Top Banner
Appetite, 1987, 9, 1-19 When Does Sucrose Increase Appetite and Adiposity? ISRAEL RAMIREZ Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104 Two methods of sucrose feeding have been employed in studies with rodents. In the nutritional method,part or all of the starchin a diet isreplaced with sucrose. In the solution method,animals maintained on a nutritionally complete diet are given a sucrose solution to drink. The solution methodis generally a more effectiveand reliable method of producing obesity except for weanling rodents. These two methods yield different results with regardto interactionswith the fat and protein content of the diet, efficiency of weight gain,disaccharide effects and effects of meal feeding. It is suggested that for the nutritional method,sucrose altersfood intake and adiposity via its effects on fat oxidation. For the solution method, the critical factor may be presenting a wet source of caloriesrather than sucrose per se. Differences in the way sucrose is fed do not account for all divergent results. Different investigators conducting similarexperiments have often obtaineddiffer- ent results. For these andother reasons, animal studies do not supportthe ideathat sucrose intake causes obesity in humans. It is widely believed that consumption of sucrose may predispose human beings to obesity. This idea has been discussed in scientific publications (e.g. Fullerton, Getto, Swift & Carlson, 1985; Geiselman & Novin, 1982) and in popular works (e.g. Shell, 1985; Yudkin, 1972). It is therefore surprising that no comprehensive reviews of the effects of sucrose on appetite or adiposity can be found except for a brief and incomplete review (Bender & Damji, 1971). Since humans consume substantial amounts of sucrose (Anderson, 1982), it is appropriate to inquire whether there is any justification for blaming sucrose for obesity. Any such inquiry should begin with a scrutiny of previous research. The purpose of the present review is to evaluate animal research concerning sucrose and obesity. PRELIMINARY CONSIDERATIONS The present review focuses on rats and mice. This reflects the author’s opinion that controlled studies in rodents are most likely to yield information about the mechanisms by which sucrose affects appetite and adiposity. A section of this review discusses the applicability of animal research to humans. The present review is also limited to This work was supported by a National Institute of Arthritis, Diabetes, Digestive and Kidney Diseases grant AM-34375. Comments from Mary Bertino, Michael Tordoff, Mark Friedman and Anne Ramirez improved this manuscript. Requests for reprints should be addressed to: Israel Ramirez, Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, U.S.A. 0195~6663/87/.040001+ 19 $03.00/O ((7 1987 Academic Press Limited
19

When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

Mar 08, 2023

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

Appetite, 1987, 9, 1-19

When Does Sucrose Increase Appetite and Adiposity?

ISRAEL RAMIREZ Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104

Two methods of sucrose feeding have been employed in studies with rodents. In the nutritional method, part or all of the starch in a diet is replaced with sucrose. In the solution method, animals maintained on a nutritionally complete diet are given a sucrose solution to drink. The solution method is generally a more effective and reliable method of producing obesity except for weanling rodents. These two methods yield different results with regard to interactions with the fat and protein content of the diet, efficiency of weight gain, disaccharide effects and effects of meal feeding. It is suggested that for the nutritional method, sucrose alters food intake and adiposity via its effects on fat oxidation. For the solution method, the critical factor may be presenting a wet source of calories rather than sucrose per se. Differences in the way sucrose is fed do not account for all divergent results. Different investigators conducting similar experiments have often obtained differ- ent results. For these and other reasons, animal studies do not support the idea that sucrose intake causes obesity in humans.

It is widely believed that consumption of sucrose may predispose human beings to obesity. This idea has been discussed in scientific publications (e.g. Fullerton, Getto, Swift & Carlson, 1985; Geiselman & Novin, 1982) and in popular works (e.g. Shell, 1985; Yudkin, 1972). It is therefore surprising that no comprehensive reviews of the effects of sucrose on appetite or adiposity can be found except for a brief and incomplete review (Bender & Damji, 1971). Since humans consume substantial amounts of sucrose (Anderson, 1982), it is appropriate to inquire whether there is any justification for blaming sucrose for obesity. Any such inquiry should begin with a scrutiny of previous research. The purpose of the present review is to evaluate animal research concerning sucrose and obesity.

PRELIMINARY CONSIDERATIONS

The present review focuses on rats and mice. This reflects the author’s opinion that controlled studies in rodents are most likely to yield information about the mechanisms by which sucrose affects appetite and adiposity. A section of this review discusses the applicability of animal research to humans. The present review is also limited to

This work was supported by a National Institute of Arthritis, Diabetes, Digestive and Kidney Diseases grant AM-34375.

Comments from Mary Bertino, Michael Tordoff, Mark Friedman and Anne Ramirez improved this manuscript.

Requests for reprints should be addressed to: Israel Ramirez, Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, U.S.A.

0195~6663/87/.040001+ 19 $03.00/O ((7 1987 Academic Press Limited

Page 2: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

2 I. RAMIREZ

experiments lasting longer than one day. While effects that appear in brief tests may provide useful clues about the regulation of appetite, it is likely that only effects that appear over many days can significantly affect adiposity.

The ideal study would be one in which animals were given nutritionally adequate diets that differed only in the relative proportion of sucrose to starch. In the present review this is called the “nutritional method”. In the author’s opinion, this is the only kind of approach that can unambiguously establish an effect of sucrose. Some studies have compared diets composed of various grains and other complex nutrients (“chow” or “stock” diets) to semisynthetic diets containing sucrose. The latter type of experiment has been excluded because it is impossible to tell whether sucrose or some other factor is responsible for any differences between animals fed such diets.

A large number of studies have been conducted in which animals were fed a complete solid diet with or without a sucrose solution. In the present review, this is called the “solution method”, An advantage of this method is that animals can select the proportion of sucrose calories that they prefer to ingest. However,,the experimenter has less control over how much sucrose is ingested. Furthermore, changes in sucrose consumption usually result in changes in consumption of protein, fat, fiber, minerals and vitamins. One of the purposes of this review is to suggest that the changes in appetite and obesity seen when sucrose solutions are offered are not due to sucrose per se.

A wide variety of measures of adiposity have been used. The most common methods are weights of selected adipose tissue depots (usually gonadal and/or retroperitoneal) or total body fat content; sometimes the results have been expressed as the ratio of fat weight to total animal weight. The present review does not attempt to evaluate the appropriateness of most such measures. Body water cannot be used as an index of obesity because two studies presented data indicating that body water correlates poorly with body fat (Al-Nagdy, Miller & Yudkin, 1970; MacRae, Nickel, Slinger & Neudoerffer, 1974 b). Liver fat content has not been used as an index of adiposity. For the purposes of this review, obesity is defined as any increase (compared to controls) in body fat or per cent body fat. Similarly, overweight is defined as any increase in total body weight.

The title reflects the reviewer’s assumption that obesity is correlated with altered appetite; an assumption which may be disputed (Slattery & Potter, 1985). Sucrose might increase obesity without affecting food intake by increasing energetic efficiency or by reducing lean body mass. Energetic efficiency has been assessed in a number of studies, usually by computing weight gain divided by calories consumed. This method is inadequate; the appropriate method is to calculate the increase in body energy content and divide by caloric intake (Rothwell & Stock, 1984). Since completely satisfactory studies are rare, data on efficiency of weight gain are described without any criticism of methodology.

NUTRITIONAL METHOD

Comparisons of rodents fed diets containing sucrose vs. starch have produced inconsistent results (see Table 1). Sometimes sucrose feeding increased weight gain, adiposity, and food intake, whereas sometimes the reverse effects were found. Counting positive and negative findings suggests that sucrose feeding leads to obesity more often than not (see counts in Table 1). Counting the studies in Table 1 also leads to the

Page 3: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 3

surprising conclusion that sucrose in the diet does not usually increase food intake. Table 1 must be interpreted cautiously because of a probable tendency of experimen- ters to publish more papers when sucrose shows positive effects than when sucrose has no effect. Individual laboratories tend to obtain similar results upon replication, resulting in a bias toward positive results in the literature.

Table 1 does not include all studies of the effects of sucrose compared to starch feeding. Studies in which the results were equivocal or in which the feeding period was shorter than two weeks were excluded. Older studies on the interactions between sucrose and essential nutrients have not been adequately represented; fortunately this literature has been reviewed by Harper & Elvehjem (1957). The net effect of eliminating such studies is to slightly underrepresent studies showing that sucrose has no or negative effects.

Variables Influencing Results

Many of the studies included in the table involve weanling rats. The age of the animals in studies using the nutritional method does not seem to be an important factor except for low-protein diets. One group of investigators obtained similar results with weanling and mature rats (Al-Nagdy, Miller, Qureshi & Yudkin, 1966; Al-Nagdy et al., 1970). Moser and Berdanier (1974) found that the type of carbohydrate fed from weaning up to 50 days of age did not affect subsequent growth response to sucrose or starch diets. On the other hand, interactions between type of carbohydrate and essential nutrients are likely to be greater for immature rats because their nutritional needs are so great (Harper & Elvehjem, 1957).

Sucrose does not produce obesity when the diet is low in protein (Marshall, Hildebrand, DuPont & Womack, 1969). Indeed, low-protein (9-l 1%) high-sucrose diets usually reduce weight gain, food intake and adiposity when fed to weanling rats but not when fed to adult rats (Harper & Katayama, 1953; Harper, Monson, Arata, Benton & Elvehjem, 1953; Harper & Spivey, 1958; Romberg & Benton, 1965; Weiner et al., 1963). This effect of sucrose seems to be due to reduced food intake (Weiner, Yoshida & Harper, 1963); pair feeding eliminates this effect (Harper et al., 1953). Conversely, for adequate protein diets, sucrose induced obesity is sometimes partly due to increased energetic efficiency-that is, an increased ratio of body weight or calories in the carcass to calories consumed (Back & Angel, 1982; Marshall et al., 1959). Indeed, sucrose-fed rats may show greater weight gain than starch- or glucose-fed rats even when the sucrose-fed rats are prevented from overeating (Feyder, 1935; MacRae et al., 1974 b). Only rarely does sucrose reduce energetic efficiency (Williams & Szepesi, 1983).

A number of nutritional studies tested the effects of meal feeding, limiting food intake to one to three hours a day. Meal feeding usually exaggerates sucrose enhancement of growth and appetite (Gardner & Michaelis, 1979; Jorgensen, Hallfrisch & Reiser, 1978; Michaelis, Scholfield, Gardner & Cataland, 1980; Reiser & Hallfrisch, 1977), although Harper and Spivey (1958) did not observe such an effect. This result is surprising because the greater osmotic pressure of sucrose compared to starch in the stomach would lead one to predict that sucrose ought to decrease rather than increase food intake in meal-fed rats (Harper & Spivey, 1958). The mechanisms mediating this meal feeding effect merit more attention.

Most nutritional studies involve rather extreme changes in diet; typically all of the carbohydrate is either sucrose or corn starch. MacRae et al. (1974 b) found that a diet containing as little as 40% sucrose stimulated weight gain, although 62% of the

Page 4: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

TA

BL

E 1

Ef

lect

s of

suc

rose

on

bod

y w

eigh

t, fo

od

inta

ke a

nd a

dipo

sity

P

Ref

eren

ce

Age

” W

eigh

tb

Food

A

dipo

sity

C

omm

ents

C

Al-

Nag

dy

et a

l. (1

966)

A

l-N

agdy

et

al.

(19

70)

Ang

elic

0 et

al.

(19

70)

Bac

k &

Ang

el (

1982

) B

erda

nier

(1

974)

B

erda

nier

&

Bur

reli

(198

0)

Ber

dani

er

et a

l. (1

979)

B

hath

ena

et a

l. (

l986

) B

ruck

dorf

er

et a

l. (

1972

) C

ohen

&

Tei

telb

aum

(1

964)

C

ohen

8c

Tei

telb

aum

(1

966)

C

ohen

et

al.

(19

72)

Dul

loo

et a

l. (

1985

) E

isa

8c Y

udki

n (1

985)

Fo

urni

er

et a

l. (1

986)

G

ardn

er

& M

icha

elis

(1

979)

H

allf

risc

h et

al.

(19

78)

Hal

lfri

sch

et a

l. (

198

1)

Har

per

& K

atay

ama

(195

3)

Har

per

et a

l. (1

953)

H

arpe

r &

Spi

vey

(195

8)

Jorg

ense

n et

al.

(19

78)

Kan

g et

al.

(19

82)

Lak

shm

ann

et a

l. (1

981)

L

aube

et

al.

(19

73)

Lau

be

et a

l. (

1978

) M

acR

ae

et a

l. (

1974

b)

Mar

shal

l et

al.

(19

59)

Mar

shal

l et

al.

(19

69)

McC

uske

r et

al.

(19

83)

Mic

hael

is

et a

l. (1

980)

W,M

W

,M

I W

W

W

W

W

W

I I I W

W

I I W

W

W

W

I W

M

W

W

W

I W

M

W

I

- - + + + + +,o

0 0 -1

0

- - 0 0 0 + +

+

-7

0 - - 0 0 0 0 + -t

+,o

0 +

0 0 + 0 - 0

0, +

- 0 + +,o

0,

-

+,o

- 0 +,o

0 0 + + + + + + + +,o

+,o

Mod

erat

e-

and

low

-pro

tein

di

ets

No

effe

ct o

f pr

emat

ure

wea

ning

Adr

enal

ecto

miz

ed

and

norm

al

rats

Fo

r W

ista

r bu

t no

t B

HE

ra

ts

Eff

ect

seen

in

lean

bu

t no

t ob

ese

rats

Wei

ght

redu

ced

only

fo

r 67

% s

ucro

se

diet

11

% a

nd

18%

pro

tein

di

ets

Rat

s an

d m

ice

Ref

ined

an

d un

refi

ned

suga

r E

ffec

t at

8 w

eeks

but

no

t at

5,

6, a

nd

10 w

eeks

of

age

Eff

ects

big

ger

in m

eal-

fed

than

ad

lib

&ur

n fe

d le

an

and

fatty

ra

ts

Hig

h-fa

t di

ets

Wei

ght

redu

ced

only

fo

r lo

w-p

rote

in

diet

s L

ow-p

rote

in

diet

s R

ats

mea

l fe

d E

ffec

ts b

igge

r in

mea

l-fe

d ra

ts

BH

E

and

Wis

tar

rats

up

to 4

00 d

ays

of a

ge o

n hi

gh-p

rote

in

diet

N

o di

sacc

hari

de

effe

ct

Food

in

take

lim

ited

to t

hat

of s

tarc

h gr

oup

Bod

y fa

t m

easu

red

at 4

00 d

ays

of a

ge

Eff

ects

see

n w

ith h

igh-

bu

t no

t fo

r lo

w-p

rote

in

diet

s W

ista

r bu

t no

t B

HE

ra

ts d

ecre

ased

in

take

w

ith s

ucro

se

Eff

ects

big

ger

in m

eal-

fed

than

ad

ii&

turn

fed

lean

an

d fa

tty

rats

Page 5: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

Mic

hael

is

et ~

1. (1

981)

M

icha

elis

et

al.

(198

3)

Mic

hacl

is

et a

l. (1

984)

M

icha

elis

et

al

. (19

86)

Mos

er

& B

erda

nier

(1

974)

N

aim

et

al.

(198

5)

Qur

eshi

et

al.

(197

0)

Rei

ser

& H

allf

risc

h (1

977)

R

ombe

rg

& B

ento

n (1

965)

R

euss

ner

et a

l. (1

963)

W

illia

ms

& S

zepe

si

(198

3)

W

W

1 W

W

W”M

ti W

W

M

Wei

ner

et a

l. (1

963)

W

W

inan

d &

Chr

isto

phe

(197

7)

M

+ + + + + 0 0 + _ 0 + _

0 +

Hyp

erte

nsiv

e an

d no

rmal

ra

ts

show

ed

disa

ccha

ride

ef

fect

-t

- L

ean

and

corp

ulen

t ra

ts

Lea

n an

d co

rpul

ent

rats

Lea

n an

d co

rpul

ent

rats

0 H

igh-

an

d lo

w-f

at

diet

s E

ssen

tial

fatty

ac

id

defi

cien

t di

ets

+

Eff

ects

bi

gger

in

m

eal-

fed

rats

-

Low

pr

otei

n di

ets

Dif

fere

nt

star

ches

co

mpa

red

+

Lar

ge

appa

rent

ef

fect

s no

t al

way

s st

atis

tical

ly

sign

ific

ant;

ad l

ibit

um an

d m

eal

fed

- F&

e di

ffer

ent

prot

eins

us

ed

- O

bese

an

d no

rmal

m

ze

Tot

al

num

ber

of f

indi

ngs:

Po

sitiv

e N

o di

ffer

ence

N

egat

ive

19

6 14

17

13

7

12

5 5

“Age

at

begi

nnin

g of

exp

erim

ent

W w

eani

ng,

I in

term

edia

te

(nea

r 1O

Og f

or r

ats)

, M

mat

ure.

b D

irec

tion

of e

ffec

t: +

, su

cros

e db

t gr

eate

r th

an

star

ch

or d

extr

in

diet

; -,

sucr

ose

diet

les

s th

an

star

ch

or d

extr

in

diet

: 0,

no

appa

rent

di

ffer

ence

bet

wee

n di

ets.

c A

nim

als

wer

e ra

ts u

nles

s in

dica

ted.

Page 6: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

6 I. RAMIREZ

carbohydrate was sucrose. Other studies suggest that large amounts of sucrose are needed to produce any effect. Cohen and Teitelbaum (1964) used diets containing 33,40 and 67% sugar; only the 67% sugar diet reliably reduced weight gain. Laube, Wojcikowski, Shatz & Pfeiffer (1978), found no significant differences in body weight among rats fed diets containing 20, 50 or 70% sucrose. Naim, Brand, Kare and Carpenter (1985) found that feeding rats 25% sucrose diets had no reliable effects on food intake, weight gain, or obesity. Nearly all studies reporting significant effects of sucrose on weight, obesity, or food intake used diets containing at least 40% of total calories as sucrose. This issue is important because human beings do not usually ingest diets as high in sucrose as have been fed to rats (Anderson, 1982).

Other Sugars

Theoretically, comparisons of sucrose with other sugars can give some clues about the mechanism of action of sucrose. If, for example, glucose diets produce effects similar to those observed with sucrose, then it may be inferred that the fructose component of sucrose is not necessary. In practice, such studies have not been as enlightening as might be desired.

Diets using glucose as the carbohydrate source may induce less weight gain than sucrose diets (Allen & Leahy, 1966; Berdanier, 1974; Feyder, 1935; MacRae et al., 1974a,b; Williams & Szepesi, 1983), similar weight gain (MacRae et al., 1974a; Waterman, Romsos, Tsai, Miller & Leveille, 1975) or even greater weight gain (Winand & Christophe, 1977). It is difficult to evaluate reports on the effects of glucose feeding on food intake because it is not always clear whether hydrous or anhydrous glucose was used, making it difficult to compute caloric intake. In two studies, fructose-containing diets resulted in food intake and weight gains similar to that seen with sucrose- containing diets (Allen & Leahy, 1966; Reussner, Andros & Thiessen, 1963). However, Winand and Christophe (1977), MacRae et al. (1974a) and Waterman et al. (1975) found that high-fructose diets produced less weight gain than high-sucrose diets.

Some evidence suggest that disaccharides (e.g. maltose and sucrose) have greater effects than monosaccharides (e.g. glucose and fructose). Maltose containing diets produce weight gains, food intake, and adiposity as great as or possibly greater than that produced by diets containing sucrose (Michaelis et al., 1978; Reussner et al., 1963; Williams & Szepesi, 1983). An even more suggestive finding is that sucrose based diets sometimes produce greater weight gain or adiposity than diets containing glucose and fructose in equimolar amounts (Michaelis et al., 1978, 1980, 1981; Williams & Szepesi, 1983), although Laube, Klor, Fussganger and Pfeiffer (1973) did not find such an effect. The disaccharide effect provides a valuable clue about the mechanism for sucrose- induced obesity; this matter is discussed in the section on mechanisms.

Can Contradictory Results be Explained?

It is possible that sucrose interacts with other components of the diet in nutritional studies. As described above, level of dietary protein is one such factor, although large changes in protein content of the diet sometimes do not alter the results appreciably (Al-Nagdy et al., 1966; Cohen & Teitelbaum, 1966). Type of protein used does not seem to be a major factor when due allowance is made for differences in protein quality; greater amounts of low quality proteins are needed to promote growth. One group of researchers observed reduced growth and appetite in rats fed sucrose diets containing

Page 7: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 7

casein, fibrin, egg albumin, soyabean protein, wheat gluten and a mixture of amino acids (Harper et al., 1953; Weiner et al., 1963). On the other hand, Marshall et al. (1969) found that sucrose diets containing either lactalbumin or wheat gluten increased weight gain and adiposity.

Type of starch and percentage of carbohydrate in the diet might influence the results obtained, but nearly all researchers used corn starch and SO-SO% carbohydrate. There appear to be slight differences between cooked and raw cornstarch but these differences are not large enough to account for the discrepancies among different studies (Harper & Katayama, 1953; Reussner et al., 1963).

The amount of dietary fat does not seem to be an important factor. In one study, sucrose stimulated growth when the diet contained either 3 or 15% fat (Marshall et al., 1959). Hallfrisch, Cohen and Reiser (1981) observed that greater obesity was produced when their 40% fat diet contained sucrose rather than starch. Naim et al. (1985) did not find any reliable effects of using sucrose in either high- or low-fat diets.

The type of carbohydrate used influences nutritional requirements for certain vitamins, apparently by altering growth of intestinal microflora (Harper & Elvehjem, 1957). It is worth noting that although commercial sucrose is a highly purified product, corn starch is not. Corn starch contains significant amounts of free fatty acids (Subcommittee on Laboratory Animal Nutrition, 1978) and much more ash than sucrose (Ramirez, unpublished work). Since investigators have used a wide variety of vitamin and mineral mixes in their diets, it is possible that some investigators used diets which are nutritionally adequate when one carbohydrate is used, but not adequate when another carbohydrate is used.

It might be that different investigators used different strains of rats and mice which respond differently to sucrose. There are reports that two of the most commonly used strains of rats, Wistar and BHE, differ from each other in response to sucrose and starch diets (Berdanier, Tobin & DeVofe, 1979; McCusker, Deaver & Berdanier, 1983). In order to determine why different strains give different results, it will first be necessary to identify the physiological and biochemical effects of sucrose which increase and decrease food intake, body weight and adiposity.

It must be concluded that, at present, it is not possible to say why published studies using the nutritional method have been so inconsistent. The most promising variables to investigate in future studies would seem to be heredity and micronutrients.

SOLUTION METHOD

No attempt has been made to cite all or even most studies employing the solution method because the purpose of this section is to compare studies using the solution method to others employing the nutritional method.

Studies employing the solution method usually show that sucrose increases caloric intake, weight gain and adiposity (Kanarek & Hirsch, 1977). Rats given sugar solutions reduce intake of solid food, partially compensating for the calories from the sugar. Early studies emphasized this compensation even though total caloric intake was slightly elevated by sugar availability (Collier & Bolles, 1968; Hausmann, 1933; Rytand, 1~943). Kanarek and Hirsch (1977) were apparently the first researchers to realize that offering rats sugar solutions provided a potentially useful animal model of obesity.

Page 8: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

8 I. RAMIREZ

Studies using the solution method are similar to those using the nutritional method in one important respect: in most cases the percentage of calories from sucrose is usually high. However, the percentage is variable between experiments; values as high as 67% (Bukowiecki, Lupien, Follea & Jahjah, 1983) and as low as 20% (Trout, Moy, Putney & Johnson, 1978) have been reported in studies in which sucrose increased caloric intake and adiposity.

Negative results can be summarized fairly easily. It appears to be difficult to produce obesity by feeding sucrose solutions to weanling rats (Hirsch, Dubose & Jacobs, 1982; Muto & Miyahara, 1972; Rothwell 8z Stock, 1984). However, even in weanling rats, slightly increased adiposity can be shown by carcass analysis (Kanarek & Marks-Kaufman, 1979). One strain of rats (S 5B/Pl) that is resistant to other kinds of dietary obesity is also resistant to sucrose solution obesity (Schemmel et al., 1982). There are a few reports that availability of sucrose solution had no effect on total caloric intake (Kenney & Collier, 1976; Friedhoff, Simon & Friedhoff, 197 1; Teague, Kanarek, Bray, Glick & Orthen-Gambill, 1981) and two reports that sucrose had no effect on caloric intake although it increased weight gain (Hill et al., 1980, Sclafani & Xenakis, 1984 a, b). These failures to find differences are rare.

There are only three situations in which sugar solutions seem to significantly decrease total caloric intake and body weight. Providing a sucrose (Hamilton, 1971) or glucose (Sclafani, 1973) solution to meal-fed rats during the meal can result in substantial weight loss and even death. Presentation of sucrose solution to rats fed high-fat diets resulted in either no change in body weight and adiposity (40 and 60% fat diets) or even reduced weight and adiposity (80% fat diet, Rattigan & Clark, 1984). When weanling rats are fed a low-protein diet, presentation of a sucrose solution can reduce caloric intake and growth (Muto & Miyahara, 1972). The first two findings are interesting because they are completely contrary to what has been found for the nutritional method (see above).

Degree of Obesity

Sclafani (1978) and Sclafani and Xenakis (1984a) suggested that the solution method is a more effective method of producing obesity than the nutritional method. In order to verify this claim, I selected all available studies in which feeding sucrose ad libitum increased adiposity. A simple index was used to assess the degree of obesity produced, the ratio of adiposity of sucrose-fed to adiposity of control animals. In studies reporting more than one measure of adiposity, the ratios for different measures were averaged. Only groups fed ad libitum and showing a positive and statistically significant effect were used. Nine studies reported that feeding sucrose solution increased body fat content (Bukowiecki et al., 1983; Faust, Johnson, Stern & Hirsch, 1978; Granneman & Wade, 1983; Kanarek & Marks-Kaufman, 1979; Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982; Trout et al., 1978; Rattigen & Clark, 1984; Schemmel et al.. 1982; Vasselli, Haraczkiewicz & Pi-Sunyer, 1982). The average ratio is 1.65 kO.13 with a range of 1.12 to 2.26. Thirteen studies reported that ad Zibitum feeding of sucrose- based diets increased body fat (Bhathena, Revett, Michaelis, Ellwood, Voyles & Recant, 1986; Hallfrisch et al., 1978,198l; Gardner 8z Michaelis, 1979; Jorgensen et al., 1978; Laube et al., 1973; Marshall et al., 1959, 1969; Michaelis et al., 1980, 1983, 1986; Reiser & Hallfrisch, 1977; Williams & Szepesi, 1983). The average ratio of sucrose fed to

Page 9: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 9

control rats for these studies is 1.31 kO.03 with a range of 1.17 to 1.57. A Mann-- Whitney U test indicated a significant effect of method of feeding sucrose (U = 25, p <O-05). This analysis minimized the difference between the two methods because groups showing no effect of ad libitum sucrose on adiposity were excluded; this occurred more often for the nutritional (11 times, see Table 1) than for the solution method (three times, Muto & Miyahara, 1972; Rattigan & Clark, 1984; Schemmel et al., 1982). It seems safe to conclude that the solution method produces more severe obesity than does the nutritional method.

That sucrose solution availability can produce obesity at all is surprising for two reasons. First, offering a sucrose solution increases energy expenditure (Rothwell & Stock, 1984). This is probably due to increased size and/or the metabolic activity of brown adipose tissue (Bukowiecki et al., 1983; Granneman & Wade, 1983; Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982; Teague et al., 1981; Rothwell & Stock, 1984) as well as increased activity of Na,K-ATPase (Flier et al., 1981). Increased thermogenesis resulting from feeding a sucrose solution decreases efficiency of weight gain (Bukowiecki et al., 1983; Granneman & Wade, 1983). These metabolic changes would lead one to expect reduced rather than increased weight gain and adiposity. This evidence for decreased metabolic efficiency sharply contrasts with results obtained with the nutritional method which suggest that sucrose feeding increases efficiency of weight gain (see above).

Secondly, offering a sucrose solution in addition to solid food results in reduced intake of protein and other essential nutrients. For example, Kanarek and Hirsch (1977) estimated that animals given sucrose solutions consumed only 10% of their calories in the form of protein whereas control rats consumed a 23% protein diet. Sucrose solution availability can increase total caloric intake even when the solid diet contains as little as 5% protein (Castonguay, Rowland & Stern, 1985). Given the nutritional studies showing that low protein intake reduces food intake and weight gain of sucrose-fed rats, it is remarkable that offering a sucrose solution is such a good method of producing obesity.

Other Sugars

Studies using the nutritional method tend to show that different sugars produce different results; disaccharides tended to produce greater weight gain and adiposity then monosaccharides (see above). Studies using the solution method reliably show that glucose and sucrose produce similar results (Castonguay & Hirsch, 1981; Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982; Trout et al., 1978). Only one study compared fructose and sucrose solutions; in this case fructose had the same effects on total caloric intake and obesity as sucrose and glucose (Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982). However, less fructose solution was consumed than sucrose solution. Another study compared fructose and glucose solutions but did not include control rats given no solution; weight gain was unaffected by the type of sugar in the solution given to four strains of rats (Aoyama et al., 1980). There is no evidence for a disaccharide effect in any study using the solution method; monosaccharides such as glucose and fructose have the same effects as sucrose.

Surprisingly, allowing rats to drink polysaccharide solutions also produces overeating, overweight, and obesity (Sclafani and Xenakis, 1984 a, b). Indeed, feeding a polpsacchqide solution might be a better method of producing obesity than feeding sugar in any form since the ratio of adiposity in rats given polysaccharide solution to

Page 10: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

10 I. RAMIREZ

adiposity in control rats was 2.9 (Sclafani & Xenakis, 1984 b), greater than that obtained in any other report in this review (see above for method of computation and other values). This finding suggested to the reviewer that obesity might be produced by any nutritive solution regardless of its sugar content.

HEREDITARY OBESITY AND SUCROSE FEEDING

Nutritional Method

It would be of interest to know the effects of hereditary susceptibility to obesity on response to sucrose feeding. Using the nutritional method, Winand and Christophe (1977) found that both normal and ob/ob mice gained less weight and became less fat when fed sugar-based diets compared to a starch based diet. On the other hand, Michaelis et al. (1983, 1984) found that sucrose feeding increased body weight and fat pad weight but not food intake in both normal and cp/cp rats. Michaelis et al. (1980) found that sucrose content of the diet had little or no effect on food intake, body weight and adiposity of ad libitum fed Zucker Fatty and lean rats. However, both genotypes showed bigger sucrose effects when they were meal fed. Both Zucker Fatty and lean rats responded to sucrose in a qualitatively similar way (Michaelis et al., 1980).

Solution Method

Studies using the solution method are equally inconsistent. Ramirez and Sprott found that yellow obese mice (1979) and ob/ob mice (1978) usually regulated caloric intake as well as or better than normal mice when given sucrose or glucose solutions to drink. The one exception was that young yellow mice (6-7 weeks of age) increased caloric intake more than did normal mice of the same age when given sucrose solutions to drink (Ramirez & Sprott, 1979). On the other hand, Flier et al. (1981) reported that whereas normal mice increased caloric intake by 27%, ob/ob mice increased caloric intake by 80% when given sucrose solution to drink; the age of the animals was described as 6 to 12 weeks of age. Zucker Fatty rats appear to regulate caloric intake as well as or better than lean rats when given a sucrose solution to drink (Castonguay et al., 1985). All the above studies used obese mutants. One study compared two unrelated strains of rats, one susceptible to high-fat diet induced obesity and one resistant. As might be expected, the susceptible strain (Osborne-Mendel) increased body weight and fat when given sucrose, whereas the resistant strain (S 5B/Pl) did not (Schemmel etal., 1982). Overall, there does not seem to be any clear relationship between hereditary obesity and susceptibility to sucrose-induced obesity.

MECHANISM OF ACTION: PREVIOUS SPECULATIONS

Very little attention has been given to understanding the mechanisms by which sugars alter appetite, weight gain and obesity. This section briefly summarizes some possibilities. Fullerton et al. (1985), Geiselman and Novin (1982) and Kanarek and Hirsch (1977) have discussed possible mechanisms in more detail.

The most obvious explanation for overconsumption of sweet foods is that rodents find sweet foods highly palatable. It is difficult to find much data supporting or

Page 11: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 11

contradicting this notion. There does not seem to be a linear correlation between the sweetness of a sugar and its effects on appetite or obesity (see earlier). Adding saccharin to glucose solutions increases glucose intake but does not increase total caloric intake (Smith & Foster, 1980). The only evidence suggesting that sweet taste may play a role is that adding saccharin to a polysaccharide solution increased calorie intake and weight gain, although this effect was not significant (Sclafani & Xenakis, 1984).

Geiselman and Novin (1982) have suggested that consumption of sugar leads to hyperinsulinemia which in turn produces hypoglycemia which increases consumption of carbohydrate leading once again to hyperinsulinemia etc. These writers present a great deal of evidence consistent with this conjecture. The most serious difficulty with their idea is the lack of direct evidence for such a vicious cycle.

Fullerton et al. (1985) argued that interactions between sugar and brain neurotrans- mitters may be involved in sugar’s effects on obesity. At the present time, there are insufficient data to judge the validity of this hypothesis.

Almost no attention has been given_ to speculations about why sugars might decrease appetite. Harper and Spivey (1958) have suggested that the osmotic pressure exerted by sugars may modulate consumption of diets containing sugars. They present data indicating an inverse correlation between the osmotic pressure that carbohydrates exert in solution and consumption of diets containing these carbohydrates in meal-fed rats. This idea could be extended to allow for differential rates of absorption of various sugars; those sugars which are absorbed slowly might exert greater osmotic effects in the intestinal tract but not in the stomach. For example, fructose is absorbed relatively slowly (Vrana 8z Fabry, 1983); this slow absorption might account for the reduced growth sometimes seen when it is the only carbohydrate in the diet (see above). Vasselli et al. (1982) have shown that slowing the absorption of sucrose pharmacologically blocks the obesity-inducing effects of a sucrose solution. This result might also be explained by the osmotic effects of unabsorbed sucrose in the gastrointestinal tract. The osmotic hypothesis, like the others, remains highly speculative.

MECHANISMSOF ACTION: ALTERNATIVE APPROACHES

Nutritional Method

Analyzing the mechanisms that operate in studies using the nutritional method will be difficult. One possible approach would be to consider the effects of sucrose on fat storage and oxidation. One theory of the regulation of food intake claims that oxidation of metabolic fuels inhibits food intake whereas storage of fuels in adipose tissue limits the availability of fuels for oxidation and hence stimulates food intake (Friedman & Stricker, 1976; Friedman & Ramirez, 1985). Sucrose feeding stimulates lipid synthesis by the liver (Back &Angel, 1982; Vrana & Fabry, 1983; Waterman et al., 1975) and hence might be expected to enhance fuel storage, thereby stimulating appetite. On the other hand, sucrose can reduce lipid synthesis in adipose tissue (Waterman et al., 1975) thereby decreasing adiposity and food intake. Whether sucrose increases or decreases appetite and adiposity may depend on the balance between these two opposing actions of sucrose. If this idea is correct, it would not be surprising that studies have produced inconsistent results.

The existence of a disaccharide effect supports the present hypothesis. Disac- charides that increase body weight and adiposity more than monosaccharides also increase levels of hepatic lipogenic enzymes (Michaelis & Szepesi, 1977; Michaelis et al.,

Page 12: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

12 I. RAMIREZ

1978, 1980) without having any consistent effect on the levels of the same enzymes in adipose tissue (Michaelis et al., 1978). Meal feeding obliterated the disaccharide effect for liver lipogenic enzymes as well as for relative epididymal adipose tissue weight (Michaelis et al., 1980). That is, for meal-fed rats sucrose had the same effects as an equimolar mixture of glucose and fructose. Thus, changes in lipogenic enzymes might be correlated with changes in adipose tissue size.

The theoretical framework proposed here suggests that the best way to analyze the effects of sucrose on appetite is to examine the effects of sucrose on storage and oxidation of metabolic fuels. In my opinion this seems to be the most promising avenue for future research.

Solution Method

Presentation of sucrose solution alters several nutritional variables simultaneously (e.g. hydration of food, variety of foods available, intake of protein, fat, fiber, vitamins and minerals, etc.). It is very easy to control intake of essential nutrients by including them in the sucrose solution. This is equivalent to offering a liquid diet identical to the solid diet except that the liquid diet contains sucrose and water. Recent studies employing this approach have produced surprising results (Ramirez, 1986). Rats given a liquid diet containing sucrose in addition to a solid diet, consumed more calories than rats given a solid diet alone, regardless of the sucrose content of the solid diet. Prolonged feeding of the liquid diet increased total caloric intake and weight gain in comparison to feeding of the solid diet alone. Variety did not seem to be a major factor because rats given liquid diet alone consumed more calories and gained more weight than rats given only a solid diet. Most surprising of all, similar effects were seen when the liquid diet contained corn starch instead of sucrose. These findings suggest that overweight induced by sucrose solutions may be the result of offering rats a hydrated rather than a dry food source.

The notion that water rather than sucrose is the critical factor might seem difficult to accept since laboratory animals are commonly given plain drinking water ad libitum. However, two lines of evidence support this hypothesis. Adding water to sucrose results in increased intake of sucrose (Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982; Sclafani & Kramer, 1983) and decreased efficiency of weight gain (Kanarek & Orthen-Gambill, 1982). Secondly, adding water to rat diets can increase caloric intake and weight gain (e.g. Adkins et al., 1967; Bernardis & Bellinger, 1982; Keane, Smutko, Krieger & Denton, 1962,1963) and even adiposity (Adkins et al., 1967). Adding water to a low protein diet can improve efficiency of protein utilization even when the rats fed wet diets are pair fed to rats fed a dry diet (Keane et al., 1963). Most studies of wet foods employed diets containing little or no sugar. Human beings commonly add water to grains before consuming them; these same grains are fed to rats in dry form in their stock diets. It is conceivable that adding water to a food before it is consumed alters the palatability or digestion of the food. It is even possible that adding water to foods alters the way they are digested, stored or oxidized. This subject deserves more attention.

RELEVANCE TO HUMANS

One of the primary reasons for conducting the research cited above was to gain insight into how sucrose might affect humans. Just how this insight can be applied,

Page 13: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 13

however, is rarely addressed. It is not appropriate to simply assume that humans will respond to diets in the same way as animals. In the author’s opinion, it is possible to make inferences about humans from animal data if two preconditions can be satisfied: (1) the situations in which the phenomena occurs in animals also occur in humans, and (2) the physiological mechanisms involved are similar in animals and humans. The first precondition requires one to have a good understanding of the conditions necessary for sucrose to increase adiposity and appetite. This precondition is not satisfied since it is unclear why sucrose does not affect appetite and adiposity in a consistent manner. The second precondition requires one to demonstrate which physiological and biochemical processes are involved in sucrose’s effects on appetite and adiposity, and whether the same processes can occur in humans. At present, our knowledge about mechanisms consists largely of speculation. Thus, there is no justification, at present, for applying results from animal studies to humans.

There are reasons, however, to think that animal studies may not be directly applicable to humans. First of all, most studies involved very large changes in the sugar content of the diet, typically 5@65°~ sucrose vs. a sugar-free diet. In contrast, humans in wealthier countries consume roughly l&24% of their calories as sucrose, and in poorer countries sucrose is an even less important nutrient (Anderson, 1982). Studies using the solution method almost invariably involve comparisons between rats given a single dry food and rats given the dry food and sugar solution. Humans, on the other hand, usually eat a wide variety of foods. Kratz and Levitsky (1979) reported that rats given a variety of foods to chose from did not appear to overeat or gain excess body weight when given solid sucrose whereas rats given sucrose plus a single food did overeat and gained excess body weight. It was pointed out above that the growth promoting effects of sucrose solutions may be due to presentation of a wet food source rather than to sucrose per se. Human foods, unlike laboratory rat diets, usually contain substantial amounts of water.

None of the above proves that sucrose cannot produce obesity in humans or that animal studies cannot help resolve this issue. Rather, better animal studies are needed. Animal studies have demonstrated that different carbohydrates can have very different effects on food intake, body weight and adiposity. Indeed, the only convincing evidence for this comes from animals.

CONCLUSIONS

Nutritional us Solution Method

Feeding sucrose to rats and mice often, but not always, increases their food intake, body weight and adiposity. One factor accounting for variable results is the method of feeding sucrose. For adult animals, replacing the starch in the diet with sucrose (nutritional method) has milder and less reliable effects than does offering the sucrose as a solution along with dry solid food (solution method). On the other hand, it is difficult to produce obesity in weanling rats with the solution method, whereas it has been frequently produced in weanling rats with the nutritional method. For the nutritional method, feeding low protein diets can block or even reverse sucrose’s obesity-inducing effects. For the solution method, offering sucrose can produce overeating even when

Page 14: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

14 I. RAMIREZ

low levels of protein are consumed. Meal feeding accentuates sucrose’s obesity inducing effects in studies employing the nutritional method. Giving sucrose solution to meal-fed rats can lead to reduced food intake and weight loss. The fat content of the diet has little effect on the results obtained with the nutritional method but has major effects on the results obtained with the solution method. Studies employing the nutritional method generally find that replacing sucrose with another sugar alters the results obtained; disaccharides often have greater effects than monosaccharides. Studies employing the solution method usually find similar results with different sugars; monosaccharides seem to be just as effective as the disaccharide sucrose.

It is probable that entirely different mechanisms are Involved in the two kinds of studies. For the solution method, one critical factor seems to be presentation of a hydrated source of calories in addition to dry food, rather than to sucrose per se. Presumably, wet foods are more palatable or digested differently than dry foods. Other factors, such as altered intake of essential nutrients, might also play a role. There is no evidence that the type of carbohydrate used in the solution has any effect on the results obtained. For studies employing the nutritional method, sucrose’s effects on lipo- genesis and fat storage may mediate its effects on obesity. Until more is known about mechanisms, animal data cannot be used to make inferences about whether sucrose is a causative agent in human obesity.

~elatio~s~~p between Appetite and Adiposity

Studies employing the nutritional method usually indicate that feeding sucrose increases the efficiency of weight gain. Thus, animals fed sucrose by the nutritional method tend to become fatter than would be expected on the basis of their caloric intake. Studies employing the solution method often give the reverse result. Rats given sucrose solutions to drink in addition to their usual food tend to become less obese than would be expected from the elevated food intake. It is of interest that feeding granular sucrose in addition to the usual food can result in increased feed efficiency.

Confidence in these conclusions is weakened by two factors. First, the literature is inconsistent, contrary exahples may be found for each conclusion. Second, most studies used an inadequate measure of efficiency of weight gain; the ratio of weight gained divided by calories consumed. More precise methods (e.g. Rothwell & Stock, 1984) should be applied in future studies.

Does Sucrose cause Obesity?

Inconsistencies in the literature make it difficult to reach definite conclusions about the relationship between sucrose feeding and appetite and adiposity. Different investigators conducting superficially similar experiments often fail to obtain similar results. Studies with hereditary obesity have been particularly inconsistent. Strain differences are probably one factor responsible for discrepant results. Such inconsis- tencies are a severe constraint on anyone arguing that sucrose is a major contributor to human obesity.

In conclusion, feeding rodents very large amounts of sucrose occasionally increases caloric intake sometimes causes mild obesity. Animal data provide no support for the idea that sucrose consumption is a major factor in the development of human obesity.

Page 15: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 15

REFERENCES

Adkins, J. S., Wertz, J. M., Boffman, R. H. & Hove, E. L. Influence of addition of water or ‘nonessential’ nitrogen on growth of rats fed low levels of essential l-amino acids. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology ana’ Medicine, 1967, 126, 50&504.

Allen, R. J. L. & Leahy, J. S. Some effects of dietary dextrose, fructose, liquid glucose and sucrose in the adult male rat. British Journhl of Nutrition, 1966, 20, 339-347.

Al-Nagdy, S., Miller, D. S., Qureshi, R. U. & Yudkin, J. Metabolic differences between starch and sucrose. Nature, London, 1966, 209, 81-82.

Al-Nagdy. S., Miller, D. S. & Yudkin, J. Changes in body composition and metabolism induced by sucrose in the rat. Nutrition and Merabolism, 1970, 12, 193-219.

Anderson, T. A. Recent trends in carbohydrate consumption. Annual Review of Nutrition, 1982,2, 113-132.

Angelico, R., Moretta, L., Improta, G. & Ialongo, P. L. Effects of dietary carbohydrates on body lipid composition and on some enzymatic activities in the rat. Nutrition and Metabolism, 1970,12, 179-191.

Aoyama, Y., Hattori, Y., Yoshida, A. & Ashida, K. Effect of glucose and fructose in a diet or a drinking water on body weight gain, liver lipid content and serum triglyceride level in four strains of rats. Nutrition Reports International, 1980, 22, 801-810.

Back, D. W. & Angel, J. F. Effects of premature weaning on the metabolic response to dietary sucrose in adult rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1982, Z 12, 978-985.

Bender, A. E. & Damji, K. B. Some effects of dietary sucrose. In J. Yudkin, J. Edelman & L. Hough (Eds). Chemical, biological ana’ nutritional aspects of sucrose. Pp. 172-182. London: Butterworths, 1971.

Berdanier, C. D. Metabolic characteristics of the carbohydrate-sensitive BHE strain of rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1974, 104, 1246-1256.

Berdanier, C. D. & Burrell, B. R. Effect of adrenalectomy on the responses of BHE rats to either a sucrose or starch diet. Journal of Nutrition, 1980, 110, 298-304.

Berdanier, C. D., Tobin, R. B. & DeVore, V. Effects of age, strain, and dietary carbohydrate on the hepatic metabolism of male rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1979, 109, 261-271.

Bernardis, L. L. & Bellinger, L. L. Effect of diet hydration on food and water intake, efficiency of food utilization and response to fast and realimentation in rats with dorsomedial hypothalamic hypophagia and growth retardation. Appetite, 1982, 3, 35-52.

Bhathena, S. J., Revett, K., Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Ellwood, K. C., Voyles, N. R. & Recant, L. Genetic obesity and dietary sucrose decrease hepatic glucagon and insulin receptors in LA/N-corpulent rats. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1986, 181, 71-77.

Bruckdorfer, K. R., Kari-Kari, B. P. B., Kahn, I. H. & Yudkin, J. Activity of lipogenic enzymes and plasma triglyceride levels in the rat and chicken as determined by the nature of the dietary fat and dietary carbohydrate. Nutrition und Metabolism, 1972, 14, 228-237.

Bukowiecki, L. J., Lupien, J., Follea, N. & Jahjah, L. Effects of sucrose, caffeine, and cola beverages on obesity, cold resistance, and adipose tissue cellularity. American Journal qf Physiology, 1983, 244, R5O&R507.

Cast&guay, T. W. & Hirsch, E. Palatability of sugar solutions and dietary selection. Physiology and Behavior, 1981, 27, 7-12.

Castonguay, T. W., Rowland, N. E. & Stern, J. S. Nutritional influences on dietary selection patterns of obese and lean Zucker rats. Bruin Research Bulletin, 1985, 14, 625-631.

Cohen, A. M. 8z Teitelbaum, A. Effect of dietary sucrose and starch on oral glucose tolerance and insulin-like activity. American Journal of Physiology, 1964, 206, 105-108.

Cohen, A. M. & Teitelbaum, A. Effect of different levels ofprotein in ‘sucrose’ and ‘starch’ diets on the glucose tolerance and growth. Metabolism, 1966, 15, 1034-1038.

Cohen, A. M., Briller, S. & Shafrir, E. Effect of longterm sucrose feeding on the activity of some enzymes regulating glycolysis, lipogenesis and gluconeogenesis in rat liver and adipose tissue. Biochimica et Biophysics Acta, 1972, 279, 129-138.

Collier, G. & Bolles, R. Some determinants of intake of sucrose solutions. Journal qfComparatice and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65, 379-383.

Dulloo, A. G., Eisa, 0. A., Miller, D. S. & Yudkin, J. A comparative study of the effects of white sugar, unrefined sugar and starch on the efficiency of food utilization and thermogenesis. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 1985, 42, 214-219.

Page 16: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

16 I. RAMIREZ

Eisa, 0. A. & Yudkin, J. Some nutritional properties of unrefined sugar and its promotion of the survival of new-born rats. British Journal of Nutrition, 1985, 54, 593-603.

Faust, I., Johnson, P. R., Stern, J. S. & Hirsch, J. Diet-induced adipocyte number increase in adult rats: a new model of obesity. American Journal of Physiology, 1978, 235, E279-E286.

Feyder, S. Fat formation from sucrose and glucose. Journal of Nutrition, 1935, 9, 457-468. Flier, J. S., Usher, P. & Deluise, M. Effect of sucrose overfeeding on Na,K-ATPase-mediated

86Rb uptake in normal and ob/ob mice. Diabetes, 1981, 30, 975-978. Fournier, R. D., Chiueh, C. C., Kopin, I. J., Knapka, J. J., DiPette, D. & Preuss, H. G. Refined

carbohydrate increases blood pressure and catecholamine excretion in SHR and WKY. American Journal of Physiology, 1986, 250, E381-E385.

Friedhoff, R., Simon J. A. & Friedhoff, A. J. Sucrose solution vs. no-caloric sweetner vs. water in weight gain. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 1971, 59, 4855486.

Friedman, M. I. & Ramirez, I. Relationship of fat metabolism to food intake. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 1985, 42, 1093-1098.

Friedman, M. I. & Stricker, E. M. The,physiological psychology of hunger: a physiological perspective. Psychological Review, 1976, 83, 409431.

Fullerton, D. T., Getto, C. J., Swift, W. J. & Carlson, I. H. Sugar, opioids and binge eating. Brain Research Bulletin, 1985, 14, 673-680.

Gardner, L. B. & Michaelis, 0. E. IV. Serum insulin and glucagon and hepatic and adipose cyclic-amp responses of Zucker rats fed carbohydrate diets ad libitum or in meals. Nutrition Reports International, 1979, 20, 845-854.

Geiselman, P. J. & Novin, D. The role of carbohydrates in appetite, hunger and obesity. Appetite, 1982,3,203-223.

Granneman, J. G. & Wade, G. N. Effect of sucrose overfeeding on brown adipose tissue lipogenesis and lipoprotein lipase activity in rats. Metabolism, 1983, 32, 202-207.

Hallfrisch, J., Lazar, F. L. & Reiser, S. Epididymal fat metabolism in rats fed sucrose or starch. Nutrition Reports International, 1978, 18, 359-367.

Hallfrisch, J., Cohen, L. & Reiser, S. Effects of feeding rats sucrose in a high fat diet. Journal of Nutrition, 1981, 111, 531-536.

Hamilton, L. W. Starvation induced by sucrose ingestion in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 77, 59-69.

Harper, A. E. & Elvehjem, C. A. A review of the effects of different carbohydrates on vitamin and amino acid requirements. Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry, 1957, 5, 754758.

Harper, A. E. & Katayama, M. C. The influence of various carbohydrates on the utilization of low protein rations by the white rat. I. Comparison of sucrose and cornstarch in 9% casein rations. Journal of Nutrition, 1953, 49, 261-275.

Harper, A. E. & Spivey, H. E. Relationship between food intake and osmotic effect of dietary carbohydrate. American Journal of Physiology, 1958, 193,483487.

Harper, A. E., Monson, W. J., Arata, D. A., Benton, D. A. & Elvehjem, C. A. Influence of various carbohydrates on the utilization of low protein rations by the white rat. II. Comparison of several proteins and carbohydrates on growth and liver fat. Journal of Nutrition, 1953,51, 523-537.

Hausmann, M. F. The behavior of albino rats in choosing foods. II. Differentiation between sugar and saccharin. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1933, IS, 419-428.

Hill, W., Castonguay, T. W. & Collier, G. H. Taste or diet balancing? Physiology and Behavior, 1980, 24, 765-767.

Hirsch, E., Dubose, C. & Jacobs, H. L. Overeating, dietary selection patterns and sucrose intake in growing rats. Physiology and Behavior, 1982, 28, 819-828.

Jorgensen, C., Hallfrisch, J. & Reiser, S. Effect of sucrose and feeding frequency on insulin sensitivity and lipogenic enzyme activites in rats with equivalent growth. Nutrition Reports International, 1978, 18, 87-98.

Kanarek, R. B. & Hirsch, E. Dietary-induced overeating in experimental animals. Federation Proceedings, 1977, 36, 154158.

Kanarek, R. B. & Marks-Kaufman, R. Developmental aspects of sucrose-induced obesity in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 1979, 23, 881-885.

Kanarek, R. B. & Orthen-Gambill, N. Differential effects of sucrose, fructose and glucose on carbohydrate-induced obesity in rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1982, 112, 15461554.

Kang, S. S., Fears, R., Noirot, S., Mbanya, J. N. & Yudkin, J. Changes in metabolism of rat kidney and liver caused by experimental diabetes and by dietary sucrose. Diabetologia, 1982, 22, 285-288.

Page 17: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 17

Keane, K. W., Smutko, C. J., Krieger, C. H. & Denton, A. E. The addition of water to purified diets and its effect upon growth and protein efficiency ratio in the rat. Journal of Nutrition, 1962, 77, 18-22.

Keane, K. W., Smutko, C. J., Krieger, C. H. & Denton, A. E. Water addition to purified diets. II. Effects of water and carbohydrate on protein efficiency ratio in rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1963,81, 87-90.

Kenney, J. J. & Collier, R. The effects of saccharin on the sucrose consumption of rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1976, 106, 388-391.

Kratz, C. M. & Levitsky, D. A. Dietary obesity: Differential effects with self-selection and composite diet feeding techniques. Physiology ana’ Behavior, 1979, 22, 245-249.

Lakshmann, F. L., Howe, J. C., Schuster, E. M. & Barnes, R. E. Response of two strains of rats to a high-protein diet containing sucrose or cornstarch. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1981, 167, 224232.

Laube, H., Klor, H. U., Fussganger, R. & Pfeiffer, E. F. The effect of starch, sucrose, glucose and fructose on lipid metabolism in rats. Nutrition and Metabolism, 1973, 15, 273-280.

Laube, H., Wojcikowski, C., Shatz, H. & Pfeiffer, E. F. The effect of high maltose and sucrose feeding on glucose tolerance. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 1978, 10, 192-195.

MacRae, A. R., Slinger, S. J. & Neudoerffer, T. S. Studies on carbohydrate digestibility and werght gam response in rats fed dietary sucrose, glucose or fructose. Nutrition ana’ Metabolism, 1974,17, 3746a.

MacRae, A. R., Nickel, I., Slinger, S. J. & Neudoerffer, T. S. Carcass composition and weight gain response of rats fed various carbohydrates at two dietary levels ofcarbohydrate and protein. Nutrition and Metabolism, 1974, 17, 47-54 b.

McCusker, R. H., Deaver, 0. E. Jr. & Berdanier, C. D. Effect of sucrose or starch feeding on the hepatic mitochondrial activity of BHE and Wistar rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1983, 113, 1327-1334.

Marshall, M. W., Hildebrand, H. E., DuPont, J. L. & Womack, M. Effect of dietary fats and carbohydrates on digestibility of nitrogen and energy supply, and on growth, body composition and serum cholesterol of rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1959, 69, 371-382.

Marshall, M. W., Womack, M., Hildebrand, H. E. & Munson, A. W. Effects of types and levels of carbohydrates and proteins on carcass composition of adult rats. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1969, 132, 227-232.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV & Szepesi, B. Specificity of the disaccharide effect in the rat. Journal of Nutrition, 1977, 21, 11861191.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Scholfield, D. J., Nate, C. S. & Reiser, S. Demonstration of the disaccharide effect in nutritionally stressed rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1978, 108, 919-925.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Scholfield, D. J., Gardner, L. B. & Cataland, S. Metabolic responses of Zucker fatty and lean rats fed carbohydrate diets ad lihitum or in meals. Journal of Nutrition, 1980, 110, 1409-1420.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Ellwood, K. C., Hallfrisch, J. & Hansen, C. T. Effect of dietary sucrose and genotype on metabolic parameters of a new strain of genetically obese rat: LA/N corpulent. Nutrition Research, 1983, 3, 217-228.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Ellwood, K. C., Judge, J. M., Schoene, N. W. & Hansen, C. T. Effect of dietary sucrose on the SHR/N-corpulent rat: a new model for insulin-independent diabetes. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 1984, 39, 612-618.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Ellwood, K. C., Tulp, 0. L. & Greenwood, M. R. C. Effect of feeding sucrose on starch diets on parameters of glucose tolerance in the LA/N-corpulent rat. Nutrition Research. 1981, 6, 95-99.

Michaelis, 0. E. IV, Martin, R. E., Gardner, L. B. & Ellwood, K. C. Effect of dietary carbohydrates on systolic blood pressure of normotensive and hypertensive rats. Nutrition Reports international, 1986, 23, 261-266.

Moser, P. B. & Berdanier, C. D. Effect of early sucrose feeding on the metabolic patterns of mature rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1974, 104, 687694.

Muto, S. & Miyahara, C. Eating behavior of young rats: experiment on selective feeding on diet and sugar solutions. British Journal of Nutrition, 1972, 28, 327-337.

Naim, M., Brand, J. G., Kare, M. R. & Carpenter, R. G. Energy intake, weight gain and fat deposition in rats fed flavored, nutritionally controlled diets in a multichoice (‘Cafeteria’) design. Journal of Nutrition, 1985, 115, 1447-1458.

Page 18: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

18 I. RAMIREZ

Qureshi, R. U., Akinyanju, P. A. & Yudkin, J. The effect of an ‘atherogenic’ diet containing starch or sucrose upon carcass composition and plasma lipids in the rat. Nutrition and Metabolism, 1970, 12, 347-357.

Ramirez, I. Analysis of sucrose-induced overeating in rats. Proceedings and Abstracts of the Eastern Psychological Association, 1986, 56, 25.

Ramirez, I. & Sprott, R. L. Hunger and satiety in genetically obese mice (C57BL/6J-ob/ob). Physiology and Behavior, 1978, 20, 257-264.

Ramirez, I. & Sprott, R. L. Regulation of caloric intake in yellow mice (C57BL/6J-AY/a). Physiology and Behavior, 1979, 22, 507-511.

Rattigan, S. & Clark, M. G. Effect of sucrose solution drinking option on the development of obesity in rats. Journal of Nutrition, 1984, 114, 1971-1977.

Reiser, S. & Hallfrisch, J. Insulin sensitivity and adipose tissue weight of rats fed starch or sucrose diets ad lib&urn or in meals. Journal of Nutrition, 1977, 107, 147-155.

Reussner, G. Jr., Andros, J. and Thiessen, R. Studies on the utilization of various starches and sugars in the rat. Journal of Nutrition, 1963, 80, 291-298.

Romberg, B. & Benton, D. A. Effect of type of carbohydrate on energy metabolism and body composition of rats fed low protein diets. Journal of Nutrition, 1965, 86, 289-297.

Rothwell, N. J. & Stock, M. J. Influence of alcohol and sucrose consumption on energy balance and brown fat activity in the rat. Metabolism, 1984, 33, 768-771.

Rytand, D. A. Hereditary obesity of yellow mice, a method for the study of obesity. Proceedings qJ’ the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1943, 54, 3401341.

Schemmel, R. A., Teague, R. J. & Bray, G. A. Obesity in Osborne-Mendel and S 5B/Pl rats: effects of sucrose solutions, castration, and treatment with estradiol or insulin. American Journal of Physiology, 1982,243, R347-R353.

Sclafani, A. Feeding inhibition and death produced by glucose ingestion in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 1973, 11, 595-601.

Sclafani, A. Dietary obesity. In G. A. Bray (Ed). Recent advances in obesity research: II. Pp. 123-132. London: Newman, 1978.

Sclafani, A. & Kramer, T. H. Dietary selection in vagotomized rats. Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System, 1983,9, 247-258.

Sclafani, A. & Xenakis, S. Influence of diet form on the hyperphagia-promoting effect of polysaccharide in rats. Li$z Sciences, 1984, 34, 1253-1259 a.

Sclafani, A. 8z Xenakis, S. Sucrose and polysaccharide induced obesity in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 1984, 32, 169-174 b.

Shell, E. R. Sweetness and health. The Atlantic, 1985, 256, 14-20. Slattery, J. M. & Potter, R. M. Hyperphagia: a necessary precondition to obesity? Appetite, 1985,

6, 133-142. Smith, J. C. & Foster, D. F. Some determinants of intake of glucose+saccharin solutions.

Physiology and Behavior, 1980, 25, 127-133. Subcommittee on Laboratory Animal Nutrition Nutrient requirements of laboratory animals.

P. 10. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Sciences, 1978. Teague, R. J., Kanarek, R., Bray, G. A., Glick, Z. & Orthen-Gambill, N. Effect of diet on the

weight of brown adipose tissue in rodents. Life Sciences, 1981, 29, 1531-1536. Trout, D. L., Moy, N. L., Putney, J. D. & Johnson, D. A. Dietary regimens for inducing mild

hyperphagia, obesity and hyperinsulinemia in rats. Nutrition Reports International, 1978, 18, 227-233.

Vasselli, J. R., Haraczkiewicz, E. & Pi-Sunyer, F. X. Effects of Acarbose (Bay g 5421) on body weight, insulin, and oral glucose and sucrose tolerance in sucrose-consuming rats. Nutrition and Behavior, 1982,1,21-32.

Vrana, A. & Fabry, P. Metabolic effects of high sucrose or fructose intake. World Review of Nutrition and Dietetics, 1983, 42, 56-101.

Waterman, R. A., Romsos, D. R., Tsai, A. C., Miller, E. R. & Leveille, G. A. Effects of dietary carbohydrate source on growth, plasma metabolites and lipogenesis in rats, pigs and chicks. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1975, 150, 220-225.

Weiner, R. P., Yoshida, M. & Harper, A. E. Influence of various carbohydrates on the utilization of low protein rations by the white rat. V. Relationships among protein intake, calorie intake, growth and liver fat content. Journal of Nutrition, 1963, 80, 279-290.

Page 19: When does sucrose increase appetite and adiposity?

SUCROSE 19

Williams, V. J. & Szepesi, B. Effect of dietary carbohydrates on food efficiency and body composition in adult male rats during normal growth and during recovery from food restriction. Nutrition Research, 1983, 5, 457468.

Winand, J. 8z Christophe, J. Effects of high-sucrose and high-fructose diets on non-obese and congenitally obese hyperglycemic Bar Harbor mice. Bibliotheca Nutritio et Dieta, 1977,25, 83-91.

Yudkin, J. Sweet and dangerous, New York: Peter H. Wyden, 1972.

Received 28 February 1986, revision 29 July 1986