Top Banner
The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors. Mel McCree a *, Roger Cutting b and Dean Sherwin c PUBLISHED HERE 2018: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2018.1446430 CITATION (Harvard): McCree, M., Cutting, R. & Sherwin, D., (2018) ‘The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: Taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors’, Early Child Development and Care, 188(7), pp. 980-996. APA: McCree, M., Cutting, R., Sherwin, D., (2018). The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: Taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors. Early Child Development and Care, 188(7), 980-996. a Department of Education and Childhood, University of the West of England, Bristol, UK. NB NOW Senior Lecturer, Early Childhood Studies, Bath Spa University [email protected] (2018) b Department of Education, Plymouth University, Plymouth, UK b Education Project Office, Wiltshire Wildlife Trust, Devizes, UK *Corresponding author: [email protected]; Dr Mel McCree, Department of Edu- cation and Childhood, Faculty of Arts, Creative Industries & Education, University of the West of England, Frenchay, Bristol, BS16 1QY. Tel. +44 117 3288837. Mel McCree is Senior Lecturer in Early Childhood at the University of the West of Eng- land. She specializes in outdoor arts, play, and learning, in particular upon socio-cultural aspects such as nature-society relations, the politics of space, restorative pedagogy and inter-cultural perspectives. She has a rich background in creative community practice. ORCID ID: 0000-0001-9477-790X
38

The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Apr 02, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: taking the scenic route

to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors.

Mel McCree a*, Roger Cutting b and Dean Sherwin c

PUBLISHED HERE 2018: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2018.1446430

CITATION (Harvard): McCree, M., Cutting, R. & Sherwin, D., (2018) ‘The Hare and

the Tortoise go to Forest School: Taking the scenic route to academic attainment via

emotional wellbeing outdoors’, Early Child Development and Care, 188(7), pp.

980-996.

APA: McCree, M., Cutting, R., Sherwin, D., (2018). The Hare and the Tortoise go to

Forest School: Taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing

outdoors. Early Child Development and Care, 188(7), 980-996.

aDepartment of Education and Childhood, University of the West of England, Bristol,

UK. NB NOW Senior Lecturer, Early Childhood Studies, Bath Spa University

[email protected] (2018)

bDepartment of Education, Plymouth University, Plymouth, UK

bEducation Project Office, Wiltshire Wildlife Trust, Devizes, UK

*Corresponding author: [email protected]; Dr Mel McCree, Department of Edu-

cation and Childhood, Faculty of Arts, Creative Industries & Education, University of

the West of England, Frenchay, Bristol, BS16 1QY. Tel. +44 117 3288837.

Mel McCree is Senior Lecturer in Early Childhood at the University of the West of Eng-

land. She specializes in outdoor arts, play, and learning, in particular upon socio-cultural

aspects such as nature-society relations, the politics of space, restorative pedagogy and

inter-cultural perspectives. She has a rich background in creative community practice.

ORCID ID: 0000-0001-9477-790X

Page 2: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Roger Cutting is Associate Professor in Education at Plymouth University in England.

He is concerned with Sustainability, Global Education and Science Education. He ad-

dresses aspects of pedagogy adopted in Education for Sustainability (EfS) in order to

provide rigorous and critical expositions of them. He is a Research Fellow for the Cen-

tre for Sustainable Futures. ORCID ID: 0000-0002-8745-6822

Dean Sherwin is Senior Project Officer at Wiltshire Wildlife Trust in England. His work

focuses on youth wellbeing and community engagement through the provision of land-

based experiential learning. Prior to this, he worked for 12 years as a secondary school-

teacher of Geography and Geology, and attained a PhD in Geology. Linked In: https://

uk.linkedin.com/in/dean-sherwin-36323963

Page 3: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

The Hare and the Tortoise go to Forest School: taking the scenic route to academic attainment via emotional wellbeing outdoors.

Can we improve children’s wellbeing through outdoor play and learning? If young children’s wellbeing improves, do they progress academically? What fac-tors help such interventions to work, and do the participants change over time? These questions were explored in a longitudinal mixed methods study tracking 11 children (aged 5 - 7 on entry), defined as disadvantaged in multiple ways, i.e. social, behavioural and economic. They attended weekly Forest School and out-door learning sessions over 3 years. The study investigates the project’s impact on the children in terms of their academic attainment, wellbeing and connection to nature. The children’s attendance and academic attainment improved in com-parison to their non-participating peers at school. The findings emphasize the im-portance of how social free play outdoors and relationships with a particular place can establish emotional resilience and self-regulation. The children’s social development and emotional wellbeing was supported by regular outdoor sessions alongside skilled practitioners. The outcomes demonstrate important links be-tween emotional learning and wellbeing developed in outdoor settings and acad-emic development, raising questions about interventions for young children with disadvantaged backgrounds.

Keywords: emotional wellbeing; self-regulation; attainment; outdoor play; out-door learning; Forest School

Page 4: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Introduction

In Aesop’s Fable ‘The Hare and the Tortoise’ the two animals compete in a race. The

boastful Hare loses by rushing and becoming distracted, whilst the Tortoise wins by go-

ing at his own measured pace. For schools, supporting children’s development can seem

like a race in the current context of pressures from government and hare-brained policy

changes. Children feel the pressures of this race and their unequal status within it. In

this paper we discuss the impacts of a three-year Forest School and outdoor learning

project for disadvantaged young children in a UK primary school. Much like the Tor-

toise in Aesop’s Fable, the project’s ethos included going at the children’s own pace

within affective, restorative learning processes (Rose, Gilbert & Smith, 2012; Roe and

Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain and tortoise mind’, focusing on

the effectiveness of slower, creative forms of learning present in what he calls a tortoise-

like undermind, as compared to a hare-like ‘d-mode’ of default, purposeful thinking.

We know that the brain is made to linger as well as rush, and that slow knowing sometimes leads to better answers. We know that knowledge makes itself known through sensations, images, feelings, inklings as well as through clear conscious thoughts. To be able to meet the uncertain challenges of the contemporary world, we need … to expand our repertoire of ways of learning and knowing to reclaim the full gamut of cognitive possibilities (Claxton, 1997, p. 201).

We consider the tortoise’s slow approach as a helpful analogy, for the study described is

a longitudinal evaluation of the project’s impacts upon the children, specifically their

wellbeing and academic development. As an introduction, we describe the theoretical

context and the impetus for the study. We then present the methods and findings. As a

complex, long-term intervention, there are multiple findings which we summarise and

discuss throughout the paper. We conclude by looking at implications.

Page 5: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Background

There is currently much interest in the impact of outdoor learning upon academic at-

tainment and wellbeing, with a growing demand for evidence (Rickinson, Hunt, Rogers

and Dillon, 2012; Department for Education and Schools (DFeS), 2006). The study de-

scribed in this paper explores the idea that, for disadvantaged young children, support-

ing wellbeing through outdoor learning can facilitate improvement in school-readiness

and achievement (Dillon and Dickie, 2012). This study aims to provide data to authenti-

cate or to refute this suggestion by evaluating the longitudinal impacts across a project’s

extended time frame. By 2013, to our knowledge, no similar study over this length of

time had been undertaken. However, during the project the body of evidence to support

long-term restorative outdoors intervention improved (Malone & Waite, 2016) and fur-

ther research is emerging. A recent 4-year study from Norway examined the relationship

between the amount of time young children attending daycare spent outdoors and their

cognitive and behavioural development. This found that time spent outdoors in pre-

school may support children's development of attention skills (Ulset, Vitaro, Brendgen,

Bekkus & Borge, 2017).

Fiennes et al. (2015) provides a systematic review of the evidence base relating

to the effectiveness of outdoor learning recommending that interventions, contexts and

outcomes be better specified so that the evidence might be collated and used more ef-

fectively. Other conclusions in the review were that almost all outdoor learning had a

positive effect on the defined outcomes, and that longer-term interventions were more

Page 6: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

effective, as well as those with good preparation and follow-up. This paper responds to

the recommendation and strengthens these conclusions.

A recent international report draws together five key evidence reviews and a

structured sample of further reports on children learning in natural environments (Mal-

one & Waite, 2016). The authors identified the type of long-term impacts that projects,

such as the one we describe, have on children’s quality of life. They placed a focus on

impacts around ‘physical health and wellbeing and ‘character capabilities’ such as ap-

plication, self-regulation, empathy, creativity, and innovation, and their capacity to be

successful learners’ (ibid, p4).

Study aims and theory of change

Aims-setting was based upon a theory of change or logic model used to clarify

intervention aims, intended outcomes and assumptions about context, participants etc.

This ensured the evaluation criteria corresponded to the desired outcomes of the stake-

holders; the school and funding educational charity. These outcomes included overall

academic performance in a set of National Curriculum (England) subjects, including

Mathematics and English. They included wider social parameters such as metrics on

attendance data and evaluative data from parents and teachers on wellbeing and atti-

tudes to school and learning. An ‘outcome’ is in itself a contested term and perhaps ill-

suited here, seeming to reflect a ‘coarsening of our imaginings of childhood’ (Butcher &

Andrews, 2009, p. 36). With multiple disadvantage factors influencing the children, we

acknowledge the complexity of their lives and the related intervention. Therefore, we

designed a holistic study of impacts which drew upon the various measured factors

Page 7: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

alongside deeper observation and interpretation. From the position of regarding the

whole child as a competent social actor in relation within wider common worlds (Tay-

lor, 2013), we sought to understand how the children felt and saw their world and acted

within particular contexts. Therefore, the theory of change considers how the project

has an impact on the children, specifically the ways in which nature connection, emo-

tional wellbeing and academic development are manifested through outdoor play and

learning, in addition to the ways in which they are perceived by others. Study questions

were:

(1) Do changes relating to wellbeing & academic development occur?

(2) Can factors that influence the relationship between outdoor experience and aca-

demic performance be identified and associated with the project?

(3) Are changes in wellbeing & academic development recognisable by the school?

(4) What are the significant changes over the longitudinal span of the project?

School context

The school studied was a small county-town school with 320 pupils on the

school roll in 2015 with an age range of 5 to 11. Situated in a rural county in south-west

England, the pupils included a social mix of families, with 26.3% eligible for Free

School Meals (FSM) (similar to the national average of 26%) and 12.8% of pupils re-

ceive Special Educational Needs (SEN) support (similar to the national average of

13%).

Page 8: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Project context

The project was developed by the Wildlife Trust Youth Wellbeing team, who

have a successful legacy of developing outdoor projects that reach children who may

not access services otherwise. Their work spans both formal and informal learning, both

in and out of school. This project was year-round, including school holidays. They visit-

ed the same local woodland each week for three years, with additional full days in holi-

days at other Wildlife Trust reserves. Activities were playful, place-based and nature-

based, including scavenger hunts, wild art, basic whittling, creative crafts, shelter build-

ing, tree climbing, 'running around games', making bird boxes, foraging, conservation

activities (coppicing), campfire cooking, willow sculpture, playing with mud, playing in

a stream. The children chose from a variety offered each week. They didn’t have to stick

with their first choice, they could move around or create their own activity. This includ-

ed lots of free open-ended play using the loose parts and affordances of the woodland.

Making hot chocolate with a fire-based kettle was the one consistent activity. After each

session they noted anything in particular they wished to do next week. For the last term,

the lead practitioner lessened resources so the children entertained themselves with what

was there, stimulating further creativity, knowledge and skills.

Methods

Sample

The participants were the children in the cohort, their teaching team, the project

practitioners and some parents. The study closely followed 11 children who took part in

the project, aged 5 - 7 on entry to 7 - 10 years old on exit. Prior to the study, the head-

Page 9: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

teacher chose the children for the project as those ‘struggling to thrive’ and seen as like-

ly to underachieve (Head, Y1). The head teacher further defined them as disadvantaged

in that they were ‘economically and emotionally disadvantaged and with special educa-

tion needs (including behaviour difficulties)’ (Head, Y1). For example, all had addition-

al help in school and Free School Meals (FSM) which the government uses for compar-

ative data. Their home lives included known elements of stress, trauma and complex

family relationships.

Ethics

Given the small size of the pre-existing group of children (13 beginning, 11

throughout), the design was limited. All the children, parents and relevant staff were

invited to decide if they wanted to participate. Full consent was confirmed prior to the

study from all participants, in line with the ethical requirements of consultancy Free

Range Creativity. Ethical guidelines from the British Educational Research Association

(BERA) were followed. Parents were consulted regarding their children’s involvement

and the meaning of informed consent. Liaison support from a trusted pastoral member

of staff was offered throughout to ensure parents’ ongoing awareness and a safe way to

raise concerns or initiate withdrawal. An initial discussion session with the children de-

veloped understanding about participation, consent and withdrawal. Children who

joined the project after this date were not part of the cohort, and children who left before

the end of the 3 years were removed. Regular check-ins with the children ensured that

they knew they did not have to engage with the researcher on any given day, and that

they could withdraw fully if they wished. Children sometimes exercised their rights

Page 10: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

through silence, not answering questions, being otherwise engaged and not wanting to

be interrupted by research activities, all of which were respected.

Design

The design applied mixed methods and adopted elements from the child-centred

Mosaic approach (Clark and Moss, 2001). We valued the children’s agency and wished

to include their perspectives, interpretations and self-reports. All the children were in-

terviewed every year using child-centred methods and involved in regular session eval-

uation. Two in-depth case studies were compiled from further twice-yearly interviews

with two children randomly selected. For the interviews, a small den was made in

school to create a special space so the children might feel freer from their everyday en-

vironment and behaviour. Within the den, the researcher used several activities, session

documentation and prompts. For example, drawing with prompts to review the year,

noting memorable experiences, high and low points, and creating a personal map of the

woods. Taken together with ongoing discussion, these prompts encouraged the children

to share verbally and made their meanings more visible. This led to interesting discus-

sions surrounding their choices and meanings. Captions were added to encapsulate how

the children said they felt about the experiences or other observations. Data analysis

was informed by the ‘Draw and Write’ method (Williams, Wetton and Moon, 1989)

mainly for illustrative purposes due to the challenges in the method posed by the subjec-

tive variation of interpretation. The data contributed alongside observation field notes

towards the thematic analysis.

Page 11: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Evaluation events in the school were used employing a two-stage Mosaic ap-

proach of community reflection and participation (Gallacher & Gallagher, 2008). Par-

ents were hard to reach and achieved a low questionnaire response rate (n=3 at entry,

n=0 at exit). They were positively involved in a halfway evaluation event, reflecting on

the emergent findings. Semi-structured focus groups continued were held with parents

at this halfway point and at entry and exit, where they reflected with their children on

significant experiences or perceptions as to how their children were responding to the

project.

Baseline and post-project questionnaires were collected at point of entry (2013)

and exit (2016) from children, parents and staff. The children completed a child-appro-

priate 6 question ‘smiley’ questionnaire about their project perceptions and experiences,

with writing assistance, at entry and exit (n=11) and verbal comments were noted. The

children’s class teachers and support staff completed questionnaires (n=7 at entry and

exit) regarding their perceptions about the particular children in their care. Staff focus

groups were held termly.

Leuven scales were used each session for wellbeing and involvement (Laevers,

2005) alongside an in-house engagement measure. Qualitative data sets included regular

fieldwork observation, focus groups, questionnaires and interviews. Quantitative design

allowed for semi-control groups in the school on attendance, attainment and nature con-

nection, affording comparison with peers not receiving the intervention, the school and

with national data. The Connection to Nature Index was applied on exit (Cheng and

Monroe, 2012). Quantitative data sets used mean averages for comparison in line with

the school records and national sets.

Page 12: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

The combined qualitative data were analysed thematically using grounded theo-

ry (Strauss and Corbin, 1998). The mixed method approach allowed for triangulation

and balanced strengths and limitations, improving validity and reducing bias.

Results: Quantitative observation measures

Wellbeing, involvement and engagement

Wellbeing and involvement were observed using Leuven scale measures (Laev-

ers, 2005) which are trusted and widespread in early childhood practice. The tool is

based around these two key indicators on Likert scales of 1 (lowest) - 5 (highest). In-

volvement refers to the degree to which a child engages in activities and is therefore an

important component in learning processes. Wellbeing refers to both a sense of being at

ease with ones self and from being free of emotional tensions and this in turn relates to

self-confidence and self-esteem (Laevers, 2005). Data sets were collected as part of

weekly in-house monitoring by the session leader and the researcher, alongside a com-

parative measure of engagement on a 1 - 5 Likert scale.

For brevity, we have summarized using mean average scores for the cohort

(n=11) over 3 years and ranges of child per year mean averages. High levels of wellbe-

ing (4.2), involvement (4.3) and engagement (4.8) on the sessions were sustained

throughout the project. The ranges were from 3.8 - 4-9 wellbeing, 3.8 - 5 involvement

and 4-5 engagement.

Page 13: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Nature connection

The validated Connection to Nature Index (Cheng & Monroe, 2012) is a 16

point closed questionnaire with a 4-factor trait measure of i) enjoyment of nature, ii)

empathy for creatures, iii) sense of oneness and iv) sense of responsibility. Responses

are on a 5-point Likert scale and scored with a mean average. A higher score of 4 or 5

indicates a strong connection to the rest of nature. It was recently field-tested with chil-

dren aged 8-12 (n=76, Bragg, Wood, Barton & Pretty, 2013). Study children were too

young on entry (aged 4-7) for available field-tested and validated questionnaires. It was

used on exit with the cohort (n=10 returned) and school year groups (n=95 returned). In

the cohort, 10% (1/10) scored below the RSPB national average of 4.05 and 70% (7 /10)

scored 4.5 or over. The mean average differences between the school (3.9, n=95), the

study cohort (4.5, n=11) and a national survey (4.05, n=1200) (Bragg et al., 2013;

RSPB, 2013) indicates that the intervention strengthened the children’s connection to

the rest of nature.

Academic attainment and attendance

Data drawn from teachers’ assessments (used for government reporting) showed

that the cohort made a variety of academic improvements, with good progress relative

to their position and perceived capabilities, and continued improvement in all three of

the subject areas (reading, writing and mathematics). Due to UK government-led

changes in assessment, results data were tracked from March 2015 to July 2016 and not

before. There are three school-based assessment levels related to key performance indi-

Page 14: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

cators of emerging (0-59%), expected (60-79%) and exceeding (80%). Progress was

measured as a shift from emerging to expected level or above.

The cohort was compared to their peers (including like-for-like e.g. Free School

Meals (FSM) and pupil premium (PPG) recipients) and their year groups. Writing at-

tainment progressed and improved by 18% (compared to 6% in the total year groups

and 7% PPG peers). Reading attainment showed improvements of 27% (compared to

13% in the total year groups and 22% PPG peers). Maths attainment both showed im-

provements of 27% (compared 15% in the total year groups and 11% PPG peers). We

cannot attribute any sole causality to the project, yet the study cohort overall had a bet-

ter rate of progress than expected, and fared well in comparison to their peers. Im-

provement is to be expected as part of attending school and maturing and the findings

do need to be seen in the context of improved attainment across the whole school.

Attendance data were tracked from a control year to the end of the project,

showing a positive difference for the study cohort (2.4% mean average increase) com-

pared with the whole school (1.1% mean average increase). Attendance is a challenge

for the cohort and their peers. Cohort attendance was below the whole school in Year 0

(by cohort 94.3% vs school 95.6%) and Year 1 (95.9% vs 96.4%), peaked above in Year

2 (97.7% vs 96.7%) and settled at a similar level as the school in Year 3 (96.7% vs

97.2%). This contributes to a recommendation that the project needed to be long term to

make a sustainable difference. The findings provide further corroboration of the wider

evidence base within the study, showing a positive impact on attainment and attendance

for the study cohort.

Findings and Discussion: Qualitative analysis themes

Page 15: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Data were analysed both from session-based fieldwork and school-based inter-

views, focus groups and Mosaic approach events. The initial session themes from Year

1 began with nurture, physicality, shared time and space, the adult role and freedom to

choose. The following themes then emerged, deepened and were refined and renamed in

Years 2 and 3 (shown with ‘quotation marks’ below). Nurture, physicality and shared

time and space remained important throughout. Shared time and space became in part

‘free social play’ and physicality became ‘physical adventure’ as intrinsic motivation for

roaming and curiosity increased. Freedom to choose became ‘choice and

independence’. Some themes faded as others emerged, such as ’the adult role’ theme. In

Year 1 the relationships to the adults became more trusting and woven into the fabric of

the sessions, then the role faded in significance as the children developed intrinsic moti-

vation, independence and self-regulation. The children became ‘socially confident

learners’ engaged in ‘nature discovery’ in their own self-led journeys, needing adult

guidance less and less to spark their interest and curiosity. Importantly, the theme of

‘self-regulation and resilience through emotional space’ clarified in Year 2, evolved out

of shared time and space and the adult role. This presents as significant in how the chil-

dren used the sessions to navigate and provide for their own wellbeing. These themes

are inter-dependent both in their nuances and in the school-based themes such as ‘be-

haviour perceptions’ and being ‘wild experts’. Other themes observed within school

were: ‘new perspectives’ and ‘whole school culture change’.

Session Themes

Establishing self-regulation and resilience through emotional space.

A longitudinal project by its nature provides a great deal of data. For brevity, we con-

Page 16: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

centrate our discussion on the theme found to be the most significant; ‘self-regulation

and resilience through emotional space’. ‘Emotional space’ here means the provision of

a physical space and time in which the children are free to be themselves and express

their emotions. Reports from the children, session staff and project practitioners stated

that this was the most constructive contribution to their wellbeing, and the children

demonstrated a clear development in this area over time. We regard this important find-

ing as a meta-theme, with others as vital ingredients towards the children achieving

greater self-regulation and resilience.

The children expressed a strong attachment to attending the project and evaluat-

ed the experience as positive. However, negative emotions featured strongly in how the

children said they often felt. For example, using a visual method that asked the children

to choose a ‘jelly-bean person’ that best represented them provided a clear majority re-

sponse for feelings of anger. Figure 1’s illustrative quotes emphasize this state of being.

It is noteworthy that 3 out of these 4 children were known to be experiencing challeng-

ing life circumstances at this time.

Sometimes I feel angry, especially [at] others.

I’m angry and bored.

Mr Angry says ‘I want a turn, I want a go!’

They’re getting on my nerves.

(Children K, J, B and H, Y3)

[Figure 1: Jellybean people sharing angry emotions]

Page 17: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

The children found ways to self-regulate within the sessions, for example Child I

through fantasy-play and den-building, and Child A through tool-mastery and social

physical play. The different choices they made reflected their different characters and

needs, and in turn the different meanings they derived from the experience. Children

exhibited challenging behaviour at times, particularly in Year 2, and were excluded from

the sessions on one or two occasions. The source of the problems were similar for both;

their relations with other children, yet for different reasons. Eventually, the sessions be-

came a safe space where they played out their emotions and overcame social conflicts.

These findings emphasise the importance of this social time with adequate affordances

in place, both physical and emotional, in order for children to find ways to develop posi-

tively, inter and intra-personally. It was not unusual to present attachment disorders or a

need to play out dysfunctional domestic situations. As time went by the children knew

they had the space and permission to express themselves in such ways and to find their

own way to self-regulate; they were learning the parameters of what was acceptable.

Coming out… has helped me manage my anger. I now know that I can just go and sit somewhere for 5 minutes. I now go outside more. I hadn’t used to. I go to the park and take my friends. (Child I)

In this way, the children reflected that such emotional time and space through outdoor

inhabitation (Ingold, 2008) had helped them in and out of school. When they had a neg-

ative response, they were able to take some time, seek help, or use other coping strate-

gies. Importantly they knew they could bounce back and that being outdoors would help

them. The children appreciated the chance to reflect and the healing they felt from time

spent well in the woods; ‘I love my life. I love nature…..It’s very peaceful here’ (Child

Page 18: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

D, Y3). There were many instances where children would do something alone rather

than with the group, often at session starts, e.g. ‘I’m doing time out. I’m digging alone.

That’s all I want to do’ (Child H, Y2). They would seek one-to-one attention from an

adult, take their space and then re-join the group once they felt able, as illustrated in this

dialogue with the adult practitioner.

- When we get into the woods, please can I go and sit somewhere by myself for five minutes?

- Of course. Why would you like to do that?

- Because this is the only time I get some peace and quiet. (Child F, Y2)

The practitioner noted that the majority of the challenging moments were at the

end of sessions and in the minibus home. The ending of the valued sessions remained a

flash point for the children even into Year 3, suggesting that they appreciated time to

unwind from their often complex lives, and found some endings difficult. Regular visits

to the same woods provided a vital familiar place to process their emotions and enjoy a

stable relationship with people and place.

Nurture

Firstly, creating stable ongoing relationships with a skilled, attuned practitioner lay a

strong foundation for the children to feel nurtured and thrive. Further, ensuring that the

basic needs of the children were met remained a priority throughout the project and a

vital part of a positive session, such as bringing spare clothes for children who didn’t

have enough to keep them warm. When the children were first getting used to being

outside, the popularity of hot drinks and snacks around the fire was not surprising yet

Page 19: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

essential. There was a tangible transformation in their behaviour when their basic needs

were met. A hot meal on a cold day and a welcoming base camp area enabled the chil-

dren to feel secure in their new environment and its regular provision had a nurturing

effect on the group. This simple provision is not to be underestimated. In Year 1, drink-

ing hot chocolate was the most popular activity for all the children and held significance

throughout. There were only a few sessions at the end of the project in Year 3 where the

practitioner did not include hot chocolate and the children commented upon this in their

final interviews. By Year 3 it seemed that the children had a solid and positive relation-

ship with, and trust in, the session leaders, the place, their peers and in themselves. It

appeared that perhaps some nurture needs were being met with wider species connec-

tion. ‘I like getting juicy blackberries’, said Child C in Year 3. ‘Fruit gives you energy

and it’s free!’

Physical Adventure

Over time the children increased their roaming range across the main session site, in

parallel with an observable increase in confidence and ownership of the space. They

named places or gave them specific associations and regularly initiated exploration to

find new secret corners which they could claim. By Year 3 they knew the woods very

well and could find their own way around a large part of them. This confidence and

trust enabled the adults to give a wider roaming radius to the children and is in part il-

lustrated by the following quotes.

Can we go to the place that makes our eyes glitter and puts a smile on our faces? (Child C)

I want to find some mysterious, messy and beautiful places. (Child D)

Page 20: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Thank you for bringing us to the woods. (Child C) (Y3)

A final significant aspect of this theme is the physical experience. Physical activities

scored highly as the children’s first preference and were consistently voted as popular

activities in each yearly evaluation. Child A’s case study shows his clear satisfaction of

the same, regular, repeated physical adventurous play and this was echoed by all the

children in their actions each week. Children frequently tested their own bodily limits,

and then pushed beyond them. In short, they learnt about their physicality through free

play.

Free social play

This theme evolved from shared time and space and is associated with ‘choice and in-

dependence’, ‘physical adventure’ and ’nature discovery’. It is impossible to untangle

the presenting behaviours of the children from the affordances of the site and the nature

of the sessions. The site had a clear agency and role within the children’s play; ‘It’s a

special place because we had fun there and that’s for us to remember’ (Child 1, Y3).

The social experience was important in various ways. Social interactions affected each

experience and its learning potential strongly. The need for friendship and positive so-

cial interactions was paramount with a strong preference for sharing time and space

with friends over other activities. An apparent need to assert positions and leadership

was observed in Year 1 group dynamics. In subsequent years, this was still a flashpoint

but was mediated most strongly through the children’s social play more than adult in-

tervention.

Page 21: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Using loose parts, meaning objects with no prescribed play task, such as a stick,

or string (Nicholson, 1971) was a way into nature discovery using creativity and helping

cultivate a learning disposition. There was a strong affordance of place in the wood-

land’s highly variable environment, rich in unique loose parts as inspiration for imagina-

tion. Physically using a tool or making an impact on the landscape was a creative stimu-

lus, and sparked off narratives and shared fantasy play sessions in groups of children.

I wanted to make a fort. We found this place…. (Child A, Y3)

It’s really fun here. I’m never going out. It’s my house. Who’s in my lovely house? (Child D, Y3)

Fantasy play had a strong role. A detailed ‘Save the Wild’ game developed and brought

the children together in co-operation. The opportunity to play and the ability to choose

on their own terms had a clear impact on the children’s ability to self-regulate and de-

velop resilience and these both relate strongly to the next theme.

Nature discovery

When I grow up I want to be a slug expert. (Child F, Y3)

I love feeling the rain on my face. (Child J, Y3)

I like meeting new friends. Human and animal friends. (Child I, Y2)

Robins have a special box, different to other birds. (Child I, Y2)

It was interesting because you can understand all the creatures’ lives (Child I, Y2).

There was an element of taught ecological education within the project, although always

led from the children’s interests or choice of activities such as pond dipping or making

Page 22: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

bird-boxes. The finding of loose parts stimulated curiosity. Additionally investigating,

collecting and taking things home was popular; ‘they remind me of all the fun I’ve

had’ (Child I, Y2). Child I had a special box and enjoyed collecting small items. She

would often take the time to investigate and the rewards she felt from this process were

demonstrated. When asked to choose an experience to remember she chose ‘sharing a

moment’ with a moth she encountered and said ‘I felt really happy, special’ (Child I,

Y2). She often documented with a camera, narrating her observations through questions

and reflection. She developed a sense of pride about being a ‘wild expert’ and becoming

a ‘socially confident learner’. Within the first half of the project at school, the staff ob-

served Child I making advances in her confidence at school. It is clear that, to some ex-

tent, investigating nature was the springboard, confirmed by Child 1 herself, her parents

and staff. The child-led sessions in the woods enabled her to thrive.

Socially confident learners

I am a survivor. I never give up. (Child D, Y3)

There’s no I in team. (Child K, Y3)

I want to help. (Child B, Y3).

The confidence of the children improved and was maintained within the sessions. Their

knowledge and skills increased symbiotically as they became more confident and ma-

ture, which in turn helped them to become more responsible, to self-regulate and to gain

resilience. This was not without some exceptions, mainly where children had challeng-

ing life circumstances or peer-to-peer conflicts. Yet this has a positive interpretation in

the breadth of emotions that they felt safe to express within the sessions and, with en-

Page 23: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

couragement, to often work things out with each other. The children became more adept

at recognizing their own emotional processes and at acting upon them in a constructive

way.

Choice and independence

During the project sessions the children chose free play, continued where they had left

off previously, or participated in offered activities. For example, digging was popular

and acted as a springboard for many experiences, as the practitioner scaffolded their in-

volvement and supported their choices and interests. The child-centred aspect of the

project led to a culture of self-directed learning and existence. Many children chose to

persist with a certain activity or pursuit over several sessions until they reached a con-

clusion or new level of mastery, with some degree of satisfaction and autonomy, as re-

flected in this dialogue with the practitioner:

- Can you catch?

- No

- Well learn then

- I will be the Master of Catching...when I am ready (Child H, Y2)

Towards the end of Year 3 the lead practitioner minimized resources and stopped

initiating activities, to see what would happen. The children were positive in their free-

dom of choice; ‘can we do anything we want? Wow!’ (Child L, Y3) and ‘can we do it

again next week?’ (Child A, Y3). If the children became disinterested, they changed for

themselves, and according to their reports, did not get bored. When consulted at the

halfway evaluation, all the children stated clearly that they wished to continue the

Page 24: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

project and demonstrated signs to their parents and teachers of enthusiasm in their rela-

tionship to nature. This newfound independence appeared in some ways to translate into

their home life: ‘I’ve been to the woods with friends. I’ve showed them where we sat

and made a fire’ (Child 1, Y3), aligning to other outdoor play findings (e.g. Ridgers,

Knowles and Sayers, 2012).

School-based themes

Within these themes we discuss what happened based in the school in relation to

the project, how it influenced the children and school staff i.e. culture changes in the

school, the change of role and resulting confidence of children.

Whole school culture change

‘[The project] has been the inspiration for all the outdoor learning development in the school’ (Head, Y2).

There was a significant and considerable culture change within the school, with

outdoor learning embraced and championed. The project was the springboard for this

burgeoning interest in outdoor learning and a widespread ‘Wild’ culture change took

place in the school. There has been, in essence, a re-wilding of the school. So much so

that the Head declared 2015-16 a Wild Year, reinforcing the access he wished every

child to have to learning outdoors. The project may well have had very different out-

comes in another school context with less enthusiastic leadership and culture. For this

reason, the significance of the changed school culture and staff interests are included in

the findings, as both an influencing factor upon the project’s success and as a result of

the project. The strong theme of culture change meant that it became important to ob-

serve the effects on the wider school as well as the children.

Page 25: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

From an interview with the head teacher at the halfway stage in March 2015, the

distance the children had travelled over the first half of the project had started to be-

come clear to him, through both observation and academic results. The head teacher

said he knew the project was working for the children and for the school. By the end of

the project, he was still cautiously positive.

It’s not always easy to measure the impact on those involved, but what would the outcomes have been if the children had not been involved so positively? (Head, Y3)

The parents were harder to reach but where they did make links with the project and

their children’s development, they were direct and positive. One trend identified was

that the parents of the study children engaged more positively with the school, as no-

ticed by staff.

Wild experts

Positive changes were visible in the children in their role as ‘wild experts’ at school and

home. The school encouraged the children to share their newfound knowledge and skills

with other children, demonstrating knowledge, pride and positivity

We were talking about King Alfred in the class and I asked if anyone had heard of him. The ‘[project] four’ all put their hands up straight away bursting to tell me..... about King Alfred's Cakes, what they were, where you find them, what they are used for and why they are called what they are. They told the whole class. It was really great to see them using what they had learnt and being so positive with it in class to tell their peers. (S1, Y1)

The project had a positive identity within the school and the study cohort informally

became ‘Wild Ambassadors’, able to demonstrate their skills to others. If the study in-

Page 26: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

tervention had only used for behaviour management, or was in a setting where outdoor

learning was perceived negatively, it is likely that the impact of the children’s growing

expertise would have been lessened. It is noteworthy that the study itself had an effect

in increasing their visibility within the school.

Behaviour perceptions

Within Years 2 and 3 some school staff were surprised by the different behaviour during

the sessions as compared to behaviour at school. In other studies (Roe and Aspinall,

2011; Borradaile, 2006), being selected to go to a forest school project for behavioural

reasons has had a negative effect. Importantly, Borradaile (2006) cautions against forest

school as a tool for behavioural management and segregation within mainstream school,

as children may feel excluded. This was not the case within the current study. Roe and

Aspinall’s research (2011) highlights the rich potential of forest school when the drive

for learner achievement is relaxed and the restorative relationship with the setting is ex-

plicit. It could be argued, from a position of affective learning (Rose et al., 2012; Waite,

2011), that the learning potential appears deepened by taking a restorative route into the

process.

Teachers had a variety of viewpoints on whether any changes of behaviour were

observable in class. The children perceived the two spaces very differently in terms of

behaviour codes and acted accordingly. When asked, they were clear about the differ-

ence in settings between project and school time, and about the behaviour codes expect-

ed. Child J commented on how new school staff acted when visiting the woods - ‘they

Page 27: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

expect us act like we are in school - it’s annoying and boring’ (Y3). The head teacher

perceived the wider benefits of the project beyond that of behaviour management and

intervened to ensure staff that understood the ethos remained on the sessions.

Several of the study cohort had an increase in challenging life circumstances at

home in Year 3. There was a visible impact on behaviour and attendance, reported by

school and session staff. It was in this year that some staff began to question whether 3

years was too long for the children to be involved.

I think Year 3 with the same children was more challenging and that 2 years or changing the group dynamic may have made it an even more positive experience (Head, Y3).

It is hard to say whether their home life challenges or the length of the project had more

impact. Additionally, and similarly to the discussion around academic development, it is

difficult to isolate parameters such as growing maturity over the time period. However,

what clearly arose was a difference in behaviour expectation that became evident over

the longer term in that the children did not want to behave in the woods as they did in

school.

New perspectives

Bringing project knowledge into class continued to have an impact on their

learning in school and helped to offset any negative impact from being selected (i.e. as

disadvantaged). When teachers, peers and parents shared ‘new perspectives’ on the

children (Murray and O’Brien, 2005), it effected how the children were viewed. Teach-

ers reflected on how not being able to attend the sessions meant they could not fully un-

Page 28: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

derstand or integrate the children’s experiences into class, or benefit from the new per-

spectives of seeing the children active in the woods.

Two staff members attended all the sessions consistently and one teacher came

out on some of the holiday sessions. These members of staff provided a vital communi-

cation bridge between the project and the school, able to help connect the children’s

lives at home, school and in the woods. For example, a school assistant (S6, Y3), com-

mented when a volunteer came and made ‘proper’ shelters with the children - ‘I don’t

think I’ve ever seen [Child D] so animated’. Then, a week later, ‘[Child D] has asked

every day about going back to the woods’. The restorative approach was embraced by

the school overall, thereby allowing the benefits of the project to have a greater impact

on their school life.

[The project] has made the staff team aware of the advantages for certain groups of children to well-being and that they need to be in the right place to learn emotional-ly and socially. Learning outdoors, especially in the natural environment, can con-tribute towards achieving this. For all children we have seen the benefit of learning in different places and in different ways. (Head, Y3)

Wellbeing visible in school

Teacher observation in school showed improved subjective wellbeing for 6 out of 11 of

the study cohort children, using entry and exit questionnaire data. The school staff

present on the sessions rated the children higher overall than the teachers who saw the

children solely in school. The difference in perceptions, from teachers, session staff and

the session practitioners, raises an interesting point about the ‘new perspectives’ gained

Page 29: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

by observing children on the sessions. A point of interest is that the staff attending the

sessions and therefore observing the children outdoors on the project as well as in

school, on average rated each child’s final wellbeing 1 or 2 points higher than did the in-

school teachers. This suggests that either their higher wellbeing had not transferred to

school, and/or was perceived differently. Further, this connected to different expecta-

tions and management strategies of behaviour and performance in each space, reflected

in the different perspectives on behaviour held by adults in different contexts. The con-

text of observation and the different experiences from which an informed adult’s per-

spective is gained therefore had an effect upon perceptions.

Teaching staff perspectives

Each year staff involved with the study children attended two or more focus groups. The

head teacher was interviewed once a year with regular informal contact throughout. The

teachers and assistants with study children in their classes, along with session assistants,

completed a questionnaire pre- and post- the project. As part of the entry questionnaire,

staff were asked to describe outcomes they hoped for. In the exit questionnaire, they

were asked which of these outcomes they thought had transferred to the children in

school. The most frequently observed outcomes were increased self-confidence, knowl-

edge about the natural environment and improved social skills. Often observed out-

comes were willingness to try new experiences, ability to apply skills learnt in class,

improved self-image, self-esteem and increased independence. The least observed out-

comes were an ability to learn in a creative way, working with others as a team and aca-

demic development through self-belief. Staff were asked whether they could link any

improvement to observable evidence of causality in the project. Their responses were:

Page 30: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

The children were definitely more able to express their enjoyment of [the project] to their peers as years went on (greater depth). Children often expressed their expe-riences and knowledge of how things were done during our own outdoor sessions. (S2)

As the school has moved towards more outdoor learning, (Child A) has been able to share his experiences with the class, as he had invariably ‘done it before’ at [the project]. This has boosted his confidence - even more! (S1)

All of these children’s knowledge of the outdoors has been outstanding. So when writing about this, they have found a greater confidence in subject matter which then allows them to focus on the skills involved for writing. I have often used the outdoors for writing inspiration because of [the project]. (S3)

(Child F) looked forward to [the project] each week. Big improvement in confi-dence. (Child G) some improvement in confidence in group situations. (S8)

From these responses, it is interesting that the most popular outcome, confi-

dence, is most often associated with enjoyment and happiness, also with knowledge of

the outdoors, being outdoors and social needs or skills. This interrelationship provides

insight into how the children’s confidence is derived. However, not all of the feedback

was positive:

I don’t think the children’s behaviour or management strategies for them worked particularly well and some children stopped enjoying it because of the behaviour of others. This also set back some children’s learning because we had to deal with issues raised at the project. (S3)

…As he is the only child in the class to go he would often brag about it. Unfortu-nately that promoted himself and it is his self-obsession that causes problems at school. (S1)

However most recognized that the changes in wellbeing and academic develop-

ment within the study cohort was through positive engagement and the championing of

outdoor learning. The cohort was encouraged to demonstrate and share their new skills

Page 31: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

and knowledge with other pupils, and was rewarded for doing so. The school was im-

pressive in both its recognition and its adoption of outdoor play and learning across the

whole school culture. These two factors were symbiotic, in that a positive approach en-

abled deeper involvement from both the study children and the whole school. The

theme of ‘behaviour perceptions’ was a limiting factor. Being ‘wild experts’ encouraged

integration by recognizing and utilizing the children’s new skills in the school and class-

room environment. This gave the children a feeling of distinction and being special, in-

creasing their confidence. ‘New perspectives’ were limited to those who could attend the

sessions, meaning that changes in wellbeing may not have been noticed so easily but

‘whole school culture change’ enabled the spread of outdoor pedagogy in an effective

way, encouraging academic development based on the ‘wild’ experiences.

Conclusions

This study explored the suggestion that, for disadvantaged children, wellbeing through

outdoor learning is important in improving school-readiness and achievement (Dillon

and Dickie, 2012). The study had a small cohort, yet the influence is clear over time, as

shown in the impact factors being better than those not participating in the project.

The findings suggest ways in which positive changes relating to the children’s

wellbeing and academic development were demonstrated. In terms of wellbeing, no-

tably their self-regulation and resilience developed, supported by project factors such as

‘emotional space’ and school-based integration. The children increased in their physical

and social wellbeing, confidence for learning and connection to the rest of nature. This

was embodied in their increasing confidence outdoors and by being recognised as ‘wild

experts’ at school. The children’s academic development across the subjects compared

Page 32: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

favourably with their equivalent peers, with positive shifts in attainment and attendance.

Given the wide range of potential parameters it is difficult to claim causality, however

the children, their parents and the school team identified the project as having primary

influence.

There is a danger when outdoor play and learning is discussed, or variants of it,

such as Forest School, in assuming that all projects are the same in delivery, impact or

outcomes. Such an assumption can be commonly found, yet no two complex interven-

tions are the same. It is worth taking note of the specific form the project has taken.

There were several outstanding factors that contributed to its success in helping the

children to be well, grow and achieve. Highly skilled outdoor practitioners designed the

project with wellbeing and self-regulation in mind and were able to support the chil-

dren’s experience through positive relationships. This includes respecting their autono-

my, agency, providing nurture, emotional time and space away from school agendas.

The focus has been not on what the children are doing or learning, but on their inhabit-

ing a living space, regularly, playfully, over a substantial period of time, with freedom

to choose, time alone and in company. There were no fixed set activities. The living

natural environments had great affordances for play, learning, growth and health, creat-

ing opportunity for success through deep relationships with nature, each other and

themselves. The children regularly visited one site, growing a sense of relationship with

place. They benefitted from exploring other sites with holiday day visits year-round.

Finally, the project was integrated positively within the school.

The project was successful in supporting the children’s wellbeing and socio-

emotional development, with time spent developing their tortoise-mind capabilities

(Claxton, 1997), such as playful, creative, intuitive, relaxed and social ways of know-

Page 33: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

ing. Providing opportunities for 'emotional space’, ‘freedom to choose’ ,’free social

play’ etc in living environments, can be seen as the scenic route to achievement, much

like the slow and steady path of the tortoise. Yet pastoral support and affective learning

is needed to help disadvantaged children succeed at school (Rose et al,, 2012). The

project provided effective pastoral support and had a role in improving their academic

development. Therefore the approach presented is a helpful and recommended interven-

tion for any school or children’s setting to employ.

Through the children’s project participation, a gap was closed between the co-

hort and the school levels of attendance and academic development. Closing the gap is a

frequent debate in public education policy (Wilson, 2014), with various interventions

and panaceas offered to enable disadvantaged children to have similar opportunities to

succeed at school compared to better-off peers. Pastoral support can include many dif-

ferent elements and interventions. The project was part of the offer for disadvantaged

children in the school, alongside specialist help and other interventions. Therefore the

project cannot be found to be solely responsible for any improvements in the children’s

wellbeing or academic development. Partnership working with the practitioners and

school leadership supported integration. Thus, the project had a greater positive impact

on the children, e.g. praised for their involvement and regarded as ‘wild experts’ within

school. The school strongly upheld and championed the project’s values, beginning a

culture change in the school and local area to deliver similar projects in future and em-

bed it sustainably as an everyday part of the school’s offer, embracing an outdoor affec-

tive ethos with contagious enthusiasm. The school support demonstrated the essential

role of values, school culture and senior leadership in creating sustainable, ‘do-able’

projects and lasting positive change. The offer for disadvantaged pupils was then ex-

Page 34: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

tended to the whole school. This is in line with strong recommendations, such as that

made by the government’s Chief Medical Officer (Brooks, 2013) for a whole school

approach and collaboration with wider communities.

Despite such strong endorsement, within government-commissioned reports and

research, outdoor play and learning has never received substantial national government

funding in UK settings. The recent Natural Connections Demonstration Project (NCDP)

has shown that children thrive when offered a rich diet of outdoor opportunities in

school and teaching staff benefit (Waite and Malone, 2016). Given the longitudinal na-

ture of the study, made alongside the NCDP project and supported by a growing body of

evidence, there is every reason to suggest strong core funding and entitlement for all

children to access outdoor play and learning. We are bold enough to suggest the Nation-

al Curriculum in England embraces the outdoors as the Scottish Curriculum for Excel-

lence has, to good effect (Learning and Teaching Scotland, 2010). In the meantime, we

encourage settings to trust in the evidence and develop positive values and understand-

ing within their teams and the wider communities they inhabit. The findings indicate

that positive wellbeing outcomes and learning competencies may best be served through

taking a scenic route, by not focusing on the goals for assessment but by engaging chil-

dren restoratively in inhabiting living environments.

‘[Children’s} learning is an underground river, you can’t see it, can’t even feel it at times. Then suddenly they soar. You can’t control it; you can’t take credit for it. It’s theirs. You have to be there, providing warmth and stability, providing tools and resources, answering questions, telling stories, having meaningful adult conversa-tions and doing meaningful adult work in their presence. But when they soar, it’s on their own wings’. (Black, 2016)

Page 35: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Blagrave Trust and Plymouth University. Owen Thompson, Freelance Researcher

With our gratitude to the peer reviewers for their helpful comments.

1. (Author no 1) led this study as Director at Free Range Creativity in partnership with Ply-mouth University. She is now a member of the Department of Education and Childhood at University of the West of England.

References

Austin, C., Knowles, Z., Richards, K., McCree, M., Sayer, J. & Ridgers N. (2015). ‘It’s

Natural to Play: Creating Enabling Environments for Physically Active Play in

the Forest Setting’, in Skelton, T., Nairn, K. and Kraftl, P. (2015) Space, Land-

scape, and Environment, Volume 2, in Skelton, T. (editor-in-chief) Geographies

of Children and Young People, Springer Singapore.

Borradaile, L. (2006). Forest School Scotland: An Evaluation. Edinburgh: Forestry

Commission.

Bragg, R. Wood, C., Barton, J. and Pretty, J. (2013). Measuring connection to nature in

children aged 8 - 12: A robust methodology for the RSPB. University of Essex:

Unpublished report for RSPB.

Brooks, F. (2013). Chapter 7: Life Stage: School Years. Chief Medical Officers’s annual

report 2012: Our Children Deserve Better: Prevention Pays. London: Depart-

ment of Health.

Butcher, H. and Andrews, J. (2009). ‘How Well Am I Doing On My Outcomes?’ In Eke,

R., Butcher, H. and Lee, M. (Eds.) Whose Childhood Is It?: The roles of chil-

dren, adults and policy makers (pp. 35-65). London: Continuum.

Page 36: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Chen-Hsuan Cheng, J. and Monroe, M. C. (2012). ‘Connection to Nature: children’s

affective attitude toward nature’. Environment and Behavior, 44(1): 31–49.

Clark, A. and Moss, P. (2001). Listening to Young Children: The Mosaic Approach.

London: National Children’s Bureau Enterprises Ltd.

Claxton, G. (1997). Hare Brain Tortoise Mind: why intelligence increases when you

think less. London, Fourth Estate.

Department for Education (DfE) (2015). Ofsted RAISEonline 2015 Summary Report for

school, datasets for 2015, Key Stages 1 and 2. (Unpublished).

Department for Education and Skills (DfES) (2006). Learning Outside the Classroom

Manifesto. Nottingham, UK: DfES.

Dillon, J. and Dickie, I. (2012). Learning in the Natural Environment: Review of social

and economic benefits and barriers. Natural England Commissioned Reports,

092. London: Natural England.

Fiennes, C., Oliver, E., Dickson, K., Escobar, D., Romans, A., & Oliver, S. (2015). The

Existing Evidence-Base about the Effectiveness of Outdoor Learning. Institute of

Outdoor Learning, UK.

Gallacher, L.A. and Gallagher, M. (2008). 'Methodological immaturity in Childhood

Research?: Thinking through ‘participatory methods’', Childhood, 15(4),

499-516.

Ingold, T. (2008). ‘Bindings against boundaries: entanglements of life in an open

world’. Environment and Planning A, 40, 1796-1810.

Laevers, F. (Ed.) (2005). Well-being and Involvement in Care Settings. A Process-ori-

ented Self-evaluation Instrument (SiCs) (Research Centre for Experiential Edu-

cation, Leuven University). Brussels: Kind & Gezin.

Learning & Teaching Scotland. 2010. Curriculum for Excellence through Outdoor

Learning. Edinburgh: Scottish Government.

Page 37: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

Malone, K. and Waite, S. (2016). Student Outcomes and Natural Schooling. Plymouth:

Plymouth University. Retrieved from: http://www.plymouth.ac.uk/research/oel-

res-net

Murray, R. and O’Brien, L. (2005). ‘Such enthusiasm - a joy to see': an evaluation of

Forest School in England. Forest Research for New Economics Foundation.

Nicholson, S. (1971). How NOT to cheat children: The Theory of Loose Parts. Land-

scape Architecture, (62), 30-34.

Ridgers, N.D., Knowles, Z.R. and Sayers, J. (2012). Encouraging play in the natural

environment: a child-focused case study of Forest School. Children's Geogra-

phies, 10(1), 49-65.

Roe, J. and Aspinall, P. (2011). The restorative outcomes of Forest School and conven-

tional school in young people with good and poor behavior. Urban Forestry and

Urban Greening, 10(3), 153-256.

RSPB (2013). Connecting with nature; finding out how connected to nature the UK’s

children are. Unpublished report. Sandy, Bedfordshire. Retrieved from: rsp-

b.org.uk/connectionmeasure

Rose, J., Gilbert, L. and Smith, H. (2012). Affective teaching and the affective dimen-

sions of learning. In Ward, S. (Ed.) A Student’s Guide to Education Studies (3rd

edn.). London: Routledge.

Rickinson, M., Hunt, A., Rogers, J. and Dillon, J. (2012). School Leader and Teacher

Insights into Learning Outside the Classroom in Natural Environments. Natural

England Commissioned Reports, 097. London: Natural England.

Strauss, A and Corbin, J.M. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and

Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory (2nd edn.). London: Sage

Taylor, A. (2013). Reconfiguring the natures of childhood. London/New York: Rout-

ledge.

Ulset, V., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Bekkus, M., & Borge, A.I.H., (2017). Time spent

outdoors during preschool: Links with children's cognitive and behavioral de-

Page 38: The Hare and the Tortoise Author's Draft Feb 2018freerangecreativity.org/wp/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/The-Hare-and-the-Tortoise...Aspinall, 2011). Claxton (1997) wrote about ‘hare-brain

velopment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 52, 69-80. doi: 10.1016/j.jen-

vp.2017.05.007

Williams, T., Wetton, N., and Moon, A. (1989). A way in: Five key areas of health edu-

cation. London: Health Education Authority.

Wilson, J. (2014). Closing the Gap with the new national primary curriculum. Carmel

Education Trust for National College of Teaching and Leadership.

Figure 1. Jellybean people sharing angry emotions.

9268 words.