Top Banner
The Founding of the Primate Station, Tenerife, Canary Islands Author(s): Marianne L. Teuber Source: The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 107, No. 4 (Winter, 1994), pp. 551-581 Published by: University of Illinois Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1423000 . Accessed: 17/02/2011 17:08 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at . http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=illinois. . Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The American Journal of Psychology. http://www.jstor.org
32

The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

Nov 11, 2014

Download

Documents

preforman
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

The Founding of the Primate Station, Tenerife, Canary IslandsAuthor(s): Marianne L. TeuberSource: The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 107, No. 4 (Winter, 1994), pp. 551-581Published by: University of Illinois PressStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1423000 .Accessed: 17/02/2011 17:08

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unlessyou have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and youmay use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at .http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=illinois. .

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printedpage of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to TheAmerican Journal of Psychology.

http://www.jstor.org

Page 2: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

History of Psychology RAND B. EVANS, EDITOR East Carolina University

The founding of the Primate Station, Tenerife, Canary Islands MARIANNE L. TEUBER Council for the Arts, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

The Primate Station on Tenerife in the Canary Islands is known for Wolfgang Kthler's experiments (1914-20) on the intelligent behavior of anthropoid apes. The Station was founded in 1912 by Max Rothmann, neurophysiologist at the University of Berlin, and sponsored by the Royal Prussian Academy of Sciences. The present article describes the circumstances and sudden appointment of Eugen Teuber as director. In addition to setting up the Station in 1913, Teuber observed the "affective calls" of chimpanzees; their "gestural language" led him to conclude that one might communicate with them by means of gestural signs like those used by deaf-mutes. His findings are compared with discoveries made 50 years later. Photographs taken by Teuber illustrate his tenure. He left a well-functioning Station to Wolfgang Kihler whose first intelligence experiment of December 31, 1913, he re- corded.

The Primate Station on Tenerife Island, off the northwest coast of Africa, became famous when Wolfgang Kihler (1887-1967) carried out his crucial experiments there in 1914 on the intelligence or in-

sightful behavior of chimpanzees. Kihler's experiments were first

published as "Intelligenzpriifungen an Anthropoiden I" by the Royal Prussian Academy of Sciences in Berlin (Kihler, 1917) and released in book form as Intelligenzpriifungen an Menschenaffen (Kohler, 1921/ 1963) to be translated into English as The Mentality of Apes (Kohler, 1925/1957). Kohler arrived at the Station late in December 1913. However, the Station, the first of its kind, had been in existence for

nearly a year before that. This article tells the story of the founding of the Tenerife Primate Station and of observations planned and made in 1912 and 1913, the period before and shortly after Kohler's arrival. What follows is largely based on documents and photographs, orig- inally belonging to Eugen Teuber (1889-1958), the first director of

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY Winter 1994, Vol. 107, No. 4, pp. 551-581 ? 1994 by the Board of 'rustees of the University of Illinois

Page 3: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

the Primate Station, and collected by him in a large folder (Teuber Folder).'

Creation of the Primate Station

The idea of founding a station for the observation of anthropoid primates was the brainchild of Max Rothmann (1868-1915), physician and neurophysiologist at the University of Berlin. Rothmann was

primarily interested in the functions of the central nervous system; he specialized in comparative studies of brain physiology and neu-

roanatomy. In his research with chimpanzees, however, he encoun- tered difficulties in maintaining them in an artificial laboratory setting, particularly in a northern climate. Because of this, as Rothmann re-

ported later, "plans were made to establish a research station in a more favorable climate, closer to the natural territories of anthropoid primates. It was immediately agreed that experiments in brain phys- iology should take second place and that, instead, psychological ob- servations and experiments on the behavior of apes-so close to humans-were to be the first order of systematic investigations" (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 3). Rothmann hoped that the behavior of the chimpanzee, gorilla, orangutan, and gibbon would be studied and compared, permitting a better evaluation of these animals on an

evolutionary scale. He recognized early the danger of their gradual extinction and called for nature preserves. It was his expectation that the "precious animals" would be observed in surroundings resembling as closely as possible their natural habitat (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, PP. 3, 5).

Rothmann submitted an application to the Royal Prussian Academy of Sciences, Berlin, as early as February 27, 1910 (Rothmann, 1910),2 in which he emphasized these hopes and suggested Tenerife Island as a suitable setting because of its climate, and closeness to Cameroon, then a German colony, where apes could be captured. Tenerife Island also offered easy access for investigators from many countries because tourism and the hotel industry had begun to flourish on Tenerife (cited from Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, pp. 3-4). This initial appli- cation was rejected, but when Rothmann reapplied in 1912, he was successful (Rothmann, 1912). He immediately paid a visit to Orotava, inland from Puerto de la Cruz on the northern coast of the volcanic island of Tenerife, largest of the Spanish Canary Islands (Figure 1). He found the climate optimal for anthropoid primates (though not

necessarily for the human experimenter); temperatures varied be- tween 16?C and 22 C in the shade (60 F to 80 F); but the humidity was constantly 72% to 80%. As Rothmann noted, a compromise had

552 TEUBER

Page 4: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

Punta del Vhnlo

Punta del Sol

PuNot de Gunmar

Punta ', Tsmanmo

Punta de lkcak

1 Putsa do Qbona

Punta Negra s '~ c^~' he, rnoa Pl N Arorni arco del PLro f Pn*d del Sordo

San Juan

Punta doe Comudn

Punto RolJ

Punta de Is R"uc

0 10 20 30 40 50 Km. I i . .. .t t I , *I - 1

Figure 1. Map of Tenerife Island, with Orotava on northern coast, inland from Puerto de la Cruz (cross indicates location of Primate Station)

to be made between what was good for the animals and what was good for the scientists (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, pp. 3-4).

In his applications, Rothmann had suggested that the Station be supported by both private and Academy of Sciences funds. In 1912, the new project was initially financed from two private sources, the Plaut and the Selenka Foundations. Continuing funding of the Station would be provided by the recently established Albert Samson Foun- dation of the Royal Prussian Academy of Sciences. Samson, a banker with interests in biology, anatomy, and anthropology, had donated one million German marks to the Academy for research purposes. He asked Wilhelm Waldeyer, whose lectures on anatomy at the Uni- versity of Berlin he had attended, to chair the Samson Foundation Board (Ash, 1982, pp. 374ff; Jaeger, 1988, pp. 99-102, n. 57).

553

Page 5: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

Once the Foundation was established in 1911, Waldeyer (who was permanent Secretary of the Physical-Mathematical Section of the Academy) appointed distinguished members of the Prussian Academy to the Board, including the physicist Max Planck, the anatomist Hans Virchow, the neuroanatomist Hermann Munk, and the psychologist Carl Stumpf. Because of the planned emphasis on behavioral obser- vations, Stumpf (1848-1936) played a major role in the selection of the director of Rothmann's proposed Primate Station. In 1912, Stumpf considered Wolfgang K6hler for the position, and Max Wertheimer and David Katz as further possibilities. Kbhler had obtained his doc- torate under Stumpf at the University of Berlin 3 years earlier. But Kohler had no experience in animal research. Stumpf's first choice for director of the new Station was Oskar Pfungst (1874-1932) who had received considerable notice for his critical review of von Osten's "Clever Hans," the horse that could supposedly count and calculate

(Pfungst, 1907/1965). Stumpf was particularly taken with Pfungst's careful observations that had brought out the fact that the horse was

responding to just noticeable signs by its master, von Osten, such as a slight nod of his head, leading the horse to calculate correctly by scraping its hoof each time there was a nod. Pfungst had also published research on anthropoid primates in the Berlin Zoo. Stumpf and Pfungst stood for a qualitative, descriptive approach to animal behavior, op- posing animal training as a basis for studying animal intelligence. Rothmann opposed animal training as well, but questioned animal observations made in zoos (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, pp. 5-6). Pfungst hesitated to accept the position, however, because he had

recently been severely bitten by one of the apes in the Berlin Zoo.

By November 1912, a director of the Station was urgently needed.

Eight young chimpanzees had been confined in narrow transport cages since September on the grounds of the Hotel Martianez at Puerto de la Cruz. Seven animals had been captured and shipped from the southern regions of Cameroon, and one young chimp, Konsul, had been purchased from aboard a ship as early as July 1912. Rothmann and Waldeyer, chairmen of the Committee on the Primate Station, feared for the lives of the chimpanzees who, by November, had been confined to their cages for 3 to 5 months.

Waldeyer proposed that Eugen Teuber, then a 23-year-old student (cand. phil.) in philosophy and psychology at the University of Berlin, go to Tenerife and set up the Station. Teuber's name was most likely brought to Waldeyer's attention by Rothmann, because a family friendship existed between the Rothmanns and the Teubers (H.-L.Teuber, 1966, p. 307, 1967, p. 6).

Carl Stumpf interviewed Eugen Teuber toward the end of Novem-

554 TEUBER

Page 6: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

ber. From the correspondence between Stumpf and Waldeyer, we know that Stumpf gained a good impression of the candidate but was concerned about his inexperience in animal psychology and the fact that he had not published anything. Waldeyer insisted that time was of the essence and that a director had to be appointed. Stumpf, unhappy because there was no time to consider any of his other candidates, Kohler, for instance, told Waldeyer that he did not wish to have his name associated with any publications of the future Station, lest the conclusion be drawn that he had recommended the director (Ash, 1982; Jaeger, 1988, p. 97, n. 53). Teuber, who did not know

anything about the exchange of letters between Stumpf and Waldeyer, believed all his life that Rothmann and Stumpf had sent him to Tenerife. On December 17, 1912, Teuber signed the contract to become director of the yet to be established Station.

Eugen Teuber grew up in Berlin; he was educated there at the College Franqais, a Gymnasium founded by French Huguenots in 1689. He spoke French fluently, and as a gifted linguist, he soon spoke Spanish as well. He had studied with Wilhelm Wundt at the University of Leipzig and was continuing his studies in psychology and philosophy at Berlin University (H.-L. Teuber, 1978).3 Highly musical and an accomplished pianist, Eugen Teuber was asked before his departure for Tenerife to record folk melodies on the Canary Islands and else- where for the "Tonarchiv" (Phonogram Archive) of the Berlin Psy- chological Institute.4

Eugen Teuber sets up the Primate Station

On January 8, 1913, Eugen Teuber and his young wife, Rose, who became his assistant, arrived at Tenerife. Teuber had contracted to serve as director for only one year, at a salary of 3,000 German marks. As later with Kohler, the contract stipulated that every 6 weeks, Teuber was to send a report on the Station and on his observations to the Prussian Academy. The 10-point contract also required that in the event of a German military mobilization (apparently considered a possibility late in 1912) or of life-threatening illness, Teuber and his wife were to leave the island at once (Teuber Folder, 1912a).

On his arrival at Tenerife, Teuber had planned to purchase or rent land on which a research station would be built; he actually brought with him professionally drawn architectural plans for such a Station, including a small playground for the animals (Teuber Folder, 1912b). The design in the style of a Bavarian mountain chalet (featuring hearts carved into the wooden shutters) fortunately was never carried out.

Teuber assessed the real estate situation soon after his arrival. In

555

Page 7: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

an urgent memorandum to the Academy in Berlin, dated January 22, 1913, he summarized his findings:

1. The purchase price of land was unusually high because of the valuable banana plantations.

2. Building a new station would take time; he was eager to get the

chimpanzees out of their confining cages and into a freer environment as soon as possible.

3. He therefore thought it best to rent a small country estate (a "Sitio") with house, garden, and land. He had inspected four such sites and found one especially suitable, the country house "La Costa" of Don Melchior Luz y Lima, several times the Alcalde (mayor) of Orotava. The monthly rent would be 94 German marks for house and land, a third of the amount budgeted for these items. La Costa was 90 meters above sea level and was protected from the winds, a site judged to be most favorable for the chimpanzees (Teuber Folder, 1913a).

Speed in freeing the chimpanzees from their cages and giving them a fairly natural environment was Teuber's major consideration. The animals had developed colds in the shady location where their cages had been placed at the Hotel Martianez. A rumor had spread that

they had been injected with tubercle bacilli for experimental purposes (Jaeger, 1988, p. 98, n. 53). Although the animals were cared for

during their long confinement by Richard von Hertwig, professor of

zoology at Munich University, who resided temporarily on Tenerife

(Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 4), and by George Perez, a local

physician, one of the female chimpanzees, who had been ill, died 3

days before Teuber's arrival (Teuber Folder, 1913b). The trustees of the Samson Foundation approved Teuber's proposal

(Teuber Folder, 1913b), and on February 19, 1913, a 7-year lease was

signed (toward the end of that period, the animals would reach sexual

maturity). The caged chimpanzees were immediately brought to their new location. La Costa became the home and study of the director

(Kohler lived there with his wife and children, 1914-18). Next to it, a large playground of about a quarter of an acre was set aside for the animals to roam. The terrain was fenced in and covered with wire netting supported by a tall pole, approximately 15 ft high. In this way, the chimpanzees could not escape but had a large territory to themselves (Figures 2, 3). At least one-half of the playground was covered with tall grass and shrubs. A jungle gym was placed in the center; there were also two banana plants and a tree. The chimpanzees immediately demolished the banana plants and robbed the tree of all its leaves. It was this tree that would become famous, when Kohler used it for his first crucial experiment, the "Fruit Basket Experiment,"

556 TEUBER

Page 8: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION 557

Figure 2. Large playground, ape house and laboratory, pole supporting wire

netting of roof, tree, jungle gym; Rose Teuber, left; caretaker and chim-

panzees, right

*' ,.~ '

'. lv:^ '* * . '

.... .?... :. ? . . . .. *

,,

I' ^?' ???r X'

''

.

* '

* ?;??e

' ...

-v- ,sj,X,, -_;_,,iS W spu - -~~~~~~~~~. .*. .'*. *. ~ ," /... :*- **.^ *^....,;: . ?*... . . *. * . ~.

.... ,,-- - -.. *; , *

.... . ,. , r .,* s .;..~ * S- ;~L~?Cu-~ .. , :'.'~,:,q~ ,, ,~ .... . . .*

il.i. I:* I _ _ ?r^s - '-'y!^ ^ * ,~~' ..,....... *t:'. m 1 * " ' m~i^r'm.^ $y^.^

Figure 3. Eugen Teuber with young chimp (note wire fence and netting of roof; beyond, tropical garden with observer from town)

- :^ . ..

- I . :

*,.?.:I JBljl ,:?iBPi

s?

liku. .( ;sr

Page 9: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

that would usher in his series of Umweg or detour experiments (cf. Figs. 2 and 10).

In this new environment, the chimpanzees regained their health. Perez continued to take care of the animals' medical needs in exchange for an annual contribution by the Samson Foundation to his hospital. Teuber dismissed his first animal caretaker when he found him un-

trustworthy and demanding too high a salary. He then hired Manuel Gonzales y Garcia (1888-1976), an illiterate, but reliable, skillful

handyman, and a fearless, kind caretaker of the animals. Gonzales remained with the Station until it closed in 1920.

An ape house with an open-air runway was built on one side of the

playground (Figure 4); the ape house had four sleeping chambers, each accommodating several young chimpanzees, for the gregarious animals preferred to sleep in groups-always consisting of the same

chimps. Figure 5 shows chimpanzees huddling on a ladder, after they had become used to their new home. The long period in cages had made all animals timid at first. However, an older female chimpanzee called Tchego (she was a Tchego-type chimpanzee) had become vi- cious, and had to be housed separately, with her own playground. Slowly, Teuber gained her confidence (Figure 6). Although Teuber had no formal training in animal care, his empathy with the chim-

panzees and recognition of their natural needs aided their adjustment to the new surroundings.

A laboratory was attached to the ape house including facilities for

photography, filming, and phonographic recording (Teuber Folder, 1913c). These facilities were used by Teuber, and also later by K6hler in his detour experiments when he photographed and filmed the

"intelligent" behavior of the chimpanzees: their tool use, stacking of boxes, swinging on doors to reach a goal, etc.5

Beginnings of research on chimpanzees The differences in approach between the first year (1913) under

Teuber and the following years under Kohler were striking. K6hler would devise specific experimental situations to elicit the chimpanzees' ingenuity and "insightful" behavior. Teuber, however, had the task of letting the animals adjust to a new, relatively free environment, of

studying their physical condition (Figure 7), and observing and re-

cording their spontaneous, inherent behavior.

Physical condition. In a letter of May 15, 1913, Rothmann re- minded Teuber to observe the physical development of the chimpan- zees carefully (Teuber Folder, 1913d). Teuber ascertained their ap- proximate ages. Five of the animals were from 5 to 6 years old, for

they were beginning to acquire their permanent teeth (Rothmann &

558 TEUBER

Page 10: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

J

Figure 4. Ape house with open-air runway Figure 5. Young chimpanzees huddle on ladder; banana plantation in the background

Page 11: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

Figure 6. Eugen Teuber with Tchego Figure 7. The underside of a chimpanzee foot

Page 12: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

Teuber, 1915, p. 8). Teuber also interviewed the personnel of the

ships that had brought the chimpanzees to Tenerife, to make sure that they had come directly from the wild, did not have any prior training, contact with civilization, or illness (aeger, 1988, p. 98).

Spontaneous behavior. Teuber observed and recorded the chim-

panzees' spontaneous behavior in groups, with Sultan, the intelligent young male, in the lead during the first few months. (Sultan later would gain fame in Kohler's experiments.) Teuber described the an- imals at play. They played "catch" around the center pole. They encircled one of the ubiquitous lizards, kept it from escaping by chasing it back to the center, until finally one chimp jumped aside and let it out (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 9). Such teasing behavior was typical of the chimpanzees. With bits of food, the chimpanzees would lure chickens to the outside of their enclosure, but as soon as

they arrived, they would poke them with sticks through the wire fence and watch them flutter away, cackling noisily (see also Kohler, 1925/ 1957, pp. 77-79, for similar observations). Kohler commented on the

chimpanzees' rambunctious behavior, saying they acted "like street urchins" (letter of July 15, 1914; in Jaeger, 1988, p. 48). Kohler would be less permissive with the animals than Teuber had been.

In addition, Teuber described the chimpanzees' so-called dances and their upright gait: "It was a remarkable sight to see two chim-

panzees walk along, arm in arm like children." Local people said that the chimpanzees acted "like a Cristiano," when they seemed to behave like humans (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 17). Further observations were made on the animals' handedness (right-handedness could not be noticed), on their social behavior, spontaneous use of tools, and

ability to differentiate colors, to name but a few items (Teuber Folder, 1913e). However, the most far-reaching observations by Teuber have to do with what may be called the gestural language of chimpanzees as a precursor of language.

Teuber had attended the lectures of Wilhelm Wundt at Leipzig University in 1909-10, at a time when Wundt's central concern was no longer physiological psychology but "V5olkerpsychologie" [folk psy- chology, or a comparative psychology of culture]. Wundt's influence would become apparent in Teuber's observations of the chimpanzees, particularly ideas from the Vilkerpsychologie on the Language of Gestures (Wundt, 1911/1973).6

Questions of communication and "language"

Rhythmic dances as communication. Teuber reported especially on the rhythmic foot stamping in chimpanzee "dances." He observed a three-part rhythm: two beats done by stamping one foot, then the

561

Page 13: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

other, the third by hand. Occasionally, these rhythms, if clapped by the experimenter, released the dance in the animal. The rhythmic dances were performed primarily by Sultan in front of the female chimps. While dancing, he often chased a young female around the sleeping quarter. Rothmann and Teuber (1915, pp. 9-10) thought the dances functioned as sexual communication.

Such rhythmic qualities of animal and "primitive" dances had also been noted by Wundt in the first volume of his Vilkerpsychologie (1911), entitled Die Sprache [Language]. Wundt considered the rhythms of dance both in animals and so-called "primitive" human cultures one of the preverbal origins of language (Wundt, 1911, pp. 276ff.). Teuber owned the first volume of Wundt's Vblkerpsychologie on language.

Chimpanzee vocal utterances as expressions of affect. Teuber recorded the chimpanzees' vocalizations on the Edison phonograph. He noted their accompanying gestures and facial expressions by pho- tographing and perhaps filming them, as the Rothmann-Teuber Work-

ing Plan had suggested: "the vocalizations to be recorded with the Edison Home Phonograph, and the accompanying gestures, facial

expressions and postures illustrated with the movie camera" (Teuber Folder, 1913f). A repeated, short o, o, o was a sign of greeting and

happiness; a short, sharp ae, ae, ae was a warning; repeated, high ee, ee, ee designated fear,7 u sounds marked sorrow or rage. Teuber con- cluded that all these were expressions of affect rather than language (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, pp. 13-14). In keeping with Wundt's ideas, Teuber stressed the emotional quality of sounds in his chim-

panzees. High-sound qualities were the natural sounds of excitation; deep (low) vowels (like u) denoted depressed, sad feelings. They are not yet language, Wundt and Teuber maintained, but an emotional

protolanguage. Similarly, Wundt had stated that interjections like oh or ah in human language interrupt structured sentences and provide emotional coloring (Wundt, 1911, pp. 319-322).

To evaluate Teuber's early findings, it is worthwhile to consider similar observations and more sophisticated experiments made dec- ades later. Detlev Ploog, at the Max-Planck Institute for Psychiatry, Munich, systematically recorded sounds and calls of new-world pri- mates, with striking results (Ploog, 1981). Ploog found that the centers for the production of species-specific sounds and calls of the squirrel monkey (similar to the calls uttered by the Tenerife chimpanzees) are not located in the neocortex (as is human speech) but in an area

adjacent to the limbic system of the old brain, with its capacity to elicit moods. The notion of "species-specific" behavior in animals was not yet current in 1913, though Rothmann and Teuber speak of "inherent behavior" (Eigenleistungen) of anthropoid primates (see

562 TEUBER

Page 14: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

Schiller, 1957). In this connection, it is remarkable that Rothmann and Teuber (1915, p. 19) ask the question of whether the sounds uttered by the chimpanzees are produced subcortically or are de-

pendent on specific neocortical centers. Gestural language and the paradigm of deaf-mute hand signals.

Along with the recording of sounds, Teuber made notations and

photographed the chimpanzees' facial expressions and gestures. A

begging gesture involved the pursing and opening of the lips, as well as expressive gestures, such as the outstretched arm and, often, the upturned open hand. Figure 8 shows Sultan begging for cocoa.

Teuber thought that the gestural language of chimpanzees was particularly well developed. He, therefore, considered the sign lan- guage of deaf-mutes far more suitable than vocalized language for communicating with chimpanzees, and he was optimistic regarding the success of teaching sign language to chimpanzees. A passage in which Teuber considers a proposal "to teach the chimpanzees a ges- tural language of the kind used by deaf-mutes with definite hand signals for definite objects or activities," also shows a note handwritten by Teuber saying: "Consider this rather likely" (Teuber Folder, 1913g).

In this connection, the link with Wundt's treatment of "gestural language" in his Volkerpsychologie (1911) is apparent. Wundt illustrated North American Indian and Neapolitan hand signals as an alternative to spoken language (pp. 173-197). What is more, Wundt devoted entire sections to the "Gestural Language of Deaf-Mutes" (pp. 145- 152, 216-223; 1973, pp. 57-63, 114-120). He noted that sponta- neous gestures eventually develop into conventions in groups of in- stitutionalized deaf-mutes; he stressed, in particular, the facial expres- sions accompanying certain gestures.8

Teuber predicted that teaching chimpanzees hand signals would be far superior to teaching them verbal language. The much later re- search by Allen and Beatrice Gardner rediscovered many of the phe- nomena that Teuber recorded on Tenerife. They confirmed in their experiments with Washoe, the infant female chimp, Teuber's idea of the value of communicating with chimpanzees through gestural sign language (Gardner & Gardner, 1969).

Teuber planned to write a report on the spontaneous gestural lan- guage he had observed when chimpanzees communicate with each other and with familiar human beings. These observations did not center on teaching and learning, but on gestural communication de- veloped freely by the chimpanzees themselves. Teuber planned to compare the expressive gestures of chimpanzees with similar "prim- itive" human gestures, often illustrated in works of art. Hans-Lukas Teuber, Eugen Teuber's son, related one such observation: Chim-

563

Page 15: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

TEUBER

Figure 8. Sultan begging for cocoa

panzees have a caressing gesture of stroking another chimpanzee with their fingers under the chin; this gesture also occurs between human beings, portrayed for instance on Greek vase paintings. Note that Eugen Teuber is using exactly that gesture to befriend Tchego (Figure

564

Page 16: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

6). He had intended to write an essay on these observations after he returned to Berlin in 1914. It was never completed because he vol- unteered and entered military service in World War I (see also Jaeger, 1988, p. 98, n. 53). Instead, Teuber concentrated on the more urgent task of putting together his reports on the first year of the Station and on the observations he had made. These reports became part of the 1915 article by Rothmann and Teuber for the Prussian Academy, with a note reading, "Since Mr. Teuber is at present at the front, the

following is based on his reports, supplemented by information from Mrs. Teuber, who assisted her husband on Tenerife" (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 4, n. 2).

Communicating via a visual sign language. Rothmann and Teuber were also interested in a language of visual symbols (Figure 9). They asked: "Can one teach chimpanzees certain signs to represent certain

objects, such as signs for banana, orange, sugar, rice, and blanket?"

(Teuber Folder, 1913h). We do not know the outcome, only the pro- gram, like other tests primarily designed by Rothmann. Premack

(1971) and Greenfield and Savage-Rumbaugh (1991) would dem- onstrate the value of such visual sign language in their much later research. Teuber considered it "quite improbable" (Figure 9, point 3) that chimpanzees would learn certain sounds as "names" for objects (e.g., oho for banana). He thought it "equally improbable" (Figure 9, point 4) that the animals would learn how to draw signs for banana, orange, etc. (Teuber Folder, 1913h).

In summary, it should be emphasized that Rothmann and Teuber

thought that the vocal utterances of chimpanzees were "affective calls." They considered it inappropriate to teach a vocal language to

chimpanzees. The fact that they could not observe any hand pref- erence contributed to their suspicion that the chimpanzee vocalizations were "subcortical," that they were not, like humans, equipped with cortical speech areas. This was recognized more than half a century later, independently of Rothmann and Teuber's observations (Ploog, 1981). Teuber noticed the chimpanzees' "gestural language" which he thought might well form the basis for teaching them hand signals like those of deaf-mutes. That idea has become the method of choice in communicating with chimpanzees. If one waits long enough, ideas and findings recur!

Teuber's indebtedness to Wundt is clearly expressed in a later re-

port: "In any case, one should point out again how felicitous the

theory of Wundt is to derive language from sounds of affect and

expressive gestures and not merely from intentions to communicate" (Teuber Folder, 1921).

565

Page 17: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

-a g ' 1 E > W . j N * . e c . c? 30 3 ?

. . C

"- " " 5i | , :* S y o D e.. A ? e ~ i, , a .< ?,

,.t .

C*> \'y s ^ n - < 5 f t

* , ._ _ , '- '- , -' - p " l'

. , t -

j. .. tl. ~

' : ~ . t ~ g

" ,.

? ' '5

og- : O 4 -- ? 3 5 I - c

9 S o $ s ' r,. .4 ~cnc~m~ :3' C : ' :. . . .

0 5 ;. .-S ), O ~ ..;

;a *1 -? ^?:, v .: N

. ? ~

P :

a r " * t ? * -.

Crs ^ 3 s " E b . sk 3~ e

$3 2 .- -

(

- t0? S

g" * s cc '

Pb? f, I*

th^^^^---- O- _ . .._..

Page 18: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

The Station in full operation In August 1913, Rothmann visited the new Station. He was well

satisfied with the good health of the seven animals (Sultan, Rana, Chica, Grande, Konsul, Tercera, Tchego-names that will be found in Kiohler's publications and Teuber's notes). He praised the circum- spect planning in setting up the Station and Teuber's sensitive handling of the chimpanzees. In the fall of 1913, Robert H. Yerkes (1886- 1956) wrote to Max Rothmann inquiring about the "planned" Station. Letters by Rothmann and Teuber assured Yerkes that the Station was no longer in the planning stage but in full operation. As is well known, Yerkes hoped to join the Tenerife Station during his sabbatical year of 1915. He would have been one of the international scientists Roth- mann wished to attract. The outbreak of World War I destroyed these plans.9

Wolfgang Kiihler's arrival and goal-oriented "insight" experiments

The dissimilarities between Teuber and Kohler could not have been greater than in the area of language. None of Kohler's experiments dealt with finding a substitute for language. Kohler noted that lan- guage and planning for a long-term future were distinguishing marks of human beings and not found in apes. Instead, Kohler stressed "language-free" experiments of goal-oriented behavior that demon- strated "insight" or thought. The goal was usually a desirable piece of food that was visible, such as a banana, but could only be reached by a "detour." After the animal had time to "survey the situation," insight came as a flash; the chimpanzees resorted to detour behavior and tool use (another detour) to reach the goal.

Kohler arrived at Tenerife in the last week of December 1913. He came from Frankfurt am Main, where he was Privatdozent (similar to assistant professor) at the Psychological Institute headed by Friedrich Schumann. There, as is well known, Kohler was a participant with Kurt Koffka in Max Wertheimer's first Gestalt-theoretical experiments starting in 1910/1911. Kbhler brought Gestalt-psychological ideas discussed with Koffka and Wertheimer to the Tenerife Primate Station.

First version of fruit basket experiment, December 31, 1913. The earliest recorded experiment done by Kohler, assisted by Eugen Teu- ber (Kohler, 1925/1957, p. 14), was the fruit basket experiment (Figures 10, 11).'1 With a familiar person like Teuber present, the animals would be more at ease, at first. Kohler started promptly after his arrival. He believed that he had only the one year, 1914 (for which he had committed himself), to do his experiments.

According to Teuber's notes (Teuber Folder, 1913i; Figure 11), the

567

Page 19: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

e' . -'.*

,e' A... ..... ..

.4 ;

/

Figure 10. Tree with fruit basket experiment. Drawing by Thekla Achenbach KOhler (from KOhler, 1921/1963; courtesy Springer Publishers, Berlin, Heidelberg).

Figure 11. First fruit basket experiment as recorded by Eugen Teuber, December 31, 1913 (Teuber Folder, 1913i)

Baum

_

_ _

_ I II I II

Page 20: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

fruit basket experiment was done for the first time on December 31, 1913. Sultan, the bright young male chimpanzee, was the subject. Kohler and the two Teubers were the observers. Teuber's report on the first fruit basket experiment is more detailed than Kbhler's and includes information about Sultan's initial "restless" behavior during the first exposure to the problem. The "gestation" period before Sultan had his "insight" and found a sudden solution to the problem is described more thoroughly. Kbhler repeated the experiment several times in 1914 and later, leading to a routine performance by Sultan in which not all of the initial behavior was included (Kohler, 1925/ 1957, pp. 14-16). From Teuber's account we also learn how much time it took Sultan to complete the first fruit basket experiment (about 10 min). Time is not mentioned by Kbhler because of the later rep- etitions. My translation of Teuber's description of the experiment follows:

Intelligence Behavior

Experiment with Sultan, XII. 31., 13., 10 a.m., before the feeding.

Arrangement: In the playground, a small basket with three pieces of banana and half an orange is hanging on a thin unobtrusive string at about 2 meters above the ground. The string runs through an iron ring fastened to the wire mesh roof of the playground and from there leads to the tree, about 3 meters away; the string ends in a wide loop that is hooked over the stump of a branch at the top of the tree [Figure 10]. From there, the basket can be moved. In addition, there is a pole on the ground, at some distance from the basket; the pole is familiar to the animal as a tool; with it, he might just barely reach the basket. Observers are Dr. Kohler and both T. [Teubers], who are hiding outside the playground and use their hands to cover their faces. The other animals are in the ape house, hence not visible, but one can hear their noise.

Sultan is led into the playground; he startles and looks up at the unusual arrangement. Then fear [crossed out in the manuscript] rest- lessness seems to set in-that he is left alone. He runs around in the playground; his excitement expresses itself in loud cries and foot stamp- ing. The animals in the ape house respond; S. [Sultan] climbs on top of a post to be able to look into the ape house through the skylight. His gaze seems to go in that direction; it is uncertain whether he also looks from above into the small basket. He climbs down and resumes his agitated running to and fro. He runs over to Tchego's quarters, jumps up to her window and drums on her door. He glances several times briefly at the observers, but since they remain indifferent and he does not see T. [Teuber], he continues undisturbed. Quite suddenly, he is sitting high up in the tree, looks briefly at the arrangement, and then, without hesitation, pulls in the string hand over hand, so that the

569

Page 21: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

basket rises quickly to the ceiling. As the basket hits the wire roof, it tilts and a piece of banana falls to the ground. S. lets go of the string, the basket slides into its old position; S. jumps down from the tree and grabs the piece of banana. He expresses his pleasure by uttering sounds and by walking upright. After he has eaten, he again looks in the direction of the observers, then quiets down and climbs the tree again; he pulls on the string. One of the observers laughs, S. looks around but is undisturbed. He pulls, and this time so vigorously that the basket breaks off and falls to the ground. He climbs down, puts some of the food into his mouth, holds the rest in one hand and the basket in the other; in this way, he walks upright around the playground, several times. Then he sits down and eats.

He did not try to lift the loop off the branch; he did not notice the pole at all.

Duration of the entire experiment, about 10 minutes. 31.12.13.

Kohler noted that in all repeat experiments with the fruit basket, Sultan perseverated in pulling vigorously on the string to make the basket fall. Sultan did not vary his behavior by, for instance, carefully lifting the loop of string off the tree and gently lowering the basket, which would have been the best solution (Kohler, 1925/1957, pp. 15-16).

Kohler and Teuber were concerned that isolating individual ani- mals, as had to be done with Sultan during the fruit basket experiment, was unnatural and made the animals fearful (Kihler, 1925/1957, pp. 14-15, p. 23, n. 1). In most of his later intelligence experiments of box stacking, pole use, etc., Kohler saw to it that Sultan had com-

panions (as the illustrations in Kohler, 1921/1963, and Kohler's films of the experiments demonstrate). Teuber stated several times that in the first year, while the animals were adjusting to their surroundings, he avoided "intelligence experiments," because in most of these sit- uations the animals had to perform in isolation (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915, p. 15). Teuber emphasized the gregarious nature of the chim-

panzees, their living and acting in groups, their need to huddle to-

gether, to touch and groom each other (Rothmann & Teuber, 1915). " Teuber's descriptions of Sultan, as he enters the playground to do the fruit basket experiment for the first time, reflects these concerns; chimpanzee group behavior is also discussed by Kohler in "Zur Psy- chologie der Schimpansen" (Kohler, 1921, 1922).

For Kohler, the important point of the fruit basket experiment was Sultan's unhesitating continuous action via the detour of the tree to reach his objective, the fruit basket. Insight and the resulting action "takes place as a single continuous occurrence," Kohler wrote (Kohler,

570 TEUBER

Page 22: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

1925/1957), pp. 21-22). The fruit basket experiment was followed

by K6hler's well-known detour experiments, involving box stacking, pole use (including pole vaulting), tool making and tool use, swinging on top of doors to get to the food, and so forth. Most of these

experiments are dated from January through May 1914. Only the fruit basket experiment has no date in Koihler's text (because of the

repetitions). Toward the end of the first half year, Kohler began his comparative

detour experiments, involving hen and dog as well as his one-year- old daughter. Ash (1982, pp. 384ff.) in his highly informative account of the Primate Station, proposes that Kohler began with detour ex-

periments involving creatures higher and lower in the evolutionary scale and then turned to the chimpanzees. This would have been the

logical sequence. But the date of the fruit basket experiment, coming as it does before all the rest, argues against this position. Kohler

recognized the heuristic value of the comparative detour experiments with hen, dog, and child, and therefore placed them in his book between the fruit basket and the other detour experiments with the

chimpanzees. Kohler's letters to Geitel, recently published by Jaeger (1988) confirm that the comparative detour experiments were done in mid-1914 and later.

Teuber and Kohler came to their research from different theoretical

backgrounds; the findings of both add to our understanding of chim-

panzee behavior. Teuber and Rothmann's published (1915) and un-

published observations anticipate recent neurophysiological and be- havioral research. Kohler's situational thought experiments (proving insight on the part of the animals) point forward to Max Wertheimer's

lifelong concern with Productive Thinking (1945) and to the work of Wertheimer's student, K. Duncker (1935/1945), who cites Kohler's Tenerife experiments repeatedly. With his language-free experiments, Kohler demonstrated how animals and humans (small children and, potentially those with aphasia, cerebral palsy, etc.) can display their

intelligence spontaneously, without training, conditioning, or the use of language.

The Teubers leave the Primate Station and return to Berlin12

Eugen and Rose Teuber had originally planned to return to Berlin

during the last week of December 1913 (Teuber Folder, 1913j). How- ever, they delayed their departure until mid-January 1914, to intro- duce Kdhler and his family to the workings of the Station and to the chimpanzees, and to participate in Kohler's first experiments (Teuber Folder, 1914a). Before leaving, Teuber gave most of his books and journal articles to the Station, among them Darwin's Descent of Man

571

Page 23: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

and The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (!), Munk's Functions

of the Cortex-all in German-William and Clara Stern's Kinder- sprache [Children's Language, 1907] (Teuber Folder, 1914b, 1914c). The Teubers traveled via Spain and Italy to Berlin (Teuber Folder, 1914d).

On August 1, 1914, Germany declared war. Until 1918, Teuber served as communications officer on the Eastern front. He would not see Rothmann again. In 1915, Rothmann, despondent over the death of his son, Otto, who had volunteered in the early days of the war, took his own life (Teuber Folder, 1914e). Waldeyer paid tribute to Max Rothmann as the founder of the Primate Station: "It was he who conceived the plan and saw to it that it was carried out" (Waldeyer- Hartz, 1917).

After the war, Teuber returned to his studies at the University of Rostock; he completed his doctorate in 1921 under Emil Utitz, pro- fessor of philosophy and aesthetics, with a dissertation on the artistic

philosophy of the Abbe Jean-Baptiste Dubos (Teuber, 1924; Utitz, 1932).'3 Eugen Teuber did not enter an academic career, however, and did not return to animal research. Having lost his small family inheritance in the German inflation, he decided to join ADREMA, a German business machine company, as scientific director. He remained in Germany until the rise of Hitler. In 1938 his firm sent him to Denmark as director of exports. He, his wife, and younger son became Danish citizens. Eugen Teuber died in 1958 at the age of 68.

The Primate Station on Tenerife after Teuber

Eugen Teuber passed on to Kbhler a well-organized primate station and a group of healthy chimpanzees, uncontaminated by any prior training, to do the carefully thought-out and revealing intelligence experiments that would gain Kbhler much of his early fame. Kohler

appreciated Teuber's prior work in planning and setting up the Sta- tion. In his first letter to Waldeyer, K6hler wrote, "I must say that I do not know many persons who would accomplish that much under often difficult circumstances ... his handling of the animals and care- ful method of observation are exemplary" (Jaeger, 1988, p. 98, n. 53, letter dated January 4, 1914).

Halfway through Kohler's first year at Tenerife, Germany declared war. On August 3, 1914, 2 days after the declaration, K6hler "left the Station because of the general mobilization in Germany," as the contract demanded of the director of the Primate Station. However, neither Kohler nor the other 60 Germans of military age could find neutral ships to take them from Tenerife past the British fleet. On

August 12, 1914, the German Consul-General in Barcelona ordered

572 TEUBER

Page 24: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

everyone who was subject to military service to remain on the island.

January 18, 1915, at the beginning of his second year, Kohler wrote to Waldeyer saying that he would be willing to direct the Station as

long as the present situation obtained (aeger, 1988, p. 99, n. 56). The Prussian Academy sanctioned Koihler's continuing directorship in the spring of 1915.

In 1918, however, the owner of La Costa decided to sell the house and land to the British shipping firm Yoeward Brothers of Liverpool, despite the rental contract which was still in force until December 1920. Kohler had to move the Station to a new location, "El Cipres," in the fall of 1918. El Cipres was one of the four sites Teuber had considered in 1913. The sale of La Costa may have been politically motivated at that time, the last year of the war (Jaeger, 1988, p. 109, n. 77; Waldeyer-Hartz, 1919). The Station was formally closed in October 1920. To this day, however, the La Costa terrain is called on Tenerife "huertas de las machangos" [gardens of the monkeys] (Jaeger, 1988, p. 98, n. 53).

Kohler returned to Germany in May 1920. Stumpf, who was ready to retire because of illness, asked Kohler to take on the directorship of the Psychological Institute at the University of Berlin, starting November 19, 1920. Kbhler held an interim appointment as full

professor at the University of Gottingen, a post that had been vacated when G. E. Muller (1850-1936) retired. On April 1, 1922, Kbhler officially became the successor to Stumpf and full professor of phi- losophy (and psychology) at the University of Berlin (for details, see Jaeger, 1990).14

Recently, assertions have been made that the Primate Station was used for espionage during World War I (Ley, 1990). The foregoing information clearly indicates, however, that the Station was established for scientific purposes. Rumors of espionage were current even before the war. On July 15, 1914, Kohler wrote in one of his delightful and frank letters to Hans Geitel, his physics professor at the Gymnasium in Wolfenbiittel: "Not a Spaniard, but an Englishman has managed to spread the rumor that the apes are only a pretense for us to engage in espionage. Perhaps a Zeppelin could land here! In the meantime, nobody takes this seriously and we enjoy the special protection of the local government here, thanks to the commendations of the Foreign Office in Berlin" (Jaeger, 1988, p. 47; Pastore, 1990). Kbhler made fun of the rumor. He certainly would not have mentioned this situation if he had really been asked to spy. Ronald Ley, the author of A Whisper of Espionage, has been taken in by prewar and wartime rumors. Ley admits that he found no confirmation anywhere "of a psychologist- turned-spy," but he wants to leave us with the suspicion. Ley tried to

573

Page 25: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

bolster his argument by leading us to believe that an antenna at La Costa proves that Kohler was intercepting and sending messages about the movements of British ships. What might look like an antenna in Thekla Kohler's drawing of La Costa (Figure 12) was actually the utility pole that connected the Station in the spring of 1914 with the recently built electric power plant at Orotava (S. Jaeger, personal communication, Berlin, June 29, 1992). The supply of electricity to La Costa had been negotiated by Eugen Teuber in 1913, prior to Kohler's arrival. A photograph of La Costa (Figure 13), showing the same view as in Figure 12, was taken in 1986 by Renate Jaeger; it features the utility pole in the place where it was in Kohler's time.

Spying may still be going on at La Costa! When Kihler returned to Berlin in 1920, he arranged for the

Tenerife chimpanzees to be housed in the Berlin Zoo. Eugen Teuber and his young son Lukas, then 4 years old, often visited the chim-

panzees at the zoo. As they approached -although 7 years, and more, had passed since the Tenerife days-the animals would come to the fence, grab it and shake it vigorously, uttering the staccato o, o, o of

joy and greeting on spotting Teuber in the crowd.

Notes A briefer version of this article was presented in 1987 at the 19th Annual Meeting of the Cheiron Society (International Society for the History of Behavioral and Social Sciences). I am grateful to the Mary Ingraham Bunting Institute, Radcliffe College, Cambridge, MA, where I was a Fellow in 1986- 87 while preparing the first version of this article. In particular, I wish to thank Siegfried Jaeger of the Psychological Institute, Free University Berlin; we exchanged information before and after the publication of his book (Jaeger, 1988). Photographs, taken by Eugen Teuber at the Primate Station in 1913, and quotations from the Eugen Teuber Documents are by per- mission of the American Philosophical Society, Philadelphia, PA. The edi- torial skills of Rand B. Evans greatly improved this article.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to M. L. Teuber, 215 Crosby Street, Arlington, MA 02174. Received for publication October 20, 1992; revision received March 8, 1993.

1. The Teuber documents and photographs remained in the possession of the Teuber family until 1988 when they were given to the Manuscripts Collection of the American Philosophical Society, Philadelphia, where the papers of Wolfgang K6hler are deposited. References throughout the text to materials in the Teuber Folder are given in a chronologically arranged list (1912-14, 1921) preceding the references.

2. Horst Heinecke (University of Jena Center for the Maintenance of Experimental Animals) recently discovered Rothmann's 1910 application (see Heinecke & Jaeger, 1993, pp. 217-218).

574 TEUBER

Page 26: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

Figure 12. La Costa, residence of the director of the Primate Station. Draw-

ing by Thekla Achenbach Kohler. Note the utility pole for electricity installed in 1914 (in Jaeger, 1988; Ley, 1990; from Kohler, 1963, courtesy Springer Publishers, Berlin, Heidelberg).

k..

S. _

Figure 13. View of La Costa, taken in 1986. Note utility pole as in 1914

(courtesy Renate Jaeger, photographer).

575

Page 27: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

3. Hans-Lukas Teuber, son of Eugen Teuber, wrote (1978, p. 879), "My father still listened to Wundt['s lectures]." Whether Carl Stumpf, critical of Wundt for many years, privately objected to Teuber's studies with Wundt, we shall never know.

4. The Tonarchiv was in the care of Erich von Hornbostel and Otto Abraham: the latter, a piano virtuoso, was a friend of Eugen Teuber. Starting in 1912, the Phonogram Archive was also supported by the Samson Foun- dation. The wax cylinders of the Tonarchiv recordings were destroyed when

they melted in the intense heat during the bombing of Berlin at the end of World War II.

5. Copies of Kihler's films, "to convince the doubters" as Kohler wrote

(Ash, 1982), are preserved in the Archives of the American Philosophical Society, Philadelphia. One film version, arranged by Kohler, is available

through the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. 6. Wundt's The Language of Gestures (1973) is a translation of chapter 2

("Die Gebardensprache") from volume 1 of his Vilkerpsychologie (1911). The work contains an introduction by A. L. Blumenthal and two essays by G. H. Mead and K. Bihler.

7. I have "translated" these sounds into English; the high ee, ee, ee is i, i, i in the German text.

8. Around the early 1900s a standard sign language for deaf-mutes (like the later American Sign Language) did not exist. There was a finger alphabet. But the gestural language that Teuber had in mind and observed included

spontaneous arm-hand signals and facial expressions that were repeated and could become conventions in "conversations"-the way Wundt described the activities of deaf-mutes living together (Wundt, 1911, 1973).

9. Yerkes then conducted his own experiments in 1915, primarily on an

orangutan, at a station set up by Yerkes's student, G. V. Hamilton, on the McCormick estate in California (at Montecito, near Santa Barbara). In 1916, Yerkes published his Mental Life of Monkeys and Apes: A Study of Ideational Behavior. Kohler later wrote: "Mr. Yerkes also believes that insight must be attributed to his experimental animal" (Kihler, 1921, p. 194, postscript; not in Mentality of Apes, 1925/1957). It might be noted that Stanley McCormick, an industrialist (International Harvester) and a graduate of Princeton Uni-

versity, with his wife, Katherine, who was a 1904 graduate in biology of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, had the interests and means to sup- port the Hamilton/Yerkes Station.

10. The drawing reproduced in Figure 10 was done by Thekla Achenbach Kohler, as were all the other drawings in "Intelligenzpriifungen an An-

thropoiden" (1917) and in subsequent book editions (Kohler, 1921/1963, 1925/1957). She was a painter and sculptor, the granddaughter of the

landscape painter Andreas Achenbach of the neo-romantic Diisseldorf School. She assisted her husband in several experiments. Kohler acknowledged her collaboration specifically in his first Academy publication (Kohler, 1915).

11. The need of monkeys and apes to live in groups, to touch each other, has been stressed especially by H. F. Harlow and M. K. Harlow (1962); in

576 TEUBER

Page 28: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

"Social Deprivation in Monkeys," the Harlows describe rhesus monkeys, not

chimpanzees. H.-L. Teuber remembered his father's reports when the Bi-

ology Panel of the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) planned a space flight by a single monkey, prior to that of man. As members of the NASA Biology Panel in the 1960s, H.-L. Teuber and H. F. Harlow warned that a single monkey sent into space might die because of the stress of isolation. Teuber suggested that control capsules with individual monkeys be set up on the ground during the very period when the monkey was being sent into space. Several of the animals on the ground died, as did the monkey sent into space.

12. In the early fall of 1913, Teuber had been asked to stay on as director of the Station, but he declined. He wanted to comply with his military service

requirements and finish his university studies. 13. When discussing the philosophy of art of Jean-Baptiste Dubos, Utitz

refers to the "treatise of my student Eugen Teuber" (1932, p. 25). 14. Kohler's interim appointment at Gottingen finds an explanation in

the custom that a full professor at the University of Berlin-the most

prestigious in Germany-had to hold a full professorship elsewhere before he could be appointed at Berlin (Jaeger, 1990, pp. 23-24).

Teuber Folder (1912-1914, 1921): List of documents and materials cited in this article

1912a Copy of unsigned contract and letter from Waldeyer, dated De- cember 8, 1912, discussing several points of the contract before it was signed on December 17, 1912; original contract in Academy Archive.

1912b Professionally drawn ground plans and elevations of a Station, designed by Erich Levy, architect. A contract (dated December 20, 1912), signed by Teuber, Levy, and Waldeyer, stipulated that there would be no obligation to carry out the construction if the

building were not commissioned by April 1, 1913. A rough sketch of the Levy plan and building is in the Academy Archive.

1913a Carbon copy of memorandum, dated Orotava, January 22, 1913; original memorandum in Academy Archive.

1913b Letter, dated February 11, 1913, from the Academy of Sciences, signed by Waldeyer, states that "the chimpanzee corpse" prepared by George Perez according to Waldeyer's instructions and sent to the Anatomical Institute, University of Berlin, had arrived in good condition in a zinc box filled with formalin alcohol. In this letter, Waldeyer accepts Teuber's plan to rent' the country house and land. A copy of the February, 11, 1913, letter is also in the

Academy Archive.

1913c Copies of financial statements (periodically submitted throughout 1913) list building materials; the expenses for fruit, cocoa, bread,

577

Page 29: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

and rice for feeding the animals, for straw and sleeping blankets; the cost of apparatus, such as a movie camera with attachments, an Edison Home Phonograph, a stopwatch, salaries for the care- taker and the construction workers, etc. (originals in Academy Archive).

1913d Letter in longhand, May 15, 1913, from Rothmann to Teuber.

1913e Carbon copies of two typed programs: "Arbeitsplan" [Working Plan, 2 pages] and "Die Untersuchung der Schimpansen" [The Investigation of the Chimpanzees, 4 pages] contain a program of observations to be made (primarily drawn up by Rothmann); orig- inals in Academy Archive. There are also numerous handwritten notes by Teuber of his observations and thoughts; some of this material would later be published in Rothmann and Teuber (1915).

1913f Arbeitsplan [working plan], page 1, point 3.

1913g The investigation of the chimpanzees (see 1913e), page 4 and note 1.

1913h Sign language [Bilderschrift]. The investigation of the chimpan- zees, page 3 (see Figure 9).

1913i Report entitled, "Intelligenzhandlungen, V. [Versuch] mit Sultan, am 31.XII.13., 10 Uhr, vor dem Fittern" [Intelligence Behavior, Experiment with Sultan, December 31, 1913, 10 A.M., before the

feeding]; 5 pages, in Teuber's handwriting. The first page is re-

produced in Figure 11.

1913j Letter, December 15, 1913, from German Consul Jakob Ahlers to Teuber, regarding travel arrangements for the return trip of

Eugen and Rose Teuber to Berlin. I wish to thank S. Jaeger for

determining the date, January 15, 1914, when Teuber left the Primate Station.

1914a Sketch of a letter by Teuber to R. v. Hertwig, Munich, written on the way home from Naples, Italy (no date). Teuber relates how

swiftly the last few weeks had passed while he was introducing Kohler to the Station and the chimpanzees.

1914b Carbon copy of typed list of books and a few journal articles given by Teuber to the Station; list also in Academy Archive.

1914c In a letter, dated February 1, 1914, from the Royal Prussian

Academy of Sciences, Waldeyer acknowledged Teuber's gift of books and inquired about what books K6hler might need. In this letter and one dated May 11, 1914, Waldeyer wrote that he had been informed of Teuber's outstanding and "exemplary" (Koh- ler's phrase) administration of the Station, and expressed his thanks.

1914d The Teubers visited the Alhambra, Granada (see the Spanish guidebook of the Alhambra in the Teuber Folder). They traveled

578 TEUBER

Page 30: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

to Sicily, Naples, Rome, and Venice before returning to Berlin

(photographs taken by Eugen Teuber at Syracuse, Pompei, Rome, Venice, etc., in the spring of 1914, are in the Teuber family collection of photographs).

1914e A ribbon commemorating the death of Otto Rothmann early in World War I (1914) was kept by Teuber among his notes on the

primate station. It should be noted that it was known that Max Rothmann suffered from depression which ran in the family.

1921 Report on Missie, With Some Observations by Liebetreu (the caretaker at the Berlin Zoo). In Berlin, several animals were renamed (e.g., Rana, who gave birth to a male chimp, was renamed Loca). Missie

may be the former Tercera.

References

Ash, M. G. (1982). The emergence of Gestalt theory: Experimental pyschology in

Germany, 1890-1920. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Uni-

versity, Cambridge, MA. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving (L. S. Lees, Trans.). Psychological

Monographs, 58(5). (From Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [On the

psychology of productive thinking]. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Original work published 1935 and reprinted 1966)

Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969, August 15). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664-672.

Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (1991). Imitation, grammatical development, and the invention of protogrammar by an ape. In N. A.

Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch, & M. Studdert-

Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language devel-

opment (pp. 235-258). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1962, November). Social deprivation in

monkeys. Scientific American, 207, 136-146. Heinecke, H., & Jaeger, S. (1993). Entstehung von Anthropoiden-Stationen

zu Beginn des 20. Jahrhunderts [Establishment of anthropoid research stations in the early 20th century]. Biologisches Zentralblatt, 112, 215- 223.

Jaeger, S. (Ed.). (1988). Briefe von Wolfgang Kohler an Hans Geitel, 1907-1920. Mit zwei Arbeiten Kihlers "Uber elektromagnetische Erregung des Trommel-

felles" und "Intelligenzprilfungen am Orang" im Anhang [Letters of Wolf-

gang K6hler to Hans Geitel, 1907-1920. With two essays by Kohler "On electromagnetic stimulation of the eardrum" and "Intelligence tests on the orangutan" in the appendix]. Passau, Germany: Passavia

University Press.

Jaeger, S. (1990). Gestaltpsychologie-Wolfgang Kihler und seine Zeit [Gestalt psychology-Wolfgang Kohler and his time]. Berlin: Free University, University Library.

Kiihler, W. (1915). Optische Untersuchungen am Schimpansen und am Haus-

579

Page 31: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

huhn, unter Mitwirkung von Frau Th. Kohler und cand. phil. M. Uibe

(Leipzig) [Optical investigations on chimpanzee and hen, with partici- pation of Mrs. Th. K6hler and doctoral candidate M. Uibe (Leipzig)]. Abhandlungen der koiniglich preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften (phy- sikalisch-mathematische Klasse), Berlin, No. 3, 1-70.

Kohler, W. (1917). Intelligenzpriifungen an Anthropoiden I [Intelligence tests on anthropoids]. Abhandlungen der kiniglisch preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften (physikalisch-mathematische Klasse), Berlin, No. 1, 1- 213.

Kohler, W. (1921). Zur Psychologie der Schimpansen [On the psychology of chimpanzees]. Abhandlungen der preussischen Akademie der Wissen-

schaften, Berlin, 686-691. Kohler, W. (1922). Zur Psychologie der Schimpansen [On the psychology

of chimpanzees]. Psychologische Forschung, 1, 2-46. Kohler, W. (1957). The mentality of apes (E. Winter, Trans.). London: Pen-

guin Paperback. (Original work published 1925) K6hler, W. (1963). Intelligenzprifungen an Menschenaffen [Intelligence tests

on anthropoids] (3d German ed.). Berlin: Springer. (First published as a book in 1921)

Ley, R. (1990). A whisper of espionage: Wolfgang Kihler and the apes of Tenerife. Garden City, NY: Avery.

Pastore, N. (1990, October). Espionage [Review of A whisper of espionage: Wolfgang Kihler and the apes of Tenerife].Journal of the History of Behavioral Sciences, 26, 366-371.

Pfungst, 0. (1965). Clever Hans, the horse of Mr. von Osten. New York: Holt.

(Original work published in German, 1907) Ploog, D. (1981). Neurobiology of primate audio-vocal behavior. Brain Re-

search Reviews, 3, 35-61; indexed in Brain Research, No. 228. Premack, D. (1971, May 21). Language in chimpanzee? Science, 172, 808-

822. Rothmann, M. (1910). [Application regarding the establishment of a primate

station]. Academy of Sciences, Central Archive, Berlin, Sign: II-VI-127, Leaf 78-82.

Rothmann, M. (1912). Uber die Errichtung einer Station zur psycholo- gischen und hirnphysiologischen Erforschung der Menschenaffen [On the establishment of a station for psychological and brain-physiological research of anthropoids]. Berliner klinische Wochenschrift, No. 42, 1981- 1985.

Rothmann, M., & Teuber, E. (1915). Aus der Anthropoidenstation auf Te- neriffa: Ziele und Aufgaben der Station sowie erste Beobachtungen an den auf ihr gehaltenen Schimpansen [From the anthropoid research station on Tenerife Island: Aims and tasks of the station as well as first observations made on the chimpanzees kept there]. Abhandlungen der

kiiniglich preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften (physikalisch-mathe- matische Klasse), Berlin, No. 2, 1-20.

Schiller, C. H. (Ed. and Trans.). (1957). Instinctive behavior: The development

580 TEUBER

Page 32: The Founding of the Primate Station -M L Teuber.pdf

FOUNDING OF THE PRIMATE STATION

of a modern concept [Essays by J. von Uexkill, K. Lorenz, D. J. Kuenen, P. H. Schiller, N. Tinbergen; introduction by K. S. Lashley]. New York: International Universities Press.

Teuber, E. (1924). Die Kunstphilosophie des Abbe Dubos [The philosophy of art of the Abbe Dubos]. Zeitschriftfulr Asthetik und allgemeine Kunst-

wissenschaft, 17, 361-410. Teuber, H.-L. (1966). Kurt Goldstein's role in the development of neuro-

psychology. Neuropsychologia, 4, 299-310. Teuber, H.-L. (1967). Wolfgang Kohler zum Gedenken [In memory of Wolf-

gang Kohler]. Psychologische Forschung, 31(1), 6-11. Teuber, H.-L. (1978). The brain and human behavior [Invited evening ad-

dress, 21st International Psychological Congress, Paris, 1976]. In H. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, & H.-L. Teuber (Eds.), Handbook of sensory phys- iology: Vol. III. Perception. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Utitz, E. (1932). Geschichte der Asthetik [History of aesthetics]. Berlin: Junker and Diinnhaupt.

Waldeyer-Hartz, W. von. (1917, January 25). Anthropoidenstation auf Te- neriffa [Anthropoid Research Station on Tenerife]. Sitzungsberichte der

k6niglich preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften, 40-42.

Waldeyer-Hartz, W. von. (1919, January 23). Anthropoidenstation auf Te- neriffa [Anthropoid Research Station on Tenerife]. Sitzungsberichte der

preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften, 31-33. Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & Row. (Pub-

lished posthumously) Wundt, W. (1911). Viilkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze

von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte. Vol. 1. Die Sprache [Folk psychology: An

investigation of the developmental laws of language, myth, and custom. Vol. 1. Language] (3d ed.; 1st ed., 1900). Leipzig, Germany: Englemann.

Wundt, W. (1973). The language of gestures. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. (Original work published 1911)

Yerkes, R. M. (1916). The mental life of monkeys and apes: A study of ideational behavior. Behavior Monographs, 3(1).

581