Top Banner
“Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses” © 2018 Manoj Thomas and Ellie J. Kyung; Report Summary © 2018 Marketing Science Institute MSI working papers are distributed for the benefit of MSI corporate and academic members and the general public. Reports are not to be reproduced or published in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, without written permission. Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 2018 Report No. 18-122 Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses Manoj Thomas and Ellie J. Kyung
77

Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Oct 16, 2021

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

“Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses” © 2018 Manoj Thomas and Ellie J. Kyung; Report Summary © 2018 Marketing Science Institute MSI working papers are distributed for the benefit of MSI corporate and academic members and the general public. Reports are not to be reproduced or published in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, without written permission.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 2018 Report No. 18-122 Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses Manoj Thomas and Ellie J. Kyung

Page 2: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Report Summary Managers and researchers increasingly use textboxes and slider scales interchangeably to collect price, donations, or willingness-to-pay information from people. For example, Priceline collects “name your own price” bid information through a textbox on its website, but through a slider scale on its mobile app. Many times, these format choices are made for interface considerations. In this study, Manoj Thomas and Ellie J. Kyung suggest that these response formats should not be used interchangeably because numeric responses elicited on slider scales can be systematically different from those elicited through textboxes. When people use textboxes to submit numeric responses, they evaluate numeric magnitudes relative to the starting point of the response range. In contrast, when people use slider scales, they evaluate numeric magnitudes by considering the visual distances from the starting point as well as the endpoint of the response range. Because of this, numeric responses on slider scales are more likely to be assimilated towards the endpoint of the scale. The authors demonstrate this endpoint assimilation effect through 11 experiments, showing that it varies for ascending and descending payment formats and that its strength depends on factors related to visualization of the slider scale itself. For ascending payment formats, where consumers offer payment values higher than a starting price (e.g., eBay), slider scales elicit higher values than textboxes. For descending payment formats, where consumers offer payment values lower than a starting price (e.g., Priceline), slider scales elicit lower values than textboxes. Across their studies, in ascending payment formats where slider scales started at a zero value, values solicited through slider scales were on average 66% higher than those solicited through textbox values. For ascending payment formats where slider scales started at non-zero values (i.e., $239, $259, $279), values solicited through slider scales were on average 5% higher than those solicited through textboxes (taking into account the starting value, the relative difference between these values in terms of distance from starting point is 52%). For descending payment formats, values solicited through textboxes were on average 10% higher than those solicited through slider scales. Marketing implications Seemingly simple interface changes can have a material effect on consumer payments: slider scales can change how consumer mentally visualize prices and shift whether they perceive them as low, medium, or high. Furthermore, this effect extends not only to slider scales, but any format that creates a visual line. Thus managers should exercise caution when making format choices involving any kind of numeric payment, bidding, or donation information. Manoj Thomas is Associate Professor of Management, Cornell University, SC Johnson College of Business and Ellie J. Kyung is Associate Professor of Business Administration, Dartmouth College, Tuck School of Business. Acknowledgments This was a truly collaborative experience; both authors contributed equally to this research and they equally enjoyed the collaboration. Supplemental materials, including experiment stimuli,

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 1

Page 3: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

additional graphs, and supplementary analyses are available in the Online Web Appendix. This research has benefitted from feedback at various seminars and conferences including Effect of Numerical Markers on Consumer Judgment and Decision Making at Moore School of Business, University of South Carolina, the New Directions in Pricing Management Research and Practice at University of Illinois, the Northeast Marketing Conference at Harvard Business School, the Marketing Department at IDC Herzliya, Marketing Seminar at Texas A&M May’s School, The Wharton School Decision Processes Colloquia, and Cornell University Marketing Seminar. The authors are grateful to Kevin Keller for his thoughtful feedback; Matthew Paronto, Brad Turner, and Geoff Gunning for helping them write clearly; Yifan He for her research assistance; Sachin Gupta, Kusum Ailawadi, Scott Neslin, Blake McShane and Paul Wolfson for their advice on statistical analyses; and Maxine Park and Roxane Park for pretesting stimuli. The authors thank Bob Burnham for his programming genius creating the convex slider scale used in experiment 5 and the anonymous Reviewer C and John Hauser for making this suggestion. Finally, the authors thank the JCR review team—the editors, three reviewers, and two trainee reviewers—for their thoughtful and constructive feedback that greatly strengthened our research.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 2

Page 4: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Are payment responses elicited through slider scales different from those elicited through

open-ended textboxes? This question is relevant for organizations that use participative pricing

strategies and researchers interested in measuring contingent valuations. The proliferation of

online transactions has increased the popularity of participative pricing models where customers

choose what they want to pay. Online bidding has become quite commonplace. eBay allows

customers to bid on the items listed on their website. WineBid allows customers to bid on wines

of their choosing. Even charity websites, such as Doctors without Borders, allow donors to enter

a donation amount of their choice. With the increasing popularity of transactions on handheld

devices such as mobile phones and tablets, organizations are increasingly using slider scales

rather than textboxes to allow people to enter values with the swipe of a finger rather than

laborious typing with thumbs. For example, Priceline—the online retailer of travel services that

allows its customers to bid on hotel rooms—uses textboxes on its webpage and slider scales on

its mobile application. Do payment responses elicited through textboxes versus slider scales

differ from each other in systematic ways? And if so, how? And why?

The answers to these questions also have implications for academic research. Researchers

often measure monetary constructs such as contingent valuation, willingness-to-pay, internal

reference prices, and latitude of price acceptance (e.g., Adaval and Monroe 2002; Burson,

Larrick, and Lynch 2009; Monroe 1971), but the extant literature is silent on the effect of

response format on these measurements. To measure these constructs, some researchers have

used open-ended textboxes (e.g., Ward and Dahl 2014), some have used discrete semantic

differential scales (e.g., O’Guinn, Tanner, and Maeng 2015), while others have used continuous

slider scales (e.g., Galak et al. 2014). We examined experiments measuring willingness-to-pay in

the Journal of Consumer Research from 2011 to 2017 and found that from 2011 to 2014, 100%

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 3

Page 5: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

of the experiments employed textboxes. However, from 2015 to 2017, 64% employed textboxes,

while 23% employed slider scales, and 10% employed discrete semantic differential scales. The

increasing use of different response formats is based on the assumption that the amount a

consumer is willing to pay is unaffected by response format: the axiom of procedural invariance

implied in standard utility theories suggests that people’s willingness-to-pay should be invariant

to response format. In this research, we question and empirically test this assumption.

To presage our results, we find that relative to textboxes, slider scales elicit more extreme

responses assimilated towards the endpoint of the response range. This occurs because slider

scales recalibrate the mental number line that people use to judge price magnitudes. When

people use textboxes to submit numeric responses, they evaluate numeric magnitudes relative to

the starting point of a response range. In contrast, when people use slider scales, they evaluate

numeric magnitudes by considering the visual distance from both the starting point as well as the

endpoint of the response range. Because of this tendency to use visual distance from the endpoint

as a magnitude cue, numeric responses on slider scales are more likely to be assimilated towards

this endpoint. This endpoint assimilation effect can cause numeric responses on slider scales to

be systematically higher or lower than those of textboxes, depending on the direction of the

response adjustment. It also renders slider scale responses more susceptible to the size and

salience of the response range endpoint. Thus, this research documents not only how scale

visualization can influence consumer payments, but also characterizes the role of the mental

number line in price perception, which is emerging as one of the most fundamental theoretical

constructs in the numerical cognition literature (Dehaene 2001; Dehaene et al. 2008).

MENTAL NUMBER LINE RECALIBRATION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 4

Page 6: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

The mental number line recalibration account begins with two foundational assumptions.

First, we assume that consumers have stable valuations only for a few basic things that they

frequently encounter, and that for most real-world situations they construct valuations during

decision making. Second, we assume that people use a non-linear mental number line to evaluate

monetary values.

Constructing Price Judgments

The constructivist account of valuations (Bettman, Luce, and Payne 1998; Burson et al.

2009; Fischhoff 1991) posits that people’s valuations of goods are subjective, entailing several

intuitive judgments and decisions that happen in quick succession. A prospective buyer begins

by assessing the desirability of a potential product or the service. If the buyer considers the

product or service very desirable, she should be willing to pay a high price for it. If she considers

it less desirable, she should want to pay a lower price. Next, contingent on the first decision, she

will have to decide what specific price is considered low or high. A price of $500 might be high

for a bottle of wine, but low for a new laptop. Per this constructivist account, the prospective

buyer will have to, on the spur of the moment given a particular context, come up with

thresholds for implicitly categorizing prices as low, medium, or high. For example, a person

considering the purchase of a laptop on eBay will have to first decide—based on an evaluation of

its attractiveness—whether the price they are willing to pay for it is relatively low, medium, or

high. If she evaluates the laptop as very attractive, she has to judge what numeric value

constitutes a high bid. On the other hand, if she considers the laptop only somewhat attractive,

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 5

Page 7: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

she must judge what numeric value constitutes a low bid. Thus when evaluating prices,

consumers create implicit markers or boundaries of price magnitude categories—such as low,

medium, and high—along their mental number line.

The Non-Linear Mental Number Line

Psychologists studying the mental representation of numbers (Dehaene 2001; Dehaene et

al. 2008; Izard and Dehaene 2008) as well as consumer researchers (Adaval 2013; Adaval and

Monroe 2002; Bagchi and Davis 2016; Biswas et al. 2013; Lembregts and Pandelaere 2013;

Monroe and Lee 1999; Schley, Lembregts, and Peters 2017; Thomas and Morwitz 2009a, 2009b;

Valenzuela and Raghubir 2015) agree that numbers are represented not only symbolically as

arithmetic and verbal codes, but also as asymbolic semantic representations best conceptualized

as a non-linear mental number line. This mental number line does not resemble the linear

arithmetic ruler. Unlike a linear arithmetic ruler with evenly spaced markers, the mental ruler in

our minds is not calibrated linearly or with uniformly spaced markers. Instead, it is an

unbounded line with diminishing discriminability: the same objective numerical difference is

seen as larger when closer to the starting point of the mental number line than if it is away from

the starting point. Just as Ernst Weber and his student Gustav Fechner postulated that the ability

to recognize a just-noticeable-difference with sensory stimuli depends on the magnitude of the

initial stimuli (e.g., people easily detect the difference between a 15- vs. 30-watt lamp, but not

the difference between a 120- vs. 135-watt lamp), numerical cognition researchers have posited

that the mental number line used to evaluate numeric stimuli follows a similar response function.

Specifically, Dehaene et al. (2008) suggested that our brain perceives the change between 10 and

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 6

Page 8: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

20 as greater than that between 20 and 30, and the change between 20 and 30 as greater than that

between 30 and 40, even though the arithmetic differences are identical. Thus, the psychological

impact of increasing a price from $10 to $20 will be greater than that of increasing it from $30 to

$40. This has implications for calibration of the mental number line: the positions of the

boundaries between evaluative categories that serve as discrimination thresholds between low,

medium, and high values are not uniformly spaced. To use a term from psychophysics (Johnson

1944), these category “limens” are compressed toward the starting value of the mental number

line as depicted in figure 1.

Recalibrating the Mental Number Line

We propose that category limens on the mental number line are not only non-linear but

also context dependent—specifically, that the natural category limens on the mental number line

can unconsciously shift depending on response format. When people use a textbox to submit a

price response, they are likely to rely on the natural non-linear number line in their mind. Their

judgments about the boundaries between low, medium, and high prices are calibrated with

respect to the starting price, with responses anchored on this starting point (see figure 1, all

figures are after the references). For example, if a buyer can bid anywhere between $200

(starting price) and $1000 (retail price) for a laptop using an open-ended textbox, then her low

bid might be around $300, her medium bid might be around $400, and her high bid around $600.

Note that all of these values are assimilated towards the starting price on the left-hand side of the

mental number line.

In contrast, slider scales provide a visual representation of a line with salient starting and

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 7

Page 9: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

end points. We propose that when using a slider scale, people judge price magnitudes by

assessing the visual distance from the starting as well as from the end point of the slider scale.

Consistent with a dual-process model of price cognition (Monroe and Lee 1999; Thomas and

Morwitz 2009a, 2009b), we propose that while the reflective part of the brain deals with

deliberative numeric comparisons, the reflexive part of the brain spontaneously assesses the

visual distances from the starting and end points of the slider scale. Price magnitude judgments

are influenced by the distance from the starting point (“how far is it from the lowest possible

value”) as well as the distance from the endpoint of the scale (“how close is it to the highest

possible value”). Thus, slider scales change the boundaries between evaluative categories—the

category limens—on the mental number line. Relative to the category limens elicited by a

textbox, the category limens elicited by a slider scale are more stretched towards the endpoint of

the slider. Continuing with the illustrative bidding example introduced earlier, if a buyer can bid

anywhere between $200 and $1000 using a slider scale, then a low bid might be around $300, a

medium bid might be around $600, and a high bid around $800.

More generally, we propose that when using a textbox for an ascending payment format,

where price magnitudes input are higher than some starting point, if Pmin is the lowest possible

price that one can pay, then category limens Plow, Pmedium, and Phigh will be marked relative to

Pmin, such that these limens are assimilated towards Pmin. However, when people use a slider

scale for an ascending payment format, the visual distances from both starting point Pmin and

endpoint Pmax are perceptually more salient, reducing the assimilation towards Pmin and

increasing the assimilation towards Pmax. People submitting a high price response will consider

not only whether the value is far enough from Pmin but also whether it is close enough to Pmax.

The shift in category limens caused by the slider format is pictorially depicted in figure 1.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 8

Page 10: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Therefore, in an ascending payment format, where payment values are higher than the starting

point, people are likely to submit higher values when they use a slider scale versus a textbox

because the markers that identify categories are shifted to the right towards the endpoint. We

refer to this as the slider scale endpoint assimilation effect. Formally,

H1a: In ascending payment formats, where prices submitted are higher than a starting price,

values elicited through a slider scale will be higher than those elicited through a textbox.

Our theorizing does not suggest that slider scales will always elicit higher values; in fact

our theory posits that the direction of the endpoint assimilation effect will reverse for descending

payment formats. In a descending payment format (e.g., Priceline), people adjust the prices

downward from the list or retail price. In such cases, the category limens on the mental number

line will be marked relative to this highest possible response. As depicted in figure 2, when using

a textbox for descending payment formats, if Pmax is the starting price, then the category limens

Phigh, Pmedium, and Plow will be marked relative to Pmax and assimilated towards Pmax. However,

when people use a slider scale, the visual distances from Pmax (starting point) and Pmin (endpoint)

are salient, reducing the assimilation towards Pmax and increasing the assimilation towards Pmin.

Thus, in a descending payment format, values elicited using a slider scale will be lower than

those elicited using a textbox. Formally,

H1b: In descending payment formats, where prices submitted are lower than a starting price,

values elicited through a slider scale will be lower than those elicited through a textbox.

Proximity to the Endpoint

The novel insight that underlies our conceptualization is that slider scales create a bias

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 9

Page 11: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

with magnitude judgments by increasing the salience of the visual distance to the endpoint of the

response scale.1 If visual distance from the endpoint is the root cause of more extreme judgments

on slider scales, then it follows that the extent of the bias will depend on proximity to the slider

endpoint. For ascending payment formats (figure 1), category boundaries for larger price values,

that are visually closer to the endpoint (right), are more likely to assimilate towards this

perceptually salient endpoint than lower price values. The endpoint assimilation effect will be

strongest for those willing to pay the most. For descending payment formats (figure 2), category

boundaries for lower price values, that are visually closer to the endpoint (left), are more likely to

assimilate towards this perceptually salient endpoint than higher price values. Thus, the endpoint

assimilation effect will be strongest for those willing to pay the least. Formally,

H2: The endpoint assimilation effect will be stronger for values that are closer to the endpoint

of the slider scale. For ascending payment formats, the endpoint assimilation effect will be

stronger for higher price values; for descending payment formats, the endpoint

assimilation effect will be stronger for lower price values.

Visual Distance as Magnitude Cue: The Context-Dependence of Sliders

Our theorizing suggests that recalibration of the mental number line occurs with slider

scales because they make salient the visual representations of the distances from the starting

point and endpoint of the response continuum. If this is the case, then relative to textbox

responses, slider scale responses should be more susceptible to contextual factors that alter the

perceived visual distances from the starting and end points. One such factor is the response

Here the term “bias” refers to the difference between the perceived magnitude primed by the slider scale and that

primed by the textbox.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 10

Page 12: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

range. If the length of a slider scale in an ascending payment format remains the same, but the

value of the endpoint changes from $1000 to $1800, payment values will increase for the slider

scale with the larger range as the payment values assimilate towards this new, higher endpoint

value. However, because people using textbox formats are less influenced by the endpoint of the

response range, altering the size of the endpoint should matter less for textbox responses.

A second factor that can influence the visual distances from the endpoints of the slider is

the relationship between the objective slider scale distances and the numbers output by the slider

as the knob is moved. If the endpoint assimilation effect is driven by the linearization of the

mental number line towards the endpoint when using slider scales, then a non-linear slider scale

wherein numerical values are a convex function of distance from starting point should attenuate

the endpoint assimilation effect. Formally, we predict:

H3: Contextual factors that alter the visual distance of the response between the starting point

and endpoint of the response continuum will have a larger effect on slider scale responses

than textbox responses.

ALTERNATIVE ACCOUNTS

As we embarked on this research, in addition to the metal number line recalibration

account, we considered several alternative accounts for why slider scales might elicit different

responses from textboxes. Two of them are particularly plausible and pertinent: (i) the response

momentum account, and (ii) the asymmetric range awareness account. While the mental number

line recalibration account is based on the numerical cognition literature, the response momentum

account is inspired by the notion of psychological momentum in human kinetics (Markman and

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 11

Page 13: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Guenther 2007), which suggests that slider scales might intensify responses because of a natural

inclination to continue the physical sliding movement once it has started in a particular direction.

People may end up pulling the slider knob farther than they intended relative to a starting point,

and past a value they might indicate in a textbox, because they are caught up in the momentum

of the movement itself. Like the mental number line recalibration account, the response

momentum account also predicts that responses elicited on a slider scale will be more extreme

than those elicited through a textbox. But unlike the previous account, the response momentum

account suggests that the effect will be restricted to scales that entail a sliding response and will

not generalize to discrete scales that simulate line visualization, but do not entail a sliding

response (e.g., response scales with radio buttons).

The asymmetric range awareness account suggests that the difference between slider

scale and textbox responses stems not from the use of visual distance as magnitude cues, but

instead from varying awareness of the response range information. It is conceivable that when

people use textboxes, they might not be aware of the full response range, particularly the

endpoint. If the effect of response format is caused only by such information asymmetry, then

making textbox users mindful of the response range should easily alleviate it. However, our

conceptualization suggests that even when textbox users are aware of the response range,

responses elicited through slider scales can be different from textbox responses because of

inherent differences in the cognitive and perceptual processes employed by the two response

formats. When textboxes are used to submit responses, respondents can volitionally and

deliberately choose the more diagnostic reference point to compare and judge numeric responses:

in most cases, this will be the starting price. However, when slider scales are used to submit

responses, the choice of reference points is less volitional and more spontaneous, driven by the

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 12

Page 14: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

visual distance of the response from the starting and end points. The automatic processing of

visual distance as a magnitude cue influences their price magnitude judgments and price

responses. Tzelgov and Ganor-Stern (2005; also see Tzelgov, Meyer, and Henik 1992) argued

many of the day-to-day human cognitive processes are partially automatic because people are not

aware of the processes, because the processes cannot be controlled, or because the process is not

volitional. Similarly, we posit that the use of visual distances from the endpoints of slider scales

as magnitude cues is a partially automatic process that occurs spontaneously without

respondents’ awareness rather than due to information asymmetry.

OVERVIEW OF EXPERIMENTS & ANALYSES

For all experiments, in order to ensure that our results are not contingent on distributional

assumptions, we used both parametric as well as non-parametric approaches to analyze the data.

All the data were analyzed using PROC GLIMMIX in SAS using both the normal and Poisson

distributions. The comparison of results assuming normal and Poisson distributions are available

in web appendix B. To ensure that the results are not driven by a few outliers, the same analyses

were conducted removing participants with responses more than three standard deviations from

the mean. The means without these outliers are reported in web appendix C. The model-free

means, standard deviations, and standard errors for all conditions are reported in table 1. Note

that because our primary interest is in the average values that participants are willing to pay, in

all studies we report model-free means that are not contingent on model fit and distributional

assumptions. In addition, the results of all studies were analyzed by comparing the medians by

conditions using non-parametric methods. Condition medians and the results of these analyses

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 13

Page 15: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

are reported in table 2. These supplementary analyses demonstrate that the results of the studies

are robust to modeling and distributional assumptions. In the following sections we discuss

results from 11 studies. Because of space constraints, we only describe results from seven studies

in detail. The results of the remaining studies are summarized in the meta-analysis and web

appendices G and H.

EXPERIMENT 1A: MUG AUCTION

In this experiment, participants bid on a mug with the clear understanding that the highest

bidder would have to pay the bid amount. We manipulated the response format for bid

submissions: half the participants submitted their bids using a slider scale, while the other half

used an open-ended textbox. Will bids elicited through a slider scale be higher than those elicited

using a textbox (hypothesis 1a)?

In addition, we controlled for awareness of range information to rule out the possibility

that the effect is driven by participants in the textbox condition who do not consider range

information. Range information—lowest and highest possible bid values—was explicitly made

salient in both conditions to rule out that the effect of response format is caused by information

asymmetry. Furthermore, participants were provided with the retail price of the product.

Method

Participants. One hundred and twenty-seven undergraduates from a U.S. university (49%

female, Mage = 21.4 years) participated in this study in exchange for course credit.

Procedure. The computer-based experiment was created using Adobe Flash and

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 14

Page 16: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

administered in a laboratory in sessions of 8 to 15 participants. This was the first study in a series

of unrelated studies. Participants were told that they would be presented with the opportunity to

bid on a Japanese insulated travel mug in a real auction. A research assistant showed the real

mug to the participants before they bid on it.

To ensure that the participants were serious about the bids, before they submitted the bids

they were asked if they understood that they would be contacted to pay the bid amount to the

experimenter in exchange for the mug [Yes / No], and those participants that clicked “no” were

given the option of not participating in the study. No participant chose this option.

The participants were then presented with an instruction screen for placing their bid with

a reminder that they would have to pay the bid price if they won the auction. They were

informed that the retail price is $24 and that they could enter any response from $0 to $150.

Thus, the range information was made equally salient for both the conditions. They were asked,

“Do you understand how to submit your bid?” [Yes / No]. Once they confirmed they understood

the instructions, they were shown the product information for the insulated mug and asked to

enter their bid. The participants then entered their response on a slider scale or textbox and

clicked on the “Place Bid” button (see figure 3). Finally, participants were asked, “In the

instructions that you just read about placing your bid, you were told that you could enter any

response between which two numbers?” [$0 to $100, $0 to $150, $0 to $200] and answered

demographic questions.

Results

Bids. The analysis was conducted using PROC GLIMMIX specifying a Poisson

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 15

Page 17: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

distribution with response format as the independent measure and bid price as the dependent

measure. It revealed a significant effect of response format where participants in the slider

condition bid more on the insulated mug (M = $11.40) than those in the textbox condition (M =

$8.01, F(1, 125) = 37.05, p < .001).

The results are robust to distributional assumptions and the type of statistical test. The

median values in the two conditions and the non-parametric test are reported in table 2. The

results from a similar analysis assuming normal distribution are available in web appendix B.

The means without the outliers are reported in web appendix C.

The highest bid, $35, was submitted by two participants in the slider condition. One of

these two participants was chosen as the winner and asked to buy the product for the bid amount.

The participant met with the experimenter and exchanged $35 for the mug. However, because

this was an undergraduate student from the subject pool, after the transaction was completed, we

returned their money and had them keep the mug as a token of goodwill.

Confound Check: Range Information. The vast majority of participants (90%) correctly

identified the range outlined in the experiment stimuli as $0 to $150, confirming that attention to

range information did not vary across the two conditions. Furthermore, removing participants

that incorrectly identified the range did not change the results. Thus, the slider scale endpoint

assimilation effect does not stem from participants in the textbox condition being unaware of the

response range.

EXPERIMENT 1B: DONATIONS TO CHARITY

In the previous experiment some participants may not have felt invested in the exercise or

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 16

Page 18: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

that they were bidding with real money. Does the effect still hold when transactions involve real

money for every individual? In experiment 1b, following the procedure used by Berman and

Small (2012), every participant was given a $1 bonus at the beginning of the study and the

choice of donating any portion of their bonus to the Intrepid Fallen Heroes Fund, a non-profit

serving U.S. military personnel injured during active duty and their families. The donations were

elicited using either a slider scale or textbox. The amount they did not donate was theirs to keep.

Method

Participants. Two hundred and four U.S.-based panelists on mTurk, verified by IP

address, participated in this study (52% female, Mage = 36.3 years). To avoid confounds from

differences in screen size, participants were only able to complete the study if they were on a

computer; mobile users were automatically blocked from responding to the study. These

procedures were followed for all subsequent experiments.

Procedure. Participants recruited were told that they would be paid 45 cents for

completing a brief study about their interest in “donating to a particular charity for U.S.

veterans.” The study was run on Veteran’s Day to make the charity more relevant for all

individuals. After starting the study, they received the following additional instructions about

receiving a $1 bonus amount, in addition to the 45 cents:

You will be paid 45 cents for completing this brief study. You will also be paid an additional $1 (as a bonus). This study is about your interest in donating to a particular charity for U.S. veterans. You will then be asked how much you are interested in donating to this charity.

They were then given a description of the Intrepid Fallen Heroes Fund (see web appendix A.1B)

and asked, “Please enter the number of cents from the $1.00 that you would like to donate to the

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 17

Page 19: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Intrepid Fallen Heroes Fund. You can enter any value from 0 to 100 cents.” All participants were

also given the average donation value: “In a previous version of this study, the average donation

was 17 cents.”2 Thus, the range information as well as the average donation value was made

clear in both conditions. Underneath, they were shown either a textbox or a slider scale to enter

their response. The endpoints of the slider scales were not numbered to rule out the possibility

that the slider format increases awareness of the numeric range. Finally, participants answered

demographic questions.

Results

Donations. The total value of donations collected from all the participants was $62.61

and this amount was donated to the charity by the experimenter. The analysis conducted with

PROC GLIMMIX specifying a Poisson distribution with response format as the independent

measure and donation amount as the dependent measure revealed a significant effect of response

format, where participants in the slider condition donated significantly more of their $1 (M =

37.8 cents) than those in the textbox condition (M = 24.0 cents, F(1, 202) = 307.29, p < .001, see

table 1). The results are robust to distributional assumptions and the type of statistical test (see

table 2, web appendix B, and web appendix C).

Discussion

The results from experiments 1a and 1b provide evidence that response formats can

The results are robust to not including this information. A similar experiment was run on Memorial Day

without including this information. The results of this experiment are summarized in web appendix G.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 18

Page 20: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

change valuations. Experiment 1a used an auction administered on students in a laboratory.

Experiment 1b used a donation task administered to a more general population as an online

study. Experiment 1a entailed amounts involving dollars ($0 to $150), whereas experiment 1b

entailed amounts involving cents (0 to 100). Yet the results from the two studies were convergent

in showing that slider scales elicit more intense bids than textboxes, supporting hypothesis 1a—

bids were 42% higher for experiment 1a and donations were 57% higher for experiment 1b

(effect sizes for each study are summarized in web appendix H). Note that although the

economic values are quite different across the two studies, the effect sizes are comparable

suggesting that participants were responding to the numerosity of the prices rather than to

economic values (Bagchi and Davis 2016; Burson et al. 2009). The range information was made

clear in both experiments; in experiment 1a, participants knew that their bid had to be between

$0 and $150, and in experiment 1b participants knew that their donation value had to be between

0 and 100 cents. In addition, in experiment 1b all participants who chose to donate had to spend

real money. Thus, the absence of range information or inconsequentiality of the response is not a

necessary condition for the observed effect. In the next two experiments, we take a closer look at

the plausible mechanisms causing endpoint assimilation when using slider scales—ruling out

several potential alternative explanations—and use a more externally valid participative pricing

context similar to that used by companies.

EXPERIMENT 2: ASCENDING PAYMENT FORMAT ON EBAY

In this experiment, we used a more externally valid decision context by giving people the

scenario of bidding on items offered by the popular auction website eBay. We asked participants

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 19

Page 21: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

who recently purchased laptops or were planning to purchase laptops to bid on three used laptops

using either a slider scale or a textbox. Each participant offered three bids, one for each laptop.

We varied the starting bids for three laptops as a within-subjects factor. This enabled us to

address whether the effect of response format holds up even when participants make several

consecutive responses in the same product category. The range information was not explicitly

provided for either format to further test the external validity of the effect as firms typically do

not provide such range information. To rule out other possible alternative accounts, ease of

response, format preference, and awareness of format influence were measured.

Method

Participants. One hundred ninety-nine U.S.-based participants on mTurk, verified by IP

address, participated in this experiment in exchange for $1 (47% women; Mage = 37.1 years). To

ensure the effect is not restricted to people who are uninvolved or unfamiliar with the product

category, we recruited only participants familiar with the category. All potential participants

were asked two screening questions—only participants who had purchased a laptop computer in

the past 24 months or intended to purchase one in the next 24 months were allowed to participate

in the study. Participants who answered “no” to both of these questions were screened out at the

beginning of the study and did not continue.

Procedure. The experiment used a 2 x 3 mixed factorial design with response format

(textbox, slider) as the randomly assigned between-subjects factor and starting bid ($239, $259,

$279) as a within-subjects factor. Participants were told they would be asked to imagine bidding

on eBay. The first screen informed them how the bidding process works on eBay. They were

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 20

Page 22: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

then asked to submit their bids on three used laptops with different starting bid prices shown in

randomized order one at a time. The information format design was as close as possible to eBay:

each bid page included a photograph of the product, the current bid price, the number of prior

bids, and detailed product information. The only difference between conditions is that

participants were randomly assigned to submit their bids either via an open-ended textbox or a

slider scale anchored at the starting bid price (see web appendix A.2). Ranges were not provided

in either condition. The bid amounts were the primary dependent variable.

Process Measures. After submitting their bids, participants were asked how easy or

difficult it was to submit the bids (1: extremely easy, 5: extremely difficult). To assess whether

participants were mindful of the biasing influence of response formats, they were asked to

describe how they arrived at the bid values for the three computers. They described their thought

process in an open-ended textbox. They were also asked direct questions about response formats.

Participants were shown the response formats side by side and asked to indicate which they

preferred [slider / textbox]. Then they were asked whether response format could change their

responses [i: No, my bid values will not be affected by the response format / ii: Yes, my bid values

will be higher when I use the slider format / iii: Yes, my bid values will be higher when I use the

textbox format]. Finally, participants submitted their demographic information.

Results

Willingness-to-pay. The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a

Poisson distribution with response format (slider vs. textbox) as a between-subjects factor and

starting bid ($239, $259, $279) as a within-subjects factor. The main effect of response format

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 21

Page 23: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

was significant, F(1, 197) = 5.61, p = .02, where values submitted by textbox (M = $280) were

lower than those submitted by slider (M = $291). Participants’ WTPs for all three laptops were

higher when submitted using a slider scale relative a textbox (all ps < .01), supporting H1a (see

figure 4, table 1). There was also a main effect of the starting bid, F(2, 393) = 98.49, p < .001,

but the two-way interaction was not significant (F < 1). These results are robust to distributional

assumptions and the type of statistical test (see table 2, web appendix B, and web appendix C).

Ease of Responding. Participants’ self-reported ease of responding was reverse-scored

such that higher scores indicated greater ease. There was no effect of response format on self-

reported ease (F < 1) and the average scores on the 5-point scale were very high in both

conditions (Mtextbox = 4.59, Mslider = 4.65).

Awareness of Bias. Were participants aware of a biasing influence of response format? In

their open-ended responses, participants mentioned that their bids were influenced by product

specifications, bid prices, initial bids, how the product looked, how much they wanted it, etc., but

none of them mentioned influence by response format. For the direct question about the

influence of response formats, the vast majority of participants (73%) reported that their bid

values would not be affected by the response format. Some (22%) suspected that their bids

would be higher when using a slider scale, and a very small proportion (5%) believed that their

bids would be higher when using textboxes. These proportions did not differ by response format

(p = .19). These results suggest that most participants do not suspect that response format could

change their answers, but when directly prompted to consider this possibility, a small proportion

suspect that they might be indicating higher values when using slider scales.

Preference for Response Formats. Participants’ preference for response format (1: prefer

slider, 0: prefer textbox) was submitted to a one-way ANOVA with response format as the

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 22

Page 24: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

predictor and revealed a significant effect of response format (F(1, 197) = 29.13, p < .001).

Participants were more likely to prefer the slider scale response format after having used a slider

scale (37%) versus a textbox (7%). These results suggest that people do not have stable

preferences for response format and adapt to the response format they use.

Discussion

These results suggest that response format manipulation can influence consumer behavior

even in more externally valid shopping situations. If eBay—a popular online shopping forum—

starts using a slider scale instead of a textbox to elicit bids then shoppers are likely to offer

higher bids, supporting hypothesis 1a. More importantly, the process measures from this

experiment suggest that we can rule out that the effect is caused by ease of use: participants

found both response formats—slider scales and textboxes—equally easy to use. Furthermore,

most participants were unaware of the surreptitious influence of response formats on their bid

values. Even when asked directly after making their judgments whether response formats might

have influenced their bids, only a small minority considers this possibility. This suggests that the

psychological mechanism underlying the slider scale effect is implicit in nature, operating

outside of people’s awareness.

Also, we employed a within-subjects factor with three different levels of starting bids.

The effect manifested for all three randomized replicates, ruling out the possibility that the effect

wears out with familiarity with the response format. Finally, this study shows that the endpoint

assimilation effect manifests both when range information is and is not provided.

To further test the number line recalibration account, in the next study, we employed a

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 23

Page 25: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

descending payment format and also varied the placement of the slider knob starting point.

EXPERIMENT 3: DESCENDING PAYMENT FORMAT ON PRICELINE

This experiment had two objectives. First, we wanted to test the effect of response

formats in the context of a descending payment format. Thus far, all experiments used ascending

payment formats where buyers submit responses higher than the starting price. In this

experiment, participants had to submit responses lower than the starting price. Participants were

informed that Priceline, an online travel reservation company, allows customers to bid for hotel

rooms. For example, a night at an expensive downtown New York City hotel room has a rack

rate of $400, but a Priceline customer could bid lower, say $300 or even $150. If the endpoint

assimilation effect stems from consideration of the visual distance from the endpoint in relation

to the starting point, per hypothesis 1b, in a descending payment format, slider scales should

result in lower numeric values than textboxes.

Second, in this experiment we introduced two slider scale conditions—one where the

slider knob moved from left to right (slider left-to-right) and one where it moved from right to

left (slider right-to-left). This allows us to address the possibility that the effect of slider scale

could be an idiosyncratic effect of the left-to-right hand movement. If the effect of slider scale is

because of the left-to-right hand movement, then reversing the direction of the hand movement

from right to left should alleviate the effect. However, if the effect is due to the visual distance

from endpoints and not because of the direction of the hand movement, then the responses in

both slider scale conditions should be lower than in the textbox condition.

Note that in this study, we used the same process measures and replicated the results of

experiment 2, but the explication and results of these measures are included in web appendix D

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 24

Page 26: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

for the sake of brevity.

Method

Participants. Three hundred and four U.S.-based participants on mTurk participated in

this computer experiment in exchange for $1 (38% women; Mage = 33.4 years). To ensure the

applicability to the relevant customer base, only participants who had made online hotel

reservations and stayed in a hotel in the past 24 months were allowed to participate in this study.

Procedure. The experiment used a 3 (response format) x 2 (hotel city) x 3 (starting price

level) mixed factorial design with response format (textbox, slider-left-to-right, slider-right-to-

left) as a randomly assigned between-subjects factor and hotel city and starting price level as

within-subjects factors to create six hotel replicates.

Participants were informed that Priceline.com’s Name Your Own Price® system

(popularly referred to as NYOP) allows travelers to bid on hotel rooms. They read that over the

last 12 months, a substantial percentage of accepted offers have resulted in savings of up to 60%

in comparison to the lowest published rates on other leading online travel sites for the same

itinerary. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: textbox, left-to-right-

slider, or right-to-left-slider. They were given information about six hotels (2 cities, three starting

price levels), one at a time, and for each they submitted their bid per room, per night in U.S.

dollars. For each hotel, they saw the city, general location of the hotel, star rating, and median

retail price of a similar hotel. (For Atlanta, the three starting prices were $161, $194, and $199;

For New York, the three starting prices were $179, $183, and $214). Note that this matches

Priceline’s actual NYOP bidding process where customers do not see the name of the specific

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 25

Page 27: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

hotel until their bid is accepted. Examples of the stimuli are shown in appendix A.3. In the left-

to-right slider condition, the knob on the slider scale was set at the lowest possible bid value and

participants had to drag it to a higher value to submit their bid. Conversely, in the right-to-left

slider condition, the knob was set at the highest possible bid value.

Finally, participants responded to the process measures used in the previous study and

submitted their demographic details. The results of the process measures were nearly identical to

experiment 2; for the sake of brevity, they are included in web appendix D.

Results

Willingness-to-pay. The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a

normal distribution with response format (left-to-right-slider, right-to-left-slider, textbox) as a

between-subjects factor and hotel replicates (city, starting price level) as within-subjects factors.

We specified a normal distribution because unlike the other studies, the descending payment

format did not result in a skewed distribution of responses.

The main effect of response format was significant, F(2, 301) = 5.30, p < .01.

Participants’ average WTP value in the left-to-right-slider condition (M = $131) was lower than

that in the textbox condition (M = $142; t(301) = -3.25, p = .001). Participants’ average WTP

value in the right-to-left-slider condition (M = $135) was also lower than that in the textbox

condition, although this contrast was marginally significant (t(301) = -1.84, p = .07). The two

slider conditions were not significantly different from each other (t(301) = -1.43, p = .15). Thus,

in a descending payment format, slider scales always reduced the WTP values regardless of the

positioning of the slider knob. The mean WTP values for starting price level by response format,

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 26

Page 28: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

collapsed across city for simplicity, are depicted in figure 5. (There are no significant interactions

between response format and city). There were also significant main effects of the city, F(1,

1504) = 265.35, p < .001, and starting price level, F(2, 1504) = 558.22, p < .001, and a two-way

interaction between response format and starting price level, F(4, 1504) = 2.40, p = .05. The

three-way interaction and two-way interactions between response format and city were not

significant (p > .30). These results are robust to distributional assumptions and the type of

statistical test (see table 2, web appendix B, and web appendix C).

Discussion

The results from experiment 3 are illuminating in several ways. First, this experiment

clearly demonstrates that slider scales do not always increase bid values. Instead, slider scales

assimilate responses to the endpoint of the response range. Participants had to respond by

bidding an amount lower than the retail price. Those who used slider scales submitted bids lower

than those using textboxes.

Second, because the starting point of the slider knob was explicitly manipulated, some

participants moved the knob to the left from the starting point (highest bid, right-to-left slider)

while others moved it to the right from the endpoint (lowest bid, left-to-right slider). Consistent

with prior research on anchoring effects in valuations (e.g., Adaval and Monroe 2002;

Mussweiler and Strack 1999), results from these studies show that starting points do serve as

anchors and influence final bids. However, manipulations of starting points did not wipe out the

effect of response format. Bids elicited through slider scales—regardless of the starting point—

were significantly lower than those elicited through textboxes. These results rule out the

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 27

Page 29: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

possibility that the slider effect stems simply from the location of the slider anchor point or it is

an idiosyncratic effect of spatial left-to-right orientation.

EXPERIMENT 4: LOW, MEDIUM, HIGH VALUES ON A NON-SLIDING SCALE

Hypothesis 2 posits that proximity to endpoint is an important moderator of the slider

scale effect. As depicted in figures 1 and 2, a visual linear number line makes the distance from

the endpoint more conspicuous, and this is more pronounced for numeric categories that are

closer to the endpoint. Therefore, bids elicited through slider scales and textboxes are more likely

to diverge when participants consider numerical values that are closer to the endpoint rather than

when the values are closer to the starting point. To test this hypothesis, we elicited three values

that exemplified low, medium, and high bid categories from each participant. We predicted that

for ascending payment formats, the effect of slider scales would be stronger for high values than

for low values.

This study also had a second objective, namely, to rule out the response momentum

account that the sliding movement—the physical action of sliding the slider knob and the

concomitant sensory responses—is responsible for the results. A priori we had to acknowledge

the possibility that the proposed effect could be idiosyncratic to the slider scale itself because of

the sliding movement in response generation, rather than because of a mental recalibration of the

number line. Slider scales might be intensifying responses because the momentum of the sliding

response makes the final response end up farther away than the intended point. To test this

alternative account, we added a new non-sliding scale condition where responses were elicited

by a scale arrayed with a large number of points in $30 increments. The starting and end points

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 28

Page 30: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

of the scale exactly matched the slider scale and visually formed an evenly spaced number line,

but did not entail the sliding movement; participants had to click on a point on the scale. If bid

intensification stems only from the sliding movement, then it should not occur in the non-sliding

scale condition, which does not require any sliding movement for a response. Conversely, if the

effect stems from recalibration of the mental number line because of visual distance from the

endpoints, then the non-sliding scale condition should result in end point assimilation similar to

the slider condition.

Method

Participants. Three hundred and thirty-three U.S.-based participants on mTurk

participated in this experiment in exchange for 51 cents (48% women; Mage = 35.1 years).

Procedures. The experiment used a 3 x 3 mixed factorial design with response format

(textbox, slider, non-sliding scale) as a randomly assigned between-subjects factor and numeric

category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor. The procedures for this experiment

were identical to experiment 2 with the following exceptions. First, participants evaluated only

one laptop with the $259 starting price and were asked to generate a low, medium, and high bid

for the item. Second, in order to keep the response range information identical across all

conditions, the bidding range was explicitly provided with a starting bid of $259 and an ending

bid of $949, the retail price for a new computer. Third, a new non-sliding scale condition was

added to the textbox and slider condition. In the non-sliding scale condition, participants could

select any bid from $259 to $949 in $30 increments where the bid values were arrayed on top of

a series of radio buttons that visually formed a number line (see figure 6 for the non-sliding

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 29

Page 31: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

condition and web appendix A.4 for all conditions). The primary dependent measures were the

three bid values submitted by each participant: low, medium, and high.

Results

Willingness-to-pay. The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a

Poisson distribution with response format (textbox, slider, non-sliding scale) as a between-

subjects factor and numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor and the bid

amount as the dependent measure. The effects of response format (F(2, 330) = 5.66, p < .001),

numeric category (F(2, 660) = 17,232.30, p < .001), and the interaction between the two (F(4,

660) = 22.08, p < .001) were all significant (see figure 7, table 1). The average bid in the textbox

condition ($415) was lower than that in the slider ($452, t(330) = -2.79, p < .01) and non-sliding

scale ($453, t(330) = -3.03, p < .01) conditions, but the slider and non-sliding scale conditions

were nearly identical to each other (t(330) = -.23, p =.82).

Furthermore, consistent with hypothesis 2, the endpoint assimilation effect for the slider

and scale conditions was stronger for higher bid values. In the low numeric category condition,

the textbox ($289), slider ($298) and scale ($299) bids were not significantly different from each

other (all ps > .13). However, in the medium numeric category condition, the average bid in the

textbox condition ($395) was lower than those in the slider ($437, t(660) = -3.47, p < .001) and

non-sliding scale ($439, t(660) = -3.75, p < .001) conditions, but the slider and non-sliding scale

conditions were nearly identical (t(660) = -0.26, p = .79). In the high numeric category condition,

the average bid in the textbox condition ($561) was lower than those in the slider ($621, t(660) =

-3.52, p < .001) and non-sliding scale ($622, t(660) = -3.71, p < .001) conditions, but the slider

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 30

Page 32: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

and non-sliding scale conditions were nearly identical (t(660) = -0.18, p =.85). These results are

robust to distributional assumptions and the type of statistical test (see table 2, web appendix B,

and web appendix C).

EXPERIMENT 5: A CONVEX SLIDER SCALE

This study was designed to delineate the role of distance visualization in the slider scale

endpoint assimilation effect. If the bias in magnitude judgments is caused by the use of visual

distance from the endpoint as magnitude cue, then if we are able to surreptitiously change the

visual distance of a bid value from the endpoint of the scale, then consumers’ evaluation of that

bid value should change. For example, on a linear slider scale that ranges from $259 to $949,

where the numeric values are a linear function of the distance from the starting point, a value of

$604 would fall squarely in the middle of the starting and end points. But if we make the

numeric values a convex function of the distance from the starting point, then $604 would appear

to be closer to the endpoint than to the starting point of the scale. Thus, the same bid value

should be perceived to be higher on a convex slider scale than on a linear slider scale. Therefore,

a convex slider scale can mimic the responses elicited through a textbox format. The present

experiment was designed to test this hypothesis.

Method

Participants. Two hundred and sixty-two U.S.-based participants on mTurk participated

in this experiment in exchange for 51 cents (58% women; Mage = 36.2 years).

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 31

Page 33: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Procedures. The experiment used a 3 x 3 mixed factorial design with response format

(textbox, linear slider, convex slider) as a randomly assigned between-subjects factor and

numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor. The procedures for this

experiment were identical to experiment 4; we used the laptop bidding context and the linear

slider and the textbox conditions were exactly the same as in the previous study. The only point

of departure was the addition of the convex slider condition in place of the non-sliding scale

condition. Participants in the convex slider condition saw a slider scale that looked very much

like the slider in the linear condition. However, unbeknownst to them the values on the slider

scale were a convex function of the distance from the starting point. In the linear slider scale

condition, the bid value y was a linear function of the distance from the starting point.

For the given range of values for x and y (x values ranged from 0 to 10 and corresponding

y values ranged from $259 to $949), the bid values in the linear slider condition were defined by

the following function:

y = 259 + 69x

However, for the convex slider condition, the bid values were a quadratic function of the

distance from the starting point:

y = 259 + 19X + 5x2

The two functions are depicted in figure 8. As depicted there, the midpoint of the linear slider

scale is $604. But on the convex slider, $604 appears between markers 6 and 7, much closer to

the end point (see web appendix A.5 for screenshots of the slider conditions). More generally, all

values on the convex slider scales appear closer to the end point relative to how they appear on

the linear slider scale. Therefore, we expected responses on the convex slider scale to be less

extreme and more closely match the responses on the textbox format. Note that operationally the

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 32

Page 34: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

linear slider scale and the convex slider scale appear very similar to each other and it is difficult

for the participants in the convex slider condition to discern that the response format they are

using is unusual. As in the previous study, the primary dependent measures were the three bid

values submitted by each participant: low, medium, and high.

Results

Willingness-to-pay. The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a

Poisson distribution with response format (textbox, linear slider, convex slider) as a between-

subjects factor and numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor and the bid

amount as the dependent measure. The effects of response format (F(2, 259) = 3.92, p = .02),

numeric category (F(2, 518) = 12,157.10, p < .001), and the interaction between the two (F(4,

518) = 8.47, p < .001) were all significant (see figure 9, table 1). The average bid in the linear

slider condition ($445) was higher than in the textbox condition ($403, t(259) = 2.71, p < .01)

and the convex slider condition ($416, t(259) = -1.96, p = .05) while the average bids in the

textbox and convex slider conditions were not significantly different from each other (t(259) =

0.76, p = .45). Non-parametric test of medians also demonstrated the same pattern (see table 2).

However, this is the only study in the set of studies wherein the results were contingent on

distributional assumptions (see web appendix B). We conducted follow up analyses for this study

to ensure the results to not depend on distribution assumptions and also ran another post hoc

condition with a concave slider scale, details of which are reported in web appendix E and F,

respectively.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 33

Page 35: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

EXPERIMENT 6: BIASING EFFECT OF RESPONSE RANGE

The final study in this research delineates the influence of visual distance from end points

by manipulating the response range (hypothesis 3). In this experiment we manipulated the

response range. We used wines as the stimuli in this study because wine prices have a wide

range, allowing us to manipulate the price range without arousing suspicion in the participants.

Participants bid on a bottle of wine being sold by an online wine retailer; half of them were told

that the bid values could range from $20 to $500 whereas the other half were told that the range

is $20 to $1000. We expected this range manipulation to change the visual distance from the

endpoint when participants use slider scales to submit bids. A bid of $260 would be equidistant

from the two end points for the smaller range slider, but it would appear farther from the

endpoint on the larger range slider. Thus, the same bid would appear smaller on the larger range

scale. The textbox responses, in contrast, would be less affected by this range manipulation

because of the absence of visual distance as a magnitude cue.

Additionally, we also tested whether participants in the slider scale conditions are less

aware of the effect of the range manipulations relative to those in the textbox conditions.

Because we conceptualize the effect of the slider scale as automatic, we predicted that

participants would be less aware of the influence of response range in the slider scale conditions.

Method

Participants. Four hundred and thirteen U.S.-based participants on mTurk participated in

this experiment in exchange for 51 cents (51% women; Mage = 37.1 years).

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 34

Page 36: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Procedures. The experiment used a 2 x 2 x 3 mixed factorial design with response format

(textbox, slider) and response range (small, large) as randomly assigned between-subjects factors

and numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor. Participants were told an

online store wanted to offer consumers the ability to bid on wines and that consumers could bid

whatever they want on wines, and that sellers would honor bids that they find reasonable.

Several screens then explained the information they would receive about the wines—this

information included the vintage of the wine, its name, the Wine Spectator rating, and a brief

description with the starting bid price. Those in the small range condition were told “You will

see the starting bid for the wines. The starting bid is the amount suggested by the seller to open

the bid. Your bid has to be higher than the starting bid. The maximum bid anyone can submit is

$500. So you can bid up to $500, if you want to.” Those in the large range condition were given

the exact same information except they were told that the maximum bid they could submit is

$1000. (See Web Appendix A.6 for screenshots). They were then told that they would be asked

to place a low, medium, and high bid for the described wine. They were then presented with

information on the wine to bid on, a 2007 Farmlands Pinot Noir with a Wine Spectator rating of

97 with a starting bid of $20, described as “Vibrant ruby with a garnet core, expresses black

cherry cola, red plum, and dry underbrush on the nose. The palate…shows some nice complexity

and depth. It’s biodynamic.”

Participants then placed low, medium, and high bids for this wine either using textboxes

or slider scales. After that they were presented with a series of confound check questions. They

were asked, “What was the starting bid for the wine?” and “What was the maximum bid anyone

could submit?” as open ended textboxes, and “To what extent was your highest bid influenced by

the maximum possible bid value?” [A great deal / A lot / A moderate amount / A little / None at

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 35

Page 37: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

all], followed by demographic questions.

Results

Willingness-to-pay. The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a

Poisson distribution with response format (textbox, slider) and response range (small, large) as

between-subjects factors and numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor

and the bid amount as the dependent measure. The pattern of means is depicted in figure 10. The

effects of response format (F(1, 409) = 28.97, p < .001) and numeric category (F(2, 818) =

9,752.79, p < .001) were both significant, as were the two-way interactions between numeric

category and response format (F(2, 818) = 123.51, p < .001) and numeric category and response

range (F(2, 818) = 116.53, p < .001) and the three-way interaction (F(2, 818) = 10.75, p < .001).

For ease of exposition, first consider the WTP values in the two between-subjects conditions

averaged across low, medium, and high bids. For low range conditions, the average WTP in the

slider condition was higher than in the textbox condition (Mtext-$500 = 64 v. Mslider-$500 = 87, t(409)

= 3.16, p < .01), and this response format effect was stronger for the large range conditions

(Mtext-$1000 = 72 v. Mslider-$1000 = 137, t(409) = 4.45, p < .01). The pattern is depicted in figure 10.

The WTP values for the three numeric categories—low, medium, and high—are also

consistent with our expectations. For the low numeric category, there was no effect of response

range in either the textbox (Mtext-$500 = 29 v. M text-$1000 = 29, t(818) = 0.59, p = .56) or slider

(Mslider-$500 = 51 v. Mslider-$1000 = 63, t(818) = 0.15, p = .88) conditions. For the medium numeric

category, there was no effect of response range in the textbox condition (Mtext-$500 = 59 v. Mtext-

$1000 = 65, t(818) = -.32, p = .75) and a marginally significant effect in slider condition (Mslider-$500

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 36

Page 38: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

= 85 v. Mslider-$1000 = 129, t(818) = -1.71, p = .09). However, in the high numeric category, while

there was no effect of response range in the textbox conditions (Mtext-$500 = 103 v. Mtext-$1000 =

123, t(818) = -1.09, p = .28), in the slider conditions, bids for the larger response range (Mslider-

$1000 = 220) were higher than those for the smaller response range (Mslider-$500 = 125, t(818) = -

3.01, p < .01). These results are robust to distributional assumptions and the type of statistical

test (see table 2, web appendix B, and web appendix C).

Endpoint Influence. After being asked the minimum and maximum possible bids that

could be placed, participants were asked to what extent the maximum possible bid influenced

their high bids. These values were reverse coded from 1 to 5 such that higher numbers indicate

greater stated influence by the endpoint. A two-way ANOVA with response range and format as

independent variables revealed only a significant interaction effect (F(1, 409) = 7.01, p < .01).

Participants in the slider conditions for both the small (M = 1.74) and large (M = 1.96) response

range indicated that the endpoint of the response range had little effect on their high bid response

(F(1, 409) = 2.00, p = .16). However, in the textbox conditions, participants in the large response

range condition (M = 1.68) indicated that the endpoint had less of an influence on their high bid

response than those in the small response range condition (M = 2.03, F(1, 409) = 5.48, p = .02).

These results suggest that participants in the textbox conditions, who were presumably relying on

deliberative numeric comparisons, were conscious of the influence of the endpoints whereas

those in the slider condition, who were influenced by automatic visual distance comparisons,

were oblivious to the influence of the endpoints.

The results support hypotheses 2 and 3. Consistent with hypothesis 3, the results show

that slider scales, compared to textboxes, are more susceptible to the manipulation of response

range. Furthermore, consistent with hypothesis 2, the effect was stronger for numeric values that

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 37

Page 39: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

were closer to the endpoint.

ADDITIONAL STUDIES & META-ANALYSIS

We conducted a single-paper meta-analysis across the seven studies outlined in the paper

and an additional four studies detailed in web appendix G. The four studies included in the web

appendix were excluded from the paper for brevity, but provide further evidence for the mental

number line recalibration explanation. Experiment A1 replicates the real donation results of

experiment 1B. The only differences between experiment 1B and experiment A1 is that in the

latter version the slider scales were not anchored on the numeric values, we did not specify an

average donation reference point, and the study was run on Memorial Day. Experiment A2 uses

a paradigm similar to experiment 2 and introduced an additional condition, textbox + slider. The

textbox + slider condition was identical to the original textbox condition, but a slider scale was

included beneath the textbox with instructions for participants to confirm their bid from the

textbox after it was entered. The results from this study suggest that using a slider scale after

responding through a textbox does not result in the same endpoint assimilation as when the slider

scale is actively used in the response generation process. Experiment A3 demonstrates a result

similar to experiment 6, that range can moderate the endpoint assimilation effect, but in a much

more bounded and narrow domain—the amount of tip given on a taxi ride can be influenced by

the response range when slider scales are used to record payments. Experiment A4 demonstrates

that the endpoint assimilation effect is attenuated for participants who were told they can only

pay with cash versus credit cards—pain of paying by cash prompted participants to focus on the

starting point rather than the endpoint of the response range (Prelec and Loewenstein 1998;

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 38

Page 40: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Prelec and Simester 2001; Soman 2003; Raghubir and Srivastava 2008).

Using the process outlined by Lipsey and Wilson (2001), the effect size across these 11

studies is 0.38, a medium effect size, with a z-test value of 9.59, p < .001, indicating that this

mean sample size effect is statistically significant. The 95% confidence interval is 0.31 to 0.46.

The details around the descriptive statistics used in the meta-analysis are summarized in web

appendix G.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Organizations and researchers have been using textboxes and bounded scales—

specifically slider scales—interchangeably to record consumers’ participative pricing decisions.

The tacit assumption underlying this practice is that consumers’ innate valuations are invariant to

cosmetic changes in response formats. Although prior research has examined the effect of slider

formats on response rates and response times (Funke 2016; Funke, Reips, and Thomas 2011;

Roster, Lucianetti and Albaum 2015), no one has empirically tested this assumption. In the

present research, we examine whether there a systematic difference in valuations elicited by

textboxes and slider scales.

Experiment 1a demonstrated that undergraduate students offered higher bids for a mug

when the bid was elicited on a slider scale relative to a textbox. Experiment 1b demonstrated,

using real money, that people donate more money when the donations are elicited on a slider

scale than when they pay using a textbox. In both experiments, the range of possible responses

was equally salient for all participants; thus the slider scale endpoint assimilation effect cannot

be attributed to differences in knowledge of range information or the highest possible response.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 39

Page 41: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Experiment 2 demonstrated that if eBay or firms using ascending payment formats were to use

slider scales instead of textboxes to elicit bids, they are likely to elicit higher bid amounts.

Experiment 3 showed that the endpoint assimilation effect depends on the direction of

adjustment; in descending participative payment formats for firms such as Priceline, where

people adjust their prices downwards, slider scales can reduce values relative to textboxes.

Experiments 4, 5, and 6 demonstrate that the effect of slider scales is due to the salience of the

visual distance from the endpoint of the response range. Experiment 4 shows that values that are

considered “high” are more affected by response formats; “low” and “medium” values are less

affected by response formats. In Experiment 5 we manipulated the visual distance from the

endpoints of the slider by using a convex slider scale. In this experiment we found that responses

on a convex slider scale are more similar to the textbox responses than to those on the linear

slider scale. In Experiment 6 we altered distance from endpoints by manipulating the response

range. As predicted, the manipulation of response range had a larger effect on slider scale

response than on textbox responses because visual distance from endpoints does not play a role

for textboxes.

Substantive Implications. Our results suggest that managers in organizations as well as

consumers engaging in participative pricing decisions should be more mindful of the response

format used for eliciting monetary payments. In situations where consumers consider offering

higher prices, using a slider scale instead of a textbox can elicit higher payments from

consumers. So retailers that auction products, such as eBay or WineBid, are likely to see higher

revenues if they use a slider scale with a wide response range. In a similar vein, service

providers, such as taxi services and restaurants, are likely to realize higher revenues when they

use a slider scale instead of a textbox. However, in situations where consumers adjust their prices

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 40

Page 42: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

downwards from the listed prices, using a slider scale can backfire. Companies like Priceline are

more likely to elicit higher bids if they use a textbox, instead of a slider scale. From a consumer

welfare perspective, it is important for consumers to realize that response formats can alter their

monetary payments and their subjective magnitude perceptions of monetary values without their

awareness.

Theoretical Implications. Although our interest in this research question was triggered by

the current practices used to elicit valuations in participative pricing contexts, the results from

this research have had a profound effect on our perspectives on numerical cognition. Although,

the notion of a compressed internal mental number line with a non-linear calibration is now

widely accepted by researchers (Dehaene 2001; Dehaene et al. 2008), to the best of our

knowledge nobody has examined how the calibration of this mental number line could be

affected by response formats. Our research compliments previous work that has shown how the

range of anchors (Beatie and Baron 1991, Kahneman 1992) or response ranges (Janiszewski and

Lichtenstein 1999, Parducci 1965) can influence consumer responses by demonstrating the

importance of visualization of these ranges. Our results suggest that for some response formats,

beyond the range itself, visual distances from endpoints can become a cue for magnitude

judgments and this cue can change the calibration of the mental number line. Salience of the

visual distance from endpoints tends to linearize the internal number line, which is usually

compressed for higher values. These results call for new theoretical models that can account for

dynamic, context-dependent calibration of the internal number line. Identifying and

characterizing the determinants of such a dynamic interplay of the contextual factors and the

intrinsic representational systems is important for the evolution of numerical cognition theories.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 41

Page 43: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

REFERENCES

Adaval, Rashmi (2013), "The Utility of an Information Processing Approach for Behavioral Price Research," AMS review, 3(3), 130-134.

Adaval, Rashmi, and Kent B. Monroe (2002), "Automatic Construction and Use of Contextual Information for Product and Price Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 572-588.

Bagchi, Rajesh, and Derick F. Davis (2011), "29 for 70 Items or 70 Items for 29? How Presentation Order Affects Package Perceptions," Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 62-73.

Bagchi, Rajesh, and Derick F. Davis (2016), "The Role of Numerosity in Judgments and Decision-Making," Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 89-93.

Berman, Jonathan Z., and Deborah A. Small, (2012) "Self-Interest Without Selfishness: The Hedonic Benefit of Imposed Self-Interest," Psychological Science, 23(10), 1193-1199.

Bettman, James R., Mary Frances Luce, and John W. Payne (1998) "Constructive Consumer Choice Processes," Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 187-217.

Biswas, Abhijit, Sandeep Bhowmick, Abhijit Guha, and Dhruv Grewal (2013), "Consumer Evaluations of Sale Prices: Role of the Subtraction Principle," Journal of Marketing 77(4), 49-66.

Burson, Katherine A., Richard P. Larrick, and John G. Lynch Jr. (2009) "Six of One, Half Dozen of the Other: Expanding and Contracting Numerical Dimensions Produces Preference Reversals." Psychological Science 20(9), 1074-1078.

Dehaene, Stanislas (2001), “Précis of the Number Sense,” Mind & language, 16(1), 16-36. Dehaene, Stanislas, Véronique Izard, Elizabeth Spelke, and Pierre Pica (2008), "Log or Linear?

Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures," Science, 320(5880), 1217-1220.

Fischhoff, Baruch (1991), "Value Elicitation: Is There Anything in There?" American Psychologist, 46(8), 835.

Funke, Frederik (2016). “A Web Experiment Showing Negative Effects of Slider Scales Compared to Visual Analogue Scales and Radio Button Scales,” Social Science Computer Review 34(2), 244-254.

Funke, Frederik, Ulf-Dietrich Reips, and Randall K. Thomas (2011), “Sliders for the Smart: Type of Rating Scale on the Web Interacts with Educational Level,” Social Science Computer Review, 29(2), 221-231.

Galak, Jeff, Joseph P. Redden, Yang Yang, and Ellie J. Kyung (2014), "How Perceptions of Temporal Distance Influence Satiation," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 118-123.

Izard, Véronique, and Stanislas Dehaene (2008), “Calibrating the Mental Number Line,” Cognition, 106(3), 1221-1247.

Johnson, Donald M. (1944), “Generalization of a Scale of Values by the Averaging of Practice

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 42

Page 44: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Effects," Journal of Experimental Psychology 34 (6): 425. Lembregts, Christophe, and Mario Pandelaere (2013), "Falling Back on Numbers: Quantitative

Specifications as Sources of Control," 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology Annual Winter Conference.

Mark W. Lipsey, and David B. Wilson (2001), Practical Meta-analysis, Vol. 49. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage publications.

Markman, Keith D., and Corey L. Guenther (2007), "Psychological Momentum: Intuitive Physics and Naive Beliefs," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(6), 800-812.

Monroe, Kent B. (1971) "Measuring Price Thresholds by Psychophysics and Latitudes of Acceptance," Journal of Marketing Research, 460-464.

Monroe, Kent B., and Angela Y. Lee (1999), "Remembering versus Knowing: Issues in Buyers’ Processing of Price Information," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 207-225.

Mussweiler, Thomas, and Fritz Strack (1999), "Comparing is Believing: A Selective Accessibility Model of Judgmental Anchoring," European Review of Social Psychology, 10(1), 135-167

O’Guinn, Thomas Clayton, Robin J. Tanner, and Ahreum Maeng (2015), "Turning to Space: Social Density, Social Class, and the Value of Things in Stores," Journal of Consumer Research 42(2), 196-213.

Prelec, Drazen, and Duncan Simester, "Always Leave Home Without It: A Further Investigation of the Credit-Card Effect on Willingness to Pay," Marketing Letters, 12(1), 5-12.

Raghubir, Priya, and Joydeep Srivastava (2008), "Monopoly Money: The Effect of Payment Coupling and Form on Spending Behavior," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(3), 213.

Roster, Catherine A., Lorenzo Lucianetti, and Gerald Albaum (2015), “Exploring Slider vs. Categorical Response Formats in Web-based Surveys," Journal of Research Practice 11(1), p. 1.

Schley, Dan R., Christophe Lembregts, and Ellen Peters (2017), "The Role of Evaluation Mode on The Unit Effect," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(2), 278-286.

Soman, Dilip (2003), "The Effect of Payment Transparency on Consumption: Quasi-Experiments From The Field," Marketing Letters, 14(3) 173-183.

Thomas, Manoj and Vicki Morwitz (2009a), “Heuristics in Numerical Cognition: Implications for Pricing,” in Handbook of Research in Pricing, Vithala Rao, ed., Edward Elgar Publishing, 132-149

Thomas, Manoj and Vicki Morwitz (2009b), “The Ease of Computation Effect: The Interplay of Metacognitive Experiences and Naïve Theories in Judgments of Price Difference,” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (February), 81-91

Tzelgov, Joseph, Joachim Meyer, and Avishai Henik (1992) "Automatic and Intentional Processing of Numerical Information." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(1), 166.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 43

Page 45: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Tzelgov, Joseph, and Dana Ganor-Stern, (2005) "Automaticity in Processing Ordinal Information." Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, 55-66.

Valenzuela, Ana, and Priya Raghubir (2015), "Are Consumers Aware of Top–Bottom But Not of Left–Right Inferences? Implications for Shelf Space Positions," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21(3), 224.

Ward, Morgan K., and Darren W. Dahl (2014), "Should the Devil Sell Prada? Retail Rejection Increases Aspiring Consumers' Desire for the Brand." Journal of Consumer Research 41(3), 590-609.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 44

Page 46: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURES

FIGURE 1: ASCENDING PAYMENT FORMATS NUMERIC CATEGORY LIMENS USED FOR TEXTBOX RESPONSES

NUMERIC CATEGORY LIMENS USED FOR SLIDER SCALE RESPONSES

Note - In an ascending payment format, in the textbox condition, because payment values are compared to the lowest possible response, the limens that separate categories are assimilated towards the starting point (top line). In the slider scale condition where the visual distances from both the starting and end points are more salient, the limens that separate internal categories tend to assimilate towards the highest possible response (bottom line). Furthermore, the shift in category limens is more pronounced for higher values that are visually closer to the endpoint.

FIGURE 2: DESCENDING PAYMENT FORMATS

NUMERIC CATEGORY LIMENS USED FOR TEXTBOX RESPONSES

NUMERIC CATEGORY LIMENS USED FOR SLIDER SCALE RESPONSES

Note - In a descending payment format, in the textbox condition, people use a mental number line where the limens that separate categories are assimilated towards the starting point that is the highest possible response (top line). In the slider scale condition where the visual distances from both the starting point (right) and endpoint (left) are salient, the limens that separate internal categories are more assimilated toward the endpoint—the lowest possible response (bottom line). Furthermore, the shift in category limens is more pronounced for lower values that are closer to the endpoint.

PLOW PMEDIUM PHIGH PMIN PMAX

PLOW PMEDIUM PHIGH PMIN PMAX

PLOW PMEDIUM PHIGH PMAX PMIN

PLOW PMEDIUM PHIGH PMAX PMIN

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 45

Page 47: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURE 3: EXPERIMENT 1A STIMULI

TEXTBOX CONDITION

SLIDER CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 46

Page 48: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURE 4: EXP 2—EFFECT OF RESPONSE FORMAT FOR ASCENDING PAYMENT FORMAT

FIGURE 5: EXP 3 - EFFECT OF RESPONSE FORMAT FOR DESCENDING PAYMENT FORMATS

268

282

290

277

294

301

250

260

270

280

290

300

310

$239 $259 $279

$

Starting Bids For Three Laptops

Textbox Slider

118

130

144

124

137

146

129

142

155

100

110

120

130

140

150

160

Level 1 Level 2 Level 3

$

Starting Price Level

Slider-Left-to-Right Slider-Right-to-Left Textbox

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 47

Page 49: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURE 6: EXP 4 STIMULI—NON-SLIDING SCALE CONDITION

FIGURE 7: EXP 4 - MODERATION BY NUMERIC CATEGORY

289

395

561

298

437

621

299

439

622

200

300

400

500

600

700

Low Medium High

$

Numeric Category

Textbox Slider Non-Sliding Scale

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 48

Page 50: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURE 8: CALIBRATION OF THE TWO SLIDERS

FIGURE 9: EXP 5 CONVEX VERSUS LINEAR SLIDERS

200

300

400

500

600

700

800

900

1000

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Bid

Valu

es ($

)

Linear

Convex

277

381

551

284

398

565

304

435

596

200

300

400

500

600

Low Medium High

$

Numeric Category

Textbox Convex Slider Linear Slider

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 49

Page 51: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

FIGURE 10: EXP 6 MODERATION BY RESPONSE RANGE

64 72 87

137

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

140

160

To $500 To $1000

$

Response Rage

Textbox Slider

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 50

Page 52: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

TABLE 1: DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS BY CONDITION

1A: Mug Auction (Ascending Payment Format)

Textbox Slider n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Mug Bid Price 62 8.01 6.64 0.84 65 11.40 9.77 1.21 1B: Veteran's Day Donations (Ascending Payment Format)

Textbox Slider n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Donation 105 24.04 30.61 2.99 99 37.76 39.13 3.93

2. eBay Bids (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Textbox Slider

Starting Bid n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE $239 97 268.02 55.54 5.64 102 276.71 42.79 4.24 $259 97 281.85 36.32 3.69 102 293.70 40.24 3.98 $279 97 290.42 28.07 2.86 102 301.36 40.30 3.99 3. Priceline Bids (Descending Payment Format: Hotel Rooms)

Textbox Slider Left-to-Right Slider Right-to-Left Starting Price n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE Level 1 100 128.59 22.80 2.28 101 117.81 23.45 2.33 103 123.89 21.09 2.08 Level 2 100 142.22 25.27 2.53 101 130.06 26.02 2.59 103 136.72 23.92 2.36 Level 3 100 154.54 28.02 2.82 101 143.90 29.52 2.94 103 145.95 29.04 2.86 4. Moderation by Distance to Endpoint (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Numeric Category

Textbox Slider Scale n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Low 110 289.33 64.38 6.14 111 297.82 72.03 6.84 112 298.64 61.76 5.84 Medium 110 395.17 114.12 10.88 111 437.47 110.98 10.53 112 439.00 93.66 8.85 High 110 560.62 214.19 20.42 111 621.20 183.93 17.46 112 621.68 167.04 15.78

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 51

Page 53: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

5. A Convex Slider (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops)

Numeric Category

Textbox Convex Slider Slider n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Low 86 276.80 41.02 4.42 88 284.43 45.43 4.84 88 303.80 63.35 6.75 Medium 86 380.79 99.22 10.70 88 397.78 103.61 11.04 88 435.19 115.59 12.32 High 86 551.22 188.83 20.36 88 564.63 200.93 21.42 88 595.92 177.03 18.87 6. Moderation by Endpoint Size (Ascending Payment Format: Wine) Range x Numeric Category

Textbox Slider

n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE $500 range

Low 107 29.39 18.07 1.75 99 50.78 62.80 6.31 Medium 107 59.41 59.66 5.77 99 84.68 80.50 8.09 High 107 102.98 115.01 11.12 99 125.31 112.75 11.33

$1000 range Low 103 29.14 14.07 1.39 104 62.90 96.63 9.48 Medium 103 65.08 72.97 7.19 104 128.77 160.53 15.74 High 103 122.64 160.97 15.86 104 219.83 262.26 25.72

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 52

Page 54: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

TABLE 2: MEDIANS AND MEDIAN TESTS BY CONDITION

1A: Mug Auction (Ascending Payment Format)

Textbox Slider Wilcoxon Two-Sample

Test Median Median Z p

Mug Bid Price 6.0 10.0 -1.56 0.06 1B: Veteran's Day Donations (Ascending Payment Format)

Textbox Slider Wilcoxon Two-Sample

Test Median Median Z p

Donation Amount 10.0 20.0 -2.31 0.01 2. eBay Bids (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops)

Textbox Slider Wilcoxon Two-Sample

Test Starting Price Median Median Z p $239 250.0 256.0 -2.80 0.003 $259 270.0 278.0 -3.43 <.001 $279 280.5 287.0 -3.81 <.001 3. Priceline Bids (Descending Payment Format: Hotel Rooms)

Textbox Slider L-to-R Slider R-to-L Wilcoxon Two-Sample Tests Textbox v. L-to-R Textbox v. R-to-L L-to-R v. R-to-L

Starting Price Median Median Median Z p Z p Z p Level 1 130.0 116.0 127.5 3.48 <.001 1.89 0.03 -1.87 0.03 Level 2 145.0 130.0 140.0 3.35 <.001 1.78 0.04 -2.04 0.02 Level 3 157.5 142.0 151.0 2.66 0.004 2.07 0.02 -0.68 0.25

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 53

Page 55: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

4. Moderation by Distance to Endpoint (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops)

Numeric Category

Textbox Slider Scale Wilcoxon Two-Sample Tests Textbox v. Slider Textbox v. Scale Slider v. Scale

Median Median Median Z p Z p Z p Low 260.0 270.0 289.0 -0.26 0.40 0.12 0.45 -0.03 0.49 Medium 370.0 401.0 424.0 -3.07 0.001 -3.14 0.0009 -0.38 0.35 High 500.0 605.0 619.0 -2.40 0.008 -2.30 0.01 0.05 0.48 5. A Convex Slider (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops)

Numeric Category

Textbox Convex Slider Slider Wilcoxon Two-Sample Tests

Textbox v. Slider Textbox v.

Convex Slider Median Median Median Z p Z p

Low 260.0 269.5 287.5 -3.67 0.0001 -0.86 0.20 Medium 350.0 373.0 425.0 -3.39 0.0004 -1.27 0.10 High 500.0 550.0 600.0 -1.81 0.04 -0.40 0.34 6. Moderation by Endpoint Size (Ascending Payment Format: Wine) Range x Numeric Category

Textbox Slider Wilcoxon Two-Sample

Test Median Median Z p

$500 range Low 25.0 30.0 -4.24 <.001 Medium 40.0 50.0 -3.81 <.001 High 60.0 75.0 -2.85 0.002

$1000 range Low 25.0 27.0 -2.76 0.003 Medium 40.0 56.0 -3.59 <.001 High 60.0 97.0 -3.47 <.001

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 54

Page 56: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX Web Appendix A: Stimuli Screenshots Web Appendix B: Summary of Results Comparing Distributions Web Appendix C: Summary of Results with Outlier Exclusions Web Appendix D: Experiment 3 Additional Process Measures Web Appendix E: Experiment 5 Log-Transformation Analysis Web Appendix F: Experiment 5 Post-Hoc Concave Slider Condition Analysis Web Appendix G: Additional Studies (included in Single Paper Meta-Analysis) Web Appendix H: Single Paper Meta-Analysis Details

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 55

Page 57: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.1A: EXPERIMENT 1A STIMULI TEXTBOX CONDITION

SLIDER CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 56

Page 58: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.1B: EXPERIMENT 1B STIMULI CHARITY DESCRIPTION

TEXTBOX CONDITION

SLIDER CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 57

Page 59: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.2: EXPERIMENT 2 STIMULI TEXTBOX CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 58

Page 60: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

SLIDER CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 59

Page 61: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.3: EXPERIMENT 3 STIMULI

TEXTBOX CONDITION

LEFT-TO-RIGHT SLIDER CONDITION

RIGHT-TO-LEFT SLIDER CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 60

Page 62: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.4: EXPERIMENT 4 STIMULI

TEXTBOX CONDITION

SLIDER CONDITION

SCALE CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 61

Page 63: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.5: EXPERIMENT 5 STIMULI

STIMULI INFORMATION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 62

Page 64: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

CONVEX SLIDER CONDITION Sliders were anchored at the left side. Values are illustrative indicating the starting point,

midpoint, and the endpoint of the line.

LINEAR SLIDER CONDITION Sliders were anchored at the left side. Values are illustrative indicating the starting point,

midpoint, and the endpoint of the line.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 63

Page 65: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

TEXTBOX CONDITION

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 64

Page 66: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX A.6: EXPERIMENT 6 STIMULI

INPUT DESCRIPTION: $500 ENDPOINT

INPUT DESCRIPTION: $1000 ENDPOINT

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 65

Page 67: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

WEB APPENDIX B: SUMMARY OF RESULTS COMPARING NORMAL AND POISSON DISTRIBUTIONS Normal Poisson

num df den df F p F p 1A: Mug Auction (Ascending Payment Format) Format 1 125 5.18 0.02 37.05 <.001

1B: Veteran's Day Donations (Ascending Payment Format) Format 1 202 7.83 0.006 307.29 <.001

2. eBay Bids (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Format 1 197 4.29 0.04 5.61 0.02 Starting Bid 2 393 44.08 <.001 98.49 <.001 Format*Starting Bid 2 393 0.21 0.81 0.30 0.74

3. Priceline Bids (Descending Payment Format: Hotel Rooms) Format 2 301 5.30 0.005 5.00 0.007 Starting Price Level 2 1504 558.22 <.001 694.68 <.001 City 1 1504 265.35 <.001 343.47 <.001 Format*Starting Price Level 4 1504 2.40 0.05 3.07 0.02

Format*City 2 1504 1.12 0.33 2.20 0.11

Starting Price Level*City 2 1504 21.21 <.001 34.71 <.001

3-way interaction 4 1504 0.33 0.86 0.35 0.84

4. Moderation by Distance to Endpoint (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Format 2 330 4.55 0.01 5.66 0.004 Numeric Category 2 660 910.92 <.001 17,232.30 <.001 Format*Numeric Category 4 660 2.97 0.02 22.08 <.001

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 66

Page 68: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Web Appendix:

5. A Convex Slider (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Format 2 259 3.77 0.02 3.92 0.02

Numeric Category 2 518 605.13 <.001 12,157.10 <.001

Format*Numeric Category 4 518 0.5 0.74 8.47 <.001

6. Moderation by Endpoint Size (Ascending Payment Format: Wine) Format 1 409 18.14 <.001 28.97 <.001

Numeric Category 2 818 187.33 <.001 9752.79 <.001

Range 1 409 8.07 0.005 1.64 0.20

Format*Numeric Category 2 818 4.84 0.008 123.51 <.001

Format*Range 1 409 4.12 0.04 0.81 0.37

Numeric Category*Range 2 818 12.50 <.001 116.53 <.001

3-way interaction 2 818 4.60 0.01 10.75 <.001

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 67

Page 69: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX C: SUMMARY OF RESULTS AFTER OUTLIER EXCLUSIONS

Data points that were three standard deviations away from the means were identified as outliers and excluded in this analysis. For studies 3, 4, 5 & 6 to be conservative, we identified the outliers for Low, Medium, and High bids separately. Removing outliers from the overall average would result in exclusion of more data points from the high numeric category conditions than from the low numeric category conditions. The results summarized below show that our statistical inferences are robust and not influenced by these outliers.

1A: Mug Auction (Ascending Payment Format) – No outlier exclusions Textbox Slider

n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE Mug Bid Price - - - - - - - -

1B: Veteran's Day Donations (Ascending Payment Format) – No outlier exclusions Textbox Slider

n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE Donation - - - - - - - - 2. eBay Bids (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Starting Price

Textbox Slider n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

$239 93 258.58 28.21 2.93 98 272.91 37.91 3.83 $259 93 276.23 22.98 2.38 98 288.35 28.76 2.90 $279 93 285.92 11.63 1.21 98 296.02 23.11 2.33

3. Priceline Bids (Descending Payment Format: Hotel Rooms) Starting Price

Textbox Slider Left-to-Right Slider Right-to-Left n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Level 1 99 128.27 22.69 2.28 100 118.52 22.48 2.25 102 124.45 20.41 2.02 Level 2 99 141.91 25.22 2.53 100 130.77 25.18 2.52 102 137.42 22.96 2.27 Level 3 99 154.54 28.02 2.82 100 144.77 28.36 2.84 102 146.47 28.70 2.84

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 68

Page 70: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

4. Moderation by Distance to Endpoint (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Numeric Category

Textbox Slider Scale n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Low 106 279.02 34.53 3.35 109 290.39 44.87 4.30 108 289.28 37.48 3.61 Medium 106 383.67 98.99 9.61 109 431.28 101.47 9.72 108 432.61 85.15 8.19 High 106 548.92 208.89 20.29 109 618.83 183.41 17.57 108 616.50 163.91 15.77 5. A Convex Slider (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Numeric Category

Textbox Convex Slider Slider n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE

Low 83 269.95 17.39 1.91 86 279.15 29.51 3.18 83 293.37 39.18 4.30 Medium 83 369.87 81.24 8.92 86 392.66 97.85 10.55 83 420.20 98.64 10.83 High 83 540.45 182.13 19.99 86 562.63 198.69 21.42 83 582.51 171.67 18.84 6. Moderation by Endpoint Size (Ascending Payment Format: Wine) Range x Numeric Category

Textbox Slider

n Mean STDV SE n Mean STDV SE $500 range

Low 107 29.39 18.07 1.75 95 40.17 34.81 3.57 Medium 107 59.41 59.66 5.77 95 73.55 59.56 6.11 High 107 102.98 115.01 11.12 95 112.87 95.09 9.76

$1000 range Low 99 28.60 13.99 1.41 92 39.34 33.06 3.45 Medium 99 52.56 35.13 3.53 92 81.47 72.02 7.51 High 99 98.30 106.20 10.67 92 140.12 140.23 14.62

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 69

Page 71: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX D: EXPERIMENT 3 ADDITIONAL PROCESS MEASURES RESULTS

In experiment 3, participants responded to the same process measures as in experiment 2. Ease of Responding. As in the previous experiment, ease of responding scores were

reverse coded. There was no effect of the condition on self-reported ease of responding (F < 1). The average ease of responding scores (reverse coded) were quite high in all three conditions (Mleft-to-right = 4.18, Mright-to-left =4.34, Mtextbox = 4.27).

Awareness of Bias. Similar to experiment 2, we examined participants’ open-ended responses and none of them mentioned that their responses might have been influenced by response format. Next we analyzed their responses to the direct question about the influence of response formats. Furthermore, as in experiment 2, participants were unaware of the effect of response formats on their responses. A vast majority (82%) reported that their bid values would not be affected by the response format. Some of them (13%) suspected that their bids would be higher using the slider scale. Only five percent believed that their bids would be higher using textboxes. These proportions did not change across the three between-subjects conditions (p = .24). Together, these results suggest that participants do not suspect that response format could change their responses. Even if they thought it might, most of their predictions were in the opposite direction—they thought the slider scale would increase their bids.

Preference for Response Formats. Participants’ preference for response formats was submitted to the same logistic regression as in experiment 2. Participants were more likely to prefer the slider scale response format in the left-to-right-slider condition (65%) and right-to-left-slider condition (68%) relative to when they used the textbox (29%). These results are consistent with those from experiment 2, again suggesting that people do not have stable preferences for a response format and adapt to whatever response format they are using.

The process measures corroborate the results from the previous experiment that the differences in valuations were not due to ease of responding; participants found both the response formats—slider scales and textboxes—equally easy to use. Additionally, as in the previous experiment, most participants were unaware of the surreptitious influence of response formats on their bid values, suggesting that the psychological mechanism underlying the slider scale effect operates outside of people’s awareness.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 70

Page 72: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX E: EXPERIMENT 5 LOG-TRANSFORMATION ANALYSIS

In experiment 5, the Format*Numeric Category interaction is not significant for the normal distribution but it is significant for the Poisson distribution. A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test of normality confirmed that the willingness-to-pay data are not normally distributed, and a visual examination of the frequency distribution of our raw data revealed that that distribution had a long right tail closely resembling a Poisson distribution, thus the reason for specifying a Poisson error term in our model reported in the paper. However, to ensure that our results are not an artifact of the Poisson distributional assumption, we ran a mixed model assuming normal distribution after transforming the raw data to reduce the long right tail. Specifically, we log transformed the data after subtracting the starting bid using the formula logWTP = log(WTP – 259 + 1). We added 1 to the right hand side to ensure that the results are not affected by omission of zeros. The results from this model with log transformation of the dependent measure and normal distribution assumption are very similar to that of the results from the model with the Poisson distribution. The effects of response format (F(2, 259) = 5.13, p < .01), numeric category (F(2, 518) = 832.9, p < .01), and the interaction between the two (F(4, 518) = 5.56, p < .01) were all significant. [To compare this with the Poisson model results, in that model also we found that the effects of response format (F(2, 259) = 3.92, p = .02), numeric category (F(2, 518) = 12,157.10, p < .01), and the interaction between the two (F(4, 518) = 8.47, p < .01) were all significant.] Moreover, the normal model with the log-transformed DV had better fit (AIC = 2565) than the normal model without the transformation (AIC = 9637). Thus, our results are not an artifact of the modeling assumption as similar results are obtained using both the Poisson model and using a normal model with the log-transformed dependent measure. We also note that, when we look at the pattern of means, support for H2 is somewhat equivocal in experiment 5 because the difference between linear scale and textbox condition is stronger for the medium bids than for high bids. However, if instead of means we consider the medians that are less susceptible to idiosyncratic responses, the effect of slider scale is the weakest for LOW bid (Linear vs. Text = 28), higher for MEDIUM bids (Linear vs. Text = 52), and the strongest for HIGH bids (Linear vs. Text = 100). Furthermore, despite these mild inconsistencies we do find in all studies that the effect of slider scale is the weakest for LOW bids relative to MEDIUM and HIGH bids.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 71

Page 73: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX F: EXPERIMENT 5 POST-HOC CONCAVE SLIDER CONDITION ANALYSIS

As a follow-up analysis to experiment 5, we ran an additional post-hoc condition with a concave slider scale where bid values were a function of the distance from the starting point using the equation: y = 259 + 119X + -5x2. The relationship between these slider scales is depicted in figure F.1 below. The screen shot of the concave slider scale stimuli is shown in figure F.2.

FIGURE F.1: CALIBRATION OF THE THREE SLIDERS

FIGURE F.2: CONCAVE SLIDER STIMULI

Participants. One hundred and one U.S.-based participants on mTurk participated in this experiment in exchange for 51 cents (51% female, Mage = 36.5 years).

200

300

400

500

600

700

800

900

1000

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Bid

Valu

es ($

)

Markers on the Slider Scale

Linear Concave Convex

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 72

Page 74: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

Procedures. The experiment procedures were nearly identical to those of experiment 5 and consisted of a single condition with a concave slider scale where the bid values displayed on the slider were determined by the equation outlined above. Note that compared to the linear slider scale, the midpoint of the concave slider scale appears at $739 (see figure F.2). Therefore, the overall pattern that we expect is that responses on the concave slider scale will be more extreme than those on the linear and convex slides scales and textboxes as the numeric category increases.

Results. To analyze the results, the data from the post hoc concave slider condition was combined with the dataset from experiment 5 that included the textbox, linear slider, and convex slider scale conditions. Because the post hoc condition was not randomly assigned, we recognize that there are limitations to interpreting the results, but they shed further light on the process of the impact of visualizing the mental number line in different ways.

The analysis was conducted with PROC GLIMMIX specifying a Poisson distribution with response format (textbox, linear slider, convex slider, concave slider) as a between-subjects factor and numeric category (low, medium, high) as a within-subjects factor and the bid amount as the dependent measure. The effects of response format (F(3, 359) = 5.75, p < .01), numeric category (F(2, 718) = 18,113,50, p < .01), and the interaction between the two (F(6, 718) = 28.82, p < .01) were all significant (see figure F.3)

FIGURE F.3: POST-HOC ANALYSIS RESULTS

The average bid in the concave slider condition ($461) was not significantly greater than in the linear slider condition ($445, t(359) = 1.11, p = .27), but was significantly greater than the convex slider ($416, t(359) = 3.06, p < .01), and textbox ($403, t(359) = 3.87, p < .01) conditions. However, for high bids, bids in the concave slider condition ($641) were significantly greater than those in the linear slider ($596, t(718) = 2.42, p = .02) condition, as well as the convex slider ($565, t(718) = 4.09, p < .01) and textbox ($551, t(718) = 4.78, p < .01) conditions. Thus the addition of the concave slider condition further underscores the role that visualization of the number line plays in consumers’ price magnitude judgments by showing a further exacerbation of the effect for large bids relative to the textbox, convex slider, and linear slider conditions.

277

381

551

284

398

565

304

435

596

295

450

641

200

300

400

500

600

700

Low Medium High

$

Textbox Convex Slider Linear Slider Concave Slider

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 73

Page 75: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX G: ADDITIONAL STUDIES (INCLUDED IN SINGLE PAPER META-ANALYSIS) Appendix Experiment Descriptions

These experiments are included in our meta-analysis but not discussed in detail in the manuscript because of space constraints.

Exp Domain Independent Measures

Dependent Measures Primary Purpose & Key Finding

A1 Memorial

Day Donation

Response format (textbox, slider) Donations Replication of experiment 1b without average donation

reference information

A2 eBay Bids

Response format (textbox, slider, slider + textbox)

Starting bid ($239, $259, $279)

Laptop Bids

Introduction of slider + textbox condition to demonstrate that the mere presence of a line without using it in decision making does not yield the endpoint assimilation effect

A3 Taxi Cab Tips

Response format (textbox, slider) Range ($50, $100, $150)

Tip amount on $58 fare

Demonstrate moderation of the endpoint assimilation effect depending on response endpoint size, even for a relatively constrained domain (e.g., tip amount)

A4 Taxi Cab Tips

Response format (textbox, slider) Payment form (cash, credit)

Tip amount on $58 fare

Demonstrate that focus on the starting point (e.g., cash payments) leads to moderation of the effect eliminating the endpoint assimilation effect relative to when people make credit payments

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 74

Page 76: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS BY CONDITION

A1: Memorial Day Donation Experiment (Ascending Payment Format) Textbox Slider

n Mean Median STDV SE n Mean Median STDV SE Donation Amount 78 18.40 0.00 27.91 3.16 76 29.20 9.50 37.81 4.34

A2: eBay Bids - Slider + Textbox Condition (Ascending Payment Format: Laptops) Textbox Slider Textbox + Slider

n Mean Median STDV SE n Mean Median STDV SE n Mean Median STDV SE Average Bid 100 281.91 268.33 44.18 4.42 104 287.47 277.67 30.45 2.99 101 282.83 272.33 31.12 3.10

A3: Cab Study - Moderation By Range (Ascending Payment Format: Tip Payment)

Range Textbox Slider

n Mean Median STDV SE n Mean Median STDV SE $0 to $75 52 66.61 65.00 4.39 0.61 52 65.77 65.00 3.95 0.55 $0 to $100 51 66.31 65.00 4.75 0.66 52 66.44 66.00 4.41 0.61 $0 to $150 51 66.35 65.00 4.12 0.58 53 69.55 68.00 9.44 1.30

A4: Cab Study: Pain of Payment (Ascending Payment Format: Tip Payment)

Payment Mode

Textbox Slider

n Mean Median STDV SE n Mean Median STDV SE Cash 104 68.03 70.00 5.29 0.52 102 69.78 68.00 10.35 1.02 Credit 103 68.02 68.00 6.68 0.66 104 69.73 68.00 6.85 0.67

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 75

Page 77: Slider Scale or Text Box: How Response Format Shapes Responses

WEB APPENDIX H: SINGLE PAPER META-ANALYSIS DETAILS We conducted the meta-analysis using the process outlined by Lipsey and Wilson (2001) for a standardized mean difference. The conditions included in the meta-analysis and the associated sample sizes, mean, and standard deviations are included below. Textbox Slider

Exp Conditions Included n Mean STDV n Mean STDV Effect Size

1A Textbox v. Slider 62 8.01 6.64 65 11.40 9.77 0.40

1B Textbox v. Slider 105 24.04 30.61 99 37.76 39.13 0.39

2 Textbox v. Slider 97 279.99 36.26 102 290.59 35.46 0.30

3 Textbox v. Slider Left + Slider Right* (reversed)

100 141.85 24.68 204 133.08 24.66 0.36

4 Textbox v. Slider + Non-Slider Scale (medium + high bids only)

110 477.90 160.02 223 529.84 132.34 0.37

5 Textbox v. Slider (medium + high bids only)

86 466.01 137.50 88 515.56 142.69 0.35

6 Textbox v. Slider 210 68.03 70.66 203 112.66 133.11 0.42

A1 Textbox v. Slider 78 18.40 27.91 76 29.20 37.81 0.33

A2 Textbox v. Slider 100 281.91 44.18 104 287.47 30.45 0.15

A3 Textbox v. Slider (across ranges $100 and $150)

155 13.18 5.69 155 21.86 15.98 0.72

A4 Text v. Slider (credit conditions only)

103 68.02 6.68 104 69.73 6.85 0.25

Note that for experiment 3 indicated by *, the means for slider and textbox were reversed when entered in the effect size analysis because we predicted an opposite effect with descending payment formats.

Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 76