Top Banner
Task-switching and ERPs 1 This is the unedited authors’ version of a paper published elsewhere. This work should be cited as follows: Capizzi M., Feher K., Penolazzi B., Vallesi A. (in press). Taskswitching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: an eventrelated potential study. Biological Psychology http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.011. This version of the article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the Biological Psychology journal. It is not the copy of record. Running Head: Task-switching and ERPs Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study Mariagrazia Capizzi a,# , Kristoffer Fehér b , Barbara Penolazzi c,d , Antonino Vallesi a,e a Department of Neuroscience, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy b Department of Psychiatric Neurophysiology, University Hospital of Psychiatry, University of Bern, Switzerland c Department of General Psychology, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy d Department of Life Sciences, Università di Trieste, Trieste, Italy e Centro di Neuroscienze Cognitive, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy # Corresponding Author’s address: Mariagrazia Capizzi Department of Neuroscience, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy Via Giustiniani, 5, 35128 Padova Phone: +39 049 821 7181 E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected] E-mails: Fehér K. ([email protected]), Penolazzi B. (barbara.penolazzi@unipd.it), Vallesi A. ([email protected]) Abstract
37

Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

May 15, 2023

Download

Documents

Tommaso Sitzia
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   1  

This  is  the  unedited  authors’  version  of  a  paper  published  elsewhere.  This  work  should  be  cited  as  follows:  Capizzi  M.,  Feher  K.,  Penolazzi  B.,  Vallesi  A.  (in  press).  Task-­‐switching  preparation  across  semantic  and  spatial  domains:  an  event-­‐related  potential  study.  Biological  Psychology  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.011.    This  version  of  the  article  may  not  exactly  replicate  the  final  version  published  in  the  Biological  Psychology  journal.  It  is  not  the  copy  of  record.  

Running Head: Task-switching and ERPs

Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential

study

Mariagrazia Capizzia,#, Kristoffer Fehérb, Barbara Penolazzic,d, Antonino Vallesia,e

aDepartment of Neuroscience, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy

bDepartment of Psychiatric Neurophysiology, University Hospital of Psychiatry, University of Bern, Switzerland

cDepartment of General Psychology, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy

dDepartment of Life Sciences, Università di Trieste, Trieste, Italy

eCentro di Neuroscienze Cognitive, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy

#Corresponding Author’s address:

Mariagrazia Capizzi

Department of Neuroscience, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy

Via Giustiniani, 5, 35128 Padova

Phone: +39 049 821 7181

E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected]

E-mails: Fehér K. ([email protected]), Penolazzi B. ([email protected]),

Vallesi A. ([email protected])

Abstract

Page 2: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   2  

Previous event-related potential (ERP) studies have identified the specific electrophysiological

markers of advance preparation in cued task-switching paradigms. However, it is not completely

clear yet whether there is a single task-independent preparatory mechanism for task-switching or

whether preparation for a switch can be selectively influenced by the domain of the task to be

performed. To address this question, we employed a cued-task switching paradigm requiring

participants to repeat or to switch between a semantic and a spatial task. The behavioural results

showed a significant switch cost for both domains. The ERP findings, however, revealed that switch

and repeat trials for semantic and spatial domains differed in the amplitude modulation of an early

P2 and a sustained negativity both expressed over fronto-central scalp regions. Further differences

between the two domains also emerged over posterior-parietal electrodes. This pattern of data thus

shows that advance preparation in task-switching can be selectively modulated by the domain of the

task to be performed.

Keywords: ERPs; advance preparation; task-set; semantic processing; spatial processing.

Page 3: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   3  

1. Introduction

A hallmark of cognitive control is the ability to flexibly switch between tasks. One of the most

used tools to investigate such ability is the task-switching paradigm in which participants have to

repeat the same task or to switch between different ones. The general finding for task-switching

paradigms is that response time (RT) gets longer and accuracy decreases for switch trials as

compared to repeat trials, a phenomenon known as the “switch cost” (see Kiesel et al., 2010;

Monsell, 2003, for reviews). The switch cost is reduced but not completely eliminated even by

providing participants in advance with an explicit cue that instructs them to change task (i.e., the

cued task-switching paradigm; Meiran, 1996). The observation that a residual switch cost still

emerges with preparation intervals longer than 1 sec (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) suggests that

advance preparation cannot fully compensate for the behavioural cost of alternating between

different tasks (see Jamadar et al., 2010a).

Some theories explain the switch cost during the cued task-switching paradigm by assuming that

an active task-set reconfiguration process would be implemented for switch trials as compared to

repeat trials in order to prioritize the new task-set against the previous one (e.g., Rogers & Monsell,

1995). Such a reconfiguration process is supposed to be time-consuming and highly dependent on

executive control. Support for this claim comes from the finding of a reduction of the switch cost

when the cue-target interval is increased and more time can thus be devoted to advance preparation.

Alternatively, other researchers attribute the switch cost to priming or other memory interference

processes from the previous task-set that would not necessarily entail executive control (e.g.,

Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994; Wylie & Allport, 2000). This idea is strengthened by the

observation that the switch cost is reduced with longer inter-trial intervals, which has been taken as

evidence that allowing ample time before the subsequent trial is presented favors the spontaneous

decay of the previous task-set interference. More recently, however, it is accepted that both

reconfiguration and interference processes would contribute to the switch cost (e.g.,

Vandierendonck, Liefooghe, & Verbruggen, 2010).

Page 4: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   4  

A number of studies using event-related potentials (ERPs) support the role of an active task-set

reconfiguration process taking place during the cue-target interval. The excellent high temporal

resolution of ERPs indeed allows researchers to track the time course of switch and repeat trials that

follow the presentation of the cue and to compare the neural activity associated with each task

condition. In such a way, it is possible to determine whether, and to what extent, switch and repeat

trials can be differentiated during the preparation interval that precedes task performance.

Two main ERP components have been often associated with task-switching effects during the

preparation interval: a sustained posterior positivity, sometimes termed “differential switch

positivity” or simply “switch positivity” (e.g., Jamadar et al., 2010a; Karayanidis et al., 2011),

emerging around 300-400 ms after cue onset, and a concurrent or later sustained frontal negativity

(e.g., Astle, Jackson, & Swainson, 2008; Lavric, Mizon, & Monsell, 2008). Both brain potentials

are typically larger for switch as compared to repeat trials (see De Baene & Brass, 2014;

Karayanidis et al., 2010, for reviews), although some studies also reported an enhanced frontal

negativity for repeat trials before target onset (e.g., Nicholson, Karayanidis, Poboka, Heathcote, &

Michie, 2005).

The switch positivity has been replicated across different studies and task manipulations (e.g.,

Kieffaber & Hetrick, 2005; Kopp, Lange, Howe, & Wessel, 2014; Li, Wang, Zhao, & Fogelson,

2012; Miniussi, Marzi, & Nobre, 2005; Nicholson et al., 2005; Rushworth, Passingham, & Nobre,

2002). A general consensus exists on the fact that this positivity would reflect anticipatory task-set

reconfiguration processes that would be especially related to switch trials. In support of this

interpretation, Karayanidis and colleagues (2011; see also Lavric, Mizon, & Monsell, 2008) found

faster switch responses to be associated with larger amplitude of the switch positivity as compared

to slower switch responses, suggesting that such a slow positivity is linked to “a switch-specific

reconfiguration process” (p. 567).

Unlike the switch positivity, the functional meaning of the frontal negativity appears more

controversial, perhaps due to the fact that this brain potential has been reported in fewer studies as

Page 5: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   5  

compared to the switch positivity. Furthermore, most of the studies observing the frontal negativity

have used a common average reference, which led to the suggestion that the frontal negativity and

the switch positivity could represent the negative and the positive components of a dipolar

distribution, respectively (see De Baene & Brass, 2014; Jamadar et al., 2010a; Karayanidis et al.,

2010; Lavric et al., 2008).

However, contrary to this claim, Astle, Jackson and Swainson (2008) found that the two brain

potentials, which were measured in the same time interval, could be dissociated in task-switching

paradigms that manipulated advance preparation of different response-sets. That is, whereas the

switch positivity was present for both overt and covert (i.e., mental counting) responses, the frontal

negativity was observed only when the task required an overt response. Moreover, in a study using

a go/no-go version of the task-switching paradigm (Astle, Jackson, & Swainson, 2006), it was

found that only the switch positivity was present following both go and no-go trials. By contrast,

there was no difference in the frontal negativity between switch and repeat trials after a no-go trial,

which suggested that this potential was sensitive to the fact that the response-set had been inhibited

in the previous trial and this effect carried over to the current trial. Taking the above studies into

account, a plausible explanation for the frontal negativity would be thus related to advance

preparation of overt response-set processes (see Karayanidis et al., 2010).

In addition to these sustained positive- and negative-going potentials, another reliable ERP

signature often reported in the task-switching literature is an early cue-locked fronto-central

positivity (P2), emerging approximately at 200 ms after cue onset, which is usually larger following

a switch cue relative to a repeat cue (e.g., Finke, Escera, & Barceló, 2012; Periáñez & Barceló,

2009; West, Langley, & Bailey, 2011). The enhanced P2 amplitude for switch trials has been

generally attributed to the functioning of an early task-set updating process that would rapidly

“detect” a relevant change in the task to be performed (see also De Baene & Brass, 2014).

To sum up, from this brief review of the main electrophysiological correlates of advance

preparation in cued task-switching paradigms, it seems clear that preparing for a switch as

Page 6: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   6  

compared to preparing for a repeat trial can differentially modulate some specific brain potentials

developing during the cue-target interval. Most of the previous task-switching studies have focused

on investigating which cognitive factors may influence the ERP markers of advance preparation.

Among others, it has been shown that the electrophysiological correlates of task-switching

preparation are sensitive to: 1) the amount of information conveyed by the cue (e.g., Karayanidis et

al., 2009; Nicholson et al., 2006), 2) the duration of cue-target and inter-trial intervals (e.g., Li et al.,

2012; Nicholson et al., 2005), 3) the specific requirements (go vs. no/go) for response selection

(e.g., Astle et al., 2006; Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2011; Jamadar et al., 2010b) and 4) the

participants’ performance (fast vs. slow switch responses) in switching between tasks (e.g.,

Karayanidis et al., 2011; Lavric et al., 2008).

Much less is known about the role played by the domain of the tasks that are manipulated in cued

task-switching paradigms. In other words, it is still unclear whether preparation for a task-switching

is accomplished by a single, task-independent, central mechanism or whether it relies on different

mechanisms according to the specific domain of the task to perform. Such a gap is mainly due to

the fact that previous ERP studies have usually focused on the contrast between switch vs. repeat

trials pooling over the tasks among which participants had to switch. This choice has been often

motivated by the finding of a null behavioural interaction between the requirements to switch/repeat

task and the specific task rules to be implemented, such that the ERP data have been averaged

across the different tasks in order to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of switch and repeat trials

(e.g., Goffaux et al. 2006; Karayanidis et al., 2009; Nicholson et al., 2006). As a consequence, it is

not completely clear to date whether task-switching preparation is domain-independent or rather it

is influenced by the domain of the task to be performed (see also Ravizza & Carter, 2008).

Among the few researchers who have investigated task-switching across different tasks, Hsieh

and Wu (2011; see also Hsieh, Wu, & Lin, 2014) compared the electrophysiological correlates of

advance preparation in task-switching between stimulus-dimensions vs. response-mappings. The

authors reported both common and distinct modulations of cue-locked ERPs associated with the

Page 7: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   7  

two task-switching types, which suggests the presence of both shared and unique mechanisms

underlying preparation to shift across different tasks.

An issue which is still poorly explored, however, is the comparison of task-switching between

tasks that are typically processed in distinct brain regions, like for instance semantic and spatial

tasks, which are known to be mainly processed on the left and right hemisphere, respectively (e.g.,

Corbetta & Shulman, 2011; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013; Thompson-Schill et al., 1997). In the

present study, we asked whether there might be different preparatory mechanisms when shifting, on

a trial-by-trial basis, between tasks that require participants to make a spatial decision vs. tasks

requiring a semantic decision. To our knowledge, only a previous study by Miniussi, Marzi and

Nobre (2005) tackled a similar research question using a cued task-switching paradigm. In their

experiment a symbolic cue predicted, with 80% validity, the stimulus-dependent task to be

performed on any given trial: a lexical-decision task (i.e., to decide whether a letter string was a real

word or not) or an angle-decision one (i.e., to decide whether an angle was acute or obtuse). The

authors found a similar scalp distribution of the switch positivity for verbal and spatial tasks, which

pointed to the conclusion that task-switching preparation would draw at least on some common

task-independent processes. Nevertheless, another key finding in Miniussi and colleagues’ (2005)

study was that frontal and parietal modulations after a cue switch were larger in the verbal task as

compared to the spatial one. This result thus suggests that the domain of the task to be performed

may also influence general task-switching preparation processes.

To further explore the electrophysiological correlates of task-switching preparation across

different domains, in the present context we decided to use a cued task-switching paradigm that,

unlike Miniussi and colleagues’ (2005) study, implemented the same stimulus materials for both

semantic and spatial tasks. This was done to maximally strengthen task-set reconfiguration for the

two domains during the preparation interval, as the appearance of the same stimuli for both

semantic and spatial tasks reduced the possibility of an additional later task-set reconfiguration

process afforded by the identity of the stimuli itself. Moreover, using exactly the same stimuli,

Page 8: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   8  

while manipulating the cognitive operations underlying different domains, allowed minimizing the

influence that the type of material could also exert on the electrophysiological correlates of task-

switching preparation. Accordingly, we designed a semantic task and a spatial task in which the

stimuli consisted of several animal pictures belonging to two semantic categories (i.e., “preys” and

“predators”) and arranged in such a way to form different spatial diagonals. The participants’ task

was to identify the deviant set of animals (either “preys” or “predators”) or the deviant angle as

compared to two other sets of animals belonging to the same semantic category or to two other

equal spatial configurations, respectively. Each task was predicted with full certainty by two

auditory cues that were assigned to the semantic and spatial domains in a counterbalanced order

across participants.

While participants performed the task, continuous electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was

recorded. To determine whether task-switching preparation differs according to the domain of the

tasks being switched, we computed three cue-locked ERP components (P2, frontal negativity and

switch positivity) that, as detailed above, have been shown to be sensitive to the requirement to shift

task being usually larger for switch than for repeat trials. On the basis of prior cued task-switching

studies, we expected to observe a reliable RT switch cost for both semantic and spatial domains. As

regards the ERP data, two main predictions could be put forward. If task-switching preparation

relies on a common task-independent mechanism, we expected to replicate the finding of larger P2,

frontal negativity and switch positivity amplitudes for switch trials as compared to repeat trials in

both domains. Conversely, if updating and/or task-set reconfiguration processes would differ

between the two domains, thus selectively influencing task-switching preparation, a general

prediction would be that semantic and spatial rules should differentially modulate the amplitude

and/or time course of the electrophysiological indexes of switch and repeat trials within the cue-

target preparation interval.

2. Method

Page 9: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   9  

2.1. Participants. Twenty-two volunteers took part in the experiment in exchange for course

credits or cash payment of 20 euro. All participants gave informed consent prior to their inclusion in

the study. They reported to have normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and normal hearing.

None of the participants had any history of drug or alcohol abuse, or history of psychiatric,

neurological or other medical illness. The study was approved by the Bioethical Committee of the

Azienda Ospedaliera di Padova and was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of

Helsinki. Data from two participants were discarded because of excessive noise in the EEG

recording. We also discarded data from two left-handed participants (average score: -92.5, range:

45-100, in the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) as they displayed a different ERP

lateralization pattern as compared to the right-handed participants (average score: 90). The data

from the remaining eighteen participants (mean age: 26.4 years, range: 20-46 years, 13 females)

were used for both behavioural and ERP analyses.

2.2. Apparatus and stimuli. Stimulus presentation and response collection were controlled by E-

prime 2 software (Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, 2002) running on a personal computer

connected to a 19″ LCD monitor. This computer was interconnected to an Intel Core laptop

computer recording continuous EEG. The stimulus materials consisted of two auditory cue stimuli,

comprising a high pitch sound with a frequency of 1500 Hz and a low pitch sound with a frequency

of 200 Hz, and of eighteen visual target stimuli that depicted land-living mammals subdivided into

9 prey and 9 predator animals. Only four-legged animals were included. All animals unambiguously

faced right and were slightly tilted (i.e., at 15° in a clockwise manner). The animals were presented

into three white circles that were arranged in a row and displayed in the center of the screen against

a grey background. Each circle contained three identical animal pictures that were positioned in

such a way to form a diagonal, with one picture positioned in the center of the circle and the other

peripheral two displayed at a distance of 2.3 cm from the central image (see Figure 1).

Page 10: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   10  

----------------------------------------------------------

PLEASE INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE

----------------------------------------------------------

The angles of the diagonal resulting from the arrangement of the three animal pictures varied

randomly between the values of 22.5°/202.5°, 45°/225°, 67.5°/247.5°, 112.5°/292.5°,135°/315° and

157.5°/337.5° from an imaginary horizontal line across the circle (the slashes mean that the angles

denote the same diagonal arrangement). The deviation from this angle could be ± 22.5° away from

the other two angles. This means, for instance, that the angle of 45° could be attained by adding

22.5° to 22.5° or by subtracting 22.5° from 67.5. Horizontal and vertical arrangements were not

included to avoid any pop-out effect of the deviant angle that could dramatically ease the spatial

task.

2.3. Procedure and Task. The task was a cued task-switching paradigm, in which an auditory

cue preceded each target presentation indicating the semantic task or the spatial task. For half of the

participants, the high pitch sound was associated with the semantic rule and the low pitch sound

with the spatial rule. For the other half of the participants, the reverse associations were used. A trial

started with the presentation of the auditory cue that was played for 300 ms via two loudspeakers

(Yamaha NX-50) located on both sides of the screen. The sound intensity was set at a comfortable

level (i.e., ¼ of the maximum volume) that was maintained constant for all the participants.

Following a fixed time interval of 1900 ms after the cue offset, the target stimuli were then

displayed for 2200 ms. Thus, there were 2200 ms from cue onset to target onset (see Androver-Roig

& Barceló, 2010, for a quite similar interval in a modified task-cueing version of the Winsconsin

Card Sorting Test). The employment of such a long cue-target interval was aimed at enabling

participants to fully develop advance preparation before target onset, thus avoiding overlapping

activity with subsequent target-related processing. Along the same line, we also decided not to vary

Page 11: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   11  

the duration of cue-target interval either between blocks of trials, since prior work has shown that

participants may rely on different preparatory strategies for short vs. long block intervals

(Nicholson et al., 2005), or within blocks of trials, not to add a further shift between short and long

intervals to our main task-switching manipulation and in order to avoid any variable foreperiod

effects (e.g., Vallesi, Lozano & Correa, 2013).

The participants’ task was to identify which of the three circles showed a deviant item with

respect to the other two according to a semantic or a spatial rule. In the semantic task, participants

had to identify the circle containing the deviant animal. For instance, if there were two circles

containing prey animals and only one circle with a predator, they had to indicate the circle

displaying the predator irrespective of the diagonal arrangement of the animals (see Figure 1 in

which the correct response is the circle on the right). By contrast, in the spatial task, participants had

to ignore the semantic category of the stimuli and focus on their spatial arrangement by indicating

the circle displaying a deviant angle as compared to the other two circles (in Figure 1 the correct

response corresponds to the circle on the left). For each task condition there was only one

univocally correct response, in the sense that the circle displaying the deviant angle could not also

contain the deviant animal or vice versa. Switch/repeat trials were equally probable and

administered randomly.

Participants responded by pressing the “j”, the “k” or the “l” keys on the computer keyboard with

the index, middle or ring finger of their right hand or, in different blocks, by pressing the “s”, the

“d” or the “f” keys with the ring, middle or index finger of their left hand, respectively. Each key

was spatially associated with each circle so that the first key was to be responded to if the deviant

circle was the one positioned on the left, the middle key if the deviant circle was the central one,

and the third key if the deviant circle was positioned on the right. The deadline for the response was

2500 ms after stimulus onset. Following a variable inter-trial interval ranging from 1000 to 1500

ms, the next trial began.

Page 12: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   12  

The experiment consisted of 8 blocks of 50 trials each. Four blocks required a left-hand response

and the remaining four a right-hand response. Half of the participants started with the left hand and

the other half with the right hand. Before the EEG session, participants were administered with both

a tutorial, which carefully explained the task, and a practice phase. At the beginning of the practice

phase, all the animal pictures were presented one by one with their name below each figure.

Participants were asked to mentally classify them as preys or predators and to let the experimenter

know in case of any doubt about the corresponding semantic category. Following this phase, they

were to perform each single-task separately before practicing the two tasks together. Half of the

participants started with the spatial task and the other half with the semantic task. Moreover, in

order to familiarize themselves with the stimulus-response mapping, they also had to change from

left to right hand, and vice versa, through the practice blocks. Specifically, half of the participants

used the left hand in the two single-task blocks and the right hand in the task-switching block. The

remaining participants started with the right hand in the single-task blocks and changed to the left

hand in the task-switching block.

Each single-task practice block consisted of 30 trials. At the end of each block, participants were

informed about their mean RTs and mean accuracy rates. If accuracy was below 66% after the first

block, subsequent mini blocks of 15 trials each were presented until participants managed to

perform the task above 66%. If accuracy was between 66% and 80%, participants could decide to

receive more training or not. If, instead, accuracy was above 80%, the program automatically

passed on the next test block. The same criteria applied to the task-switching practice, which

consisted of a first block of 40 trials and of subsequent mini blocks of 20 trials each administered

only if participants’ performance failed to reach the threshold of 66% of accuracy (or if participants

wished to practice more when accuracy was between 66% and 80%).

In the practice phase, participants received a feedback which varied according to their

performance after each trial (the Italian word for “Correct” displayed in blue or the Italian words for

“Incorrect” and “No response. Try to be faster” displayed in red for 1500 ms). In addition to the

Page 13: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   13  

practice phase, both at the beginning of the EEG session and after the first four blocks of

experimental trials (i.e., when the hand used to respond had to be changed), participants were

briefly presented with a short practice block of 8 trials. Like what stated above, they had to reach

the threshold of 66% of accuracy to start the proper experimental session. A short rest between

blocks of trials was allowed.

2.4. EEG recording. Participants were seated in front of the computer monitor and were

instructed to avoid eye blinks and movements during cue and stimulus presentation. The EEG was

recorded using BrainAmp amplifiers (Brain Products, Munich, Germany) from 64 Ag/AgCl

electrodes that were mounted on an elastic cap (EASYCAP GmbH, Germany) according to the

extended 10-20 system. Electrooculographic (EOG) activity was recorded with an electrode placed

under the left eye and was also monitored through the scalp electrodes positioned in the proximity

of both eyes. Impedances for each channel were measured and adjusted until they were kept below

10 kΩ before testing. All electrodes were referenced to FCz during the recording and were re-

referenced off-line to the average of all of the electrodes. An electrode positioned at AFz served as

the ground electrode. Raw data were band-pass filtered between 0.1-100 Hz and digitized at a

sampling rate of 500 Hz.

3. Analysis

3.1. Behavioural data analysis. Data from practice trials, the first trial of each block, trials with

errors and trials without responses were discarded from the RT analysis. Mean RTs for correct

responses and Accuracy (percentage of correct responses) were analyzed separately through

repeated-measures ANOVAs with Hand (left, right), Domain (semantic, spatial) and Switching

from the previous task (repeat, switch) as within-participants factors.

Page 14: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   14  

3.2. Electrophysiological data analysis. Signal pre-processing was performed using BrainVision

Analyzer 2.0 (Brain Products GmbH). Raw data were first band-pass filtered off-line with cutoffs at

0.1 and 30 Hz (Butterworth zero phase, 12 dB/Oct). An ocular correction algorithm based on

independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the continuous data to correct for eye

movements and blink activity. Electrodes that were consistently bad during the entire recording

were replaced through spherical spline interpolation (Perrin, Pernier, Bertrand, & Echallier, 1989).

Overall, only four electrodes (T8, TP10, CPz and AF8) were interpolated for three participants (one

electrode each for two participants and two electrodes for another participant). The data were then

re-referenced to the average of all electrodes. They were finally segmented into epochs [-200, 2200

ms] with respect to the cue onset (baseline ±50 ms around the cue-onset; see Jamadar et al., 2010a).

Epochs were discarded if, on any channel, absolute difference between two sampling points

exceeded 30 µV/ms, if peak-to-peak deflections in a segment exceeded ±100 µV within intervals of

200 ms, if amplitude exceeded a value of ±80 µV and if activity was lower than 0.1 µV within

intervals of 200 ms. Finally, each epoch was visually inspected and trials containing any residual

artifact were manually removed. After artifact rejection, the total numbers of artifact-free trials per

condition were: 1394 for the semantic-repeat, 1286 for the semantic-switch, 1226 for the spatial-

repeat and 1243 for the spatial-switch condition. A minimum criterion of 28 artifact-free trials per

condition and participant was required to ensure a sufficient signal-to-noise ratio. Only trials with

correct behavioural responses were analysed. In addition, practice trials and the first trial of each

block were excluded from further analysis. Four separate grand average waveforms were

constructed relative to cue categories: semantic-repeat, semantic-switch, spatial-repeat and spatial-

switch, according to whether semantic and spatial trials were signaled either by the same cue (i.e.,

“semantic-semantic” or “spatial-spatial” trial sequences) or by a different cue with respect to that

used in the previous trial (i.e., “spatial-semantic” or “semantic-spatial” sequences).

The ERP analysis focused on the following cue-locked potentials: frontal P2, frontal negativity

and switch positivity that were chosen on the basis of visual inspection of the grand average

Page 15: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   15  

waveforms and according to prior literature. The P2 amplitude was analyzed over fronto-central

electrodes (left: F1, F3, FC1, FC3; midline: Fz, FCz; right: F2, F4, FC2, FC4) in a time window

ranging from 200 to 240 ms after cue onset (see Finke, Escera, & Barceló, 2012 and West, Langley,

& Bailey, 2011, for very similar analysis windows and electrodes). A repeated-measures ANOVA

tested for amplitude differences in the P2 with Domain (semantic, spatial), Switching from the

previous task (repeat, switch) and Electrode side (left, midline, right) as within-participant factors.

Regarding the analysis of the frontal negativity and the switch positivity, ERP amplitudes were

measured from fronto-central (left: F1, F3, FC1, FC3; midline: Fz, FCz; right: F2, F4, FC2, FC4)

and posterior-parietal (left: P1, P3, PO3, PO7; midline: Pz, POz; right: P2, P4, PO4, PO8)

electrodes, respectively, where the two brain potentials were maximally expressed (e.g.,

Karayanidis et al., 2009; West, Langley, & Bailey, 2011). As depicted in Figures 3 and 4, negative

and positive waveforms over fronto-central and posterior-parietal scalp regions almost overlapped

in time. Therefore, we analyzed the frontal negativity and the switch positivity within the same

latency range. Three time bins of 600 ms each were selected in order to explore the time course of

switch and repeat trials across the whole cue-target interval: (1) 400-1000 ms, (2) 1000-1600 ms,

and (3) 1600-2200 ms. Amplitude differences over fronto-central and posterior-parietal regions

were tested using a five-way repeated-measures ANOVA with the within-participant factors of

Scalp region (fronto-central, posterior-parietal), Time bin (1, 2, 3), Domain (semantic, spatial),

Switching from the previous task (repeat, switch) and Electrode side (left, midline, right). For the

scalp region factor, we pooled over all the above-mentioned electrodes that were contained within

each region. For all ERP analyses, amplitude was calculated as the mean voltage measured across

the pooled electrodes that were included in a particular montage (e.g., left electrodes side) and

within the specified temporal window. Significant effects of Electrode side were reported only if

they interacted with Domain, Switching or both. The Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied

when sphericity assumption was violated according to the Mauchly’s test (Jennings & Wood 1976).

Corrected degrees of freedom and corrected probability values are reported.

Page 16: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   16  

For both ERP and behavioural analyses, post-hoc comparisons with the Tukey’s HSD test were

used to analyze both pair-wise comparisons within significant interactions and significant main

effects involving more than two levels.

4. Results

4.1. Behavioural results

4.1.1. RTs. The significant main effect of Switching [F(1,17)=20.55, p<.001, partial η2=.5]

showed that participants were slower when they had to switch from one task to another rather than

when the same task was repeated. The main effect of Domain was also significant [F(1,17)=34.31,

p<.0001, partial η2=.6] being RTs longer for the semantic domain than for the spatial domain. The

switch cost was not affected by the domain of the task to be performed as revealed by the non-

significant Domain x Switching interaction [F<.1] (see Figure 2.A).

----------------------------------------------------------

PLEASE INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE

----------------------------------------------------------

Although there was no asymmetrical switch cost in our data, as indexed by the lack of a

significant Domain x Switching interaction (e.g., Martin et al., 2011), we calculated for both

domains an index of task-switching by computing the RT difference between switch and repeat

trials (62 ms for the semantic domain vs. 58 ms for the spatial domain) in order to directly compare

the magnitude of the switch cost across the two domains. A paired two-tailed t-test on these

behavioural indexes confirmed no difference in the switch cost between semantic and spatial

domains [t(17)=.21, p=.83]. The main effect of Hand and all the interactions involving Hand as a

factor were not significant (all ps>.1). Accordingly, in the EEG analysis the data across the two

hands were collapsed in order to increase the signal-to-noise ratio.

Page 17: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   17  

4.1.2. Accuracy. The main effect of Switching [F(1,17)=6.54, p=.02, partial η2=.2] mirrored the

RTs data by showing that participants were less accurate in the switch condition as compared to the

repeat condition. There was also a significant main effect of Domain [F(1,17)=4.62, p=.04, partial

η2=.2] indicating that accuracy was higher for the semantic domain than for the spatial one, a result

that goes in the opposite direction with respect to the RTs data showing a speed-accuracy trade off.

The interaction between Domain and Switching was far from significance [F(1,17)=2.06, p=.1,

partial η2=.1] (see Figure 2.B). None of the remaining terms of the ANOVA reached statistical

significance (all ps>.1).

4.2. Electrophysiological results

Cue-locked ERPs over fronto-central and posterior-parietal electrodes as a function of repeat and

switch trials are displayed separately for the semantic and the spatial domain in Figures 3 and 4,

respectively.

The ANOVA on the frontal P2 amplitude showed only a marginal significant Domain x Switching

interaction [F(1,17)=3.83, p=.06, partial η2=.1]. Post-hoc comparisons for this interaction revealed

that repeat spatial trials tended to elicit a larger P2 amplitude as compared to switch trials (p=.06),

whereas there was no difference between repeat and switch trials in the semantic domain (p=.9).

The ANOVA carried out on fronto-central and posterior-parietal regions showed both a

significant main effect of Scalp region [F(1,17)=7.82, p=.01, partial η2=.3], indexing a more

positive voltage over posterior-parietal electrodes than over fronto-central ones, and a significant

main effect of Time bin [F(2,34)=7.31, p=.002, partial η2=.3] with a more negative voltage in the

first time bin as compared to the second one (p=.001). There was no difference between the second

and third time bins as well as between the first and third time bins (ps>.1). Scalp region interacted

with Time bin [F(1.36, 23.12)=37.49, p<.001, partial η2=.6], with Time bin and Switching [F(1.29,

21.96)=11.05, p=.002, partial η2=.3] and with Domain and Switching [F(1,17)=28.59, p<.001,

partial η2=.6]. Time bin also interacted with Switching [F(2,34)=5.81, p=.006, partial η2=.2] and

Page 18: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   18  

with Domain and Switching [F(2,34)=3.39, p=.04, partial η2=.1]. These two-way and three-way

interactions were better qualified by a significant four-way interaction between Scalp, Time bin,

Domain and Switching [F(2,34)=6.06, p=.005, partial η2=.2]. Post-hoc comparisons for this four-

way interaction showed the following results.

In the first time bin (400-1000 ms), there was no difference for the semantic domain between

repeat and switch trials in either the fronto-central or the posterior-parietal scalp region (ps>.6).

Conversely, for the spatial domain switch trials in the first time bin were associated with a larger

positivity over posterior-parietal electrodes (p=.0002) and with a concomitant larger negativity over

fronto-central ones (p=.001).

In the second time bin (1000-1600 ms), post-hoc comparisons showed that whereas for the spatial

domain the frontal negativity was larger for switch trials as compared to repeat trials (p=.006), an

opposite pattern was found for the semantic domain with a larger frontal negativity for repeat trials

as compared to switch trials (p=.003). There was no difference between switch and repeat trials

over the posterior-parietal region for both semantic and spatial domains (all ps>.1)

In the third time bin (1600-2200 ms), post-hoc comparisons for the spatial domain confirmed a

larger frontal negativity for switch trials as compared to repeat trials (p=.02) and no difference

between the two trial types over the posterior-parietal region (p=.8). For the semantic domain, a

larger frontal negativity was instead associated with repeat trials as compared to switch trials

(p=.0001), whereas the opposite was observed for the posterior-parietal region with more negative

amplitude for switch trials as compared to repeat trials (p=.003).

There were no other significant main effects or interactions except for the Time bin x Switching x

Electrode side interaction [F(2.04, 34.76)=4.14, p=.02, partial η2=.1]. Post-hoc comparisons for this

interaction showed that in the first time bin switch trials were associated with a larger positive

amplitude as compared to repeat trials over the left electrode side (p=.001), whereas there was no

difference between switch and repeat trials in either the midline and the right side (ps>.1). In both

Page 19: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   19  

the second and the third time bin, switch trials did not differ from repeat trials in either of the three

electrode sides (all ps>.1).

------------------------------------------------------------------------

PLEASE INSERT FIGURES 3 AND 4 ABOUT HERE

------------------------------------------------------------------------

As already mentioned in the Behavioural results section, although the magnitude of the switch

cost did not differ between the two domains, the semantic task was associated with longer RTs as

compared to the spatial one. Accordingly, one might wonder whether the differences observed

between the two domains in the modulation of the P2, frontal negativity and switch positivity truly

reflected the involvement of different task-switching preparation processes or whether instead they

could be partly accounted for by the different task demands exerted by semantic and spatial

domains. In order to explore this possibility, we re-analysed the ERP data after having equated RTs

between the two domains. To do so, we excluded from the analysis of each domain a subset of the

fastest and slowest responses for both switch and repeat task conditions with the constraint,

however, that no more than 15 trials for each participant and condition should be rejected in order to

maintain an acceptable number of trials for the subsequent ERP analysis.

Behaviourally, this re-analysis confirmed a significant main effect of Switching [F(1,17)=14.89,

p=.001, partial η2=.4], with longer RTs when participants had to switch from one task to another as

compared to when they had to repeat the same task. The lack of a significant Domain x Switching

interaction [F(1,17)=0.43, p=.5, partial η2=.02] showed that there was no evidence for the switch

cost to be affected by the domain of the task to perform. As expected, after having equated RTs for

the two domains, the main effect of Domain was no longer significant [F(1,17)=1.3, p=.2, partial

η2=.07]. Crucially for our goal, the ERP statistical analysis of the trials equated for the two domains

replicated all the main results reported above.

Page 20: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   20  

5. Discussion

The main aim of the current study was to investigate whether advance task-switching preparation

could be differentially modulated according to the domain of the task to be performed, that is,

semantic or spatial. In order to strengthen task-set reconfiguration processes for the two domains

and minimize the influence that the type of material could have on the electrophysiological

correlates of advance preparation, we designed a novel paradigm that allowed us to administer the

same set of stimuli for both semantic and spatial domains.

The behavioural results replicated previous task-switching studies, being RTs longer and accuracy

lower for switch trials as compared to repeat trials. The data also showed participants to be slower

in the semantic task than in the spatial task in line with former work (e.g., Miniussi et al., 2005).

Importantly, however, the magnitude of the switch cost did not differ between semantic and spatial

domains and accuracy was higher for the semantic one. These results thus show that the main effect

under investigation in the present work, namely, the switch cost was similar for the two domains

despite the differences observed between semantic and spatial tasks in both RTs and accuracy data.

To summarize the ERP results, waveforms elicited by semantic and spatial domains showed

several differences that emerged as early as 200 ms after cue onset in the latency range of the

frontal P2. Whereas for the spatial domain the P2 amplitude tended to be larger for repeat trials as

compared to switch trials, for the semantic domain the P2 amplitude was not modulated by the

requirement to repeat or to switch task. Later on, the two domains differed reliably in the

modulation of both frontal and posterior brain potentials.

On the one hand, when participants switched from the semantic to the spatial domain, during the

400-1000 ms time bin switch spatial trials elicited both a larger positivity over posterior-parietal

electrodes and a concomitant larger negativity over fronto-central ones. From 1000 ms until the end

of the cue-target interval, spatial ERPs were only characterized by a more sustained negativity for

switch trials as compared to repeat trials over the fronto-central scalp region. On the other hand,

when participants switched from the spatial to the semantic domain, there was no difference

Page 21: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   21  

between switch and repeat trials during the first time bin (400-1000 ms) over either the fronto-

central or the posterior-parietal region. A later modulation within the semantic domain was found

over the fronto-central scalp region, with repeat trials being associated with larger negative

amplitude as compared to switch trials, in both the second and the third time bin. Such frontal

modulations were accompanied, in the third time bin (1600-2200 ms), by a more sustained

negativity for switch trials than for repeat trials over the posterior-parietal region.

The first difference between switch and repeat trials as a function of domain was already observed

in the time range of the frontal P2, which tended to be larger for repeat trials than for switch trials in

the spatial domain only. Therefore, the modulation of the P2 in the spatial domain differed from

previous results of enhanced P2 amplitude following a switch cue as compared to a repeat cue (e.g.,

Finke, Escera, & Barceló, 2012; Periáñez & Barceló, 2009; West, Langley, & Bailey, 2011). One

might interpret this pattern of data as reflecting an encoding benefit due to repetition of the same

cue. In the task-switching literature, there has been indeed a great deal of controversy regarding the

fact that employing a 1:1 mapping between cues and tasks may confound task-switch costs with

cue-switch costs (e.g., Logan & Bundesen, 2003). To overcome this problem, some studies have

used a 2:1 mapping between cues and tasks in such a way that a cue change could be also associated

with a task repetition (e.g., Hsieh and Wu, 2011). Although we acknowledge that in our design a

task change was always preceded by a cue change, this cannot account for our frontal P2 results in

the spatial domain. First, the association between cues and tasks was counterbalanced across

participants and the cues differed minimally at the physical level. Second, and more importantly, if

the modulation of the P2 observed in the spatial domain was related to cue repetition, we should

have expected to find the same pattern also for the semantic domain, which was not the case. Such

results thus challenge the idea that the cue-locked frontal P2 would merely detect a change in the

task to be performed since, as shown here, it was sensitive both to the requirement to repeat the

same task and to the domain of the task that needed to be repeated.

Page 22: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   22  

Another ERP deflection that was influenced by our task requirements was the switch positivity,

which was maximally expressed over the posterior-parietal scalp region in a time window ranging

from 400 to 1000 ms (see Figures 3 and 4, middle panel). Several studies have already highlighted

the importance of this brain potential in anticipatory preparation for a change in task (e.g.,

Karayanidis et al., 2010). Within the framework of the task-set reconfiguration theory, the presence

of a larger posterior positivity for switch trials vs. repeat trials would index the active

reconfiguration of the new task-set against the previous irrelevant one (e.g., Nicholson et al., 2005).

Hence, our finding of an enhanced positivity for switch spatial trials as compared to repeat ones fits

well with this account. However, it has been proposed that advance reconfiguration could also take

place on repeat trials, although to a lesser extent than what required in reconfiguring the new task-

set on switch trials. This would be further encouraged when switch and repeat trials have the same

probability of being presented within a block (e.g., Brass & von Cramon, 2004; Nicholson et al.,

2005). Supporting this proposal, repeat trials that are intermixed with switch trials have been found

to elicit a larger positivity as compared to all-repeat trials presented on single-task baseline blocks,

which suggests that some task-set reconfiguration processes could also occur on mixed repeat trials

(e.g., Wylie et al., 2009).

On the basis of the above-mentioned evidence, it might be reasonable to assume that in the current

study participants may have adopted different visual-attention strategies to accomplish the semantic

task with respect to the spatial one and that this could have enhanced reconfiguration processes on

both switch and repeat semantic trials (albeit note that this was not sufficient to eradicate the

behavioural switch cost in the semantic task). Namely, whereas on semantic trials it is likely that

participants adopted a speed-wise strategy more based on local processing in order to identify the

specific deviant animals among those included in the three circles, on spatial trials they had to scan

the animal pictures in a more global manner to pick up the deviant angle. This difference in strategy

formation would explain the finding of a more similar positive waveform for both switch and repeat

trials in the semantic domain vs. the spatial domain.

Page 23: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   23  

Alternatively, it could be possible to hypothesize that switch and repeat semantic trials both

elicited the same sustained positivity because of the greater difficulty of the semantic task, which

was indeed associated with longer RTs. However, this explanation does not hold to the extent that

the same ERP pattern was replicated even after having equated RTs for the two domains. Moreover,

the magnitude of the switch cost did not differ between the semantic and the spatial domain, which

rules out the possibility that the similar positivity observed for switch and repeat semantic trials

could be attributed to general task difficulty or to asymmetrical switch costs.

The employment of different strategies and the likely presence of different reconfiguration

processes for the two domains could have played a more critical role in the differential expression

of the switch positivity associated with semantic and spatial domains. Such cognitive factors may

also help explaining the different negative modulations and time courses that characterized the two

domains over the fronto-central scalp region. In all the three time bins considered for the ERP

analysis, switch spatial trials were more negative than repeat trials over fronto-central electrodes.

Conversely, in the semantic domain, within the first time bin there was no difference between

switch and repeat trials over the same fronto-central region. Afterwards, repeating the semantic task

gave rise to an increased negativity with respect to switching from the spatial to the semantic one, a

pattern that was significantly present during both the second and the third time bin (from 1000 to

2200 ms).

It is difficult to pinpoint the functional meaning of the differential modulation of the frontal

negativity by semantic and spatial domains shown here because, as outlined in the Introduction

section, no agreement has been reached yet on the role of this brain potential. Recently, it has been

suggested that the frontal negativity might reflect a general task preparation mechanism that would

not be specific for switch trials (e.g., Karayanidis et al., 2011). However, if we assume that in the

present context the frontal negativity was due exclusively to generic anticipation, then it would

make sense to predict a larger negativity for repeat trials than for switch trials in both domains,

which was not what we observed. In addition to the findings from the switch positivity, these results

Page 24: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   24  

thus point to the conclusion that the frontal negativity may also be affected in a different way by the

specific participants’ strategies activated during the preparation interval.

Our results for both the frontal negativity and the switch positivity differ from the study by

Miniussi and collaborators (2005) who found larger negative frontal and parietal modulations after

a cue switch to be associated with the more difficult verbal task. Several factors such as different

task requirements and timing parameters might have played a role in the differential outcome

between the two studies. Nevertheless, it should be emphasized here that asking participants to shift

across tasks that implemented exactly the same stimuli for both semantic and spatial domains

differentially influenced the specific ERP markers of task-switching preparation. Whilst this finding

points to the conclusion that task-switching preparation would draw on distinct task-dependent

mechanisms, it should be finally considered whether these results could be partially attributed to the

specific task domain transition employed in the current study. In other words, because our

participants had always to switch between two different domains (from semantic to spatial and vice

versa), one might argue that some carry-over interference effects would have come into play when

disengaging attention from one domain to the other, and that this eventually influenced task-

switching preparation processes. The same concern applies to Miniussi and colleagues’ (2005)

study, which also used a between-domain shift design, even if in their case this factor did not result

in dramatic differences between spatial and verbal domains in the expression of the ERP markers of

task-switching preparation. This finding thus suggests that our results cannot be explained solely by

the employment of a between-domain shift paradigm since, unlike our study, Miniussi and

colleagues (2005) showed common task-switching preparation mechanisms in the context of a

similar between-domain shift manipulation.

Along the same line, it should also be noted that the majority of previous ERP task-switching

studies have usually employed a switch “between” different tasks (i.e., letter task vs. digit task; e.g.,

Nicholson et al., 2005). Yet, since these studies typically collapsed the task factor, it is not possible

Page 25: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   25  

to get a complete picture of how the combination of task domain manipulation and between-domain

shift transition might affect task-switching preparation processes.

Nevertheless, another way of testing whether different types of task-switching would rely on

shared or distinct mechanisms is to manipulate task domain in a block-wise manner by keeping the

task domains among which participants have to switch separate across different blocks of trials

(e.g., Vallesi et al., 2015) or across different groups of participants (e.g., Hsieh & Wu, 2011). A

value of a within-domain shift design with respect to a between-domain shift one is that it allows

investigating task-switching preparation processes as a function of task domain by controlling for

possible carry-over interference effects. However, in the case of a between-participants study, a

drawback of using different groups of participants to compare distinct task-switching types is that it

is not possible to control for inter-subjects variability, which could also be a confounding variable.

Taking into account the above issues, perhaps a better manipulation to improve our understanding

of task-switching preparation as a function of task domain would be to orthogonally manipulate

task domain transition (within-domain shift and between-domain shift) in a full experimental design

and within the same individuals. Future studies should thus employ, within the same blocks, two

different tasks for each task domain (see Kieffaber and Hetrick, 2005, for a partial attempt in this

direction, with a design employing both within- and between-modality switches).

Finally, it should be acknowledged that some researchers recently begun to combine into the same

experiment single-shifts (i.e., a switch between stimulus dimensions: color or shape, or between

response effectors: hand or foot) and dual-shifts (i.e., a concurrent switch of both stimulus

dimensions and response effectors) in order to understand whether a dual-shift condition would be

associated with similar or distinct anticipatory processes as compared to a single-shift condition

(see Hsieh, Wu & Lin, 2014; Tieges et al., 2007; West, Bailey, & Langley, 2009, for ERP studies,

and Hübner et al., 2001; Philipp & Koch, 2010, for behavioural evidence). The main advantage of

using this kind of design is that it allows researchers to parametrically manipulate the “task shift

load” (i.e., single vs. dual-shift conditions; cf. Tieges et al., 2007) in order to explore how the ERP

Page 26: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   26  

markers of task-switching preparation would be modulated by the complexity of the switch

operation required on dual-shift vs. single-shift trials. However, when implementing this dual-shift

design in the context of a task domain manipulation, like the one used here, it is also important to

keep in mind that other factors such as higher memory load and concurrent reconfiguration of

multiple elements could indeed influence the genuine effects of task domain on task-switching

preparation processes.

To sum up, this brief review of the literature on different task-switching designs highlights the

importance of carefully selecting, according to the specific task goal of the study, the experimental

design that is more suitable to investigate task-switching ability. Here, we showed that a between-

domain shift transition across semantic and spatial tasks selectively influenced the specific ERP

signatures of task-switching preparation. Our results thus suggest that when participants have to

shift on a trial-by-trial basis between two tasks belonging to two separate cognitive domains, task-

switching preparation would rely on distinct mechanisms. Future studies will clarify whether it is

possible to generalize these conclusions to experimental settings in which task domain is

manipulated, for instance, in a block-wise manner.

Page 27: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   27  

Acknowledgments

MC and AV are funded by the European Research Council Starting grant n° 313692 (FP7/2007-

2013) to AV. The authors wish to thank Giovanni Galfano for his support in participants’

recruitment, Ettore Ambrosini for his help with statistical matters and Sandra Arbula for her

assistance with data collection. Thanks go also to Città della Speranza, Padova, for its invaluable

logistic support.

Page 28: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   28  

References

Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic

control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Conscious and nonconscious information

processing: Attention and performance XV (pp. 421–452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Androver-Roig, D., & Barceló, F. (2010). Individual differences in aging and cognitive control

modulate the neural indexes of context updating and maintenance during task switching. Cortex, 46

(4), 434-50.

Astle, D. E., Jackson, G. M., & Swainson, R. (2006). Dissociating neural indices of dynamic

cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: an ERP study of task switching. Brain Research,

1125, 94–103.

Astle, D. E., Jackson, G. M., & Swainson, R. (2008). Fractionating the cognitive control required

to bring about a change in task: a dense-sensor event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 20, 255–267.

Barceló, F., Escera, C., Corral, M. J., & Periáñez, J. A. (2006). Task switching and novelty

processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 18 (10), 1734-48.

Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing components of task preparation with

functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 609-620.

Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G.L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu. Rev.

Neurosci. 34, 569-599.

De Baene, W., & Brass, M. (2014). Dissociating strategy-dependent and independent components

in task preparation. Neuropsychologia, 62, 331-340.

Fairhall, S.L., & Caramazza, A. (2013). Brain regions that represent amodal conceptual

knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 10552-10558.

Page 29: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   29  

Finke, M., Escera, C., & Barceló, F. (2012). The effects of foreknowledge and task-set shifting as

mirrored in cue- and target-locked event-related potentials. PLoS One, 7(11):e49486. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0049486.

Gajewski, P.D., & Falkenstein, M. (2011). Diversity of the P3 in the task-switching paradigm.

Brain Research, 1411, 87-97.

Goffaux, P., Phillips, N.A., Sinai, M., & Pushkar, D. (2006). Behavioural and electrophysiological

measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions. Biological Psychology, 72,

278–290.

Hsieh, S., & Wu, M. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of preparation and implementation

for different types of task shifts. Brain Research, 1423, 41-52.

Hsieh, S., Wu, M., & Lin, F. (2014). Neural correlates of response-effector switching using event-

related potentials. Biological Psychology, 103, 332-348.

Hübner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as source of residual

shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 640-653.

Jamadar, S., Hughes, M., Fulham, W.R., Michie, P.T., & Karayanidis, F. (2010a). The spatial and

temporal dynamics of anticipatory preparation and response inhibition in task-switching.

NeuroImage, 51, 432-49.

Jamadar, S., Michie, P.T., & Karayanidis, F. (2010b). Sequence effects in cued task-switching

modulate response preparedness and repetition priming processes. Psychophysiology, 47, 365-386.

Jennings, J. R., & Wood, C.C. (1976). The e-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures

analyses of variance. Psychophysiology, 13, 277–278.

Karayanidis, F., Mansfield, E. L., Galloway, K. L., Smith, J. L., Provost, A., & Heathcote, A.

(2009). Anticipatory reconfiguration elicited by fully and partially informative cues that validly

predict a switch in task. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 202–215.

Page 30: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   30  

Karayanidis, F., Jamadar, S., Ruge, H., Phillips, N., Heathcote, A., & Forstmann, B.U. (2010).

Advance preparation in task-switching: converging evidence from behavioral, brain activation and

model-based approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 1:25.

Karayanidis, F., Provost, A., Brown, S., Paton, B., & Heathcote, A. (2011). Switch-specific and

general preparation map onto different ERP components in a task-switching paradigm.

Psychophysiology, 48 (4), 559-68.

Kieffaber,P.D., Hetrick, W.P. (2005) Event-related potential correlates of task switching and

switch costs. Psychophysiology, 42, 56–71.

Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A., and Koch, I. (2010).

Control and interference in task-switching – A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 849-874.

Kopp, B., Lange, F., Howe, J., & Wessel, K. (2014). Age-related changes in neural recruitment

for cognitive control. Brain and Cognition, 85, 209-219.

Lavric, A., Mizon, G., & Monsell, S. (2008). Neurophysiological signature of effective

anticipatory task-set control: a task-switching investigation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28,

1016–1029.

Li, L., Wang, M., Zhao, Q-J., & Fogelson, N. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the cost of

task switching: An ERP study. PloS One 7:e42233.

Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control

in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 29, 575-599.

Martin, C.D., Barceló, F., Hernandez, M., & Costa, A. (2011). The time course of the

asymmetrical “local” switch cost: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology,

86, 210-218.  

Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.

Page 31: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   31  

Miniussi, C., Marzi, C.A., & Nobre, A.C. (2005). Modulation of brain activity by selective task

sets observed using event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1514-1528.

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134–140.

Nicholson, R., Karayanidis, F., Davies, A., & Michie, P. T. (2006). Components of task set

reconfiguration: Differential effects of “switch-to” and “switch-away” cues. Brain Research, 1121,

160-176.

Nicholson, R., Karayanidis, F., Poboka, D., Heathcote, A., & Michie, P.T. (2005).

Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. Psychophysiology, 42 (5),

540–554.

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory.

Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.

Periáñez, J.A., & Barceló, F. (2009). Updating sensory versus task representations during task-

switching: Insights from cognitive brain potentials in humans. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1160-1172.

Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., & Echallier, J.F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential

and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 72(2), 184-7.

Philipp, A.M., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task

representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 383-411.

Ravizza, S. M., & Carter, C. S. (2008). Shifting set about task switching: Behavioural and neural

evidence for distinct forms of cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2924-2935.

Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive

tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,124, 207–231.

Rushworth, M.F.S., Passingham, R., & Nobre, A.C. (2002) Components of switching intentional

set. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1139–1150.

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh:

Psychology Software Tools Inc.

Page 32: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   32  

Thompson-Schill, S.L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G.K., & Farah, M.J. (1997). Role of left inferior

prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.,

94, 14792-14797.

Tieges, Z., Snel, J., Kok, A., Plat, N., & Ridderinkhof, R. (2007). Effects of caffeine on

anticipatory control processes: Evidence from a cued task-switch paradigm. Psychophysiology, 44,

561-578.

Vallesi, A., Arbula, S., Capizzi, M., Causin, F., & D’Avella, D. (2015). Domain-independent

neural underpinning of task-switching: An fMRI investigation. Cortex, 65, 173-183.

Vallesi, A., Lozano, V.N., & Correa, A. (2013). Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a

function of the inter-trial interval duration. Cognition, 127, 22-30.

Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: Interplay of

reconfiguration and interference. Psychological Bullettin, 136, 601-626.

West, R., Bailey, K., & Langley, M.M. (2009). An investigation of the neural correlates of

attention and effector switching using ERPs. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 9,

190-201.

West, R., Langley, M.M., & Bailey, K. (2011). Signaling a switch: Neural correlates of task

switching guided by task cues and transition cues. Psychophysiology, 48, 612-623.

Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of ‘switch costs’.

Psychological Research, 63, 212-233.

Wylie, G. R., Murray, M. M., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2009). Distinct neurophysiological

mechanisms mediate mixing costs and switch costs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 105-

118.

Page 33: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   33  

Figure captions

Figure 1. Example of stimulus material. When the task required a semantic decision, participants

had to choose the circle containing the deviant animal (i.e., prey or predator) as compared to the

other two circles. In the figure, the circle on the right was the target since it contained a predator

(i.e., tiger), whereas the other two displayed prey animals (i.e., deer and zebra). By contrast, in

case of a spatial decision, the correct response would have been the circle on the left since the

arrangement of the animal pictures created a deviant angle as compared to the other two circles.

Page 34: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   34  

Figure 2. (A). Mean response times (RTs) and (B) percentage of correct responses as a function of

Domain (semantic, spatial) and Switching from the previous task (repeat, switch). Vertical bars

represent standard errors of the mean.

Page 35: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   35  

Figure 3. (A) Cue-locked grand averages of the semantic ERP waveforms recorded at pooled

fronto-central electrodes (top panel) and posterior-parietal electrodes (middle panel) as a function

of Switching from the previous task (repeat, switch) and Electrode side (left, midline, right). ERPs

of interest (P2, frontal negativity and switch positivity) are marked on midline electrodes only for

general visualization. (B) Differences in the ERP topography between switch and repeat trials for

the semantic domain. The difference maps (switch minus repeat) are shown for the time bins used

for the ERP analysis of the P2, frontal negativity and switch positivity.

Page 36: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   36  

Figure 4. Cue-locked grand averages of the spatial ERP waveforms recorded at pooled fronto-

central electrodes (top panel) and posterior-parietal electrodes (middle panel) as a function of

Switching from the previous task (repeat, switch) and Electrode side (left, midline, right). ERPs of

interest (P2, frontal negativity and switch positivity) are marked on midline electrodes only for

general visualization. (B) Differences in the ERP topography between switch and repeat trials for

the spatial domain. The difference maps (switch minus repeat) are shown for the time bins used for

the ERP analysis of the P2, frontal negativity and switch positivity.

Page 37: Task-switching preparation across semantic and spatial domains: An event-related potential study.

Task-switching and ERPs   37