Top Banner
Andrews University Andrews University Digital Commons @ Andrews University Digital Commons @ Andrews University Master's Theses Graduate Research 2020 The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee (Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural (Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural Environments Environments Amanda Marie Moore Andrews University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.andrews.edu/theses Part of the Biology Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Moore, Amanda Marie, "The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee (Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural Environments" (2020). Master's Theses. 183. https://digitalcommons.andrews.edu/theses/183 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Research at Digital Commons @ Andrews University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ Andrews University. For more information, please contact [email protected].
105

The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

Mar 04, 2023

Download

Documents

Khang Minh
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

Andrews University Andrews University

Digital Commons @ Andrews University Digital Commons @ Andrews University

Master's Theses Graduate Research

2020

The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee

(Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural (Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural

Environments Environments

Amanda Marie Moore Andrews University, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.andrews.edu/theses

Part of the Biology Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Moore, Amanda Marie, "The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean Manatee (Trichechus Manatus Manatus) To Locate Food in Their Natural Environments" (2020). Master's Theses. 183. https://digitalcommons.andrews.edu/theses/183

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Research at Digital Commons @ Andrews University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ Andrews University. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

ABSTRACT

THE USE OF MULTIPLE SENSORY MODALITIES BY THE ANTILLEAN MANATEE

(TRICHECHUS MANATUS MANATUS) TO LOCATE FOOD IN THEIR NATURAL

ENVIRONMENTS

By

AMANDA MOORE

Chair: Daniel Gonzalez-Socoloske, Ph.D.

Page 3: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

ABSTRACT OF GRADUATE STUDENT RESEARCH

Thesis

Andrews University

College of Arts and Sciences

Title: THE USE OF MULTIPLE SENSORY MODALITIES BY THE ANTILLEAN MANATEE

(TRICHECHUS MANATUS MANATUS) TO LOCATE FOOD IN THEIR NATURAL

ENVIRONMENTS

Name of researcher: Amanda Marie Moore

Name and degree of faculty chair: Daniel Gonzalez-Socoloske, Ph.D.

Date completed: July 2020

Manatees are herbivorous aquatic mammals found in the coastal and inland waters of the Atlantic

Ocean. All three manatee species are currently listed as vulnerable on the IUCN red list and there still

remains much unknown about their ecology. It is currently unknown what sensory modalities manatees

use to locate their food in the wild. A literature review of the Paenungulata clade (sirenians,

proboscideans, and hyracoideans) was conducted in order to compare and contrast what is known about

the sensory modalities of the clade, to better understand the sensory modalities of manatees, particularly

the ones they use to locate their food. Manatees have a higher frequency range for hearing than elephants,

who have the best low-frequency hearing range known to mammals; hearing range of hyrax is unknown.

All members of Paenungulata have vibrissae assisting in tactile abilities and potentially compensate for

other senses such as hearing or vision. The ability to smell in manatees and hyrax is unknown, but

elephants have been found to have an excellent sense of smell. Manatees, elephants, and hyrax have

dichromatic vision. A preliminary experiment was designed to test manatee feeding modalities in the

wild. The objectives of this study were to determine if the proposed methodology, modified for an aquatic

environment from Renda & Roux (2017), was capable of testing manatee sensory use by limiting the

sensory cues provided. Sensory modalities used in locating food were tested in two ways: when they

Page 4: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

know where the food is located, within a short distance, and when the food is placed randomly throughout

their habitat, at long distances. In this study, we were able to show that the experimental design works,

and provide preliminary data. In the short distance dichotomous choice trials, the percent of correct

choices were 67% for the chemoreception + vision, 60% for chemoreception only, and 60% for vision

only, with 50% being the rate of chance. For long distance experiments, the mean minimum time in

hours it took manatees to consume the food placed randomly along their habitat of San San-Pond Sak

River, Panama was 12.0 hours for chemoreception + vision, more than 22 hours for chemoreception only,

and 6.89 hours for the control (no box). Due to the small sample size, no definitive conclusion could be

made as to which sensory modality manatees use to find food, but our results support the idea that

manatees use multiple modalities, chemoreception + vision, to locate food. Additional trials are needed in

order to perform statistical analysis on the data.

Page 5: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

Andrews University

College of Arts and Sciences

THE USE OF MULTIPLE SENSORY MODALITIES BY THE ANTILLEAN MANATEE

(TRICHECHUS MANATUS MANATUS) TO LOCATE FOOD IN THEIR NATURAL

ENVIRONMENTS

A Thesis

Presented in Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Master of Science

by

Amanda Marie Moore

July 2020

Page 6: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

©Copyright by Amanda M. Moore 2020

All Rights Reserved

Page 7: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

vii

THE USE OF MULTIPLE SENSORY MODALITIES BY THE ANTILLEAN MANATEE

(TRICHECHUS MANATUS MANATUS) TO LOCATE FOOD IN THEIR NATURAL

ENVIRONMENTS

A thesis

presented in partial fulfillment

of the requirements for the degree

Master of Science

by

Amanda Marie Moore

APPROVAL BY THE COMMITTEE:

_______________________________ Daniel Gonzalez-Socoloske, Ph.D., Chair

__________________________________

Benjamin Navia, Ph.D.

__________________________________ June 18, 2020

Robert E. Zdor, Ph.D. Date approved

Page 8: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES …………………………………………………………………………… x

LIST OF TABLES ……………………………………………………………………………. xi

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS ………………………………………………………………… xii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ……………………………………………………………………. xiii

CHAPTERS

1. INTRODUCTION ………………………………………………………………… 1

2. REVIEW OF SENSORY MODALITIES OF THE EXTANT PAENGULATA

CLADE……………………………………………………………………………….. 5

a. BRIEF HISTORY OF EVOLUTIONARY DIVERGENCE OF

PAENUNGULATA…………………………………………………………. 5

b. HEARING………………………………………………………………… 6

i. Hearing Anatomy of Sirenians……………………………………. 6

ii. Hearing Physiology of Sirenians…………………………………. 9

iii. Hearing Anatomy of Proboscidea……………………………….. 11

iv. Hearing Physiology of Proboscidea……………………………... 12

v. Hearing Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea…………………. 13

c. TOUCH…………………………………………………………………… 13

i. Tactile Anatomy of Sirenia……………………………………….. 13

ii. Tactile Physiology of Sirenia…………………………………….. 18

iii. Tactile Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea…………………. 20

iv. Tactile Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea………………….. 22

d. CHEMORECEPTION (TASTE AND SMELL)………………………….. 23

i. Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Sirenia……………… 23

ii. Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea………... 26

iii. Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea………... 29

e. VISION……………………………………………………………………. 30

i. Visual Anatomy of Sirenians……………………………………… 30

Page 9: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

ix

ii. Visual Physiology of Sirenians…………………………………… 31

iii. Visual Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea………………….. 32

iv. Visual Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea…………………... 33

f. VESTIBULAR (BALANCE AND SPATIAL ORIENTATION)…………. 33

i. Vestibular Anatomy & Physiology of Sirenia…………………….. 33

ii. Vestibular Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea and

Hyracoidea……………………………………………………….. 34

g. CONCLUSION……………………………………………………………. 34

3. PRELIMINARY ASSESSMENT FOR HOW MANATEES (TRICHECHUS MANATUS

MANATUS) USE SENSORY MODALITIES TO FIND FOOD…………………..…. 39

a. INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………. 39

b. METHODS………………………………………………………………… 43

i. Study Site…………………………………………………………... 43

ii. Design……………………………………………………………... 44

iii. Trials……………………………………………………………... 46

c. RESULTS………………………………………………………………….. 49

d. DISCUSSION……………………………………………………………… 54

i. Short Distance Dichotomous Choice Trials:

Chemoreception + Vision………………………………………….. 59

ii. Short Distance Dichotomous Choice Trials: Chemoreception

Only……………………………………………………………….. 59

iii. Short Distance Dichotomous Choice Trials: Vision

Only……………………………………………………………….. 60

iv. Long Distance Trials……………………………………………… 61

4. CONCLUSION………………………………………………..……………………. 64

REFERENCES ………………………………………………………………………………… 69

APPENDIX ……………………………………………………………………………………. 88

A. Right/Left Preference Towards Boxes Chosen Versus Where Food was placed…. 88

B. Long Distance Experiments Raw Data……………………………………………. 89

C. Still Shots of Manatees Participating in Experiments……………………………... 91

Page 10: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

x

LIST OF FIGURES

1. Manatee Perioral Bristle Fields……………………………………………… 15

2. Box Design………………………………………………………………………….. 46

3. Constructed Boxes…………………………………………………………………... 47

4. Choice Trial Set-Up…………………………………………………………………. 50

5. Choice Trial Results…………………………………………………………………. 52

6. Long Distance Experimental Results………………………………………………… 53

7. Manatee Eating Banana Leaves……………………………………………………… 54

Page 11: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

xi

LIST OF TABLES

1. Summary of Sensory Modalities Between the Members of the Paenungulata Clade…… 38

2. Box Design and Sensory Cues…………………………………………………………... 45

Page 12: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

xii

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

ABR Auditory Brainstem Response

AEP Auditory Evoked Potential

dB Decibel

FSC Follicle Sinus Complex

GPS Global Positioning System

IACUC Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee

IUCN International Union for Conservation of Nature

kHz Kilohertz

re 1 µPa Reference Pressure in Water

TNT Trinitrotoluene

UV Ultraviolet

Page 13: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

xiii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Daniel Gonzalez-Socoloske, for his mentorship during my

graduate program. I am grateful for the opportunities he provided and time and sacrifices he made for

research to be conducted in Panama, which included the need for him to translate for me in-country and

teaching me the skills and flexibility of conducting field research. I appreciate his patience and dedication

to conducting this research and helping me complete my thesis.

I would like to thank Dr. Benjamin Navia and Dr. Robert Zdor for their time and contributions to

the completion of my thesis. I would additionally like to thank the faculty of the Department of Biology at

Andrews University for the opportunity to study here and the support they have provided while

completing my master’s degree.

A special thanks to Sixto Herrera and Juan McDonald for assisting with the research conducted in

Panama by driving the boats, helping place the boxes along the river, and providing bananas and banana

leaves. Without them, the research would not have been possible.

Thank you to Mindy McLarty for accompanying me in Panama, especially during the absence of

Dr. Gonzalez-Socoloske, and assisting with construction of boxes, collection of data, and for enduring the

heat and sandflies.

I would like to thank my parents, Richard and Sharon Moore, for their love and support

throughout my educational experience. An additional thanks to my dad for helping create a prototype

plexiglass box before conducting the research, without him, building the boxes in Panama would have

been a disaster.

Funding for this project was provided by the Andrews University Graduate Grants in Aid of

Research, Andrews University Faculty Research Grant, Woodland Travel Fund Department of Biology

Andrews University. Additionally, a special thanks to Total Plastics Int’l for the generous discount

Page 14: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

xiv

provided on the cut sheets of plexiglass used to construct the plexiglass boxes vital to conducting this

research.

Page 15: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

The order Sirenia consists of manatees and dugongs, of which there are four extant

species within two families. Three species of manatees are found within the family Trichechidae:

the West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus), the Amazonian manatee (Trichechu inunguis),

and the West African manatee (Trichechus senegalensis). The West Indian manatee can be split

into two subspecies, the Florida manatee (T. m. latirostris) and the Antillean manatee (T. m.

manatus); (Domning & Hayek, 1986). One living species of dugong is found within the family

Dugongidae, the dugong (Dungong dugon); (Marmontel, de Souza, & Kendall, 2016; Marsh,

O’Shea, & Reynolds, 2011).

The range of habitats of West Indian manatees is the most extensive including marine,

estuarine, and freshwater habitats; from the eastern coast of the United States south to Brazil

(Deutsch et al., 2008; de Meirelles et al., 2018). The Florida manatee is commonly found along

the Atlantic coast of the United States and in the Gulf of Mexico while the Antillean manatee is

found from the Bahamas, Cuba and Puerto Rico, south to Brazil (Reynolds et al., 2018).

Amazonian manatees live in freshwater habitats throughout the Amazon River basin and are the

only entirely freshwater species of manatees (Denkinger, 2010; Marmontel, de Souza, &

Kendall, 2016). West African manatees are found off the western coast of Africa from

Mauritania down to Angola (Gonzalez-Socoloske & Olivera-Gomez, 2012; Powell, 1996; Silva

& Araujo, 2001). Dugongs are found in marine habitats from East Africa to Vanatu, including

the Red Sea, Arabian Sea, Indian Ocean, and South China Sea (Marsh & Sobtzick, 2019).

Page 16: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

2

All sirenians are listed as vulnerable on the IUCN red list. Habitat loss and hunting have

led to a decline of all sirenian species. Conservation efforts by the United States have helped

stabilize populations of the Florida manatee (Runge et al., 2015). Humans pose the largest threat

to manatees due to hunting, habitat loss, and boat strikes (Domning, 1982; Gerstein, 2002).

Hunting is most common in developing countries given the amount of meat provided by a

manatee and the leather that can be made from their hide. Cold stress affects the Florida manatee

as water temperatures drop in the winter months and manatees seek refuge at artificial warm

water reserves such as power plants; when power plants shut down temporarily, many manatees

have died as a result (Laist & Reynolds, 2005). Florida manatees are also more vulnerable to

boat strikes given the higher amount of recreational boat use in the waters of Florida where

manatees reside. Noise pollution from boats has also been found to mask calls, affecting

manatees’ ability to communicate with conspecifics, most notably affecting mother-calf

interactions (Gerstein, 2002; Reynolds, 2017). Manatees are also vulnerable to fluctuations in

population size due to their reproduction rate: a gestation of 12-14 months and one offspring at a

time raised for one-and-a-half to two-and-a-half years, results in a slow increase in population

size (Reep & Bonde, 2006).

Conservation efforts to preserve sirenians includes regulating boat traffic, decreasing boat

speeds, and hunting bans (Domning, 1982; Gerstein, 2002). Loss of habitat for sirenians includes

a probable loss of food source, increasing the need to find other sources of vegetation. How they

locate food sources is therefore an important aspect of understanding how to better conserve this

species, as habitat loss and other anthropogenic factors prohibiting their ability to find food could

lead to a decline in the species. However, if it is not understood how they find food, it cannot be

known if their ability to find food is being altered.

Page 17: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

3

Most research conducted on manatees has been conducted on the West Indian manatee,

specifically, the Florida manatee (Reep & Bonde, 2006). In contrast, the West African manatee is

the least studied species of the manatees. The Amazonian manatee is the smallest member of the

order Sirenia and uniquely, does not have fingernails on the end of each flipper like the West

Indian and West African manatee (Husar, 1977). The West Indian and West African manatee

share similar habitat characteristics as well as both inhabiting marine, estuarine, and/or

freshwater areas. However, the West African manatee is only found in tropical waters whereas

West Indian manatees are found in subtropical waters as well (Reynolds et al., 2018; Jimenez,

2005).

It is not known which senses manatees use to find food in the wild. Manatee sensory

abilities include touch, hearing, vision, and chemoreception, similar to other mammals. Manatees

have vibrissae on their face as well as postcranial hairs distributed across their body which have

a tactile function. The hearing range of manatees is from 0.5 kHz-46 kHz, with maximum

sensitivity around 16-18 kHz (Gerstein et al., 1999). Manatees are known to have color vision

which is thought to be best suited for sensitivity to recognize the presence of objects in the water

rather than acuity to discriminate what objects are (Bauer et al., 2003a; Griebel & Schmid, 1996;

Piggins, Muntz, & Best, 1983). Manatees have about 3,000-31,000 taste buds on their tongues,

likely playing a role in chemoreception abilities (Bills, 2011; Lowell & Flanigan, 1980; Reep &

Bonde, 2006; Thewissen, 2008). Olfaction abilities are unknown; however, manatees have an

olfactory bulb, suggesting the potential for olfaction to be a part of chemoreception abilities in

manatees.

The diet of manatees, while almost entirely herbivorous, is quite broad as it consists of

over 108 genera of freshwater and saltwater plants and algae (Alves-Stanley, Worthy, & Bonde,

Page 18: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

4

2010; Gonzalez-Socoloske, 2013; Hartman, 1979; Ledder, 1986; Lefebvre et al., 2000;

Reynolds, 1981) In addition to seagrasses consumed in marine areas, manatees also feed on

terrestrial plants along the shoreline as well as algae and floating vegetation (Aleman, 2011).

While the food characteristics influencing food choice are not well known, it has been suggested

that manatees prefer vegetation with high digestible fiber (Gonzalez-Socoloske, 2013).

In this thesis I will review the literature (chapter 2) on sirenian sensory abilities in the

context of their closest living relatives by comparing and contrasting what is known about the

sensory modalities of the Paenungulata clade (i.e. Sirenia, Proboscidea, and Hyracoidea). The

modalities I will examine are: hearing, touch, chemoreception, vision, and balance. Once we

understand what sensory abilities manatees have, we can begin to think about which ones they

use to locate their food.

In chapter 3 I modify the experimental design to test feeding sensory modalities of Renda

& Roux (2017) to be suited for an aquatic environment. First, I set out to determine if the

proposed methodology was practical in controlling sensory cues provided and testing manatee

sensory use in the locating of food. Second, I began experimental trials testing the sensory

modalities used by manatees to locate food when they were habituated to where food was

located, within a short distance, and when food was placed randomly in their habitat, at long

distances. The results of this study should not only provide a methodology for conducting

sensory experiments on wild manatees, but also allow for a more thorough understanding of the

sensory modalities used by manatees to locate food at short and long distances.

In chapter 4 I draw overall conclusions from my literature review and my

experimentations on wild manatee sensory modalities. I also provide recommendations for future

research.

Page 19: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

5

CHAPTER 2

REVIEW OF SENSORY MODALITIES OF THE EXTANT PAENUNGULATA CLADE

Brief history of evolutionary divergence of Paenungulata

Placental mammals are classified into the Eutherian subclass, which has been subdivided

into four superorders: Eurochontoglires, Laurasiatheria, Xenarthra, and Afrotheria. Afrotheria is

further subdivided and consists of a clade known as Paenungulata. Members of the Afrotheria

superorder outside the Paenungulata clade include: Afrosoricida (tenrecs, otter shrews, golden

moles), Macroscelidea (elephant shrews), and Tubulidentata (aardvarks). The Afrotherian clade

Paenungulata consists of Proboscidea (elephants), Sirenia (manatees and dugongs), and

Hyracoidea (hyrax). Members of Paenungulata (Sirenia, Proboscidea, and Hyracoidea) are

mammals which form a strongly supported monophyletic clade and have presumably originated

from Africa according to various molecular loci and biogeographic evidence (Pardini et al.,

2007; Stanhope et al., 1998).

The most genetically derived group, in terms of ecological niche, are the sirenians, with

different lines of genetic and morphological evidence indicating they belong within the

Paenungulata clade. Through genetic analysis it can be determined which terrestrial mammals

share a common ancestor with extant, marine mammals, such as sirenians with Proboscidea and

Hyracoidea (Cornelis et al., 2014; Kuntner, May-Collado, & Agnarsson, 2010; Irwin & Arnason,

1994). Although sirenians have undergone individual convergent events to become marine

mammals, they share many common phenotypic adaptations with other marine mammals such as

locomotion, sensory abilities, communication, management of buoyancy, and large oxygen

stores (Foote et al., 2015). As a result, Sirenians are often the outliers when it comes to

Page 20: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

6

phylogenetics as they have no evolutionary relationship with other marine mammal orders

(Kellogg et al., 2007). Sirenians and proboscideans have many similarities, including double

apex hearts, herbivorous diets, and similar teeth with a ‘conveyor belt’ system replacing teeth as

they are worn down (Shoshani, 1998). Sirenians and hyracoideans, have postcranial hairs in

common (Reep, Marshall, & Stoll, 2002; Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011).

While the exact relatedness of the Paenungulata clade continually undergoes dispute due

to inconsistent molecular studies and unclear origins of the Paenungulata trichotomy (Kellogg et

al., 2007; Murphy, Pevzner, & O’Brien, 2004; Stanhope et al., 1998), genetic analysis has helped

confirm relatedness between members of the Afrotheria superorder as well as provide evidence

for sirenians being most directly related to proboscideans (Domning & Hayek, 1986; Kuntner,

May-Collado, & Agnarsson, 2010; Thewissen & Domning, 1992). However, alternative

evidence suggests that sirenians are most closely related to hyracoideans (Cornelis et al., 2014;

Springer et al., 2003; Tabuce et al., 2007). Another way to support phylogenetic relatedness,

besides genetic analysis, is through similar anatomic and physiological adaptations. In this

review we will discuss the similarities and differences found between the various sensory

systems of the Paenungulata clade (Table 1).

HEARING

Hearing Anatomy of Sirenians

The anatomy of the sirenian ear is unique compared to other mammals (Chapla et al.,

2007; Ketten, Odell, & Domning, 1992) and may be adapted for underwater communication

between conspecifics (Chapla, 2006). Sirenians have no pinna, or external ear, and therefore lack

the musculature of the pinna, with a small opening leading to their external auditory meatus (ear

Page 21: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

7

canal), which is a non-cartilaginous, marginally curved tube, separated from the tympanic

membrane (ear drum) by fatty tissue (Chapla & Rommel, 2003; Chapla et al., 2007; Chapla,

2006; Marshall et al., 2007; Nummela & Yamato, 2018). The tympanic membrane of sirenians

has two distinct regions, the pars tensa and pars flaccida, as in terrestrial mammals, which

suggests the tympanic membrane responds in a frequency dependent manner similar to cats

(Pickles, 1982). The tympanic membrane of sirenians bulges outward, rather than inwards as in

many terrestrial mammals, due to the medial position of the cartilaginous keel of the malleus

supporting the midline of the tympanic membrane; this may be an adaptation for underwater

hearing (Chapla, 2006). The tympanic membrane of sirenians has been found to be thicker and

larger than in many mammals, assisting with the transmission of acoustic energy from the

tympanic membrane to their enlarged ossicular chain (Nummela & Yamato, 2018). The

tympanic bone is ring shaped, unlike that of a true auditory bulla (Chapla, 2006). The dome

portion of the periotic bone, which is superior to the tympanic bone, increases in size over the

lifetime of a manatee as it deposits an outer growth layer yearly (Marmontel et al., 1996). The

periotic bone is joined to the squamosal by a syndesmotic joint which in turn is fused to the

zygomatic process by cartilaginous and membranous connections (Chapla, 2006; Domning &

Hayek, 1986; Ketten, Odell, & Domning, 1992). The zygomatic process of sirenians is an

enlarged, oil-filled, bony sponge, with considerable mass, but less stiffness than compact bone of

similar size (Domning & Hayek, 1986; Ketten, Odell, & Domning, 1992). The ossicles of the

middle ear are the densest of any mammal and are denser than the periotic bone where the

cochlea resides (Nummela & Yamato, 2018). The density of ossicles adds stiffness to the

ossicular chain, helping compensate for the large ossicular size (Ketten, 1998). The center of

gravity of the ossicular chain is off-center of the rotational axis, increasing inertial momentum

Page 22: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

8

(Chapla & Rommel, 2003). The increased mass of the ossicular chain, as well as the off-center

axis rotation, may enhance bone conduction of low frequencies (Mason, 2004). The size of the

manatee’s middle ear is similar to that of elephant’s, suggesting that it should have optimum

hearing at low frequencies (Chapla, 2006) which is different to what has been documented for

the hearing ranges of manatees (Gerstein et al., 1999). Manatees may have adapted middle ear

anatomy by increasing tympanic membrane thickness and stiffness, compensating for the large

surface area, in order to register shorter wavelengths and hear high frequency sounds (Chapla,

2006).

The inner ear of sirenians is a combination of aquatic and terrestrial mammalian features.

The anatomy of the West Indian manatee ear is not specialized in that the cochlea and basilar

membrane structures, as well as spiral ganglion cell densities, do not function in ultra- or infra-

sonic hearing. However, auditory ganglion cell sizes are larger in comparison to terrestrial

mammals (Ketten, 1998). It was noted by Home (1815) that the dugong cochlea is between one

to one and a half turns while the West Indian manatee cochlea is one and three-quarters turns

(Ketten, Odell, & Domning, 1992) suggesting manatees have a wider range of hearing than

dugongs (West, 1985).

The pathway that sound travels through the ear of sirenians remains unclear due to the

disconnect between the external auditory meatus and the tympanic membrane (Chapla, 2006;

Nummela & Yamato, 2018). There are multiple hypothesis; the sound may pass through the

zygomatic process to the ear, they may use a method of bone conduction via the ribs and/or

vertebral column, sound waves may pass through the soft tissues found in the head of the

manatee to the middle ear cavity to be processed, or the airspaces in the middle ear may use

underwater pressure transduction for hearing (Chapla, 2006; Nummela & Yamato, 2018).

Page 23: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

9

Furthermore, it has been found that temperature gradients in soft tissues affect sound

transmission through the tissues of the head and it is suggested that similar mechanisms may

exist in manatees, given the network of blood vessels throughout their head (Chapla, 2006).

Hearing Physiology of Sirenians

Hearing is suggested by Hartman (1979) to be the primary sensory modality used by

sirenians. Manatee vocalizations have been recorded to be between 0.5-25 kHz, suggesting

hearing ranges would encompass these frequencies (Frisch & Frisch, 2003; Nummela & Yamato,

2018). AEPs performed by Bullock, Domning, & Best (1980) provided responses from 0.2 to 35

kHz in West Indian manatees. Bullock, O’Shea, & McClune (1982) found results with similar

upper limits of 35 kHz on the West Indian manatee as well. ABRs performed by Klishin et al.

(1990) on Antillean manatees found optimal frequencies to occur between 5-20 kHz at 25-30 dB

re 1 µPa with maximum frequency responses occurring at 60 kHz above 90 dB re 1 µPa.

Behavioral testing performed by Gerstein et al. (1999) on two male, captive manatees found

hearing sensitivity of West Indian manatees to be 0.4-46 kHz with their frequency range of best

hearing to be between 6 and 20 kHz. Greatest sensitivity in the hearing range of the two West

Indian manatees tested was from 16-18 kHz at about 50 dB re 1 µPa (Gerstein et al., 1999).

Additional behavioral tests were conducted on two captive male West Indian manatees and

hearing sensitivity was found to be 0.25-90.5 kHz with peak sensitivity from 8-32 kHz (Gaspard

et al., 2012). At frequencies of 90.5 kHz, one manatee refused to participate, but the sound was

detected as suggested by his behavioral responses (Gaspard et al., 2012). These results not only

support each other, providing an approximate range of hearing for manatees, but they also

coincide with the range of manatee vocalizations which have been recorded for West Indian

manatees as well as Amazonian manatees (Bullock et al., 1980; Gerstein, 1995). Manatees

Page 24: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

10

therefore have good hearing abilities at higher frequencies but poor hearing abilities at lower

frequencies (Gerstein, 2002).

Gerstein et al. (1999) were able to obtain results down to 0.015 kHz by one of the

manatees tested, with an observed change in behavior (he rotated his body ~45 degrees to the left

and bent his head down while listening) while detecting these infrasonic sounds generated 1.5 m

from the manatees position. However, when Gaspard et al. (2012) tested these same frequencies

3 m away from the manatee’s position, they did not obtain a response. It has been suggested that

they switch methods of detection from hearing via the auditory system to hearing via tactile

receptors, possibly using their postcranial hairs to assist in the recognition of sound (Gerstein et

al., 1999; Reep et al., 1998). However, position and distance to the source of the sound

influences the particle motion for the same acoustic pressures, therefore, the closer the manatee

is to the infrasonic sound source, the more probability of using tactile methods for processing

sound (Gaspard et al., 2012).

Dugongs and manatees, while both members of the order Sirenia, are from different

families, and thereby have the potential for different hearing abilities. It is suggested by

Anderson & Barclay (1995) that while the hearing ranges of manatees and dugongs are similar,

the vocalization ranges of dugongs and manatees differ greatly. Manatee vocalizations occur

from about 0.5 kHz to 25 kHz while dugong vocalizations occurring from about 0.6 kHz to 18

kHz (Anderson & Barclay, 1995; Frisch & Frisch, 2003; Nummela & Yamato, 2018). While

both of these vocalization ranges lie within the suggested sirenian hearing range of 0.3-46 kHz, it

is likely that dugongs have a broader sensitivity range than manatees, given their wider range of

vocalization frequencies. Further research is needed on dugong hearing as subjective

(behavioral) and objective (ABR, AEP) tests have not been conducted on dugongs.

Page 25: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

11

Hearing Anatomy of Proboscidea

The pinna of the elephant ear is suggested to have three functions: regulating body

temperature, communication (warning conspecifics), and sound localization (Heffner et al.,

1982). The distal end of the external auditory meatus of elephants has a sphincter-like muscle

surrounding the opening, allowing for contraction of the muscle to occlude the opening from the

environment to the external auditory meatus upon tactile stimulation (O’Connell-Rodwell, 2007).

The external auditory meatus of the elephant is an open, straight, patulous, horizontal tube,

approximately nine-and-a-half inches in length and about one inch in diameter, terminating at the

tympanic membrane, or the start of the middle ear (Blair, 1717). The tympanic membrane of

elephants has an area of 1000 mm2 (Heffner et al., 1982). Animals whose auditory system

contain enlarged tympanic membranes and stapedial foot plates are thought to have auditory

systems adjusted to hearing low-frequency sounds better than high-frequency sounds (Garstang,

2004). The middle ear of proboscideans, similar to sirenians, houses colossal ossicles which may

assist in bone conduction (O’Connel, Hart, & Arnason, 1999). Ossicles amplify sounds in the

middle ear as well as transmit sound waves via mechanical energy to hydraulic energy when they

reach the oval window of the cochlea (Garstang, 2004).

The inner ear of proboscideans, like sirenians, is primitive in formation in that they do

not have a round window and cochlear canaliculus for circulating perilymphatic fluid to the

cranial region as in other mammals, but rather a perilymphatic duct leaves the inner ear via the

perilymphatic foramen, similar to reptiles, which may assist in detecting vibrations (O’Connell,

Hart, & Arnason, 1999). The cochlea of elephants is filled with fluid and is the characteristic

spiral shape, consisting of two spirals (Garstang, 2004; von Bekesy, 1960). It was noted by Von

Bekesy (1960) that the cochlea of elephants has the sharpest resonance of animals studied,

Page 26: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

12

meaning sounds waves received at their natural resonant frequencies respond with a higher

amplitude or loudness than waves of the same size received at non-resonant frequencies. This

suggests elephants have multiple resonant frequencies and are capable of distinguishing between

frequencies of narrow bandwidth better than other animals (O’Connell-Rodwell, 2007).

The head of the African elephant is aerated by sinuses in addition to bones of the cranium

being arranged to form diploe (spongy, cancellous bone, separating layers of the cortical skull

bone). Furthermore, the size of the head of an animal- the distance between the ears, as measured

around the ears, divided by the speed of sound, is inversely related to the animals ability to hear

high-frequency; mammals with large heads therefore hear low-frequency sounds better due to

their larger head and ears causing a head-shadow effect to occur and attenuate high-frequency

sounds (Heffner & Heffner, 1981; Heffner & Heffner, 1982). These features, in addition to the

fatty deposits in the skull and the solid mandible may assist with low-frequency sound detection

(O’Connell-Rodwell, 2007).

Hearing Physiology of Proboscidea

Elephants have been found to have the best low-frequency hearing of any mammal tested,

with hearing abilities ranging from 0.016 to 12 kHz and their best frequency of hearing occurring

at 1 kHz according to subjective hearing test (Heffner & Heffner, 1982). Calls of the African and

Asian elephants occur in the range of 0.014-0.035 kHz (Ben-Ari, 1999; Larom et al., 1997)

which coincides with their low-frequency hearing ability. Low frequency sounds used by

elephants are able to travel farther due to being less attenuated by things such as air temperature,

wind, trees and shrubs, and the time of day calls are produced (Larom et al., 1997; Pierce, 1981).

It has been suggested by Garstang et al. (1995) that the calls of elephants may travel distances of

10 km or more under ideal conditions. In addition to the low frequency sound produced by

Page 27: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

13

elephants, their calls contain high energy, occurring with intensities up to 117 dB (Ben-Ari,

1999). Furthermore, elephants have been witnessed engaging in listening behaviors in captivity

and in the wild such as, raising and stiffening their ears while simultaneously swinging their head

back and forth; such behavior may aid in their ability to hear and localize sounds (Ben-Ari, 1999;

Heffner et al., 1982).

Hearing Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea

Research conducted on hyracoideans is limited in the realm of hearing anatomy and

physiology. However, extensive research has been done on the highly social behavior of hyrax as

well as the complex acoustic signaling used by hyrax to communicate with conspecifics

(Demartsev et al., 2014). In a spectrogram of hyrax wails, chucks, and snorts (Weissman et al.,

2019), the fundamental frequency of their calls appears to occur between 1-5 kHz depending on

the call, with harmonic frequencies occurring between 6-12 kHz, suggesting that the hyrax

hearing range should encompass at least 1-12 kHz. No studies show the raw anatomical

constructs of the ear anatomy or the hearing ranges for hyrax, however, it is known that they at

least possess a pinna, often aiding in sound localization. Further research is needed in these

areas.

TOUCH

Tactile Anatomy of Sirenia

Tactile hairs have the highest number of afferent fibers, mechanoreceptors, and types of

mechanoreceptors assisting in their sensitive detection of sensory input. Tactile hairs which are

true vibrissae are known as follicle-sinus complexes (FSCs) because of their blood-filled sinus

ring which surrounds the follicle (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011). Vibrissae are further denoted by

Page 28: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

14

their dense innervations and the dense connective tissue which forms a capsule. Vibrissae are

important for perceiving cues from the environment as well as allowing for behavioral responses

to those cues. Sirenians are the only species that have only true vibrissae as the only hairs on

their body (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011).

Sirenians are thought to be specialists of tactile sensations given the number and

distribution of vibrissae (tactile hairs) distributed across their body (Gaspard et al., 2013; Sarko

et al., 2007). Tactile perception is the combination of a variety of senses including, but not

limited to, kinesthesia, proprioception, and interoception (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall, 2018). Most

research conducted on marine mammals, including sirenians, focuses on the sensory hairs and

mechanoreception of the skin. However, manatees have not been found to have

mechanoreceptors associated with their skin, but have mechanoreceptors associated with their

postcranial hairs, suggesting their postcranial sensory hairs may function differently from other

marine mammals and are possibly more similar in function to their terrestrial relatives (Bauer,

Reep, & Marshall, 2018; Sarko et al., 2007).

Sirenians have three types of “hair” on their face: bristles, hair, and bristle-like hair.

Bristles are the thickest and stiffest of the three, while hair is the same hair which appears

throughout the postcranial body. Bristle-like hairs have a thickness and stiffness between that of

hair and bristles, and are evenly distributed on the oral disk only (Reep et al., 1998). The oral

disk is the region of the mouth and orofacial ridge, between the upper lips, containing stiff,

bristle-like hairs (Reep et al., 1998).

Page 29: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

15

Figure 1. Manatee Perioral Bristle Fields. Perioral bristle fields U1-U4 and L1-L2 of manatees.

Modified from Reep et al., 2001.

Sirenians perioral bristles, which are modified vibrissae, combined with their facial hairs

function in both feeding and touch (Reep et al., 1998). Manatees have six identified fields of

perioral bristles on their face: four fields (U1-U4) are located on each side of the upper bristle

pad and oral cavity, also known as the supradisk (Figure 1), and two fields (L1-L2) are on each

side of the lower bristle pad, or chin, inside the oral cavity (Figure 1); (Reep et al., 1998; Reep et

al., 2001). About 110 bristles are on each side of the face, with little difference in distribution,

diameter, or length of the bristles between fields or facial sides (Reep et al., 1998). U1-U4

bristles vary in length and thickness and are used in different ways during the feeding cycle,

whether everted or retracted, working in variance or unison (Marshall et al., 1998; Reep et al.,

1998).

Supradisk hairs are densest by the nares and least dense by the caudal periphery. Hair is

denser on the face than in the dispersion of postcranial hairs (Reep et al., 1998). The head region

Page 30: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

16

which contains about 2,000 vibrissae has about 109,000 myelinated axons supplying information

from the follicles to the brain via the trigeminal nerve. The postcranial body has about 3,300

vibrissae, with a total of about 100,000 myelinated axons supplying information to the brain

from the follicles via spinal afferents (Bauer et al., 2018). All information conveyed from the

over 200,000 myelinated axons is sent to the lobulated somatosensory nuclei in the brainstem;

information from the face and head terminate at the trigeminal nuclei while information from the

postcranial body terminates at the cuneate-gracile complex (Bauer et al., 2018).

It has been suggested that each bristle found on the face of manatees is represented by a

single, large, rindenkern (clusters of neurons in particular areas of the sirenian cerebral cortex;

small rindenkerns have few neurons in a cluster while large rindenkerns have numerous neurons

present in the cluster). The bristles found on the facial region of manatees are each represented

by a large rindenkern (110 bristles: 116 rindenkern) while body hairs are represented by small

rindenkerns (Reep et al., 1998; Sarko et al., 2007). The face has the highest density of hair, with

the oral disk having the highest density of all regions, which may assist with tactile exploration.

Hair density decreases with age as body size increases with age, and the number of hairs remains

about the same, which may assist juvenile sirenians in tactile learning (Reep et al., 1998).

The brain of sirenians is proportionally skewed to over-represent somatosensory input

and output, about 25% of the neocortex in manatees is devoted to tactile information (Sarko et

al., 2007). While manatees lack several muscles needed for facial expressions, the orbicularis

oris acts as a muscular sling assisting in the protrusion and movement of perioral vibrissae

(Marshall et al., 2007). In addition, the face of manatees is highly innervated by a number of

nerves, with the facial bristles, lips, and perioral regions having motor innervations allowing for

movements to occur separately or in unison to assist in feeding behaviors in addition to

Page 31: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

17

providing somatosensory input (Marshall et al., 2007; Reep et al., 1998). Postcranial hairs are

also present on the fluke of manatees, suggesting Bichoff’s nucleus, in the manatee brain, may

represent the fluke as it does the tails of other mammals. The fluke of sirenians seems to play an

important role in navigating through their environment and may help compensate for poor

eyesight (Sarko et al., 2007).

Dugongs and manatees differ in tactile physiology in that dugongs are benthic grazers,

whereas manatees will eat floating aquatic vegetation or rooted aquatic vegetation, therefore,

they require slightly different methods of food acquisition in addition to differential deflections

of the snout (Baeur, Reep, & Marshall, 2018; Marshall et al., 2003). Dugong snouts can be

divided into five regions as noted by Marshall et al. (2003): oral disk, supradisk, chin, upper and

lower perioral bristle fields. Dugongs have a snout deflected 70 degrees and their rostrums are

down-turned to allow the perioral region to be almost parallel with the substrate (Marshall et al.,

2003). The oral disk of the dugong is convex when relaxed, and the oral disk can be extended

during feeding by opening the lateral furrows, followed by the extension of the lateral flaps,

changing the shape of the oral disk from arch-shaped to semicircular (Marshall et al., 2003).

Dugongs have the same six fields of perioral bristles as do manatees; four fields on the upper lip

and two on the lower lip, however dugongs have about 40% more bristles than manatees

(Marshall et al., 2003; Reep et al., 1998). The U1 bristles of dugongs participate in vegetation

manipulation, unlike that of the manatee, and dugongs use their U2 bristles in a sweeping

manner to introduce food into the mouth (Marshall et al., 2003). Dugongs and manatees are

divergent in their perioral bristle use in that dugongs U2 bristles do not meet at the midline,

preventing prehensile capabilities like that of manatees (Marshall et al., 2003). Dugongs, similar

to manatees use their oral disk and perioral bristles when exploring an environment and both

Page 32: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

18

manatees and dugongs have been observed using their flippers to remove food stuck in their

bristles (Marshall et al., 2003). While manatees and dugongs differ in their snout morphologies

and vibrissal use, these differences allow for different feeding behaviors, trophic ecologies, and

ecological niches (Marshall et al., 2003).

Tactile Physiology of Sirenia

It is believed that the abundant tactile ability found in dugongs is to help compensate for

their poor eyesight (Dexler & Freund, 1906). Dexler & Freund (1906) also suggest that touch

and hearing are the most highly developed senses in dugongs and may help compensate for the

senses which do not have much function if any, such as sight, taste, and smell. It was determined

by Bauer et al. (2012) that manatees have highly developed tactile capabilities, with sensitive

discrimination of textures. Manatees use vibrissae not only for feeding and exploration, but also

for problem solving. It is believed that the highly sensitive tactile processing of manatees should

help provide them with a detailed view of the world which other senses, such as sight, may not

provide (Bauer et al., 2012). Manatees were able to detect oscillations using their vibrissae

generated by water movements between 0.05-.15 kHz when located approximately 14 cm from

the source, which is well below their measured hearing range (Gerstein et al., 1999; Gaspard et

al., 2013).

The lips of sirenians are bilobed; when feeding, the lobes open, allowing the bristles to

extend to the food source, grasp the vegetation, and tuck it into the cleft between the lip lobes,

pulling the food into the mouth, this is known as oripulation (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall, 2018;

Marshall et al., 1998). The U2 bristles of the left and right fields act in a grasping manner when

feeding while the L1 bristles push the food into the oral cavity (Marshall et al., 1998). The use of

the bristles, or perioral vibrissae, to grasp and manipulate food is uniquely used by sirenians,

Page 33: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

19

however the grasping pattern has been found to differ depending on the vegetation consumed

(Marshall et al., 1998). The lips and oral disk of sirenians therefore act as a muscular hydrostat,

similar to the trunks of proboscideans, capable of complex movements due to the musculature

and vibrissal arrangement (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall, 2018).

The oral disk is involved with a flare response seen at the beginning of a feeding cycle, as

well as being used in tactile scanning during exploration of the environment (Hartman, 1979;

Reep et al., 1998). The oral disk is also very sensitive to human touch in that when any portion is

touched, they respond by grabbing the stimulus with their U2 bristles (Hartman, 1979; Marshall

et al., 1998; Reep et al., 1998). Similar to manatees, dugongs have a reflex of raising their head

up a few inches from the bottom when the corners of their mouth are touched (Dexler & Freund,

1906).

It was suggested by Gerstein et al. (1999) that manatees may “hear” low frequency

sounds via their postcranial hairs. Sarko et al. (2007) also suggest that low-frequency sounds in

water may stimulate the mechanoreceptors associated with the postcranial hairs of manatees, as

the vibrissae of the face and postcranial body are disturbed. Bauer, Reep, & Marshall (2018)

suggest that vibrissae may help sense changes in the three-dimensional environment marine

mammals live in. Furthermore, data suggests that the anatomically specialized vibrissae of

manatees act as a lateral line system, detecting hydrodynamic stimuli (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall,

2018; Gaspard et al., 2013). This makes sense as the sensory hairs move due to water

displacement or direct contact, when the hairs are deflected, they make contact with the wall of

the follicle which contains mechanoreceptors, changing the mechanical signal to an electrical

signal to be sent via axons to the brain, providing a picture of the environment via

mechanoreception (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall, 2018).

Page 34: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

20

Tactile Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea

Elephants are unable to put their head to the ground and as such, their trunk plays an

important role in tactile discrimination, as well as for use in obtaining food (Endo et al., 2001).

The distal tip of elephant trunks has what is known as “fingers”, the African elephant has two

“fingers”, while the Asian elephant has one “finger” and as a result, African and Asian elephants

may have slightly different interactions with objects and food as they contain different prehensile

abilities (Hoffmann, Montag, & Dominy, 2004; Frei, 2016). The trunk of the elephant contains

about 40,000 muscles assisting in the remarkable mechanical dexterity elephants maintain (Frei,

2016; Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). The trunk of elephants has multiple roles including,

grasping of food, tactile recognition and chemosensory recognition via the vomeronasal organ

(Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). Elephants have two types of vibrissae present at the end of their

trunk, these two types of vibrissae are regular vibrissae and vellus vibrissae. Regular vibrissae

are abundant and present in the skin surrounding the trunk finger while vellus vibrissae are

contained beneath the surface of the skin of the trunk tip (Rasmussen & Munger, 1996).

Vibrissae have dense sensory innervation by hundreds of axons and free nerve endings which are

abundant in the superficial dermis (Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). The tip of the trunk can be

used to detect environmental vibrations when placed on the ground as well as having dexterity

allowing for objects, as small as a grain of rice, to be picked up; it is assumed that the trunk is

also used to determine size, shape, texture, and temperature of objects (Rasmussen & Munger,

1996).

The skin of elephant’s bodies and trunks contains hair which is innervated through their

thick epidermis as seen by their papillary ridges, which increases the surface area of the skin.

Page 35: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

21

However, the trunk finger skin is thinner, similar to hairless skin of humans (Rasmussen &

Munger, 1996).

The vellus vibrissae hair shaft is surrounded by a loose connective tissue capsule fused

with the dense connective tissue of the dermis and is innervated by axons which are

encompassed by a vascular sinus and thought to be associated with lanceolate and Merkel cells

(Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). The hair shaft of the vellus vibrissae does not go past the skin

surface, similar to the human eyelid. Vellus vibrissae are intermingled with regular vibrissae and

both are thought to contain lanceolate and Merkel terminals, which are associated with

mechanoreceptors. The pattern of vibrissae of the elephant trunk is surrounded by skin which has

dense sensory innervation (Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). The skin of the trunk tip of elephants

contains two types of corpuscular nerve endings, small Pacinian and multibranched simple

corpuscles, with the lateral portion of the dorsal tip containing more corpuscles than the ventral

trunk tip. Pacinian corpuscles are extremely sensitive and thought to play a role in elephants as

auditory receptors, detecting vibrations as small as Brownian motion. Vibrissae of the elephant’s

trunk are not innervated by muscles and therefore do not function as whiskers and cannot move.

However, the elephant trunk vibrissae assist the trunk in performing as a specialized sensory

organ, detecting vibrations, as well as performing tactile functions with precise grasping and

dexterity (Rasmussen & Munger, 1996). Dehnhardt, Friese, & Sachser (1997) found that

elephants can perceive differences as small as 0.25 mm in the width of grooves using their

trunks, demonstrating excellent tactile discrimination and functionality.

Elephants have been observed engaging in multiple tactile behaviors including standing

with their sides touching, submissive elephants backing themselves into the sides of dominant

elephants, entwining trunks with conspecifics, performing mouth checks, as well as

Page 36: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

22

antagonizing and disciplining of conspecifics by actions such as placing their trunk on the head

of a conspecific and pushing down; the function of these behaviors is not widely understood, but

demonstrates a wide use of tactile sensations (Langbauer, 2000). It has been observed that

different body parts of elephants are used to initiate tactile behaviors, however, the trunk is most

commonly used (Makecha, Fad, & Kuczaj, 2012). Tactile behaviors play an important role in the

social interactions of Asian and African elephants, even though they have slight variations in

their interactions (Makecha, Fad, & Kuczaj, 2012).

Tactile Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea

Vibrissae are commonly found distributed across the face and body of mammals where

they are most likely to encounter stimuli, with the densest areas of distribution being where

tactile information is most easily collected and detected (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011). Hyrax

have true vibrissae distributed in rows down their back and limbs, interspersed amongst their

pelage, as well as four supraorbital vibrissae on their head, about five vibrissae on their throat,

and numerous common mystacial vibrissae. Vibrissae are also found on the inside of their wrist,

shoulders, back, flanks, belly, above the anus, and thighs (Sale, 1970; Sarko, Rice, & Reep,

2011). It is believed that the vibrissae of hyrax assist in environmental exploration, alerting them

of predators or threats by conspecifics, as well as providing navigational cues to compensate for

poor visual capabilities (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011; Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015). Furthermore,

hyrax have no tail, and as a result, it is thought their vibrissae may help them sense the

environment similar to the function of a tail (Sale, 1970).

The vibrissae of hyracoideans, similar to sirenians, have dense distributions of Merkel

endings, assisting in detecting the direction the hair is deflected (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2011).

Hyracoidean vibrissae are true vibrissae as they each contain a follicle-sinus complex (FSC)

Page 37: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

23

containing a follicle core, dense innervations, and connective tissue capsule with a surrounding

blood supply (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015). Unique to hyrax are the Merkel endings which

innervate the FSCs as they include dermal bulges containing Merkel complex endings at the rete

ridge collar level (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015). Mystacial FSCs of hyrax are unique in their

morphology as they contain longitudinal ridges and grooves which extend from the cavernous

sinus to the lower ring sinus level and are packed with axons ascending from the deep vibrissal

nerves; two deep vibrissal nerves are responsible for the innervations of the FSCs of all body

regions in hyrax (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015). Hyrax also possess guard hairs located proximal

to the vibrissae; however, guard hairs do not contain a blood-filled ring sinus or a capsule of

dense connective tissue, but are rather associated with sebaceous glands and lanceolate endings

(Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015). The vibrissae of hyrax likely serve complementary functions given

their unique structure, which include detecting large hair deflections by the Merkel endings of

the rate ridge collar, and smaller disturbances at the ring sinus level. Lanceolate endings, present

in the guard hairs of hyrax, function as stretch receptors and encode information on vibrissal

movement and deflection rate (Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015).

CHEMORECEPTION (TASTE AND SMELL)

Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Sirenia

As noted by Wartzok & Ketten (1999), the extent of chemoreception abilities in manatees

remains poorly known, but has been suggested to be important due to their active mouthing of

conspecifics and environmental novelties (Hartman, 1979). Sirenian tongues are characterized as

being small, in relation to body size, slender, but vertically thick, no sulci present on the dorsum,

hard, finger-shaped, with cuticular spines in the apex region of the tongue. Their tongues also

have large patches on the side of the wall, and the presence of abnormal gustatory organs formed

Page 38: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

24

by the lingual papillae. In manatees these organs are found in multi-fossulate, upward swellings,

while in dugongs they are found in the downward growing lateral pits (Yamaskai et al., 1980).

The apex of the tongue in manatees has been found to be rounded while the tongue of dugongs is

truncated (Yamasaki, Komatsu, & Kamiya, 1980). The free tip of the Florida manatee tongue

was found to contain many hair-like filiform papillae (Levin & Pfeiffer, 2002). The ventral

portion of the manatee tongue has extensive attachments to the floor of the mouth, limiting

mobility. The tongue of dugongs has lateral strips on either side due to the longitudinal elevation

in the mid-part of the dorsum, while the anterior dorsum has hard spines similar to manatees, just

shaggier. Sirenians have pits on the lateral sides of the dorsal part of their tongue, however, in

dugongs, these pits contain more taste buds than in the fossulate of manatees (Yamasaki,

Komatsu, & Kamiya, 1980). In Florida manatees, numerous filiform papillae were found to be

distributed over the dorsal surface of the back of the tongue with multiple raised fungiform-like

papillae distributed on the dorsum, with taste buds restricted to the foliate papillae of the tongue

root (Bills, 2011; Levin & Pfeiffer, 2002).

Sirenian tongues are extremely muscular with the posterior portion having adipose and

muscular tissue (Yamasaki, Komatsu, & Kamiya, 1980). The function of the tongue in manatees

is thought to include food manipulation, tasting, and chemoreceptive functions such as salinity,

hormone, and protein detection (Levin & Pfeiffer, 2002). The majority of papillae on the tongue

of manatees serve a mechanical function rather than taste functions, with fungiform papillae

lacking taste buds altogether in manatees (Levin & Pfeiffer, 2002).

Olfactory bulbs are present in sirenians, although small, suggesting sirenians are capable

of a sense of smell (Thewissen, 2018). The presence of taste buds on the sirenian tongue

provides the potential for chemoreception to occur via taste as well (Lowell & Flanigan, 1980;

Page 39: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

25

Reep & Bonde, 2006, Thewissen, 2008). A portion of the manatee brain is devoted to scent

detection and interpretation and it is suggested by Hartman (1979) that manatees may possess

modified taste or smell receptors on their lips and/or the roof of their mouth given their mouthing

behavior (Reep, Finlay, & Darlington, 2007). The herbivorous diet of sirenians increases their

vulnerability to toxins in plants, which are often detected in mammals by a strong bitter taste,

given this risk, a need for good taste detection in manatees would be warranted (Bills, 2011).

Manatees have a reduced sense of smell given their olfactory bulb structure. However, they are

thought to have a heightened sense of taste within the localized region of the tongue root (Bills,

2011). Manatees are estimated to have between 3,233-31,140 taste buds, giving them a similar

number of taste buds as in bovines (Bills, 2011). The number of taste buds, as well as their

distribution and arrangement in the mouth of the manatees indicates that a sense of taste is

important to their survival (Bills, 2011).

Different foraging strategies and diets have led to differing chemosensory structures and

distributions in many animals, and manatees may not be an exception. Male manatees are able to

detect chemical cues from the urine of female manatees, entirely underwater, and as such, it is

likely that the chemicals are received via aqueous transmission either by delivery to the olfactory

epithelium, to the taste buds, or by an unknown mechanism (Bills, 2011). It is also suggested that

manatees may use olfactory receptors when their snouts are above the water and their nares open,

such as when eating at the surface. When simultaneous breathing occurs between mother/calf

pairs, or when conspecific congregations at hot springs occur, they breathe in unison as part of

thermoregulation. However, given that the nose of sirenians remains closed when submerged, it

is unlikely that the olfactory epithelium is capable of transmitting chemicals to the oropharynx

due to the valve separating the nasal passage and the oropharynx (Bills, 2011).

Page 40: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

26

Sirenians do not have a vomeronasal organ, which is often associated with pheromone

detection, which may increase the role of the tongue in chemoreceptive abilities (Bills, 2011;

Levin & Pfeiffer, 2002). Proboscidea and sirenians have similar herbivorous diets, suggesting

similar structures allowing for taste abilities and nutrient detection (Bills, 2011). It appears that

manatees have more taste buds per papilla than hyrax (Yoshimura et al., 2008). Male manatees

have exhibited flehmen-like movements using their muzzle when near a female manatee in

estrus; moving their nose as well as lateral and medial portions of the lips to expose the inner

portion of the lip (Bills, 2011). Flehmen movements are conducted by elephants using their

trunks to direct scents to their vomeronasal organ for detection, however, it is thought manatees

use these movements for a different reason due to their lack of a vomeronasal organ (Bills,

2011). Manatees lack a vomeronasal organ, while both elephants and hyraxes possess one.

However, the turbinate structure in the nose of manatees is most similar to that of hyrax as the

ethmoid region contains three turbinates positioned similarly between the two species while the

elephant has nine convoluted ethmoid turbinates (Bills, 2011).

Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea

The tongues of elephants have been found to have few taste buds in the upper epithelium

of the secondary papillae, which are innervated by plexiform nerves; these taste buds however

have indefinite boundaries and are lacking taste pores (Kubota, 1967). The role of elephant’s

taste buds in chemoreception remains unclear and needs further research as there is limited

literature available on this topic.

Chemical signals play many roles, and are valuable in terms of reproduction and the

inter- and intra-sexual interactions that take place as a result of sexual cues (Rasmussen &

Page 41: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

27

Schulte, 1998). Chemical signals in elephants are known to be able to be extracted from the urine

of males in musth, as females and other males respond to the urine of males in musth in various

ways depending on their own status (Rasmussen & Schulte, 1998). It is also thought that the ano-

genital area of elephants may provide chemical signals, as it is the part of the body of

conspecifics most commonly touched by the trunk (Rasmussen & Schulte, 1998). There is a

temporal gland which excretes a watery, chemically-sparse fluid, known as frontalin, while

males are in musth, and females may have excretions from this gland from time to time as well

(Rasmussen & Schulte, 1998; Rasmussen & Greenwood, 2003). Other sources of excretions

which may play a role in chemical signaling of elephants include: saliva, mucus secretions from

the eye, ear wax, and secretions produced by interdigital glands. Regardless of where the signal

is produced, it is the trunk that initially picks up the chemical signal and transports it to either the

vomeronasal organ or the nasal turbinates of the main olfactory bulb; the trunk thus acts as a

modified nose in elephants as the elephant places the end of the trunk inside the mouth, near the

openings of the vomeronasal ducts (Lazar et al., 2002; Ngwenya et al., 2011; Rasmussen &

Schulte, 1998). The nasal turbinates of elephants, found in the main olfactory bulb, are covered

by a protein-rich mucus layer containing millions of olfactory receptor cells, increasing their

surface area and ability to smell; however, it is the vomeronasal organ that senses chemical

signals, such as pheromones, relating to the status of conspecifics (Lazar et al., 2002; Rasmussen

& Schulte, 1998). The main olfactory bulb of elephants differs from other mammals in that it

lacks an internal plexiform layer, and the glomeruli of the glomerular layer are organized in two

to four honey-comb layers, and the deep part of the glomerular layer has a high density of cells

as well as myelinated fibers (Ngwenya et al., 2011). Given the high density of cells, myelinated

fibers, and protein-rich mucus layer of the elephant’s nasal passage, and the trunk which delivers

Page 42: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

28

chemicals to the nasal passage directly, it is likely the elephant has a good sense of smell,

especially in regards to the detection of chemical signals from conspecifics.

The main olfactory bulb is the first site for volatile chemical signals to be received and

processed via the olfactory epithelium (Ngwenya et al., 2011). Elephants lack an accessory

olfactory bulb used to process non-volatile chemical signals such as pheromones, however, they

have a vomeronasal organ which processes similar signals (Ngwenya et al., 2011). Lack of an

accessory olfactory bulb is similar to sirenians, who also lack this portion of the olfactory

system, in addition to having a rudimentary main olfactory bulb, which is thought to be an

aquatic adaptation (Ngwenya et al., 2011). Elephants have a unique adaptation to their olfactory

system, compared to other mammals, in that they have an olfactory ventricle which is an

extension of the lateral ventricle and is lined with flat ependymal cells (Ngwenya et al., 2011).

Olfaction seems to be an important sensory system for elephants given the olfactory bulb and

rhinencephalon, regions of their brain devoted to processing olfactory information, are quite

large (Ngwenya et al., 2011). The main olfactory bulb is composed largely of the periventricular

layer, which causes more white matter to be found in the olfactory bulb of elephants than is

commonly found in other mammals (Ngwenya et al., 2011). As white matter consists of

myelinated axons, signals transmitted via the olfactory bulb will be able to travel faster in

elephants than other mammals, suggesting a faster processing of olfactory signals and possibly a

better overall sense of smell than in many terrestrial mammals.

Elephants are capable of distinguishing different odors and scents, which has been

demonstrated by their ability to distinguish between different ethnic groups in Kenya based

solely on smell (Plotnik et al., 2014). They have demonstrated the ability to learn via olfaction as

well as containing the ability to retain olfactory information for long periods of time, and locate

Page 43: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

29

food using olfactory cues (Plotnik et al., 2014). Elephants are capable of detecting the presence

of TNT, a non-volatile chemical, which is difficult to detect, supporting the notion that elephants

have a good sense of smell (Miller et al., 2015).

Chemoreception Anatomy & Physiology of Hyracoidea

The center of the hyraxes’ back, known as the dorsal spot or dorsal gland, often seen as a

different color than the surrounding pelage, is important in providing olfactory stimuli to

conspecifics and potentially predators; this spot is often sniffed by conspecifics upon

confrontation (Sale, 1970; Stoddart & Fairall, 1981). Stoddart and Fairall (1981) found that when

an odor stimulus was presented to hyrax, their heart rate decreased, demonstrating a response to

chemical stimuli and suggest a receptor for the chemical stimuli. The vomeronasal organ of

hyrax is embedded in the anterior cartilage of the paraseptalis and leads to the nasopalatinus duct

which has both oral and nasal openings (Stobel, Junold, & Fischer, 2010). The nasal structure of

hyrax is not unusual for terrestrial mammals and demonstrates functionality throughout their life

history as well as playing a role in chemoreception (Stobel, Junold, & Fischer, 2010).

The tongue of hyrax is conical in shape with fungiform papillae on the sides of the tongue

as well as on the ventral surface and lingual apex. On the dorsal surface of the tongue, filiform,

fungiform, and foliate papillae were found (Yoshimura et al., 2008). Foliate papillae are present

on the lateral sides of the torus mandibularis (Emura, Okumura, & Chen, 2008). No vallate taste

buds have been found in the oral cavity of hyrax (Yoshimura et al., 2008). Few taste buds have

been found to be present on the top epithelium of the large fungiform papillae while numerous

taste buds have been found in the epithelial wall of the slit-like groove (Yoshimura et al., 2008).

The exact role of the tongue and the taste buds in chemoreception of hyrax remains unclear and

needs further research.

Page 44: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

30

VISION

Visual Anatomy of Sirenians

The visual abilities of sirenians remains poorly studied, with opposing views as to

manatees’ abilities (Bauer et al., 2003b; Dexler & Freund, 1906; Griebel & Schmid, 1995;

Hartman 1979; Piggins, Muntz, & Best, 1983). Manatee eyes are small, and almost round, with a

nearly round lens used most likely in aquatic navigation (Griebel & Schmid, 1996; Griebel &

Schmid, 1997). The pigmented portion of the cornea is vertically elongated in manatees which is

unusual for terrestrial or marine mammals (Mass et al., 1996). The optic nerve of manatees is

thin while the retina contains both rod-like and cone-like photoreceptors, allowing for the

potential of color vision as well as assisting with night vision (Griebel & Schmid, 1996; Newman

& Robinson, 2006; Piggins, Muntz, & Best, 1983). It remains unknown whether sirenians

possess a tapetum lucidum, which assists with night vision, but it is possible given proboscideans

have a tapetum lucidum, and both orders are diurnal (Piggins, Muntz, & Best, 1983).

Griebel & Schmid (1996) suggested the numerous ganglion cells centered in the ventral

retina may serve as an area centralis, however, Mass et al., (1996) found the retina of manatees

to be lacking an area centralis. This combined with the pattern of ganglion cell distribution, as

well as the number of ganglion cells, suggests manatees have the lowest degree of retinal

differentiation known to marine mammals, which coincides with their reduced visual regions in

the brain (Mass et al., 2012; Sarko et al., 2007). Manatees have a unique ciliary body, important

for preserving normal functioning of the eye, as they have a dual- vein system, with venous

outflow involving a large elevated vein posteriorly and at the base, which join with the choroidal

veins of the eye (Natiello & Samuelson, 2005). Manatees however lack a ciliary muscle,

Page 45: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

31

suggesting their vision is controlled by vasculature rather than musculature when focusing on

objects nearby in their environment (Natiello & Samuelson, 2005).

Little research is available about the eye of the dugong, however it has been noted that

the eye of the dugong has a narrow opening between the eye lids, a round pupil, a cornea that is

very curved, and an iris which is blackish-brown, that is mobile, and retracts when light is

presented (Dexler & Freund, 1906). Further research is needed on the anatomy and physiology of

the sirenian eye.

Visual Physiology of Sirenians

Observations of daily behaviors by manatees, such as exploring their environment,

approaching objects head on, and investigation of unknown auditory stimuli with their eyes,

suggests manatees use vision as part of daily tasks (Gerstein 1994; Hartman 1979; Natiello,

Lewis, & Samuelson, 2005). The precise ability of manatees to see above water remains

unknown, however they have demonstrated the ability to discriminate between objects above

water while submerged (Griebel & Schmid, 1996; Natiello, Lewis, & Samuelson, 2005). Bauer

et al. (2003a) found that manatees are able to visually focus on objects which are one meter or

more away, while underwater, and research suggests that manatees’ vision is better suited for

focusing on objects at a distance, rather than close up. When manatees are within one meter of an

item, they are thought to use monocular cues, as they tend to turn their bodies from side to side,

examining objects with one eye at a time (Natiello, Lewis, & Samuelson, 2005).

Griebel & Schmid (1996) used behavioral discrimination experiments which

demonstrated that manatees could discriminate blue and green from greys. However, manatees

were unable to discriminate blue-green blends (e.g. turquoise) and red from greys, suggesting

Page 46: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

32

dichromatic vision capabilities, similar to dogs, which is supported by the presence of two types

of cone cells (Cohen et al., 1982; Griebel & Schmid, 1996). It was also determined by Griebel &

Schmid (1996) that manatees do not seem to be sensitive to the UV-part of the light spectrum

which likely allows for better vision in their low visibility environments as UV vision would add

a blue haze to everything, and decrease long distance visual abilities. Manatees often inhabit

brackish water environments, which increases darkness and reduces contrast, limiting farsighted

vision; however, color vision may help enhance contrast and visual abilities in brackish water

habitats (Griebel & Schmid, 1996). Manatees have demonstrated the ability to discriminate

between brightness levels, comparable to the abilities of carnivorous fur seals, which is

surprising given they forage on plants and navigate through shallow waters (Griebel & Schmid,

1997).

Visual Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea

Elephant eyes, although appearing large, are rather small compared to their body size

(Hatfield et al., 2003). The pupil of the elephant is round without any noted specialization, while

the retina of elephants has been found to be positioned to allow for the eyes to scan as the trunk

moves (Hatfield et al., 2003; Pettigrew et al., 2010). The tapetum lucidum of elephants is similar

to that of artiodactyls, however, it is not as reflective as in other mammals (Pettigrew et al.,

2010). The relationship between high ganglion cell densities (greater than 4,000 cell/mm2) and

tapetum lucidum intensity combined with the bluish-color of the tapetum lucidum, suggest that

night foraging for elephants is important. Elephants are most likely diurnal, which is supported

by visual anatomy and physiology according to current literature (Pettigrew et al., 2010). The

visual pigments present in proboscideans, similar to those present in sirenians, suggest

dichromatic color vision (Shyan-Norwalt et al., 2010; Yokoyama et al., 2005). Ganglion cell

Page 47: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

33

topography has been used to estimate elephant visual acuity; however, further research is needed

in determining visual acuity and function of the elephant’s visual system (Pettigrew et al., 2010).

Visual Anatomy and Physiology of Hyracoidea

Little information is known about the visual system of hyraxes, but it is noted by Sale

(1970), due to the social behavior and underground living quarters of hyrax, that it is unlikely

that hyrax use vision much, if at all. However, it is also mentioned throughout the literature, that

the raising of hair on the dorsal spot on hyrax is likely used as an alarm signal, which would need

to be seen by conspecifics in order to be useful, suggesting visual abilities are present (Sale,

1970). Newman & Robinson (2006) noted that hyraxes have dichromatic color vision and are

diurnal, similar to their sirenian relatives. Further research is needed on the visual abilities of

hyraxes.

VESTIBULAR (BALANCE AND SPATIAL ORIENTATION)

Vestibular Anatomy and Physiology of Sirenia

The vestibular system, while not often considered one of the five basic senses, is an

important sensory system. Sensory organs are those that send information to the brain for

understanding and perceiving our environment, of which, the vestibular system plays a key role

as it not only provides information regarding spatial orientation, but also head position and

balance, and assists with posture and head stabilization during movement (Martin & Clark,

2012).

The semicircular canals of sirenians have been examined both for the manatee and

dugong and have been found to have small arcs, or curves (Spoor, 2018). Furthermore, sirenians

have reduced range of motion for their necks, but do not have an associated reduction of the

Page 48: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

34

semicircular canals as cetaceans do (Spoor, 2018). Ketten, Odell, & Domning (1992) suggest

that the semicircular canals were poorly developed with small canals. Recent research however

suggests the size of their semicircular canals is similar to that of terrestrial mammals who have

slow movements similar to sirenian’s slow, swimming movements (Spoor, 2018). As mentioned

previously, vestibular systems are often involved in head stabilization, however, given sirenians

do not make fast head movements, stabilization is not as important for them as it is for cetaceans

or other fast-moving mammals and may be why their semicircular canals are reduced. As the

semicircular canals are still present, they likely assist with other vestibular functions used by

sirenians (Spoor, 2018). It is important to note as well that the flippers of sirenians are thought to

serve as paddles and balancing organs, and are likely associated with the vestibular system of

sirenians (Dexler & Freund, 1906).

Vestibular Anatomy & Physiology of Proboscidea and Hyracoidea

No research was found on the vestibular anatomy or physiology of proboscideans or

hyracoideans. Further research is needed in these areas.

CONCLUSION

While the sirenian ear anatomically supports low frequency hearing, studies done by

Gerstein et al. (1999) and Gaspard et al. (2012) suggest manatees hear best at higher frequencies;

it is likely environmental pressures and noise have led to manatees adapting their range of best

hearing to higher frequencies so they can hear within their natural habitats. Proboscideans on the

other hand have large external ears allowing for excellent hearing, with elephants being able to

hear infrasonic sounds and having hearing best adapted to low frequencies; low frequency

sounds are able to travel long distances and allow for communication to occur between elephants

Page 49: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

35

which are within about 10 km of each other (Garstang et al. 1995). Further research is needed on

the hearing anatomy and physiology of hyracoideans; however, the range of their calls suggests

their hearing range should encompass 1-12 kHz (Weissman et al., 2019).

Sirenians possess vibrissae on their face which assist in tactile discovery and perception

of their food and environment (Sarko et al., 2007). It is possible that the tactile hairs of sirenians

help compensate for other senses which may not be as well developed (Dexler & Freund, 1906).

Sirenians also possess postcranial hairs which likely serve a tactile function and may work

similar to a lateral line system (Bauer, Reep, & Marshall, 2018; Gaspard et al., 2013). It has also

been suggested that the postcranial hairs of sirenians may assist in low frequency hearing when

within close range of the sound source (Gerstein et al., 1999). Proboscideans do not have

documented postcranial hairs like sirenians or hyracoideans, however, they do have hairs

distributed across their body (Rasmussmen & Munger, 1996). The trunks of proboscideans serve

an important role in tactile functions as it is able to perform prehensile abilities and mechanical

dexterity in addition to grasping food and serving as a chemoreceptive organ (Rasmussen &

Munger, 1996; Hoffmann, Montag, & Dominy, 2004). Proboscideans possess vibrissae at their

trunk tip assisting in tactile sensations and behaviors, with the trunk engaging most often in

tactile behaviors (Makecha, Fad, & Kuczaj, 2012). Hyracoideans have vibrissae or postcranial

hairs distributed throughout their pelage and they are thought to assist in environmental

exploration and sensation as well as signaling alert calls to conspecifics (Sale, 1970; Sarko, Rice,

& Reep, 2011; Sarko, Rice, & Reep, 2015).

The extent of sirenians chemoreceptive abilities remains unknown (Wartzok & Ketten,

1999) however, it is likely present and important given their mouthing behavior of environmental

objects (Hartman, 1979). The sirenian tongue has been found to possess about 3,000-31,000 taste

Page 50: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

36

buds, allowing for chemoreception to potentially occur via taste rather than olfaction; olfaction is

difficult given their fully aquatic lifestyle with closed nostrils when submerged (Bills, 2011;

Lowell & Flanigan, 1980; Reep & Bonde, 2006, Thewissen, 2008). However, given that

sirenians retain an olfactory bulb, it suggests they are capable of smell (Thewissen, 2018).

Similar to sirenians, proboscideans have few taste buds, however, their role in chemoreception

remains unknown and needs further research (Kubota, 1967). Proboscideans receive chemical

signals via the nostrils located at the trunk tip in two ways. The nostrils transport chemical

signals to the vomeronasal organ by curling their trunk tip into their mouth and directing the

chemical signals to the vomeronasal organ through a duct located in the roof of the mouth.

Alternately, they can receive chemical signals through the nostrils on the trunk tip which are sent

up the trunk, through the nasal turbinates, to be received by the olfactory bulb (Lazar et al., 2002;

Ngwenya et al., 2011; Rasmussen & Schulte, 1998). Proboscideans lack an accessory olfactory

bulb, which is similar to the lack of vomeronasal organ in sirenians (Ngwenya et al., 2011).

Hyracoideans have demonstrated decreased heartrates in the presence of certain chemical

stimuli, suggesting chemoreception mechanisms are present and is likely to occur largely via

olfaction given their nasal structure is similar to that of many other terrestrial mammals (Stobel,

Junold, & Fischer, 2010). The tongue of hyracoideans has few taste buds similar to their

proboscidean and sirenian relatives, however, the role of their taste buds in chemoreception

remains unclear and needs further research (Yoshimura et al., 2008).

The eye of sirenians, while small, is thought to be adapted for focusing on objects at a

slight distance rather than close up (Bauer et al., 2003a). Manatees have demonstrated the ability

to use dichromatic color vision, potentially allowed for better brightness discrimination and

contrast enhancement in their environments (Cohen et al., 1982; Griebel & Schmid, 1996). It is

Page 51: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

37

suggested by current literature that proboscideans also have dichromatic color vision (Shyan-

Norwalt et al., 2010; Yokoyama et al., 2005), however, visual acuity has not been determined.

Based on the eye anatomy of proboscideans, their vision is likely suited for diurnal use

(Pettigrew et al., 2010). More research is needed on the vision of hyracoideans, however it is

believed due to their lifestyle that vision is not used much, if at all, but is thought to be present

given the visual alarm calls used (Sale, 1970).

Little research has been conducted on the vestibular systems of Paenungulata, however, it

is known that sirenians have reduced semicircular canals which are similar in size to slow

moving terrestrial mammals (Spoor, 2018). Further research is needed on the vestibular systems

of proboscideans and hyracoideans as no information could be found at this time.

Surprisingly, sirenians have the most research available on their sensory anatomy and

physiology with the majority of information being supplied by research conducted on West

Indian manatees. Hyracoideans have the least amount of research conducted on their sensory

systems which is likely due to their low survival rate when kept in captivity in addition to their

geographic range in Africa and the Middle East.

Page 52: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

38

Table 1. Summary of sensory modalities between the members of the Paenungulata clade.

Sirenia Proboscidea Hyracoidea References

Hearing No Pinna.

Hearing Range: 0.25-

90kHz.

Best Hearing: 6-32

kHz.

Pinna Present.

Hearing Range:

0.016-12 kHz.

Pinna Present.

No known hearing

range.

Gaspard et al.,

2012; Gerstein et

al., 1999; Heffner

& Heffner, 1982

Touch Three types of tactile

hair on face. Tactile

hairs may assist in

hearing. Tactile hairs

play a role in

oripulation and

feeding.

“Finger” at end of

the trunk have two

types of vibrissae.

Vibrissae of trunk

assist in detecting

vibrations and may

have a role as

auditory receptors.

May assist with

vision.

Vibrissae

distributed across

their body,

potentially used to

navigate and sense

environment. May

compensate for

poor vision.

Hoffmann,

Montag, &

Dominy, 2004;

Rasmussen &

Munger, 1996;

Reep et al., 1998;

Sarko, Rice, &

Reep, 2011)

Chemoreception Tongue potentially

used for taste and

chemoreception.

Olfactory bulb

present. No

vomeronasal organ.

Three nasal

turbinates.

Vomeronasal

organ and

olfactory bulb

present. Few taste

buds on tongue.

Good sense of

smell and

chemical

detection. Nine

nasal turbinates.

Vomeronasal

organ for

chemoreception.

Taste buds (fewer

than sirenians).

Three nasal

turbinates.

Bills, 2011; Levin

& Pfeiffer, 2002;

Ngwenya et al.,

2011; Plotnik et

al., 2014; Stobel,

Junold, & Fischer,

2010; Thewissen,

2018

Vision Round, small eyes.

Dichromatic color

vision. Diurnal vision.

Vision best suited for

distance.

Small eyes.

Unique retinal

position to

accommodate

trunk. Dichromatic

color vision.

Diurnal vision.

Dichromatic color

vision. Diurnal

vision.

Bauer et al.,

2003a; Cohen et

al., 1982; Griebel

& Schmid, 1996;

Hatfield et al.,

2003; Newman &

Robinson, 2006;

Pettigrew et al.,

2010; Shyan-

Norwalt et al.,

2010; Yokoyama

et al., 2005

Vestibular Flippers serve as

balancing organs.

Semicircular canals

reduced in size,

similar to slow-

moving terrestrial

mammals; used for

spatial orientation and

balance.

Unknown. Unknown. Dexler & Freund,

1906; Spoor, 2018

Page 53: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

39

CHAPTER 3

PRELIMINARY ASSESSMENT FOR HOW MANATEES (TRICHECHUS MANATUS

MANATUS) USE SENSORY MODALITIES TO FIND FOOD

INTRODUCTION

Manatees, members of the mammalian order Sirenia are completely aquatic herbivores.

Manatees commonly occupy rivers, estuaries, and coastal marine waters, filling a unique

ecological niche. How manatees find their food in their natural habitats remains poorly

understood. Griebel & Schmid (1997) noted that little is known about the senses of manatees,

especially those involved in orientation, navigation, and taste; however, given their herbivorous

diets, their sensory adaptations are of special interest. Studies conducted in the past 20 years

have sought to rectify the limited information known about the senses of manatees which were

discussed extensively in chapter two. In our study, we sought to develop a methodology that

could be used to test the sensory modalities used by manatees to locate food as well as better

understand how the senses of manatees played a role in finding food in their environment when

they knew it was within a close proximity and when it was randomly placed in their habitat.

In a study conducted on bat eared fox and the sensory modalities they used to find food,

cups were used to limit the sensory cues provided, with insects being used as the food item

(Renda & Roux, 2017). Manatees being fully aquatic makes following this experimental design

difficult as food has to be submerged in the water for the manatees to easily access. Furthermore,

sensory cues provided have to be received through the water, rather than air given their lifestyle,

adding a level of complexity to our experimental design. Lastly, as manatees are herbivorous,

their food are plants rather than insects, likely limiting sensory cues such as audition.

Page 54: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

40

Studying manatees and conducting behavioral tests on them is not an easy task. Not only

are manatees large, completely aquatic mammals, but they are also highly protected under

international laws, and live in tannin-stained water decreasing visibility, making them difficult to

observe. Manatees are also slow moving, spending most of their time submerged underwater,

and are not social animals, except for mother-calf pairs, making it difficult to get large sample

size numbers. Furthermore, the waters inhabited by manatees are commonly inhabited by other

animals such as alligators, crocodiles, and sharks amongst other animals which pose a risk to

humans. Manatees require water temperatures to be above 20°Celsius, so they are commonly

found in tropical regions where temperatures are high and rain occurs often, making working

conditions less than ideal for experimenters.

Manatees are particular in the foods they choose to eat (Gonzalez-Socoloske, 2013;

Gonzalez-Socoloske & Olivera-Gomez, 2019). Manatees are known to consume a variety of

seagrasses as well as algae, often consuming one to seven different plant species as determined

by fecal analysis (Castelblanco-Martinez et al., 2009). Manatees are generalists, consuming

plants from many families (Colares & Colares, 2002; Gonzalez-Socoloske & Olivera-Gomez,

2019; Marsh, O’Shea, & Reynolds, 2011). Plants consumed by manatees are often found

submerged, floating, or along the shoreline of their habitats. However, habitats of many

manatees do not allow for year-round access to food, and as a result the variety of food

consumed changes temporarily and spatially resulting in the need to have a way to find food

when it is available.

Multiple senses could be involved in locating food, including: touch, sound, chemicals

via taste and/or smell, or vision. By understanding what senses are used by manatees to find

food, we can better protect them by making sure the sensory cues they use to find food are not

Page 55: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

41

blocked by artificial factors, such as fertilizer run-off in their environment, which may block the

chemical signals potentially used by manatees to locate food.

As noted by Marshall et al. (1998) and Reep et al. (2011), manatee vibrissae are used for

tactile purposes, such as inspecting objects before they place them in their mouth. The plants

manatees eat are chosen in part through touch. Once vibrissal contact is made with the plant, the

manatee chooses whether or not to consume that particular plant likely based on various

attributes including texture, chemical production, and possibly appearance. Therefore, it is

thought that tactile receptors of the vibrissae play a role in determining which plants to feed on

(Lowell & Flanigan, 1980), but it is not thought to play a role in the initial discovery of food and

therefore, was not tested in this study as a modality used for finding food.

Hartman (1979) suggested hearing to be the principal sensory modality used by sirenians

and cetaceans. It is not thought to play a role in the initial discovery of food by manatees as

plants do not make noise underwater as carnivorous prey would. While there may be a link

between conspecific chewing sounds and the ability to locate a food source, in this study we

were interested in discovering how the food source was first discovered by a manatee, and as

such, did not test auditory cues as a sensory modality used to find food.

Sirenian vision, although initially considered to be poor given their eye size and shape, is

thought to play a role in focusing on objects, such as food, from a distance (Bauer et al., 2003a;

Dexler & Freund, 1906; Piggins, Muntz, & Best, 1983). Manatees also have dichromatic vision,

which may enable better discrimination in the low light conditions of their natural habitats as it is

more difficult to distinguish color in low light conditions (Griebel & Schmid, 1996).

Page 56: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

42

Chemoreception abilities of manatees remains poorly known and understood (Wartzok &

Ketten, 1999). However, it is suggested they have chemoreceptive abilities given behaviors such

as mouthing one another (Reynolds, Powell, & Taylor, 2008). Plants may provide manatees with

chemical cues, similar to the cues prey provide carnivorous predators underwater. The extent of

the manatee’s ability to smell is unknown since they close their nasal openings when submerged,

making it difficult for chemicals to enter the nasal cavity (Reynolds, Powell, & Taylor, 2008).

The tongues of manatees contain a few taste buds, allowing for chemoreception to potentially

occur via taste (Lowell & Flanigan, 1980; Reep & Bonde, 2006; Thewissen, 2008).

It is likely that manatees, like other mammals, use multiple modalities in the course of

locating food within their natural habitats (Hartman, 1979; Piep et al., 2008; Renda & Roux,

2017). Manatees have been observed closing their eyes when approaching an object, switching

from visual to tactile cues before touching the object, suggesting the use of multiple modalities in

daily tasks (Reep & Bonde, 2006).

There is currently no research on the memory abilities of manatees, however, research

conducted on elephants, a close relative of manatees, suggests the use of spatial memory to

recognize conspecifics and recall locations of food and water sources (Bates, Poole, & Byrne,

2009; Byrne, Bates, & Moss, 2009; Dale, 2008). It is agreed that the use of memory or spatial

cognition is important for animals to survive, and it is reasonable to think therefore, that

manatees, similar to other mammals, may use memory for locating food in their natural

environments (Macpherson & Roberts, 2010).

In this study we hope to better understand the sensory modalities used by manatees to

find their food. By conducting our research on the feeding ecology of manatees, we will be able

Page 57: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

43

to increase not only what is known about them, but hopefully provide information to allow for

better protection of their habitats.

The objectives of this study are as follows: Objective 1. To develop and test a

methodology that can test the sensory modalities used by wild manatees to locate food. Objective

2. Preliminarily test which sensory modalities manatees use to find food when they know where

the food is, within a short distance and when they do not know where food is located, at a long

distance, using that methodology.

METHODS

STUDY SITE

Field experiments were conducted in the San San-Pond Sak Wetland Reserve (Humedal

de San-San Pond Sak), Bocas del Toro, Panama. San San-Pond Sak Wetland Reserve is a dark

water habitat, with the water column resembling the color and appearance of tea due to tannins

staining the water. As a result, it is difficult to see through the water much of the day unless the

sun is angled just right (Gonzalez-Socoloske et al., 2015).

Banana leaves and banana fruits were used as the food items in all trials to test the

sensory modalities of manatees in the San San-Pond Sak River. Bananas and banana leaves

which are naturally consumed by the manatees in this river, pose no known threat to their

wellbeing. Banana leaves and bananas show up in the river occasionally due to year-round

harvesting in the surrounding banana plantations. Local fishermen have often observed manatees

eating banana leaves and bananas, and those who used to hunt manatees in the area know it is

effective bait for attracting manatees. Banana leaves and banana fruit have also been used in this

region to attract manatees for previous experiments; during manatee captures using bananas and

Page 58: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

44

their leaves in this region, manatees were able to find this food no matter where they were placed

in the habitat (Gonzalez-Socoloske et al., 2015). Therefore, manatees must be using a modality,

other than memory of previous locations or random chance, to locate food in their environment.

For each trial, banana leaves were folded, broken, and hung along with banana fruit,

which were scored with a machete, to allow chemicals to disperse into the water. Food was hung

inside the box, partially submerged in the water. Banana leaves and banana fruit will be referred

to as food items hereafter. Banana leaves and banana fruit were used in similar quantities in all

trials.

DESIGN

Five, clear plexiglass boxes were constructed to be 0.61 m x 0.61 m x 0.61 m, using 5.99

mm thick plexiglass, with wooden support frames across the top to allow for the boxes to be

hung from the mangrove trees. Boxes were caulked on the inside seams using clear caulk. Clear

duct-tape® was placed on the outside edges of the boxes to provide additional support. Two

boxes were constructed to be clear, with an open bottom, to test chemoreception + vision. Two

boxes were constructed to be clear with an open bottom, but had pieces of hook-velcro placed

throughout the inside of the box so that black felt could be attached to make the box opaque,

when needed, in order to test chemoreception. One box was constructed to be clear with a closed

bottom in order to be water-tight, to test vision only (Table 2, Figures 2 and 3).

Page 59: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

45

Table 2. Box Design Allowing for the Control of Sensory Cues Provided.

Chemoreception Vision

Clear Open Box +a +

Black Open Box + -b

Clear Closed Box - +

a (+) indicates sensory cues provided

b (-) indicates sensory cues eliminated

Manatees were habituated to the plexiglass boxes for the week prior to the start of

experiments by leaving clear and black open boxes out with food items in them in an alcove

amongst the mangrove trees along San San-Pond Sak river where choice trial experiments were

conducted (9°32’1.896”N, 82°31’30.846”W). Plexiglass boxes were placed along the mangrove

edge with a minimum depth of about a meter. Clear and black boxes with food items were also

placed randomly along the river to assist in the habituation process. The habituation process

helped determine if the designed methodology allowed for the control of sensory cues provided

and if manatees would enter the boxes and eat the food items from the various box types.

During the habituation period, a two-tiered viewing post was built consisting of two

benches on the branches of the mangrove trees. The upper bench was about three meters above

and a meter to the side of where the boxes were hung for the short distance dichotomous choice

trials. The lower tier was about two meters above and two meters to the side of where the boxes

were hung for the short distance dichotomous choice trials. The upper bench allowed for two

people to sit safely and the lower tier allowed for one person to sit safely.

Page 60: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

46

Trials

Sensory modalities were tested in two ways, short distance trials and long-distance trials.

Short distance trials were conducted using dichotomous choices in order to test the ability of

manatees to find food when they knew food was in the area. Long distance trials were conducted

by placing boxes in random locations where food had not been previously placed in order to test

the sensory modalities used to find food.

Short distance dichotomous choice trials were conducted in an alcove of mangrove trees

found along San San-Pond Sak River, Panama (9°32’1.896” N, 82°31’30.846” W). All boxes

were hung from the same location in the mangrove trees, approximately one-meter apart. Boxes

were submerged to approximately equal depths, about three-quarters of their height. Food items

were hung within one of the boxes. The box the food items were hung in was randomized by

using head and tales. No more than three trials in a row could occur with food in the same box,

positioned left or right, in order to prevent the manatees from learning which box the food was

in.

Figure 2. Box Design. Pictured above (A) clear open box with a banana leaf hanging inside to

test chemoreception + vision, (B) black open box with a banana leaf hanging inside to test

chemoreception only, (C) clear closed box with a banana leaf hanging inside to test vision only.

C A B

Page 61: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

47

Figure 3. Constructed Boxes. Pictured above are the constructed boxes (A) clear open box (B)

black open box (C) clear closed box.

Due to the difficulty of seeing through the water, underwater cameras were placed inside

the test boxes to record the manatees inside the box, in addition to field notes of observations.

For each short distance dichotomous choice trial cameras were mounted at the top of each box,

with the camera angled down to record the opening of the box. The cameras were numbered and

the number of the camera in the box was recorded, along with whether the box had food or no

food. Food items were hung in the designated box and the cameras were reset by canoeing from

our post to where the boxes were positioned. We then canoed back to the post, where we could

observe the manatees from above, and where the canoe could be docked away from the

experimental setup. One person always remained at the post to record the subjects’ choice as the

manatees often began the trial before the canoe was docked. Additionally, the observer recorded

the subjects’ choices during each trial in addition to being videotaped; video footage trumped

observer results if a discrepancy occurred.

Short distance dichotomous choice trials conducted testing chemoreception + vision used

the clear, open boxes. Trials were considered complete once the manatee entered the box, with

the adult manatees fitting about the rostral quarter of their body when inside the entire box.

A C B

Page 62: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

48

Manatees were allowed to eat the food items for a few minutes as a reward and keep them

participating in the study, then we canoed over to reset the trials by placing new food in the

designated box, and the cameras reset. When we canoed to reset trials, the manatees would swim

to the other side of the alcove or into the open water nearby, and then come back after the canoe

was no longer near the boxes.

Short distance dichotomous choice trials conducted testing chemoreception only, used the

opaque (black), open boxes. Black pieces of felt were mounted on hook-velcro in both boxes to

make them both opaque; clear duct tape® was also used to keep the felt in place as needed.

Trials were considered complete once the manatee entered the box or swam up to one of the

boxes. Manatees were allowed to eat the food items for a few minutes as a reward, then the trial

was reset by placing new food in the designated box, and the cameras reset. Trials were

conducted one after the other, with multiple trials taking place within one day, until the manatees

ceased to participate or environmental conditions did not allow proper visualization. Short

distance trials were conducted over the course of three days, all open clear box trials the first

day, all open black box trials the second day, and all closed clear box trials the third day.

Manatees were found to be most active in these trials from about 1400-1600 hours.

Short distance dichotomous choice trials conducted testing vision only used a clear,

closed box and a clear, open box due to limited supplies. Food items always remained in the

clear, closed box and the entire box was moved from left to right as needed, per the heads/tails

designation. Trials were considered complete once the manatee swam up to one of the boxes

from the side or from below. After a choice was made, food items were placed near the boxes as

a reward and they were allowed to eat for a few minutes before it was removed. The trial was

then reset by placing the box with food on the designated side and the cameras reset.

Page 63: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

49

Long distance trials were conducted by hanging food items inside the clear open

plexiglass boxes, testing chemoreception + vision, as well as being placed inside the black open

boxes, testing chemoreception only, and without boxes, serving as controls. Boxes with food

items or food items without boxes were hung randomly along the San San-Pond Sak River,

Panama where food had not been previously placed. Boxes were submerged about three-quarters

of their height when initially placed, understanding that water levels changed with the tides. The

time, date, and box type placed along the river was recorded, along with each boxes’ GPS

location. The time and date of when the boxes were checked were recorded continually until the

food was no longer present or had been partially eaten. The box was then moved to a new

location at least two meters away, and new food items hung within the box or without a box.

Data was collected by watching the video recordings of manatees as well as reviewing

field notes. Data are discussed in the results section below.

IACUC procedures were followed for Andrews University.

The permit needed for conducting research on manatees in San San-Pond Sak Wetland

Reserve was applied for and granted from Ministerio de Ambiente de Panamá (Miambiente) in

July of 2019.

RESULTS

During the habituation period we began with all boxes having a hinged door on the

bottom and a rope to pull to open the door to retrieve the food. However, due to time, we

removed the doors as it would have taken too long to habituate them to opening the doors, if they

were even able to. During the habituation period, we drilled holes near the top to allow chemicals

to disburse faster into the river as that seemed to be a limiting factor in the experimental design.

Page 64: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

50

After the doors were removed, we found that the manatees entered the clear open and black open

boxes and ate the food out of them. It was noted that when food items without a box, clear open

box, and black open box were hung in a close proximity, food items without a box were

consumed first, then food items in the clear open box, and lastly, food items in the black open

box were consumed. After a week of habituation and making the needed modifications to the

boxes for use in limiting sensory cues provided by food, we were able to proceed to the

experiments.

Figure 4. Choice Trial Set-Up. (A) Clear open boxes set up for choice trial experiments testing

the ability of manatees to find food using chemoreceptive + visual cues. (B) Black open boxes

set up for choice trial experiments testing the ability of manatees to find food using only

chemoreceptive cues.

For the short distance dichotomous choice trials (Figure 4), the percent of correct choices

was calculated for each trial type (e.g. clear open box) by totaling the number of times manatees

chose the correct box containing the food item divided by the number of trials for the box type.

For the clear open box, testing chemoreception + vision (n=9), the percent of correct choices was

67% or 6 out of 9 trials (Figure 5). The black open box, testing chemoreception only (n=5), had

correct choices 3 out of 5 trials or 60% of the time and the clear open box (Figure 5), testing

vision only (n=5), had correct choices 3 out of 5 trials or 60% of the time (Figure 5). The percent

of correct choices was compared to the rate of occurrence at or above the level of chance (50%)

A B

Page 65: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

51

with all results occurring above the level of chance (Figure 5). Data could not be analyzed for

these trials using statistical analysis given the small sample sizes.

For the short distance dichotomous choice trials, it was a concern that the animals would

swim to the same box every time, positioned left or right, since the same animals may participate

in multiple trials. Food item position was randomized between boxes to help prevent this bias.

Results showed no preference to the box in the left or right position demonstrated for the black

open box, however for the clear open box, manatees chose the left box 7 out of 9 trials and for

the clear closed box, manatees chose the right box for 5 out of 5 trials (Appendix A). Video

footage did not allow for clear differentiation of participating manatees, although there were

usually a few in the area during our trials, with one mother calf pair being documented on many

accounts. The mother was distinguishable in video footage given her engorged mammary glands.

If more than one animal approached the boxes, only the first one to enter or approach the box

counted in our results, although the rate of this occurrence is unknown as the video footage only

captured the manatee inside the open boxes and multiple manatees approaching the box was not

seen in the footage using the closed boxes.

Video footage from the short distance dichotomous choice trials was only available for

11 of the 19 short distance dichotomous choice trials due to corrupt files, recordings which were

not long enough, and manatees not captured in the footage due to angle of the camera or manatee

position relative to the camera angle. Video footage for the open clear box trials was available

for four of the nine trials. Two of the trials using video footage and observer data had a

discrepancy, resulting in video footage results being used in the data analysis. Video footage for

the open black box trials was available for two of the five trials. For the two trials which video

footage was available, observed data coincided with video data. Video footage was available for

Page 66: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

52

all closed clear box trials and all results were consistent with the observed data. As a result, there

was a discrepancy rate of about 18% (2 of 11 trials) between video footage and observer data in

our trials.

Figure 5. Choice Trial Results. Percent of correct choices for each box type in choice trials is

displayed above. Clear open box (67%), black open box (60%), and clear closed box (60%). The

dashed line represents 50% or the rate of chance.

For the long-distance trials, the average minimum time in hours it took manatees to find

the food items placed randomly along San San-Pond Sak River, Panama was calculated for the

various box types (Appendix B). Average minimum time was calculated by computing the

amount of time from initial placement to the time of the check when food was last seen for each

trial, adding together the times for a trial type (e.g. clear open box), then dividing by the number

of trials that took place. The average minimum time for the food items to be eaten from the clear

open box (n=3), testing chemoreception + vision, was 12.0 hours (Figure 6). The average

minimum time for food items to be eaten when hung without a box (n=9), serving as a control,

was 6.89 hours (Figure 6). Manatees never consumed food from the black open boxes in the long

distance trials, but rather two boxes were removed at 22 hours (Appendix B) giving results of

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

Clear Open Box Black Open Box Clear Closed Box

Per

cent

of

Co

rrec

t C

ho

ices

Box Type

n=5 n=5 n=9

Page 67: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

53

greater than 22 hours for two trials and one result of greater than 24 hours was obtained for

manatees to find and consume food items. This resulted in the average minimum time for the

food items to be found and consumed from the black open box (n=3), testing chemoreception

only to be more than 22 hours (Figure 6). Animals were not seen nearby when boxes were being

placed for the long distance trials.

Figure 6. Long Distance Experimental Results. Mean minimum time in hours it took manatees

to find and consume the food items placed randomly along the San San-Pond Sak River, Panama

in the various box types. n=3 for boxes; n=9 for no box condition. Standard deviation for the

food items to be eaten from the clear open box and leaves only, no box was 10.69 hours and 9.88

hours, respectively. *Food items were never consumed from the black open box and no standard

deviation can be calculated.

During long distance experimental trials, when boxes were placed along the river,

manatees consumed food items without boxes and food items in the clear open boxes. During

short distance dichotomous choice trials, manatees consumed food items in clear open boxes and

in the black open boxes as well as food items placed outside the closed boxes as rewards (Figure

7). Furthermore, they approached the clear closed box containing food items although they could

not retrieve them in the short distance dichotomous choice trials. Manatees entered boxes and no

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

Clear Open Box Black Open Box Leaves Only

Mea

n M

inim

um

Tim

e in

Ho

urs

to

Fin

d

Ban

ana

Lea

ves

Box Type

a

n=3

n=3*

n=9

Page 68: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

54

sense of panic was demonstrated while in the box and boxes were large enough that manatees

were able to easily maneuver in and out of the boxes. When manatees were within the entire box,

and their muzzle was seen above the water while they were eating, it appeared about one-quarter

of their body length was within the box.

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to determine if the designed methodology could be used

with manatees to control for the sensory cues emitted from the food source in order to test the

sensory modalities used by manatees to find food when they know food is in the area and when it

is randomly placed throughout their environment. Sensory modalities tested were

chemoreception + vision, chemoreception, and vision. The short distance dichotomous choice

trials were designed to test what sensory modalities manatees use to hone in on food location

when they know it is in the vicinity. Long distance trials were designed to test what sensory

modalities manatees use to find food randomly in their environment, and how long it takes them

to find and consume the food, depending on the sensory modalities given. Furthermore, we

aimed to determine whether there was a difference in sensory modalities used for the different

Figure 7. Manatee Eating Banana Leaves. Antillean manatee (Trichechus manatus

manatus) eating the leaves placed outside the boxes during choice trial experiments.

Page 69: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

55

task, honing in on food location when within a short distance, versus finding it randomly

throughout their habitat, when food is located farther away.

Overall, limitations for this study were due to lack of time in the field and not knowing

what was needed, given this study was the first of its type. Given more funds, more plexiglass

could have been flown into Panama for more boxes to be constructed. More plexiglass would

have allowed for more boxes to be constructed and more experiments to be run simultaneously,

providing increased sample sizes for all experiments. Originally, we planned to take enough

plexiglass for 10 boxes, but due to the cost of shipment, we thought five boxes would be

sufficient. Additionally, the cameras were a limitation in that they were not reliable, however the

number of cameras, SD cards, and batteries we had were sufficient, had the cameras worked as

intended. Also, given more time to conduct experiments, we could have further increased our

sample sizes allowing for statistical analysis and further conclusions to be drawn, however the

time it took to get permits and boxes made in the field, delayed the start of experimental trials by

a few days.

While similar quantities of each banana leaves and banana fruit were used between trials,

it would be best to use the same number of leaves and banana fruit in every trial to prevent the

potential of varying quantities of chemical cues or a slightly differing visual image provided by

the food items in each trial.

The black open box posed a unique problem in that it appeared manatees were hesitant to

enter the box (Appendix C). When conducting short distance dichotomous choice trials testing

chemoreception only, manatees were only observed to enter the black open box once to retrieve

food, and did not enter again, however, our sample size was small (n=5). They were observed

swimming up to, and around the correct box, but they would not enter the box. None of the black

Page 70: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

56

open boxes were eaten from in the long-distance trials, while most other locations using no boxes

or clear open boxes were discovered within a 24-hour period of being placed along the river.

However, it was also observed that during the afternoon hours, when manatees were most active

in participating in our experiments, the sun and water caused the opaque plexiglass to act as a

mirror. The effect of manatees potentially seeing a manatee reflected by the box may have been a

confounding factor in this experiment. This confounding factor could be eliminated given more

time to familiarize the manatees with the boxes, eliminating the boxes as a variable in the

experiments.

A limitation to the vision only trials was supplies due to limited funds, as previously

mentioned. We did not have enough plexiglass sheets to construct two, clear closed boxes.

However, since the manatees could not enter the boxes for this trial, a choice was counted when

a manatee swam up to a box, making no difference as to whether the bottom sheet of plexiglass

was present. The box appeared the same from the side with or without the bottom sheet of

plexiglass, unless they swam under the box, and at that point the choice had already been made

per our criteria. The food was always placed in the closed box and the positions of the boxes

changed rather than the position of the food as occurred in the other trials. Therefore, this

limiting factor is not thought to threaten the integrity of the experimental trials testing vision. If

repeated in the future, it is recommended that one more clear closed box be constructed and used.

For the short distance dichotomous choice trials, sample sizes were too small for statistics

to be conducted and as a result, caution should be taken in major implications made by this

study. It is important to note that during short distance dichotomous choice trials, manatees had

been conditioned to know food was present in the area. Food was placed in that location the

week prior to conducting experiments as well as being left out over the weekend and at night

Page 71: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

57

while trials were not being conducted. Manatees were found to be most active in the alcove short

distance dichotomous choice trials were conducted from about 1400-1600 hours, which was

when the sun hit the water making it easiest for the observers to see through.

One note to be made is in regards to the box position manatees swam to during short

distance dichotomous choice trials when making their choice (Appendix A). Results were

compared to the rate of occurrence at or above the level of chance (Figure 5). This means, with

the box being randomly placed either in the left position or right position, if they always swim to

the same box position, they would swim to the box containing food items approximately 50% of

the time per the law of probability. By results being above 50% it is suggested there are factors,

such as sensory cues, influencing the decision of what box is approached to obtain food. For the

chemoreception + vision experiments, using the clear open box, the manatees chose the box in

the left position for seven of nine trials and only swam to the box positioned on the right two of

nine trials with 67% accuracy in their choice. However, during the trials of the black open box,

testing chemoreception, the manatees swam to the box positioned on the right three of five trials

and to the box positioned on the left for two of the five trials, with 60% accuracy in their choice.

During the trials of the clear closed box, testing vision, the manatees swam to the box positioned

on the right for all five trials with 60% accuracy in their choice. It does not appear that manatees

preferred a given box position overall, however, in the vision experiments, they always chose the

box positioned on the right and in the chemoreception + vision experiment, they chose the box

positioned on the left seven out of nine trials. The manatees participating and making the choices

were not distinguished from one another in this study, and is not thought to be a variable, as we

were interested in how they find the food, not who found the food or how many. However, if

different manatees were present on the different days each trial type was conducted, that may

Page 72: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

58

account for the difference in box position preference. There could also be other external,

unknown factors in the manatees swimming a majority, if not all the time, to the box in the same

position for certain trials, such as the direction of the current. It has been shown in African

elephants, a close relative of manatees, that they have a preference to choosing objects on the

right side versus the left in choice trials when food was placed in one of two pots on the floor,

five meters apart from each other, and four meters from the elephant participating in the trial

(Dale, 2008). It is possible manatees have a preference to sides as well, however that was not

necessarily demonstrated in this study, but should be looked at in future studies with larger

sample sizes.

While the sensory cues provided by the food items with no boxes and the clear open

boxes were the same, the box played a role in some way, given it took almost twice as long for

the manatees to eat the food items from the clear open boxes versus food items just hanging in

the water. It is possible the boxes limited the diffusion of chemicals into the water for manatees

to readily detect. When banana leaves were broken and placed in the water, you could see the

chemicals on the surface of the water. We drilled holes on the tops of the open boxes to help the

chemicals be able to diffuse in the case the box prevented them from spreading – still, no box

being present allows for faster diffusion of the banana leaf chemicals even with the boxes having

holes drilled in them.

We did not put a time limit on the long-distance experiments initially while running

them, and left the boxes out for days if untouched, except for two black boxes which were

removed at 22 hours. However, after reviewing the data, 24-hours is sufficient time for the

manatees to discover the food items or not, limiting sensory cues. Therefore, in the future, a time

limit of 24-hours should be placed on the experiments and at that time if the plant material has

Page 73: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

59

not been eaten, move the box or food items to a new location. Additionally, the boxes needed to

be checked more frequently. Weather, time of day, and boat access limited the ability of

checking the boxes at consistent intervals, however, conducted again, it is recommended boxes

be checked at consistent intervals, day and night, rain or shine.

SHORT DISTANCE DICHOTOMOUS CHOICE TRIALS: CHEMORECEPTION + VISION

The goal of this study was to determine which sensory modality manatees use to locate

food in their natural habitats. The percent of correct choices for the clear open box, testing

chemoreception + vision (n=9) was 67% or 6 out of 9 trials (Figure 5). These results are not

surprising as manatees were provided multiple sensory modalities, chemoreception and vision,

during these trials, and literature suggests animals perform better at finding food when provided

multiple modalities rather than just one. Consistent with research conducted on primates, grey

mouse lemurs, animals are often able to find food better when provided multiple sensory

modalities rather than when limited to just one sensory modality (Piep et al., 2008). However,

how well these sensory modalities function alone is also of interest.

SHORT DISTANCE DICHOTOMOUS CHOICE TRIALS: CHEMORECEPTION ONLY

The black open box, testing chemoreception only (n=5), had correct choices 3 out of 5

trials or 60% of the time (Figure 5). Accuracy of choosing the correct box decreased when visual

cues were removed, which makes sense given many mammals perform better when provided

multiple sensory cues, as mentioned previously, rather than limited to the use of one sensory

modality (Piep et al., 2008). It is not known if the velcro or felt released any chemicals into the

water, however, there were the same number and size of velcro pieces in each box as well as the

felt being approximately the same size in each box. If chemicals were released by these

Page 74: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

60

materials, they were likely in equal quantities during the trials. The duct-tape used to help secure

the felt was on the top of the box, positioned above of the water, so that likely did not released

chemicals into the water, however, if it did it would have also been in approximately equal

quantities between the boxes. Accuracy of choosing the correct box may have also decreased due

to the manatees being repulsed by the mirror-action of the black box.

Little is understood about chemoreceptive mechanisms and their functionality in

manatees; however, chemoreception is thought to exist in manatees given their frequent

mouthing of objects in their environment (Hartman, 1979; Wartzok & Ketten, 1999; Reynolds,

Powell, & Taylor, 2008). It is thought chemoreception occurs via taste buds in sirenians given

their inability to open their nostrils while submerged in order for chemicals to enter the nasal

cavity (Lowell & Flanigan, 1980; Reep & Bonde, 2006; Reynolds, Powell, & Taylor, 2008;

Thewissen, 2008). While caution should be taken in drawing conclusions from this experiment,

given the sample size and lack of statistical analysis, preliminary results suggest chemoreception

plays a role in manatees ability to find food, given their rate of success in choice trials, testing

chemoreception, was above the rate of chance (50%) and their ability to find and consume food

within the black open boxes.

SHORT DISTANCE DICHOTOMOUS CHOICE TRIALS: VISION ONLY

The clear closed box, testing vision only (n=5), had correct choices 3 out of 5 trials or

60% of the time (Figure 5). Accuracy of choosing the correct box decreased when chemical cues

were removed, which makes sense given many mammals perform better when provided multiple

sensory cues, as mentioned previously, rather than when limited to the use of one sensory

modality (Piep et al., 2008). However, the accuracy of choosing the correct box was the same

(60%) when only one sensory modality, chemoreception or vision, was used.

Page 75: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

61

Vision of manatees was originally considered to be poor due to the size and shape of their

eyes (Dexler & Freund, 1906). However, behaviors observed by Hartman (1979) and Gerstein

(1994) propose vision is used daily by manatees through environmental exploration. Research

suggests that the vision of manatees is best suited for focusing on objects from a distance (one

meter or more) rather than within a close proximity (Bauer et al., 2003a). It has been shown that

manatees have dichromatic color vision, allowing them to see the blue and green parts of the

color spectrum, which may provide contrast in their habitats with limited visibility, due to

tannin-stained water (Cohen et al., 1982; Griebel & Schmid, 1996). While caution should be

taken in drawing conclusions from this experiment, given the sample size and lack of statistical

analysis, preliminary results suggest it is likely that vision plays a role in manatees ability to find

food, given their rate of success in choice trials, testing vision, was above the rate of chance

(50%) and their ability to approach the clear closed box in attempts to obtain food.

LONG DISTANCE TRIALS

During long distance trials, food was placed randomly along San San-Pond Sak River,

Panama using the three different experimental types: food items hanging in the water without a

box, food items hanging in clear open plexiglass boxes, and food items hanging in black open

plexiglass boxes. From this experiment we can see that they find and consume food fastest when

the food items are hung in the water without a box (6.89 hours, Figure 6) and they never

demonstrated the ability to find and eat food items when they are in a black open box (more than

22 hours, Figure 6). However, due to limited data we are unable to conclude which sensory

modality manatees use when finding food randomly located in their environment.

It is possible that different senses provide different ranges for discovering food in their

natural habitat and the use of multiple senses allows better discernment in the locating of food

Page 76: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

62

(Piep et al., 2008). Both chemoreception and vision have limited understanding in regards to

their anatomy and physiology in manatees, requiring further research to better understand how

manatees utilize chemoreception and vision in unison and in combination, in their daily tasks,

such as finding food.

Manatees may find food in their natural environment based off of location and memory.

Manatees may stumble upon food when swimming through their environment and be able to

recall later where food was found, developing a food map of sorts from the location of food.

When swimming around new places, food may be found, and again remembered and recalled at

a later time. While this seems unlikely to be the primary way manatees discover food, rather than

using their senses, it may be a way they find food every now and again. The possibility that

manatees are able to remember and recall the location of food is reasonable given that spatial in

memory has been found to be used by a number of animals in order to find food including

primates (Janson, 1998), sheep (Dumont & Petit, 1998), cattle (Laca, 1998), and birds (Bennett,

1993; Shettleworth, 1990). No studies were found that have been conducted on the spatial

memory abilities of sirenians; further research should be conducted in this area.

In conclusion, we were able to demonstrate that our experimental design does work, and

that the sensory modalities used by manatees to find food can be tested using this methodology

(Appendix C). It is recommended these methods be used to test manatees in captivity in order to

determine the sensory modalities used by manatees to locate food at short distances. It is also

indicated from our study that more time be given to familiarize the manatees with the boxes to

eliminate the box from being a confounding factor during the experiment. It is thought from

these preliminary results that chemoreception + vision, that is, multiple modalities, are most

efficient for manatees to find food in their natural environment, but chemoreception and vision

Page 77: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

63

can be used independent of each other to find food, likely with decreased success. From our

results it cannot be concluded if chemoreception or vision is able to provide more information to

the manatee about food location as they approach. The extent to which chemoreception or vision

are able to be used independently to find food, whether honing in on the exact location of food,

or finding it from a distance, remains unknown and needs further research with increased sample

size so statistical analysis can be conducted. From our limited data, we were also unable to

determine a difference of sensory modalities used for locating food at short versus long

distances, but should be considered in future studies.

Page 78: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

64

CHAPTER 4

CONCLUSIONS

While sirenians, specifically manatees, are the members of the Paenungulata clade which

have the most research conducted on their sensory modalities, there is still much research to be

done on their sensory modalities as well as the sensory modalities of the other members of the

clade. While initial anatomical analysis of sirenian structures, such as the eyes or ear bones of

manatees, suggested a certain degree of functionality, behavioral analysis of these sensory

systems has increased the understanding of how these structures function. Functionality and

usefulness of structures was often found to be improved from what was initially thought based

only on the anatomical structures. However, studies conducted testing visual acuity, ability to

discriminate between parts of the color spectrum, or hearing tests conducted on manatees, are

limited to a couple of published studies supporting these claims, as a result, there is often not

much conflicting evidence. It is recommended that more studies be conducted, especially

behavioral, to validate previous studies and enhance understanding where possible.

It is also noteworthy that most studies conducted on sirenians are based off of West

Indian manatees, specifically, Florida manatees (Reep & Bonde, 2006). While this is a good

start, it is important that all species and subspecies of manatees be tested similarly to see if there

are differences within the sirenian order (Gonzalez-Socoloske, 2013). Given their different

environments, habitats, and amount of human contact, amongst other things, it would not be

surprising if differences are found within the sirenian order. Little research has been conducted

on dugongs, given their location, protected status, and inability to be easily kept in captivity.

However, more information is needed on dugongs, given their different diets, fully marine

lifestyle, and varied anatomical features, it seems unlikely that they would have the same

measures and functionality of sensory modalities as manatees do.

Page 79: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

65

By reviewing the sensory modalities of the Paenungulata clade, similarities were

expected to be found between sirenians, proboscideans, and hyracoideans that may reveal further

information about their sensory abilities. However, it was found that sirenians have had more

research conducted on their sensory abilities than proboscideans or hyracoideans. Similarities

and differences between the sensory modalities of the Paenungulata clade are outlined in Table

1. Overall, proboscideans and sirenians have hearing ranges that differ extensively, with

proboscideans being capable of low frequency hearing and sirenians having hearing better suited

for high frequencies. It is possible the difference in hearing abilities between proboscideans and

sirenians is due to their different environments, with proboscideans being terrestrial and sirenians

being fully aquatic. Manatees, dugongs, elephants, and hyrax all have at least one type of

vibrissae on their body. All have taste buds in varying quantities. Manatees and dugongs lack a

vomeronasal organ unlike elephants and hyrax. The vision of all members is reduced, with

elephants seemingly having the best vision of the clade. Vestibular research is needed for

proboscideans and hyracoideans, but sirenians are known to have reduced semicircular canals to

assist with spatial orientation and balance as well as their flippers assisting with balancing.

From previous research conducted on manatees, it was believed that vision and

chemoreception are present in manatees, but the extent to which they are used for everyday

navigation of their environments remains unknown as well as the role they play in the acquisition

of food. In our study, we set out to develop a methodology that could be used to test individual

and combined sensory modalities used by manatees to locate food in their environment. We also

sought to find which sensory modalities manatees use to find food and which sensory modalities

were more important for finding food and if that differed based on whether they knew food was

nearby or if they did not know the location of food.

Page 80: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

66

In our study we were able to develop an underwater methodology to test vision +

chemoreception, vision only, and chemoreception only of manatees. Plexiglass boxes were

constructed to be clear open, black open, and clear closed. Boxes were hung from mangrove tress

along San San-Pond Sak wetlands, Panama for short distance dichotomous choice trials, when

manatees knew food was nearby and for long distance trials, when food was placed randomly

along the river. Manatees demonstrated the ability to enter boxes without distress and consumed

the food items from all open boxes, as well as approached the closed boxes.

While the hearing thresholds of manatees have been determined, it is thought unlikely

that manatees use sound as a way of locating food, given they have an herbivorous diet and most

plants do not provide auditory cues. However, conspecific mastication sounds may alert

manatees to where food is, as they often eat where food is found (Kikuchi et al., 2014; Reynolds,

1981). It was determined that touch was not a likely sensory modality used by manatees to

initially find food given tactile cues require close proximity, and have been found to be used in

determining which food is consumed, but are not likely used in the locating of food initially.

In our research, we tested vision and chemoreception together and apart in order to try

and determine which sensory modality was used to find food. While data were limited, we

believe manatees use both chemoreception and vision when finding food in their natural habitat.

Manatees were found to have the greatest success finding food correctly, when they knew it was

nearby, by using vision + chemoreception. When one of the sensory modalities was taken away,

either vision or chemoreception, their accuracy in choice decreased to 60%, but remained above

the level of chance and had no meaningful difference from choice experiments using multiple

modalities.

Page 81: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

67

When testing the ability of manatees to find food which was randomly placed around

their natural habitat of San San-Pond Sak River, Panama, they most readily located and

consumed food items which were uninhibited by a box, and it took them about twice as long to

locate and consume food items in clear boxes with vision + chemoreceptive cues provided.

When visual cues were removed, manatees never consumed the food items from inside the black

boxes during our limited number of trials.

From past research, and the research we conducted in Panama, it can be concluded that

manatees have a sense of chemoreception, however, it cannot be differentiated from our research

whether it was a sense of smell or taste that was able to direct the locating of food. Further

research is needed on the anatomical structures and testing of these two senses, if possible, to

determine how they function in these mammals.

While the rate at which the boxes testing vision only were chosen was above the rate of

chance, the manatees always swam to the right in these trials, making it difficult to determine

whether or not they saw the food in that box, or just swam to the box on the right for unknown

reasons. Previous research has suggested that manatees use vision to maneuver their habitats and

as a result, it is likely they use vision to find food, it is just not well understood to what extent

vision plays a role in the locating of food. Vision could potentially be used by manatees to find

food from a distance of one meter or more, or they may use it when within a close proximity of

about 30 cm, or both (Bauer et al., 2003a).

As for a difference in the sensory cues used to find food when the manatees knew that

food was in the vicinity for the choice trials, versus when the food was randomly placed at

locations around the river, conclusions could not be drawn at this time given the limited data

available from our study. However, it was noted that it took the manatees about half the time to

Page 82: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

68

consume the food items without a box versus those with a box. This suggests, they are either

hesitant to enter the box or the box does not allow chemicals to disburse as quickly, indicating

that the chemicals are an important cue and that in the future, more holes should be drilled

throughout the box to allow for chemical dispersion to occur more rapidly in the water.

All members of the order Sirenia are vulnerable according to the IUCN red list. As a

result, their conservation and better preservation of their habitats is of great importance for these

species. In our study, we focused on the sensory modalities used by manatees to locate food and

how those could be tested. It is important to understand the sensory modalities, especially those

used to find food, in order to better conserve their habitats. If the sensory cues used by manatees

are being inhibited in the environment by anthropogenic factors and/or habitat loss, we need to

know what those sensory cues are in order to allow for their transmission in the aquatic

environment and ability to be received by manatees. If manatees are unable to find food in an

environment, due to limiting factors, there could be consequences including moving to new areas

or even, death. In this study, we hope to have provided more information regarding the sensory

modalities of manatees, aiding in the successful conservation of this vulnerable species.

Page 83: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

69

REFERENCES

Aleman, A. A. (2011). El Manati. In R. Borroto-Paez & C. A. Mancina (Eds.), Mamiferos en

Cuba (pp. 178-185). Vasa, Finland: UPC Print.

Alves-Stanley, C. D., Worthy, G. A., & Bonde, R. K. (2010). Feeding preferences of West

Indian manatees in Florida, Belize, and Puerto Rico as indicated by stable isotope

analysis. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 402, 255-267.

Anderson, P.K., & Barclay, M.R. (1995). Acoustic signals of solitary dugongs: physical

characteristics and behavioral correlates. Journal of Mammalogy, 76(4), 1226-1237.

Bates, L.A., Poole, J.H., & Byrne, R.W. (2009). Elephant cognition. Current Biology, 18(13),

R544-R546.

Bauer, G.B., Colbert, D.E., Gaspard, J.C., Littlefield, B., & Fellner, W. (2003a). Underwater

visual acuity of Florida manatees (Trichechus manatus latirostris). International Journal

of Comparative Psychology, 16(2), 129-142.

Bauer, G.B., Mann, D., Reynolds, J.E., & Colbert, D (2003b). Sensory processes of manatees.

Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission Contract, Final report.

Bauer, G.B., Gaspard, J.C., Colbert, D.E., Leach, J.B., Mann, D., & Reep, R. (2012). Tactile

discrimination of textures by Florida manatees (Trichechus manatus latirostris). Marine

Mammal Science, 28(4), 456-471.

Bauer, G.B., Reep, R.L., Marshall, C.D. (2018). The tactile senses of marine mammals.

International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 31, 1-28.

Page 84: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

70

Ben-Ari, E.T. (1999). A throbbing in the air: the discovery of infrasonic communication among

elephants has given researchers a whole new way of hearing things. BioScience, 49(5),

353-358.

Bennett, A.T.D. (1993) Spatial memory in a food storing corvid. Journal of Comparative

Physiology A, 173, 193-207.

Bills, M.L. (2011). Description of the chemical senses of the Florida manatee, Trichechus

manatus latirostris, in relation to reproduction [Doctoral dissertation, University of

Florida].

Blair, P. (1717). A Description of the Organ of Hearing in the Elephant, with the Figures and

Situation of the Ossicles, Labyrinth and Cochlea in the Ear of That Large Animal.

Communicated to the Royal Society, by Dr. Patrick Blair, R. S. S. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 30(351-363), 885–898.

Bullock, T.H., Domning, D.P., & Best, R.C. (1980). Evoked brain potentials demonstrate hearing

in a manatee (Trichechus inunguis). Journal of Mammalogy, 61(1), 130-133.

Bullock, T.H., O’Shea, T.J., & McClune, M.C. (1982). Auditory evoked potentials in the West

Indian manatee (Sirenia: Trichechus manatus). Journal of Comparative Physiology, 148,

547-554.

Byrne, R.W., Bates, L.A., & Moss, C.J. (2009). Elephant cognition in primate perspective.

Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 4, 65-79.

Page 85: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

71

Castelblanco-Martínez, D. N., Morales-Vela, B., Hernández-Arana, H. A., & Padilla-Saldivar, J.

(2009). Diet of the manatees (Trichechus manatus manatus) in Chetumal Bay, Mexico.

Latin American Journal of Aquatic Mammals, 7(1-2).

Chapla, M.E. & Rommel, S.A. (2003). Comparative gross morphology of the manatee outer and

middle ear. In: Fifteenth Biennial Marine Mammal Conference poster presentation,

Greensboro, NC.

Chapla, M.E. (2006). Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris) outer and middle ear

morphology: potential sound conduction pathways and middle-ear mechanism [Master’s

thesis, Florida State University]. DigiNole: FSU’s Digital Repository.

Chapla, M.E., Nowacek, D.P., Rommel, S.A., & Sadler, V.M. (2007). CT scans and 3D

reconstructions of Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris) heads and ear bones.

Hearing Research, 228(1-2), 123-135.

Cohen, J.L., Tucker, G.S., & Odell, D.K. (1982). The photoreceptors of the West Indian

manatee. Journal of Morphology, 173(2), 197-202.

Colares, I.G. & Colares, E.P. (2002). Food plants eaten by Amazonian manatees (Trichechus

inunguis, Mammalia: Sirenia). Brazilian Archives of Biology and Technology, 45(1), 67-

72.

Cornelis, G., Vernochet, C., Malicorne, S., Souquere, S., Tzika, A.C., Goodman, S.M., Catzeflis,

F., Robinson, T.J., Milinkovitch, M.C., Pierron, G., Heidmann, O., Dupressoir, A., &

Heidmann, T. (2014). Retroviral envelope syncytin capture in an ancestrally diverged

mammalian clade for placentation in the primitive Afrotherian tenrecs. PNAS, 41, E4332-

E4341.

Page 86: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

72

Dale, R.H.I. (2008). The spatial memory of African elephants (Loxodonta africana): durability,

interference, and response biases. N.K. Innis (Ed.), x(pp.143-169). Cambridge, MA: MIT

Press.

Dehnhardt, G., Friese, C., & Sachser, N. (1997). Sensitivity of the trunk of Asian elephants for

texture differences of actively touched objects. Zeitschrift für Säugetierkunde, 62, 37-39.

Demartsev, V., Kershenbaum, A., Ilany, A., Barocas, A., Ziv, E.B., Koren, L., & Geffen, L.

(2014). Male hyraxes increase song complexity and duration in the presence of alert

individuals. Behavioral Ecology, 25(6), 1451-1458.

De Meirelles, A.C.O., Carvalho, V.L., & Marmontel, M. (2018). West Indian Manatee

Trichechus manatus in South America: Distribution, Ecology and Health Assessment. In:

Rossi-Santos M., Finkl C. (eds) Advances in Marine Vertebrate Research in Latin

America. Coastal Research Library, vol 22. Springer, Cham.

Denkinger, J. (2010). Status of the Amazonian manatee (Trichechus inunguis) in the Cuyabeno

Reserve, Ecuador. Avances, 2, b29-b34.

Deutsch, C. J., Self-Sullivan, C., & Mignucci-Giannoni, A. (2008). Trichechus manatus. The

IUCN Red List of Threatened Species, 2008(e.T22103A9356917).

Dexler, H., & Freund, L. (1906). Contributions to the physiology and biology of the dugong. The

American Naturalist, 40(469), 49-72.

Domning, D. P. (1982). Commercial exploitation of manatees Trichechus in Brazil c. 1785–

1973. Biological Conservation, 22(2), 101-126.

Page 87: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

73

Domning, D. P., & Hayek, L. A. C. (1986). Interspecific and intraspecific morphological

variation in manatees (Sirenia: Trichechus). Marine Mammal Science, 2(2), 87- 144.

Dumont, B. & Petit, M. (1998). Spatial memory of sheep at pasture. Applied Animal Behaviour

Science, 60(1), 43-53.

Endo, H., Hayashi, Y., Komiya, T., Narushima, E., & Sasaki, M. (2001). Muscle architecture of

the elongated nose in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximas). Journal of Veterinary

Medical Science, 63(5), 533-537.

Emura, S., Okumura, T., & Chen, H. (2008). Morphology of the lingual papillae and their

connective tissue cores in the cape hyrax. Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica, 85(1),

29-34.

Foote, A.D., Liu, Y., Thomas, G.W.C., Vinar, T., Alfoldi, J., Deng, J., Dugan, S., Elk, C.E.,

Hunter, M.E., Joshi, V., Khan, Z., Kovar, C., Lee, S.L., Lindblad-Toh, K., Mancia, A.,

Nielsen, R., Qin, X., Qu, J., Raney, B.J., Vijay, N., Wolf, J.B.W., Hahn, M.W., Muzny,

D.M., Worley, K.C., Gilbert, M.T.P., & Gibbs, R.A. (2015). Convergent evolution of the

genomes of marine mammals. Nature Genetics, 42, 272-275.

Frei, F. (2016, Nov 20). What does people fascinate about the elephants? Elephant Trunk.

Upalich. https://en.upali.ch/trunk/

Frisch, K. & Frisch, S.A. (2003). Low-frequency vocalizations in the Florida manatee

(Trichechus manatus latirostris). Fifteenth Biennial Marine Mammal Conference poster

presentation. Greensboro, NC.

Page 88: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

74

Garstang, M. Larom, D., Raspet, R., & Lindeque, M. (1995). Atmospheric controls on elephant

communication. Journal of Experimental Biology, 198, 939-951.

Garstang, M. (2004). Long-distance, low-frequency elephant communication. Journal of

Comparative Physiology A, 190, 791-805.

Gaspard, J.C., Bauer, G.B., Reep, R.L. Dziuk, K., Cardwell, A., Read, L., & Mann, D.A. (2012).

Audiogram and auditory critical ratios of two Florida manatees (Trichechus manatus

latirostris). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 215(9), 1442-1447.

Gaspard, J.C., Bauer, G.B., Reep, R.L., Dziuk, K., Read, L., & Mann, D.A. (2013). Detectino of

hydrodynamic stimuli by the Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris). Journal

of Comparative Physiology A, 199, 441-450.

Gerstein, E.R. (1994). The manatee mind: discrimination training for sensory perception of West

Indian manatees (Trichechus manatus). Marine Mammals: Public Display and Research,

vol 1, p 10-21.

Gerstein, E.R., Gerstein, L., Forsythe, S.E., & Blue, J.E. (1999). The underwater audiogram of

the West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 105(6), 3575-3583.

Gerstein, E.R. (2002). Manatees, bioacoustics and boats: hearing tests, environmental

measurements and acoustic phenomena may together explain why boats and animals

collide. American Scientist, 90(2), 154-163.

Griebel, U., & Schmid, A. (1996). Color vision in the manatee (Trichechus manatus). Vision

Research, 36, 2747-2757.

Page 89: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

75

Griebel, U., & Schmid, A. (1997). Brightness discrimination ability in the West Indian manatee

(Trichechus manatus). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 200, 1587-1592.

Gonzalez-Socoloske, D., & Olivera-Gomez, L. D. (2012). Gentle giants in dark waters: Using

side-scan sonar for manatee research. The Open Remote Sensing Journal, 5, 1-14.

Gonzalez-Socoloske, D. (2013). Aspects of the feeding ecology of the Antillean manatee

(Trichechus manatus manatus) in the wetlands of Tabasco, Mexico (Doctoral

dissertation). Duke University, Durham, North Carolina.

Gonzalez-Socoloske, D. Olivera-Gomez, L.D., Reid, J.P., Espinoza-Marin, C., Ruiz, K.,

Glander, K.E. (2015). First successful capture and radio tagging of a West Indian

manatee (Trichechus manatus) in Panama: feasibility of capture and telemetry

techniques. Latin American Journal of Aquatic Mammals, 10(1), 52-57.

Gonzalez-Socoloske, D., & Olivera-Gomez, L.D. (2019). Food choice by an Antillean manatee

in Tabasco, Mexico. Journal of Marine Animals and Their Ecology, 11(1), 19-32.

Hatfield, J.R., Samuelson, D.A., Lewis, P.A., & Chisholm, M. (2003). Structure and presumptive

function of the iridocorneal angle of the West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus),

short-finned pilot whale (Globicephala macrorhynchus), hippopotamus (Hippopotamus

amphibius), and African elephant (Loxodonta africana). Veterinary Ophthalmology, 6(1),

35-43.

Hartman, D.S. (1979). Ecology and behavior of the Manatee (Trichechus manatus) in Florida.

K.W. Kenyon, J.C. Moore (Eds.). Special Publication No.5, American Society of

Mammologists.

Page 90: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

76

Heffner, H.E., & Heffner, R.S. (1981). Functional interaural distance and high-frequency hearing

in the elephant. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 70(6), 1794-1795.

Heffner, R.S., & Heffner, H.E. (1982). Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maximus): absolute

sensitivity, frequency discrimination, and sound localization. Journal of Comparative and

Physiological Psychology, 96(2), 926-944.

Heffner, R., Heffner, H., & Stichman, N., & Zoo, R.M. (1982). Role of the elephant pinna in

sound localization. Animal Behaviour, 30(2), 628-630.

Hoffmann, J.N., Montag, A.G., & Dominy, N.J. (2004). Meissner corpuscles and somatosensory

acuity: the prehensile appendages of primates and elephants. The Anatomical Record Part

A, 281A, 1138-1147.

Home, E. (1815). On the milk tusks, and organ of hearing of the dugong. Proceedings of the

Royal Society of London, 2(0), 144-155.

Husar, S. L. (1977). Trichechus inunguis. Mammalian Species, (72), 1.

Irwin, D.M., & Arnason, U. (1994). Cytochromeb gene of marine mammals: phylogeny and

evolution. Journal of Mammalian Evolution, 2, 37-55.

Janson, C.H. (1998). Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin

monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 55(5), 1229-1243.

Jimenez, I. (2005). Development of predictive models to explain the distribution of the West

Indian manatee Trichechus manatus in tropical watercourses. Biological Conservation,

125(4), 491-503.

Page 91: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

77

Kellogg, M.E., Burkett, S., Dennis, T.R., Stone, G., Gray, B.A., McGuire, P.M., Zori, R.T., &

Stanyon, R. (2007). Chromosome painting in the manatee supports Afrotheria and

Paengulata. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 7(1), 1-6.

Ketten D.R., Odell D.K., & Domning D.P. (1992) Structure, Function, and Adaptation of the

Manatee Ear. In: Thomas J.A., Kastelein R.A., Supin A.Y. (eds) Marine Mammal

Sensory Systems. Springer, Boston, MA

Ketten, D.R. (1998). Marine mammal auditory systems: a summary of audiometric and

anatomical data and its implications for underwater acoustic impacts. The National

Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration.

https://repository.library.noaa.gov/view/noaa/3070

Kikuchi, M., Akamatsu, T., Gonzalez-Socoloske, D., de Souza, D., Olivera-Gomez, L.D., & da

Silva, V.M.F. (2014). Detection of manatee feeding events by animal-borne underwater

sound recorders. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom,

94(6), 1139-1146.

Klishin, V.O., Diaz, R.P., Popov, V.V., & Supin, A.Y. (1990). Some characteristics of hearing of

the Brazilian manatee, Trichechus inunguis. Aquatic Mammals, 16(3), 139-144.

Kubota, K. (1967). Comparative anatomical and neurohistological observations on the tongues of

elephants (Elephas indicus and Loxodonta africana). The Anatomical Record, 157, 505-

516.

Kuntner, M., May-Collado, L.J., Agnarsson, I. (2010). Phylogeny and conservation priorities of

afrotherian mammals (Afrotheria, Mammalia). Zoologica Scripta, 40(1), 1-15.

Page 92: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

78

Laca, E.A. (1998). Spatial memory and food searching mechanisms of cattle. Journal of Range

Management, 51, 370-378.

Laist, D.W., & Reynolds, J.E. (2005). Florida manatees, warm-water refuges, and an uncertain

future. Coastal Management, 33(3), 279-295.

Langbauer, W.R. (2000). Elephant communication. Zoo Biology, 19(2000), 425-445.

Larom, D., Garstang, M., Payne, K., Raspet, R., & Lindeque, M. (1997). The influence of

surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations.

The Journal of Experimental Biology, 200, 421-431.

Lazar, J., Greenwood, D.R., Rasmussen, L.E.L., & Prestwich, G.D. (2002). Molecular and

functional characterization of an odorant binding protein of the Asian elephant, elephas

maximus: implications for the role of lipocalins in mammalian olfaction. Biochemistry,

41, 11786-11794.

Ledder, D. (1986). Food habits of the West Indian manatee, Trichechus manatus latirostris.

South Florida. MSc Thesis. University of Miami. Coral Gables, Florida.

Lefebvre, L. W., Reid, J. P., Kenworthy, W. J., & Powell, J. A. (2000). Characterizing manatee

habitat use and seagrass grazing in Florida and Puerto Rico: Implications for conservation

and management. Pacific Conservation Biology, 5(4), 289-298.

Levin, M.J., & Pfeiffer, C.J. (2002). Gross and microscopic observations on the lingual structure

of the Florida manatee Trichechus manatus latirostris. Anatomia, Histologia,

Embyrologia, 31, 278-285.

Page 93: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

79

Lowell, W.R., & Flanigan, W.F. (1980). Marine mammal chemoreception. Mammal Review,

10(1), 53-59.

Macpherson, K. & Roberts, W.A. (2010). Spatial memory in dogs (Canis familiaris) on a radial

maze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124(1), 47-56.

Makecha, R., Fad, O., Kuczaj, S.A. (2012). The role of touch in the social interactions of Asian

elephants (Elephas maximus). The International Society for Comparative Psychology, 25,

60-82.

Mason, M.J. (2004). Functional morphology of the middle ear in Chlorotalpa golden moles

(Mammalia, Chrysochloridae): predictions from three models. Journal of Morphology,

261, 162-174.

Marmontel, M., Nhea, T.J., Kochman, H.I., Humphrey, S.R. (1996). Age determination in

manatees using growth-layer-group counts in bone. Marine Mammal Science, 12(1), 54-

88.

Marmontel, M., de Souza, D. A., & Kendall, S. (2016). Trichechus inunguis. The IUCN Red List

of Threatened Species, 2016(e.T22102A43793736).

Marsh, H., O'Shea, T. J., & Reynolds III, J. E. (2011). Ecology and Conservation of the Sirenia:

Dugongs and Manatees. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Marsh, H. & Sobtzick, S. (2019). Dugong dugon (amended version of 2015 assessment). The

IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2019: e.T6909A160756767.

Page 94: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

80

Marshall, C.D., Huth, G.D., Edmonds, V.M., Halin, D.L., & Reep, R.L. (1998). Prehensile use of

perioral bristles during feeding and associated behaviors of the Florida manatee

(Trichechus manatus latirostris). Marine Mammal Science, 14(2), 274-289.

Marshall, C.D., Maeda, H., Iwata, M., Furuta, M., Asano, S., Rosas, F., & Reep, R.L. (2003).

Orofacial morphology and feeding behavior of the dugong, Amazonian, West African

and Antillean manatees (Mammalia: Sirenia): functional morphology of the muscular-

vibrissal complex. The Zoological Society of London, 259, 245-260.

Marshall, C.D., Vaughn, S.D., Sarko, D.K., & Reep, R.L. (2007). Topographical organization of

the facial motor nucleus in Florida manatees (Trichechus manatus latirostris). Brain,

Behavior and Evolution, 70, 164-173.

Martin, F.N., & Clark, J.G. (2012). Introduction to Audiology (Eleventh Edition). Pearson.

Mass, A.M., Odell, D.K., Ketten, D.R., & Supin, A.Y. (1996). Ganglion layer topography and

retinal resolution of the Caribbean manatee Trichechus manatus latirostris. General

Biology, 355(3), 427-430.

Mass, A.M., Ketten, D.R., Odell, D.K., & Supin, A.Y. (2012). Ganglion cell distribution and

retinal resolution in the Florida manatee, Trichechus manatus latirostris. The Anatomical

Record, 295, 177-186.

Miller, A.K., Hensman, M.C., Hensman, S., Schultz, K., Reid, P., Shore, M., Brown, J., Furton,

K.G., & Lee, S. (2015). African elephants (Loxodonta africana) can detect TNT using

olfaction: implications for biosensor application. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 171,

177-183.

Page 95: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

81

Murphy, W.J., Pevzner, P.A., & O’Brien, S.J. (2004). Mammalian phylogenomics comes of age.

Trends in Genetics, 20(12), 631-639.

Natiello, M., Lewis, P., & Samuelson, D. (2005). Comparative anatomy of the ciliary body of the

West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus) and selected species. Veterinary

Ophthalmology, 8(6), 375-385.

Natiello, M., & Samuelson, D. (2005). Three-dimensional reconstruction of the angioarchitecture

of the ciliary body of the West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus). Veterinary

Ophthalmology, 8(6), 367-373.

Newman, L.A., & Robinson, P.R. (2006). The visual pigments of the West Indian manatee

(Trichechus manatus). Vision Research, 46, 3326-3330.

Ngwenya, A., Patzke, N., Ihunwo, A.O., & Manger, P.R. (2011). Organisation and chemical

neuroanatomy of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana) olfactory bulb. Brain

Structure & Function, 216, 403-416.

Nummela, S. & Yamato, M. (2018). Hearing. In Marine Mammal Encyclopedia (Third Ed., pp.

466-467). New York: New York: Academic Press.

O’Connell, C., Hart, L.A., & Arnason, B.T. (1999). Comments on “elephant hearing”. The

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(3), 1122-1123.

O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E. (2007). Keeping an “ear” to the ground: seismic communication in

elephants. Physiology, 22(4), 287-294.

Page 96: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

82

Pardini, A.T., O’Brien, P.C.M., Fu, B., Bonde, R.K., Elder, F.F.B., Ferguson-Smith, M.A.,

Yang, F., & Robinson, T.J. (2007). Chromosome painting among Proboscidea,

Hyracoidea and Sirenia: support for Paenungulata (Afrotheria, Mammalia) but not

Tethytheria. Proceedings of The Royal Society B, 274(1615), 1333-1340.

Pettigrew, J.D., Bhagwandin, A., Haagensen, M., & Manger, P.R. (2010). Visual acuity and

heterogeneities of retinal ganglion cell densities and the tapetum lucidum of the African

elephant (Loxodonta africana). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 75, 251-261.

Pickles, J.O. (1982). An introduction to the physiology of hearing. Academic Press, London.

Piep, M., Radespiel, U., Zimmermann, E., Schmidt, S., & Siemers, B. M. (2008). The sensory

basis of prey detection in captive-born grey mouse lemurs, Microcebus murinus. Animal

Behaviour, 75(3), 871–878.

Pierce, A.D. (1981). Acoustics: an introduction to its physical principles and applications. New

York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Piggins, D., Muntz, W.R.A., & Best, R.C. (1983). Physical and morphological aspects of the eye

of the manatee Trichechus inunguis NATTERER 1883: (Sirenia: mammalia). Marine and

Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology, 9, 111-130.

Plotnik, J.M., Shaw, R.C., Brubaker, D.L., Tiller, L.N., & Clayton, N.S. (2014). Thinking with

their trunks: elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude nonrewarding

alternatives. Animal Behaviour, 88, 91-98.

Powell, J. A. (1996). The distribution and biology of the West African manatee (Trichechus

senegalensis Link, 1795). United Nations Environment Programme, Regional Seas

Programme, Oceans and Coastal Areas, Nairobi, Kenya.

Page 97: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

83

Rasmussen, L.E.L., & Munger, B.L. (1996). The sensorineural specializations of the trunk tip

(finger) of the Asian elephant, Elephas maximus. The Anatomical Record, 246, 127-134.

Rasmussen, L.E.L., & Schulte, B.A. (1998). Chemical signals in the reproduction of Asian

(Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants. Animal Reproduction

Science, 53, 19-34.

Rasmussen, L.E.L., & Greenwood, D.R. (2003). Frontalin: a chemical message of musth in

Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Chemical Senses, 28(5), 433-446.

Reep, R.L., Marshall, C.D., Stoll, M.L., & Whitaker, D.M. (1998). Distribution and innervation

of facial bristles and hairs in the Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris).

Marine Mammal Science, 14(2), 257-273.

Reep, R.L., Stoll., M.L., Marshall., C.D., Homer, B.L., & Samuelson, D.A. (2001).

Microanatomy of facial vibrissae in the Florida manatee: the basics for specialized

sensory function and oripulation. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 58, 1-14.

Reep, R.L., Marshall, C.D., & Stoll, M.L. (2002). Tactile hairs on the postcranial body in Florida

manatees: a mammalian lateral line? Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 59, 141-154.

Reep, R.L., & Bonde, R.K. (2006). The Florida Manatee Biology and Conservation. Gainesville,

FL: University Press of Florida.

Reep, R.L., Finlay, B.L., Darlington, R.B. (2007). The limbic system in mammalian brain

evolution. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 70, 57-70.

Page 98: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

84

Reep, R.L., Gaspard, J.C., Sarko, D., Rice, F.L., & Mann, D.A. (2011). Manatee vibrissae:

evidence for a “lateral line” function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1225,

101-109.

Renda, S., & le Roux, A. (2017). The sensory ecology of prey detection in the bat-eared fox

(Otocyon megalotis). Behaviour, 154(2), 227–240.

Reynolds III, J. E. (1981). Behavior patterns in the West Indian manatee, with emphasis on

feeding and diving. Florida Scientist, 233-242.

Reynolds III, J.E., Powell, J.A., & Taylor, C.R. (2008). Manatees. In Marine Mammal

Encyclopedia (Second Ed., pp. 682-692). New York: NY: Academic Press

Reynolds III, J.E. (2017). Florida Manatees: Biology, Behavior, and Conservation. Baltimore,

MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Reynolds III, J.E., Powell, J.A., Diagne, L.W.K., Barton, S.L., & Scolardi, K.M. (2018)

Manatees. In Marine Mammal Encyclopedia (Third Ed., pp. 558-566). New York: New

York: Academic Press.

Runge, M. C., Langtimm, C. A., Martin, J., & Fonnesbeck, C. J. (2015). Status and threats

analysis for the Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris), 2012. USGS, 2331-

1258.

Sale, J.B. (1970). Unusual external adaptations in the rock hyrax. Zoologica Africana, 5(1), 101-

113.

Sarko, D.K., Johnson, J.I., Switzer, R.C., Welker, W.I., & Reep, R.L. (2007). Somatosensory

nuclei of the manatee brainstem and thalamus. The Anatomical Record, 290, 1138-1165.

Page 99: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

85

Sarko, D.K., Rice, F.L., & Reep, R.L. (2011). Mammalian tactile hair: divergence from a limited

distribution. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1225 (2011), 90-100.

Sarko, D.K. Rice, F.L., & Reep, R.L. (2015). Elaboration and innervation of the vibrissal system

in the rock hyrax (Procavia capensis). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 85, 170-188.

Shettleworth, S.J. (1990). Spatial memory in food-storing birds. Philosophical Transactions of

the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 329, 143-151.

Shoshani, J. (1998). Understanding proboscidean evolution: a formidable task. Trends in

Ecology & Evolution, 13(12), 480-487.

Silva, M.A., & Araujo, A. (2001). Distribution and current status of the West African manatee

(Trichechus senegalensis) in Guinea-Bissau. Marine Mammal Science, 17(2), 418-424.

Shyan-Norwalt, M.R., Peterson, J., King, B.M., Staggs, T.E., & Dale, R.H.I. (2010). Initial

findings on visual acuity thresholds in an African elephant (Loxodonta africana). Zoo

Biology, 29, 30-35.

Spoor, F. (2018). Balance. In Marine Mammal Encyclopedia (Third Ed., pp. 59-60). New York:

New York: Academic Press

Springer, M.S., Murphy, W.J., Eizirik, E., & O’Brien, S.J. (2003). Placental mammal

diversification and the Cretaceous-Tertiary boundary. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 1056-1061.

Stanhope, M.J., Madsen, O., Waddell, V.G., Cleven, G.C., de Jong, W.W., & Springer, M.S.

(1998). Highly congruent molecular support for a diverse superordinal clade of endemic

African mammals. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 5(3), 501-508.

Page 100: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

86

Stobel, A., Junold, A., & Fischer, M.S. (2010). The morphology of the eutherian ethmoidal

region and its implications for higher-order phylogeny. Journal Zoological Systematics

Evolutionary Research, 48(2), 167-180.

Stoddart, D.M., & Fairall, N. (1981). Electrocardiographic technique for studying olfactory

response in the rock hyrax Procavia capensis L. (mammalia: hyracoidea). Journal of

Chemical Ecology, 7(2), 257-263.

Tabuce, R., Marivaux, L., Adaci, M., Bensalah, M., Hartenberger, J., Mahboubi, M., Mebrouk,

F., Tafforeau, P., Jaeger, J. (2007). Early tertiary mammals from North Africa reinforce

the molecular Afrotheria clade. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,

274, 1159-1166.

Thewissen, J.G.M., & Domning, D.P. (1992). The role of phenacodontids in the origin of the

modern orders of ungulate mammals. Journal of Vertebrate Paleontology, 12, 494-504.

Thewissen, J.G.M. (2008). Sensory Biology: Overview. In Marine Mammal Encyclopedia

(Second Ed., pp. 765-773). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Thewissen, J.G.M. (2018). Sensory Biology. In Marine Mammal Encyclopedia (Third Ed.,

pp.847-848). New York: New York: Academic Press

Von Bekesy, G. (1960). Experiments in Hearing. E.G. Wever (Ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-

Hill.

Wartzok, D., & Ketten, D.R. (1999). Marine Mammal Sensory Systems. J.E. Reynolds III, &

S.A. Rommel (Eds.). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

Page 101: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

87

Weissman, Y.A., Demartsev, V., Ilany, A., Barocas, A., Bar-Ziv, E., Shnitzer, I., Geffen, E., &

Koren, L. (2019). Acoustic stability in hyrax snorts: vocal tightrope-walkers or wrathful

verbal assailants? Behavioral Ecology, 31(1), 223-230.

West, C.D. (1985). The relationship of the spiral turns of the cochlea and length of the basilar

membrane to the range of available frequencies in ground dwelling mammals. Journal of

the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 1091-1101.

Yamasaki, F., Komatsu, S., & Kamiya, T. (1980). A comparative morphological study on the

tongues of manatee and dugong (Sirenia). The Scientific Reports of the Whales Research

Institute, 32, 127-144.

Yokoyama, S., Takenaka, N., Agnew, D.W., & Shoshani, J. (2005). Elephants and human color-

blind deuteranopes have identical sets of visual pigments. Genetics, 170, 335-344.

Yoshimura, K., Hama, N., Shindo, J., Kobayashi, K., & Kageyama, I. (2008). Light and scanning

electron microscopic study on the lingual papillae and their connective tissue cores of the

cape hyrax Procavia capensis. Journal of Anatomy, 213, 573-582.

Page 102: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

88

APPENDIX A

Right/Left preference towards boxes chosen versus where food was placed.

Box Type Side Food Was On Box Chosen

Clear Open Box Right Right

Clear Open Box Right Right

Clear Open Box Left Left

Clear Open Box Right Left

Clear Open Box Left Left

Clear Open Box Left Left

Clear Open Box Left Left

Clear Open Box Right Left

Clear Open Box Right Left

Black Open Box Right Right

Black Open Box Right Left

Black Open Box Left Left

Black Open Box Right Right

Black Open Box Left Right

Clear Closed Box Left Right

Clear Closed Box Right Right

Clear Closed Box Right Right

Clear Closed Box Right Right

Clear Closed Box Left Right

Page 103: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

89

APPENDIX B

Long Distance Experiments Raw Data. Below are the GPS locations, box types, dates and

times boxes were put into as well as removed from San San-Pond Sak River.

GPS Location

Box

Type

Date

Put

Out

Time

Put

Out Date: Times Checked

Date

Eaten

Time

Eaten

Total

Time

9°32’3.0552”N,

82°31’24.7728”W

Black

Open

Box

15

July

2019 851

15 Jul 19: 1100, 1515,1730; 16 Jul

19:1730; 7/17/19:0821 *Box

removed 7/17/19:0821 >24

9°32'8"N,

82°31'24"W

Black

Open

Box

17

Jul

2019 1745

18 Jul 2019:0950, 1201 *Box

removed 18 July 2019:1500 >22

9°32'1"N,

82°31'18"W

Black

Open

Box

17

Jul

2019 1745

18 Jul 2019: 0946,1205 *Box

removed 18 Jul 2019:1510 >22

9°31’32.8872”N,

82°31’34.6368”W

Clear

Open

Box

15

Jul

2019 838 15 Jul 2019: 1100

15 Jul

2019 1530

>3.5

hrs,

<7 hrs

9°32’5.7264”N,

82°31’34.6368”W

Clear

Open

Box

15

Jul

2019 1538

15 Jul 2019: 1723;

16 Jul 2019:1724

17 Jul

2019 844

>24

hrs,

<41

hrs

9°32'6"N, 82°31'

33"W

Clear

Open

Box

17

Jul

2019 850 17 Jul 2019: 1140, 1420, 1733

18 Jul

2019 1000

>8.5

hrs,

<25

hrs

9°32’6.9864”N,

82°31’28.1892”W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

15

Jul

2019 845 15 Jul 2019: 1100

15 Jul

2019 1527

>2.25

hrs,

<6.5

hrs

9°32’5.4564”N,

82°31’21.4932”W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

15

Jul

2019 858 15 Jul 2019: 1100, 1515

15 Jul

2019 1730

>6

hrs,

<7.5

hrs

Page 104: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

90

9°31’57.1368”N,

82°31’26.3928”W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

15

Jul

2019 905

15 Jul 2019: 1100, 1515, 1730; 16

Jul 2019:1300,1730; 17 Jul

2019:0821,1145,1420,1730; 18 Jul

2019:1210,1451 *Box removed

7/18/19:1450

>24+

hours

9°32'2"N, 82°,

31'19"W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

17

Jul

2019 830

17 Jul

2019 1145

>0

hrs,

<3 hrs

9°32'12"N, 82° 31'

29"W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

17

Jul

2019 840

17 Jul

2019 1140

>0

hrs,

<3 hrs

9° 36' 9"N, 82°

31'26"W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

17

Jul

2019 1145

17 Jul 2019:1425, 1740; 18 Jul

2019:0956, 1201, 1458 *Box

removed 18 Jul 2019:1500

>27.5

hours

9° 31' 59"N, 82°

31'16"W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

17

Jul

2019 1150

17 Jul

2019 1415

>0

hrs,

<2.5

hours

9°32'2" N, 82°

31'19"W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

17

Jul

2019 1420 17 Jul 2019:1745

18 Jul

2019 948

>3.5

hrs,

<29.5

hrs

9°32’4.6068”N,

82°31’20.5608”W

Leaves

Only

(No

Box)

18

Jul

2019 950 18 Jul 2019:1203

18 Jul

2019 1505

>2.25

hrs,

<5.25

hrs

Page 105: The Use of Multiple Sensory Modalities by the Antillean ...

91

APPENDIX C

Still Shots of Manatees Participating in

Experiments. (1a-d) Manatee eating

banana leaf in a clear open box (2)

Manatee swimming beneath black open

box. (3) Manatee approaching clear open

box. (4a-c) Manatee inside clear open box

eating bananas and banana leaves. 1a

1b 1c

1d 2

3 4a

4b 4c