible and shared by teachers and

    students. The following questions from the survey fell into this theme:

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    provide time for students to discuss their problem solving.& When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    allow students to share their thinking.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    facilitate discussions on how problems are solved.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    model her thinking processes.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    allow students to generate questions regarding content.

    &

    When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher shouldprovide problem-solving activities for students.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should ask

    students to explain how they came up with their answers.

    Throughout this factor talk about thinking in which the teacher was the facilitator

    is central to the understanding of metacognition according to the participants. The

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    13/20

    res po ns es o f the p articipa nts ind ic ated the tea ch er a s a g uide to b uild in g

    metacognitive thinking strategies for students. This was identified as both teachers

    and students shared thinking through talk as they generated questions, explained how

    they responded to questions, and interpreted how they solved problems. The

    participants acknowledged the importance of explicit modeling in teaching studentsto become metacognitive. This included explaining the mental processes, not simply

    telling about the processes. This concept went beyond teachers teaching for

    conditional knowledge or building talk in the classroom in that the participants

    identified the students as being able to be describe their metacognition as the key to

    teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies. The participants responses

    aligned with the literature on metacognition as an integration of declarative,

    procedural, and conditional knowledge.

    Pedagogical understanding of conditional knowledge (Conditional) The second factor

    discovered through the data analysis was related to the teacher as guide; but went further. In

    this theme, the participant described the importance of demonstrating how, why, and under

    what conditions to use metacognitive teaching strategies. Participants identified the strategy

    of debriefing as a tool for increasing student awareness of which strategies were helpful

    when working to achieve an objective.

    The following questions from the survey fell into this theme:

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    explain the mental processes used to answer inferential questions.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    increase their awareness of the strategy and understanding of its power by relating it tospecific task objectives.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    debrief them after a lesson to review the thinking processes that helped students learn

    the content.

    & You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive

    thinking if they were able to describe their actions as being able to explain what was

    learned.

    In this factor the act of debriefing about strategy was central. The participants rating of this

    factor demonstrated a pedagogical understanding of metacognition that described metacogni-tive thinking strategies as evaluating and revising as you go, strategies to regulate and direct

    thinking and learning; and picking the best strategy for the task at hand. What made this theme

    unique was that it highlighted the understanding that metacognition requires an evaluation of

    the task and choosing the correct strategy in which to complete the task.

    Theme two: touching the surface of metacognitive thinking strategies

    The second theme indicated that despite the robust identification of teaching metacognitivethinking strategies as an active process, which is also awareness of cognition. Awareness is

    different from active learning because it only asks students to know what or when a

    problem occurs. This theme had two factors that contributed to its development: (1)

    teaching metacognitive thinking strategies makes students aware and (2) teaching

    metacognitive thinking strategies was providing students with assignments that could lead

    to strategy use.

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    14/20

    Making students aware (Declarative) The topic dealt with telling students what to do and

    making them aware of processes. The participants responses in this theme were more about

    the recognition of metacognition rather than a teaching of how to be metacognitive. This

    theme was closely aligned with the qualitative responses, which identified metacognition as

    an awareness of thinking, rather than an active process of thinking about ones thinking.

    The questions that make up this factor were:

    & You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive

    thinking if they spent most of their time planning the logistics of their final

    presentations before fully developing their models.

    & A teacher is modeling for students. Rate the modeling script regarding its helpfulness in

    guiding students metacognitive thinking. Watch me think out loud while I try to

    predict what this story is going to be about. The title is Twisted, by Laurie H. Anderson.

    There is a picture of one of those flexible pretzel pencils on the cover. The pencil makes

    me think that this book will take place mostly in a school because pencils are used inschools.

    & You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive

    thinking if they are able to describe how and why they plan to use each of the six

    simple machines to create a roller coaster

    & You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive

    thinking if they are aware of the reasoning involved in completing a Venn Diagram.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should

    recognize that strategies are general and thus do not worry about the task for which we

    implement the strategies.

    This factor puts the teacher at the center in making students aware of their actions. The

    teacher was not having students talk about their thinking but simply had them recognize

    that they were thinking.

    Providing assignments (Procedural) This topic described activities or the values of certain

    activities that helped students gain metacognitive thinking strategies. The assignments

    required the students be metacognitive, but the processes did not guide students in

    metacognitive thinking. In this case, the framework was present, but factors such as

    debriefing and discussing thinking processes were absent. The questions that made up this

    factor were:

    & You are evaluating students metacognitive processing. Rate the level of metacognitive

    thinking if they are asked to complete an essay that describes the events of Shermans

    March on Atlanta including the who, what, where, when and why.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies the problem-solving

    activities are more important than time for students to talk about the activities.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies the teacher should

    spend most of her time telling students how to fill out the strategy worksheet.

    & When teaching students to use metacognitive thinking strategies, the teacher should ask

    inferential questions and check the accuracy of student answers.

    This theme isolated declarative knowledge in that students were told about metacog-

    nition and to do it; but not told how or under what conditions they should be metacognitive.

    The results of this theme demonstrated that participants highly valued making students

    aware of metacognitive thinking processes and providing a framework in which they could

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    15/20

    be metacognitive. Yet, what was missing in this theme was the teaching of metacognition.

    Throughout the study, responses indicated that participants juggled the need for integrating

    declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge through active teaching and making

    students aware.

    Quantitative analysis results

    To study the SME model, we used the commonly-used model-fit indices as Chi-square,

    comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), incremental fit index (IFI), Akaike

    information criterion (AIC), parsimony comparative fit index (PCFI), and root mean square error

    of approximation (RMSEA) (Baggozzi & Yi, 1988; Byrne, 1998; Jaccard & Wan, 1995; Kline,

    1998). In the first stage, we fit the Hypothetical Model of Metacognition (Fig. 1) to our study

    data. The model fit indices revealed the moderate model fit (2=184.61/df=155/p

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    16/20

    The declarative knowledge only determined the pedagogical practice with a weak but

    significant correlation (=.11).

    The SEM analysis presented causal relationships of the three components ofknowledge of metacognition and their pedagogical knowledge. The SEM results

    revealed that the conditional knowledge directly influences the procedural knowledge

    developed and self-perceptions of pedagogical knowledge and indirectly affects the

    declarative knowledge through the procedural knowledge as a mediator. The

    procedural knowledge directly influences the declarative knowledge and the self-

    perceptions of pedagogical knowledge. The declarative knowledge directly influences

    their self-perception of pedagogical knowledge. The SEM analysis connected with

    theory in that understanding when and where to apply metacognitive strategies

    (conditional knowledge) may determine what procedures (procedural knowledge) to

    follow and in turn what the strategies (declarative knowledge) to apply, and finally

    affects how they implement the knowledge of metacognition in practice (pedagogical

    knowledge). This finding provided us with empirical evidence of how teachers

    knowledge of metacognition related and influenced each other and encouraged us to

    explore further on teachers awareness of metacognition and their metacognitive

    thinking strategies.

    .67

    Declarative

    Conditional

    .01

    Procedural

    .42

    Pedagogical.20

    Item 4e4

    .31

    Item 3e3

    .17

    Item 6e6

    .60

    Item 8e8

    .07

    Item 9e9

    .24

    Item 7e7.49

    .32

    Item 17 e17

    .43

    Item 20 e20

    .43

    Item 18 e18

    .66

    .32

    Item 11 e11

    .21

    Item 12 e12

    .52

    Item 16 e16

    .37

    Item 15 e15

    .36

    Item 19 e19

    .11

    Item 13 e13.27

    Item 5e5

    d2

    d1 d3

    .05

    Item 1e1

    .26

    Item 2e2

    .26

    Item 10 e10

    .33

    Item 14 e14

    .57

    .33

    .77

    .33

    -.45

    -.35

    .28

    .26

    -.27

    -.22

    .12

    .24

    .51

    .60

    .34

    .34

    .25

    .41

    .52

    .22

    .51

    .45

    .82

    .37.21

    .65

    .58

    .61

    .72

    .57

    .11 .46.56

    .10

    .44

    Fig. 2 Final fitted model with standardized effects at the.05 level

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    17/20

    Discussions and limitations

    This study examined the pedagogical understandings of metacognition teachers working

    towards a Masters Degree in Education. The pedagogical understandings of metacognition

    involved the nature of what it means to teach and how students learn strategies thatencourage them to be metacognitive.

    Teachers understandings of metacognition appear to be related to their perceptions of

    the instructional strategies that assist students in becoming metacognitive. The participants

    in this study reported that metacognition was an active process and that teaching students to

    become metacognitive also was an active process that required engagement and practice. In

    addition, participants also considered metacognition as the awareness of processes and

    reported that providing appropriate assignments would assist students in becoming

    metacognitive. Thus, data demonstrated that the individual teachers understanding of

    metacognition was related to the instructional strategies they perceived to be effective in

    helping students to become metacognitive. The research findings indicate that teachers may

    benefit from professional development on the differences between engagement and

    awareness when guiding students to implement metacognitive strategies.

    The second research question sought to understand the instructional routines that

    teachers valued in teaching students to be metacognitive. The data indicated that the

    participants valued a variety of strategies that aligned with the research on teaching. They

    valued demonstration, scaffolding, teaching conditional knowledge, and providing students

    time to demonstrate their learning. In addition, the participants recognized value in

    providing assignments that assisted students metacognitive thinking and taking the time to

    help students to be self-aware of cognitive processes. Teacher professional developmentand teacher education programs should implement practices that support an understanding

    in instructional routines that improve students metacognition.

    The study results also suggested that there are significant relationships among participants

    conditional knowledge, declarative knowledge, and procedural knowledge as parts of their

    pedagogical knowledge of the metacognition. As demonstrated in the analysis, participants

    pedagogical knowledge of metacognition is a combination between declarative, procedural, and

    conditional knowledge. There was a significant impact of participants procedural knowledge

    on their declarative knowledge, which affected their pedagogical knowledge of metacognition.

    The conditional knowledge significantly impacted on pedagogical knowledge. In addition the

    procedural knowledge also directly impacted on the pedagogical knowledge. The causalrelationship between the different knowledge types related to the complexity of the concept of

    metacognition and indicated the foundations for teachers to have a pedagogical understanding

    of metacognition. In order to assist pre-service and practicing teachers in developing this

    complex pedagogical understanding of metacognition teacher educators should focus on the

    three factors and how they relate in the application of metacognition.

    Teachers rich understanding of metacognition included the concept of a metacognitive

    person as someone who monitors his/her understanding and uses strategies to regulate

    understanding. This study investigated K12 teachers understandings of metacognition

    regarding instruction. Since the participants in this study were predominately seekingadvanced degrees in education, it was assumed that they had some familiarity with the

    instructional practices known to improve metacognitive thinking in students. This

    instruction includes direct explanation of the what, how, and when of strategies; modeling

    of strategies followed by scaffolded student practice with the strategies (Paris, Lipson &

    Wixson, 1994; Pressley, 2002). In short, the participants pedagogical understanding of

    metacognition included knowledge of how to scaffold and guide students, how to

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    18/20

    demonstrate thinking, knowledge of the strategies, knowledge of students, and knowledge

    of when to implement strategies (Griffith & Ruan, 2005; Gourgey, 1999). A teachers

    pedagogical understanding of metacognition requires that they understand what is needed to

    successfully teach students to be metacognitive. Thus teachers should make a point to

    include instruction on how to instruct students to become metacognitive.The participants in this study demonstrated that they had a rich pedagogical

    understanding of metacognition, but within this understanding there appeared to be

    contradiction. For instance, the group identified the teaching of metacognition as an active

    process; but also commented on the importance of making students aware of metacognition

    and providing assignments that may encourage students to be metacognitive. They identify

    the teaching of metacognition as both implicit and explicit.

    We propose that participants responses included both implicit and explicit aspects of

    instruction on metacognition because there may be discrepancy between what the

    participants know they should do and his/her practice. Since this survey did not ask

    participants to report what they do in the classroom, but to rate practices for teaching

    metacognitive thinking, the results may indicate that the participants know what is right, but

    want to give credit to what they do in the classroom. Many teachers are forced to follow

    mandated programs that may not reflect a rich pedagogical understanding of metacognition,

    or they feel stressed by the amount of material they need to cover. Perhaps the idea of

    providing a student with the time and space to discuss his/her thinking sounds good in

    theory, but may not reflect what they actually do. Teachers are absolutely willing to invest

    effort in the instruction of metacognition within their lessons, but they need the tools for

    implementing metacognition as an integral part of their lessons... (Veenman et al., 2006, p.

    10). This is significant because previous research has noted that it is easier to transformteachers knowledge about subject area than their knowledge about the nature of teaching

    and learning (Borko & Putnam, 1996). These teachers appeared to have an academic

    understanding of what is necessary for teaching students to be metacognitive; but they also

    seem to value activities that are not highly correlated with helping students to become

    metacognitive. Therefore, professional development and graduate course work needs more

    emphasis on the instructional strategies that encourage metacognition.

    Teachers self-reports presented in this study provided some insight regarding the

    complex relationships between the concepts of metacognition and thoughts about teaching

    metacognition. We addressed what teachers understood metacognition is, and how they

    should teach it. We address procedural knowledge as providing assignments that requirestudents to apply particular metacognitive strategies. Thus this study only studied the

    teachers declarative knowledge of the procedures for metacognition but did not directly

    measure what teachers actually did in their classroom. This is a limitation of the current

    study in that it did not address the actual employment of metacognitive skills during

    teaching, but simply presented the planning for them. In the future study, we would like to

    explore how to further accurately measure metacognition constructs and select larger

    representative samples to investigate the dispositions of metacognition in teaching.

    References

    Aguirre, J., & Speer, N. M. (1999). Examining the relationship between beliefs and goals in teacher practice.

    The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18(3), 327356.

    Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos (Version 7.0.0) [Computer software]. Spring House: Amos Development

    Corporation.

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    19/20

    Baggozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structure equation models. Journal of academy of

    marketing and science, 16(1), 1421.

    Baker, L. (2002). Metacognitve comprehension instruction. In C. C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.),

    Comprehension instruction: research-based best practices (pp. 7795). New York: Guilford.

    Baylor, A. L. (2002). Expanding preservice teachers metacognitive awareness of instructional planning

    through pedagogical agents. Educational Technology Research & Design, 50(2), 5

    22.Black, P., & Wiliam, D. (1998). Inside the black box: raising standards through classroom assessment. Phi

    Delta Kappan, 80(2), 139148.

    Borko, H., & Putnam, R. T. (1996). Learning to teach. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of

    Educational Psychology. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: basic concepts,

    appli-cations, and programming. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Carrell, P. L., Gajduske, L., & Wise, T. (1998). Metacognition and EFL/ESL reading. Instructional Science,

    26(12), 97112.

    Clark, K. F., & Graves, M. F. (2005). Scaffolding students' comprehension of text. The Reading Teacher, 58

    (6), 570580.

    Creswell, J. W. (1994). Research design and qualitative approaches. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Cummins, C., Stewart, M. T., & Block, C. C. (2005). Teaching several metacognitive strategies togetherincreases students' independent metacognition. In S. Isreal, C. Block, K. Bauserman, & K. Kinnucan-

    Welsch (Eds.), Metacognition in literacy learning: theory, assessment, instruction, and professional

    development (pp. 227296). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Desautel, D. (2009). Becoming a thinking thinker: metacognition, self-reflection, and classroom practice.

    Teachers College Record, 111(8), 19972020.

    Durkin, D. (19781979). What classroom observations reveal about reading comprehension instruction.

    Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 481533.

    Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. F. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of

    intelligence (pp. 231235). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Gourgey, A. F. (1999). Teaching reading from a metacognitive perspective: theory and classroom practice.

    Journal of College Reading and Learning, 30(1), 85

    94.Griffith, P. L., & Ruan, J. (2005). What is metacognition and what should be its role in literacy instruction?

    In S. Isreal, C. Block, K. Bauserman, & K. Kinnucan-Welsch (Eds.), Metacognition in literacy learning:

    theory, assessment, instruction, and professional development (pp. 318). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates.

    Harpaz, Y. (2007). Approaches to teaching thinking: toward a conceptual mapping of the field. Teachers

    College Record, 109(8), 18451874.

    Hartman, H. J. (2001). Metacognition in learning and instruction: theory, research and practice. Dordrecht:

    Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Isreal, S. E., & Massey, D. (2005). Metacognitive think-alouds: Using a gradual release model with middle

    school students. In S. Isreal, C. Block, K. Bauserman, & K. Kinnucan-Welsch (Eds.), Metacognition in

    literacy learning: theory, assessment, instruction, and professional development (pp. 183189).

    Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Jaccard, J., & Wan, C. K. (1995). Measurement error in the analysis of interaction effects between continuous

    predictors using multiple regression: multiple indicator and structural equation approaches. Psycholog-

    ical Bulletin, 117, 348357.

    Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford.

    Leat, D., & Lin, M. (2007). Developing pedagogy of metacognition and transfer: some signposts for the

    generation and use of knowledge and the creation of research partnerships. British Educational Research

    Journal, 29(3), 383414.

    Mokhtari, K., & Reichard, C. A. (2002). Assessing students metacognitive awareness of reading strategies.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 249259.

    Paris, S. G., Lipson, M. Y., & Wixson, K. K. (1994). Becoming a strategic reader. In R. B. Ruddell, M. R.

    Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (4th ed., pp. 788810).

    Newark: International Reading Association.Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research & evaluation methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension strategies instruction: a turn-of-the-century status report. In C. C. Block & M.

    Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: research-based best practices (pp. 1127). New York: Guilford.

    Pressley, M., Wharton-McDonald, R., Mistretta-Hampston, J. M., & Echevarria, M. (1998). The nature of literacy

    instruction in ten grade 4/5 classrooms in upstate New York. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 159194.

    Randi, J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2005). Revisiting definitions of reading comprehension. Just

    what is reading comprehension anyway? In S. Isreal, C. Block, K. Bauserman, & K. Kinnucan-Welsch

    Pedagogical understandings

  • 7/28/2019 Wilson & Bai (2010)

    20/20

    (Eds.), Metacognition in literacy learning: theory, assessment, instruction, and professional development

    (pp. 1939). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Schraw, G. (2001). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. In H. J. Hartman (Ed.), Metacognition in

    learning and instruction: theory, research and practice (pp. 316). Boston: Kluwer.

    Schreiber, F. J. (2005). Metacognition and self-regulation in literacy. In S. Isreal, C. Block, K. Bauserman, &

    K. Kinnucan-Welsch (Eds.), Metacognition in literacy learning: theory, assessment, instruction, andprofessional development (pp. 215240). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.

    Washington: National Academy Press.

    Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Metacognition, abilities, and developing expertise: what makes an expert student?

    Instructional Science, 26, 127140.

    Tei, E., & Stewart, O. (1985). Effective studying from text: applying metacognitive strategies. Forum for

    Reading, 16(2), 4655.

    Tesch, R. (1990). Qualitative research: analysis types and software tools. New York: Falmer.

    Tishman, S., Perkins, D., & Jay, E. (1995). The thinking classroom: teaching and learning in a culture of

    thinking. Needham, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Torff, B., & Warburton, E. D. (2005). Assessments of teachers beliefs about classroom use of critical-

    thinking activities. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(1), 155

    179.Veenman, M. V. J., van Hout-Wolters, B., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual

    and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 314.

    Wenden, A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language learning. Applied Linguistics, 19, 515537.

    Zhang, L. J. (2001). Awareness in reading: EFL students' metacognitive knowledge of reading strategies in

    an acquisition-poor environment. Language Awareness, 10, 268288.

    Zhang, L. J. (2008). Constructivist pedagogy in strategic reading instruction: exploring pathways to learner

    development in the English as a second language (ESL) classroom. Instructional Science, 36, 89116.

    Zohar, A. (1999). Teachers' metacognitive knowledge and the instruction of higher order thinking. Teaching

    and Teacher Education, 15, 413429.

    Zohar, A. (2006). The nature and development of teachers metastrategic knowledge in the context of

    teaching higher order thinking. The Journal of Learning Sciences, 15(3), 331377.

    N.S. Wilson, H. Bai