Top Banner
Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance 1 The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance Grant Gordin Sociology 489H Nancy Martin, Ph.D and Oliver Wang, Ph.D April 30, 2012
62

The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Mar 27, 2023

Download

Documents

Mark Chen
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     1  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

The  Correlations  Between  Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance  

 

 

 

 

 

 

Grant  Gordin  

 

 

 

 

Sociology  489H  

Nancy  Martin,  Ph.D  and  Oliver  Wang,  Ph.D  

April  30,  2012  

Page 2: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     2  

Abstract  

In  our  current  society,  we  believe  that  men  and  women  must  behave  in  a  certain  

manner  and  adhere  to  social  expectations  of  masculinity  and  femininity.    If  an  individual  

does  not  properly  perform  his  or  her  gender—male  masculinity  and  female  femininity—he  

or  she  sacrifices  his  or  her  status  as  a  heterosexual,  regardless  of  his  or  her  actual  sexual  

orientation.    This  phenomenon,  known  as  the  conflation  of  gender  and  sexual  orientation,  

refers  to  the  infusing  of  one’s  gender  performance—that  which  is  masculine  or  feminine—

with  sexual  orientation.    By  equating  gender  and  sexuality,  we  can  draw  conclusions  about  

an  individual’s  behavior  based  on  whether  he  or  she  is  homosexual  or  heterosexual;  on  the  

other  hand,  we  also  believe  that  we  can  judge  and  evaluate  an  individual’s  current  or  

eventual  sexual  orientation  based  upon  his  or  her  behavior.      

This  study  investigates  whether  or  not  gay  individuals  engage  in  cross-­‐sex-­‐typed  

behavior  at  a  higher  rate  than  straight  individuals  and  determines  if  sexual  orientation  

correlates  with  gender  performance.    Two  hundred  respondents  completed  a  written  

survey  examining  the  gender  of  a  list  of  forty-­‐one  behaviors,  and  another  two  hundred  

respondents  completed  a  second  written  questionnaire  investigating  how  often  different  

groups  engage  in  the  same  forty-­‐one  behaviors.    After  relating  men  and  women’s  sexual  

orientations  with  frequency  of  behavior,  this  study  found  that  there  was  no  significant  

relationship  between  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  and  his  or  her  performance  of  

gender  in  seventy-­‐two  out  of  eighty-­‐two  of  the  cases  examined.    Of  the  remaining  ten  cases,  

only  three  cases  demonstrated  a  moderate  or  strong  relationship  between  sexuality  and  

behavior.    The  data  confirms  that  sexuality  and  behavior  are  largely  independent  from  one  

another.  

Page 3: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     3  

A  Boy  Caught  Between  Expectations  

At  some  point  in  my  childhood,  I  started  to  realize  that  people  had  very  clear  

expectations  of  me.    My  parents,  my  friends,  and  my  teachers—they  all  had  this  idea  in  

their  head  that  I  was  supposed  to  act  a  certain  way,  as  if  there  was  a  right  and  wrong  way  

to  act  in  elementary  school.    I’m  pretty  sure  that  my  teachers  held  parent-­‐teacher  

conferences  with  my  mom  and  dad  on  more  than  one  occasion  to  discuss  my  “behavior”  in  

class  and  on  the  playground.    You’d  probably  think  I  was  some  rowdy  kid,  getting  myself  

into  trouble  with  the  other  kids,  bullying,  getting  into  fights,  calling  people  names,  or  

something.    Something  that  might  potentially  stand  out  as  problematic  and  worth  a  

telephone  call  home.      

Well,  you’d  be  wrong—not  because  I  didn’t  make  a  really  dumb  decision  once  in  

awhile,  because  I  most  surely  did,  but  because  I  didn’t  act  like  a  boy.    At  least,  that’s  what  

they  told  me.    In  retrospect,  I’m  pretty  sure  that  I  acted  like  a  boy  the  majority  of  the  time.    

After  all,  my  favorite  activities  during  recess  were  basketball  and  handball,  and  I  enjoyed  

roughhousing  with  the  other  kids.    I  watched  Dragonball  Z  religiously,  I  thought  about  

video  games  about  ninety  percent  of  the  time,  and  boy-­‐oh-­‐boy,  did  I  like  girls.    But,  I  also  

put  my  hands  on  my  hips  a  lot,  wore  turtlenecks,  wrote  love  notes,  listened  to  the  

Backstreet  Boys  and  Britney  Spears,  and  identified  a  lot  more  with  the  girls  in  my  

classrooms.    Somehow,  my  teachers—and,  eventually,  my  parents—worried  that  my  

behavior  was  indicative  of  a  “feminine”  boy  on  the  path  toward  homosexuality.      

Ironically,  not  much  has  exactly  changed  today.    During  high  school,  my  father  sat  

me  down  and  asked  me  if  I  was  “having  thoughts  about  my  masculinity.”    And,  all  

throughout  college,  my  mother  told  me  that  I  dressed  and  “looked”  gay.    When  I  broke  up  

Page 4: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     4  

with  one  of  my  ex-­‐girlfriends  during  college,  her  friends  allegedly  asked  her  if  it  was  

because  I  had  finally  come  out  of  the  closet.    Even  now,  my  closest  friends  make  fun  little  

jokes  about  me  being  “gay”  all  the  time  based  on  how  I  act  and  the  activities  in  which  I’m  

involved,  and  newly  met  strangers  mistake  me  for  being  gay  on  a  semi-­‐regular  basis  when  

I’m  without  my  significant  other.  

The  truth  is,  I’ve  always  known  that  I  had  plenty  of  feminine  tendencies  and  

characteristics  just  as  I  have  a  whole  assortment  of  masculine  ones.    But,  I  never  truly  

realized  how  strongly  people  felt  that  I  was  in  the  closet  based  on  my  everyday  behavior.    

And,  ultimately,  this  just  doesn’t  really  make  any  sense  to  me.    Activities,  appearance,  and  

habits—these  things  don’t  really  have  anything  to  do  with  what  genitalia  and  sex  I  prefer.    

Does  my  singing  in  choir  or  wearing  pink  clothing  somehow  lead  others  to  predict  that  I’m  

homosexual?    Can  you  tell  me  if  someone  is  gay  or  straight  simply  based  on  how  they  act?  

There’s  no  way  that  I’m  the  only  kid  who  grew  up  wondering  these  same  things.    For  

all  of  the  girls  who  grew  up  playing  sports  and  punching  their  teachers  in  the  gut  and  for  all  

of  the  boys  who  spent  their  childhood  in  their  sister’s  room  applying  makeup  and  dancing  

to  the  Spice  Girls,  I  dedicate  this  research  to  you.  

Exploring  Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance—the  Research  Question  

Does  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  correlate  with  his  or  her  gender  

performance?    After  all,  society’s  beliefs  and  attitudes  toward  sexual  orientation  extend  far  

beyond  sexual  attraction  and  desire.    At  the  most  basic  level,  sexual  orientation  is  nothing  

more  than  a  means  to  categorize  sexual  attraction  based  upon  the  biological  sex  of  the  

desirer  and  the  desired.    We  label  opposite-­‐sex  attraction—that  is,  male-­‐toward-­‐female  and  

female-­‐toward-­‐male  attraction—as  heterosexuality,  while  simultaneously  categorizing  

Page 5: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     5  

same-­‐sex  attraction  as  homosexuality.    Finally,  the  term  bisexuality  serves  to  categorize  

those  who  desire  both  the  same  and  the  opposite  sex  of  themselves.  

Beyond  these  fundamental  definitions,  however,  we  associate  sexual  orientation  

with  a  long  and  extensive  list  of  other  characteristics—things  like  one’s  personal  

appearance,  preferred  activities,  favorite  colors,  likes  and  dislikes,  and  hobbies.    Society  

teaches  us  that  there  are  behaviors  that  we  can  expect  to  observe  within  each  group  when  

paired  with  biological  sex.    A  heterosexual  man,  for  example,  should  behave  quite  

differently  than  a  homosexual  man,  just  as  a  heterosexual  woman  should  conduct  herself  

differently  than  a  homosexual  woman.    What,  then,  are  the  behaviors  that  we  associate  

with  straight  and  gay  men  and  women,  and  how  do  we  examine  how  individual  people  

conduct  themselves  based  on  the  dichotomy  of  gender?    These  are  the  questions  that  I  wish  

to  examine  in  the  existing  literature  on  the  subject  of  sexuality  and  gender,  as  well  as  in  my  

own  research  and  study.  

Defining  Gender  Performance  

Before  we  can  analyze  one’s  behavior  as  a  separate  notion  from  sexual  orientation  

or  biological  sex,  we  need  a  way  to  determine  how  groups  of  people  behave  and  conduct  

themselves  differently.    Common  among  both  the  sociological  and  psychological  schools  of  

thought,  the  term  “gender  performance,”  sometimes  called  “gender  role,”  refers  to  “all  

those  things  that  a  person  says  or  does  to  disclose  himself  or  herself  as  having  the  status  of  

boy  or  man,  girl  or  woman,  respectively”  (Zucker  32).    In  other  words,  an  individual’s  

gender  performance  is  essentially  his  or  her  series  of  behaviors  that  imply  his  or  her  

gender.    Certain  behaviors  suggest  the  status  of  a  man,  while  other  actions  and  manners  

demonstrate  the  status  of  a  woman.      

Page 6: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     6  

In  “Reflections  On  the  Relation  Between  Sex-­‐Typed  Behavior  in  Childhood  and  

Sexual  Orientation  in  Adulthood,”  Kenneth  J.  Zucker  indicates  that  “the  term  gender  role  has  

been  used  extensively  by  developmental  psychologists  to  refer  to  behaviors,  attitudes,  and  

personality  traits  that  a  society,  in  a  given  culture  and  historical  period,  designates  as  

masculine  or  feminine”  (Zucker  33).    Gender  performance  or  role,  consequently,  is  not  an  

implication  of  biological  sex—male  or  female—but  rather  an  analysis  of  the  social  meaning  

of  acting  like  a  male  or  female.    To  act  as  a  man  is  to  perform  masculinity;  conversely,  to  act  

as  a  woman  is  to  perform  femininity.    Therefore,  as  a  result,  a  male  can  perform  a  “female”  

gender  by  engaging  in  activities  and  behaviors  that  his  given  society  deems  as  feminine,  or  

vice  versa.    Sheila  Koenig  identifies  the  ability  to  perform  one’s  gender  in  “Walk  Like  a  Man:  

Enactments  and  Embodiments  of  Masculinity  and  the  Potential  for  Multiple  Genders,”  

where  she  explains  that  “…  all  gender  is  a  performance,  designed  to  reinforce  the  categories  

of  ‘man’  and  ‘woman’  as  dictated  by  heteronormative,  patriarchal  culture”  (Koenig  193).    

So,  according  to  Koenig,  if  the  implications  of  what  it  means  to  be  a  man  or  a  woman  are  

subject  to  the  fluid  notion  of  culture,  then  gender  cannot  be  absolute.    Such  is  the  findings  

of  lesbian  writers  Esther  Newton  and  Shirley  Walton,  who  declared  that  “gender  categories  

are  learned  by  all,  and  are  ‘natural’  to  none”  in  the  article  “The  Misunderstanding:  Toward  a  

More  Precise  Sexual  Vocabulary”  (Newton  &  Walton  170).    The  concepts  of  masculinity  and  

femininity  are  not  determined  by  biology;  instead,  they  exist  within  our  social  structure,  

susceptible  to  eternal  change  and  redefinition  as  society  evolves  over  time.    What  is  

masculine  for  one  generation  might  be  feminine  in  the  next.    If  this  is  true,  as  Koenig,  

Newton,  and  Walton  believe,  then  gender  performance  does  not  accurately  predict  

biological  sex,  much  less  sexual  orientation.  

Page 7: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     7  

What  is  Masculine?    What  is  Feminine?  

As  previously  stated,  there  is  no  absolute  masculine  activity  or  feminine  behavior.    

That’s  not,  of  course,  to  say  that  we  don’t  have  a  current  sociological  expectation  of  what  it  

means  to  be  masculine  or  feminine.  

At  the  very  general  level,  Tomas  Almaguer  indentifies  a  vital  component  of  

masculinity  and  femininity  as  it  pertains  to  sexuality.    In  his  article  “Chicano  Men:  A  

Cartography  of  Homosexual  Identity  and  Behavior,”  Almaguer  states,  “’To  give’  is  to  be  

masculine,  [and]  ‘to  receive’  is  to  be  feminine”  (Almaguer  476).    In  the  context  of  his  study,  

he  was  referring  to  the  dynamic  between  two  males  during  an  act  of  anal  sex.    The  

penetrator,  or  “giver,”  in  this  instance,  exuded  masculinity,  while  the  penetrated  individual  

in  the  scene,  or  “receiver,”  displayed  femininity.    His  analysis  of  the  implications  of  

penetrating  or  being  penetrated,  however,  extends  to  the  heterosexual  scene,  as  well.    After  

all,  the  idea  of  “fucking”  demonstrates  power  and  masculinity;  simultaneously,  “being  

fucked”—or,  “receiving,”  as  Almaguer  articulates,  demonstrates  femininity.    In  their  article,  

Esther  Newton  and  Shirley  Walton  affirm  this  binary,  adding  that  “masculinity  equals  

sexual  power,  [while]  femininity  equals  sexual  powerlessness”  (Newton  &  Walton  172).    

Consequently,  when  it  comes  to  sexual  intercourse  of  any  kind,  to  penetrate  is  to  be  

powerful  and  masculine.  

C.  J.  Pascoe  employs  another  strategy  in  determining  gender  performance  by  

examining  that  which  is  not  masculine:  “being  stupid  or  incompetent,  dancing,  caring  too  

much  about  clothing,  being  too  emotional,  or  expressing  interest  (sexual  or  platonic)  in  

other  guys”  (Pascoe  432).    Pascoe’s  book  Dude,  You’re  a  Fag:  Masculinity  and  Sexuality  in  

High  School  identifies  these  behaviors  based  on  the  difference  between  masculine  and  

Page 8: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     8  

“unmasculine,”  highlighting  a  uniquely  important  component  of  gender.    To  evaluate  

gender  based  upon  that  which  is  or  isn’t  masculine  (rather  than  to  examine  what  is  

masculine  and  what  is  feminine)  groups  gender  into  categories  of  what  I  will  call  “in-­‐the-­‐

bubble”  or  “outside-­‐the-­‐bubble.”    In  the  case  of  examining  masculinity  and  femininity  as  

two  ends  of  a  spectrum—or  even  as  a  binary  of  categorization  absent  of  any  middle  

ground—each  group  is  “in-­‐the-­‐bubble.”    There  are  merely  two  separate  bubbles  that  serve  

to  classify  masculinity  and  femininity,  respectively.    Conversely,  when  grouped  as  that  

which  is  masculine  and  unmasculine,  Pascoe  exhibits  how  we  effectively  privilege  

masculinity  over  femininity  and  suggest  that  we  should  all  aspire  to  act  like  men.    Those  

who  fail  to  act  like  men  fall  “outside-­‐the-­‐bubble,”  where  their  gender  is  failed  masculinity  

rather  than  femininity.  

Additionally,  the  realm  of  sports  and  athletics  is  a  key  breeding  ground  for  the  

reproduction  of  both  masculinity  and  femininity.    As  for  masculinity,  in  the  article  “Openly  

Gay  Athletes:  Contesting  Hegemonic  Masculinity  in  a  Homophobic  Environment,”  Eric  

Anderson  remarks,  “Sports  (particularly  contact  sports)  have  been  described  as  a  place  in  

which  hegemonic  masculinity  is  reproduced  and  defined,  as  an  athlete  represents  the  ideal  

of  what  it  means  to  be  a  man”  (Anderson  860).    He  goes  on  to  explain  that  “sport  [serves]  as  

a  test  of  masculinity  in  Western  societies  …  [and]  athletes  are  often  the  unofficial  rule  

enforcers  of  hegemonic  masculinity,”  furthering  the  idea  that  sports  serve  as  a  place  where  

men  can  properly  learn  how  to  act  like  men  (Anderson  862,  869).    Through  Anderson’s  

definitions,  to  play  a  sport,  especially  contact  sports  like  football,  hockey,  basketball,  and  so  

forth,  is  to  engage  in  a  masculine  identity.    Susan  Cahn  adds  that  “in  the  early  decades  of  the  

twentieth  century  …  sport  functioned  as  a  male  preserve  …  [where  men]  demonstrated  and  

Page 9: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     9  

affirmed  their  manhood”  in  her  work  “From  the  ‘Muscle  Moll’  to  the  ‘Butch’  Ballplayer:  

Mannishness,  Lesbianism,  and  Homophobia  in  U.S.  Women’s  Sports.”    Both  authors  point  to  

sport  as  a  performance,  where  the  field  or  court  literally  operates  as  a  stage  for  the  players  

to  display  their  masculinity  (Cahn  335).    Now,  as  sport  is  a  contest,  the  game  often  becomes  

less  a  game  of  football  or  basketball  and  more  a  competition  of  “who  is  the  most  masculine  

on  the  field?”    So,  what  behavior  on  the  field,  then,  is  truly  masculine?    To  answer,  Cahn  

labels  “aggression,  physicality,  competitive  spirit,  and  athletic  skill  as  masculine  attributes  

necessary  for  true  manliness”  (Cahn  335).  

On  the  flip  side,  Susan  Cahn  also  comments  on  women  in  the  field  of  athletics  and  

sports  based  on  early  twentieth  century  critical  view  of  female  athleticism,  which  stated  

that  “woman  athletes  would  become  manlike,  adopting  masculine  dress,  talk,  and  

mannerisms  …  [collapsing]  into  an  all-­‐encompassing  concept  of  ‘mannishness,’  a  term  

signifying  female  masculinity”  (Cahn  335).    On  one  hand,  the  existence  of  mannish  women  

aids  the  idea  that  biological  sex  and  gender  performance  are  uniquely  separate  from  one  

another.    However,  critics  of  female  athletics  in  the  1930s  argued  that  mannishness,  this  

concept  of  a  woman  adopting  a  masculine  identity,  was  unbecoming  of  a  woman.    An  

American  Mercury  medical  reporter,  for  instance,  claimed  that  women  who  enter  sport  “act  

like  men,  talk  like  men,  and  think  like  men  …  [and  surrender]  their  sex”  (Cahn  337,  339).    

The  notion  of  surrender  one’s  sex  is  vital  to  Cahn’s  critique—after  all,  a  woman  acting,  

talking,  and  thinking  “like  a  man”  only  speaks  of  her  gender  performance  as  masculine,  not  

of  her  biological  sex  as  a  woman.    But,  the  idea  that  a  woman  surrenders  being  a  woman  by  

engaging  in  athletics  and  sports  suggests  a  link  between  our  actions  and  our  biological  sex  

that  Cahn  believes  does  not  exist.  

Page 10: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     10  

Moreover,  gender  performance  does  not  simply  refer  to  an  individual’s  activities  

and  behaviors,  but  his  or  her  appearance,  as  well—especially  choice  of  clothing.    In  her  

book,  C.  J.  Pascoe  found  that  “according  to  boys  at  River  [High  School],  fags  cared  about  the  

style  of  their  clothes,  wore  tighter  clothes,  and  cared  about  cleanliness  …  similarly,  nonfags  

were  not  supposed  to  care  about  dirtying  their  clothes”  (Pascoe  434).    Pascoe’s  research  

suggests  that  even  caring  about  your  appearance  or  clothing—something  that  isn’t  a  

behavior  or  action,  but  simply  a  manner  of  thought—lends  itself  to  femininity.    After  all,  

“’real’  guys  [don’t]  care  about  their  appearance”  (Pascoe  435).    Perhaps  the  singularly  most  

ironic  facet  of  any  of  the  reviewed  literature,  however,  is  the  explanation  that  “to  not  care  

about  one’s  clothes,  or  to  make  fun  of  those  who  care  about  their  clothes,  ironically,  is  to  

also  care  about  one’s  appearance  …  in  this  sense,  masculinity  [is]  the  carefully  crafted  

appearance  of  not  caring  about  appearance”  (Pascoe  435).    In  other  words,  an  individual  is  

masculine  as  long  as  he  doesn’t  seem  to  care  about  his  appearance.    However,  should  he  

spend  too  long  gazing  into  the  mirror,  applying  make-­‐up,  trying  on  clothes,  cleaning  oil  

stains,  and  so  forth,  he  ceases  to  be  a  “real  guy”  and  becomes  something  less  than  a  man,  

embracing  something  “beneath”  masculinity.  

By  making  sense  of  what  behaviors  we  perceive  to  be  masculine  or  feminine,  this  

study  can  better  understand  the  individual  components  that  make  up  the  social  roles  of  

male  and  female—and,  as  an  extension,  the  expectations  of  a  heteronormative  society.      

Defining  the  Conflation  of  Gender  and  Sexual  Orientation  

The  conflation  of  gender  and  sexual  orientation  refers  to  the  infusing  of  one’s  

gender  performance—that  which  is  masculine  or  feminine—with  sexual  orientation.    By  

equating  gender  and  sexuality,  we  can  draw  conclusions  about  one  side  of  the  coin  by  

Page 11: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     11  

analyzing  the  other.    For  instance,  in  modern  society,  we  believe  that  we  can  somehow  

predict  an  individual’s  behavior  if  we  know  whether  he  or  she  is  homosexual  or  

heterosexual;  on  the  other  hand,  we  also  believe  that  we  can  judge  and  evaluate  an  

individual’s  current  or  eventual  sexual  orientation  based  upon  his  or  her  behavior.    If  these  

relationships  exist,  gender  performance  and  sexual  orientation  are  as  interconnected  as  

they  possible  could  be.    However,  if  these  relationships  and  supposed  connections  prove  to  

be  false,  our  unique  behaviors  and  actions  as  individuals  should  be  free  of  sexual  

connotation,  just  as  our  sexual  orientations  should  be  free  of  assumed  gender.  

One  important  study  that  examines  the  potential  link  between  sexual  orientation  

and  gender  is  Kenneth  Zucker’s  Reflections  On  the  Relation  Between  Sex-­Typed  Behavior  in  

Childhood  and  Sexual  Orientation  in  Adulthood,  which  found  that  “childhood  gender  

nonconformity  [is]  the  strongest  predictor  of  [adult  homosexuality]  in  men  and  the  second  

strongest  predictor  of  [adult  homosexuality]  in  women,  …  surpassed  only  be  homosexual  

involvements  in  adolescence”  (Zucker  36).    In  his  study,  Zucker  deduced  that  children  who  

engaged  in  cross-­‐sex-­‐typed  behavior—that  is,  determined  masculine  behavior  in  girls  or  

determined  feminine  behavior  in  boys—were  far  more  likely  to  be  homosexual  in  

adulthood.    His  findings  suggest  that  adolescent  behavior  (when  analyzed  through  

gendered  lenses)  does,  indeed,  serve  to  predict  eventual  sexual  orientation.      

Popular  assumption  and  opinion  in  the  United  States  certainly  appears  to  agree  with  

Zucker’s  findings.    In  his  article,  Eric  Anderson  states  that  “the  mandates  of  masculinity  …  

stand  in  contrast  to  homosexuality,”  indicating  that  there  is  an  absolute  perceived  

association  between  gender  and  sexuality  (Anderson  863).    Furthermore,  C.  J.  Pascoe  

affirms,  “to  be  a  fag  [is],  by  definition,  the  opposite  of  masculine,  whether  the  word  [is]  

Page 12: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     12  

deployed  with  sexualized  or  nonsexualized  meaning”  (Pascoe  433).    According  to  both  

Anderson  and  Pascoe,  society  views  masculinity  and  homosexuality  in  men  as  constructs  

that  cannot  coexist  based  on  the  inherent  definitions  of  the  words,  alone.    Consequently,  a  

man  who  is  not  masculine  must  be  homosexual.    Esther  Newton  and  Shirley  Walton  extend  

these  assumptions  to  the  realm  of  women,  indicating  that  “a  heterosexual  woman  [who  

perceives]  her  erotic  identity  as  being  somehow  ‘unfeminine,’  for  example,  domineering,  

clumsy,  or  fat,  …  [is]  almost  by  cultural  definition,  supposed  to  be  a  lesbian”  (Newton  &  

Walton  169-­‐170).    In  a  similar  way,  the  constructs  of  femininity  and  homosexuality  in  

women  cannot  coexist,  either.    A  woman  is  either  feminine  and  straight  or  masculine  and  

lesbian—there  is  little  room  in  society  for  a  third  alternative.  

Once  again,  the  realm  of  sports  and  athletics  plays  a  vital  role  in  the  understanding  

of  the  conflation  of  gender  and  sexual  orientation.    Eric  Anderson  finds  that  “women’s  

athleticism  in  itself  is  a  contradiction  to  femininity,  so  female  athletes  are  frequently  

assumed  to  be  lesbians,”  and  he  goes  on  to  establish  that  “in  the  narrow  field  of  sport,  

where  heterosexuality  is  compulsory  and  homosexuality  is  taboo,  effeminacy  and  gayness  

are  essentially  considered  the  same”  (Anderson  866,  873).    Once  again,  society  creates  a  

paradigm  where  an  individual’s  behavior  is  culturally  defined  by  his  or  her  biological  sex  

and  sexual  orientation.    We  do  not  perceive  his  or  her  gender  performance  as  a  third  

category,  but  rather  as  an  extension  for  understanding  and  determining  his  or  her  sexual  

orientation.    Straight  women  should  be  feminine—if  they  aren’t,  they  must  be  gay.    The  

same  assumptions  go  for  every  combination.    Straight  men  should  be  masculine,  gay  men  

should  be  feminine,  and  gay  women  should  be  masculine.    Essentially,  we  assume  one’s  

“inversion”  of  sexual  orientation  in  a  heteronormative  society  to  imply  his  or  her  

Page 13: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     13  

“inversion”  of  gender  performance,  as  well.    So,  if  a  man  is  involved  in  a  contact  sport,  a  

breeding  ground  for  masculinity,  he  must  be  straight.    Likewise,  if  a  woman  is  involved  in  

athletics,  she  must  be  gay.    As  Susan  Cahn  puts  it,  “The  image  of  women  athletes  as  

mannish,  failed  heterosexuals  represents  a  thinly  veiled  reference  to  lesbianism  in  sport”  

(Cahn  334).  

Problematic  Implications  of  the  Conflation  of  Gender  and  Sexual  Orientation  

So,  the  underlying  question—so  what?    So  what  if  we  assume  somebody  is  

homosexual  based  on  his  or  her  behavior?    It’s  actually  a  good  question  worth  review,  as  

there  are  plenty  of  unexplored  conflicts  that  arise  from  associating  behavior  with  sexual  

orientation.  

For  one  thing,  an  individual’s  fear  of  being  labeled  as  homosexual  causes  both  

straight  and  closeted  gay  men  and  women,  alike,  to  overcompensate  for  their  “correct”  

gender.    C.  J.  Pascoe,  as  an  example,  found  that  “boys  reminded  themselves  and  each  other  

that  at  any  moment  they  could  become  fags  if  they  were  not  sufficiently  masculine”  (Pascoe  

434).    Pascoe’s  study,  as  previously  mentioned,  took  place  at  a  high  school,  a  place  where  

individuals  transition  from  children  to  adults  and  begin  to  truly  form  their  identities  as  

human  beings.    When  children  must  pay  closer  attention  to  how  they  properly  “fit  in”  to  a  

system  of  gender  expectation  than  to  the  genuine  relationships  that  they  form  with  their  

peers,  how  can  we  expect  men  and  women  to  learn  intimacy  at  all?    Men,  in  particular,  

cannot  bond  with  one  another  out  of  fear  of  the  homosexual  label  that  surrounds  them.    

Any  sign  of  “weakness”—ironically  usually  likened  to  femininity—and  a  man  abandons  his  

manhood.  

Page 14: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     14  

Secondly,  women  who  wish  to  break  out  of  heterosexual  and  feminine  social  norms  

must  do  so  at  the  risk  of  their  sexual  orientation  and  eligibility  as  true  women.    Susan  Cahn  

expands  on  the  lesbian  stereotype  in  athletics  and  how  it  “[exerts]  pressure  on  athletes  to  

demonstrate  their  femininity  and  heterosexuality,  viewed  as  one  and  the  same”  (Cahn  340).    

Ultimately,  though,  Cahn’s  research  has  implications  that  extend  far  beyond  the  scope  of  

athletics—it  demonstrates  that  a  woman’s  desire  to  step  “outside  of  her  place”  and  to  do  

anything  unbecoming  of  a  feminine  woman  is  social  and  romantic  suicide.  

Furthermore,  the  conflation  of  gender  and  sexual  orientation  actually  discourages  

individuals  from  coming  out  of  the  closet.    Before  a  man  or  woman  comes  out  as  

homosexual  (or  bisexual),  he  or  she  is  a  whole  human  being,  complete  with  characteristics,  

quirks,  flaws,  strengths,  and  weaknesses.    After  the  coming  out  process,  he  or  she  is  simply  

“gay.”    The  out  individual  loses  almost  all  of  the  other  components  of  his  or  her  identity  and  

degrades  into  a  one-­‐dimensional  shell  of  a  human  being  in  the  eyes  of  society.    And,  why?    

Simply  because  we  as  a  society  believe  that  we  know  everything  about  a  person  and  can  

predict  everything  else  there  is  to  know  about  him  or  her  simply  based  on  his  or  her  sexual  

orientation.    Why  get  to  know  somebody  if  you  can  supposedly  predict  how  he  or  she  

behaves,  what  he  or  she  likes  and  dislikes,  and  how  he  or  she  thinks?    In  society’s  eyes,  

homosexuality  replaces  one’s  personality  rather  than  adding  to  it.  

Sex  I  Desire  vs.  Sexual  Orientation  

For  the  purposes  of  my  research  in  “The  Correlations  of  Sexual  Orientation  and  

Gender  Performance,”  it  was  vital  that  I  collected  data  based  upon  what  biological  sex  an  

individual  sexually  desired  rather  than  his  or  her  “sexual  orientation.”    There  are  numerous  

reasons  as  to  why  individuals  are  more  likely  to  report  their  desired  sex  than  to  report  

Page 15: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     15  

their  sexual  orientation,  reasons  grounded  in  culture,  self-­‐awareness,  and  fear.    I  will  refer  

to  this  phenomenon  as  the  “Sex  I  Desire  vs.  Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  for  the  purposes  of  

my  research.  

Firstly,  an  individual’s  culture  plays  a  significant  role  in  his  or  her  formation  or  

disclosure  of  identity—especially  in  regards  to  sexual  orientation.    In  Tomas  Almaguer’s  

study  of  Chicano  men,  he  found  that  “only  a  segment  of  homosexually  active  Chicano  men  

identify  as  ‘gay,’  …  [and  that]  none  of  the  active  inserter  participants  in  homosexual  

encounters  [in  his  study]  ever  [considered]  himself  a  ‘homosexual’  or  to  be  ‘gay’”  

(Almaguer  473,  477).    For  many  Chicano  men,  homosexuality  is  based  upon  the  role  one  

plays  in  a  homosexual  act,  not  based  upon  actual  sexual  attraction  at  all.    The  male  

penetrator  preserves  his  heterosexuality  and  status  as  a  “normal  …  male,”  even  while  

engaging  in  anal  sex  with  another  man  (Almaguer  475).    However,  the  penetrated  man  

loses  his  manhood  and  male  status—and,  subsequently,  his  heterosexuality.    As  such,  

“inserter”  men  such  as  these  would  not  actively  identify  as  homosexual,  but  could  still  

identify  sexual  desire  toward  other  men.    Moreover,  John  D’Emilio’s  “Capitalism  and  Gay  

Identity”  states  that  “in  the  seventeenth  century  …  there  was,  quite  simply,  no  ‘social  space’  

in  the  colonial  system  of  production  that  allowed  men  and  women  to  be  gay  …  there  were  

certain  homosexual  acts—sodomy  among  men,  ‘lewdness’  among  women—in  which  

individuals  engaged,  but  family  was  so  pervasive  that  colonial  society  lacked  even  the  

category  of  homosexual  or  lesbian  to  describe  a  person”  (D’Emilio  470).    For  cultures  

where  there  is  no  social  space  to  identify  as  homosexual—but  still  plenty  of  same-­‐sex  

desire—asking  about  sexual  desire,  itself,  is  much  more  effective  than  asking  about  sexual  

identity  or  sexual  orientation.  

Page 16: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     16  

Another  component  of  the  “Sex  I  Desire  vs.  Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  is  the  

concept  of  self-­‐awareness—an  individual’s  mere  knowledge  of  his  or  her  sexual  

orientation,  in  the  first  place.    In  her  article  “In  the  Trenches:  LGBT  Students  Struggle  With  

School  and  Sexual  Identity,”  author  Melinda  Miceli  explains  that  “the  literature  on  

developmental  models  suggests  that  individuals  became  consciously  aware  of  their  

homosexual  identities  at  an  average  age  range  of  19-­‐21  for  males  and  21-­‐23  for  females  …  

[and]  more  current  research  on  gay  and  lesbian  youth  suggests  …  [that]  the  range  is  now  

15-­‐18  for  males  and  17-­‐20  for  females”  (Miceli  186).    In  many  cases,  especially  around  

collegiate  age  of  late  teens  and  early  twenties  (where  I  will  be  conducting  my  research),  

individuals  may  not  even  be  fully  aware  of  their  sexual  orientation.    However,  they  may  be  

somewhat  aware  of  sexual  desire  that  they  have  had  for  individuals  of  the  opposite  or  the  

same  sex  as  themselves.    Once  again,  my  research  will  account  for  this  possibility.  

Finally,  the  last  and  perhaps  most  important  component  of  the  “Sex  I  Desire  vs.  

Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  is  the  fear  of  labels  that  we  have  developed  as  a  backlash  

against  modern  social  desire  to  label  just  about  everything  it  can.    It’s  a  very  logical  

reservation  to  have,  after  all—why  would  you  define  as  something  that  comes  with  a  

negative  connotation  or  stigma?    Miceli  elaborates  on  this  fear  of  labeling,  stating  that  

“same-­‐sex  feelings  [make  you]  a  specific  type  of  person,  which  socially  [marks]  you  as  

someone  who  would  be  the  target  of  ostracism”  (Miceli  187).    My  research  certainly  does  

not  wish  to  force  individuals  to  identify  at  the  risk  of  social  status,  and  so  it  strays  away  

from  the  term  “sexual  orientation”  in  the  surveys,  altogether.    Additionally,  for  those  who  

would  define  as  bisexual,  Beverly  Yuen  Thompson  describes  her  own  experience  claiming  a  

bisexual  identity  in  “Fence  Sitters,  Switch  Hitters,  and  Bi-­‐Bi  Girls:  An  Exploration  of  Hapa  

Page 17: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     17  

and  Bisexual  Identities”:  “…  Others  would  analyze  me  through  their  monosexual  template  

of  understanding  …  [and  demand]  to  know,  ‘Which  do  you  really  like  better,  boys  or  girls?’”  

(Thompson  174).    Once  again,  I  do  not  wish  to  alienate  any  population  of  people  by  

requiring  a  self-­‐reported  sexual  identity.    I  only  wish  to  ask  about  and  measure  sexual  

attraction—a  fleeting  idea,  a  thing  removed  from  identity  or  formation  of  character—

rather  than  about  orientation—an  intrinsic  quality  perhaps  a  bit  too  close  to  home.  

The  Research  Design  and  Hypothesis  

This  research  ultimately  investigates  if  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  is  related  

to  his  or  her  overall  performance  of  gender.    In  order  to  conduct  such  a  study,  however,  one  

must  establish  the  kinds  of  behaviors  that  individuals  associated  with  heterosexuality  and  

homosexuality.    Rather  than  assume  that  all  Americans  share  a  common  idea  of  what  it  

means  to  “act”  gay  or  straight,  this  study  further  investigated  the  link  between  gender  

performance  and  sexual  orientation—straight  male  masculinity,  straight  female  femininity,  

gay  male  femininity,  and  gay  female  masculinity.    While  most  prior  research  has  shown  that  

biological  sex,  gender,  and  sexual  orientation  are  all  uniquely  separate  attributes  that  do  

not  individually  predict  or  determine  one  another,  this  study  will  examine  how  most  

Americans  still  perceive  an  inherent  relationship  between  straight  males  and  masculinity,  

straight  females  and  femininity,  gay  males  and  femininity,  and  gay  females  and  masculinity.      

Additionally,  I  hypothesized  that  there  would  be  no  relationship  between  an  

individual’s  sexual  orientation  and  the  way  in  which  he  or  she  performed  his  or  her  gender.    

I  expected  that  behaviors  that  respondents  labeled  as  feminine  would  be  just  as  prominent  

among  straight  men  as  they  were  among  gay  men,  and  I  expected  that  behaviors  

Page 18: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     18  

respondents  labeled  as  masculine  would  be  just  as  commonly  observed  among  straight  

women  as  they  were  among  gay  women.  

Overview  and  Justification  for  the  Design  

My  research  consisted  of  two  separate  surveys.    The  first  survey’s  purpose  was  to  

establish  how  people  view  different  sorts  of  behaviors  and  actions  through  the  lenses  of  

gender.    From  this  initial  survey,  I  was  able  to  sufficiently  divide  and  categorize  certain  

behaviors  as  either  more  masculine  or  more  feminine  based  on  the  average  response  

rather  than  solely  based  on  existing  literature  or  popular  stereotype.    Subsequently,  the  

second  survey  consisted  of  the  same  list  of  behaviors—instead,  however,  it  asked  the  

respondent  if  he  or  she  individually  engaged  in  the  behaviors  rather  than  simply  how  he  or  

she  viewed  them.    Most  importantly,  both  surveys  asked  that  respondents  self-­‐report  

sexual  orientation  and  gender.    For  sexual  orientation,  respondents  used  a  seven-­‐point  

Kinsey  Scale  absent  of  the  term  “sexual  orientation,”  at  all,  which  offered  him  or  her  a  much  

wider  range  of  sexual  identity  than  merely  the  ternary  of  heterosexuality,  bisexuality,  and  

homosexuality.    Meanwhile,  respondents  self-­‐reported  gender  on  a  five-­‐point  scale  that  

paralleled  the  scale  they  used  to  identify  the  gender  of  a  certain  behavior  on  the  first  

survey.    By  analyzing  the  results  of  both  surveys,  I  could  determine  if  there  was  any  

substantial  variation  between  the  kinds  of  behaviors  that  gays  and  straights  exhibit—as  

well  as  the  perceived  “gender”  of  the  behaviors,  themselves.    

Additionally,  the  first  survey  allowed  me  to  eliminate  the  obstacle  of  “choosing”  the  

gender  for  individual  behaviors,  as  it  had  the  respondents  determine  the  masculinity  or  

femininity  of  the  behaviors  for  me.    This  way,  my  research  could  easily  justify  what  it  

Page 19: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     19  

deemed  to  be  masculine  or  feminine—and,  at  the  same  time,  build  a  strong  foundation  for  

its  second  survey.  

This  study  investigated  college  students  across  the  California  State  University,  Long  

Beach  (CSULB)  campus  by  distributing  the  two  surveys  in  large  classrooms,  campus  

restaurants  and  cafeterias,  the  campus  library,  and  other  study  and  lounge  areas  outside  

various  departments  and  disciplines.    No  one  person  completed  both  surveys,  however—

this  way,  the  first  survey  did  not  influence  the  respondents’  answers  on  the  second  survey  

or  vice  versa.    

Subjects  and  Population  of  Study  

The  unit  of  analysis  for  “The  Correlations  of  Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  

Performance”  was  people,  and  my  population  of  interest  was  all  college  students.    I  used  a  

convenience  sample  by  distributing  both  surveys  to  all  sorts  of  classrooms  at  the  

university—large  classes  of  all  division  levels  and  departments.    Just  as  no  one  individual  

completed  both  surveys,  no  one  classroom  completed  the  same  survey.    As  I  administered  

the  two  surveys,  I  alternated  between  them  so  that  every  other  person  completed  a  

different  survey.    I  strived  for  a  combined  sample  size  of  about  four  hundred,  with  two  

hundred  of  the  respondents  completed  each  survey,  and  I  received  a  total  of  386  usable  

surveys  in  all.  

The  Methodology  

As  previously  indicated,  my  research  used  the  survey  method  to  collect  data.    Both  

of  the  unique  surveys  were  written,  and  I  administered  and  collected  them  anonymously.    

In  addition  to  general  demographics,  the  first  survey  listed  a  series  of  behaviors  and  

asked  respondents  to  label  each  one  as  either  more  masculine  or  more  feminine  based  on  a  

Page 20: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     20  

five-­‐point  scale.    A  “1”  represented  an  answer  of  “very  masculine,”  while  a  “5”  indicated  a  

response  of  “very  feminine.”    A  “3”  designated  the  middle  of  the  spectrum,  indicating  a  

response  of  “neutral,”  leaving  a  “2”  and  a  “4”  to  signify  “slightly  more  masculine”  and  

“slightly  more  feminine,”  respectfully.    Each  respondent  circled  one  of  the  numbers  to  

indicate  how  masculine  or  feminine  he  or  she  individually  perceived  the  behavior  to  be—

not  to  indicate  how  masculine  or  feminine  he  or  she  thought  society  perceived  the  behavior  

to  be.    The  survey  made  this  distinction  exceedingly  clear.  

The  following  is  an  example  of  a  behavior  from  the  first  survey:  

Playing  basketball—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5    Very  Masculine                Slightly  More                    Neutral                          Slightly  More                Very  Feminine                            Masculine                                        Feminine  

 Meanwhile,  the  second  survey  listed  the  same  series  of  demographics  and  

behaviors.    However,  instead  of  asking  respondents  to  interpret  the  gender  of  the  

behaviors,  the  survey  asked  if  the  respondent  individually  engaged  in  them.    The  second  

survey  did  not  allude  to  nor  inquire  about  the  perceived  masculinity  or  femininity  of  each  

behavior.    There  were  four  possible  responses  for  the  second  survey:  “often,”  “sometimes,”  

“rarely,”  and  “never.”  

The  following  is  a  corresponding  example  of  a  behavior  from  the  second  survey:  

Do  you  play  basketball?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

 Both  surveys  also  inquired  about  the  respondent’s  sexual  orientation  using  a  

modified  seven-­‐point  Kinsey  Scale  rather  than  asking  him  or  her  to  identify  him  or  herself  

as  strictly  heterosexual,  bisexual,  or  homosexual.    Furthermore,  instead  of  using  the  self-­‐

identifying  terms  “heterosexual,”  “bisexual,”  and  “homosexual”  to  describe  the  possible  

Page 21: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     21  

answers,  I  merely  asked  respondents  to  indicate  the  biological  sex  to  which  they  were  

sexually  attracted.    For  example,  a  response  of  “0”  represented  “only  the  opposite  sex,”  and  

an  answer  of  “6”  signified  “only  the  same  sex.”    The  rest  of  the  numbers  signified  as  follows:  

a  “1”  equaled  “mostly  the  opposite  sex,”  a  “2”  equaled  “slightly  more  the  opposite  sex,”  a  “3”  

equaled  “both  sexes  equally,”  a  “4”  equaled  “slightly  more  the  same  sex,”  and  a  “5”  equaled  

“mostly  the  same  sex.”    Each  respondent  circled  the  most  appropriate  number.    By  asking  

about  sexual  desire  rather  sexual  orientation,  explicitly,  I  hoped  to  avoid  the  “Sex  I  Desire  

vs.  Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  and  the  stigmas  attached  to  sexual  orientation.  

The  following  is  an  example  from  the  surveys:  

What  sex  do  you  sexually  desire  (circle  the  most  appropriate  number)?  

       0     1      2     3     4     5                          6        Only  the                      Mostly  the              Slightly  more          Both  sexes            Slightly  more              Mostly  the                Only  the  opposite  sex      opposite  sex      the  opposite  sex            equally                  the  same  sex                  same  sex                  same  sex  

 Finally,  both  surveys  also  required  that  the  respondents  self-­‐reported  gender  on  a  

five-­‐point  scale  that  mimicked  the  scale  that  they  used  to  analyze  the  gender  of  individual  

behaviors  on  the  first  survey.    Once  again,  a  “1”  represented  an  answer  of  “very  masculine,”  

while  a  “5”  indicated  a  response  of  “very  feminine.”    A  “3”  designated  the  middle  of  the  

spectrum,  indicating  a  response  of  “neutral,”  leaving  a  “2”  and  a  “4”  to  signify  “slightly  more  

masculine”  and  “slightly  more  feminine,”  respectfully.    By  utilizing  the  same  scale  to  

analyze  both  the  gender  of  any  given  behavior  and  the  gender  of  individual  people,  my  

research  lent  itself  to  efficient  analysis.      

Techniques  of  Analysis  and  Groupings  of  Behaviors  

To  adequately  analyze  the  extensive  list  of  behaviors  in  the  surveys,  I  grouped  

certain  behaviors  based  upon  a  shared  theme.    There  were  a  total  of  forty-­‐one  behaviors  

Page 22: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     22  

outlined  in  the  surveys,  grouped  into  eight  separate  categories.    By  organizing  behaviors  

into  groups,  I  was  able  to  analyze  how  masculine  or  feminine  individuals  perceive  types  of  

behaviors  to  be  rather  than  just  individual  unrelated  actions.    Additionally,  if  sexual  

orientation  and  gender  performance  proved  to  correlate  with  another,  I  expected  to  see  

higher  correlations  among  certain  categories  than  in  others.    Each  category  contained  both  

stereotypically  masculine  and  stereotypically  feminine  behaviors—and,  in  most  cases,  for  

every  assumed  masculine  action,  there  was  a  closely  related  assumed  feminine  action.    

The  following  chart  represents  the  eight  forms  of  categorization  and  the  behaviors  

that  belong  to  each  group:  

Categorization  of  Behaviors  Category  Name   Category  Description   Behaviors  Appearance   Looks,  clothing,  and  

appearance  Wearing  pink  clothing  Wearing  blue  clothing  Wearing  tight  clothing  Wearing  baggy  clothing  Wearing  makeup  Styling  your  hair  

Arts   Participation  in  visual  or  performing  arts  

Singing  in  choir  Singing  in  a  band  Dancing  ballet  Dancing  hip-­‐hop  

Athletics   Sports  and  other  athletic  activities  

Playing  basketball  Playing  football  Playing  tennis  Playing  softball  Playing  hockey  Working  out  

Communication   Communicative  habits  in  dialogue  and  social  interactions  

Speaking  quickly  Speaking  slowly  Talking  with  your  hands  

Domestics   Activities  in  the  home   Cooking  food  Managing  finances  Cleaning  the  house  

Food  and  Drink   Types  of  things  that  people  consume  

Drinking  beer  Drinking  tequila  Drinking  whiskey  Eating  salad  Eating  steak  

Page 23: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     23  

Media   Preference  of  media  outlets   Watching  action  films  Watching  romantic  films  Listening  to  pop  music  Listening  to  rock  music  Playing  video  games  

Sex  and  Romance   Sexual  habits  and  dating  habits  

Penetrating  during  sex  Being  penetrated  during  sex  Performing  oral  sex  Receiving  oral  sex  Inflicting  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  Writing  love  letters  Using  online  dating  websites  Paying  for  dates  

 Ethical  Concerns  

The  only  ethical  concern  for  my  research  was  the  potential  emotional  harm  to  

participants  that  issues  of  sexual  orientation  and  sexuality  could  have  created.    However,  

since  I  conducted  this  study  through  written  surveys,  participants  could  simply  cease  

completing  the  survey  questions  at  any  time  if  the  subject  matter  disturbed  them.    

Otherwise,  the  surveys  were  fairly  uncontroversial  and  primarily  dealt  with  everyday  

behaviors  and  actions.    I  obtained  informed  consent  by  indicating  that  a  respondent’s  

participation  and  completion  of  the  survey  was  completely  voluntary.    Finally,  all  of  the  

data  that  I  collected  was  entirely  anonymous,  as  there  was  no  identifying  information  on  

the  surveys  nor  any  order  or  method  to  my  collection  of  them.  

Potential  Benefits  and  Risks  of  the  Methodology  

My  research  intended  to  investigate  whether  or  not  one’s  sexual  orientation  and  

gender  performance  were  related  by  conducting  two  entirely  separate  surveys—one  

focused  on  the  underlying  “gender”  of  behaviors,  and  the  second  concentrated  on  how  

often  individuals  of  different  sexual  orientations  engage  in  said  behaviors.    By  splitting  data  

Page 24: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     24  

collection  into  two  separate  surveys,  I  opened  myself  to  a  wide  range  of  potential  strengths  

and  weaknesses  in  my  design.  

As  for  the  benefits,  by  having  individuals  only  complete  one  of  the  two  surveys,  I  

effectively  halved  the  time  it  took  for  somebody  to  finish  one,  drastically  increasing  the  

probability  that  students  would  agree  to  both  attempt  and  complete  one.    Each  survey  was  

only  one  double-­‐sided  page  in  length,  so  it  was  easily  accessible  and  far  from  visually  

intimidating.    Additionally,  individuals  should  have  felt  less  pressure  when  identifying  how  

masculine  or  feminine  something  was,  as  I  refrained  from  asking  them  if  they  personally  

engaged  in  the  behavior.    A  man,  for  instance,  would  theoretically  be  more  likely  to  label  

what  he  does  as  masculine  if  he  was  asked  to  indicate  how  often  he  does  it,  just  as  a  woman  

would  be  more  likely  to  label  something  as  feminine  if  she  had  to  admit  to  doing  it,  as  well.    

By  eliminating  one’s  need  to  admit  to  what  he  or  she  personally  did,  I  should  have  received  

more  honest  results.    On  the  flip  side,  for  those  who  completed  the  second  survey,  

respondents  should  have  been  far  more  likely  to  accurately  judge  how  often  they  engaged  

in  behaviors  since  they  were  not  required  to  examine  the  masculine  or  feminine  

implications  behind  them.    As  a  whole,  splitting  my  study  into  two  surveys  made  for  a  far  

more  honest  and  less  invasive  experience  for  my  participants.  

Conversely,  there  were  a  few  drawbacks,  as  well.    By  having  different  people  

complete  different  information  without  the  use  of  a  truly  random  sample,  I  could  have  

potentially  had  very  different  groups  of  people  taking  each  survey  and  ended  up  with  

skewed  information.    To  try  to  minimize  this  potential  weakness,  I  made  sure  to  have  at  

least  one  hundred  and  fifty  respondents  complete  each  survey—the  more  cases,  the  more  

valid  my  results.    Moreover,  I  succeeded  in  alternating  handing  out  each  survey  in  

Page 25: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     25  

classrooms  to  reduce  the  probability  of  a  single  class  skewing  my  findings.    As  a  whole,  the  

strengths  of  my  design  far  outweighed  its  weaknesses.      

Overview  of  the  Results  

Initially,  my  research  ultimately  sought  to  investigate  the  relationship  between  

sexual  orientation  and  gender  performance.    However,  after  compiling  data  from  all  386  

surveys,  my  study  observed  several  other  potential  relationships  between  five  major  

categories:  sex—the  sex  of  the  respondent,  sexual  orientation—the  sexual  orientation  of  the  

respondent,  frequency  of  behavior—how  often  the  respondent  engages  in  different  

behaviors,  perception  of  behavior—how  masculine  or  feminine  the  respondent  perceives  

different  behaviors  to  be,  and  self-­reported  gender—how  masculine  or  feminine  the  

respondent  perceives  himself  or  herself  to  be.    The  following  results  contributed  toward  a  

greater  understanding  of  these  five  groups  and  the  associations  among  them.  

Perception  of  Behaviors  

From  the  first  survey,  my  research  could  determine  how  respondents  perceived  

behaviors  on  a  five-­‐point  scale  of  gender,  where  a  value  of  “1”  represented  extreme  

masculinity,  a  value  of  “5”  signified  extreme  femininity,  and  a  value  of  “3”  corresponded  to  

gender  neutrality.    The  average  value  of  each  behavior  represented  how  masculine  or  

feminine  respondents  perceived  it  to  be,  as  illustrated  on  the  following  chart.    As  a  value  

decreases  from  “3,”  the  gender  becomes  more  masculine.    Conversely,  as  a  value  increases  

from  “3,”  the  gender  becomes  more  feminine.    The  chart  lists  the  behaviors  from  most  

masculine  to  most  feminine  within  each  of  the  eight  categories:  

Perception  of  Behaviors  Category  Name   Behaviors   Mean  Value     Gender  Appearance   Wearing  baggy  clothing   2.17  

Page 26: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     26  

Wearing  blue  clothing   2.87   Masculine  Styling  your  hair   3.34  Wearing  tight  clothing   3.71  Wearing  pink  clothing   4.12  

 

Wearing  makeup   4.67  

Feminine  

Singing  in  a  band   2.76  Dancing  hip-­‐hop   2.89  

Masculine  

Singing  in  choir   3.29  

Arts  

Dancing  ballet   4.21  Feminine  

Playing  football   1.61  Playing  hockey   1.75  Playing  basketball   2.27  Working  out   2.78  

Masculine  

Playing  tennis   3.11  

Athletics  

Playing  softball   3.57  Feminine  

Speaking  slowly   2.94   Masculine  Speaking  quickly   3.30  

Communication  

Talking  with  your  hands   3.31  Feminine  

Managing  finances   2.95   Masculine  Cooking  food   3.26  

Domestics  

Cleaning  the  house   3.61  Feminine  

Drinking  whiskey   2.19  Drinking  beer   2.43  Eating  steak   2.69  Drinking  tequila   2.93  

Masculine  Food  and  Drink  

Eating  salad   3.47   Feminine  Playing  video  games   2.18  Watching  action  films   2.50  Listening  to  rock  music   2.66  

Masculine  

Listening  to  pop  music   3.35  

Media  

Watching  romantic  films   3.96  Feminine  

Penetrating  during  sex   1.94  Paying  for  dates   2.07  Inflicting  pain  for  sexual  pleasure   2.73  Receiving  oral  sex   2.74  

Masculine  

Using  online  dating  websites   3.13  Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure   3.15  Performing  oral  sex   3.22  Writing  love  letters   3.63  

Sex  and  Romance  

Being  penetrated  during  sex   4.13  

Feminine  

 Out  of  all  eight  categories  of  behaviors,  “Appearance”  and  “Sex  and  Romance”  had  

the  greatest  range  of  masculine  and  feminine  behaviors,  while  “Athletics”  was  the  most  

Page 27: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     27  

concentrated  toward  one  side  or  the  other.    Finally,  the  “Communication”  category  had  the  

narrowest  range  of  masculine  and  feminine  behaviors.  

Distribution  of  Sexual  Orientation  

In  inquiring  about  sexual  desire  on  a  modified  seven-­‐point  Kinsey  Scale  absent  of  

the  labels  of  heterosexuality,  bisexuality,  or  homosexuality,  this  study  observed  a  much  

greater  range  of  “sexual  orientations”  than  most  previous  research.  About  three-­‐quarters  

(76.7%)  of  respondents  identified  as  sexually  desiring  only  the  opposite  sex,  leaving  over  

23%  of  the  data  as  indicative  of  something  other  than  strict  heterosexuality.    The  following  

table  represents  the  percentage  distribution  of  responses:  

Distribution  of  Sexual  Orientation  Sex  the  Respondent  Sexually  Desires   Percentage  of  Respondents  Only  the  opposite  sex   76.7%  Mostly  the  opposite  sex   14.2%  Slightly  more  the  opposite  sex   2.1%  Both  sexes  equally   1.9%  Slightly  more  the  same  sex   0.3%  Mostly  the  same  sex   2.1%  Only  the  same  sex   2.7%  

 Distribution  of  Self-­Reported  Gender  

Both  surveys  asked  that  the  respondent  evaluate  his  or  her  own  gender  on  the  same  

five-­‐point  scale  used  to  analyze  the  gender  of  behaviors.    Once  again,  a  “1”  corresponded  

with  “very  masculine,”  while  a  “5”  signified  “very  feminine.”    A  “3”  specified  gender  

neutrality,  indicative  of  neither  masculinity  nor  femininity.    The  average  value  for  gender  

among  all  respondents  was  a  3.08,  leaning  slightly  feminine.    The  following  table  

represents  the  distribution  of  self-­‐reported  gender  for  all  respondents:  

Distribution  of  Self-­‐Reported  Gender  Self-­‐Reported  Gender  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Very  masculine   10.0%  

Page 28: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     28  

Slightly  more  masculine   26.4%  Neutral   23.7%  Slightly  more  feminine   25.5%  Very  feminine   14.3%  

 In  general,  the  distribution  suggests  that  individuals  are  more  likely  to  perceive  

themselves  as  “slightly”  more  masculine  or  feminine  rather  than  as  “very”  masculine  or  

feminine.    Additionally,  it  demonstrates  that  almost  one  fourth  of  all  respondents  reported  

gender-­‐neutrality,  refusing  to  assume  a  masculine  or  feminine  identity.    However,  when  

moderated  by  the  sex  of  the  respondent—male  and  female—the  percentage  distribution  

looks  quite  different:  

Distribution  of  Self-­‐Reported  Gender  (moderated  by  sex)  Sex  of  the  Respondent   Self-­‐Reported  Gender  of  the  

Respondent  Percentage  of  Respondents  

Very  masculine   21.8%  Slightly  more  masculine   54.9%  Neutral   17.6%  Slightly  more  feminine   3.5%  

Male      

Very  feminine   2.1%  Very  masculine   0.5%  Slightly  more  masculine   4.9%  Neutral   28.3%  Slightly  more  feminine   42.4%  

Female        

Very  feminine   23.9%    A  cross  tabulation  reveals  a  statistically  significant  relationship  between  self-­‐

reported  gender  and  sex  (p  <  .001).    The  distribution  of  self-­‐reported  gender  differs  among  

men  and  women,  specifically  for  gender-­‐neutrality.    Over  28%  of  women  reported  a  neutral  

gender,  while  only  17.6%  of  men  identified  as  gender-­‐neutral.    Meanwhile,  76.7%  of  men  

identified  with  their  socially  expected  gender  of  masculinity,  while  only  66.3%  of  women  

reported  a  feminine  gender  identity.    The  data  suggests  that  women  are  less  likely  to  

actively  engage  in  a  feminine  identity  than  men  are  to  actively  engage  in  a  masculine  one.    

Page 29: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     29  

However,  men  and  women  reported  about  the  same  rate  of  gender  inversion—where  men  

identify  as  more  feminine  or  women  identify  as  more  masculine.    About  5.6%  of  males  

reported  a  feminine  gender,  and  a  similar  5.4%  of  females  reported  a  masculine  gender.  

Distribution  of  Demographics  

Each  survey  asked  about  seven  major  demographics  in  addition  to  gender  

performance  and  sexual  orientation.    These  seven  categories  were  age,  class  standing,  

college  major,  occupation,  race,  religion,  and  sex.  

Age:    Out  of  the  386  participants  in  the  study,  the  average  age  was  21.99  years  old.  

Class  Standing:    The  “average”  class  standing  value  in  the  study  was  a  3.47,  where  a  

“1”  represented  a  “freshman,”  a  “2”  indicated  a  “sophomore,”  a  “3”  signified  a  “junior,”  a  “4”  

denoted  the  status  of  a  “senior,”  a  “5”  signified  a  “super  senior,”  and  a  “6”  indicated  a  

“graduate  student.”    The  average  value  of  3.47,  then,  represents  the  class  standing  of  

between  a  junior  and  senior,  and  the  following  table  outlines  the  percentage  distribution  of  

all  respondents:  

Distribution  of  Class  Standing  Class  Standing  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Freshman   5.7%  Sophomore   16.4%  Junior   32.1%  Senior   24.8%  Super  Senior   13.2%  Graduate  Student   7.8%  

 College  Major:    After  participants  listed  their  college  majors  on  the  surveys,  I  

grouped  and  coded  majors  by  college  department  with  the  intent  of  synthesizing  the  

information  and  representing  as  many  schools  of  study  as  possible.    A  table  describing  the  

entire  distribution  of  college  majors  and  departments  is  located  in  the  appendix  section  

Page 30: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     30  

(Table  #1).    Overall,  the  data  was  comprised  of  fifty-­‐three  different  disciplines  at  CSULB,  

with  the  three  most  common  departments  being  Music,  Family  and  Consumer  Sciences,  and  

Management  and  Human  Resources  Management.  

Occupation:    Participants  had  the  option  of  listing  their  occupation  if  they  were  

employed  outside  of  school.    With  well  over  one  hundred  different  responses,  my  research  

attempted  to  group  the  occupations  into  nineteen  major  categories,  as  illustrated  on  the  

percentage  distribution  table  located  in  the  appendix  section  (Table  #2).    Only  148  out  of  

386  participants  listed  an  occupation—38.3%—so  the  remaining  61.7%  of  the  respondents  

were  simply  listed  as  “missing.”    The  two  most  common  occupations  among  all  respondents  

were  in  the  “retail  /  sales”  and  “food  service  /  restaurant  /  lodging”  categories.      

Race:    The  following  table  outlines  the  percentage  distribution  of  race  among  all  

respondents.    The  most  common  reported  race  was  non-­‐Hispanic  white,  with  Hispanic  

white  and  Asian  making  up  the  bulk  of  the  rest.    A  surprising  10.5%  of  all  respondents  

chose  to  identify  with  an  unlisted  race,  so  it  is  very  possible  that  multiracial  individuals  did  

not  realize  that  they  could  check  more  than  one  race  and,  instead,  selected  “Other.”  

Distribution  of  Race  Race  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Non-­‐Hispanic  White   32.3%  Hispanic  White   23.1%  Black   4.3%  Native  American   1.3%  Asian   22.6%  Other   10.5%  Multiracial   5.9%  

 Religion:    The  following  table  represents  the  percentage  distribution  of  religion  

among  all  respondents.    Almost  all  of  the  respondents  identified  as  either  Christian  (43%)  

Page 31: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     31  

or  non-­‐religious  (32.4%).    16.8%  of  individuals  chose  not  to  identify  with  any  of  the  given  

religions.      

Distribution  of  Religion  Religion  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Christianity   43.0%  Islam   0.8%  Judaism   1.1%  Buddhism   5.4%  Hinduism   0.5%  Non-­‐Religious   32.4%  Other   16.8%  

 Sex:  The  surveys  offered  five  different  responses  for  the  category  of  sex,  including  

male,  female,  intersexed,  transgendered,  and  other.    However,  no  respondents  chose  to  

identify  as  intersexed  or  transgendered,  and  only  two  identified  as  “other.”    Consequently,  

this  study  cannot  effectively  analyze  a  third  category  for  sex  and  will,  instead,  focus  on  the  

binary  of  male  and  female.    The  subsequent  table  illustrates  the  percentage  distribution  of  

biological  sex  among  all  respondents:  

Distribution  of  Sex  Sex  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Male   42.6%  Female   56.9%  Other   0.5%  

 Relating  Sexual  Orientation  and  Frequency  of  Behavior—the  Research  Question  

Following  the  collection  of  data,  my  research  ultimately  sought  to  determine  if  an  

individual’s  sexual  orientation  correlated  with  his  or  her  gender  performance  in  any  way.    

In  order  to  investigate  the  existence  of  a  relationship  between  sexuality  and  behavior,  I  

utilized  the  Pearson  R  correlation.    Since  participants  reported  both  frequency  of  behavior  

and  sexual  orientation  on  a  numeric  scale,  I  could  effectively  translate  these  numbers  into  

values  and  deduce  their  meanings.    A  positive  value  for  Pearson’s  R  signified  a  positive  

Page 32: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     32  

relationship  between  sexual  orientation  and  frequency  of  behavior,  expressed  as  “as  

‘gayness’  goes  up,  frequency  of  behavior  goes  up.”    Conversely,  a  negative  value  for  

Pearson’s  R  represented  a  negative  relationship  between  sexual  orientation  and  frequency  

of  behavior,  expressed  as  “as  ‘gayness’  goes  up,  frequency  of  behavior  goes  down.”    Due  to  

the  structure  of  the  modified  seven-­‐point  Kinsey  Scale,  where  a  “0”  referred  to  sexual  

desire  of  “only  the  opposite  sex”  and  a  “6”  signified  sexual  desire  of  “only  the  same  sex,”  

higher  values  were  indicative  of  what  this  research  refers  to  as  “gayness.”    Similarly,  I  

coded  frequency  of  behavior  on  a  four-­‐point  scale,  where  a  “3”  meant  “often,”  a  “2”  denoted  

“sometimes,”  a  “1”  symbolized  “rarely,”  and  a  “0”  meant  “never.”    In  other  words,  the  higher  

the  value,  the  more  often  individuals  engaged  in  the  behavior.  

However,  my  research  required  that  the  correlations  between  sexuality  and  

frequency  of  behavior  accounted  for  the  sex  of  the  respondents.    After  all,  since  the  sexual  

orientation  and  gender  conflation  is  based  upon  sex-­‐specific  inversion,  what  is  indicative  of  

“gayness”  for  men  will  be  entirely  different  than  what  is  indicative  of  “gayness”  for  women.    

Consequently,  this  study  ran  Pearson  R  correlations  between  sexual  orientation  and  

behavior  for  each  of  the  forty-­‐one  behaviors  when  moderated  by  sex  (male  /  female),  

resulting  in  a  total  of  eighty-­‐two  total  investigated  categories.  

Out  of  all  eighty-­‐two  categories,  there  were  ten  occurrences  of  statistically  

significant  correlations  between  sexual  orientation  and  gender  performance.    In  other  

words,  there  was  no  conclusive  evidence  to  show  that  sexual  orientation  and  behavior  

were  related  87.8%  of  the  time.    Additionally,  no  single  behavior  correlated  with  sexual  

orientation  for  both  men  and  women,  and  only  two  behaviors  were  strongly  related  to  

sexual  orientation.    The  following  chart  outlines  each  behavior’s  relationship  with  sexual  

Page 33: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     33  

orientation  and  the  statistics  behind  it.    If  the  probability  value  is  greater  than  .05  (p  >  .05),  

the  result  is  not  significant  and  is  therefore  inconclusive.    If  the  probability  value  is  less  

than  .05  (p  <  .05),  the  result  is  significant  and  is  therefore  conclusive.    An  asterisk  denotes  

instances  where  the  result  is  statistically  significant  and  the  behavior  is  related  to  sexual  

orientation.    In  such  cases,  the  greater  the  Pearson’s  R  value  is  (closer  to  a  value  of  1),  the  

stronger  the  positive  relationship  (as  “gayness”  goes  up,  frequency  of  engaging  in  the  

behavior  goes  up).    Conversely,  the  lesser  the  Pearson’s  R  value  is  (closer  to  a  value  of  -­‐1),  

the  stronger  the  negative  relationship  (as  “gayness”  goes  up,  frequency  of  engaging  in  the  

behavior  goes  down).    For  both  positive  and  negative  relationships,  the  closer  the  value  is  

to  0,  the  weaker  the  relationship.  

Relationship  Between  Sexual  Orientation  and  Frequency  of  Behavior  (moderated  by  sex)  Pearson’s  R  &  Sig.  (*)  Category  Name   Behaviors  Male   Female  

Meaning  of  Relationship  for    Cases  of  Statistical  Significance  

Wearing  baggy  clothing   R  =  -­‐.142   R  =  .113   N/A  Wearing  blue  clothing   R  =  -­‐.124   R  =  -­‐.036   N/A  Styling  your  hair   R  =  .072   R  =  .032   N/A  Wearing  tight  clothing   R  =  .147   R  =  .085   N/A  Wearing  pink  clothing   R  =  .265*   R  =  -­‐.211   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  

frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Appearance  

Wearing  makeup   R  =  .099   R  =  .071   N/A  Singing  in  a  band   R  =  .051   R  =  .096   N/A  Dancing  hip-­‐hop   R  =  .056   R  =  .059   N/A  Singing  in  choir   R  =  .316*   R  =  -­‐.045   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  

frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Arts  

Dancing  ballet   R  =  .216*   R  =  -­‐.043   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Playing  football   R  =  -­‐.106   R  =  .003   N/A  Playing  hockey   R  =  -­‐.142   R  =  -­‐.056   N/A  Playing  basketball   R  =  -­‐.127   R  =  .133   N/A  Working  out   R  =  -­‐.159   R  =  -­‐.008   N/A  Playing  tennis   R  =  .132   R  =  -­‐.136   N/A  

Athletics  

Playing  softball   R  =  -­‐.090   R  =  -­‐.046   N/A  Speaking  slowly   R  =  .101   R  =  .182   N/A  Communication  Speaking  quickly   R  =  .153   R  =  -­‐.423*   As  women’s  gayness  increases,  

frequency  of  behavior  decreases  

Page 34: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     34  

  Talking  with  your  hands   R  =  .028   R  =  .107   N/A  Managing  finances   R  =  -­‐.176   R  =  -­‐.038   N/A  Cooking  food   R  =  .072   R  =  .043   N/A  

Domestics  

Cleaning  the  house   R  =  .152   R  =  .064   N/A  Drinking  whiskey   R  =  .031   R  =  .121   N/A  Drinking  beer   R  =  .160   R  =  .160   N/A  Eating  steak   R  =  .079   R  =  .036   N/A  Drinking  tequila   R  =  .200   R  =  -­‐.062   N/A  

Food  and  Drink  

Eating  salad   R  =  .222*   R  =  .091   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Playing  video  games   R  =  .056   R  =  .003   N/A  Watching  action  films   R  =  -­‐.169   R  =  -­‐.115   N/A  Listening  to  rock  music   R  =  .080   R  =  .154   N/A  Listening  to  pop  music   R  =  .141   R  =  .022   N/A  

Media  

Watching  romantic  films   R  =  .272*   R  =  .039   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Penetrating  during  sex   R  =  -­‐.044   R  =  .264*   As  women’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Paying  for  dates   R  =  -­‐.104   R  =  .206   N/A  Inflicting  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  

R  =  .066   R  =  .206   N/A  

Receiving  oral  sex   R  =  .177   R  =  .177   N/A  Using  online  dating  websites   R  =  .124   R  =  .046   N/A  Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  

R  =  .284*   R  =  .204   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Performing  oral  sex   R  =  .216*   R  =  .120   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

Writing  love  letters   R  =  .118   R  =  .051   N/A  

Sex  and  Romance  

Being  penetrated  during  sex   R  =  .888*   R  =  .038   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  frequency  of  behavior  increases  

 Relating  Sexual  Orientation  and  Perception  of  Behavior  

Upon  investigating  the  data,  my  research  seemed  to  stumble  across  a  phenomenon  

where  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  was  related  to  his  or  her  perception  of  behavior  

when  moderated  by  sex.    In  other  words,  just  by  looking  at  the  data,  it  appeared  as  if  

respondents  viewed  behaviors  as  more  masculine  or  feminine  based  on  their  own  sexual  

orientation.    For  instance,  for  men,  the  data  seemed  to  suggest  that  as  an  individual  “got  

gayer”—as  “gayness”  went  up—he  was  more  likely  to  label  “playing  hockey”  as  more  

Page 35: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     35  

feminine.    After  running  Pearson  R  correlations  on  all  eighty-­‐two  categories  (forty-­‐one  

behaviors  for  both  men  and  women),  eleven  cases  exhibited  statistically  significant  

relationships  between  sexual  orientation  and  perception  of  behavior.    Out  of  these  eleven  

cases,  nine  referred  to  males.    Much  like  the  relationships  between  sexual  orientation  and  

frequency  of  behavior,  a  very  small  13.4%  of  the  observed  cases  were  statistically  

significant,  so  this  study  cannot  effectively  claim  that  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  

correlates  with  how  he  or  she  perceives  behaviors  when  moderated  by  sex.    However,  since  

this  study  found  little  previous  research  on  the  subject,  the  topic  likely  warrants  further  

investigation.  

The  following  table  represents  only  the  eleven  cases  where  sexual  orientation  and  

perception  of  behavior  were  related.    All  other  cases  had  a  probability  value  of  more  than  

.05  (p  >  .05)  and  were  inconclusive.    A  positive  relationship  signified  that  as  “gayness”  

increased,  the  gender  value  of  a  behavior  increased,  meaning  that  the  behavior  was  

increasing  in  femininity  (since  a  “5”  indicated  very  feminine).    Meanwhile,  a  negative  

relationship  indicated  that  as  “gayness”  increased,  the  gender  value  of  a  behavior  

decreased,  meaning  that  the  behavior  was  increasing  in  masculinity  (since  a  “1”  

represented  very  masculine).    The  closer  the  Pearson’s  R  value  is  to  1,  the  stronger  the  

positive  relationship,  while  the  closer  the  Pearson’s  R  value  is  to  -­‐1,  the  stronger  the  

negative  relationship.  

Relationship  Between  Sexual  Orientation  and  Perception  of  Behavior  (moderated  by  sex)  Category  Name   Behaviors   Sex  of  the    

Respondent  Pearson’s  R  &  Sig.  (*)  

Meaning  of  Relationship  

Wearing  pink  clothing   Male   R  =  -­‐.314*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  masculinity  increases  

Appearance  

Wearing  makeup   Male   R  =  -­‐.356*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  masculinity  increases  

Page 36: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     36  

    Female   R  =  -­‐.244*   As  women’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  masculinity  increases  

Singing  in  a  band   Male   R  =  .293*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Arts  

Dancing  ballet   Male   R  =  -­‐.279*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  masculinity  increases  

Athletics   Playing  hockey   Male   R  =  .381*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Domestics   Managing  finances   Male   R  =  .272*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Food  and  Drink   Drinking  tequila   Female   R  =  .215*   As  women’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Media   Watching  action  films   Male   R  =  .275*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Paying  for  dates   Male   R  =  .269*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  femininity  increases  

Sex  and  Romance  

Being  penetrated  during  sex  

Male   R  =  -­‐.380*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  behavior’s  masculinity  increases  

 Relating  Self-­Reported  Gender  and  Perception  of  Behavior  

In  addition  to  sexual  orientation,  the  data  also  suggested  that  an  individual’s  self-­‐

reported  gender  and  his  or  her  perception  of  behaviors  were  occasionally  related.    By  

running  a  one-­‐way  ANOVA  statistical  test,  my  research  compared  the  gender  of  the  

respondents  to  the  average  assigned  gender  of  each  behavior  and  found  thirteen  cases  of  

statistical  significance.    However,  since  my  study  could  analyze  self-­‐reported  gender  

independent  from  sex,  the  total  possible  cases  added  to  only  forty-­‐one  instead  of  the  usual  

eighty-­‐two.    In  other  words,  almost  32%  of  the  cases  yielded  statistical  significance,  a  much  

higher  percentage  than  any  other  previous  category  of  investigation.  

The  following  graphs  represent  only  the  thirteen  cases  where  self-­‐reported  gender  

and  the  gender  of  behaviors  were  related  with  statistical  significance.    Once  again,  a  

gender-­‐value  closer  to  “5”  represents  femininity,  while  a  gender-­‐value  closer  to  “1”  

indicates  masculinity.  

Page 37: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     37  

Playing  basketball:  sig.  =  .000*  (p  <  .05)  

   

Playing  football:  sig.  =  .016*,  (p  <  .05)  

   

   Penetrating  during  sex:  sig.  =  .045*,  (p  <  .05)  

 

Page 38: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     38  

 Watching  action  films:  sig.  =  .001*,  (p  <  .05)  

   

Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure:  sig.  =  .005*,  (p  <  .05)  

       Drinking  Beer:  sig.  =  .040*,  (p  <  .05)  

 

Page 39: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     39  

 Being  penetrated  during  sex:  sig.  =  .000*,  (p  <  .05)  

   

Working  out:  sig.  =  .001*,  (p  <  .05)  

     Dancing  ballet:  sig.  =  .046*,  (p  <  .05)  

 

Page 40: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     40  

 Wearing  baggy  clothing:  sig.  =  .029*,  (p  <  .05)  

   

Talking  with  your  hands:  sig.  =  .013*,  (p  <  .05)  

     Watching  romantic  films:  sig.  =  .033*,  (p  <  .05)  

 

Page 41: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     41  

 Listening  to  Rock  Music:  sig.  =  .016*,  (p  <  .05)  

   

In  general,  there  are  two  kinds  of  distributions  throughout  the  thirteen  statistically  

significant  cases.    The  first  kind  of  distribution  is  a  direct  linear  relationship—as  

individuals  identify  as  more  masculine  or  feminine,  they  perceive  a  behavior  to  be  more  

masculine  or  feminine.    For  instance,  for  “playing  basketball,”  the  more  feminine  the  

individual,  the  more  feminine  and  less  masculine  he  or  she  perceived  the  behavior.    

“Working  out”  follows  a  similar  pattern,  where  the  more  feminine  the  individual,  the  more  

feminine  and  less  masculine  he  or  she  perceived  the  behavior.    In  most  cases,  the  linear  

relationships  progress  toward  gender  neutrality  from  masculinity  or  femininity  rather  than  

crossing  over  from  masculinity  to  femininity  or  vice  versa.    In  these  kinds  of  distributions,  

those  who  identify  as  very  masculine  and  those  who  identify  as  very  feminine  are  in  

disagreement  about  the  gender  of  the  behaviors.  

The  second  most  common  distribution,  as  seen  in  “listening  to  rock  music,”  “playing  

football,”  and  “dancing  ballet,”  is  a  sort  of  curve,  where  both  very  masculine  and  very  

feminine  individuals  agree  about  the  masculinity  or  femininity  of  a  behavior.    However,  

those  closer  to  gender-­‐neutrality  perceive  the  behaviors,  themselves,  as  closer  to  gender-­‐

Page 42: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     42  

neutral.    In  these  cases,  the  extremes  on  both  sides  of  the  gender  spectrum  are  in  

agreement  about  the  behavior,  while  the  gender-­‐neutral  maintain  a  perception  of  the  

behavior  more  closely  related  to  how  they  view  themselves.  

Relating  Sexual  Orientation  and  Self-­Reported  Gender  

As  one  might  expect,  sexual  orientation  correlated  with  self-­‐reported  gender,  

though  the  relationship  was  fairly  weak.    The  following  table  represents  the  Pearson  R  

correlations  statistical  test  between  sexuality  and  gender  when  moderated  by  sex.    A  

positive  relationship  signifies  that  as  an  individual’s  “gayness”  increases,  his  or  her  self-­‐

reported  gender  increases  in  value—or,  where  a  higher  value  is  indicative  of  femininity,  

femininity  increases.    Conversely,  a  negative  relationship  demonstrates  that  as  a  

respondent’s  “gayness”  increases,  he  or  she  identifies  as  more  masculine.  

Relationship  Between  Sexual  Orientation  and  Self-­‐Reported  Gender  (moderated  by  sex)  Sex  of  the  Respondent   Pearson’s  R  &  Sig.  (*)   Meaning  of  Relationship  Male   R  =  .227*   As  men’s  gayness  increases,  self-­‐

reported  femininity  increases  Female   R  =  -­‐.193*   As  women’s  gayness  increases,  self-­‐

reported  masculinity  increases    

Relating  Sexual  Orientation  and  Age  

In  discussing  the  “Sex  I  Desire  vs.  Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  in  the  literature  

review,  I  hypothesized  that  individuals  would  be  more  likely  to  simply  report  sexual  desire  

toward  the  same  sex  than  to  fully  identify  with  the  construct  of  homosexuality,  as  college  

students  might  not  yet  be  cognitively  aware  of  their  sexual  orientations.    For  the  purposes  

of  confirming  my  reasoning  for  asking  about  sexual  desire  without  the  labels  of  sexual  

orientation,  I  conducted  another  Pearson  R  correlation  statistical  test  between  sexuality  

and  age.    As  expected,  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  was  related  to  his  or  her  age,  

Page 43: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     43  

though  the  relationship  was  surprisingly  weak—the  older  the  respondent,  the  more  likely  

he  or  she  identified  same-­‐sex  sexual  attraction.    Pearson’s  R  was  a  value  of  .123  with  a  

significance  of  .017,  indicating  a  very  weak  positive  relationship  between  sexuality  and  age.    

The  result  was  statistically  significant  (p  <  .05).  

Relating  Sex  and  Frequency  of  Behavior—Do  Sex  and  Gender  “Line  Up”?  

While  this  study  was  largely  concerned  with  the  perceived  associations  between  

sexual  orientation  and  gender  performance,  it  also  sought  to  investigate  the  definitions  of  

masculinity  and  femininity,  themselves.    In  particular,  according  to  the  investigated  

literature,  gender  performance  refers  to  all  things  that  a  person  says  or  does  to  project  the  

status  of  male  or  female—masculinity  denotes  the  status  of  a  male,  while  femininity  

denotes  the  status  of  a  female.    Consequently,  if  a  behavior  occurred  more  often  among  

men  than  women,  the  behavior,  itself,  should  have  been  indicative  of  masculinity.    Likewise,  

if  women  engaged  in  the  behavior  more  often  than  men,  the  individual  behavior  should  

have  held  the  status  of  femininity.    In  cases  where  the  frequency  of  behavior  and  

perception  of  behavior  did  not  align—such  as  when  men  engaged  in  a  feminine  behavior  

more  often  than  women—sex  and  gender  remained  independent  from  one  another.  

In  order  to  explore  the  connections  between  frequency  of  behavior  and  perception  

of  behavior,  my  research  found  the  average  value  for  frequency  of  behavior  when  

moderated  by  sex.    By  coding  frequency  of  behavior  on  a  four-­‐point  scale,  where  a  higher  

value  of  “3”  indicated  “often”  and  a  lower  value  of  “0”  represented  “never,”  a  higher  average  

value  demonstrated  a  higher  average  frequency  of  behavior.    My  research  then  used  an  

independent  t-­‐test  to  determine  if  the  average  frequency  of  a  given  behavior  for  men  was  

different  than  for  women  at  a  statistically  significant  level.    In  doing  so,  it  revealed  which  

Page 44: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     44  

sex  engaged  in  behaviors  more  often.    Only  those  relationships  with  statistical  significance  

(p  <  .05)  were  conclusive.  

Finally,  this  study  compared  the  results  of  the  independent  t-­‐test  with  the  “mean”  

assigned  gender  of  each  behavior  in  order  to  determine  if  sex  and  gender  “lined  up”—

males  and  masculinity  or  females  and  femininity.    If  men  engaged  in  a  behavior  more  often  

than  women,  sex  and  gender  would  “line  up”  if  respondents  perceived  the  behavior  to  be  

more  masculine.    Meanwhile,  if  women  engaged  in  a  behavior  more  often  than  men,  sex  and  

gender  would  only  “line  up”  if  respondents  perceived  the  behavior  to  be  more  feminine.  

Once  again,  a  gender  value  closer  to  “1”  indicated  masculinity,  while  a  gender  value  closer  

to  “5”  suggested  femininity.    In  the  following  table,  an  asterisk  denotes  that  the  difference  

of  the  mean  frequency  of  behavior  between  males  and  females  is  statistically  significant.  

Relationship  Between  Sex,  Frequency  of  Behavior,  and  Perception  of  Behavior  Mean  Frequency    of  Behaviors  

Category  Name   Behaviors   Mean  Gender    of  Behaviors  

Male   Female  

Sig.  (*)   Meaning  of  Relationship    for  Cases  of  Statistical  Significance  

Wearing  baggy  clothing   2.17   1.12   1.17   .638   N/A  Wearing  blue  clothing   2.87   2.33   2.24   .325   N/A  Styling  your  hair   3.34   1.89   2.29   .004*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Wearing  tight  clothing   3.71   1.38   2.11   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Wearing  pink  clothing   4.12   0.46   1.54   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

Appearance  

Wearing  makeup   4.67   0.07   2.48   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Singing  in  a  band   2.76   0.47   0.30   .165   N/A  Dancing  hip-­‐hop   2.89   1.13   1.45   .037*   Sex  &  gender  do  not  line  up  Singing  in  choir   3.29   0.57   0.64   .648   N/A  

Arts  

Dancing  ballet   4.21   0.15   0.44   .005*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Playing  football   1.61   1.05   0.32   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Playing  hockey   1.75   0.26   0.11   .043*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Playing  basketball   2.27   1.40   0.73   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Working  out   2.78   2.24   2.03   .087   N/A  Playing  tennis   3.11   0.72   0.53   .125   N/A  

Athletics  

Playing  softball   3.57   0.41   0.26   .103   N/A  Speaking  slowly   2.94   1.68   1.46   .038*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Speaking  quickly   3.30   1.92   2.21   .005*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

Communication  

Talking  with  your  hands   3.31   1.96   2.33   .003*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

Page 45: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     45  

Managing  finances   2.95   2.24   2.21   .806   N/A  Cooking  food   3.26   2.11   2.15   .698   N/A  

Domestics  

Cleaning  the  house   3.61   1.08   2.38   .011*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Drinking  whiskey   2.19   1.40   0.80   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Drinking  beer   2.43   1.93   1.38   .001*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Eating  steak   2.69   2.21   1.70   .004*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Drinking  tequila   2.93   1.32   1.17   .344   N/A  

Food  and  Drink  

Eating  salad   3.47   2.03   2.12   .437   N/A  Playing  video  games   2.18   2.07   1.11   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Watching  action  films   2.50   2.55   2.31   .008*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Listening  to  rock  music   2.66   2.13   2.01   .424   N/A  Listening  to  pop  music   3.35   1.71   2.04   .008*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

Media  

Watching  romantic  films   3.96   1.71   2.31   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Penetrating  during  sex   1.94   2.37   0.30   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Paying  for  dates   2.07   2.24   1.19   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Inflicting  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  

2.73   0.58   0.32   .030*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

Receiving  oral  sex   2.74   1.71   1.33   .021*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  Using  online  dating  websites  

3.13   0.20   0.05   .020*   Sex  &  gender  do  not  line  up  

Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure  

3.15   0.44   0.39   .655   N/A  

Performing  oral  sex   3.22   1.42   1.41   .932   N/A  Writing  love  letters   3.63   0.96   0.84   .295   N/A  

Sex  and    Romance  

Being  penetrated  during  sex  

4.13   0.13   1.87   .000*   Sex  &  gender  line  up  

 According  to  the  data,  the  sex  of  the  respondent  was  related  to  frequency  of  

behavior  in  twenty-­‐six  out  of  forty-­‐one  behaviors,  or  about  63.4%  of  the  time.    Out  of  those  

twenty-­‐six  behaviors,  twenty-­‐four  demonstrated  a  link  between  sex  and  gender—men  

engaged  in  masculine  behaviors  more  often  than  women,  and  women  engaged  in  feminine  

behaviors  more  often  than  men.    However,  in  the  cases  of  “dancing  hip-­‐hop”  and  “using  

online  dating  websites,”  sex  and  gender  did  not  align  with  social  expectations.    For  “dancing  

hip-­‐hop,”  even  though  respondents  labeled  it  as  more  masculine,  women  engaged  in  the  

behavior  more  often  than  men.    Similarly,  men  used  online  dating  websites  more  frequently  

than  women  even  though  respondents  perceived  the  behavior  as  more  feminine.    In  other  

Page 46: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     46  

words,  the  social  constructs  of  male  masculinity  and  female  femininity  did  not  apply  in  a  

total  of  seventeen  out  of  forty-­‐one  cases,  or  41.5%  of  the  time.    The  results  suggest  that  

while  sex  and  gender  performance  are  more  often  related  than  sexual  orientation  and  

gender  performance,  they  may  still  remain  independent  from  one  another.  

Making  Sense  of  the  Data—An  Analysis  of  Sex,  Sexuality,  and  Gender  

At  its  most  fundamental  form,  an  analysis  of  the  data  demonstrates  that  the  

constructs  of  biological  sex,  sexual  orientation,  and  gender  performance  are  largely  

separate  attributes  that  do  not  normally  individually  affect  or  rely  on  one  another.    In  

regards  to  the  original  research  question,  an  individual’s  sexual  orientation  does  not  

necessarily  correlate  with  much  of  his  or  her  gender  performance,  and  this  study  cannot  

effectively  reject  the  null  hypothesis.    The  majority  of  the  time,  there  is  no  relationship  

between  an  individual’s  object  of  sexual  desire  and  everyday  behavior.  

In  the  following  sections,  this  study  will  use  its  results  and  findings  to  further  

analyze  the  three  main  topics  of  sex,  sexuality,  and  gender  and  their  role  in  the  social  

world.  

Sex—Being  a  Male  or  Female  in  the  Social  World  

An  individual’s  biological  sex—male  and  female—plays  a  vital  role  in  his  or  her  

perception  and  understanding  of  the  world.    While  my  research  allowed  room  for  multiple  

sexes,  respondents  overwhelming  chose  to  identify  with  the  binary  of  male  and  female.    

With  the  exception  of  self-­‐reported  gender,  nearly  every  technique  of  analysis  relied  on  

first  moderating  the  data  with  biological  sex.    After  all,  there  is  no  conclusive  gay  or  straight  

way  to  behave—wearing  pink  clothing,  for  instance,  has  an  entirely  different  meaning  for  a  

Page 47: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     47  

woman  than  it  does  for  a  man.    Through  the  lenses  of  sex,  we  can  develop  a  better  

understanding  of  how  sex  affects  the  numerous  other  investigated  relationships  in  the  data.  

Biological  sex  proved  to  affect  the  existence  of  relationships  among  gender  

performance  and  sexual  orientation;  however,  it  did  not,  itself,  necessarily  correlate  with  

gender  performance  or  sexual  orientation.    Men  were  not  more  or  less  likely  to  identify  as  

gay  than  women,  nor  were  they  necessarily  more  or  less  likely  to  engage  in  many  of  the  

different  behaviors.    Instead,  biological  sex  served  as  a  moderating  force  in  making  sense  of  

the  rest  of  the  data.  

That  said,  there  was  some  evidence  of  a  relationship  between  sex  and  behavior  for  

certain  behaviors.    The  following  chart  represents  the  categorical  breakdown  of  the  

twenty-­‐six  associated  behaviors:  

Categorical  Distribution  of  Behaviors  Related  to  Sex  Category  Name   Percentage  of  Behaviors    Appearance   66.7%  Arts   50.0%  Athletics   50.0%  Communication   100.0%  Domestics   33.3%  Food  and  Drink   60.0%  Media   80.0%  Sex  and  Romance   66.7%  

 By  analyzing  the  kinds  of  behaviors  that  were  linked  to  the  sex  of  the  respondent,  

my  research  can  form  a  clearer  understanding  of  the  data.    The  results  suggest  that  an  

individual’s  communication  habits  in  dialogue  and  social  interactions  are  strongly  related  

to  his  or  her  biological  sex.    Additionally,  an  individual’s  preference  of  media  outlets  is  

overwhelmingly  subject  to  his  or  her  status  as  a  male  or  female.    Thus,  while  sex  usually  

serves  as  a  moderating  force  independent  of  gender  and  sexuality,  it  does  seem  to  share  a  

Page 48: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     48  

relationship  with  all  those  behaviors  that  pertain  to  communication  habits  and  preference  

of  media.  

The  subsequent  question,  then,  seems  obvious—why  do  males  and  females  engage  

in  communicative  and  media-­‐related  behaviors  at  such  different  rates?    One  possible  

explanation  refers  to  biology.    Perhaps  an  individual’s  rate  of  speech,  for  example,  is  

grounded  in  biology  rather  than  in  environmental  and  social  learning.    Or,  perhaps  a  

person’s  inclination  to  watch  romantic  films  is  based  on  a  sex-­‐based  biological  

predisposition  toward  romance.    However,  the  biological  explanation  seems  to  fail  when  

moderated  by  sexual  orientation—gay  women  engaged  in  the  behavior  of  “speaking  

quickly”  far  less  frequently  than  straight  women,  and  gay  men  watched  romantic  films  

more  frequently  than  straight  men.    If  the  biological  argument  proved  correct,  it  would  

necessitate  that  sexual  orientation  affect  behavior.  

Another  more  likely  explanation  refers  to  the  size  of  the  media  and  communication  

categories,  both  of  which  included  a  smaller  overall  number  of  behaviors  than  some  of  the  

other  categories.    Communication,  in  particular,  contained  only  three  behaviors—as  a  

result,  the  relationship  between  communicative  behaviors  and  biological  sex  could  be  

attributed  to  chance.  

However,  due  to  the  existence  of  such  a  strong  relationship  between  certain  

categories  of  behaviors  and  sex,  this  study  recommends  that  the  social  sciences  conduct  

further  research  on  sex-­‐based  communicative  styles  and  preference  of  media  that  makes  

room  for  the  moderating  force  of  sexual  orientation.  

 

 

Page 49: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     49  

Sexuality—Sexual  Desire  Removed  from  Sexual  Orientation  

In  addition  to  the  investigated  correlations  between  sexual  orientation  and  gender,  

this  study  also  shed  light  on  the  notion  of  “sexual  orientation,”  itself.    By  inquiring  about  the  

direction  and  object  of  sexual  desire  rather  than  about  homosexuality  or  heterosexuality,  

my  research  uncovered  that  over  23%  of  all  respondents  reported  some  sort  of  sexual  

desire  toward  the  same  sex.    This  figure  is  significantly  higher  than  the  generally  accepted  

belief  that  around  10%  of  the  population  is  “gay.”    Of  course,  definitions  for  what  

constitutes  “gayness”  likely  do  not  include  those  who  identify  as  still  desiring  “mostly”  the  

opposite  sex.    However,  by  grouping  “mostly  the  opposite  sex”  and  “only  the  opposite  sex”  

as  the  “straight”  category  and  grouping  “slightly  more  the  opposite  sex,”  “both  sexes  

equally,”  “slightly  more  the  same  sex,”  “mostly  the  same  sex,”  and  “only  the  same  sex”  as  

the  gay  category,  about  9.1%  of  the  respondents  still  fell  under  the  gay  category.    This  

figure  is  comparable  to  the  “one-­‐in-­‐ten”  generally  accepted  belief.  

Categorical  Distribution  of  Sexual  Orientation  Category  for  Sexual  Orientation   Percentage  of  Respondents  “Straight”  Group   90.1%  “Gay”  Group   9.1%  

 Arguably  the  most  interesting  result  of  the  entire  study  is  the  percentage  of  

respondents  who  reported  sexual  desire  of  “mostly  the  opposite  sex”—about  14.2%  in  all.    

Just  over  one  out  of  every  seven  people  indicated  predominate  sexual  desire  toward  the  

opposite  sex  in  addition  to  occurrences  of  same-­‐sex  desire.    It  is  important  to  note  that  the  

survey  did  not  inquire  about  physical  attraction—instead,  it  asked  respondents  to  indicate  

the  sex  they  sexually  desired.    These  results  contribute  significantly  toward  queer  theory  

and  the  belief  that  every  individual  engages  in  his  or  her  own  unique  sexual  orientation,  

Page 50: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     50  

removed  from  the  ternary  of  heterosexuality,  bisexuality,  and  homosexuality  in  society.    If  

individual  degrees  of  sexual  orientation  do  not  exist,  then  all  those  respondents  who  

reported  mixed  sexual  desire—20.6%  in  total—would  simply  belong  to  the  “bisexual”  

group.    In  other  words,  without  room  for  individual  sexualities,  more  than  one  out  of  every  

five  individuals  would  be  labeled  as  bisexual.  

As  for  the  correlations  between  sexual  orientation  and  gender,  behavior  was  only  

associated  with  sexual  orientation  about  12.2%  of  the  time.    Over  87%  of  the  time,  an  

individual’s  actions  and  behavior  were  not  indicative  of  his  or  her  sexual  orientation.    

These  results  stand  somewhat  oppositional  to  Kenneth  J.  Zucker’s  determination  that  

masculine  behavior  in  girls  and  feminine  behavior  in  boys  was  a  predictor  of  eventual  

homosexuality.    With  the  exception  of  only  ten  out  of  eighty-­‐two  total  combinations  of  

behaviors,  how  a  person  acts  and  what  sex  the  person  desires  were  not  associated  with  one  

another.    

To  better  understand  the  ten  behaviors  that  did  correlate  with  sexual  orientation,  

the  following  chart  groups  certain  kinds  of  behaviors  in  the  same  way  as  in  the  analysis  of  

biological  sex:  

Categorical  Distribution  of  Behaviors  Related  to  Sexual  Orientation  Category  Name   Percentage  of  Behaviors    Appearance   16.7%  Arts   50.0%  Athletics   0.0%  Communication   33.3%  Domestics   0.0%  Food  and  Drink   20.0%  Media   20.0%  Sex  and  Romance   44.4%  

 

Page 51: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     51  

The  data  demonstrates  that  behaviors  pertaining  to  the  performing  arts  and  to  

sexual  and  dating  habits  were  more  often  correlated  with  sexual  orientation  than  any  other  

kinds  of  behaviors.    As  for  the  association  between  “sex  and  romance”  and  sexual  

orientation,  the  most  likely  explanation  refers  to  the  differing  nature  of  sexual  intercourse  

and  customs  between  gay  and  straight  individuals.    For  example,  in  a  sexual  interaction  

between  two  homosexual  men,  “being  penetrated  during  sex”  probably  occurs  more  

frequently  due  to  the  higher  rate  of  anal  sex  among  gay  men  than  straight  men.    

Additionally,  for  many  straight  men,  as  seen  in  Tomas  Almaguer’s  study  of  Chicano  men,  

“being  penetrated  during  sex”  is  analogous  to  emasculation  and  sexual  humiliation.    In  the  

homosexual  community,  such  a  stigma  toward  “receiving”  sexual  penetration  does  not  

exist.    In  general,  as  sexual  orientation  actually  implies  sexual  habits,  a  higher  rate  of  

association  among  the  “sex  and  romance”  category  is  largely  unsurprising.    However,  even  

still,  only  44.4%  of  those  behaviors  within  the  category  proved  to  correlate  with  sexual  

orientation—over  half  of  the  time,  sexual  and  dating  habits  were  not  indicative  of  sexuality.  

For  the  performing  arts,  however,  analysis  is  somewhat  more  complicated.    With  

50%  of  the  behaviors  in  the  category  relating  to  sexual  orientation,  it  is  entirely  probable  

that  singing  and  dancing,  in  particular,  operate  as  a  sort  of  social  obligation  for  gay  men  in  

the  same  way  that  sports  and  athletics  operate  as  a  social  obligation  for  straight  men.    

Much  like  the  pressure  straight  men  feel  to  behave  a  certain  way  and  engage  in  certain  

masculine  behaviors,  gay  men  might  also  feel  pressured  to  assimilate  into  a  “feminine”  

archetype  due  to  the  influence  of  society.      

 

 

Page 52: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     52  

Gender—Perceptions  of  Masculinity  and  Femininity  

Rather  than  rely  on  stereotypes  to  determine  the  gender  of  behaviors,  this  study  had  

participants  determine  the  masculinity  or  femininity  of  different  actions,  themselves.    In  

doing  so,  my  research  could  analyze  perception  of  behavior  as  a  unique  category  unto  itself  

and  compare  it  to  sexual  orientation.    In  other  words,  this  study  sought  to  determine  if  

there  was  a  relationship  between  sexual  orientation  and  how  individuals  interpreted  

behaviors.    In  the  end,  only  eleven  out  of  eighty-­‐two  cases  demonstrated  a  statistically  

significant  relationship  between  the  two,  only  one  more  valid  case  than  in  the  relationship  

between  sexual  orientation  and  frequency  of  behavior.  

Perhaps  the  strongest  reason  for  why  sexual  orientation  and  perception  of  behavior  

were  related  in  these  eleven  cases  refers  to  the  concept  of  validation.    Through  validation,  

an  individual  reaffirms  his  or  her  own  actions  as  “in  sync”  with  that  which  is  expected  of  his  

or  her  sex.    For  instance,  in  the  case  of  “wearing  makeup”  for  men,  as  a  respondent’s  

“gayness”  increases,  he  perceives  “wearing  makeup”  to  be  more  masculine.    Since  

respondents  perceived  “wearing  makeup”  to  be  very  feminine,  gay  men  who  wear  makeup  

might  validate  their  behavior  by  labeling  it  as  more  masculine  than  other  men  who  do  not  

wear  makeup.    The  same  kind  of  reasoning  could  apply  to  “dancing  ballet”  for  men,  where  

an  increase  in  “gayness”  correlated  with  a  behavior’s  masculinity.    Once  again,  since  

respondents  labeled  “dancing  ballet”  as  a  very  feminine  behavior,  gay  men  who  engage  in  

the  behavior  might  perceive  it  to  be  more  masculine  than  other  men  who  choose  not  to  

engage  in  the  behavior.  

On  the  other  hand,  validation  might  also  act  to  reaffirm  what  an  individual  chooses  

not  to  do.    For  example,  in  regards  to  “watching  action  films”  for  men—the  gayer  the  

Page 53: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     53  

individual,  the  more  likely  he  labeled  the  behavior  as  feminine.    In  this  scenario,  it  is  as  if  

the  gay  respondent  is  aware  that  most  people  perceive  “watching  action  films”  to  be  

masculine;  consequently,  since  he  does  not  engage  in  the  behavior,  he  validates  his  not  

watching  action  films  by  separating  the  behavior  from  masculinity.  

Conclusions  to  the  Report  

At  the  end  of  the  day,  I  truly  hoped  that  sexual  orientation  and  gender  performance  

had  nothing  to  do  with  one  another.    Because,  if  my  research  found  there  to  be  a  significant  

relationship  between  sexuality  and  behavior—if  gay  men,  gay  women,  straight  men,  and  

straight  women  were  all  supposed  to  act  a  certain  way—then  what  becomes  of  those  who  

do  not  conform?    Ultimately,  the  null  hypothesis—that  there  is  no  relationship  between  

sexual  orientation  and  gender  performance—was  correct  87.8%  of  the  time,  and  no  single  

behavior  correlated  with  sexual  orientation  for  both  men  and  women.  

If  for  nothing  else,  there  are  two  main  topics  worth  a  home  in  your  memory  as  a  

reader.    The  first  refers  to  the  “Sex  I  Desire  vs.  Sexual  Orientation”  paradox  discussed  

throughout  the  report.    In  modern  social  science,  we  as  researchers  attempt  to  make  room  

for  multiple  categories  among  subjects  once  perceived  as  binaries.    We  make  allowance  for  

both  a  third  biological  sex  called  intersex,  once  thought  of  as  male  or  female,  and  for  the  

fluidity  of  gender,  once  seen  as  purely  masculine  or  feminine.    We  attempted  to  do  

something  similar  with  the  Kinsey  Scale,  allowing  for  varying  degrees  of  heterosexuality  

and  homosexuality.    However,  as  20.6%  of  the  respondents  in  this  study  demonstrate,  

degrees  of  sexual  orientation  and  the  notion  of  bisexuality  cannot  serve  to  label  the  dozens  

upon  dozens  of  sexualities.    The  construct  of  sexual  orientation,  alone,  alienates  those  who  

identify  as  anything  other  than  purely  heterosexual  and  discourages  sexual  exploration.    

Page 54: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     54  

What  we  should  instead  seek  to  investigate  is  object  and  direction  of  sexual  desire,  itself—

the  actual  sex  a  person  sexually  desires.    Through  the  “Sex  I  Desire”  approach,  we  as  

researchers  can  avoid  alienating  different  cultures  with  equally  different  ideas  than  our  

own  of  what  it  means  to  “be”  gay  or  straight.  

Secondly,  the  overwhelming  lack  of  relationships  between  sexual  orientation  and  

gender  performance—87.8%  of  all  cases  observed  in  this  study—establishes  sexuality  and  

gender’s  recurrent  independence  from  one  another.    In  most  cases,  an  individual’s  choice  of  

behavior  should  not  in  any  way  be  indicative  of  his  or  her  sexual  orientation.    Conversely,  

we  as  a  society  must  take  measures  to  break  the  stereotypes  that  allow  us  to  assume  one’s  

sexuality  based  on  what  he  or  she  does.    At  the  end  of  the  day,  sexual  orientation  refers  to  

what  sex  somebody  sexually  desires.    Almost  88%  of  the  time,  there  exist  no  implications  

beyond  sexual  desire.    In  other  words,  sexual  orientation  is  about  how  you  think,  not  about  

how  you  act.    As  a  result,  we  cannot  effectively  observe  sexual  orientation—people  must  

present  it  to  us.  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Page 55: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     55  

Bibliography  

Almaguer,  T.  (1993).  Chicano  men:  A  cartography  of  homosexual  identity  and  behavior  In  

H.  Abelove  &  M.  A.  Barale  (Eds.),  The  Lesbian  and  Gay  Studies  Reader  (pp.  473-­‐486).  

New  York,  NY:  Routledge.  

Anderson,  E.  (2002).  Openly  gay  athletes:  Contesting  hegemonic  masculinity  in  a  

homophobic  environment.  Gender  &  Society,  16(6),  860-­‐877.  

Cahn,  S.  K.  (1993).  From  the  "muscle  moll"  to  the  "butch"  ballplayer:  Mannishness,  

lesbianism,  and  homophobia  in  u.s.  women's  sports.  Feminist  Studies,  19(2),  334-­‐343.  

D’Emilio,  J.  (1993).  Capitalism  and  gay  identity.  In  H.  Abelove  &  M.  A.  Barale  (Eds.),  The  

Lesbian  and  gay  studies  reader  (pp.  467-­‐476).  New  York,  NY:  Routledge.  

Koenig,  S.  (2002).  Walk  like  a  man:  Enactments  and  embodiments  of  masculinity  and  the  

potential  for  multiple  genders.  Journal  of  Homosexuality,  43(3-­‐4),  193-­‐200.  

Miceli,  M.  (2003).  In  the  trenches:  Lgbt  students  struggle  with  school  and  sexual  identity.  In  

M.  Stombler,  D.  M.  Baunach,  E.  O.  Burgess,  D.  Donnelly  &  W.  Simonds  (Eds.),  Sex  

Matters:  The  Sexuality  and  Society  Reader  (pp.  185-­‐193).  Boston,  MA:  Pearson.  

Newton,  E.,  &  Walton,  S.  (2000).  The  misunderstanding:  Toward  a  more  precise  sexual  

vocabulary.  In  M.  A.  Barale,  J.  Goldberg,  M.  Moon  &  E.  K.  Sedgwick  (Eds.),  Margaret  

Mead  Made  Me  Gay:  Personal  Essays,  Public  Ideas  (pp.  167-­‐175).  Durham,  NC:  Duke  

University  Press.  

Pascoe,  C.  J.  (2007).  Dude,  you're  a  fag:  Masculinity  and  sexuality  in  high  school.  (pp.  430-­‐

443).  Berkeley,  CA:  University  of  California  Press.  

Thompson,  B.  Y.  (2000).  Fence  sitters,  switch  hitters,  and  bi-­‐bi  girls:  An  exploration  of  hapa  

and  bisexual  identities.  Frontiers,  21(1/2),  171-­‐180.  

Page 56: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     56  

Zucker,  K.  J.  (2008).  Reflections  on  the  relation  between  sex-­‐typed  behavior  in  childhood  

and  sexual  orientation  in  adulthood.  Journal  of  Gay  &  Lesbian  Mental  Health,  12(1-­‐2),  

29-­‐59.  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Page 57: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     57  

Appendix  

Table  #1—  

Distribution  of  College  Major  by  Department  College  Department  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Accountancy   2.1%  Advanced  Studies  in  Education  and  Counseling   2.9%  Anthropology   0.5%  Art   3.7%  Asian  and  Asian-­‐American  Studies   0.3%  Astronomy  and  Physics   0.5%  Biological  Sciences   2.4%  Biomedical  and  Clinical  Engineering   0.5%  Business  Administration   0.3%  Chemical  Engineering   0.3%  Chemistry  and  Biochemistry   1.3%  Civil  Engineering  and  Construction  Engineering  Management   0.8%  Communication  Studies   4.3%  Communicative  Disorders   2.1%  Computer  Engineering  and  Computer  Science   1.6%  Criminal  Justice   2.1%  Dance   0.3%  Design   0.5%  Economics   0.5%  Electrical  Engineering   0.8%  Engineering  Technology   0.3%  English   4.3%  Environmental  Science  and  Policy   1.3%  Family  and  Consumer  Sciences   8.5%  Film  and  Electronic  Arts   2.9%  Finance   1.3%  Geography   0.3%  Geological  Sciences   0.5%  Health  Care  Administration   1.6%  Health  Science   3.7%  History   1.6%  Human  Development   1.6%  Information  Systems   0.5%  International  Business   0.3%  Journalism  and  Mass  Communication   0.5%  Kinesiology   3.7%  Liberal  Studies   1.1%  Linguistics   0.5%  

Page 58: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

Sexual  Orientation  and  Gender  Performance     58  

Management  and  Human  Resources  Management   5.1%  Marketing   1.1%  Mathematics  and  Statistics   0.5%  Mechanical  and  Aerospace  Engineering   1.9%  Music   13.3%  Nursing   1.9%  Political  Science   0.8%  Psychology   4.3%  Recreation  and  Leisure  Studies   0.8%  Religious  Studies   0.3%  Romance,  German,  Russian  Languages  and  Literatures   0.5%  Social  Work   0.5%  Sociology   3.2%  Theatre  Arts   0.5%  Women’s  Gender  and  Sexuality  Studies   0.3%  More  than  one  major   1.9%  Undecided   0.8%  

 Table  #2—  

Distribution  of  Occupation  Occupation  of  the  Respondent   Percentage  of  Respondents  Accountancy  /  Finance  /  Stocks   1.0%  Customer  Service  /  Human  Relations   1.6%  Education  /  Counseling  /  Childcare   5.7%  Entertainment  /  Performing  Arts   2.1%  Fashion  /  Design   1.0%  Food  Service  /  Restaurant  /  Lodging   8.0%  Health  Care  /  Medicine   0.3%  Law  Enforcement  /  Security   1.0%  Management  /  Ownership   1.3%  Manual  Labor   0.8%  Military   0.5%  Office  Work  /  Clerical  Work   1.8%  Real  Estate  /  Leasing   0.5%  Retail  /  Sales   8.3%  Technology  /  Engineering   0.3%  Transportation   0.5%  Volunteering  /  Internship  /  Assistant   2.6%  Writing  /  Editing  /  Media   0.3%  More  than  one  job   0.8%  Missing   61.7%  

Page 59: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

     

Survey  A    

For  the  following  list  of  behaviors,  please  indicate  how  masculine  or  feminine  that  you  personally  feel  each  behavior  is.    There  is  no  right  or  wrong  answer,  and  you  are  not  in  any  way  obligated  to  complete  this  survey.    You  may  skip  a  question  should  you  feel  uncomfortable  providing  an  answer  to  it.    Circle  a  number  between  1  and  5  based  on  the  following  spectrum:  

               1              2       3                      4                                  5                Very  Masculine                Slightly  More                    Neutral                          Slightly  More                Very  Feminine                            Masculine                                        Feminine  

Playing  basketball—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Wearing  pink  clothing—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Singing  in  choir—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Wearing  tight  clothing—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Playing  football—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Penetrating  during  sex—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Watching  action  films—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Writing  love  letters—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Speaking  slowly—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Using  online  dating  websites—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Playing  tennis—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Listening  to  pop  music—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Receiving  pain  for  sexual  pleasure—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Wearing  blue  clothing—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Drinking  beer—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Cooking  food—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Being  penetrated  during  sex—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Working  out—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Playing  video  games—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Dancing  ballet—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Receiving  oral  sex—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Playing  softball—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Drinking  tequila—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5                    

Page 60: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

     

             1              2       3                      4                                  5                Very  Masculine                Slightly  More                    Neutral                          Slightly  More                Very  Feminine                            Masculine                                        Feminine  

Inflicting  pain  for  sexual  pleasure—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Wearing  baggy  clothing—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Talking  with  your  hands—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Managing  finances—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Watching  romantic  films—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Eating  salad—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Dancing  hip-­‐hop—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Performing  oral  sex—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Playing  hockey—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Listening  to  rock  music—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Paying  for  dates—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Singing  in  a  band—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Speaking  quickly—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Drinking  whiskey—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Cleaning  the  house—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Wearing  makeup—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Eating  steak—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5  Styling  your  hair—                  1              2       3                      4                                  5    In  your  own  opinion,  how  masculine  or  feminine  do  you  think  you  are  (based  on  the  same  scale)?                  1              2       3                      4                                  5    Demographics:  

Age:  ___________________      College  Major:  __________________________  Occupation  (if  applicable):  __________________  

Class  Standing:    Freshman    Sophomore    Junior    Senior    Super  Senior    Graduate  Student  

Race:    Non-­‐Hispanic  White    Hispanic  White    Black    Native  American    Asian    Other  

Religion:    Christianity    Islam    Judaism    Buddhism    Hinduism    Non-­‐Religious    Other  

Sex:    Male    Female    Transgendered    Intersexed    Other  

What  sex  do  you  sexually  desire  (circle  the  most  appropriate  number)?  

    0     1     2     3     4     5                          6                Only  the                      Mostly  the                Slightly  more              Both  sexes            Slightly  more              Mostly  the                Only  the        opposite  sex          opposite  sex        the  opposite  sex              equally                  the  same  sex                  same  sex                  same  sex

Page 61: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

     

Survey  B    

For  the  following  list  of  behaviors,  please  indicate  how  often  that  you  personally  engage  in  each  behavior.    There  is  no  right  or  wrong  answer,  and  you  are  not  in  any  way  obligated  to  complete  this  survey.    You  may  skip  a  question  should  you  feel  uncomfortable  providing  an  answer  to  it.    Check  the  most  appropriate  box  based  on  the  following  four  responses:    

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never    Do  you  play  basketball?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  wear  pink  clothing?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  sing  in  choir?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  wear  tight  clothing?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  play  football?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  penetrate  during  sex?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  watch  action  films?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  write  love  letters?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  speak  slowly?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  use  online  dating  websites?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  play  tennis?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  listen  to  pop  music?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  receive  pain  for  sexual  pleasure?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  wear  blue  clothing?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  drink  beer?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  cook  food?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  get  penetrated  during  sex?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  work  out?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  play  video  games?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  dance  ballet?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  receive  oral  sex?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  play  softball?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  Do  you  drink  tequila?  

 Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Page 62: The Correlations Between Sexual Orientation and Gender Performance

     

Do  you  inflict  pain  for  sexual  pleasure?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  wear  baggy  clothing?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  talk  with  your  hands?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  manage  your  finances?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  watch  romantic  films?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  eat  salad?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  dance  hip-­‐hop?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  perform  oral  sex?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  play  hockey?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  listen  to  rock  music?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  pay  for  dates?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  sing  in  a  band?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  speak  quickly?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  drink  whiskey?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  clean  the  house?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  wear  makeup?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  eat  steak?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

Do  you  style  your  hair?    Often      Sometimes                Rarely      Never  

 In  your  own  opinion,  how  masculine  or  feminine  do  you  think  you  are?  

               1              2       3                      4                                  5                Very  Masculine                Slightly  More                    Neutral                          Slightly  More                Very  Feminine                            Masculine                                        Feminine    Demographics:  

Age:  ___________________      College  Major:  __________________________    Occupation  (if  applicable):  __________________  

Class  Standing:    Freshman    Sophomore    Junior    Senior    Super  Senior    Graduate  Student  

Race:    Non-­‐Hispanic  White    Hispanic  White    Black    Native  American    Asian    Other  

Religion:    Christianity    Islam    Judaism    Buddhism    Hinduism    Non-­‐Religious    Other  

Sex:    Male    Female    Transgendered    Intersexed    Other  

What  sex  do  you  sexually  desire  (circle  the  most  appropriate  number)?  

    0     1     2     3     4     5                          6                Only  the                      Mostly  the                Slightly  more              Both  sexes            Slightly  more              Mostly  the                Only  the        opposite  sex          opposite  sex        the  opposite  sex              equally                  the  same  sex                  same  sex                  same  sex