Top Banner
SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES i Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities and Educational Outcomes: Examining the Contribution of Sex-Role Identification David Hugh Reilly BPsych (Hons) BSoft. Eng. (Hons) School of Applied Psychology Griffith Health Griffith University, Gold Coast Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Applied Psychology February 2019
387

sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

May 10, 2023

Download

Documents

Khang Minh
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES i

Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities and Educational Outcomes: Examining the Contribution of Sex-Role Identification

David Hugh Reilly

BPsych (Hons) BSoft. Eng. (Hons)

School of Applied Psychology

Griffith Health

Griffith University, Gold Coast

Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Applied Psychology

February 2019

Page 2: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES ii

Abstract

Sex differences in cognitive ability have been documented in psychological

research for over a century, and the research area has seen considerable changes in

theoretical perspectives and methodology. While males and females do not differ in

general intelligence, an extensive body of literature documents sex differences in more

specific cognitive tasks (for reviews see Halpern, 2000; Kimura, 2000; Maccoby &

Jacklin, 1974). Males on average perform at a higher level on tasks that rely on visual-

spatial ability, and this has been linked to later gender gaps in quantitative abilities such

as mathematics and science and to the underrepresentation of women in science,

technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM)-related fields. Females as a group do

better at tasks involving verbal and language abilities which have been linked to wide

gender gaps in reading and writing, as well as the underrepresentation of men in post-

secondary education. Some researchers have argued that sex differences in cognitive

ability are declining in response to social changes in the roles and status of women, but

methodological limitations and use of convenience samples have limited previous

enquiries seeking to test that hypothesis.

The aim of this course of research was twofold. Firstly, using the statistical

technique of meta-analysis to examine the evidence for sex differences in visual-spatial,

verbal and quantitative abilities, and - given the passage of time - whether they were

declining in response to changes in the roles of men and women in society. This was

addressed through a series of studies that examined: i) nationally representative samples

of student testing data from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP)

in the United States, ii) cross-cultural samples of student testing data from the

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA). Secondly, to determine the

contribution of sex-typed personality traits and behaviours (collectively referred to as

sex-role identity) to the development of individual differences in visual-spatial and

verbal ability.

This goal was addressed through a sequence of three experimental studies.

Empirical study 1 sought to provide the most comprehensive assessment of the sex-role

mediation hypothesis conducted to-date, by examining performance across a range of

visual-spatial and verbal ability tasks. Subjects high in masculinity performed better on

visual-spatial tasks, while subjects high in femininity performed better on verbal

Page 3: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES iii

language tasks. Mediation analysis showed that sex-role identification acted as a

mediator of the sex difference in cognitive tasks.

Having found evidence for sex-role differences, Empirical Study 2 sought to

test whether the observed sex-role differences reflected latent ability, or alternately the

role of stereotype threat and task labelling on performance. The way in which a person

appraises the testing situation (and the types of skills a task may require) can work hand

in hand with sex-role conformity pressures to increase or to decrease task performance.

Finally, Empirical Study 3 sought to address a limitation in the existing

theoretical models for sex differences in cognitive ability, namely that males and

females show different patterns of self-estimation of intellectual ability (termed the

male-hubris female-humility problem). Study 3 examined the contribution of sex-role

identity to self-estimated intelligence, as well as the accuracy of personal judgements of

ability by administering the Cattel’s Culture Fair Test of Intelligence. Results showed

that the degree of masculine identification predicted self-estimated intelligence scores.

A large body of research has shown that self-appraisal of intellectual abilities and self-

efficacy beliefs guide the selection of coursework in secondary and tertiary education

and form an important component of career decision-making. This may explain to some

degree gender-specific differences in certain fields of STEM.

Collectively, the results of these studies are used to refine existing

psychobiosocial models of sex differences in cognitive abilities, to explain both the

differences between males and females but also within-sex variability. It suggests

masculine and feminine sex-role identification is an important individual differences

factor to consider, and that these shape intellectual self-image and self-efficacy beliefs.

Page 4: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES i

STATEMENT OF ORIGINALITY

This work has not previously been submitted for a degree or diploma in any university. To the best of my knowledge and belief, the thesis contains no material previously published or written by another person except where due reference is made in the thesis itself.

[signature redacted]

__________________________

David H. Reilly,

December 2018

Page 5: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES ii

Acknowledgement of Published and Unpublished Papers Included in this Thesis

Section 9.1 of the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research (“Criteria for Authorship”), in accordance with Section 5 of the Australian Code

for the Responsible Conduct of Research, states:

To be named as an author, a researcher must have made a substantial scholarly contribution to the creative or scholarly work that constitutes the research output, and be able to take public responsibility for at least that part of the work they contributed. Attribution of authorship depends to some extent on the discipline and publisher policies, but in all cases, authorship must be based on substantial contributions in a combination of one or more of:

conception and design of the research project analysis and interpretation of research data drafting or making significant parts of the creative or scholarly work or

critically revising it so as to contribute significantly to the final output.

Section 9.3 of the Griffith University Code (“Responsibilities of Researchers”), in

accordance with Section 5 of the Australian Code, states:

Researchers are expected to: Offer authorship to all people, including research trainees, who meet the

criteria for authorship listed above, but only those people. accept or decline offers of authorship promptly in writing. Include in the list of authors only those who have accepted authorship Appoint one author to be the executive author to record authorship and

manage correspondence about the work with the publisher and other interested parties.

Acknowledge all those who have contributed to the research, facilities or materials but who do not qualify as authors, such as research assistants, technical staff, and advisors on cultural or community knowledge. Obtain written consent to name individuals.

Included in this thesis is the paper in Chapter 6 for which I am the sole author.

Additionally, included in this thesis are the papers in Chapters 3, 4, 5, 7 and 8 which are

co-authored with other researchers. My contribution to each co-authored paper is

outlined at the front of the relevant chapter, and I acknowledge the support and guidance

of my supervisors in preparing these manuscripts. The bibliographic details of these

publications are included for each chapter, along with a copyright statement.

Page 6: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES iii

List of Publications and Conference Papers Arising from this PhD Research Programme

Journal articles

(Listed in order in which these articles appear in this thesis):

1. Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2017). Gender differences in spatial

ability: Implications for STEM education and approaches to reducing the gender gap for

parents and educators. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Visual-Spatial Ability: Transforming

Research into Practice (pp. 195-224). Switzerland: Springer International.

2. Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and

science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662. doi: 10.1037/edu0000012

3. Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2018). Gender differences in reading and

writing achievement: Evidence from the National Assessment of Educational Progress

(NAEP). American Psychologist. doi: 10.1037/amp000035

4. Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7),

e39904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

5. Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-

analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521-535. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

6. Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in

specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147-158. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Additional Articles and Conference Papers referenced

(but not included due to space requirements)

7. Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2017). Investigating gender differences in

mathematics and science: Results from the 2011 Trends in Mathematics and Science

Survey. Research in Science Education, 1-26. doi: 10.1007/s11165-017-9630-6

8. Reilly, D. (2015). Gender differences in reading from a cross-cultural perspective- the

contribution of gender equality. Paper presented at the International Convention of

Psychological Science, Amsterdam, Netherlands. doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.18218.72647

Page 7: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES iv

List of Figures

Chapter 1:Introduction

Figure 1.1. Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis …………….…………..………10

Figure 1.2. New student enrolment in higher education diplomas and degrees in Australia,

separated by gender……………………………………………………….…..…..……….16

Chapter 2: Literature Review

Figure 2.1 Sex differences in mathematical performance on the SAT-M over the past two

decades (1996-2016)…………..………………………………………………. ………....62

Chapter 9: Empirical Study 2

Figure 9.1. Sex-role mediation theory of cognitive abilities……………………………..144

Figure 9.2 Mean performance on the Group Embedded Figures Test…………….……..155

Figure 9.3 Mean performance on the Vandenberg Mental Rotation ……………...…….157

Figure 9.4 Mean completion time for the Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task….……….158

Figure 9.5 Mean number of words generated in verbal fluency task ……………...…….160

Chapter 10: Empirical Study 3

Figure 10.1 Stimulus material used for self-estimation of intelligence………………… 182

Figure 10.2 Distribution of measured IQ scores in the sample…………………....……. 187

Figure 10.3 Rosenberg General Self-Esteem scores across sex/sex-role categories……. 188

Figure 10.4 Self-estimated IQ scores across sex-role categories, for males and

females……………………………………………………………………………………190

Figure 10.5 Scatterplot of association between self-estimated and psychometric IQ, for

males and females respectively…………………………………………………………..192

Figure 10.7 Indirect effect of sex on SEI, with masculine sex-roles acting as a mediator195

Chapter 11 Discussion

Figure 11.1 Biosociocultural model of sex-role identification acquisition………………240

Figure 11.2 Sex-role mediation theory of cognitive development……………………… 242

Page 8: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES v

Acknowledgements

I will be eternally grateful to my primary supervisor, Professor David Neumann

for his guidance, encouragement and support throughout my candidature. Not every

supervisor pairing is ideal, but I sincerely believe that this one was bespoke in every

way. David encouraged me to publish early, and often, which gave my work a wider

scrutiny through the peer-review process and offered excellent grounding in academic

publishing. His encouragement and support meant a great deal to me as a fledgling

researcher: David grants his research students with independence and autonomy but at

the same time is always waiting in the wings to scaffold and support them when they

need it. During my initial data collection he’d often pop on by to check in, and then as

he saw I grew in confidence the visits would be more spaced out, and sometimes he’d

just stick his head in the door unobtrusively to see that all was proceeding well. It might

seem like a small thing, but to know that your supervisor has confidence in you means a

great deal. His door was door was always open, and one of the most wonderful things

about his supervision (and talking with fellow PhD and honours students, it’s a

commonly recurring theme) is that despite his extremely busy teaching, research and

administrative schedule, without exception, he makes time for everyone. His notes and

revisions on chapters and manuscripts are always carefully considered, showing depth

of thought, and his feedback was always supportive and encouraging. And having been

privileged to have had him as a statistics lecturer during my undergraduate, I have tried

to emulate (with a few embellishments of my own) his teaching style. It was a truly

strange experience, that transition from student/teacher to then an equal collaborator on

a research project, and then an independent researcher. I am so very grateful that he

took a chance on me.

Likewise I am immensely grateful to have had Assoc. Professor Glenda

Andrews as my associate supervisor. To have one competent supervisor is a boon, but

Page 9: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES vi

to have two was a genuine blessing. At every stage of my candidature Glenda has been

supportive and encouraging, and brought to bear her expertise with cognitive and

educational psychology, especially in the design and planning of studies. For example, I

might not have included psychometrically measured intelligence in the form of the

Cattell Culture Fair IQ Test but for her, which added greater depth to the study and its

conclusions. Her contributions have been invaluable, and just like David she invested a

significant amount of time, thought and energies into this programme of research.

Glenda also has such a keen eye for grammar and style as well, and was a real asset in

preparing manuscripts for submission. I’ve sometimes joked with her that she missed

her calling as a book-editor, because even after the umpteenth revision she will spot a

way to tighten the writing (clearly, by the length of this acknowledgment, brevity is not

my forte!) or make its meaning clearer. When suggesting changes, Glenda also took the

extra time to explain why and provide really detailed feedback – I know that it has

made me a better writer as a result, and for that I am extremely grateful.

I’d also like to acknowledge the contribution of my original supervision team,

Professor Liz Conlon as primary supervisor and Dr Heather Green as associate

supervisor. Heather’s familiarity with the literature on sex differences in cognitive

ability was invaluable early on in guiding me to particular books and studies that were

important. I brought to the project passion and idealism, but Liz tempered this with

scientific objectivity and healthy scepticism of the research literature: identifying a

number of serious gaps in the research literature (such as the significant passage of time

that had occurred, and some prominent researchers who questioned whether sex

differences were present in modern samples). Much of the meta-analytic reviews in this

thesis were to plug those gaps in the literature that she identified, and provide objective

and comprehensive tests of sex differences and similarities in cognitive abilities. So

while the research was carried out independently, you can still see echoes of her

Page 10: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES vii

contributions there that should be acknowledged. Liz has been, and remains still, a

researcher whom I greatly admire, and I am grateful for her approval.

On a personal note, I’d also like to thank my loyal family who have been with

me and supporting me on this journey all the way. Anyone who has embarked on

postgraduate study knows that it is very much a team effort, and there steadfast support

has meant so much. It is with great sadness that not all of them will be able to see me

graduate though. During my candidature my father was diagnosed with aggressive lung

cancer and passed within a few months of his initial diagnosis. He was always fiercely

proud of my accomplishments and I know that he will be looking on.

My mother was also diagnosed a few years earlier with a substantially sized

malignant brain tumour, and I am immensely grateful for those extra years we have

experienced together. I have cherished every day with her, and though most people

believe me to be her carer it’s really always been the other way round. She was the best

mother a son could ever wish for, and though I tell her this often I know there’s a part of

her that feels unworthy of such praise. I wish but for a moment she could see herself

through my eyes. She understands me better than I know myself, and is my dearest

friend and confidant. I am the man I am today because of her, and it’s with no small

measure of pride that I can see so very much of her personality, worldview, and intellect

in myself. She was a primary school teacher for just over 50 years, and my love of

educational psychology and learning comes from her.

Her bravery and stoicism is inspiring, and in the original draft of these

acknowledgements I’d written that I’m so grateful that she’ll be there to see me finally

graduate. But just days before completing my PhD, we received the news that there was

a new tumour in her right hemisphere. We had been so happy, and now the future is so

uncertain. But I made her a promise to finish this PhD, and at least that is a promise I

have been able to keep to her. She brings such joy (aptly named), and I cannot conceive

Page 11: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES viii

of a world without her in it. It has been said that there’s no greater tragedy than the loss

of a child to a parent. But those who say this did not have Roma Joy Reilly as a mother.

Though it may seem selfish, I’ve long hoped that she would outlive me so that I would

be spared the pain of losing her. I yet hope that this will be the case, many many years

hence!

My twin brother, Michael, has always been my greatest champion and I’m truly

blessed to have had his support and love while travelling this road. I know that there

have been times of personal self-doubt, but he has had faith in me always. It’s hard to

adequately convey how deep the bond between twin brothers can be…. it endures

throughout all the decades of your life (and hopefully beyond). He’s always been the

stronger of the two of us, and so caring and protective. I was blessed to have had him as

my twin, and because he –hates- compliments or praise I commit to writing words he

would otherwise ignore :P

I would also like to acknowledge the many researchers along the way (and my

apologies if there are any I have inadvertently overlooked) who took the time to answer

questions, provide the benefit of their perspectives, or provide advance access to

manuscripts or research materials. Firstly the late Professor Doreen Kimura who was

immensely supportive during my early review of the literature in this area and took the

time to send me copies of many of her less accessible publications, copies of her 2D

mental rotation test, along with an encouraging hand written letter. I also acknowledge

the support of the late Professor Sandra Bem, who provided initial guidance on the use

of the Bem Sex Role Inventory. I wish both these researchers were still here to read

these words of gratitude.

When gathering a feel for the research landscape, I found the literature reviews

and books of Professor Diane Halpern invaluable. Though I’ve not had the pleasure of

meeting her in person, I found several of her lectures on the topic of sex differences on

Page 12: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES ix

Youtube which provided a good grounding for the ‘unanswered’ researched questions in

our field. I also would like to thank Professor Alice Eagly whom I did have the pleasure

of meeting in person at a conference in Amsterdam while participating in a panel

discussion. Though I was only an early PhD candidate at the time (and so quite in awe

meeting some of the leading experts in my field!), Professor Eagly was welcoming and

treated me as a peer – something that I was immensely grateful for at the time. I’d also

like to thank Professor David C. Geary kindly answered questions on evolutionary

psychology and verbal abilities, as well as provided electronic copies of his book and

other publications, while Professor Adrian Furnham provided copies of stimuli

materials used in his experiments and answered further questions about the male hubris,

female humility effect. Professor Janet Hyde also answered questions on her work, and

Dr Michael Peters provided access to a redrawn version of the Vandenberg Mental

Rotation Test (VMRT) stimuli material that could be electronically administered.

I’d also like to personally thank Dr Michael Borenstein for his assistance in

learning the statistical technique of meta-analysis. His book, “Introduction to Meta-

analysis”, was invaluable, but he also kindly answered questions about more advanced

topics such as meta-regression and provided early access to a copy of a more powerful

version of his incredible Comprehensive Meta-Analysis package. I also found the

guides to conducting a meta-analysis by Professor Robert Rosenthal deeply useful as

well. I’d also like to thank the assistance and guidance of Professor Margaret Signorella,

who offered supportive and encouraging advice early on, especially about additional

statistical techniques for testing publication bias effects.

One of my favourite sayings about the scientific method is by Sir Isaac Netwon,

is “If I have seen further, it is by standing on the shoulders of giants”, which can be

traced back to the Latin phrase nanos gigantum humeris insidentes (dwarves standing

on the shoulders of giants). It is an expression of humility but also a recognition and

Page 13: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES x

expression of gratitude to the work of those who have gone forth before us. The phrase

will be instantly recognisable to any researcher or PhD student, of course, as it has been

immortalised on the front page of Google Scholar; but not everyone will know its

providence or understand its true meaning. But I found myself reflecting on it often

while doing literature reviews on Google Scholar, and just how much my own work has

been influenced by them. The reference list in this thesis attests to their many

contributions to the research literature. It has been a privilege to add – if only just a little

– to that body of scientific knowledge, but it is only by the grace of those who have

gone before me and laid the foundations.

Page 14: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES xi

Table of contents

Chapter 1 - Introduction ................................................................................................ 1

1.1 Definition of key terms ........................................................................................ 1 1.1.1 Sex versus gender differences ....................................................................... 1 1.1.2. Intelligence versus specific cognitive abilities. ............................................. 3

1.2 Background to the topic ....................................................................................... 5 1.3. Importance of sex difference research in educational psychology ..................... 11

1.3.1 Underrepresentation of women in STEM fields, and STEM literacy ........... 11 1.3.2 Sex differences in literacy, schooling, and entry to higher education ........... 15 1.3.3 Summary of educational importance ........................................................... 19

1.4 Research questions ............................................................................................ 20 1.5.1 RQ 1: Magnitude of sex differences in cognitive abilities ........................... 22 1.5.2 RQ 2: Contribution of sex-role identification to cognitive performance ...... 22 1.5.3 RQ 3: Contribution of situational factors to cognitive sex differences ......... 23 1.5.4 RQ 4: Contribution of sex-role identification to self-estimated intelligence . 23

1.5 Overview of current research ............................................................................. 24 Chapter 2 – Literature Review .................................................................................... 34

2.1 Summary of Research Findings ......................................................................... 34 2.2.1 Verbal abilities. .......................................................................................... 35 2.2.2 Visual-spatial ability. .................................................................................. 57 2.2.3 Quantitative ability. .................................................................................... 57 2.2.4 Memory. ..................................................................................................... 66

2.2 Popular Beliefs about Intelligence ..................................................................... 75 2.1.1 Self-Estimation of Intelligence.................................................................... 76 2.1.2 Estimation of other’s intelligence. ............................................................... 78 2.1.3 Popular beliefs about sex differences in specific cognitive abilities. ............ 79

2.3 Theoretical perspectives on sex differences in cognitive abilities ....................... 80 2.3.1 Biological Explanations for Sex Differences ............................................... 81 2.3.2 Psychosocial Explanations for Sex Differences ........................................... 87 2.3.3 Macro-level Cultural Contributions ............................................................ 97 2.3.4 Nash’s Sex-Role Mediation Theory .......................................................... 103

2.4 Summary of Literature Review Findings ......................................................... 108 Chapter 3 – Gender Differences in Spatial Ability ..................................................... 132 Chapter 4 – Sex Differences in Mathematics and Science Achievement .................... 133 Chapter 5 – Sex Differences in Reading and Writing................................................. 134 Chapter 6 – Cross-Cultural Patterns of Reading, Mathematics and Science Literacy . 135 Chapter 7 – Meta-Analysis of Sex-Role Mediation Effect for Visual-Spatial Ability . 136 Meta-Analysis Summary and Prelude to Empirical Studies ....................................... 137 Chapter 8 – Empirical Study 1 – Sex and Sex-Role Differences in Specific Cognitive Abilities .................................................................................................................... 138 Chapter 9 – Empirical Study 2 – Effect of Task-Labelling, Stereotype Threat, and Sex-Role Identification on Cognitive Performance ........................................................... 139

Overview of Gender Differences in Specific Cognitive Abilities ........................... 143 Sex-Role Mediation as an Explanation for Sex differences ................................ 144 Sex-typing of Cognitive Tests and Task-labelling .............................................. 147 Gender Stereotypes, and Stereotype Threat ....................................................... 147 The Present Study ............................................................................................. 148

Page 15: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES xii

Method ................................................................................................................. 150 Participants ....................................................................................................... 150 Measures ........................................................................................................... 150 Procedure .......................................................................................................... 153

Results .................................................................................................................. 154 Labelling of Spatial Task ................................................................................... 154 Stereotype-Threat .............................................................................................. 156 Stereotype Lift .................................................................................................. 158 Manipulation Check .......................................................................................... 160

Discussion............................................................................................................. 161 Sex-typing of cognitive tasks and task-labelling ................................................ 161 Stereotype Threat/Lift ....................................................................................... 162 Implications and Limitations ............................................................................. 164

Conclusions .......................................................................................................... 166 References ............................................................................................................ 167

Chapter 10 – Empirical Study 3 - Sex and Sex-Role Differences in Self-Estimated Intelligence (SEI) ...................................................................................................... 175

Overview .............................................................................................................. 175 Method ................................................................................................................. 180

Participants ....................................................................................................... 180 Procedure .......................................................................................................... 181 Measures ........................................................................................................... 181

Results .................................................................................................................. 186 Sex Role Classification ..................................................................................... 186 Cattell’s Culture Fair Intelligence Test (CCFIT) ................................................ 186 General and Academic Self-Esteem ................................................................... 188 Self-Estimated Intelligence (SEI) Scores ........................................................... 189 Bivariate Correlations ....................................................................................... 191 Predictors of Sex Differences in Self-Estimated Intelligence ............................. 191 Self-estimates of multiple intelligences ............................................................. 197

Discussion............................................................................................................. 199 Explanations for Sex Differences in Self-Estimated Intelligence........................ 200 Self-estimates of multiple intelligences ............................................................. 203 Implications and limitations .............................................................................. 204

Conclusions .......................................................................................................... 207 References ............................................................................................................ 209

Chapter 11 - Discussion ............................................................................................ 216 11.1 Magnitude of sex differences in cognitive abilities ........................................ 217 11.2 Contribution of sex-role identification to cognitive performance ................... 225 11.3 Contribution of situational factors to cognitive sex differences ...................... 229 11.4 Contribution of sex role identification to self-estimated intelligence .............. 232 11.5 Collective findings and implications for theory building ................................ 237 11.6 Directions for future research and limitations ................................................ 247 11.7 Practical Implications for Childhood Education ............................................. 249 11.8 Final Conclusions .......................................................................................... 253 References ............................................................................................................ 255

Appendices ............................................................................................................... 262 Appendix A1 – SAT Mathematics Meta-Analysis ................................................. 262 Appendix A2 – SAT Verbal Meta-Analysis .......................................................... 263 Appendix A3 – Academic Self-Esteem Measures .................................................. 264

Page 16: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 1

Chapter 1 - Introduction

“The world cannot afford the loss of the talents of half its people if we are to solve the

many problems which beset us” – Rosalyn Yalow, Nobel Laureate for Medicine 1977

From the beginning of formal scientific inquiry into the nature and structure of

human intelligence, researchers have sought to understand how biological and

environmental factors contribute to cognitive ability. It remains an important goal of

cognitive and developmental psychology (Jensen, 1998; Neisser et al., 1996). One

question in particular commands the attention of researchers and lay-persons alike: that

of group differences between males and females in cognitive ability (Caplan and Caplan

1994; 1997; Eagly, 1995; Hyde, 2005). In particular, the past four decades have seen a

rapid expansion of interest in this topic, and this has coincided with substantial changes

in the status of women, advances in research methodology, and a renewed focus on

sociocultural contributions to sex differences. As a result, many of the earlier

assumptions about sex differences in cognitive ability have been called into question,

and experimental observations of the distant past are not guaranteed to replicate in the

present-day. Before moving to an overview of the topic, a brief review of nomenclature

is offered for the reader.

1.1 Definition of key terms

1.1.1 Sex versus gender differences

Historically the term “human sex differences” had been used to refer to observed

differences between males and females in behaviour or cognitive performance. While

the term “sex differences” is still used at the present time, many researchers opt instead

for the term “gender differences”. Usage of these terms varies considerably in the

Page 17: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 2

literature, with some authors preferring the term sex to denote differences between

males and females as groups (Halpern, 1994), while other authors prefer the term

gender to denote a sociocultural origin for male-female differences (Unger, 1979).

Strong and impassioned arguments have been made for both terms (Eagly, 1995; Frieze

& Chrisler, 2011; Halpern, 2011), and each may be used interchangeably even by the

same author, depending on editorial preference of the journal in which their work is

published. When conducting a literature review, both “sex differences” and “gender

differences” must be used to gain a full view of the literature, although the prevalence of

their usage varies by discipline. Studies published in medical and clinical journals (for

example, on the effect of sex hormones on cognition) have a preference for the term

“sex differences”, while some psychological and feminist psychology journals have

editorial policies specifying the term “gender differences”. In other psychology journals

such as “Intelligence” the term sex differences is used almost exclusively. Kaiser

(2015) has even proposed a change in nomenclature to refer to the topic as

“sex/gender”, on the basis that most researchers in the field acknowledge the futility in

trying to disentangle biological and psychosocial contributions, and instead embrace

what Halpern (2000, 2011) has termed a psychobiosocial model of sex differences.

Following the practice of Eagly (1987), and Halpern (2000), the term “sex

differences” has been employed herein without prejudice to refer to group differences

between males and females. Doing so is not intended to impute that their origin is solely

or even chiefly biologically determined. Previously published literature included as

chapters of the thesis may also employ the term ‘gender differences’ due to the editorial

policy of particular scientific journals and publishers (e.g., Frieze & Chrisler, 2011).

The term “gender stereotypes” is also used to refer to commonly held stereotypes about

the abilities of males and females, as these reflect sociocultural beliefs held about the

Page 18: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 3

roles and abilities of men and women that are not necessarily accurate portrayals of

reality.

One additional term used throughout this thesis that is important to clarify is

“sex-role identification” (sometimes referred to in the literature as sex-role identity, or

just sex-roles). This term is defined as the degree to which an individual incorporates

stereotypically masculine or feminine personality traits, interests, and behaviours. There

is wide variability in the degree to which individuals incorporate stereotypically

masculine and/or feminine personality traits into their self-identity: some persons will

embody traditionally masculine agentic traits, some will embody traditionally feminine

expressive traits, while still others will incorporate a blend of both (termed

psychological androgyny). These issues will be described in detail later, but it is

important to note that variations in sex-role identification are normative in the general

population and that all sex-role categories ought to be respected and privileged. It is also

distinct from the diagnosis of gender identity disorder (GID; Bartlett, 2000; Zucker &

Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). Some researchers (e.g., Frieze & Chrisler, 2011) prefer the

terms gender-roles and gender-role identity (in the same manner as gender differences

may be preferred over sex differences), but given how similar these terms are to what

the DSM-V labels a pathological condition (American Psychiatric Association, 2014),

the term sex-role identification has been retained to avoid confusion.

1.1.2. Intelligence versus specific cognitive abilities.

Intelligence is a complex, multi-facet construct that encompasses a broad range

of cognitive abilities (Carroll, 1993; Sternberg, 2014). When men and women are

compared at a population level, their level of general intelligence as measured by

standardised tests of intelligence results in an equivalent mean IQ score (Halpern, 2000;

Neisser et al., 1996). In one of the most comprehensive reviews of general intelligence

Page 19: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 4

and assessment of mental ability, Jensen (1998) concluded that there is no evidence of

sex differences in general intelligence at a population level. These findings

notwithstanding, robust sex differences are found in more specific tests of cognitive

abilities. For example, males as a group typically score higher than females on tests of

visual-spatial reasoning, while sex differences are reversed for language-based tasks.

Some older literature has made reference to sex differences in intelligence, a

practice that can lead to confusion and the mistaken impression that researchers are

attempting to argue that one sex is ‘smarter’ in some way than the other (Halpern &

Lamay, 2000). This can also be easily misconstrued by laypersons and media when

talking about research findings. Halpern (2000, 2011) advocated the use of the term

“sex differences in cognitive abilities”, as it makes clear to the reader that aptitude for

specific types of tasks is being investigated rather than overall intellectual functioning.

This is particularly important, given the historical legacy of scientific bias against

women from the scientific community in the 19th and early 20th centuries, whereby it

was claimed that women were morally and intellectual inferior to men. Such arguments

were supported by flawed evidence provided by the field of phrenology, and

neuroanatomical arguments over total brain size (Shields, 1975). Another well-

established finding is that males typically rate their intelligence as higher than do

females in self-reports (Szymanowicz & Furnham, 2011), and that women generally

underestimate their intellectual achievements which Beyer (1990) has termed a self-

degrading bias. For this reason, avoiding misconstrual of scientific research findings as

pertaining to overall intelligence is paramount, as it might further entrench existing

negative gender stereotypes about intellectual functioning. Accordingly, the term “sex

differences in cognitive abilities” has been adopted herein.

Page 20: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 5

1.2 Background to the topic

The issue of whether differences in cognitive ability exist between males and

females, and if so how meaningful those differences are, is of profound importance to

educators, parents, policy makers, and the general public who all have an interest in

raising the educational standards of both boys and girls (Halpern et al., 2007a). It was

once regarded that sex differences in cognitive abilities were inevitable and the result of

biological differences between the brains of males and females (Kimura, 1992, 2000) –

a position known as biological determinism. With further advances in neuroimaging,

closer inspection reveals relatively few structural differences beyond brain volume

(Aaron et al., 2005; Hines, 2011). Similarly, endogenous hormones alone explain either

a relatively small amount of variance in cognitive performance (Gouchie & Kimura,

1991; Hausmann, Schoofs, Rosenthal, & Jordan, 2009) or no associations are found

(Herlitz, Reuterskiöld, Lovén, Thilers, & Rehnman, 2013; Kocoska-Maras et al., 2011;

Puts et al., 2010). At best, the evidence of activational effects of sex hormones is mixed

and inconsistent in healthy non-clinical adults (for a review see Miller & Halpern,

2013). There is also wide variability in individual performance with some males and

females performing significantly better (or significantly worse) than their same-sex

peers (referred to as within-sex variability, as opposed to between-sex variability).

Moreover, considerable cross-cultural variation can be seen in the magnitude of

cognitive-sex differences (Guiso, Monte, Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008; Reilly, 2012;

Riegle-Crumb, 2005). This variation suggests that the emergence of sex differences in

the general population is at least partially determined by social and cultural processes,

such as cultural beliefs and educational practices. Researchers (e.g., Hyde, 2005) have

also argued that the size of sex differences has been decreasing over time in response to

changes in the relative status of men and women in society. Such a premise would be

Page 21: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 6

inconsistent with the notion of immutable sex differences in cognitive ability (Feingold,

1988; Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, & Williams, 2008). Taken together, this implies that

the emergence of sex differences in cognitive abilities at the population level is not

fixed, and may thereby be subject to intervention.

Substantial sex differences are found for some types of cognitive tasks, but on

other tasks males and females perform equally well (Zell, Krizan, & Teeter, 2015). For

example, females tend to score higher on tests that measure aspects of verbal ability

(Hyde & Linn, 1988), while males show higher performance on tests of spatial ability

(Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). However, sex differences in

quantitative ability (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974), such as mathematical and scientific

reasoning, remain contentious. Some researchers find evidence of small but still

influential differences in quantitative reasoning (Hedges & Nowell, 1995), while others

argue any observed differences in maths are so small, in fact, that they can be

categorised as ‘trivial’ (Hyde & Linn, 2006). Many of the inconsistencies in the

literature may be the result of highly selective samples that do not generalise well to the

general population (Becker & Hedges, 1988). Alternately there may be other (as yet

unidentified) factors that covary across samples with sex differences in cognitive ability

such as sex role identification, or socioeconomic status (Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco,

Newcombe, & Huttenlocher, 2005; McGraw, Lubienski, & Strutchens, 2006; Nash,

1979).

The issue of whether differences in cognitive ability between males and females

still exist in modern samples, and if so how meaningful are those differences, is a

question of interest to multiple stakeholders (educators, parents, policy makers, and the

wider society). But making such a determination requires a strong body of evidence,

and in their zeal, a number of researchers have been slightly premature in declaring the

Page 22: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 7

problem solved. Feingold (1988) claimed sex differences in cognitive ability were

“disappearing”, but this conclusion was not widely accepted and strongly critiqued. In

an effort to clarify the matter, Hedges and Nowell (1995) published the largest analysis

of student testing data for the USA ever conducted, demonstrating a pattern of sex

differences across three decades that was not diminishing. More recently, Caplan and

Caplan (2005, p. 25) have called investigating gender a “perseverative search for sex

differences”, but in their review omitted key sources of data (such as Hedges and

Nowell) that were disconfirming. Similarly, Hyde (2005) advanced the “gender

similarities hypothesis” which states that psychological sex differences are either very

small or trivial in magnitude. In a response, Lippa (2006) noted the selective omission

of key reviews where larger effect sizes were found. For example, many of the sex

differences reported in Hedges and Nowell were higher, including reading, writing,

mathematics, science, and visual-spatial ability. On the subject of disappearing gender

gaps, Halpern (1989, 2014) has quipped that in the investigation of sex differences,

“what you see depends on where you look”.

One aim of the present programme of research is to apply the statistical

technique of meta-analysis to a series of large, demographically representative datasets

of student achievement that for various reasons have not been tested for sex differences,

thus addressing a gap in the research literature (see meta-analysis chapters). This may

help resolve some of the inconsistencies in the literature and provide greater clarity to

such debates over whether the magnitudes of sex differences are decreasing over time.

Educators and policy-makers need timely and well-informed evidence for decision-

making. Further, resolution of these issues are a prerequisite for planning educational

interventions (Liben & Coyle, 2014), as well as the allocation of limited resources by

educators to address disparities in educational outcomes. But the cost of accepting a

Page 23: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 8

false null hypothesis may be high – it might have a chilling effect on further research

into factors contributing to sex differences in cognitive ability, as well as further

development of educational interventions aimed at achieving equality of outcomes.

Preiss and Hyde (2010, p. 312) warn that little research has “tested targeted

interventions designed to close specific ability gaps […] though the development of

such interventions are important. Regardless of the reasons for gender differences in

specific cognitive abilities, whether biological, social, or cultural, it is likely that

interventions can be developed to help most students”. Thus there is a lack of

consensus, but this debate might be better informed with timely data.

Furthermore, when sex differences are reliably found for cognitive tasks, it

poses the additional question of their origins (Halpern, 2011; Kimura, 2000; Stanley,

Benbow, Brody, Dauber, & Lupkowski, 1992). While sex differences in cognitive

ability have been studied since the very beginning of psychometric assessment of

intelligence (for a review see Shields, 1982), theoretical perspectives on their causes

have shifted considerably over this time period. Archer (1996) has termed these origin

theories. Early theoretical debate on sex differences proposed strong and immutable

biological factors (nature), while later theorists argued that early differences in

socialisation experiences and environmental factors might better explain differences in

cognitive ability (nurture). More recently, the limitations of both the nature and nurture

perspective in isolation have being realised (Halpern & Tan, 2001; Priess & Hyde,

2010). Increasingly, researchers acknowledge the need for a more comprehensive

theoretical framework that encompasses biological, social and psychological factors,

which Halpern (2000, 2011) has termed psychobiosocial models of sex differences.

Such models are a network of multiple levels of input featuring proximal causes such as

biology and early socialization experiences as well as more distal causes such as

Page 24: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 9

cultural influences and sex-role stereotypes(Wood & Eagly, 2002). Such a perspective

may also help explain individual differences in the development of cognitive ability, and

the reasons why some males and females perform at lower or higher level than their

same-sex peers. As noted by Hyde (2005), there is greater within-gender variability

than between-gender differences, an observation first made by E. L. Thorndike (1914)

who argued for the importance of understanding individual differences factors.

Investigating individual difference factors around gender (such as sex-role

identification, endorsement of gender stereotypes, etc.) may shed light on the

mechanisms involved, which can inform the development of educational interventions.

A consensus statement by researchers in the area of cognitive sex differences is

that there is a need for both basic and applied research to improve educational outcomes

for boys and girls (Halpern et al., 2007b; Neisser et al., 1996). Thus, the second aim of

this programme of research was to investigate the contribution of an individual

differences factor, sex-role identification, on the development of sex-typed cognitive

abilities. Nash (1979) had hypothesised that sex-role identification acted as a mediator

Page 25: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 10

of sex differences in intellectual functioning leading to the improved acquisition of

visual-spatial and verbal abilities (see Figure 1.1).

Figure 1.1. Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis. Contribution of sex/gender is

mediated by sex-role identification, with masculine sex-roles promoting the development of

visual-spatial reasoning and feminine sex-roles encouraging the development of verbal

abilities and general language proficiency.

A meta-analysis by Signorella and Jamison (1986) of studies investigating the

sex-role mediation hypothesis found support for visual-spatial ability, but a lack of

empirical studies that actually tested verbal abilities prohibited drawing any conclusion

for language. In the passage of time since, society has seen considerable change in the

status of men and women (Auster & Ohm, 2000), including cultural prescriptions for

Sex/Gender

Masculine sex-role identification (instrumental/agentic traits)

Visual-Spatial Ability

Feminine sex-role identification (expressive/communal traits)

Verbal and Language Ability

Page 26: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 11

sex-roles. This merits further investigation, to test whether the association between sex-

role identification and verbal/visual-spatial abilities is still found in modern samples.

Similar predictions are also made by Eagly and Wood’s (1999) social role theory (see

Section 2.3.3.1), which posits that psychological sex differences in thought and

behaviour arise from the segregation of society into masculine and feminine sex-roles.

1.3. Importance of sex difference research in educational psychology

Equality of educational outcomes is a desirable social good – if one or more

subgroups in society lag significantly behind the majority, this may result in deleterious

outcomes not just for individual members of that group (reduced job security, lower

socioeconomic status) but also for society as a whole (entrenching social disadvantage).

Indeed a fundamental tenet of egalitarian societies is equality of education outcomes,

and the issue of sex differences in education has been given considerable attention by

psychological researchers, educators, parents and policy makers (Dwyer, 1973; Halpern,

1997; Halpern et al., 2007b; Newcombe et al., 2009).

There are two central educational issues that highlight the importance of

additional research in this area. The first is the underrepresentation of women in

science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM)-related fields and STEM

literacy. The second is the pattern of lower educational aspirations and achievement of

men beyond compulsory schooling.

1.3.1 Underrepresentation of women in STEM fields, and STEM literacy

The skills required for the workforce have substantially changed in recent

decades. Whereas once a basic proficiency in reading, writing, and mathematics were

seen as important educational milestones, in recent years, advanced mathematical, and

scientific literacy have been seen as important skills for occupational success.

Additionally, continued scientific progress depends largely on the quality of the pool of

Page 27: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 12

future scientists, and inspiring new entrants to pursue science and technology is critical

for maintaining a competitive and growing economy (President’s Council of Advisors

on Science and Technology, 2010). Current shortfalls in the number of STEM graduates

required for industry are currently mitigated to some degree by importing expertise from

abroad (e.g., the H1-B visa in the United States, and the 457 visa for Australia), but the

need to broaden the base of the STEM-ready workforce has long been acknowledged by

the scientific profession, including the numbers of women participating in scientific and

technology research. Though modest progress has been made towards closing gaps in

recent decades, women continue to be underrepresented in their participation in STEM-

related fields in most Western nations including the United States (National Science

Foundation, 2017), Australia (Bell, 2010), as well in large parts of the developing world

(Sugimoto, Larivière, Ni, Gingras, & Cronin, 2013). In the Australian context, this

concern is matched by government policy action - considerable efforts have been made

by the Australian Federal Government in recent years to address gender inequalities in

STEM opportunities and labour supply (outlined in Latimer et al., 2019). Despite

making some progress in recent years, globally women make up only 28.8% of the

scientific research workforce and less than a third in Western nations. The greatest

gender imbalance starts at tertiary and postgraduate studies, which has been termed the

“leaky pipeline” problem. At every step after compulsory schooling

(college/undergraduate, postgraduate, PhD, etc.) women become rarer and rarer, and

when they leave study women often do not return (Alper & Gibbons, 1993). There are,

however, exceptions to this general rule: women comprise approximately 70% of

graduates at each degree level for psychology, and there are slightly more women than

men in life sciences (Hanson, Schaub, & Baker, 1996; National Science Foundation,

Page 28: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 13

2017). This distinction is important to note, because it suggests that it is not due to other

social factors such as career-family pressures, or lack of ability or interest.

The question of the underrepresentation of women is complex and yet to be fully

understood (Ceci & Williams, 2011; Halpern, 2007), but many researchers have argued

that the early sex differences in mathematics and science achievement at school may

play a role. Although the effect size for mean sex differences in mathematics and

science may be small, Hyde et al. (2008) has noted that larger sex differences are found

for more complex problem solving tasks during the high school years (Brunner, Krauss,

& Kunter, 2008). Researchers have also observed that there is a pronounced disparity in

the sex ratio of high achievers in mathematics and science who are more likely to go on

to pursue further STEM-related studies (Benbow, Lubinski, Shea, & Eftekhari-Sanjani,

2000; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2015). Additionally, gender stereotyping

associating science with masculinity helps to shape girls’ attitudes towards mathematics

and science, resulting in reduced mathematics and science self-efficacy beliefs during

adolescence and young adulthood (McGraw et al., 2006; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews,

2017). A large body of research by Eccles (1994, 2007) and colleagues have shown that

attitudes towards science and mathematics, cultural sex-role stereotypes, and individual

self-efficacy beliefs guide students either towards (or away from) the pursuit of STEM

careers (Eccles, 2013; Else-Quest, Mineo, & Higgins, 2013; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood,

Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002).

While the importance of increasing the representation of women in STEM

professions has been argued as an economic and logistical issue for industry, there are

further reasons to see it as a desirable social good. Affording women the opportunity to

pursue a STEM-related career if they so choose is also important as a matter of wage

equity - wages in these traditionally male-dominated professions are typically much

Page 29: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 14

higher than the median wage and offer greater job security (Hill, Corbett, & St. Rose,

2010). For example, in the United States, STEM workers earn 29% more than their non-

STEM counterparts, and growth in STEM occupations exceeds growth in non-STEM

occupations (Office of the Chief Economist, 2017). Similar findings of employment

growth are found in Australia (Office of the Chief Scientist, 2014). Providing equality

of educational outcomes in science and mathematics is therefore an issue of gender

equity (Halpern et al., 2007b; Hyde & Lindberg, 2007), and can ensure that important

subgroups (such as women) do not get left behind. The skills required for the

workforce have substantially changed in recent decades. More advanced mathematical

and scientific literacy are now seen as important skills for occupational success even if

one does not seek to become a scientist or computer programmer.

Science literacy is also important for full participation in society. For example,

attaining a basic science literacy is important for understanding health-related

information (such as leading healthy lifestyles, or the need for preventative

vaccination), as well as important social and political issues that impact on society (such

as the science of climate change, need for public funding of space research, or any one

of a myriad of other issues where STEM and public policy intersect). Given that women

are often more influential than men in family medical decision-making and health

information seeking (Abrahamson, Fisher, Turner, Durrance, & Turner, 2008; Beier &

Ackerman, 2003; Washington, Burke, Joseph, Guerra, & Pasick, 2009), a basic

proficiency in understanding of scientific and medical issues is also important for a

healthy society. For example, research suggests that poorer health literacy is associated

with a reduced likelihood to undergo routine preventative screening and poorer

treatment outcomes (Vahabi, 2007). Sex differences researchers often stress the

importance of increasing the representation of women in STEM-fields as an

Page 30: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 15

occupational/economic issue, but traditionally have placed less emphasis on the social

consequences of reduced STEM literacy in women.

1.3.2 Sex differences in literacy, schooling, and entry to higher education

The second issue concerns lower educational outcomes for males (reading and

writing literacy, completion of schooling and pursuit of tertiary education). Historically

women had lower educational attainment than men for the early half of the 20th century

due to societal barriers (Alexander & Eckland, 1974). From the 1960’s onward changes

in societal attitudes towards the status of women saw a rise in the number of women

completing high school and seeking further education across most developed nations. In

more recent decades, the pattern has completely reversed – boys are more likely to drop

out of high school before completion than girls (85% versus 78%) (Table A2.4: OECD,

2011), including in Australia where the sex differences in Grade 12 completion rates has

now reached 10% (Marks, 2008). Women now significantly outnumber men in

attending college education in the United States (Conger & Long, 2010), and similar

patterns are found internationally. For example in the context of Australia, since 1985

more women than men have entered tertiary studies each year. The trend appears stable,

if not slightly widening (see Figure 1.2). Once enrolled, males have a significantly

higher dropout rate in their first year of study and lower overall completion rates (70.9%

versus 75.5%), as shown in a cohort analysis of Australian students from 2005 through

to 2013 (Department of Education, 2014). Across OECD nations, far more females

than males enrol in further tertiary education, with the only three exceptions being

Switzerland, Turkey and Japan (OECD, 2016). Jacob (2002) notes that this increases for

low-income and minority students where women are 25 percent more likely than men to

enrol in tertiary education.

Page 31: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 16

Figure 1.2. New student enrolment in higher education diplomas and degrees in Australia, separated by gender. Datasource: Higher Education Students Time Series Tables 1979-2000; Individual Yearly Reports 2000-2015, https://www.education.gov.au/student-data

Compounding the issue of disparities in educational attainment, there are also

pronounced sex differences in reading literacy (Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Lynn & Mikk,

2009; Mullis, Martin, Kennedy, & Foy, 2007), grammar (Stanley et al., 1992) and

writing skills (Reynolds, Scheiber, Hajovsky, Schwartz, & Kaufman, 2015). A full

appreciation of the extent of the male-female gap in reading and writing can be gained

by considering the effect size for reading and writing. While sex differences in

mathematics and science are typically small in magnitude by Cohen’s (1988) effect size

guidelines, sex differences in reading and writing achievement typically fall in the

medium to large range. But many of the datasets used in these analysis are dated, and

further research is needed with modern samples (see Chapter 5). But there is tentative

50,000

100,000

150,000

200,000

250,000

300,000

350,000

Enro

llmen

t

Year

Commencing students, by gender

Male Female

63,793

Page 32: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 17

evidence that this reading gap remains. In a recent educational assessment of reading

literacy attainment in OECD nations (PISA 2012), girls outperformed boys in reading

on average by the equivalent of a full year of schooling (OECD, 2015; Indicator A10).

Unlike the rise in womens’ educational aspirations, the issue of sex differences

in reading and writing literacy is not a recent phenomenon – in a systematic review of

the research literature available at the time, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) noted that sex

differences in language were ‘firmly established’ (p. 351). Nowell and Hedges (1998)

reviewed several decades (1960-1994) of nationally representative testing data for

students in the U.S.A., finding the gender gap in language proficiency (reading and

writing) had remained stable over a 34 year period. That there was no change (either

increasing or decreasing) is an important consideration for educators, because it

demonstrates that improving the educational aspirations of girls and women has not

come at the cost of boys’ academic achievement.

Just as the lowered educational aspirations of girls and women were once an

important target for intervention as a matter of gender equality, a number of educational

researchers have expressed concern that the combination of poorer language

development in reading and writing skills and a pattern of lower educational aspiration

in boys and men merits educational intervention (Alon & Gelbgiser, 2011; Buchmann,

DiPrete, & McDaniel, 2008; Entwisle, Alexander, & Olson, 2007). Childhood reading

ability is also a strong predictor of eventual adult socioeconomic status, even after

controlling for the effect of birth SES (Ritchie & Bates, 2013). Sex differences in

educational achievement can be seen from primary school and continuing through to

high school, with girls achieving higher grades than boys, including in mathematics and

science (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Perkins, Kleiner, Roey, & Brown, 2004). Well-

developed literacy skills are essential for academic success across all levels of

Page 33: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 18

compulsory schooling (Dockrell, Lindsay, & Palikara, 2011). For those students who do

enter tertiary education, there is also a significant female advantage in coursework

grades and GPA (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Perkins et al., 2004). In a recent meta-

analysis of scholastic achievement, Voyer and Voyer (2014) reported a significant sex

difference between male and female students in college and university of d = .21

[95%CI = .17 to .25], which is a small effect size but exceeds Hyde and Grabe’s (2008)

critical value for nontrivial sex differences by a factor of two.

The issue of lowered educational expectations and educational attainment for

males is a complex and contentious issue, with a wide variety of non-cognitive and

social factors contributing to the gender gap. However, females do approach tertiary

education with a substantial advantage over their male peers in reading and writing

proficiency (Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Lynn & Mikk, 2009) and it has been argued that

sex differences in reading and language proficiency may be at least partially responsible

for lower commencement and completion rates (Buchmann & DiPrete, 2006).

Successful tertiary education requires a variety of skills, including the ability to read

and comprehend written material such as textbooks, readings, scientific papers and

other documents. It also requires students to write essays and reports, which form part

of student assessment. If male students enter tertiary education and training without

fully developing their language competency, it could have a deleterious effect on

educational success.

In addition to tertiary education, reading and writing are important skills in their

own right. Regardless of whether a student decides to pursue tertiary studies, seek a

trade qualification, or enters the workforce directly, educators and policy-makers see

value in citizens attaining reading and writing literacy for full participation in society.

While there are manual jobs and trade professions that do not require such skills, in the

Page 34: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 19

future increased automation and disruptive technological change may reduce or

eliminate the need for unskilled or lowly-skilled labour (Muro, Maxim, & Whiton,

2019). Increasingly economists and public policy makers see automation as a gendered

issue, as those professions most likely to be automated (e.g., manufacturing, assembly,

driving) are disproportionately male-dominated, while occupations that are more

resistant to the threat of automation (e.g., nursing and medicine, child- and aged- care)

are largely female-dominated (AlphaBeta, 2017). This means that those males who are

without higher reading and writing skills may encounter difficulties reskilling and

pursuing tertiary education or seeking retraining if required. Economic predictions of

labour market trends predict dramatically higher male unemployment as a result of

automation (Bloom, McKenna, & Prettner, 2018; Muro et al., 2019), but improved

literacy would improve opportunities for retraining in new skills. I would argue that

reducing or substantially eliminating sex differences in reading and writing is an

important target as a matter of gender equity and social cohesion.

1.3.3 Summary of educational importance

Systemic disparities in educational achievement can have profound

consequences for men and women’s lives beyond their schooling (Priess & Hyde, 2010;

Riegle-Crumb, 2005). In the United States, for example, gender equity in educational

outcomes is mandated by Title IX of the Education Amendments Act of 1972, and has

led to considerable efforts to increase the number of girls studying mathematics and

science classes at high school (Walters, 2010), as well as substantial funding of basic

and applied research. Other equality of educational outcomes legislation requires

governmental agencies like the National Science Foundation to collect annual data on

the number of women starting and completing postgraduate training in a STEM field, as

well as those entering and leaving the workforce (National Science Foundation, 2017),

Page 35: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 20

so as to track whether educational initiatives translate into real world outcomes. Similar

initiatives can be found internationally (UNESCO, 2012), making the

underrepresentation of women in STEM a high profile research issue. However, the

issue of lowered educational aspirations for males (as well as poorer reading and writing

skills), receives a comparatively less attention by researchers at a time when many

male-dominated occupations are threatened by automation. Both issues (women in

STEM, men’s educational aspirations and reading/writing literacy) are important targets

for further study and worthy in their own right.

Gender gaps cross all strata of society and have may impact the life outcomes of

a significant portion of society (Buchmann et al., 2008). In a debate on the merits of

conducting and publishing sex difference research in American Psychologist, Eagly

(1990, p. 562) has called the scientific debate over gender differences “one of the most

important scientific debates of our time”, while Halpern, Beninger and Straight (2011,

p. 266) argue that furthering understanding of sex differences is “crucial” to improving

educational achievements and aspirations for both genders. Though cognitive ability

alone is but one of many factors associated with educational success, it is highly tied to

academic self-efficacy beliefs and motivation, and guides adolescent and young adult

career decision-making processes especially for stereotypically gendered professions

(Eccles, 2013). But sound educational and public policy decisions require sound

empirical evidence (Halpern et al., 2011), and much of the literature in this area is

dated.

1.4 Research questions

There are important research questions about sex differences that need to be

addressed to advance the field theoretically and to provide the necessary information to

guide educational and wide society policies. As mentioned there is considerable debate

Page 36: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 21

in the literature over fundamental issues such as the magnitude of sex differences in

cognitive abilities. This has proved difficult to answer definitively for the reasons

outlined earlier, including small sample sizes and selection of samples that are not

representative of the general population. On this subject, Halpern (1989) once noted that

what researchers see depends on where (and how) they look. As a result, the present

literature is shaped by the choices and ideology of the researchers in the field. For

example cognitive sex differences tend to be larger in adolescence and young

adulthood, so testing for sex differences only at earlier ages serves to bolster evidence

for the null hypothesis (while older-aged samples that might have revealed meaningful

differences were sometimes not examined). Additionally, the vast majority of sex

difference research focuses on cognitive tasks where females score lower than males

(e.g., visual-spatial ability), while the issue of language differences where opposite

trends are found (e.g., verbal ability) is less often investigated. Furthermore if sex

differences in cognitive ability are even partly the product of psychosocial factors (such

as the roles of women and men in society), and if these have changed with the passage

of time, it raises the question of temporal stability. Even the most carefully selected

demographically representative sample is limited to a single cohort in time. There is a

need for further basic research to determine the magnitude of cognitive sex differences

to address these gaps in the scientific literature.

Despite the identification of sex differences in cognitive ability quite early in the

history of psychometrics, relatively modest progress has been made in explaining why

such group differences emerge at the population level. A variety of explanations have

been proposed (see Section 2.3) but three areas that seem promising are the contribution

of sex-role identification to cognitive performance, the role of situational factors (such

as appraisal of test content, or knowledge of gender stereotypes), and sex differences in

Page 37: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 22

self-estimated intelligence (if one believes they are lower in intelligence than their

peers, it may become a self-fulfilling prophecy). Accordingly, four research questions

(RQ) have been identified for this research project. These are listed below followed by a

short outline.

1.5.1 RQ 1: Magnitude of sex differences in cognitive abilities

The first research question aimed to examine the sex differences hypothesis in

modern samples, and to provide an estimate of the magnitude of effect sizes for verbal

ability and quantitative ability. This hypothesis was tested by examining archival data in

language usage (reading and writing) and quantitative reasoning (mathematics and

science literacy). By examining archival data collected over a prolonged time period

(for example, the National Assessment of Educational Progress, NAEP in U.S. samples)

it also allowed for testing of the hypothesis that changes in societal values and the roles

of men and women in society have subsequently reduced or eliminated sex differences

in cognitive ability. The question of whether there are still observable sex differences in

visual-spatial ability in modern samples has been convincingly demonstrated by meta-

analytic reviews of spatial ability (Voyer et al., 1995); no further research on this matter

is needed at the present time. Examining large international assessments of student

achievement (e.g., OECD’s Programme for International Student Assesment, PISA)

also allows for investigation of cross-cultural patterns, to determine the portion of sex

differences that arise independent of sociocultural factors.

1.5.2 RQ 2: Contribution of sex-role identification to cognitive performance

The second research question concerned the contribution of sex-role

identification to the development of sex differences in cognitive ability. Specifically, it

examined Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation theory. This theory predicts that masculine

personality traits are associated with greater visual-spatial ability, and that feminine

Page 38: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 23

personality traits are associated with greater verbal and language ability (Dwyer, 1974;

Nash, 1974). Androgynous subjects (high masculinity and high femininity) should score

highly on both visual-spatial and verbal tasks. A meta-analysis of research studies had

shown support for the theory (Signorella & Jamison, 1986), but the authors note that

included studies were subject to a number methodological limitations including sample

size and breadth of tasks investigated. Halpern (2000) reviewed support for the sex-role

mediation hypothesis in some length, nothing that while there was an initial spurt of

promising research the hypothesis had “not held up well” (p. 243) in subsequent

decades (at least for language tasks). Given the passage of time, it might also be the case

that the theory lacks predictive validity for modern samples. Before recruiting subjects

for the experimental study (Chapter 8), a meta-analytic review (see Chapter 7) of

empirical studies (both published and unpublished) was conducted to examine the

association between masculine personality traits and visual-spatial ability in modern

samples published since the Signorella and Jamison (1986) review.

1.5.3 RQ 3: Contribution of situational factors to cognitive sex differences

The third research question concerned the role of the testing environment (such

as task instructions) and stereotype threat on cognitive performance (investigated in

Chapter 9). The approach sought to experimentally manipulate participant’s perceptions

of the test content as being either masculine or feminine in nature by changing task

instructions and providing additional information. The sex-role identification held by

participants was also measured. The methodology thus allowed for testing the separate

and joint effects of sex-role identification and stereotype threat.

1.5.4 RQ 4: Contribution of sex-role identification to self-estimated intelligence

The fourth and final research question concerned sex differences in self-

estimated intelligence (SEI). As noted earlier, males and females perform equivalently

Page 39: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 24

on psychometrically measured intelligence (g) and IQ (Halpern, 2000; Jensen, 1998).

However males report significantly higher perceptions of their own intelligence than do

females. The fourth research question sought to determine whether sex differences in

self-estimated intelligence might also be explained by sex-role identification;

specifically that masculinity would be associated with higher self-estimated IQ, while

femininity would be associated with lower self-estimated IQ. This is reported in Chapter

10.

1.5 Overview of current research

Chapter 2 outlines a literature review of theoretical perspectives on the

development of sex differences in specific cognitive abilities, as well as a summary of

empirical research findings for verbal and language abilities. Chapter 3 presents a

literature review specifically on visual-spatial reasoning. From thereon, the current

programme of research was divided into two sections. The first section outlines a series

of meta-analyses examining archival data on student testing data from large nationally-

representative samples in the United States (National Assessment of Educational

Progress; Chapters 4 and 5), and internationally from Programme for International

Student Assessment (PISA; Chapter 6), to determine the magnitude of sex differences in

reading, writing, mathematics and science achievement. The second section contains

empirical studies, investigating the contribution of sex-role identification to sex-typed

cognitive abilities (visual-spatial and verbal abilities), sex-role conformity pressures,

and finally to self-estimated intelligence scores. Given the passage of time since Nash’s

(1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis was conceived, it is possible that changes in sex-

role norms and gender stereotypes might have rendered it outdated, or that past research

findings might not replicate to modern cohorts of students. Before recruiting

participants for the primary empirical study, it was deemed prudent to conduct a meta-

Page 40: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 25

analysis of research findings on the contribution of masculine sex-role identification to

visual-spatial ability (Chapter 7). The lack of sufficient studies investigating the second

part of the sex-role mediation hypothesis (feminine sex-role identification and verbal

abilities) precluded conducting a similar meta-analytic review.

Chapter 8 presents an investigation into the sex-role mediation hypothesis.

While a considerable number of studies have sought to test the hypothesis previously,

they have been hampered by substantial methodological limitations. These include i)

inadequate sample sizes and low statistical power (Hansen, Jamison, & Signorella,

1982), ii) employing ad-hoc, peer-rated, or psychometrically invalid measures of sex-

role identification (an issue addressed further in Signorella & Jamison, 1986), iii)

administering tests to one gender only (e.g., Newcombe & Dubas, 1992), iv) examining

associations between sex-roles and visual-spatial ability but neglecting to measure

verbal abilities, and v) considering only one type of visual-spatial ability (e.g., mental

rotation) rather than a broad range of tasks (i.e., spatial perception, spatial

visualization). The research reported in Chapter 8 aimed to address these limitations

from previous research.

Chapter 9 presents an investigation into the joint effects of sex-role

identification and situational factors (such as task instructions, and knowledge of gender

stereotypes) on cognitive performance on two visual-spatial tasks. The final empirical

study considers another important psychosocial factor in the development of sex

differences in cognitive abilities, that of intellectual self-concept. Chapter 10

investigates sex differences in self-estimated intelligence, an important contributor to

personal self-efficacy and intellectual functioning. Recall that men and women as a

group do not differ in general intelligence, but a considerable number of studies have

identified that men self-report their intelligence to be significantly higher than women

Page 41: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 26

(termed the male-hubris female-humility problem by Furnham, Hosoe and Tang, 2001).

Chapter 10 aimed to test whether masculine or feminine sex-role identification might

offer an explanation for the apparent gender gap in self-appraisal of intellect.

Finally Chapter 11 contains a general discussion of the implications of the

empirical studies presented in this thesis, along with the findings from archival research

into patterns of sex differences outlined in the meta-analyses. From this, an updated

psychobiosocial model of sex differences is presented. The model incorporates new

findings on sex-role identification, intellectual self-concept, as well as broader macro-

level cultural contributions such as gender segregation and inequality.

References

Aaron, M. W., Courtney, L. K., Julie, D. F., Lori, A. K., Jamie, R. M., Kunal, S., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2005). College students lack knowledge of standard drink volumes: Implications for definitions of risky drinking based on survey data. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(4), 631-638. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000158836.77407.E6

Abrahamson, J. A., Fisher, K. E., Turner, A. G., Durrance, J. C., & Turner, T. C. (2008). Lay information mediary behavior uncovered: exploring how nonprofessionals seek health information for themselves and others online. Journal of the Medical Library Association : JMLA, 96(4), 310-323. doi:10.3163/1536-5050.96.4.006

Alexander, K. L., & Eckland, B. K. (1974). Sex differences in the educational attainment process. American Sociological Review, 39(5), 668-682. doi:10.2307/2094313

Alon, S., & Gelbgiser, D. (2011). The female advantage in college academic achievements and horizontal sex segregation. Social Science Research, 40(1), 107-119. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.06.007

Alper, J., & Gibbons, A. (1993). The pipeline is leaking women all the way along. Science, 260, 409-411. doi:10.1126/science.260.5106.409

AlphaBeta. (2017). The Automation Advantage. Retrieved from Sydney, Australia: American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary

explanations compatible? American Psychologist, 51(9), 909-917. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.51.9.909

Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in contemporary American society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 43(7/8), 499-528. doi:10.1023/A:1007119516728

Bartlett, N. H. (2000). Is gender identity disorder in children a mental disorder? Sex Roles, 43(11/12), 753-785.

Page 42: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 27

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection and variability in studies of gender differences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(2), 183-184. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Beier, M. E., & Ackerman, P. L. (2003). Determinants of health knowledge: an investigation of age, gender, abilities, personality, and interests. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 439-448. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.439

Bell, S. (2010). Women in science: The persistence of gender in Australia. Higher Education Management and Policy, 22(1), 1-19. doi:10.1787/hemp-v22-art3-en

Benbow, C. P., Lubinski, D., Shea, D. L., & Eftekhari-Sanjani, H. (2000). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability at age 13: Their status 20 years later. Psychological Science, 11(6), 474-480. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00291

Beyer, S. (1990). Gender differences in the accuracy of self-evaluation of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5), 960-970. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.960

Bloom, D. E., McKenna, M. J., & Prettner, K. (2018). Demography, Unemployment, Automation, and Digitalization: Implications for the Creation of (Decent) Jobs, 2010–2030. Retrieved from Bonn, Germany:

Brunner, M., Krauss, S., & Kunter, M. (2008). Gender differences in mathematics: Does the story need to be rewritten? Intelligence, 36(5), 403-421. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.002

Buchmann, C., & DiPrete, T. A. (2006). The growing female advantage in college completion: The role of family background and academic achievement. American Sociological Review, 71(4), 515-541. doi:10.1177/000312240607100401

Buchmann, C., DiPrete, T. A., & McDaniel, A. (2008). Gender inequalities in education. Annual Review of Sociology, 34(1), 319-337. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.34.040507.134719

Caplan, J. B., & Caplan, P. J. (2005). The perseverative search for sex differences in mathematics ability. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 25-47). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1994). Thinking critically about research on sex and gender. New York: Harper Collins.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1997). Do sex-related cognitive differences exist, and why do people seek them out? In P. J. Caplan, M. Crawford, J. S. Hyde, & J. T. E. Richardson (Eds.), Gender differences in human cognition (pp. 52-80). New York: Oxford University Press.

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities : a survey of factor-analytic studies New York: Cambridge University Press.

Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011). Understanding current causes of women's underrepresentation in science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(8), 3157-3162. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014871108

Conger, D., & Long, M. C. (2010). Why are men falling behind? Gender gaps in college performance and persistence. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 627(1), 184-214. doi: 10.1177/0002716209348751

Department of Education. (2014). Completion Rates of Domestic Bachelor Students – A Cohort Analysis, 2005-2013. Department of Education, Australian Government Retrieved from https://docs.education.gov.au/system/files/doc/other/cohortanalysis2005-2013.pdf.

Dockrell, J., Lindsay, G., & Palikara, O. (2011). Explaining the academic achievement at school leaving for pupils with a history of language impairment: Previous academic

Page 43: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 28

achievement and literacy skills. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 27(2), 223-237. doi:10.1177/0265659011398671

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 198-208. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198

Dwyer, C. A. (1973). Sex differences in reading: An evaluation and a critique of current theories. Review of Educational Research, 43(4), 455-467. doi:10.2307/1170076

Dwyer, C. A. (1974). Influence of children's sex role standards on reading and arithmetic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(6), 811-816. doi:10.1037/h0021522

Eagly, A. H. (1987). Reporting Sex Differences. American Psychologist, 42(7), 756-757. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.42.7.755

Eagly, A. H. (1990). On the advantages of reporting sex comparisons. American Psychologist, 45(4), 560-562. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.560

Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50(3), 145-158. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.50.3.145

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54(6), 408-423. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408

Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women's educational and occupational choices. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18(4), 585-609. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb01049.x

Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren't more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 199-210). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Eccles, J. S. (2013). Gender and achievement choices. In E. T. Gershoff, R. S. Mistry, & D. Crosby (Eds.), Societal Contexts of Child Development: Pathways of Influence and Implications for Practice and Policy (pp. 19-34). New York: Oxford University Press.

Else-Quest, N. M., Mineo, C. C., & Higgins, A. (2013). Math and science attitudes and achievement at the intersection of gender and ethnicity. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3), 293-309. doi:10.1177/0361684313480694

Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (2007). Early schooling: The handicap of being poor and male. Sociology of Education, 80(2), 114-138. doi:10.1177/003804070708000202

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43(2), 95-103. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95

Frieze, I. H., & Chrisler, J. C. (2011). Editorial policy on the use of the terms “Sex” and “Gender”. Sex Roles, 64, 789-790. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9988-2

Furnham, A., Hosoe, T., & Tang, T. (2001). Male hubris and female humility? A cross-cultural study of ratings of self, parental, and sibling multiple intelligence in America, Britain, and Japan. Intelligence, 30(1), 101-115. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00080-0

Gouchie, C., & Kimura, D. (1991). The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16(4), 323-334. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(91)90018-O

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320(5880), 1164-1165. doi:10.1126/science.1154094

Page 44: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 29

Halpern, D. F. (1989). The disappearance of cognitive gender differences: What you see depends on where you look. American Psychologist, 44(8), 1156-1158. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.44.8.1156

Halpern, D. F. (1994). Stereotypes, Science, Censorship, and the Study of Sex Differences. Feminism & Psychology, 4(4), 523-530. doi:10.1177/0959353594044006

Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. American Psychologist, 52(10), 1091-1102. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1091

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F. (2007). Science, sex, and good sense: Why women are underrepresented in

some areas of science and math. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren't more women in science?: top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 121-130). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F. (2014). Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities: Implications for STEM

Education [Video file]. Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M.

(2007a). Sex, math and scientific achievement: Why do men dominate the fields of science, engineering and mathematics? Scientific American Mind, 18(6), 44-51. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanbrain0512-26

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007b). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1-51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011). Sex differences in intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 253-272). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., & Lamay, M. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 229-246. doi:10.1023/A:1009027516424

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using a psychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brain and Cognition, 45(3), 392-414. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1287

Hansen, M. J., Jamison, W., & Signorella, M. L. (1982). Spatial performance, activity preferences, and masculinity-femininity. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C.

Hanson, S. L., Schaub, M., & Baker, D. P. (1996). Gender stratification in the science pipeline: A comparative analysis of seven countries. Gender and Society, 10(3), 271-290. doi:10.1177/089124396010003005

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences - A psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(3), 389-401. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.019

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269(5220), 41-45. doi:10.1126/science.7604277

Herlitz, A., Reuterskiöld, L., Lovén, J., Thilers, P. P., & Rehnman, J. (2013). Cognitive sex differences are not magnified as a function of age, sex hormones, or puberty development during early adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(3), 167-179.

Page 45: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 30

Hill, C., Corbett, C., & St. Rose, A. (2010). Why so few? Women in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics. Washington, DC.: American Association of University Women (AAUW).

Hines, M. (2011). Gender development and the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 69-88. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113654

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

Hyde, J. S., & Grabe, S. (2008). Meta-analysis in the psychology of women. In F. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: a handbook of issues and theories (pp. 142-173). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about the nature of gender differences and the implications for gender equity. In S. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equity through education (2nd ed., pp. 19-32). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science, 321(5888), 494-495. doi:10.1126/science.1160364

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 53-69. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.104.1.53

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2006). Gender similarities in mathematics and science. Science, 314(5799), 599-600. doi:10.1126/science.1132154

Jacob, B. A. (2002). Where the boys aren't: non-cognitive skills, returns to school and the gender gap in higher education. Economics of Education Review, 21(6), 589-598. doi:10.1016/S0272-7757(01)00051-6

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509-527. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00421

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor : the Science of Mental Ability. London: Praeger. Kaiser, A. (2015). Re-conceptualizing “sex” and “gender” in the human brain. Zeitschrift für

Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 220(2), 130–136. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000104

Kimura, D. (1992). Sex differences in the brain. Scientific American, 267(3), 119-125. Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kocoska-Maras, L., Zethraeus, N., Rådestad, A. F., Ellingsen, T., von Schoultz, B.,

Johannesson, M., & Hirschberg, A. L. (2011). A randomized trial of the effect of testosterone and estrogen on verbal fluency, verbal memory, and spatial ability in healthy postmenopausal women. Fertility and sterility, 95(1), 152-157.

Latimer, J., Cerise, S., Ovseiko, P. V., Rathborne, J. M., Billiards, S. S., & El-Adhami, W. (2019). Australia's strategy to achieve gender equality in STEM. The Lancet, 393(10171), 524-526. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32109-3

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex difference in spatial skill. Psychological Science, 16(11), 841-845. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01623.x

Liben, L. S., & Coyle, E. F. (2014). Developmental interventions to address the STEM gender gap: Exploring intended and unintended consequences. In L. S. Liben & R. S. Bigler (Eds.), The Role of Gender in Educational Contexts and Outcomes (Vol. 47, pp. 77-115). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Page 46: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 31

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479-1498. doi:10.2307/1130467

Lippa, R. A. (2006). The Gender Reality Hypothesis. American Psychologist, 61(6), 639-640. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.61.6.639

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames, 13(1), 3-13. doi:10.3176/tr.2009.1.01

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Marks, G. N. (2008). Accounting for the gender gaps in student performance in reading and mathematics: evidence from 31 countries. Oxford Review of Education, 34(1), 89-109. doi:10.1080/03054980701565279

McGraw, R., Lubienski, S. T., & Strutchens, M. E. (2006). A closer look at gender in NAEP mathematics achievement and affect data: Intersections with achievement, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(2), 129-150. doi:10.2307/30034845

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37-45. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Kennedy, A. M., & Foy, P. (2007). PIRLS 2006 International Report: IEA’s progress in international reading literacy study in primary schools in 40 countries. Chestnut Hill.

Muro, M., Maxim, R., & Whiton, J. (2019). Automation and Artifical Intelligence: How machines are affecting people and places. Washington, D.C.: Brookings Institution.

Nash, S. C. (1974). Conceptions and concomitants of sex-role stereotyping. Columbia University, Ann Arbor. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (302721823)

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. In M. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263-302). New York: Academic Press.

National Science Foundation. (2017). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2017. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation Retrieved from https://www.nsf.gov/statistics/2017/nsf17310/.

Neisser, U. C., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., . . . Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77-101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77

Newcombe, N. S., Ambady, N., Eccles, J. S., Gomez, L., Klahr, D., Linn, M., . . . Mix, K. (2009). Psychology’s role in mathematics and science education. American Psychologist, 64(6), 538-550. doi:10.1037/a0014813

Newcombe, N. S., & Dubas, J. S. (1992). A longitudinal study of predictors of spatial ability in adolescent females. Child Development, 63(1), 37-46. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03593.x

Nowell, A., & Hedges, L. V. (1998). Trends in gender differences in academic achievement from 1960 to 1994: An analysis of differences in mean, variance, and extreme scores. Sex Roles, 39(1), 21-43. doi:10.1023/A:1018873615316

OECD. (2011). Education at a Glance 2011: OECD Indicators. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development. Retrieved from http://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/education-at-a-glance-2011_eag-2011-en.

OECD. (2015). Education at a Glance 2015: OECD Indicators. Paris, France: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.

Page 47: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 32

OECD. (2016). Education at a Glance 2016: OECD Indicators. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development.

Office of the Chief Economist. (2017). STEM Jobs: 2017 Update. Washington, DC: Department of Commerce.

Office of the Chief Scientist. (2014). Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics: Australia’s Future. Canberra: Australian Government.

Perkins, R., Kleiner, B., Roey, S., & Brown, J. (2004). The High School Transcript Study: A decade of change in curricula and achievement, 1990–2000. (Report No. NCES 2004455). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

President’s Council of Advisors on Science and Technology. (2010). Prepare and Inspire: K-12 Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math (STEM) Education for America’s Future. Washington, D.C.

Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender and academic abilities and preferences. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology (pp. 297-316). New York: Springer.

Puts, D. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Burriss, R. P., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58(2), 282-289. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e39904. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662. doi:10.1037/edu0000012

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2017). Investigating gender differences in mathematics and science: Results from the 2011 Trends in Mathematics and Science Survey. Research in Science Education, 1-26. doi:10.1007/s11165-017-9630-6

Reynolds, M. R., Scheiber, C., Hajovsky, D. B., Schwartz, B., & Kaufman, A. S. (2015). Gender differences in academic achievement: Is writing an exception to the gender similarities hypothesis? The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 176(4), 211-234. doi:10.1080/00221325.2015.1036833

Riegle-Crumb, C. (2005). The cross-national context of the gender gap in math and science. In L. V. Hedges & B. Schneider (Eds.), The social organization of schooling (pp. 227-243). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Ritchie, S. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Enduring links from childhood mathematics and reading achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1301-1308. doi:10.1177/0956797612466268

Shields, S. A. (1975). Functionalism, Darwinism, and the psychology of women. American Psychologist, 30(7), 739-754. doi:10.1037/h0076948

Shields, S. A. (1982). The variability hypothesis: The history of a biological model of sex differences in intelligence. Signs, 7(4), 769-797. doi:10.2307/3173639

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 207-228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207

Stanley, J. C., Benbow, C. P., Brody, L., Dauber, S., & Lupkowski, A. (1992). Gender differences on eighty-six nationally standardized aptitude and achievement tests. In N. Colangelo, S. G. Assouline, & D. L. Ambroson (Eds.), Talent development: Proceedings from the 1991 Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development (pp. 42-65). Unionville, NY: Trillium Press.

Page 48: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 33

Sternberg, R. J. (2014). Teaching about the nature of intelligence. Intelligence, 42, 176-179. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.08.010

Sugimoto, C., Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., & Cronin, B. (2013). Global gender disparities in science. Nature, 504(7479), 211-213. doi:10.1038/504211a

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, mathematical, spatial and verbal intelligence: Four meta analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 493-504. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001

Thorndike, E. L. (1914). Educational Psychology, Vol. 3: Mental work and fatigue and individual differences. New York: Teachers College.

UNESCO. (2012). Fact Sheet: Women in Science (2012). UNESCO Institute for Statistics. Retrieved from http://www.uis.unesco.org/FactSheets/Documents/sti-women-in-science-en.pdf.

Unger, R. K. (1979). Towards a redefinition of sex and gender. American Psychologist, 34(11), 1085-1094. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.11.1085

Vahabi, M. (2007). The impact of health communication on health-related decision making: A review of evidence. Health Education, 107(1), 27-41. doi:10.1108/09654280710716860

Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. doi:10.1037/a0036620

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250-270. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250

Walters, J. (2010). Recasting Title IX: Addressing gender equity in the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics professoriate. Review of Policy Research, 27(3), 317-332. doi:10.1111/j.1541-1338.2010.00444.x

Washington, P. K., Burke, N. J., Joseph, G., Guerra, C., & Pasick, R. J. (2009). Adult daughters’ influence on mothers’ health-related decision making: An expansion of the subjective norms construct. Health education & behavior : the official publication of the Society for Public Health Education, 36(5 Suppl), 129S-144S. doi:10.1177/1090198109338904

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 699-727. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.5.699

Zell, E., Krizan, Z., & Teeter, S. R. (2015). Evaluating gender similarities and differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist, 70(1), 10-20. doi:10.1037/a0038208

Zucker, K. J., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2008). Gender identity disorder in children. In D. L. Rowland & L. Incrocci (Eds.), Handbook of Sexual and Gender Identity Disorders (pp. 376-422). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Page 49: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 34

Chapter 2 – Literature Review

This section presents an overview of research into sex differences, divided into

three parts. Firstly, it offers an overview of empirical evidence of sex differences in

specific cognitive abilities reported in the literature and how this evidence has changed

with the passage of time. For ease of reference, a summary of this material is presented

in Table 2.1, including estimates of effect size strength. Secondly, it presents an

overview of popular beliefs by laypeople about the nature of sex differences in

intelligence of the sexes which often differs from reality. Finally, it reviews theoretical

perspectives on the origins of sex differences in cognitive abilities.

2.1 Summary of Research Findings

As outlined earlier, males and females are equivalent in overall intelligence.

When representative samples of men and women are compared at a population level

(e.g., Deary, Thorpe, Wilson, Starr, & Whalley, 2003), their psychometrically assessed

general intelligence (IQ) is equivalent (Colom, Juan-Espinosa, Abad, & Garcıa, 2000;

Jensen, 1998; Neisser et al., 1996). Sternberg (2014, p. 178) concluded that “there is no

evidence, overall, of sex differences in levels of intelligence” while Halpern (2000, p.

218) concluded that “sex differences have not been found in general intelligence”.

However a large number of studies have noted that the performance of males shows

more statistical variance (i.e., there are a greater number of high- and low-achieving

males at the extreme tails of the distribution), termed the greater male variability

(GMV) hypothesis (H. Ellis, 1904; Feingold, 1992; Shields, 1982). This means that

even if there were no mean sex differences in intelligence, recruiting from highly

selective samples that are high in ability (such as college student subject pools, or a

group of gifted and talented) may show higher male intelligence scores. Thus the reader

is cautioned against a small number of contrarian studies (e.g., Irwing & Lynn, 2005)

Page 50: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 35

showing purported sex differences in general intelligence, but which are

methodologically flawed in the representativeness of their sample selection.

Nonetheless, while there negligible sex differences in psychometrically assessed

intelligence, patterns of sex differences are frequently observed for more specific

cognitive abilities (i.e., some types of cognitive tasks show a female advantage and

others show a male advantage). Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) conducted the first

rigorous review of the available sex difference literature. Their review concluded that

there were robust sex differences for three types of cognitive tasks: verbal abilities,

visual-spatial abilities, and quantitative reasoning (mathematical and scientific

reasoning). Each are reviewed herein. Subsequent researchers such as Halpern (2000)

have also identified sex differences for other types of cognitive abilities such as memory

(Herlitz & Rehnman, 2008), moral cognition (Jaffee & Hyde, 2000; You, Maeda, &

Bebeau, 2011), nonverbal perception (Hall, 1978; LaFrance & Vial, 2016), and

emotional intelligence (Petrides & Furnham, 2000) so the reader is cautioned that a

tripartite classification of sex differences is not exhaustive. But as these traits involve

other non-cognitive processes (e.g., social, emotional), they are beyond the scope of this

review which is confined to intellectual functioning. A review of sex differences in

memory is also presented as memory (particularly working memory) affects

performance on other cognitive tasks (Daneman, 1991; Peng et al., 2017; Schmader &

Johns, 2003)

2.2.1 Verbal abilities.

In a classical text on differential psychology, Anastasi (1958) concluded that

females were superior to males in language ability from infancy throughout adulthood.

Halpern (2000, p. 93) argued that “evidence from a variety of sources supports the

findings that, on average, females have better verbal abilities than males”. While the

Page 51: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 36

extent of sex differences in other types of cognitive ability may be frequently disputed,

sex differences in language and verbal ability are generally regarded as “fairly well

established” (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974, p. 351), while Hyde and Linn (1988)

acknowledged “there is a clear consensus” (p. 53) to the point where it has become one

of the few generally accepted truisms in sex difference research (L. Ellis et al., 2008;

Galsworthy, Dionne, Dale, & Plomin, 2000; Kimura, 2000). But just what types of task

constitute verbal ability, and do males and females differ on all language tasks?

Halpern and others have argued that verbal ability is not a unitary construct (J.B.

Carroll, 1941; Halpern, 2000), and instead encompasses a wide range of tasks rather

than a single underlying ability. Unlike other types of cognition such as visual-spatial

ability there exists no widely accepted formal criteria for defining and organising verbal

and language abilities. Even the term ‘verbal abilities’ is a misnomer because the

commonly accepted lay understanding of verbal pertains to spoken utterances rather

than written ones, which is why my preferred terminology (adopted herein) is verbal

and language abilities. The term applies to any task involving words and language,

covering both language reception (listening and reading) and language production

(orally or in written form). Thus it encompasses a broad constellation of tasks,

including verbal fluency, grammar, spelling, reading and writing, oral comprehension,

speech production, vocabulary, as well as related tasks such as synonym generation and

verbal analogies. By convention these tasks are generally conducted in a speaker’s

native language (which may not always be English, and hence sex differences are not

confined to a single language or culture), but there is also a body of evidence

demonstrating greater female proficiency for second language acquisition. Additionally

significant sex differences have been found for pictographic tasks that require reading

novel/unfamiliar characters such as the Digit-Symbol Test. This is commonly regarded

Page 52: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 37

as a measure of coding/processing speed, but also involves activation of language

regions and phonological skills (Majeres, 2007; Royer, 1978).

Maccoby and Jacklin (1974, p. 351) concluded that females tend to score higher

than males on all verbal ability tasks. They wrote “Girls score higher on tasks involving

both receptive and productive language, and on ‘high-level’ verbal tasks (analogies,

comprehension of difficult written material, creative writing) as well as upon the

"lower-level' measures (fluency). The magnitude of the female advantage varies, being

most commonly about one-quarter of a standard deviation.”

As was the practice at the time, Maccoby and Jacklin produced what has been

termed a narrative review as the statistical techniques of meta-analysis were not yet

developed. Hyde and Linn (1988; Table 3) conducted a subsequent meta-analysis of

research into verbal ability, finding a slight but non-trival sex difference favouring

females of d = -.11 across all ages, and d = -.20 in adults. However there was

considerable variation in magnitude and direction across types of tasks. Females

performed better on speech production (d = -.33), anagrams (d = -.22) and general

language ability across mixed tasks (d = -.20), but did not significantly differ in

vocabulary and there were only negligible differences in reading (d = -.03) and writing

(d = -.09). Males, however, did do significantly better than females on tasks of verbal

analogies (d = +.16) which differs from Maccoby and Jacklin’s conclusion. Hyde and

Linn concluded, somewhat contentiously, that such evidence demonstrates that

“differences in verbal ability no longer exist”, p. 53.

The technique of meta-analysis can be used to aggregate research findings and

objectively measure the size of experimental effects, giving greater credibility to the

conclusions drawn than traditional vote-counting or narrative literature reviews (Hedges

& Olkin, 1985; Rosenthal, 1984), such as those reported by Maccoby and Jacklin

Page 53: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 38

(1974). It is limited, however, by the selection of studies that comprise the dataset and

whether it represents a complete view of the research litearture (Rosenthal & DiMatteo,

2001). Selective inclusion or omission of important studies (“cherry-picking”) will

therefore lead to a biased interpretation but which carries the air of legitimacy. Given

that verbal abilities is a broad constellation of tasks with a nebulous definition, it is

entirely reasonable that some tasks be excluded. However, in a critique Stumpf (1995)

argued that Hyde and Linn’s meta-analysis is characterised by several important

omissions: namely the tasks that Maccoby and Jacklin had identified as showing larger

sex differences, such as writing, grammar/language usage and verbal fluency. By way

of example, Hyde, Geiringer and Yen report an extremely large effect size (d = -.76) for

a verbal fluency task, but Hyde and Linn chose not to include that verbal fluency task in

their analysis.

Additionally a number of large nationally representative samples of reading and

writing reported by Hedges and Nowell (1995) which fell in the time frame defined by

Hyde and Linn for analysis (up to 1986) were also excluded without offering any

justification. Their inclusion would have dramatically increased the overall effect size

calculation, as well as the effect size for reading and writing abilities. Therefore Hyde

and Lynn’s meta-analysis is likely to be a lower-bound estimate of the true effect size in

verbal and language abilities, and runs contrary to the conclusions drawn by other

prominent researchers in this field (David C Geary, 1998; Halpern, 2011; Hedges &

Nowell, 1995; Kimura, 2000; Miller & Halpern, 2013; Neisser et al., 1996). However if

there are meaningful differences between males and females in language ability then

this should be reflected in other objective measures of performance such as student

grades in English and other language classes. A subsequent meta-analysis conducted by

Voyer and Voyer (2014) examined this question by investigating sex differences in

Page 54: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 39

student grades. Based on an analysis of 81 studies, the authors report that females

earned significantly higher grades in English and language classes, with a small to

medium effect size (d = .37).

2.2.1.1 Developmental differences.

When do sex differences in language and verbal abilities appear, and do they

persist into adulthood? All healthy children undergo the same developmental trajectory,

from early cooing and babbling as an infant, to the beginnings of speech and repetition

of heard words as a toddler around 12 months of age, followed by an explosive growth

in vocabulary from 12-24 months after which vocabulary growth becomes linear. Sex

differences in language have been observed from infancy, with newborn girls beginning

cooing and babbling sooner, and engaging in greater vocalization (Balint, 1948;

Gatewood & Weiss, 1930; Korner, 1973). Fenson et al. (1994) report that between 8

and 16 months infant girls understood more words than boys while Halpern (2000)

notes that onset of speech is on average about 1 month earlier in girls than boys. Girls

also show an early advantage in vocabulary size. In a study on vocabulary growth rates

in infants, Huttenlocker et al. (1991) found that while there is only a modest 13-word

difference between girls and boys at age 16 months, this quickly expands to a 51-word

difference by 20 months and 115-word difference by 24 months. A similar study by

Fenson et. al., (1994) also found that after the age of 14 months, infant girls produced

more words with greater sentence complexity. This trend was observed until at least 30

months of age when the study ended. When children enter kindergarten, girls already

show a significant advantage in reading, and this trend continues throughout primary

school (Robinson & Lubienski, 2011) and high school (Lynn & Mikk, 2009). Girls also

tend to be better at spelling than boys from early childhood onwards, as well

demonstrating greater proficiency in punctuation, grammar and language usage from

Page 55: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 40

primary school at least until high school (Lynn, 1992; D. J. Martin & Hoover, 1987;

Reynolds, Scheiber, Hajovsky, Schwartz, & Kaufman, 2015; Stanley, Benbow, Brody,

Dauber, & Lupkowski, 1992). There is also evidence that in cognitively healthy older

adults, females perform better on tasks involving word definition and verbal learning

(Rabbitt, Donlan, Watson, McInnes, & Bent, 1995), verbal fluency (De Dreu, Greer,

Van Kleef, Shalvi, & Handgraaf; Lezack, 1995; Maylor et al., 2007; K.W. Schaie,

1996), and general verbal ability (Maitland, Intrieri, Schaie, & Willis, 2000).

Although evidence of sex differences between boys and girls in child samples

may seem compelling, the most frequently invoked refutation is that they reflect

different rates of maturation (i.e., precocious language development for girls and a

developmental delay for boys but ultimately they will reach the same standard). For

example, in early childhood girls have a somewhat larger vocabulary than boys, but

Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) claimed that parity is achieved by age 3. However, for

many language tasks there is evidence of continuity into later development so simple

differences in rates of maturation may be discounted if sex differences persist into

adulthood. For example, a nationally representative study of adult literacy in the United

States found that the sex difference observed in reading and writing in children endures

into adulthood with males demonstrating poorer reading and writing proficiency

(Kutner et al., 2007). Similar findings are reported below for other language tasks such

as verbal fluency, punctuation and grammar. Indeed Hyde and Linn (1988) found a

developmental trend across all language tasks surveyed (subject to the limitations about

underestimation noted earlier), with quite small sex differences observed during early

childhood but an increase into adulthood (d = .20 across all tasks). So there is little

support for the differential rates of maturation argument as an explanation for apparent

sex differences in language.

Page 56: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 41

2.2.1.2 Reading and writing.

One of the most frequently observed differences in language between males and

females is the pronounced sex difference in reading ability, but the magnitude can vary

considerably across samples depending on demographic factors such as socioeconomic

status, ethnicity, rural versus urban sampling (Fenson et al., 2000; Fernald, Marchman,

& Weisleder, 2013; Kaufman, McLean, & Reynolds, 1988). Less seldom investigated

are differences in writing which tend to be appreciably larger. To quantify this issue,

Hedges and Nowell (1995) published a landmark review on sex differences for a range

of tasks using a number of large, nationally representative samples, including the

National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP 1971-1992). They found that girls

significantly outperformed boys in the domain of reading in each year of assessment

(ranging from d = -.18 to -.30). Furthermore they found that the performance of males

was more variable than that of females with an average variance ratio (VR) of 1.12.

This resulted in an overrepresentation of boys as poor readers, ranging from 1.5 to 2

males for every female falling in the bottom 10th percentile. Similarly, there were also

substantially sized sex differences in writing (ranging from d = -.49 to -.55), as well as

greater male variability. This resulted in sex ratios for students falling in the bottom 10th

percentile of between 2.6 and 3.3 males to every female (Nowell & Hedges, 1998, p.

38). The magnitude of these effect sizes observed would be inconsistent with Hyde and

Linn’s conclusion that sex differences in verbal abilities no longer exist, but as noted

earlier were not included in their review.

Sex differences in reading achievement are not confined to particular cultures or

nationalities. Unlike sex differences in quantitative reasoning that show substantial

variation in size and direction, sex differences in reading achievement are found

universally across all nations which would be consistent with a biological contribution.

Large international assessments such as the Progress in International Reading Literacy

Page 57: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 42

(PIRLS) and PISA show appreciable effect sizes favouring females in all countries

(Guiso, Monte, Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008; Lynn & Mikk, 2009).

While the evidence for sex differences in reading achievement is strong,

relatively few studies have examined the extent sex differences in writing skills,

especially in modern samples. Nowell and Hedges (1998) reported a detailed meta-

analysis of NAEP writing data from the period 1984-1994, finding substantially sized

sex differences in writing favouring females (ranging from d = -.49 to -.55), and that

gender ratios for students falling in the bottom 10th percentile were between 2.6 and 3.3

males to every female (Nowell & Hedges, 1998, p. 38). Although numerous waves of

NAEP assessment data have been collected and annual reports note significantly greater

female writing performance, there has been no attempt to quantify the extent of sex

differences in writing through meta-analysis (an issue addressed in Chapter 5).

Three studies have examined the magnitude of sex differences in writing

proficiency by examining standardization data from two educational assessment tools.

Camarata and Woodcock (2006) presented data from the normative samples of the

Woodcock-Johnson cognitive and achievement batteries, a large representative sample

of males and females aged 5 through to 79. Females scored significantly higher in

writing achievement, with an average effect size across the lifespan of d = -.33. A

similar finding was reported by Scheiber, et al. (2015) who analysed a large sample of

adolescents and young adults completing the Kaufman Test of Educational

Achievement- Second Edition (KTEA-II), which measures participants’ reading, writing,

and mathematics. While no difference was found in mathematics, females scored higher

than males on the tests of reading and writing ability. The effect size for reading was

small (d = -.18), but the effect size for writing (d = -.40) was twice as large as that for

reading. Additionally, Pargulski and Reynolds (2017) present data from the Weschler

Page 58: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 43

Individual Achievement Test- Third Edition (WIAT-III) Written Expression scale,

finding a small but non-trivial effect size of d = -.25 with girls scoring significantly

higher than boys across all age groups.

While educators are well aware that boys are overrepresented in formal

diagnoses of dyslexia and reading impairment (Hawke, Olson, Willcut, Wadsworth, &

DeFries, 2009; Limbrick, Wheldall, & Madelaine, 2008; S. E. Shaywitz, Shaywitz,

Fletcher, & Escobar, 1990), it is less common knowledge that boys are also

overrepresented with writing disorders. For example, in a population-based assessment

of all children born in the town of Rochester, Minnesota between 1976 and 1982 that

was not subject to a gendered referral bias, Katsuic et al. (2006) found that twice as

many boys than girls met the criteria for a clinically significant writing disorder.

Berninger et al., (2008) report a strong comorbidity with reading and writing disorders,

which suggests that they may share a common neurological impairment.

2.2.1.3 Phonological coding and perceptual speed.

Phonological coding is the ability to produce and manipulate information about

the sound structure of verbal stimuli, and to convert between letters and the sounds they

represent. Deficits in phonological coding are linked to difficulties in reading words

(Vellutino, Scanlon, & Tanzman, 1994). There is evidence that there are sex differences

in phonological awareness (Coltheart, Hull, & Slater, 1975). For example, women are

faster than men at counting the number of letters in the English alphabet that contain the

phoneme ‘ee’, while McGuiness and Courtney (1983) found that women make fewer

errors than men when asked to determine whether a target letter is present in word lists

delivered orally. Majeres (2007) has argued that an advantage in phonological coding

contributes to the sex difference in perceptual speed found on some cognitive tasks, as

they are more easily able to convert from symbols to sounds. Consider for example the

Page 59: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 44

digit-symbol substitution task employed in a number of intelligence tests (Jensen, 1998;

Wechsler, 1958), which requires subjects to rapidly convert between unfamiliar

symbols and numbers. Girls and women are able to read these pictographs and translate

them more quickly than men with appreciable effect sizes. Hedges and Nowell found

small to medium sized effects (ranging from d = -.21 to -.43), while Lynn and Mulhern

(1991) found a medium to large effect on the WISC-R digit-symbol coding task in

children d = -.69 and small to medium effects sizes have been found by Feingold (1992)

with adults with the WAIS/WAIS-R, d = -.34. A review by Roivainen (2011) reached

similar conclusions for the WAIS-III and Woodcock-Johnson instruments.

Another perceptual speed task involving speeded reading is the finding A’s task

(Halpern & Tan, 2001; Kimura & Hampson, 1994). Participants are presented with lists

of words arranged in columns spread out over several pages, and asked to cross out the

letter ‘A’ whenever it is encountered within a time limit. Halpern and Tan (2001)

reported a medium to large effect size on this task d = -.77. However many reviews of

verbal and language abilities overlook these pronounced differences in speeded reading

for novel stimuli and phonological coding.

2.2.1.4 Vocabulary.

Sex differences in vocabulary develop extremely early. Between the ages of 1

and 2 years, girls score significantly higher than boys on a range of vocabulary

measures including vocabulary production, sentence length and sentence complexity

(Feldman et al., 2000; Huttenlocher et al., 1991). Girls also demonstrate better

comprehension of spoken words, and as toddlers their vocabulary comprehension and

production expands at a much faster rate than boys until at least 26 months of age

(Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Reznick & Goldfield, 1992). Maccoby and Jacklin (1974)

claimed though that girls and boys appear to reach parity after two to three years, and so

Page 60: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 45

the research question of sex differences in vocabulary for adolescents and young adults

has been seldom investigated. However a careful review of the literature does show

some instances where sex differences in vocabulary have been tested and reported. For

example, in a cross-sectional study of elementary school students, Gates (1961) reported

sex differences in performance on vocabulary tests of girls and boys (grades 2 through

8), which subsequent meta-analysis shows to be a small but non-trivial effect size

(weighted d = -.23, Z = -13.25, p < .001). Similarly in a large sample of primary and

secondary school students Lynn and Wilson (1993) found girls significantly

outperformed boys on the Mill Hill Vocabulary Test (MHVT) which measures

participants’ ability to provide the meaning of target words. Weschler (1958) reported a

small sex difference in performance for the Vocabulary subscale with the original

standardization sample of the WAIS (ages 16-64), with females scoring slightly higher

than males, d = -.11, though cautioned that items were carefully chosen to minimise the

extent of sex differences and that it may be an underestimate of the true effect size.

Furthermore the meta-analysis by Hyde and Linn (1998, pg. 61) found that in young

adults aged 19 to 25, there was a small gender difference in vocabulary favouring girls,

d= -.23.

Other studies have failed to find meaningful sex differences in vocabulary tests

(Storck & Looft, 1973). For example Kaufman, McLean and Reynolds (1988) report no

significant sex differences in the vocabulary subtest of the WAIS-R for the American

standardization sample of adults, which differed from Weschler’s observations of

performance with the earlier WAIS instrument. A similar null finding was reported by

Storck and Looft (1973) who reported a detailed analysis of vocabulary word

definitions in a cross-sectional sample (ages 6-66). However, in younger samples Lynn

and Mulhern (1991) report that boys scored slightly higher than girls on the WISC-R

Page 61: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 46

vocabulary subtest for both the Scottish and American standardization samples, while

Kramer et al. (1988; 1997) found a slight but statistically significant difference

favouring males aged 5-16. The meta-analysis by Hyde and Linn (1988) across ages

found no sex difference in adolescence but curiously significantly greater vocabulary

for women in the age category of 19-25 (d = -.23). Therefore it is difficult to determine

their extent (if any), and further research is required. Presumably these discrepant

findings reflect demographic and sampling differences, as well as differences across

instruments. Later revisions of the WISC and WAIS were purposively chosen to

minimise sex differences so that separate norm tables would not be needed for males

and females (Halpern, 2000; Wechsler, 1958).

Vocabulary tests are recognized as a good representative of verbal-intelligence

scales, but do not predict achievement in most language tasks (Guilford, 1967). While

in younger children vocabulary may be a good measure of language proficency, in older

children and adults vocabulary is less a measure of language ability and more a measure

of crystallized intelligence/level of education (where one would not necessarily expect

to find sex differences in modern samples). Indeed many studies often use vocabulary

as a proxy for crystallised intelligence, as it requires less training and time to adminster.

At present time there is insufficient evidence to conclude there are sex differences

present in vocabulary in late adolescence and adulthood because few studies have

examined this question. However larger sex differences are often still found for some

tasks that involve access to vocabulary, such as verbal fluency (reviewed below). While

absolute vocabulary size and recognition of words may not differ between males and

females, it is possible that females are simply more adept at organizing and retrieving

lexical knowledge (see below).

Page 62: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 47

2.2.1.4 Verbal fluency

One of the largest reported sex differences in this area is that of verbal fluency

(Halpern & Tan, 2001; Hines, 1990), with effect sizes ranging from small to large

across samples and type of verbal fluency task. Methodology for verbal fluency tasks

varies, but it generally involves asking participants to generate a list of words matching

a particular criteria (e.g., words beginning with the letter F, or words that are a type of

animal) under strict timing conditions (typically 60 seconds). Although this can be

administered orally if individually tested, typically it is administered in a written format

suitable for group administration. To date though, there have been no studies

investigating sex differences in the format of administration so it is not clear whether

the act of writing confers any advantage (such as mental cueing of related words, or

differences in writing speed between males and females).

Sincoff and Sternberg (1987) have argued that language performance can be

divided into two broad domains – verbal comprehension, and verbal fluency. Though

both are equally important for successful verbal functioning, Sincoff and Sternberg note

there has been a clear preference by researchers and educators in focusing heavily on

assessment of verbal comprehension but seldom investigating, assessing or teaching

verbal fluency. Estes (1974) has argued that verbal fluency also involves additional

cognitive processes, including strategies to organise their thinking and seek an optimum

search strategy. Weiss et al., (2006) have provided functional imaging evidence that

men and women may use different strategies for verbal fluency generation tasks and

that females use a balance of clustering and switching strategies to maximise word

production.

There are three categories of verbal fluency tasks: phonological, semantic, and

synonym generation. Phonological verbal fluency measures the ability to produce lists

of words beginning with a target letter, and is a commonly performed

Page 63: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 48

neuropsychological task (Lezack, 1995; Tombaugh, Kozak, & Rees, 1999), sometimes

referred to as the Controlled Oral Word Association task (COWA; Benton & Hamsher,

1989). Additional restrictions may be imposed to increase the difficulty of the task, such

as the requirement that only unique words be produced (fast may be given, but not

faster or fastest), and that people’s names and brand/product names are not permitted.

This is repeated for several letters (most commonly ‘F A S’) to produce a continuous

score, though variations on the stimuli letters are made for non-English languages (e.g.,

Kosmidis, Vlahou, Panagiotaki, & Kiosseoglou, 2004). Sex differences are frequently

found for this type of task. For example, Weiss et al. (2003) found a medium-sized

effect (d = -.45) in college-aged subjects, while Herlitz et al. (1999) found a similarly

sized effect (d = -.49). There also appears to be an interaction between sex and years of

education. Ruff et al. (1996, Table 3) found a medium effect size (d = -.53) in adult

samples (16 to 70 years old) who had completed a college education, but a relatively

small sex differences (d = -.10) across all education levels. Loonstra, Tarlow and Sellers

(2001) reached a similar conclusion (d = -.15) in a meta-analysis of COWA studies in

neurologically healthy adults, noting an interaction with education. This may be

reflective of historical effects of female educational attainment of the past, while in

modern samples of college students with greater female representation considerably

larger sex differences in phonological verbal fluency are typically found. Bucking

against this trend though, a large cross-sectional study of adults across the lifespan

conducted by Schaie and Hertzog (1983) found substantial differences amongst older

adults, as did a later study of neurologically healthy adults ages 35-80 by Herlitz,

Nilsson, and Bäckman (1997), d = -.16. However, some studies have found only slight

or non-significant sex differences (Tombaugh et al., 1999), particularly in extremely

young children (Hurks et al., 2006; Regard, Strauss, & Knapp, 1982) as there appears to

Page 64: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 49

be a developmental trend towards larger gender gaps with age and education.

Nonetheless in studies with adults, sex differences in phonological verbal fluency are so

consistently found that neurologists have proposed separate norms tables for male and

female subjects (Benton & Hamsher, 1989; Bolla, Lindgren, Bonaccorsy, & Bleecker,

1990; Loonstra et al., 2001; Ruff et al., 1996).

Semantic category verbal fluency tasks require participants to produce words

that belong to a particular semantic category (e.g., fruits & vegetables, animals, etc.),

and studies typically find much larger sex differences than for phonological verbal

fluency. While fruits and animals are the most frequently chosen stimuli as they are

accessible to both children and adults (Gladsjo et al., 1999), more difficult versions of

the test employ less commonly accessed semantic categories such as types of clothing,

professions, games, tools, vehicles, etc. Effect sizes for semantic verbal fluency studies

vary depending on the type and age of sample (Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-Nuuttila,

2001). For example Gordon and Lee (1986) reported a medium effect size (d = -.44) for

semantic category task in a college-aged sample, while Aceveo et al. (2000) reported

similar effect sizes in English (d = -.54) and Spanish (d = -.37) samples of healthy older

adults. Like phonological verbal fluency tasks there also appears to be a strong

interaction between sex and years of education. Additionally Capitani et al. (1999) have

claimed though that the choice of stimuli may confer a gendered advantage due to

differences in familiarity with those domains; specifically they found that females

produced more words for fruits but males produced more words for tools. Thus great

care should be taken to select gender-neutral semantic categories. Failure to do so would

constitute a methodological flaw in sex difference studies.

Synonym generation verbal fluency tasks require subjects to recall the lexical

meaning of a word and generate appropriate synonyms, in a similar fashion to the

Page 65: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 50

thesaurus. Such tasks are considerably more difficult than other verbal fluency, as the

subject must recall the definition of a target word and spontaneously generate words

with similar meanings while inhibiting those that are unrelated. This is also used as a

neuropsychological screening tool, referred to as the Controlled Associates Test

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). Sex differences in synonym generation are

extremely large. For example, Hines (1990) reported an effect size of d = 1.2) on a

synonym generation task with college-aged samples, while Rahman and Wilson (2003)

found an effect size of d = 1.05 in a sample of men and women. Both these studies

showed a substantial female advantage. However, few studies have employed synonym

generation tasks and it is not clear whether these represent outliers.

In summary, the magnitude of sex differences in fluency varies considerably

depending on the type of sample and whether phonological, semantic, or synonym

verbal fluency tasks are employed. In college or university educated samples, effect

sizes for phonological and semantic are medium while synonym tasks are large in size.

2.2.1.5 Listening skills and comprehension.

One area of verbal abilities that there are few sex differences is listening skills

and comprehension. This is an important observation, as serious deficits

in listening ability between the sexes would be one pathway by which more general

language deficits might arise. Badian (1999) examined the entire population of a school

district followed from pre-kindergarten through to grade 7, finding no difference

between boys and girls in the SAT listening comprehension subtest in each annual test.

Earlier research on listening comprehension by Haberland (1959) similarly found no

difference between men and women in college student samples across three types

of listening comprehension tests. Keith, Reynolds, Patel and Ridley (2008) examined a

cross-section of participants aged 6 to 59 using the Woodcock-Johnson battery of

Page 66: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 51

cognitive tests, finding only minimal sex difference in oral comprehension scores (d = -

.06). Though females scored significantly higher in oral comprehension, it falls short of

Hyde’s criterion for non-trivial sex differences. Nonetheless, a number of high profile

reviews (e.g., David C. Geary, 2010; Kimura, 2000) make the general claim that there

are sex differences in listening comprehension. At present time there is insufficient

evidence in support of that claim.

2.2.1.6. Verbal learning.

A large body of research has found that there are sex differences in what are

termed verbal learning tasks which require subjects to commit to memory word lists

(Herlitz et al., 1997; Kramer et al., 1988), with females generally scoring higher on

recall in adult and child samples. However females also score higher than males on

other types of memory tests such as object location and digit-span tasks (see Section

2.2.4). Therefore it is unclear whether greater female proficiency is the result of

employing verbal stimuli, or instead actual differences in working memory. For ease of

classification, discussion of sex differences in verbal learning have been represented as

primarily a memory task, but some narrative reviews classify it as a distinct verbal

ability.

2.2.1.7 Spelling, punctuation and grammar.

The question of sex differences in spelling is most frequently investigated in

studies with children where it is a common finding that girls score significantly higher

than boys at all grades of schooling (Allred, 1990; Berninger et al., 2008). For example,

in samples of students assessed in Australia as part of NAPLAN testing, girls score

substantially higher than boys in spelling across all grades (Appendix A3). In the

context of educational testing, the most widely administered test of spelling proficiency

is the Spelling subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT; Bennett, Seashore, &

Page 67: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 52

Wesman, 1982), which is a standardised test of educational achievement. Stanley et al.

(1992) reported the effect size magnitude from the 1980 standardization sample of the

DAT, which was a nationally representative sample of 61,000 American students in

Grades 8-12. Girls scored considerably higher than boys in spelling in every high school

grade (effect sizes ranging from d = -.38 in Grade 8 to d = -.50 by Grade 12). An earlier

analysis by Feingold (1988) also reports medium-sized differences in spelling on the

DAT for earlier standardizations in 1947, 1962 and 1972 with a developmental trend

towards wider gender gaps with age. Similarly, Lynn (1992) found significantly higher

female performance with the British standardization sample of approximately 10,000

students, with a developmental trend towards larger sex differences with age. Berninger

et al. (2008) have noted that the cause of pronounced spelling deficits is not clearly

understood or well researched, but that they are frequently observed in students (and

their parents) who are diagnosed with dyslexia. Furthermore, childhood spelling

problems usually persist into adulthood even if they no longer qualify for a diagnosis of

dyslexia (Lefly & Pennington, 1991). Spelling impairments are also related to other

writing deficits in composition, as well as handwriting (Berninger et al., 2008).

Another area where males and females have been shown to differ is in their

grammatical usage. The above mentioned DAT battery measures punctuation and

grammatical knowledge in the Language Usage subtest, which presents subjects with a

series of sentences and asks the reader to identify which segment (if any) contains an

error. Stanley et al. (1992) report small to medium effect sizes favouring girls

(approximately d = -.40), which mirrors that found in earlier decades by Feingold

(1988) with the same instrument. Greater proficiency in the understanding of correct

grammar may also contribute to the observed sex difference in writing noted earlier.

Page 68: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 53

2.2.1.8 Speech production.

As noted earlier, Hyde and Linn concluded that the largest sex difference in

verbal abilities was for tasks involving speech production (d = -.33) which is a small to

medium- sized effect. As noted earlier, sex differences in speech production are

frequently observed in infants with girls speaking earlier and producing more complex

speech. Boys are also more likely than girls to exhibit developmental speech delays and

specific language impairment (SLI; J. Stevenson & Richman, 2008; Tomblin et al.,

1997). The trend towards greater proficiency and usage in speech continues into early

childhood. For example, Brownwell and Smith (1973) observed length and complexity

of verbalization of speech in four-year-old children across dyads, triads, and small

groups, finding that young girls produced substantially more words than young boys

(average d = -.58), while similar findings were reported by Busswell (1980) in second

and third grade elementary students. A meta-analytic review by Leaper and Smith

(2004) of childrens’ talkativeness found that sex differences are present but much

smaller in older children however. Furthermore there is little evidence that adult men

and women differ in the number of words spoken during a day, despite popular cultural

stereotypes and media reports (Elliot, 2009). For example, in a highly cited study Mehl

et al. (2007) reported an analysis of daily speech of men and women over a period of 4-

7 days, finding no evidence that women were more talkative than men. A subsequent

meta-analytic review of similar studies by Leaper and Ayers (2007) found that contrary

to prevailing gender stereotypes, men actually spoke slightly more words in a day than

women. However the authors concluded that the difference was negligible in

magnitude, so presumably the sex differences in amount of speech production observed

in children declines during adolescence or adulthood, presumably due to social factors.

There are also sex differences in the quality of speech production reported in the

literature. Males are greatly overrepresented in the prevalence of stuttering (Halpern,

Page 69: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 54

1997; Yairi & Ambrose, 1992), though it is not clear to what extent this is the result of

referral bias. Halpern (2000) notes that there are three to four times more males than

females who stutter. Silverman and Zimmer (1982) also note that when present, the

severity of impairment for stuttering is greater in males and may cause more distress.

One reason that this may be the case is that females are generally more proficient in

speech articulation, which may buffer against speech impairment. In a series of studies

on the quality of speech articulation, Hampson and Kimura (1988) and Hampson (1990;

1990) have shown that women articulate more clearly and accurately than men,

especially for speeded articulation tasks (e.g., speeded counting, colour reading, and

syllable repetition). However, they also found that intra-individual performance is

influenced by estrogen hormone levels.

Such findings have been independently replicated by other researchers who have

found that women articulate vowels and consonants more clearly and with greater

accuracy (Kempe, Puts, & Cárdenas, 2013; Wadnerkar, Cowell, & Whiteside, 2006).

Women are also able to read aloud lists of words significantly faster than men and with

fewer errors (Majeres, 1999). Sex differences in speech disfluency are also pronounced.

Hall (1984) reports that girls and women generate fewer pauses, hesitations, and filler

utterances (e.g., umm, err, etc.) in unscripted speech, and are less likely to commit

articulatory retrieval errors (e.g., misspeaking the wrong word). These differences in

speech were not insubstantial - Hall reported that 9 out of 10 men have more pauses and

filler utterances than the average woman, and 3 out of 4 women commit fewer speech

errors than the typical man.

2.2.1.9 Second language acquisition.

While most studies focus on sex differences in primary language acquisition, a

number of studies have found that females may be better equipped to learn a second

Page 70: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 55

language. In childhood studies girls consistently outperformed boys in studies from the

UK (Burstall, 1975), Ireland (Lynn & Wilson, 1993), Israel (Lewey & Chen, 1974, as

cited by Lynn & Piffer, 2011), and Sweden (Ekstrand, 1980). Importantly these studies

involve objective tests of actual ability rather than awarded grades. Additionally, Payne

and Lynn (2011) found a similar female advantage on reading comprehension in

college-aged samples from the United States (d = -.49). On the basis of these and earlier

studies, Payne and Lynn concluded that females are better equipped to learn a second

language. Females also surpass their male peers in grades for language-related classes

during early childhood (Pomerantz, Altermatt, & Saxon, 2002) and adolescence (Mau &

Lynn, 2000) and adulthood (Voyer & Voyer, 2014), with a small to medium sized effect

d = -.37. However one factor that might influence language acquisition is that girls

generally had more favourable attitudes to learning a new language (S. C. Baker &

MacIntyre, 2000; Burstall, 1975) and just as with reading, motivation to master a

second language may differ.

2.2.1.10 Verbal reasoning and analogies.

One exception to the general rule of greater female proficiency for verbal tasks

may be for verbal reasoning and analogies. The meta-analysis by Hyde and Linn (1988)

found that males actually outperformed females on tasks involving verbal analogies d =

+.16, a small but non-trivial effect size. A difficulty in interpreting this result though is

that performance on verbal reasoning tasks also requires recruitment of other non-

language cognitive processes, such as the routine application of logic and executive

functioning. When differences between males and females are observed for such a task,

this impurity makes it difficult to determine whether it is due to group differences in

verbal and language abilities, or instead in some other cognitive component. For

example, Colom et al. (2004) examined sex differences in verbal reasoning in a large

Page 71: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 56

sample of university students by having them complete linear syllogisms exercises (e.g.,

“Jane is better than Peter, Peter is better than Paul, Who is worse?”). Colom found that

subjects develop a mental spatial diagram for solving such analogies. Though a small

male advantage on verbal reasoning was found (d = +.16), the authors found this

difference vanished when statistically controlling for visual-spatial ability (on which

males typically perform higher). Thus some tasks of verbal reasoning that appear, at

least on the surface, to be dealing with verbally presented information may in fact be

drawing on other cognitive processes such as spatial and working memory. Thus any

observed sex differences in verbal reasoning should be interpreted cautiously as it may

reflect sex differences in other cognitive processes.

Another prominent finding reported in the literature is that males perform

slightly higher on the verbal reasoning component of the SAT exam used to assess

students suitability for college entry (Feingold, 1988; Halpern, 2000). This is a multiple

choice test assessing verbal reasoning and verbal comprehension, but Halpern (2000)

notes that it is heavily weighted with analogies which favour males. This finding is

consistent across each year of assessment, though a meta-analysis of the effect shows it

to be almost trivial in magnitude d + .0454 [95% CI = .04 to .05; Appendix A2]. Spelke

(2005) has cautioned that conclusions drawn from such a sample provides only tentative

evidence because it is not a representative sample: significantly greater numbers of

females choose to sit the SAT exams while the male sample is more selective. In order

to rule out this possibility, Mau and Lynn (2001) examined data from the Baccalaureate

and Beyond (B&B) 1992-1994 longitudinal study, which is a demographically

representative sample of American college graduates. They found that males had

significantly outperformed females on their SAT Verbal completed before starting

college, though the effect size was relatively small (d = +.14). Lynn (1994) also

Page 72: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 57

reported a slight male advantage on verbal reasoning tests in standardization samples for

the WISC and WAIS intelligence tests in the United States, Europe and China. Taken

together with other verbal reasoning studies the evidence shows a slight male advantage

on such tasks but it is questionable to what extent such tasks tap actual verbal ability as

they are not cognitively “pure”. Additionally there has been one study by Strand, Deary

and Smith (2006) that found significantly higher female performance for verbal

reasoning in a nationally representative sample of British children (n = 320,000,

comprising almost half the child population) on the Cognitive Abilities Test (CogAT)

Version 3, though the effect size was relatively small, d = -.15. Therefore it is highly

likely that test content and construct validity are a factor, and that sex differences for

analogies (if any) are relatively small.

2.2.2 Visual-spatial ability.

Sex differences in visual-spatial ability appear to be the most reliably found and

have larger effect sizes that most other cognitive tasks (Halpern, 2011), with males

scoring considerably higher on average than females for most visual-spatial tasks.

Voyer et al. (1995) conducted the most comprehensive meta-analysis to date, finding

robust effect sizes and no support for declining sex differences over time. A full review

of the literature on visual-spatial reasoning is deferred until Chapter 3, presented as a

publication output.

2.2.3 Quantitative ability.

Halpern (2000) noted that the term quantitative reasoning is broadly defined as a

heterogeneous set of cognitive aptitudes encompassing tasks involving both

mathematical and scientific reasoning. Quantitative reasoning also draws on other

cognitive abilities, including general reasoning, working memory, visuospatial abilities,

semantic memory for facts and concepts, and advanced problem-solving skills (Halpern

Page 73: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 58

et al., 2007). Some authors have used the term quantitative abilities quite narrowly to

refer to proficiency with numbers (e.g., calculation), but for the purposes of this review,

a more expansive definition encompassing performance in either mathematical or

scientific domains is adopted. This differs somewhat from the terminology

“Quantitative Ability” (Gq) under the Cattell-Horn-Carrol (CHC) model of cognitive

abilities (J. Horn, 1994; J. L. Horn, 1991; McGrew, 1997), as it includes broader

scientific problem solving tasks than solely mathematics.

Historically, scientific problem solving was not included in the initial

intelligence batteries that CHC theory used for factor analysis, but has been included in

other intelligence batteries such as the Armed Services Vocational Battery (ASVAB).

Carrol (1993), however, has argued that quantitative reasoning is a broader construct

than just computation skill, as it also includes logical reasoning processes “in order to

arrive at correct conclusions. The reasoning processes may be either inductive, or

deductive, or both” (p. 246). Thus it incorporates elements of logical reasoning and the

scientific method. Additionally, Vernon’s (1950) hierarchical model of intelligence also

includes scientific and mathematic reasoning under a common factor (k:m).

In educational psychology the term quantitative reasoning is used more broadly

to encompass both mathematical and scientific tasks (Halpern et al., 2007). Educators

attempt to measure these two aspects in isolation through standardised tests of

mathematical or science achievement, though one often involves drawing on content

and skills from the other and performance on these tests are strongly correlated (r > .80;

see Chapter 6). The issue of sex differences in mathematical performance has received

greater attention than that of science achievement, but both domains are equally

important for the problem of the underrepresentation of women in STEM. This is

because developing a sense of mastery and competency in these domains is strongly

Page 74: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 59

linked to the decision to undertake further studies in STEM-fields (Jacobs, Lanza,

Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002; Simpkins, Davis-Kean, & Eccles, 2006).

Hyde, Fennema, and Lamon (1990) conducted the first meta-analysis of studies

examining sex differences in mathematical performance. The results showed a weighted

mean effect size favouring males of d = +.20. Hyde et al. then excluded from their effect

size calculation all data from the Scholastic Aptitude Test of Mathematics (SAT-M),

noting that the weighting would have inflated the overall effect size calculation,

reaching a final effect size of d = +.15. The authors therefore concluded that there were

negligible sex differences, with males scoring somewhat higher on some tasks and

females scoring higher on others. However, this interpretation of the analysis has been

challenged by other prominent researchers. Halpern and Wright (1996) noted that Hyde

et al’s conclusion obscures important differences across type of sample and task, as well

as age-related trends. For example, in their meta-analysis, Hyde et al. found strong

developmental differences across age groups. Young girls (age groups 5-10, and 11-14)

scored slightly higher in mathematical performance than boys (d = -.07), but this pattern

reverses shortly after puberty and upon entering high school as noted earlier by other

researchers (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Nash, 1979), with a stronger intensification of

sex differences in the period between 8th and 12th grade. Students aged 15-18 show a

small male advantage (d = +.29) in the high school years even after excluding the SAT-

M, but the gender gap widens to a medium-sized difference in college years (d =+.41,

19-25) and beyond (d = +.59, 26 and older) which Halpern and Wright argued cannot be

dismissed as being ‘trivial’. It also mirrors the developmental trend found for the

Arithmetic subtest in intelligence tests, with no significant differences found in the

Page 75: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 60

WISC but substantially higher scores in males under the WAIS for adolescents and

adults (Kaufman & Lichtenberger, 2006).

Hyde et al. also found that the representativeness of the sample was a strong

moderator – gender gaps were considerably larger in highly select samples (e.g.,

academically gifted, high SES), and lower in samples drawn from the general

population. This raises the question of whether convenience samples demonstrate a

selection bias, and whether sex differences in mathematics might be found with more

representative samples. The joint effects of selection and variability is a methodological

issue Becker and Hedges (1988) warned about when testing hypotheses about sex

differences and similarities.

A subsequent meta-analysis by Lindberg, Hyde, Petersen, and Linn (2010)

reached similar conclusions to their earlier review supporting the null hypothesis, but

also included more representative nationally representative data from the NAEP and

several longitudinal studies. Heterogeneity analyses of effect sizes showed that these

studies were not comparable “and vary along some dimension(s)”, p. 1130. Moderator

analysis presented by Lindberg, Hyde, Petersen and Linn found differences across

nationalities, with U.S. and Australian samples showing a modest male advantage of d =

+.10, and that there were age-related differences for high school students d = +.23 and

college d = .18. However their conclusion was the same as earlier that it indicated no

sex difference. A further limitation of their analysis was that they did not examine what

the combined effect of mean sex differences and greater variability had on the sex-ratios

of high achieving mathematical students, which earlier researchers identified as being

considerably sized (Benbow, Lubinski, Shea, & Eftekhari-Sanjani, 2000). Both studies

Page 76: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 61

also constrained their focus to mathematical performance, and did not consider

scientific reasoning which is typically larger (Halpern et al., 2007).

Given the issues of representativeness and other limitations noted above, a

stronger evidence base is needed to test the hypothesis of sex differences in quantitative

reasoning. Hedges and Nowell (1995) conducted a meta-analysis on sex differences in

mathematics and science, drawing on large nationally representative assessments of

U.S. student performance student testing data over three decades (1960-1992). Males

significantly outperformed females across all years, with small effect sizes (d = +.11 to

d = +.26) found for mathematics and small to medium effect sizes for science (d = +.11

to d = +.50). The authors also examined the combined effect of mean sex differences

and greater male variability by examining the sex ratios of those performing at the

upper-right tail of the ability distribution (95th percentile). High achieving males

outnumbered females by a factor of 2:1 in mathematics, and ranged from 2.5:1 to as

high as 7:1 for science achievement. While a pioneering study, the considerable passage

of time since their analysis and purported changes in gender stereotypes and the status

of women has led some authors (e.g., Caplan & Caplan, 2005) to question whether

similar outcomes would be found with modern samples (an issue addressed in Chapter

4).

Another line of evidence on the magnitude of sex differences comes from the

Scholastic Assessment (formerly, Aptitude) Test of Mathematics (SAT-M), which plays

a critical effect on college admissions in the United States (see Figure 2.1). Halpern and

Wright (1996, p. 6) noted that the gender gap in SAT-M scores has a ”huge” impact on

college admissions and cannot be easily dismissed as being insignificant. A meta-

analysis (see Appendix A1) on SAT-M results shows a weighted average effect size of

d = +.30 [95%CI= .29 to .31], drawn from a sample size of 30.3 million over the past

Page 77: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 62

two decades (1996-2016). However Spelke (2005) has argued that inferring sex

differences from such data may be problematic in that it is not a representative sample

of the general population: the decision to sit the SAT-M exam is made by the student

(and hence self-selected), and significantly more girls sit the exam than boys. The

sample is, however, representative of potential college applicants and the sex difference

effect has persisted over time with minimal change.

Figure 2.1. Sex differences in mathematical performance on the SAT-M over the past

two decades (1996-2016). Source: College Board Total Group Profile Reports.

Another useful source of information are standardization samples used in

development and validation of tests of academic achievement. In order to develop tables

of statistical norms, samples must be recruited across a range of ages that reflect the

underlying population in terms of socioeconomic status, ethnicity, level of education,

etc. These norms are used to evaluate a subject’s performance relative to other similarly

450

460

470

480

490

500

510

520

530

540

550

1996 1998 2000 2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016

Mea

n S

AT-

M s

core

.

Historical trend for Scholastic Assessment Test for Mathematics (SAT-M)

Males

Females

Page 78: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 63

aged peers, for the purposes of evaluating giftedness as well as conditions such as

dyscalculia.

Kaufman, Kaufman, Liu and Johnson (2009) reported data from the adult

standardization sample of the Kaufmann Test of Educational Achievement II, Brief

Form (KTEA-II Brief). The Math Composite subtest measures subjects on a broad

range of arithmetic and applied problem-solving tasks. Adult males scored significantly

higher than females, d = .28, which is a small but non-trivial effect size. Similarly,

Camarata and Woodcock (2006) examined gender differences in the quantitative

reasoning factor (Gq) for the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Tests (WJ-77, WJ-R, WJ

III), which comprises two mathematical subtests. Math Fluency measures the ability to

add, subtract, and multiply rapidly and is a test of speed, while the Quantitative

Concepts test involves mathematical formula and identifying number patterns. The

authors found a small gender difference in favour of males across all ages d = .16, but

which reached a peak for ages 19-34, d = .47 which is a medium effect size. More

considerable gender differences existed for subjects aged 50-79, (d = .84) but this

cannot be separated from the historical educational disadvantage experienced by women

of that generation and are likely to be a cohort effect.

In the United States, an assessment comparable to the SAT-M is also conducted

across various fields of science. High school students can elect to undertake rigorous

coursework and complete the College Advanced Placement (AP) examinations, earning

course credit in their chosen subject area and preparing them for college. Sex

differences are frequently observed in college AP exams (Buck, Kostin, & Morgan,

2002; Moore, Combs, & Slate, 2012). For example, a meta-analysis of sex differences

in performance on AP exams in the 2016 year1 showed small to medium sized effects in

1 Source College Board Advanced Placement Exam Dataset (https://research.collegeboard.org/programs/ap/data/archived/ap-2016)

Page 79: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 64

Biology (d = +.27), Computer Science (d = +.10), Chemistry (d = +.31), Physics –

Electricity and Magnetism (d = +.17), Physics – Mechanics (d = +.32), but no

significant difference in Psychology (d = .00). Although these samples are also self-

selected, they do indicate the presence of non-trivial sex differences in science

achievement at the end of compulsory schooling in the United States. Willingham and

Cole (1997) also report data from nationally representative samples of twelfth grade

students, showing small to medium effect sizes across general tests of science (average

d = +.17). They also report data on the Armed Services Vocational Battery (ASVB)

tests of general science (d = +.36), mechanical comprehension and reasoning (d = +.83)

and understanding of electronics and circuits (d = +.78) which present considerably

harder test content.

While these lines of evidence are highly US-centric, further conclusions can be

drawn by examining cross-cultural patterns of sex differences in educational

achievement. Guiso et al (2008) examined student testing data from the 2003

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), which offers a measure of

student achievement in reading, mathematics and science in students at age 15 which is

typically the final year of compulsory schooling in OECD nations. Their analysis found

that boys scored higher than girls in mathematics in all but three of the 40 participating

nations (Guiso et al., 2008, Figure S1A), but that the magnitude of the gender gap was

diminished in countries with higher national levels of gender equality. Thus the

presence of a gender gap in mathematics is not inevitable, and shows substantial cross-

cultural variability. A meta-analysis by Else-Quest et al. (2010) on the same 2003 wave

reached a similar conclusion, but also extended this to include younger participants

from the 2003 Trends in Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS). Though science

achievement was also measured in PISA and TIMSS, the authors of both studies did not

Page 80: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 65

report an analysis of this dataset (an issue addressed in Chapter 5). However a

subsequent study by Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2017)2 did examine mathematics

and science achievement in younger students measured as part of the 2011 TIMSS

wave. The study found substantial cross-cultural variability in size and direction of sex

differences. Some countries reported higher male performance in science, while other

countries showed a reversal of direction and reported higher female performance. Thus

gender differences in science achievement, like mathematics, appear to be highly

malleable to sociocultural factors.

Compounding the difficulty of interpreting whether sex differences in

quantitative reasoning are meaningful is the test and grade discrepancy. When males as

a group have higher test scores than females on tests of mathematics in science, it is

presumed that this represents genuine differences in latent ability (i.e., actual

quantitative reasoning skills). However, in an analysis of high school transcript data

from the United States (where boys score higher than girls on NAEP mathematics and

science tests), Shettle et al. (2007) observed that girls actually earn higher grades in

mathematics and all fields of science. Subsequently, a meta-analysis by Voyer and

Voyer (2014) found the same effect across a longer time frame with girls earning higher

grades than boys in mathematics and science, at least for the United States. Dekhtyar et

al. (2018) report a similar pattern of higher female grades in mathematics and science

for students in Sweden. Thus one cannot preclude the possibility that (especially on

high-stakes tests) girls show diminished performance due to factors such as test anxiety

and stereotype threat, but are equal or greater in actual ability when alternate forms of

assessment (such as laboratory reports, essays, and assignments) are employed.

2 Ommitted due to space requirements

Page 81: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 66

Alternately the sex differences observed for language tasks may be responsible for

higher grades with alternate assessment to solely high-stakes exams.

While the magnitude of sex differences in mathematics and science may be

disputed, one area where there is clear consensus among researchers is on attitudes and

self-efficacy towards mathematics. In a meta-analysis Hyde et al. (1990) found that

males report greater confidence and more positive attitudes towards mathematics across

all age groups, and such findings are routinely reported in the literature. Insufficient

studies have been undertaken for a science-focused meta-analysis, but Reilly, Neumann

and Andrews (2017) report data from the 2011 wave of TIMSS, noting that boys in

eighth grade reported more positive attitudes to science and greater science self-

efficacy. Else-Quest et al. (2013) has argued that mathematics attitudes and self-

confidence might be a stronger contributor to the underrepresentation of women in

STEM than actual aptitude, though attitudes and abilities appear to be strongly

intertwined. Nosek et al. (2009) also found that the strength of national implicit

associations of mathematics with masculinity predicted the magnitude of sex

differences in PISA test scores for mathematics and science achievement.

Collectively, these meta-analyses affirm the position held by Maccoby and

Jacklin (1974) that sex differences in quantitative reasoning are well established, but

that there may be a lack of consensus on the magnitude of the gender gap for modern

samples. Additionally, gender stereotypes associating mathematics and science with

masculinity and lower self-efficacy beliefs for females may be contributing factors.

2.2.4 Memory.

Evidence from a variety of studies suggest that there are observable sex

differences in memory (Halpern, 2011), but Herlitz, Nilsson and Bäckman (1997)

remark that the issue of sex differences in memory has been largely overlooked by

Page 82: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 67

researchers in narrative and literature reviews. Stumpf (1995) notes that research in this

area has been hampered by issues of reliability with instruments, representativeness of

samples, and that performance differs across modality (e.g., verbal memory, spatial

location, etc.). This has led to inconsistencies across studies, as typically only one type

of memory is tested in a given sample. Additionally there are methodological issues

with the level of difficulty of memory tasks. For example, recognition tasks (indicating

whether the target stimuli matches that seen previously) are significantly easier than

recall tasks ( provide the name, type or location without retrieval cues) (Geffen, Moar,

O'Hanlon, Clark, & Geffen, 1990). Additionally there exists considerable research on

sex differences in memory using animal models (Jonasson, 2005), but this is not

reviewed as research findings diverge considerably from patterns observed in humans.

In order to provide a comprehensive assessment of sex differences in the ability

to memorise material, Stumpf and Jackson (1994) examined memory performance

(across verbal and visual modalities) as part of a larger cognitive assessment battery

used for the screening of applicants for medical school in West Germany. Their sample

was large (approximately 97,000), and collected over a period of a decade. Women

performed significantly higher than men on a common memory factor, with an effect

size of d = -.56 (medium) across tasks.

Only a handful of other studies have tested memory across multiple modalities,

often to examine the effects of aging in clinical or non-clinical samples of older persons

(R. D. Hill et al., 1995). Unfortunately this limits the conclusions that can be drawn as

they cannot be generalised to younger samples, and similar concerns may be raised

about the representativeness of college-aged students from subject pools. But there is

one other study that has investigated sex differences in memory using a community

sample, and across multiple modalities. Herlitz et al. (1997) investigated differences in

Page 83: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 68

short-term memory using a range of traditional and novel measures. Subjects were

asked to memorise a series of 12-word lists while completing a distractor task (sorting

playing cards), as a measure of verbal memory. They were also tested on their ability to

recall newly acquired facts, by being asked to memorise a series of 20 fictitious

statements about celebrities (e.g., “Person X collects stamps as a hobby”), and asked to

later recall the statement associated with each person. Subjects also completed a variety

of cued recall and recognition tasks for objects, and were asked to remember the name

associated with a series of 16 colour photos of ten year old children as a measure of

visual memory. Subjects were also asked at the end of the session to recall and name the

activities they had participated in, testing episodic memory. Women scored significantly

higher across all short term memory tasks, with small to medium effect sizes. But there

was no sex difference reported on a control measure of general knowledge and

vocabulary.

Most research on sex differences in memory focus on a single modality for

tighter experimental control, with verbal learning memory being the most frequently

tested. Typically this involves learning lists of words with a distractor task or time

interval. For example Kramer, Delis and Daniel (1988) examined a community sample

of 136 men and women matched on age and educational level. Subjects completed the

California Verbal Learning Test, which requires subjects to memorise a list of 16 words

across five trials. The number of words recalled is the dependent variable, and then

subjects are given an interference list followed by a 20 minute delay. This allows for a

delayed recall measurement. Women performed significantly higher than men, with a

medium effect size across trials (from d = -.37 to d = -.50). A more detailed study by

Geffen et al. (1990) differentiated between recall and recognition verbal memory. They

recruited cross-sectional community sample of neurologically healthy adults aged 16-

Page 84: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 69

86, reporting separate analyses for immediate recall (d = -.57), twenty minute delayed

recall (d = -.48) and recognition (d = -1.0) tasks. These studies are representative of

verbal memory studies in methodology and findings, but to date a meta-analysis has not

been conducted across studies. It is unclear whether sex differences in memorising

verbal lists of words is the result of superior verbal and language abilities in females or

instead the result of sex differences in episodic memory (Herlitz & Rehnman, 2008).

Additionally, Lynn and Irwing (2008) reported a meta-analysis of performance on the

digit span tasks of the WISC/WAIS, which requires subjects to store sequences of digits

in memory and repeat them back to the examiner in the same order as they were

presented. The effect size is considerably smaller (d = -.13 for girls, d = -.12 for

women), but consistent with the pattern of higher female performance for verbal

memory tasks.

A number of studies have also investigated sex differences for visual memory.

Patterns of sex difference differ across tests. Males tend to score slightly higher on

visual-spatial working memory, with a meta-analysis reported by Voyer, Voyer and

Saint-Aubin (2017) finding a small sex difference of d = +.15 across studies. This might

well be a result of the general male advantage on visual-spatial tasks. However it is

equally possible that greater visual-spatial working memory affords an advantage when

attempting visual-spatial problems. However, studies employing other types of visual

memory tasks have found females score better than males, and these studies are often

overlooked in reviews that conflate spatial working memory with visual memory more

generally (e.g., A. C. Hill, Laird, & Robinson, 2014). For example girls and women

were more adept at recognising introduced faces and recalling their names (Larrabee &

Crook, 1993; West, Crook, & Barron, 1992). A meta-analysis by Herlitz and Lovén

(2013) found a small to medium effect size across studies, d = -.36 for learned faces.

Page 85: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 70

Contrary to gender stereotypes about navigation, Galea and Kimura (1993) found that

females are better able to memorise landmarks, learn routes on a map, and make fewer

errors than males, with medium-sized effects d = -.45.

Females are also more adept at remembering information presented visually.

Silverman and Eals (1992) developed the object identity memory task, which presents

subjects with an array of line drawings (such as common household objects, animals,

tools, etc.) that they were asked to memorise. After a delay subjects were presented with

a second page containing new and old items. Women were significantly better than men

at recognising items that did not match the original, and this finding has been

subsequently replicated with children and other adults. Another type of visual memory

task that shows greater female performance is memory of object locations. Crook,

Youngjohn and Larrabee (1990) first reported a sex difference for object locations after

designing the Misplaced Objects Test, which presents subjects with a list of 20

household objects located on an overhead map of a simulated house. After a delay of 40

minutes they are asked to recall the locations of the objects, with females scoring

significantly higher than males with a small to medium effect size d = -.36. This and a

similar study by Silverman and Eals sparked a wave of subsequent studies on visual

memory for objects. A subsequent meta-analysis by Voyer (2007) found these effects to

be robust, with a small female advantage for object identity d= -.23, and object location

tasks, d = -.27. Collectively these results suggest higher female performance for most

visual memory tasks, but that a small non-trivial male advantage exists for visual-spatial

working memory.

Some studies have also examined whether there are sex differences in memory

for identification of voices (sometimes termed earwitness recognition), but the findings

are inconsistent. While several studies found superior female performance, especially

Page 86: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 71

when hearing female voices (Roebuck & Wilding, 1993; Wilding & Cook, 2000), other

studies found no meaningful difference (Yarmey, Yarmey, Yarmey, & Parliament,

2001). Finally one study examined sex differences for odour recognition, finding

women were better at recognition of learned odours after study-test intervals of up to

21 days (Lehrner, 1993).

In summary, regardless of the modality chosen, women demonstrate superior

episodic memory abilities (Herlitz et al., 1997; Herlitz & Rehnman, 2008). The sole

exception to this general rule is for visual-spatial working memory (Voyer et al., 2017).

Such studies generally find around a small to medium effect, though as Stumpf noted

this is an under-researched area and there may be sex differences in other aspects of

memory that have yet to be examined.

Page 87: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 72 Table 2.1 –

Summary of Sex Differences in Specific Cognitive Abilities

Class Category Developmental trend Direction Magnitude of Effect

No difference Small Medium Large

Verbal

Ability

Reading Increases with age F > M

Writing Increases with age F > M

Phonological Coding/

Perceptual Speed

Unclear from literature F > M

Vocabulary Small until late adolescence, may

reach parity in adulthood

F > M (child)

No diff. (adult)

Verbal Fluency Increases with age F > M

Listening Comprehension No effect No diff.

Spelling Increases with age F > M

Grammar Increases with age F> M

Speech Production Increases with age F > M

d = 0 .10 .20 .30 .40 .50 .60 .70 .80

Page 88: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 73

Table 2.1 continued …..

Class Category Developmental trend Direction Magnitude of Effect

No difference Small Medium Large

Verbal

Ability

Second language Acquisition Unclear from literature F > M

Verbal Reasoning Unclear from literature M > F

Visual

Spatial

Mental Rotation Increases with age M > F

Spatial Perception Increases with age M > F

Spatial Visualization Increases with age M > F

Spaciotemporal Ability Unclear M > F

Mathematical Reasoning Increases with age M > F

d = 0 .10 .20 .30 .40 .50 .60 .70 .80

Page 89: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 74

Quantitative

reasoning

Scientific Reasoning Increases with age M > F

Memory Verbal Memory No age effects F > M

Visual Memory Unclear F > M

Object identity/location Increases with age F > M

Visual-Spatial WM No age effects M > F

Note: Literature review of visual-spatial reasoning is deferred until Chapter 3.

Page 90: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 75

2.2 Popular Beliefs about Intelligence

Intelligence is a socially desirable trait across most - if not all - cultures (Alicke,

1985; Shackelford, Schmitt, & Buss, 2005), and most lay persons have intuitive beliefs

about what constitutes “intelligence” even when they lack to ability to formally define

it. Ordinary laypersons may operate with somewhat different perceptions of what

constitutes intelligence than are held by intelligence researchers. Furthermore there are

commonly held beliefs (“folk wisdom”) about sex differences in intelligence which hold

that males and females hold different intellectual capabilities (Halpern, Straight, &

Stephenson, 2011), that they have different “learning styles” and ought to be taught in

separate classes (Halpern, Eliot, et al., 2011), or that hormones affect cognition to such

a degree that one gender or another might be unsuitable for certain occupational tasks

(Halpern, 1997). Indeed, I have personal experience of such folk wisdom. After having

one of my earlier papers on sex differences in visual-spatial ability accepted for

publication in a moderately prestigious feminist psychology journal, I learned that the

publisher had (independently of the journal) prepared a press release highlighting that

study as scientifically confirming the folk wisdom that women cannot drive or navigate

as well as men. Needless to say this was not a desirable outcome and the press release

was cancelled, but it does serve to illustrate the readiness in the community to confirm

commonly held stereotypes about sex differences in intellectual functioning.

The beliefs of laypersons on the intersection of sex and intelligence are

important to consider for several reasons. Firstly, they demonstrate in our culture a

readiness to anticipate and confirm that there are inherent differences between the sexes

in intellectual capabilities (even when there are not). Secondly, sex stereotypes held by

parents, teachers, and the wider society may become self-fulfilling prophecies

(Rosenthal & Jacobson, 1968), in that they subtly influence the way that children are

Page 91: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 76

treated. Parental and teacher expectations are important socializers of sex stereotypes,

exerting an effect at home and in the educational system (Hyde & Lindberg, 2007).

Thirdly negative sex stereotypes may be internalised by individual boys and girls which

leads to gendered differences in self-concept, self-efficacy, interest in scholastic

subjects and motivation to achieve (Parker, Van Zanden, & Parker, 2018).

Halpern (1997, p. 1091) has claimed that the question of sex differences in

intelligence “is among the most politically volatile topics in contemporary psychology”.

This argument was made because of the polarising effect the topic can have on

laypeople, and the implications that it can hold for the way in which boys and girls are

educated. Hare-Mustin and Marecek (1988) conceptualised these two divergent

positions as alpha bias (assuming large and immutable sex differences, or the “Men are

from Mars, Women are from Venus” view popularised by Gray) and beta bias

(assuming minimal or non-existent differences between males and females, such as the

position held by Hyde and colleagues). The two perspectives are also worth considering

when critically evaluating research in this area, as the ideological position of individual

researchers may be relevant in cases where a highly selective review of the literature is

presented that omits key evidence (e.g., Lippa, 2006 in critiquing Hyde (2005)).

2.1.1 Self-Estimation of Intelligence.

The way in which we see ourselves can profoundly impact our educational

aspirations, as well as our academic self-esteem and self-efficacy. So an important

theoretical question is just how accurate are our self-evaluations of intelligence? Kruger

and Dunning (1999) espoused the rather pessimistic view that people generally hold

overly optimistic views of their own intellectual functioning, which has been termed the

“above-average effect” (Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak, & Vredenburg, 1995;

Kruger, 1999), also is sometimes referred to as the “Lake Wobegon” effect after the

Page 92: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 77

fictional American town. Essentially this is the tendency of the typical person to believe

she or he is somewhat above average in intelligence (which for a normally distributed

psychological characteristic defies statistics). Upholding a self-concept that is more

positive than objective reality may actually be adaptive and serve a number of

psychological functions (e.g., esteem maintenance) and translate to improved self-

efficacy (Alicke, 1985; Greenwald, 1980). While the above-average effect has been

consistently replicated, it is also important to note that not every person exhibits

unrealistically high estimates. Under some conditions a related “below-average-effect”

can be found, particularly when social comparisons are made to groups involving

positive stereotypes (Kruger, 1999), such as stereotypes about racial grouping or

gender.

Despite the cognitive bias of the above-average effect, when asked to provide an

estimate of their own intelligence relative to others, males consistently report higher

estimates than do females. This effect was first reported by Hogan (1978) who found

that females provided lower estimates of their IQ score than males in an American

sample, and was subsequently replicated with a British sample (Beloff, 1992). Although

actual psychometric intelligence was not measured in these studies, given that actual IQ

does not differ between the sexes in the general population these seemed surprising

findings with no definitive cause. The researchers speculated that they reflected either

cultural sex stereotypes, or a strategy of self-minimisation on the part of females for

social desirability reasons and boastfulness for males.

These studies sparked considerable research into sex differences in self-

estimated intelligence (SEI), defined as people’s estimates of their own intellectual

abilities relative to the general population. This is typically assessed by presenting

participants with a histogram bell curve with appropriate text anchors, and instructions

Page 93: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 78

explaining the bell-curve distribution of intelligence in the general population. The

problem has been termed by Furnham et al. (2001) as the male-hubris female humility

problem (MHFH), and has been replicated cross-culturally across dozens of

nationalities. While initial studies focused exclusively on overall IQ, research has been

extended to cover more specific cognitive abilities. In a meta-analytic review,

Syzmanowicz and Furnham (2011) found robust sex differences for general intelligence

(d = .37), mathematical intelligence (d = .44), visual-spatial intelligence (d = .43).

Somewhat surprisingly there was also a marginal but significant effect for estimates of

verbal intelligence (d = +.07), which runs contrary to the general research finding of

greater verbal abilities in females. While the effect itself seems robust, sex differences

in SEI are not easily explained and may reflect a combination of social motives

(boastful hubris on the part of males, humility on the part of females), courtship

strategy, sex stereotypes about intellectual ability, or the pattern of lower general self-

esteem in females (Gentile et al., 2009; Kling, Hyde, Showers, & Buswell, 1999). The

latter explanation remains as yet untested, but is investigated in Chapter 10.

2.1.2 Estimation of other’s intelligence.

If social motives or courtship strategy were the sole mechanisms involved in

SEI, then presumably when asked to provide an objective estimate of other people’s

intelligence the hubris/humility effect should not be present. In the original study by

Hogan (1978), participants were also asked to provide an estimate of the intelligence of

their mother and their father. Fathers were rated as more intelligent than mothers, even

though there are no sex differences in general intelligence present in the community.

The effect has been replicated numerous times (Beloff, 1992; Furnham & Rawles,

1995), but such an effect should be interpreted cautiously. It could be argued that the

effect actually be a reflection of systemic educational and occupational inequalities of

Page 94: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 79

the time (i.e., higher male educational advancement) rather than genuine beliefs that

men are inherently “smarter”.

Furnham and Gasson (1998) took a different approach, and instead asked

parents to provide an estimation of the intelligence of their own children. Parental

beliefs may be a particularly important mechanism in the socialisation of sex

stereotypes, as parental educational expectations may influence a child’s own view of

their capabilities (Jodl, Michael, Malanchuk, Eccles, & Sameroff, 2001). Sons were

rated as more intelligent than daughters (d = .67), and this effect has also been

replicated several times (Furnham, 2000; Furnham, Reeves, & Budhani, 2002). Thus

such a pattern might better fit with cultural beliefs about gender stereotypes.

2.1.3 Popular beliefs about sex differences in specific cognitive abilities.

As mentioned above, one apparent explanation for sex differences in SEI might

be the effect of gender stereotypes (e.g., males are inherently “smarter”, and hence their

abilities are over-estimated). Surprisingly few studies have actually sought to test this

hypothesis, and determine whether laypersons are accurate judges of psychological sex

differences. Swim (1994) found across a range of behavioural characteristics,

laypersons were consistent in the direction of sex differences but tended to

underestimate their magnitude (beta bias). However Swim did not include any specific

questions on intelligence differences, rather focusing on a subset of cognitive abilities.

More recently Halpern, Straight and Stephenson (2011) specifically asked for

estimations about sex differences in cognitive abilities to determine their accuracy. They

asked participants to estimate cognitive sex differences across a range of 12 tasks, and

then compared the estimates provided to actual sex difference studies. Their results

showed that participants were generally accurate about the direction of sex differences

for specific cognitive abilities, but underestimated their magnitude. Crucially though,

Page 95: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 80

their study did not enquire about global differences in intelligence (IQ). If there are

gender stereotypes about overall intelligence, it would appear that these are implicit

biases (expressed in SEI or about other individuals) rather than explicitly held beliefs

about a smarter sex (i.e., males/females are generally smarter).

One notable exception to beliefs about sex differences in intelligence appears to

be the ability of cognitive multitasking (defined as the ability to execute two or more

cognitive tasks concurrently, or using task-switching). Szameitat et al. (2015) reported

the results of large study recruiting from the UK, USA, Germany, Netherlands and

Turkey, finding that over half of study participants expressed a belief in sex differences

in multitasking. Of those participants, 80% believed that women were simply better

multi-taskers, despite a lack of research studies to support such a belief (Szameitat et al.,

2015). Thus not all lay gender stereotypes reflect negatively on women, and some may

suggest women have a modest advantage in some skills even if not empirically

supported.

2.3 Theoretical perspectives on sex differences in cognitive abilities

While sex differences in cognitive ability have been studied since the beginning

of psychometric assessment of intelligence (for a review see Shields, 1982), theoretical

perspectives on their origins have shifted considerably over this time period. Early

theoretical debate on sex differences proposed strong and immutable biological factors

(nature), while later theorists argued that early differences in socialisation experiences

of boys and girls and environmental factors (such as parental and teacher expectations,

gender stereotypes, etc.) might better explain differences in cognitive ability (nurture).

More recently, the limitations of both the nature and nurture perspective in isolation

have been recognised (Halpern & Tan, 2001; Priess & Hyde, 2010). Increasingly, sex

difference researchers acknowledge the need for a more comprehensive theoretical

Page 96: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 81

framework that encompasses nature, nurture, which Halpern and Tan (2001) has termed

a psychobiosocial model of sex differences. Less seldom acknowledged by researchers

are macro-level sociocultural factors contributing to male-female group differences.

These include cultural beliefs and practices (such as gender stereotyping of intellectual

domains like language and STEM), as well as larger cultural issues such as general

attitudes towards women, gender equality, and the stratification of educational,

occupational and political representation along gendered lines (termed the gender

segregation hypothesis). These macro-level factors may help to explain some of the

cross-cultural variability in the magnitude of sex differences in cognitive ability.

2.3.1 Biological Explanations for Sex Differences

Men and women differ in a number of important characteristics beyond their

reproductive capabilities, including height, physical strength, and lifespan. Any

explanation for sex differences in cognition must rightly consider whether these make a

contribution. But in doing so, one must also evaluate such claims with a stronger degree

of intellectual rigour than was present in earlier centuries. Take for example the

observation that due to their natural height advantage, males on average have slightly

larger brains than females - but recall that also men and women do not differ

significantly on tests of general intelligence (psychometrically measured IQ). The

assumption had been made by early anatomists that larger brains would confer some

degree of intellectual advantage (for an historical account see Shields, 1975), and was

mounted as a scientific argument for the intellectual infirmity of women. Indeed, there

are some current researchers (i.e. Lynn, 1994, 2016) who continue to make this

argument (intelligence is correlated with brain size; males have larger brains; males

must therefore be smarter and IQ tests showing no difference are wrong).

Page 97: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 82

Advances in scientific methodology, such as neuroimaging, have meant that

biological arguments have been held to greater scrutiny, and some once ‘well

established’ biological claims have been found to stand on shaky foundations. For

example, support for putative sex differences in brain structure is now highly disputed

(for a discussion see Fine, 2010b), and limited to a small number of brain regions

including the hypothalamus, amygdala, and corpus callosum (Hines, 2010), as well as

proportionately greater development of Broca’s and Wernicke’s area in females which

are involved in speech production and language (Harasty, Double, Halliday, Kril, &

McRitchie, 1997; Robichon, Giraud, Berbon, & Habib, 1999). Other claims have held

up better, such as sex differences in lateralization of brain function for language (Levy,

1969; B. A. Shaywitz et al., 1995). Males who are right-handed tend to exhibit greater

activity in the left hemisphere when performing language processing tasks, whereas

females show a greater likelihood for bilateral pattern of brain activation (reviewed in

Kansaku & Kitazawa, 2001). But even these findings are disputed (Wallentin, 2009),

due to inconsistencies across studies (Sommer, Aleman, Bouma, & Kahn, 2004).

Although there has been a shift in the literature away from biologically-based

arguments for sex differences in cognition, there remain several areas which maintain

strong scientific support. These are reviewed herein, but the interested reader is

cautioned that this list is not exhaustive. It is acknowledged that there are other

theoretical perspectives (such as neuroanatomical differences in brain structure, or

lateralization of brain function) that have been proposed, and are contentious. Fine

(2010a, 2010b) summarises the debate on methodological limitations of neuroimaging

studies, including extremely small sample sizes, confounding variables and failure to

replicate findings. Attention has been given to those topics where there is some degree

of scientific consensus. Three sources of evidence are outlined: effects of sex hormones

Page 98: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 83

during early development, activational effects of sex hormones, and finally biological

differences between males and females predicted by evolutionary psychology.

2.3.1.1 Sex hormones during early foetal development.

One of the most prominent explanations for sex differences in cognitive

performance has been the effect of sex hormones (Kimura, 2000). While males and

females produce both classes of sex hormones to some extent, typically there is greater

androgen production in males and greater estrogen and progesterone production in

females. This difference starts early, with observable differences in testosterone

concentration of foetuses found as early as 8 weeks gestation (Hines, 2010). Production

of androgens in males remains high between 8 and 24 weeks, then reduces shortly

before birth (Hines, 2010). During this developmental window sex hormones contribute

to the organisation and development of the brain, and shape gender development

(Berenbaum & Beltz, 2011, 2016).

While sex-specific differences in hormone production outlined above are

present, during some pregnancies the developing foetus will receive additional exposure

to sex hormones. This exposure can affect both sexes (Auyeung et al., 2009) and may

lead to enduring changes in brain development and sex-typed behaviour (Berenbaum &

Beltz, 2016; Hines, 2015). For example, girls diagnosed with the condition congenital

adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) are exposed to high levels of androgens prenatally, and

exhibit a stronger preference for playing with boys’ toys than female relatives

(Berenbaum & Hines, 1992; Hines, 2006). Such girls also perform better than their

same-sex peers on tasks of spatial ability later in life (Puts, McDaniel, Jordan, &

Breedlove, 2008). Longitudinal studies have also found that levels of foetal

testosterone assayed from amniotic fluid is also associated with similar male-typical

patterns of play behaviour and interests in boys and girls (Auyeung et al., 2009).

Page 99: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 84

One indicator of early prenatal androgen exposure is the ratio between the length

of the second (index) and fourth fingers (2D:4D) in adults. Some studies have found

associations between lower 2D:4D ratios and performance on visual-spatial tasks for

both sexes (Collaer, Reimers, & Manning, 2007; Falter, Arroyo, & Davis, 2006; Peters,

Manning, & Reimers, 2007), but other studies have failed to find an association (Puts et

al., 2008; Voracek, Pietschnig, Nader, & Stieger, 2011). The inconsistent evidence may

be due to measurement issues associated with digit ratios, or that digit ratios are only a

crude proxy for actual levels of androgen exposure (Miller & Halpern, 2013). When

combined with the stronger evidence found in amniotic fluid assays and CAH studies

(Auyeung et al., 2009; Hines, 2015), there appears to be some effect of sex hormones on

the developing brain, even if it is only subtle.

The exact mechanism responsible for influencing cognition though is unclear –

while prenatal exposure to sex hormones might have a direct effect on cognition, since

such children also show increased male-typical behaviour and interests, they may be

self-selecting situations that promote spatial development. Because spatial ability

requires environmental input and practice for development (Baenninger & Newcombe,

1989), childrens’ toys and play can be important sources of spatial experiences and

training. Many stereotypically masculine activities (such as construction blocks and

models) promote spatial development (Doyle, Voyer, & Cherney, 2012), and in this

way the developmental effects of sex hormones may be acting indirectly.

2.3.1.2 Activational effects of sex hormones.

While sex hormones contribute to brain development, they also exert an

activational role on human cognition and behaviour. In normal development, the

production of sex hormones increases considerably with puberty, and continues long

into adulthood. This developmental milestone coincides with an intensification of

Page 100: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 85

gendered interests and behaviours (Halim & Ruble, 2010), as well as a widening of the

gender gap for many types of cognitive ability (Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990; Voyer

et al., 1995), leading to the intuitive appeal of sex hormones as an explanation.

However, research findings examining the association between endogenous sex

hormone levels and actual cognitive tasks in men and women have produced only

mixed support for such claims. Some studies have found little to no association (Halari

et al., 2005; Puts et al., 2010), while other studies have found testosterone levels are

correlated with visual-spatial performance and estrogen levels to performance on verbal

tasks (Griksiene & Ruksenas, 2011; Hausmann, Slabbekoorn, Van Goozen, Cohen-

Kettenis, & Gunturkun, 2000; Kimura, 1996). This suggests that the activational effects

of sex hormones may indeed be quite subtle, and dependent on context. Some theorists

have speculated that the effect of hormones may be mediated by other factors such as

the expression of sex-roles (Smith, Deady, Sharp, & Al-Dujaili, 2013) and gender

stereotypes (Hausmann, Schoofs, Rosenthal, & Jordan, 2009). Alternatively, the

activational effects of sex hormones may be dwarfed by the earlier influences of

hormones on brain development.

Further evidence for a contribution of sex hormones on cognition comes from

samples that, due either to natural aging or specific medical conditions, are somewhat

lower in natural hormone production. Production of testosterone decreases in men due

to natural aging, but when given testosterone therapy they show improved performance

on tests of spatial ability (Janowsky, Oviatt, & Orwoll, 1994). Women receiving

hormone replacement therapy for the treatment of menopause show an improvement in

their verbal memory (Maki, 2001) and verbal fluency (Maki, Rich, & Shayna

Rosenbaum, 2002), but it is also associated with a decline in spatial ability (Drake et al.,

2000).

Page 101: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 86

2.3.1.3 Evolutionary psychological arguments of innate differences.

Another purported biological contribution to the emergence of sex differences in

cognitive ability at the population level comes from the field of evolutionary

psychology. Darwin (1871) first proposed that sexual selection as a result of

evolutionary pressures led to a differentiation in the roles of men and women. In our

distant hunter-gatherer past, men would be required to travel long distances to track and

hunt animals, a task requiring strong spatial perception and navigation skills (Silverman

& Eals, 1992). Those males who lacked an aptitude in spatial ability would be unable to

accrue sufficient resources to thrive in traditional hunter-gatherer societies, and thus be

less likely to propagate their genes to future generations (Buss, 1995). In contrast,

women fulfilled the role of the gatherer of local food and assumed childrearing duties.

Since these roles have less need for spatial proficiency but emphasize other adaptive

traits such as nurturing and fine-motor skills (Halpern, 2000), over successive

generations evolutionary forces may have developed sex-specific proficiencies. This

argument has been expanded upon by evolutionary psychologists (Archer, 1996). Given

that spatial ability is believed to lay down a foundation for quantitative reasoning,

Geary (1996) proposed that evolutionary pressures led to group differences between

males and females in spatial and mathematical reasoning. A limitation of this argument

is that it is a post-hoc explanation for observed group differences, formulated to explain

existing behaviour (Cornell, 1997). Furthermore, evolutionary psychology has yet to lay

down a similarly compelling argument as to why sex differences in language and verbal

abilities might emerge, especially for relatively “recent” traits (at least on an

evolutionary time-scale) such as reading and writing proficiency.

Page 102: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 87

2.3.2 Psychosocial Explanations for Sex Differences

The prominence of biological contributions to sex differences has declined in

recent years, with an increasing recognition by researchers of the role of socialisation

factors on psychological sex differences in behaviour and cognition (Leaper &

Friedman, 2007). While there are indeed many similarities and shared experiences, from

infancy onwards parents and caregivers treat boys and girls differently in sometimes

subtle, and sometimes quite overt ways that contribute to a child’s gender development

(Bussey & Bandura, 1999). A large number of psychosocial explanations have been

proposed for observed sex differences in cognitive ability, including behavioural

conditioning and modelling (social cognitive theory), cognitive perspectives such as

gender schema theory (Bem, 1981b), gender stereotyping about the abilities of males

and females, and so on. While an exhaustive list is beyond the scope of this review, the

most prominent and recurring themes will be highlighted. Most are complementary

rather than contradictory in that they acknowledge the contributions of other

perspectives (Leaper & Friedman, 2007), but place greater emphasis on particular

processes (C. L. Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002). All share a commonality in that

they are proposed to arise from the different socialisation experiences of boys and girls.

2.3.2.1 Differential socialisation experiences.

One of the most frequently encountered responses parents receive when

announcing a pregnancy is to be asked “Is it a boy, or a girl?”. While either of these

options is a cause for celebration, it does serve to illustrate just how salient the attribute

of gender is in our society. Parents and caregivers treat boys and girls differently in

sometimes subtle, and sometimes overt ways that contribute to a child’s gender

development (Bussey & Bandura, 1999; Siegal, 1987). For example, in an extensive

meta-analysis of studies involving observations of parental interaction with their

Page 103: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 88

children, Leaper, Anderson and Sanders (1998) found that mothers tend to be more

talkative with daughters than sons, and use more supportive speech. Parents also tend to

discuss emotional experiences with their daughters more than their sons, as well as

using a wider and more nuanced range of emotional vocabulary (Adams, Kuebli, Boyle,

& Fivush, 1995; Fivush, 2014; Flannagan & Perese, 1998). These varied experiences

may provide girls and boys differential opportunities to practice and develop language

proficiency (Clearfield & Nelson, 2006), as well as contributing to sex differences in

emotional sensitivity and expression (Brody & Hall, 2000; Chaplin, Cole, & Zahn-

Waxler, 2005). While boys and girls typically receive the same literacy instruction upon

entering formal schooling, early childhood experiences that encourage language and

scaffold literacy acquisition can be beneficial (Neumann, Hood, & Neumann, 2009;

Neumann & Neumann, 2010). Some studies have found parental differences in teaching

experiences with sons and daughters (Flannagan & Baker-Ward, 1996; Flannagan &

Perese, 1998), but others find no child-gender effect (Tenenbaum & Leaper, 1998), or

that child-gender effects may vary between mothers and fathers (Leaper, 2002). Further

research is required to determine the extent of child-gender effects in parent-child

conversations and teaching experiences, but there is tentative evidence to suggest some

socialisation differences between boys and girls.

To investigate the proposition that daughters and sons are treated differently by

parents, Lytton and Romney (1991) conducted a meta-analytic review of studies

observing parent-child interactions across a number of socialization areas. Somewhat

surprisingly, they found only weak evidence of differential socialization practices

between boys and girls in many areas. It should be acknowledged, however, that their

meta-analysis was limited to the empirical studies available at the time (for a review of

more recent studies, see Leaper & Friedman, 2007). No doubt there are many

Page 104: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 89

similarities and shared experiences of girls and boys, but there are also fundamental

differences that sustain the gender socialization process. One area in which this is

particularly apparent is in the encouragement (or discouragement) of sex-typed play and

activities. Lytton and Romney found that both mothers (d = .34) and fathers (d = .49)

strongly encouraged sex-typed activities and play in their children along traditional

gender norms. But children also actively contribute to this process as well. Children as

young as three years of age develop a preference for playing with children of the same-

sex (Maccoby, 1998; C. L. Martin & Ruble, 2004). Gender-atypical patterns of play

may be highly discouraged or even a source of ostracism (Egan & Perry, 2001; Young

& Sweeting, 2004). Recall also that children often express a preference for sex-typed

toys and play activities that is partially biologically driven (Alexander & Hines, 2002;

Hines & Alexander, 2008). Children may also self-select those that appear more

enjoyable or hold greater interest (particularly with increasing age and greater

autonomy) These choices may in turn be reinforced (or discouraged) by parents (Idle,

Wood, & Desmarais, 1993), and so the process may represent an iterative process

guided by a variety of biological and socialisation factors.

Piaget (1951, 1968) argued that play provides children with opportunities to

practice socially valued skills that contribute to their social and cognitive development,

especially when playing with parents and their peers. The nature of play has important

implications for cognitive development (for a review see Liben, Schroeder, Boriello, &

Weisgram, 2018).These early experiences also communicate messages to children about

sex-typed behaviour, and gender stereotypes. Two proposed mechanisms have been

suggested to contribute to greater sex differences in cognitive ability. Firstly, a number

of researchers had found associations between stereotypically masculine play and toys

and the development of spatial ability (Connor & Serbin, 1977; Sherman, 1967), which

Page 105: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 90

has been suggested contributes to proficiency in quantitative reasoning. Baenninger and

Newcombe (1995) showed that environmental input and practice is required to fully

develop spatial ability, and a number of researchers have shown that gender

stereotyping of activity and leisure interests predicts future spatial performance

(Cherney & Voyer, 2010; Doyle et al., 2012; Signorella, Jamison, & Krupa, 1989).

Differential opportunities to practice and develop their visual-spatial reasoning is

therefore one mechanism by which the pronounced sex differences in visual-spatial

abilities might emerge.

Secondly, differential socialization practices serve to reinforce stereotypes about

the roles of men and women and divisions of labour in society (a point elaborated in

Section 2.3.3.1 “Social Role Theory”). Cognitive skills that are seen as less relevant and

less desirable for one sex may be less practiced, and hold less interest/motivation

particularly in the face of difficulties (such as that experienced when reading for the first

time, or solving difficult mathematics problems). For example, reading is

stereotypically regarded as being feminine (Dwyer, 1974; McGeown, Goodwin,

Henderson, & Wright, 2011; Steffens & Jelenec, 2011), and boys show reduced interest

in reading than girls (L. Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Marinak & Gambrell, 2010; Millard,

1997). Conversely, mathematics and science are stereotypically associated with males

and masculinity (Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Steffens & Jelenec, 2011), and

girls rate their mathematics and science ability considerably lower than boys (Eccles,

Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993; Reilly et al., 2017). Thus there may be multiple

pathways whereby cultural gender stereotypes and socialization differences between

boys and girls contribute to the emergence of sex differences in cognitive ability.

Page 106: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 91

2.3.2.2 Cognitive-developmental model of sex-typing.

Kohlberg (1966) proposed a cognitive-developmental model of gender,

emphasising that it was an active intellectual process rather than merely passive learning

through behavioural reinforcement and punishment. Kohlberg was the first to move

beyond observation of child behaviour towards their use of language, and to use

interview methods to study childrens’ thoughts and beliefs about gender. Children

quickly acquire their concepts about gender through a process known as sex-typing

(Kagan, 1964; Kohlberg, 1966), the beginnings of which can be seen in the first two

years of life (Kohlberg & Ullian, 1974; Perry, White, & Perry, 1984) and increases

steadily with age. By age 3, most children can label themselves as a boy or a girl, and

can label the gender of others with partial accuracy (Kohlberg & Ullian, 1974). Children

develop an internal mental model of cultural prescriptions of maleness and femaleness

which are termed sex-roles (for the roles and behaviours which were stereotypically

associated with masculinity and femininity, but these expand with age to also include

sex-specific self-concepts, personality traits, interests and cultural/intellectual pursuits).

Kohlberg and Ullian (1974) argued that gender takes on a “tremendous importance in

organizing the childs’ social perceptions and actions” (p. 210)…. in essence, that gender

takes on a centrality in a child’s perceptions of self and others, which Bem (1993)

termed the “lenses of gender” for the way in which it colours our perspectives.

Kohlberg and Ullian noted that between ages 6 and 7, most children attain

gender constancy (the belief that gender is fixed, and a boy cannot become a girl or vice

versa), and begin to develop more sophisticated sex-role concepts about the sex-typing

of toys, clothing, household objects and even occupations. Serbin, Powlishta, Gulko,

Martin and Lockheed (1993) report that even by age 5 children attain an understanding

of sex-typed personality traits, and can identify basic traits (such as “emotional”, or

“strong”) that are stereotypically feminine and masculine. This finding has been

Page 107: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 92

reproduced cross-culturally, and by age 8 most children show adult-equivalent

performance on labelling tasks (Best, 1982; Best et al., 1977). Beliefs in inherent

differences between males and females can be found across most cultures and time

periods (Kite, Deaux, & Haines, 2008; Williams & Best, 1990), though the strength of

these stereotypes does vary.

Kohlberg’s cognitive developmental model of sex-typing has held up well given

the passage of time, and has laid down the foundations for other theoretical perspectives

such as gender schema theory, and the role of gender stereotyping of activities and

interests. It documented the process of sex-typing that other theories would incorporate,

but makes no specific predictions about sex differences in cognitive abilities.

2.3.2.3 Gender Schema Theory and sex-role identification

An extension of Kohlberg’s cognitive-developmental model of gender was

advanced by Bem (1981a), who proposed gender schema theory to account for the sex-

typing process. The term cognitive schema denotes a cognitive structure or pattern of

thought that helps organize and process categories of information and the relationships

between them (Neisser, 1976). A schema helps guide our perceptions of the world, and

helps draw our attention to information that is schema-relevant. Initially, children start

out with a fairly primitive gender schema, noticing obvious physical differences such as

height, clothing, and hair. The gender schema is later refined with more nuanced

properties associated with gender such as behaviour, personality traits, and interests, in

the developmental progression described by Kohlberg and later researchers (Halim &

Ruble, 2010; Kohlberg & Ullian, 1974; Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, 2006). The

gender schema then becomes a prescriptive standard to uphold, and motivates the

individual to regulate his or her behaviour to conform to cultural definitions of

masculinity or femininity (Bem, 1981a).

Page 108: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 93

While an understanding of sex-role concepts is attained in early to middle

childhood, there is wide variability in the degree to which individuals integrate aspects

of femininity and masculinity into their personality. Earlier personality theorists had

believed that masculinity and femininity represented a single personality trait (MF) that

existed along a continuum as bipolar opposites (i.e., being high in one meant being low

in the other). But in a landmark psychometric review, Constantinople (1973) presented

a growing evidence base that they were not unidimensional and instead represented two

distinct personality clusters (masculine and feminine), which Bakan (1966) labelled as

agency and communality respectively. Men are thought to be agentic – assertive,

competitive, achievement-driven, and dominant. By way of contrast, women are seen as

more communal and expressive – friendly, cooperative, concerned with the wellbeing of

others, and emotionally/verbally expressive. But sex-role identification does not always

fall neatly into two binary categories of male and female. Some children identify with

stereotypically masculine sex-roles (instrumental or agentic) while others identify more

strongly with stereotypically feminine (expressive or communal) sex-roles. Still others

acquire a combined blend of both masculinity and femininity into their self-concept,

termed psychological androgyny (Bem, 1974; Spence & Helmreich, 1979). Less

commonly, a small percentage of the population identify with neither masculine or

feminine roles.

Sex-roles act as a self-regulatory mechanism: highly sex-typed children and

adults are motivated to keep their behaviour and self-concepts consistent with the

traditional gender norms of their biological sex (Maccoby, 1990; C. L. Martin & Ruble,

2004), which Egan and Perry (2001) have termed felt pressure. Such individuals are

limited in their behavioural repertoire and will self-select activities and interests that

conform to traditional gender norms. Others whose gender schema is more “fuzzy” and

Page 109: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 94

less prescriptively defined may see masculine and feminine sex-roles as

complementary, and incorporate both into their self-concept (Bem, 1983, 1984).

Although felt pressure is usually considered in the context of sex-typed social

behaviour, it also applies to performance in sex-typed academic domains such as

language and mathematics/science (McGeown et al., 2011; Nosek et al., 2002; Nosek et

al., 2009; Oswald, 2008; Steffens & Jelenec, 2011). A number of theorists (e.g., Nash,

1979; Sherman, 1967) have proposed that sex-role identification might be an important

mediator of sex-differences in cognitive ability, but an elaboration on these issues and

specific mechanisms is deferred until Section 2.3.4.

2.3.2.4 Social Cognitive Theory.

Bussey and Bandura (1999) proposed social cognitive theory as an explanation

for sex differences. Social cognitive theory argues that childrens’ behaviours are shaped

by reinforcements and punishments (coming initially from parents and caregivers, then

transitioning to peers and other adults) as well as through the powerful influence of

imitation and modelling of the behaviour of others (Bussey & Bandura, 1984). For most

children, these learning experiences become internalised and form a core part of a

child’s identity which both guides and restricts future behaviours, including gendered

behaviour. This gendered behaviour can be expressed in various ways, such as leisure

activities and interests, and provides the opportunity to practise and refine socially

useful skills (such as language competence, or refining spatial ability). In this context,

modelling may play a particularly important role, as young children develop aspirations

to achieve a certain profession. Parents and teachers may also subtly transfer cultural

stereotypes, particularly when certain areas of study are stereotypically perceived as

masculine (like mathematics and science) or feminine (reading, languages, and social

sciences).

Page 110: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 95

2.3.2.6 Sex and gender stereotypes about cognitive ability.

Another theoretical perspective that has been proposed as contributing to sex

differences in cognitive ability is that of sex stereotypes (now more commonly referred

to as gender stereotypes, a practice adopted hereon). Beliefs that males and females

possess different attributes and behaviours extends across all age groups, time periods

and cultures (Kite et al., 2008), though the strength of such beliefs does vary across

cultures and individuals. Gender schema theory cautioned that cultural prescriptions of

masculinity and femininity can become self-fulfilling prophecies, a view shared by

other researchers.

As outlined earlier, gender stereotypes are learned early and are highly resistant

to change. Collectively, parents and teachers hold different educational expectations for

boys and girls (Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990; Frome & Eccles, 1998; Jussim &

Harber, 2005), especially for mathematics and science. Even in the absence of the overt

endorsement of gender stereotypes, implicit stereotypes associating

mathematics/science with masculinity and arts and language with femininity are strong

(Nosek et al., 2002). Consequentially young girls in elementary school report lower

self-efficacy and competence beliefs in mathematics than boys (Eccles et al., 1993),

even though sex differences on standardised tests of mathematics achievement show

minimal sex differences in this age group (Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990). Girls also

report lower self-efficacy beliefs about science, and this pattern is found cross-culturally

in adolescents(Reilly et al., 2017). Boys also report lower self-efficacy and competence

in reading and language domains (L. Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Eccles et al., 1989;

Eccles et al., 1993), although sex differences in actual reading ability can be quite large.

This makes it difficult to establish whether lower self-efficacy and interest for reading

are the product of negative gender stereotypes or a reflection of accurate self-appraisal.

Page 111: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 96

Gender stereotypes are important to consider for several reasons. Firstly,

reduced self-efficacy beliefs can be associated with avoidance behaviour and learned

helplessness. If one believes that one’s ability is fixed and associated with factors

outside of one’s control, motivation to improve will consequentially be lower. For

example, when children are asked to complete a novel task and are told that the task is

better suited for one gender or the other, those in the stereotyped group are less likely to

persist when encountering difficulty and generally show impaired performance

(Cimpian, Mu, & Erickson, 2012).

The two most prominent gender stereotypes are the association of reading and

language proficiency as being feminine, and the association of mathematics and science

as being inherently masculine. This can be regarded as a distal factor contributing to sex

differences in cognitive ability, in that these gender stereotypes are learned early and

shape how an individual approaches intellectual domains over the course of a lifetime.

Secondly, there is evidence to suggest that even when one is quite proficient at a

cognitive task, knowledge of group differences in ability can undermine cognitive

performance through the mechanism of stereotype threat (Steele, 1997, 1998),

especially for high-stakes standardised tests. As such, stereotype threat represents a

more proximal factor contributing to sex differences in cognitive ability. It also raises

the uncomfortable possibility that some (as yet unmeasurable) component of sex

differences in cognitive ability may be an artefact of the testing environment and not an

accurate reflection of actual ability. By way of example, a consistent finding in the

literature is that girls and women achieve higher grades in most academic subjects,

including mathematics and science subjects (Alon & Gelbgiser, 2011; Voyer & Voyer,

2014). Yet males score higher than females on standardised tests of mathematics and

science (Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2015), suggesting that

Page 112: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 97

there is a gap between performance in testing situations and achievement overall

(Halpern, 2011). Any comprehensive psychobiosocial model of sex differences must

consider the role of gender stereotypes (both distal and proximal) in the emergence of

sex differences in cognitive ability at the population level (Halpern & Lamay, 2000).

An important example of the effect of self-fulfilling prophecies in an educational

context comes from the classic educational study by Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968).

The researchers had children complete a purported screening test for intellectual ability

in first grade, and identified to teachers a subset of children as intellectually gifted for

their age. In actual fact the basis for selection was not on their psychometrically

measured IQ - the children identified were randomly selected. But those designated as

‘gifted’ showed a greater increase in psychometrically measured IQ (approximately 8

IQ points) over the course of a year than the control students, highlighting that teacher

and parental expectations can exert a powerful influence on intellectual functioning.

Now, imagine a similar hypothetical experiment where half the participants are assigned

to a social category that identified them as mathematically gifted but poor in reading

and language proficiency. Even if there were no initial differences in initial starting

ability, over the course of their schooling gender stereotypes might well become self-

fulfilling prophecies, shaping how children see themselves in relation to intellectual

domains like reading and STEM.

2.3.3 Macro-level Cultural Contributions

Most psychobiosocial models of sex differences consider the role of biological

and social processes within a given sample (typically but not always drawn from the

USA), but neglect to consider the contribution of larger macro-level cultural factors

such as cultural beliefs and practices or different educational systems. Miller and

Halpern (2013) note that studies examining extremely large datasets (e.g., n > 100,000)

Page 113: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 98

have found considerable cross-cultural variability for sex differences in cognitive

ability.

Such datasets also provide an excellent opportunity to test theoretical arguments

about the origins of sex differences in cognitive ability. Where the source of sex

differences are largely the product of internal biological factors (such as those reviewed

earlier), the pattern of sex differences should be universal (i.e., all countries show

superior female performance or all countries show superior male performance) and

largely homogenous. This is the case for sex differences in reading (Guiso et al., 2008;

Lynn & Mikk, 2009), and for visual-spatial ability (I. Silverman, Choi, & Peters, 2007).

Where the source of sex differences are largely the product of external psychosocial

factors, and to the extent that these factors may vary in strength from one country to

another, we should see a pattern of sex differences that are more heterogeneous. The

heterogeneity may come in two forms: firstly in magnitude (e.g., some countries show

negligible or very small effect sizes while others show larger effect sizes), or secondly

in direction (i.e., some countries show superior male performance, but others show

superior female performance for the same task). By way of example, cross-cultural

patterns of sex differences in mathematics and science in adolescence demonstrate both

properties. Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2017) reported data from the TIMSS 2011

wave that are highly heterogeneous in both direction (greater female performance is

found in some countries, greater male performance in others) and magnitude (some

nations have quite large sex differences in achievement while other nations show

negligible gender gaps). This would suggest that mathematics and science outcomes are

highly malleable and that under the right environmental and cultural conditions, any

outcome is possible.

Page 114: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 99

The two most prominent theoretical perspectives on cultural contributions to the

development of sex differences are Eagly and Wood’s (2011) social role theory, and the

gender stratification hypothesis (D. P. Baker & Jones, 1993; Riegle-Crumb, 2005).

While biological and psychosocial forces may represent proximal factors for individual

differences in cognitive ability, these macro-level cultural factors represent a more distal

influence in the emergence of group differences between males and females (i.e.,

population effects, rather than for specific individuals).

2.3.3.1 Social Role Theory.

Eagly (1987, 1997) proposed social role theory as a contrast to arguments made

by evolutionary psychology. Rather than sex differences in behaviour and cognition

being primarily biologically driven by the abilities and limitations of one gender or

another, the theory posits that sex differences are largely socially constructed and arise

from the historical gendered segregation of labour and responsibilities in society. Eagly

and Wood (2016) argue that sex differences in behaviour and cognition reflect sex-role

beliefs that, in turn, represent cultural perceptions of womens’ and mens’ social roles in

society. Social role theory proposes two direct mechanisms by which these are realized:

firstly, societal divisions between the roles and responsibilities of males and females

lead to the formation and perpetuation of gender stereotypes and sex-role beliefs;

secondly, sex-roles beliefs act as a self-regulatory process that constrains thought and

behaviour in a gender-consistent manner (see Section 2.3.2.3), as well as providing an

evaluative framework of expectations for the behaviour of others.

Eagly and Wood (2011) note that in post-industrial societies, men are more

likely than women to be employed full time, that they occupy higher status roles and

positions of authority, and that women are more likely than men to fulfil caretaking

roles either in the home or in the workforce. Observation of these divisions of labour

Page 115: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 100

between the sexes produces expectancies about their underlying disposition and

capabilities: because men and women generally assume different social and

occupational roles, cultural beliefs develop that men and women possess different traits

and capabilities (e.g., men occupy positions of leadership, therefore men are seen as

dominant and assertive, whereas women have greater representation in caring

professions, therefore they are more caring and nurturing). These expectancies form the

basis of consensually-shared beliefs held by society, or gender stereotypes. The

gendered division of roles may also exert an influence through observation and

modelling (see Section 2.3.2.4), helping to shape a child’s occupational and intellectual

aspirations, particularly in male-dominated fields such as STEM or in female-dominated

fields such as education, childcare and nursing. Eagly and Wood (2016, p. 459) note

that because these sex-roles are seen to “reflect innate attributes of the sexes, they

appear natural and inevitable”. To the extent that womens’ and mens’ roles remain

unchanged they will be transmitted generationally, but unlike evolutionary

psychological perspectives, social role theory suggests they may be subject to

intervention (for example, by increasing the availability and acceptance of counter-

examples such as female engineers or male teachers).

All cultures share beliefs about essential differences between males and females

(Best, 1982; Williams & Best, 1990), but the magnitude of gender stereotypes does vary

across countries. The underlying premise behind social role theory (psychological sex

differences are the result of observation of men and womens’ social roles) provides a

testable hypothesis that can be examined cross-culturally. It predicts that there will be

an observable relationship between the representation of women in the workforce and

the magnitude of sex differences in cognitive ability. There is some empirical support

for such claims. For example, Else-Quest, Hyde and Linn (2010) investigated sex

Page 116: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 101

differences in mathematics achievement conducted for the Programme for International

Student Assessment (PISA) 2003 wave. The researchers found that womens’ share of

higher labour market positions and the percentage of scientific researchers who were

female both significantly predicted the magnitude of sex differences in mathematics. It

also suggests that directly challenging gender stereotypes (e.g., scientists are male) and

increasing availability of counter-exemplars might also affect positive change in the

magnitude of sex differences (Nosek et al., 2009), and is consistent with research

showing the importance of female role models and mentoring for increasing

representation of women in STEM-related fields (Carli, Alawa, Lee, Zhao, & Kim,

2016).

2.3.3.2 Gender stratification hypothesis.

In a similar manner to social role theory, the gender stratification hypothesis

argues that a contributing factor to sex differences in cognitive abilities is gender

inequality throughout all levels of society (including occupational roles, educational

attainment and political representation). In some research studies, it has been termed the

gender segregation hypothesis, or the gender equality hypothesis. The distinction

between social role theory and the gender stratification hypothesis is that social role

theory makes a causal attribution and provides a mechanism. That is, beliefs about

gender stereotypes are derived from observation of the division in society of mens’ and

womens’ roles, and adherence to these gendered social roles and power structures

results in sex differences. By contrast, the gender segregation hypothesis is more

tentative, and less prescribed. Individual factors such as female representation in

parliament or the workforce that show a significant correlation with sex differences are

not necessarily causal, but rather are a general proxy for attitudes towards women and

gender equality. Women in countries where gender equality is low face additional

Page 117: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 102

barriers to access education or the labour market (e.g., discouragement by parents and

teachers to pursue higher education, lack of availability of maternity leave, absence of

anti-discrimination protection in hiring practices, etc.) independently of their own

ability level and self-efficacy. These are aggregated to produce an objective measure of

a nation’s level of gender equality (Else-Quest & Grabe, 2012). Fortin (2005) found

that traditional sex-role attitudes of a country are associated with a reduction in female

educational participation and their participation in the labour market. Women also

encounter fewer female role models in positions of power (especially in highly male-

dominated professions), which are particularly important in challenging negative gender

stereotypes in fields such as STEM.

Baker and Jones (1993) were the first to investigate gender segregation of roles

by examining cross-cultural patterns of sex differences in the Second International

Mathematics Study (SIMS), a large international assessment of mathematics

achievement that was conducted in 1964. Baker and Jones found medium-sized

negative correlations with a range of societal measures of gender segregation in

education and the workforce, such that sex differences were smaller with greater

representation of women. Such findings should be interpreted cautiously, however,

given the small number of countries participating (n = 19), and age of the findings

(considerable change in womens’ roles may have taken place; sex differences in

mathematics may be smaller than for previous generations).

More recently, Riegle-Crumb (2005) repeated the analysis using data from the

Third International Mathematics and Science Survey (TIMSS) conducted in 1995.

Although there was no association between gender stratification and achievement in

maths and science, there were significant associations for attitudes towards mathematics

and science (i.e., where there was less segregation, girls’ attitudes towards science and

Page 118: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 103

mathematics was more positive). Another study by Guiso, Monte, Sapienza, and

Zingales (2008) conducted a similar analysis but with older children from the

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), finding that sex differences in

mathematics achievement disappear in countries with greater gender equality. However,

some researchers have reported a failure to replicate these findings with later waves of

PISA data (Tao & Michalopoulos, 2017), leading to some uncertainty over support for

the gender segregation hypothesis.

To date, the gender stratification hypothesis has been applied almost exclusively

to mathematics and science achievement. Guiso et al (2008) also examined whether

there were similar patterns for sex differences in reading (where females outperform

males). They found a positive correlation with gender equality, such that more gender

equal nations had larger sex differences in reading.

2.3.4 Nash’s Sex-Role Mediation Theory

As outlined above, there are a large number of biological and psychosocial

explanations for the development of sex differences between males and females as a

group. However as Halpern et al., (2007) have noted, there is also greater within-gender

variability than between-gender differences, and substantial overlap between the sexes.

Why do some males perform poorly on visual-spatial and quantitative reasoning tasks

compared to their male peers, and similarly females with verbal and language abilities?

An integrated theory that could explain group differences and individual differences in

cognitive ability would be a significant advancement on current explanations, as would

a theory that bridged the divide between biological and psychosocial perspectives.

The present course of research examines a promising theoretical explanation for

sex differences that integrates aspects of biology, psychosocial and cultural

contributions - Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation theory of cognitive sex differences.

Page 119: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 104

Specifically, Nash (1979) proposed that masculine sex-role identification facilitated the

cultivation of visual-spatial reasoning, while feminine sex-role identification

encouraged the development of verbal and language abilities (see Figure 2.1). In a

review of sex-role identification and cognitive ability, Nash wrote: “For some people,

cultural myths are translated into personality beliefs which can affect cognitive

functioning in sex-typed intellectual domains” (p. 263). The sex-role mediation theory

posited two distinct pathways by which sex-roles might influence the development of

cognitive abilities. It was the synthesis of two ideas (performance on a cognitive task

could be influenced by the perceived sex-typing of the task, and that sex-role

identification could provide additional opportunities to hone and practise one’s talents)

offered by Nash (1979) that placed development of cognitive ability in a social context,

where sex-role identification encourages or discourages development of intellectual

potential. It also acknowledges the interaction between biological and psychosocial

factors, in that the sex-role identification process may also be influenced by hormonal

expression and prenatal experiences (Knafo, Iervolino, & Plomin, 2005), or by early

socialization experiences and cultural stereotype (Chaplin et al., 2005; Eccles et al.,

1990; Fagot & Hagan, 1991).

Figure 2.1 – Dual-pathway mechanism of Nash’s sex-role mediation theory

Sex

Masculine sex-role (instrumental/agentic traits)

Spatial Ability

Feminine sex-role (expressive/communal traits)

Differential practice

and sex-typing of activities

Language Ability

Differential practice

and sex-typing of activities

Page 120: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 105

Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation theory arose from two earlier studies by

Milton (1959) and Sherman (1967) into sex-roles and intellectual performance. Milton

had hypothesised that sex differences in cognitive performance might be at least

partially influenced by the perceived sex-role appropriateness of tasks, and

demonstrated that experimentally altering perceptions of problem-solving tasks to be

stereotypically masculine or feminine influenced cognitive performance (Milton, 1958).

Sherman proposed an entirely different mechanism by which sex-role identification

might affect performance. Rather than addressing sex-typing of tasks and sex-role

conformity pressures, Sherman (1967) hypothesised that the sex difference between

males and females on visual-spatial tasks might simply be the result of differential

levels of practice between boys and girls. Many childrens’ leisure pursuits and activities

were highly sex-typed at the time (and some remain so today). Activities that promote

spatial learning such as mechanical drawing, carpentry, model building, construction

blocks, and organised sports3 provided additional learning experiences that promote the

development of visual-spatial reasoning. Boys generally choose to participate in such

activities when made available, and typically spend more hours on these activities than

girls (Casey, 1996). Sherman used the analogy of a bent twig, in reference to the old

adage “as the twig is bent, so shall the tree grow” n.d. Even initially quite tiny

biological sex differences in visual-spatial reasoning might interact with environmental

experiences that promote learning (the twig), and affecting the direction of intellectual

growth over a prolonged period. Subsequently, this has been referred to as the Bent

Twig Theory of sex differences (Casey, 1996; Doyle et al., 2012). Robust associations

3 Historically, organized sport at the time in the United States was seen as more gender appropriate for boys and this bias was reflected in research conducted at the time (B. Stevenson, 2010). Following legislative change mandating equality of educational opportunities for girls (Title IX legislation), attitudes towards encouragement and support for girls and organized sport improved. Subsequently a moderately strong association between sport and spatial reasoning has been found for males and females (Voyer & Jansen, 2017), supporting part of Sherman’s argument.

Page 121: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 106

have been found between retrospective self-reports on childhood spatial activities

questionnaires and visual-spatial performance (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1995; Doyle

et al., 2012; Signorella et al., 1989), suggesting that they do provide additional practice

and learning opportunities.

Similar arguments had been put forward by other researchers for the

development of verbal and language abilities. Kagan (1964) studied the sex-role

classification of subjects in school and observed that most students classified reading as

being sex-typed as feminine. This view has been replicated in more modern samples

(Lynch, 2002; Martino, 2001; Millard, 1997; Shapiro, 1990), with consequential effects

for boys and girls on reading motivation and self-efficacy. Like visual-spatial ability,

the primary mechanism by which sex-role identification would affect reading skill

would be self-selection of activities that lead to differential levels of practice. For

example, feminine sex-role identification is associated with more favourable attitudes

towards reading, as well as significantly greater amounts of leisure reading (Turner,

1983), giving additional training time on reading and language development. But

performance on a verbal or language task might also be affected by the test-taker’s

perceptions of the sex-role appropriateness of the task and cultural stereotypes,

especially on standardised tests of achievement and grades.

In a meta-analysis of the association of sex-role identification and cognitive

performance, Signorella and Jamison (1986) note that only a handful of studies have

investigated the relationship between verbal ability and femininity. Most studies have

examined reading in children, though these are subject to methodological issues such as

small sample sizes and restricted age ranges. Schickendanz (1973) examined a male-

only sample of third-grade students drawn from several elementary schools, finding a

weak positive association between feminine sex-role identification and reading ability, r

Page 122: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 107

= .21. Similarly, Dwyer (1974) examined a cross-section of 384 students from Grades

2-12, finding that sex-role beliefs about reading significantly predicted reading ability,

even after controlling for sex and grade level. However Nash (1974) found an

interaction between sex and sex-role identification in a sample of 207 children (ages 11-

14). For girls, feminine sex-role identification was associated with higher reading

scores, but surprisingly a significant effect was not found in boys for this sample. Other

studies have found similar findings with children and reading motivation (Turner,

1983). More recently McGeown, Goodwin, Henderson and Wright (2011) examined

reading motivation and ability in a sample of 182 primary school children from the UK

aged 8-11. Students completed the Children’s Sex Role Inventory (CSRI), a

questionnaire on reading motivation and interest, as well as completing a standardised

test of reading comprehension and survey of attitudes towards reading. They found that

sex-role identification was a better predictor of reading performance than biological

gender, with feminine identification showing a moderately strong positive association

with reading motivation and self-efficacy beliefs. However the study failed to find a

meaningful association with reading skill on a standardised test. Finally, a recent study

by Ehrtmann and Wolter (in press) examined the effect of gender-role orientation on

reading achievement in a nationally representative German sample of secondary

students. They found that students who endorsed traditional gender-roles showed lower

test performance for reading than those endorsing an egalitarian gender-role orientation.

However a limitation of their study was the reliability and validity of their gender-role

orientation measure which was operationalized differently to established measures of

sex-role orientation.

Taken together, there is tentative evidence of a sex-role mediation effect for

reading attitudes, motivation and self-efficacy, but the hypothesis has not been strongly

Page 123: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 108

tested for actual reading ability. Whether there would be transfer effects to performance

on other types of language tasks is also unclear, because so few studies have tested the

sex-role mediation hypothesis for broader language tasks (Signorella & Jamison, 1986).

Only one identified study has employed reliable measures of sex-role identification and

verbal ability in a modern sample. Ritter (2004) examined a sample of 79 college

students, finding that feminine and androgynous adults scored higher on a verbal

fluency measure in males (d = 1.42), and females (d = .99). Further research is required

to determine whether this is the result of differential levels of practice, sex-role

appropriateness, or some combination of both.

2.4 Summary of Literature Review Findings

There are four main aspects of cognitive ability that show noteworthy sex

differences (see Table 2.1). These include the three domains identified by Maccoby and

Jacklin (1974), which were verbal and language abilities, visual-spatial ability, and

quantitative reasoning. But later empirical research and scholarship identified that sex

differences were also present in memory (recognition and recall) across multiple

modalities, though such findings are typically overlooked by broad reviews (e.g., Hyde,

2005). Researchers generally concur that there are robust sex differences for verbal and

language abilities as well as for visual-spatial, but many aspects of verbal ability are

under-investigated and lack replication with modern samples. However cross-cultural

studies show that sex differences in quantitative reasoning are not found in all samples,

and may be at least partially influenced by socio-cultural factors.

There is also a wide body of research that investigates the way in which people

think and feel about intelligence (in oneself, and in others). Males typically rate their

intelligence as higher than do females (male-hubris, female humility effect), but on

average both sexes estimate the intelligence of male relatives as being higher than

Page 124: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 109

female relatives. This observation runs contrary to established evidence showing that

males and females do not differ in measured psychometric and general intelligence.

However, research does show that when asked about specific cognitive abilities, lay

persons are fairly accurate in their estimation of actual sex differences in the general

population.

A variety of explanations have been offered for the emergence of sex differences

in cognitive ability, which have been termed origin theories. These include biological

explanations such as the contribution of sex hormones on the brain of the developing

foetus, as well as activational effects that are strongest after puberty (a time where the

gender gap in sex-typed cognitive abilities widens). Evolutionary psychologists have

also argued that there may be genetic differences between biologically female (XX

chromosome) and male (XY chromosome) that have been shaped by the division of

male and female roles throughout human prehistory. In recent decades though there has

been a shift away from biological determinism and towards acknowledgement of the

differences in early socialisation experiences of boys and girls, and the ongoing

contribution of sex-roles and gender stereotypes. There is also a growing recognition

that macro-level cultural factors such as gender equality and implicit gender beliefs

have on the intellectual interests and performance of boys and girls. A growing body of

literature has identified a variety of mechanisms by which sex differences in specific

cognitive abilities are made manifest, and most sex difference researchers endorse a

broad psychobiosocial model rather than any single origin theory. One theory that

encompasses biological and psychosocial contributions is Nash’s sex role mediation

theory of sex differences, which holds that the process of sex-role identification leads to

self-selection of activities and interests resulting in differential levels of training in

specific cognitive abilities. Additionally, the perceived sex-typing of a given task also

Page 125: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 110

contributes to cognitive performance: when perceived sex-typing of the task is

incompatible with an individual’s sex-role identification this may result in lowered

performance. Though there is fair support for the sex-role mediation theory on visual-

spatial tasks much of the literature is dated and few studies have investigated verbal and

language abilities.

Origin theories of sex differences are important as they may identify targets for

educational and psychosocial interventions, but are difficult to test empirically – when a

study or meta-analysis reports a difference between males and females, the relative

contribution of biological and psychosocial factors cannot be determined, especially if a

sample is selective and not representative. But the tenability of such origin theories can

be tested cross-culturally by examining variability in the direction and magnitude of the

gender gap. A strong biological contribution would be consistent with observed

differences in verbal and language abilities, as well as visual-spatial reasoning.

However quantitative reasoning as measured by educational achievement in

mathematics and science is highly culturally variable: in some nations females score

higher than males, while in others there may be no difference whatsoever or that males

score higher than females. This pattern of observations would be more consistent with

psychosocial origin theories. At present, there exists no cross-cultural studies of sex

differences in memory.

References Acevedo, A., Loewenstein, D. A., Barker, W. W., Harwood, D. G., Luis, C., Bravo, M., . . .

Duara, R. (2000). Category Fluency Test: Normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 760-769.

Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33(5-6), 309-323. doi:10.1007/bf01954572

Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children's toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(6), 467-479. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00107-1

Page 126: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 111

Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6), 1621-1630. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621

Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., & Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(5), 804-825. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804

Allred, R. A. (1990). Gender differences in spelling achievement in grades 1 through 6. The Journal of Educational Research, 83(4), 187-193. doi:10.1080/00220671.1990.10885955

Alon, S., & Gelbgiser, D. (2011). The female advantage in college academic achievements and horizontal sex segregation. Social Science Research, 40(1), 107-119. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.06.007

Anastasi, A. (1958). Differential psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary

explanations compatible? American Psychologist, 51(9), 909-917. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.51.9.909

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., & Hines, M. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychological Science, 20(2), 144-148. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02279.x

Badian, N. A. (1999). Reading disability defined as a discrepancy between listening and reading comprehension: A longitudinal study of stability, gender differences, and prevalence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2), 138-148. doi:10.1177/002221949903200204

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20(5), 327-344. doi:10.1007/BF00287729

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1995). Environmental input to the development of sex-related differences in spatial and mathematical ability. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(4), 363-379. doi:10.1016/1041-6080(95)90007-1

Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally. Baker, D. P., & Jones, D. P. (1993). Creating gender equality: Cross-national gender

stratification and mathematical performance. Sociology of Education, 66(2), 91-103. doi:10.2307/2112795

Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children's motivation for reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(4), 452-477. doi:10.1598/rrq.34.4.4

Baker, S. C., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2000). The role of gender and immersion in communication and second language orientations. Language Learning, 50(2), 311-341.

Balint, M. (1948). Individual differences of behavior in early infancy, and an objective method for recording them: I. Approach and the method of recording. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 73(1), 57-79. doi:10.1080/08856559.1948.10533462

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection and variability in studies of gender differences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(2), 183-184. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Beloff, H. (1992). Mother, father and me: Our IQ. The Psychologist, 5, 309-311. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. doi:10.1037/h0036215

Page 127: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 112

Bem, S. L. (1981a). The BSRI and Gender Schema Theory: A reply to Spence and Helmreich. Psychological Review, 88(4), 369-371. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.369

Bem, S. L. (1981b). Gender Schema Theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354-364. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354

Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. Signs: Journal of women in culture and society, 8(4), 598-616. doi:10.1086/493998

Bem, S. L. (1984). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual and empirical integration. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Psychology and Gender (pp. 179–226). Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.

Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Benbow, C. P., Lubinski, D., Shea, D. L., & Eftekhari-Sanjani, H. (2000). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability at age 13: Their status 20 years later. Psychological Science, 11(6), 474-480. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00291

Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. L., & Wesman, A. G. (1982). Differential Apptitude Tests: Administrator's handbook. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. d. (1989). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, Iowa: AJA Associates.

Berenbaum, S. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2011). Sexual differentiation of human behavior: effects of prenatal and pubertal organizational hormones. Frontiers in neuroendocrinology, 32(2), 183-200. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.03.001

Berenbaum, S. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2016). How early hormones shape gender development. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 7, 53-60. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.011

Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preference. Psychological Science, 3, 203-206. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00028.x

Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46(2), 151-172. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.007

Best, D. L. (1982). An overview of findings from children’s studies of sex-trait stereotypes in 23 countries. In R. Roth, H. Asthana, D. Sinha, & J. B. H. Sinha (Eds.), Diversity and Unity in Cross-Cultural Psychology (pp. 261-271). Amsterdam: Swets.

Best, D. L., Williams, J. E., Cloud, J. M., Davis, S. W., Robertson, L. S., Edwards, J. R., . . . Fowles, J. (1977). Development of sex-trait stereotypes among young children in the United States, England, and Ireland. Child Development, 48(4), 1375-1384. doi:10.2307/1128496

Bolla, K. I., Lindgren, K. N., Bonaccorsy, C., & Bleecker, M. L. (1990). Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46(5), 623-628. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199009)46:5<623::aid-jclp2270460513>3.0.co;2-c

Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender, emotion, and expression. In M. B. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 338–349). New York: Guilford Press.

Brownell, W., & Smith, D. R. (1973). Communication patterns, sex, and length of verbalization in speech of four‐year‐old children. Speech Monographs, 40(4), 310-316. doi:10.1080/03637757309375809

Buck, G., Kostin, I., & Morgan, R. (2002). Examining the relationship of content to gender-based performance differences in Advanced Placement exams. ETS Research Report Series, 2002(2), i-34.

Page 128: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 113

Burstall, C. (1975). Primary French in the Balance. Educational Research, 17(3), 193-198. doi:10.1080/0013188750170304

Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50(3), 164-168. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.50.3.164

Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1984). Influence of gender constancy and social power on sex-linked modeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1292.

Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 100, 676-713. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676

Buswell, J. H. (1980). The relationships between oral language competency and reading achievement of second and third grade students. (Doctor of Education), University of Northern Colorado, Ann Arbor. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (303056003)

Camarata, S., & Woodcock, R. (2006). Sex differences in processing speed: Developmental effects in males and females. Intelligence, 34(3), 231-252. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.12.001

Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., & Barbarotto, R. (1999). Gender affects word retrieval of certain categories in semantic fluency tasks. Cortex, 35(2), 273-278. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70800-1

Caplan, J. B., & Caplan, P. J. (2005). The perseverative search for sex differences in mathematics ability. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 25-47). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Carli, L. L., Alawa, L., Lee, Y., Zhao, B., & Kim, E. (2016). Stereotypes about gender and science: Women ≠ Scientists. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 244-260. doi:10.1177/0361684315622645

Carroll, J. B. (1941). A factor analysis of verbal abilities. Psychometrika, 6(5), 279-307. doi:10.1007/bf02288585

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities : a survey of factor-analytic studies New York: Cambridge University Press.

Casey, M. B. (1996). Understanding individual differences in spatial ability: A nature/nurture interactionist framework. Developmental Review, 16(3), 241-260. doi:10.1006/drev.1996.0009

Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion, 5(1), 80-88. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80

Cherney, I. D., & Voyer, D. (2010). Development of a spatial activity questionnaire I: Items identification. Sex Roles, 62(1), 89-99. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9710-9

Cimpian, A., Mu, Y., & Erickson, L. C. (2012). Who is good at this game? Linking an activity to a social category undermines children’s achievement. Psychological Science, 23(5), 533-541. doi:10.1177/0956797611429803

Clearfield, M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers’ speech and play behavior with 6-, 9-, and 14-month-old infants. Sex Roles, 54, 127-137. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-8874-1

Collaer, M. L., Reimers, S., & Manning, J. T. (2007). Visuospatial performance on an Internet line judgment task and potential hormonal markers: Sex, sexual orientation, and 2D:4D. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 177-192. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9152-1

Page 129: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 114

Colom, R., Contreras, M. J., Arend, I., Leal, O. G., & Santacreu, J. (2004). Sex differences in verbal reasoning are mediated by sex differences in spatial ability. The Psychological Record, 54(3), 365-372. doi: 10.1007/bf03395479

Colom, R., Juan-Espinosa, M., Abad, F. J., & Garcıa, L. F. (2000). Negligible sex differences in general intelligence. Intelligence, 28(1), 57-68. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(99)00035-5

Coltheart, M., Hull, E., & Slater, D. (1975). Sex differences in imagery and reading. Nature, 253(5491), 438-440. doi:10.1038/253438a0

Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine-and feminine-activity-preference scales for preschoolers: Correlates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development, 48(4), 1411-1416. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1977.tb03947.x

Constantinople, A. (1973). Masculinity-femininity: An exception to a famous dictum? Psychological Bulletin, 80(5), 389-409. doi:10.1037/h0035334

Cornell, D. G. (1997). Post hoc explanation is not prediction. American Psychologist, 52, 1380-1380. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.52.12.1380.a

Crook III, T. H., Youngjohn, J. R., & Larrabee, G. J. (1990). The Misplaced Objects Test: A measure of everyday visual memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(6), 819-833. doi:10.1080/01688639008401024

Daneman, M. (1991). Working memory as a predictor of verbal fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20(6), 445-464.

Darwin, C. (1871). The decent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.

De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015316108

Deary, I. J., Thorpe, G., Wilson, V., Starr, J. M., & Whalley, L. J. (2003). Population sex differences in IQ at age 11: the Scottish mental survey 1932. Intelligence, 31(6), 533-542. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00053-9

Dekhtyar, S., Weber, D., Helgertz, J., & Herlitz, A. (2018). Sex differences in academic strengths contribute to gender segregation in education and occupation: A longitudinal examination of 167,776 individuals. Intelligence. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2017.11.007

Doyle, R. A., Voyer, D., & Cherney, I. D. (2012). The relation between childhood spatial activities and spatial abilities in adulthood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33, 112-120. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.01.002

Drake, E., Henderson, V., Stanczyk, F., McCleary, C. A., Brown, W. S., Smith, C. A., . . . Buckwalter, J. G. (2000). Associations between circulating sex steroid hormones and cognition in normal elderly women. Neurology, 54(3), 599-599.

Dwyer, C. A. (1974). Influence of children's sex role standards on reading and arithmetic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(6), 811-816. doi:10.1037/h0021522

Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Eagly, A. H. (1997). Sex differences in social behavior: Comparing social role theory and evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1380-1383. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1380.b

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2011). Social role theory. Handbook of theories in social psychology, 2, 458-476.

Page 130: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 115

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2016). Social Role Theory of Sex Differences. In N. A. Naples (Ed.), The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Gender and Sexuality Studies (pp. 458-476): John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents' socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46(2), 183-201. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Flanagan, C. A., Miller, C., Reuman, D. A., & Yee, D. (1989). Self‐concepts, domain values, and self‐esteem: Relations and changes at early adolescence. Journal of Personality, 57(2), 283-310. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00484.x

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differences in children's self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64(3), 830-847. doi:10.2307/1131221

Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: a multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 451-463. doi: 10.1037//0012-I649.37.4.45I

Ekstrand, L. H. (1980). Sex differences in second language learning? Empirical studies and a discussion of related findings. International Review of Applied Psychology, 29(1-2), 205-259. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1980.tb00891.x

Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., & Harman, H. H. (1976). Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Elliot, L. (2009). Pink Brain, Blue Brain. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Ellis, H. (1904). Man and woman: A study of human secondary sexual characteristics.

London: Walter Scott. Ellis, L., Hershberger, S. L., Field, E., Wersinger, S., Pellis, S., & Geary, D. C. (2008). Sex

differences: Summarizing more than a century of scientific research. New York: Psychology Press.

Else-Quest, N. M., & Grabe, S. (2012). The political is personal: Measurement and application of nation-level indicators of gender equity in psychological research Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36(2), 131-144. doi:10.1177/0361684312441592

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 103-127. doi:10.1037/a0018053

Else-Quest, N. M., Mineo, C. C., & Higgins, A. (2013). Math and science attitudes and achievement at the intersection of gender and ethnicity. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3), 293-309. doi:10.1177/0361684313480694

Estes, W. K. (1974). Learning theory and intelligence. American Psychologist, 29(10), 740-749. doi:10.1037/h0037458

Fagot, B. I., & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parent reactions to sex-stereotyped behaviors: Age and sex effects. Child Development, 62(3), 617-628. doi:10.2307/1131135

Falter, C., Arroyo, M., & Davis, G. (2006). Testosterone: activation or organization of spatial cognition? Biological Psychology, 73(2), 132-140. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.011

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43(2), 95-103. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A new look at an old controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62(1), 61-84. doi:10.3102/00346543062001061

Page 131: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 116

Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs‐Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71(2), 310-322. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00146

Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, J. S., & Thal, D. (2000). Reply: Measuring variability in early child language: Don't shoot the messenger. Child Development, 71(2), 323-328. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00147

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., . . . Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1-185. doi:10.2307/1166093

Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234-248. doi:10.1111/desc.12019

Fine, C. (2010a). Delusions of gender : how our minds, society, and neurosexism create difference. New York: W. W. Norton.

Fine, C. (2010b). From scanner to sound bite: Issues in interpreting and reporting sex differences in the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(5), 280-283. doi:10.1177/0963721410383248

Fivush, R. (2014). Emotional content of parent-child conversations about the past. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect in development: The Minnesoda Symposia on child psychology (Vol 26) (Vol. 26, pp. 39-72). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Flannagan, D., & Baker-Ward, L. (1996). Relations between mother-child discussions of children's preschool and kindergarten experiences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 423-437. doi:10.1016/s0193-3973(96)90035-0

Flannagan, D., & Perese, S. (1998). Emotional refererences in mother-daughter and mother-son dyads' conversations about school. Sex Roles, 39, 353-367.

Fortin, N. M. (2005). Gender role attitudes and the labour-market outcomes of women across OECD countries. oxford review of Economic Policy, 21(3), 416-438.

Frome, P. M., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Parents' influence on children's achievement-related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 435-452. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.435

Furnham, A. (2000). Parents’ estimates of their own and their children's multiple intelligences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 583-594. doi:10.1348/026151000165869

Furnham, A., & Gasson, L. (1998). Sex difference in parental estimates of their children's intelligence. Sex Roles, 38(1-2), 151-162. doi:10.1023/A:1018810723493

Furnham, A., Hosoe, T., & Tang, T. (2001). Male hubris and female humility? A cross-cultural study of ratings of self, parental, and sibling multiple intelligence in America, Britain, and Japan. Intelligence, 30(1), 101-115. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00080-0

Furnham, A., & Rawles, R. (1995). Sex differences in the estimation of intelligence. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 10(3), 741-748.

Furnham, A., Reeves, E., & Budhani, S. (2002). Parents think their sons are brighter than their daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimations and estimations of their children's multiple intelligences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(1), 24-39. doi:10.1080/00221320209597966

Galea, L. A., & Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in route-learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(1), 53-65. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90174-2

Page 132: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 117

Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non‐verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3(2), 206-215. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00114

Gates, A. I. (1961). Sex differences in reading ability. The Elementary School Journal, 61(8), 431-434.

Gatewood, M. C., & Weiss, A. P. (1930). Race and sex differences in newborn infants. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 38(1-4), 31-49. doi:10.1080/08856559.1930.10532250

Geary, D. C. (1996). Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematical abilities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(2), 229-247. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00042400

Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female : the evolution of human sex differences: American Psychological Association.

Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female : the evolution of human sex differences (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Geffen, G., Moar, K., O'Hanlon, A., Clark, C., & Geffen, L. (1990). Performance measures of 16–to 86-year-old males and females on the auditory verbal learning test. The clinical neuropsychologist, 4(1), 45-63. doi:10.1080/13854049008401496

Gentile, B., Grabe, S., Dolan-Pascoe, B., Twenge, J. M., Wells, B. E., & Maitino, A. (2009). Gender differences in domain-specific self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology; Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 34-45. doi:10.1037/a0013689

Gladsjo, J. A., Schuman, C. C., Evans, J. D., Peavy, G. M., Miller, S. W., & Heaton, R. K. (1999). Norms for Letter and Category Fluency: Demographic Corrections for Age, Education, and Ethnicity. Assessment, 6(2), 147-178. doi:10.1177/107319119900600204

Gordon, H. W., & Lee, P. A. (1986). A relationship between gonadotropins and visuospatial function. Neuropsychologia, 24(4), 563-576. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(86)90100-4

Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35(7), 603-618. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.7.603

Griksiene, R., & Ruksenas, O. (2011). Effects of hormonal contraceptives on mental rotation and verbal fluency. Psychoneuroendocrinology. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.03.001

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math.

Science, 320(5880), 1164-1165. doi:10.1126/science.1154094 Haberland, J. A. (1959). A comparison of listening tests with standardized tests. The Journal

of Educational Research, 52(8), 299-302. Halari, R., Hines, M., Kumari, V., Mehrotra, R., Wheeler, M., Ng, V., & Sharma, T. (2005).

Sex differences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 104-117. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.104

Halim, M. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Gender identity and stereotyping in early and middle childhood. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology (pp. 495-525). New York: Springer.

Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85(4), 845-857. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.85.4.845

Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. American Psychologist, 52(10), 1091-1102. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1091

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 133: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 118

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A.

(2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1-51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x

Halpern, D. F., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde, J. S., . . . Martin, C. L. (2011). The pseudoscience of single-sex schooling. Science, 333(6050), 1706-1707. doi:10.1126/science.1205031

Halpern, D. F., & Lamay, M. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 229-246. doi:10.1023/A:1009027516424

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011). Beliefs about cognitive gender differences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for size. Sex Roles, 64(5-6), 336-347. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using a psychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brain and Cognition, 45(3), 392-414. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1287

Halpern, D. F., & Wright, T. M. (1996). A process-oriented model of cognitive sex differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 8(1), 3-24. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90003-5

Hampson, E. (1990). Estrogen-related variations in human spatial and articulatory-motor skills. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15(2), 97-111. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(90)90018-5

Hampson, E. (1990). Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle. Brain and Cognition, 14(1), 26-43. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(90)90058-V

Hampson, E., & Kimura, D. (1988). Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102(3), 456-459. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.102.3.456

Harasty, J., Double, K. L., Halliday, G. M., Kril, J. J., & McRitchie, D. A. (1997). Language-associated cortical regions are proportionally larger in the female brain. Archives of Neurology, 54(2), 171-176. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1997.00550140045011

Hare-Mustin, R., & Marecek, J. (1988). The meaning of difference: Gender theory, Postmodernism, and Psychology. American Psychologist, 43(6), 455-464. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.43.6.455

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences - A psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(3), 389-401. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.019

Hausmann, M., Slabbekoorn, D., Van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & Gunturkun, O. (2000). Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114(6), 1245-1250. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.6.1245

Hawke, J. L., Olson, R. K., Willcut, E. G., Wadsworth, S. J., & DeFries, J. C. (2009). Gender ratios for reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 15(3), 239-242. doi:10.1002/dys.389

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269(5220), 41-45. doi:10.1126/science.7604277

Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Page 134: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 119

Herlitz, A., Airaksinen, E., & Nordström, E. (1999). Sex differences in episodic memory: the impact of verbal and visuospatial ability. Neuropsychology, 13(4), 590-597. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.590

Herlitz, A., & Lovén, J. (2013). Sex differences and the own-gender bias in face recognition: A meta-analytic review. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1306-1336. doi:10.1080/13506285.2013.823140

Herlitz, A., Nilsson, L.-G., & Bäckman, L. (1997). Gender differences in episodic memory. Memory & cognition, 25(6), 801-811. doi:10.3758/bf03211324

Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex Differences in Episodic Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 52-56. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00547.x

Hill, A. C., Laird, A. R., & Robinson, J. L. (2014). Gender differences in working memory networks: A BrainMap meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 102, 18-29. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.06.008

Hill, R. D., Grut, M., Wahlin, Å., Herlitz, A., Winblad, B., & Bäckman, L. (1995). Predicting memory performance in optimally healthy very old adults. Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 1(1), 57-67.

Hines, M. (1990). Gonadal hormones and human cognitive development. In J. Balthazart (Ed.), Hormones, Brain and Behaviour in Vertebrates (pp. 51-63). New York: Karger.

Hines, M. (2006). Prenatal testosterone and gender-related behaviour. European Journal of Endocrinology, 155, S115-S121. doi:10.1530/eje.1.02236

Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 448-456. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005

Hines, M. (2015). Early androgen exposure and human gender development. Biology of Sex Differences, 6(1), 1-10. doi:10.1186/s13293-015-0022-1

Hines, M., & Alexander, G. M. (2008). Monkeys, girls, boys and toys: A confirmation: Letter regarding "Sex differences in toy preferences: Striking parallels between monkeys and humans". Hormones and Behavior, 54(3), 478-479. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.012

Hogan, H. W. (1978). IQ self-estimates of males and females. The Journal of social psychology, 106(1), 137-138. doi:10.1080/00224545.1978.9924160

Horn, J. (1994). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 443-451). New York: Macmillan.

Horn, J. L. (1991). Measurement of intellectual capabilities: A review of theory. In K. S. McGrew, J. K. Werder, & R. Woodcock (Eds.), Woodcock-Johnson technical manual: A reference on theory and current research to supplement the WJ-R examiner's manuals (pp. 197-232). Allen, Texas: DLM.

Hurks, P., Vles, J., Hendriksen, J., Kalff, A., Feron, F., Kroes, M., . . . Jolles, J. (2006). Semantic category fluency versus initial letter fluency over 60 seconds as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in healthy school-aged children. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(5), 684-695. doi:10.1080/13803390590954191

Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236-248. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.27.2.236

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 139-155. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139

Page 135: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 120

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., Ryan, M., Frost, L. A., & Hopp, C. (1990). Gender comparisons of mathematics attitudes and affect. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14(3), 299-324. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1990.tb00022.x

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about the nature of gender differences and the implications for gender equity. In S. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equity through education (2nd ed., pp. 19-32). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 53-69. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.104.1.53

Idle, T., Wood, E., & Desmarais, S. (1993). Gender role socialization in toy play situations: Mothers and fathers with their sons and daughters. Sex Roles, 28(11), 679-691. doi:10.1007/bf00289987

Irwing, P., & Lynn, R. (2005). Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 505-524. doi: 10.1348/000712605x53542

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509-527. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00421

Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 703-726. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.126.5.703

Janowsky, J., Oviatt, S., & Orwoll, E. (1994). Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(2), 325-332. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.108.2.325

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor : the Science of Mental Ability. London: Praeger. Jodl, K. M., Michael, A., Malanchuk, O., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. (2001). Parents' roles

in shaping early adolescents' occupational aspirations. Child Development, 72(4), 1247-1266. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00345

Jonasson, Z. (2005). Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: a review of behavioral and biological data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(8), 811-825. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.006

Jussim, L., & Harber, K. D. (2005). Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies: Knowns and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controversies. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(2), 131-155. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0902_3

Kagan, J. (1964). Acquisition and significance of sex-typing and sex-role identity. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 1, pp. 137-168). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Kansaku, K., & Kitazawa, S. (2001). Imaging studies on sex differences in the lateralization of language. Neuroscience research, 41(4), 333-337. doi:10.1016/S0168-0102(01)00292-9

Katsuic, S. K., Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Bagniewski, S. M., & Weaver, A. L. (2006). Written Language Learning Disorder : Incidence in a population-based birth cohort, 1976-1982, Rochester, Minnesota. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(5), 439-440.

Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman, J. C., Liu, X., & Johnson, C. K. (2009). How do educational attainment and gender relate to fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and academic skills at ages 22–90 years? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(2), 153-163. doi:10.1093/arclin/acp015

Kaufman, A. S., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (2006). Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence. New York: Wiley.

Page 136: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 121

Kaufman, A. S., McLean, J. E., & Reynolds, C. R. (1988). Sex, race, residence, region, and education differences on the 11 WAIS-R subtests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(2), 231-248. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198803)44:2<231::AID-JCLP2270440224>3.0.CO;2-J

Keith, T. Z., Reynolds, M. R., Patel, P. G., & Ridley, K. P. (2008). Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages 6 to 59: Evidence from the Woodcock–Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 502-525. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.001

Kempe, V., Puts, D. A., & Cárdenas, R. A. (2013). Masculine men articulate less clearly. Human Nature, 24(4), 461-475. doi:10.1007/s12110-013-9183-y

Kimura, D. (1996). Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones influence human cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(2), 259-263. doi:10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80081-x

Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kimura, D., & Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by

fluctuations in sex hormones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(2), 57-61. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769964

Kite, M. E., Deaux, K., & Haines, E. L. (2008). Gender stereotypes. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: a handbook of issues and theories (2nd ed., pp. 205–236). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3-to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407-428. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2001_6

Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 470-500. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.470

Knafo, A., Iervolino, A. C., & Plomin, R. (2005). Masculine girls and feminine boys: Genetic and environmental contributions to atypical gender development in early childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 400-412. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.400

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children's sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby & R. G. D'Andrade (Eds.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82-172). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Kohlberg, L., & Ullian, D. (1974). Stages in the development of psycho-sexual concepts and attitudes In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp. 209-222). New York: Wiley.

Korner, A. F. (1973). Sex differences in newboawrds with special reference to differences in the organization of oral behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(1), 19-29. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1973.tb01863.x

Kosmidis, M. H., Vlahou, C. H., Panagiotaki, P., & Kiosseoglou, G. (2004). The verbal fluency task in the Greek population: Normative data, and clustering and switching strategies. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 164-172. doi:10.1017/S1355617704102014

Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., & Daniel, M. (1988). Sex differences in verbal learning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(6), 907-915. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198811)44:6<907::AID-JCLP2270440610>3.0.CO;2-8

Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., O'donnell, L., & Prifitera, A. (1997). Developmental sex differences in verbal learning. Neuropsychology, 11(4), 577.

Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 221-232. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.77.2.221

Page 137: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 122

Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121-1134. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.77.6.1121

Kutner, M., Greenberg, E., Jin, Y., Boyle, B., Hsu, Y.-c., & Dunleavy, E. (2007). Literacy in everyday life: Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy. Washington, DC.: National Center for Education Statistics.

LaFrance, M., & Vial, A. C. (2016). Gender and nonverbal behavior. In D. Matsumoto, H. C. Hwang, & M. G. Frank (Eds.), APA handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 139-161). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1993). Do men show more rapid age-associated decline in simulated everyday verbal memory than do women? Psychology and Aging, 8(1), 68-71. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.8.1.68

Leaper, C. (2002). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Borenstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (pp. 189-255). Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3-27. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3

Leaper, C., & Ayres, M. M. (2007). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in adults' language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(4), 328-363. doi:10.1177/1088868307302221

Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2007). The socialization of gender. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 561-587). New York: Guilford Press.

Leaper, C., & Smith, T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children's language use: talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 993-1027. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.993

Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41(1), 141-162.

Lehrner, J. P. (1993). Gender differences in long-term odor recognition memory: verbal versus sensory influences and the consistency of label use. Chemical Senses, 18(1), 17-26. doi:10.1093/chemse/18.1.17

Levy, J. (1969). Possible basis for the evolution of lateral specialization of the human brain. Nature, 224(5219), 614-615. doi:10.1038/224614a0

Lezack, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Liben, L. S., Schroeder, K. M., Boriello, G. A., & Weisgram, E. S. (2018). Cognitive consequences of gendered toy play. In E. S. Weisgram & L. M. Dinella (Eds.), Gender Typing of Children's Toys: How Early Play Experiences Impact Development (pp. 295-364). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Limbrick, L., Wheldall, K., & Madelaine, A. (2008). Gender ratios for reading disability: Are there really more boys than girls who are low-progress readers? Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 13(2), 161-179. doi:10.1080/19404150802415781

Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L., & Linn, M. C. (2010). New trends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1123-1135. doi:10.1037/a0021276

Lippa, R. A. (2006). The Gender Reality Hypothesis. American Psychologist, 61(6), 639-640. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.61.6.639

Page 138: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 123

Loonstra, A. S., Tarlow, A. R., & Sellers, A. H. (2001). COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Applied Neuropsychology, 8(3), 161-166. doi:10.1207/s15324826an0803_5

Lynch, J. (2002). Parents’ self-efficacy beliefs, parents’ gender, children’s reader self-perceptions, reading achievement and gender. Journal of Research in Reading, 25(1), 54-67. doi:10.1111/1467-9817.00158

Lynn, R. (1992). Sex differences on the Differential Aptitude Test in British and American Adolescents. Educational Psychology, 12(2), 101-102. doi:10.1080/0144341920120201

Lynn, R. (1994). Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: A paradox resolved. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(2), 257-271. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90030-2

Lynn, R. (2016). Sex differences in intelligence. Mankind Quarterly, 57(1), 5. Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental arithmetic, digit span, and g

defined as working memory capacity. Intelligence, 36(3), 226-235. doi:DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.06.002

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames, 13(1), 3-13. doi:10.3176/tr.2009.1.01

Lynn, R., & Mulhern, G. (1991). A comparison of sex differences on the Scottish and American standardisation samples of the WISC-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 12(11), 1179-1182. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90082-M

Lynn, R., & Piffer, D. (2011). Sex differences in foreign language ability: an evolutionary theory. International Journal of Anthropology, 26(1-2), 75-84.

Lynn, R., & Wilson, R. G. (1993). Sex differences in cognitive abilities among Irish primary and secondary school children. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 14(2), 293-300. doi:10.1080/03033910.1993.10557932

Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 267-296. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267

Maccoby, E. E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender constancy in sex-differentiated development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1990(47), 5-20. doi:10.1002/cd.23219904703

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Maitland, S. B., Intrieri, R. C., Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (2000). Gender differences and changes in cognitive abilities across the adult life span. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 7(1), 32-53. doi:10.1076/anec.7.1.32.807

Majeres, R. L. (1999). Sex differences in phonological processes: Speeded matching and word reading. Memory & cognition, 27(2), 246-253.

Majeres, R. L. (2007). Sex differences in phonological coding: Alphabet transformation speed. Intelligence, 35(4), 335-346. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.005

Maki, P. M. (2001). Enhanced verbal Mmmory in nondemented elderly women receiving hormone-replacement therapy. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(2), 227-233.

Maki, P. M., Rich, J. B., & Shayna Rosenbaum, R. (2002). Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia, 40(5), 518-529. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00126-9

Page 139: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 124

Marinak, B. A., & Gambrell, L. B. (2010). Reading motivation: Exploring the elementary gender gap. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49(2), 129-141. doi:10.1080/19388070902803795

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children's search for gender cues : Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 67-70. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x

Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903-933. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903

Martin, D. J., & Hoover, H. D. (1987). Sex differences in educational achievement: A longitudinal study. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 7(1), 65-83. doi:10.1177/0272431687071007

Martino, W. (2001). Boys and reading: Investigating the impact of masculinities on boys' reading preferences and involvement in literacy. The Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 24(1), 61-74.

Mau, W.-C., & Lynn, R. (2000). Gender differences in homework and test scores in mathematics, reading and science at tenth and twelfth grade. Psychology, Evolution & Gender, 2(2), 119-125. doi:10.1080/14616660050200904

Mau, W.-C., & Lynn, R. (2001). Gender differences on the Scholastic Aptitude Test, the American College Test and college grades. Educational Psychology, 21(2), 133-136. doi:10.1080/01443410125298

Maylor, E. A., Reimers, S., Choi, J., Collaer, M. L., Peters, M., & Silverman, I. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in cognition across adulthood: Age is kinder to women than to men regardless of sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 235-249. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9155-y

McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2011). Gender differences in reading motivation: does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 328-336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x

McGrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of the major intelligence batteries according to a proposed comprehensive Gf–Gc framework. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests and issues. New York: Guilford.

McGuinness, D., & Courtney, A. (1983). Sex differences in visual and phonetic search. Journal of Mental Imagery, 7(1), 95-104.

Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramírez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). Are women really more talkative than men? Science, 317(5834), 82-82. doi:10.1126/science.1139940

Millard, E. (1997). Differently Literate: Gender identity and the construction of the developing reader. Gender and Education, 9(1), 31-48. doi:10.1080/09540259721439

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37-45. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011

Milton, G. A. (1958). Five studies of the relation between sex-role identification and achievement in problem solving. Office of Naval Research: Washington, D.C.

Milton, G. A. (1959). Sex differences in problem solving as a function of role appropriateness of the problem content. Psychological Reports, 5, 705-708. doi:10.2466/pr0.1959.5.3.705

Moore, G. W., Combs, J. P., & Slate, J. R. (2012). Advanced Placement Exams participation and performance: A national study of gender differences. e-International Journal of Educational Research, 3(3), 18-32.

Page 140: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 125

Nash, S. C. (1974). Conceptions and concomitants of sex-role stereotyping. Columbia University, Ann Arbor. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (302721823)

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. In M. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263-302). New York: Academic Press.

Neisser, U. C. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Fancisco: Freeman. Neisser, U. C., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., . . .

Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77-101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77

Neumann, M. M., Hood, M., & Neumann, D. L. (2009). The scaffolding of emergent literacy skills in the home environment: A case study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 36(4), 313-319. doi:10.1007/s10643-008-0291-y

Neumann, M. M., & Neumann, D. L. (2010). Parental strategies to scaffold emergent writing skills in the pre‐school child within the home environment. Early Years, 30(1), 79-94. doi: 10.1080/09575140903196715

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male, me= female, therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44-59. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44

Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., . . . Gonsalkorale, K. (2009). National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26), 10593–10597. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809921106

Nowell, A., & Hedges, L. V. (1998). Trends in gender differences in academic achievement from 1960 to 1994: An analysis of differences in mean, variance, and extreme scores. Sex Roles, 39(1), 21-43. doi:10.1023/A:1018873615316

Oswald, D. L. (2008). Gender stereotypes and women's reports of liking and ability in traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32(2), 196-203. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00424.x

Pargulski, J. R., & Reynolds, M. R. (2017). Sex differences in achievement: Distributions matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 272-278. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.016

Parker, P. D., Van Zanden, B., & Parker, R. B. (2018). Girls get smart, boys get smug: Historical changes in gender differences in math, literacy, and academic social comparison and achievement. Learning and Instruction, 54, 125-137. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.09.002

Payne, T. W., & Lynn, R. (2011). Sex differences in second language comprehension. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 434-436. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.026

Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., . . . Tao, S. (2017). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 48-76. doi:10.1037/bul0000124

Perry, D. G., White, A. J., & Perry, L. C. (1984). Does early sex typing result from children's attempts to match their behavior to sex role stereotypes? Child Development, 55(6), 2114-2121. doi:10.2307/1129784

Peters, M., Manning, J. T., & Reimers, S. (2007). The effects of sex, sexual orientation, and digit ratio (2D: 4D) on mental rotation performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 251-260. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9166-8

Page 141: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 126

Petrides, K., & Furnham, A. (2000). Gender differences in measured and self-estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex Roles, 42(5), 449-461.

Piaget, J. (1951). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. (C. Gattegno & F. M. Hodgson, Trans.). New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd.

Piaget, J. (1968). Six psychological studies. New York: Vintage Books. Pomerantz, E. M., Altermatt, E. R., & Saxon, J. L. (2002). Making the grade but feeling

distressed: Gender differences in academic performance and internal distress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 396–404. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.396

Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender and academic abilities and preferences. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology (pp. 297-316). New York: Springer.

Puts, D. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Burriss, R. P., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58(2), 282-289. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005

Puts, D. A., McDaniel, M. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Spatial ability and prenatal androgens: meta-analyses of congenital adrenal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D: 4D) studies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 100-111. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9271-3

Rabbitt, P., Donlan, C., Watson, P., McInnes, L., & Bent, N. (1995). Unique and interactive effects of depression, age, socioeconomic advantage, and gender on cognitive performance of normal healthy older people. Psychology and Aging, 10(3), 307-313. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.3.307

Rahman, Q., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). Large sexual-orientation-related differences in performance on mental rotation and judgement of line orientation tasks. Neuropsychology, 17(1), 25. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.17.1.25

Regard, M., Strauss, E., & Knapp, P. (1982). Children's production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55(3), 839-844.

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662. doi:10.1037/edu0000012

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2017). Investigating gender differences in mathematics and science: Results from the 2011 Trends in Mathematics and Science Survey. Research in Science Education, 1-26. doi:10.1007/s11165-017-9630-6

Reynolds, M. R., Scheiber, C., Hajovsky, D. B., Schwartz, B., & Kaufman, A. S. (2015). Gender differences in academic achievement: Is writing an exception to the gender similarities hypothesis? The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 176(4), 211-234. doi:10.1080/00221325.2015.1036833

Reznick, J. S., & Goldfield, B. A. (1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 406-413. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.3.406

Riegle-Crumb, C. (2005). The cross-national context of the gender gap in math and science. In L. V. Hedges & B. Schneider (Eds.), The social organization of schooling (pp. 227-243). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Ritter, D. (2004). Gender role orientation and performance on stereotypically feminine and masculine cognitive tasks. Sex Roles, 50(7), 583-591. doi:f10.1023/B:SERS.0000023077.91248.f7

Page 142: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 127

Robichon, F., Giraud, K., Berbon, M., & Habib, M. (1999). Sexual dimorphism in anterior speech region: An MRI study of cortical asymmetry and callosal size. Brain and Cognition, 40(1), 241-246.

Robinson, J. P., & Lubienski, S. T. (2011). The development of gender achievement gaps in mathematics and reading during elementary and middle school: Examining direct cognitive assessments and teacher ratings. American Educational Research Journal, 48(2), 268-302. doi:10.3102/0002831210372249

Roebuck, R., & Wilding, J. (1993). Effects of vowel variety and sample lenght on identification of a speaker in a line-up. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7(6), 475–481. doi:10.1002/acp.2350070603

Roivainen, E. (2011). Gender differences in processing speed: A review of recent research. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), 145-149. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.021

Rosenthal, R. (1984). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 59-82. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59

Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Royer, F. L. (1978). Sex differences in symbol-digit substitution task performance. Intelligence, 2(2), 145-151. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(78)90005-3

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, pp. 858-932). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1996). Benton Controlled Oral Word Association Test: Reliability and norms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(4), 329-338. doi: 10.1016/0887-6177(95)00033-x

Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seattle longitudinal study. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging, 4th ed. (pp. 266-286). San Diego: Academic Press.

Schaie, K. W., & Hertzog, C. (1983). Fourteen-year cohort-sequential analyses of adult intellectual development. Developmental Psychology, 19(4), 531.

Scheiber, C., Reynolds, M. R., Hajovsky, D. B., & Kaufman, A. S. (2015). Gender differences in achievement in a large, nationally representative sample of children and adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 52(4), 335-348. doi:10.1002/pits.21827

Schickedanz, J. A. (1973). The relationship of sex-typing of reading to reading achievement and reading choice behavior in elementary school boys. (PhD), University of Illinois, Urbana, IL. Retrieved from PProQuest Dissertations and Theses (302644654)

Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 440-452. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440

Serbin, L. A., Powlishta, K. K., Gulko, J., Martin, C. L., & Lockheed, M. E. (1993). The development of sex typing in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(2), 1-95.

Shackelford, T. K., Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), 447-458. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.023

Page 143: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 128

Shapiro, J. (1990). Sex-role appropriateness of reading and reading instruction. Reading Psychology, 11(3), 241-269. doi: 10.1080/0270271900110304

Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Constable, T., Skudlarskl, P., Fulbright, R. K., . . . Gore, J. C. (1995). Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language. Nature, 373, 607-609.

Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of the connecticut longitudinal study. JAMA, 264(8), 998-1002. doi:10.1001/jama.1990.03450080084036

Sherman, J. A. (1967). Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of intellectual functioning. Psychological Review, 74(4), 290-299. doi:10.1037/h0024723

Shettle, C., Rhep, S., Mordica, J., Perkins, R., Nord, C., Teodorovic, J., & Brown, J. (2007). The nation's report card: America's high school graduates—Results from the 2005 NAEP high school transcript study. National Center for Educational Statistics: Washington, DC.

Shields, S. A. (1975). Functionalism, Darwinism, and the psychology of women. American Psychologist, 30(7), 739-754. doi:10.1037/h0076948

Shields, S. A. (1982). The variability hypothesis: The history of a biological model of sex differences in intelligence. Signs, 7(4), 769-797. doi:10.2307/3173639

Siegal, M. (1987). Are sons and daughters treated more differently by fathers than by mothers? Developmental Review, 7(3), 183-209. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(87)90012-8

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 207-228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207

Signorella, M. L., Jamison, W., & Krupa, M. H. (1989). Predicting spatial performance from gender stereotyping in activity preferences and in self-concept. Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 89-95. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.89

Silverman, E.-M., & Zimmer, C. H. (1982). Demographic characteristics and treatment experiences of women and men who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 7(2), 273-285. doi:10.1016/0094-730X(82)90013-4

Silverman, I., Choi, J., & Peters, M. (2007). The Hunter-Gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: Data from 40 countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 261-268. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6

Silverman, I., & Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 533-550). New York: Oxford University Press.

Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longitudinal examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 70-83. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.70

Sincoff, J. B., & Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Two faces of verbal ability. Intelligence, 11(4), 263-276. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(87)90010-9

Smith, M. J. L., Deady, D. K., Sharp, M. A., & Al-Dujaili, E. A. (2013). Sex-role orientation in men is related to salivary testosterone levels. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 2013. doi:10.4236/jbbs.2013.37054

Sommer, I. E., Aleman, A., Bouma, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Do women really have more bilateral language representation than men? A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain, 127(8), 1845-1852. doi:10.1093/brain/awh207

Page 144: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 129

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science?: A critical review. American Psychologist, 60(9), 950-958. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950

Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1979). On assessing “androgyny”. Sex Roles, 5(6), 721-738. doi:10.1007/bf00287935

Stanley, J. C., Benbow, C. P., Brody, L., Dauber, S., & Lupkowski, A. (1992). Gender differences on eighty-six nationally standardized aptitude and achievement tests. In N. Colangelo, S. G. Assouline, & D. L. Ambroson (Eds.), Talent development: Proceedings from the 1991 Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development (pp. 42-65). Unionville, NY: Trillium Press.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613-629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613

Steele, C. M. (1998). Stereotyping and its threat are real. American Psychologist, 53(6), 680-681. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.6.680

Steffens, M., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereotypes regarding math and language: Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, but not for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64(5), 324-335. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x

Sternberg, R. J. (2014). Teaching about the nature of intelligence. Intelligence, 42, 176-179. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.08.010

Stevenson, B. (2010). Beyond the classroom: Using Title IX to measure the return to high school sports. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 92(2), 284-301. doi:10.1162/rest.2010.11623

Stevenson, J., & Richman, N. (2008). The prevalence of language delay in a population of three‐year‐old children and its association with general retardation. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 18(4), 431-441. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1976.tb03682.x

Storck, P. A., & Looft, W. R. (1973). Qualitative analysis of vocabulary responses from persons aged six to sixty-six plus. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65(2), 192-197. doi:10.1037/h0034907

Strand, S., Deary, I. J., & Smith, P. (2006). Sex differences in cognitive abilities test scores: A UK national picture. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(3), 463-480. doi:10.1348/000709905x50906

Stumpf, H. (1995). Gender differences in performance on tests of cognitive abilities: Experimental design issues and empirical results. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(4), 275-287. doi:10.1016/1041-6080(95)90002-0

Stumpf, H., & Jackson, D. N. (1994). Gender-related differences in cognitive abilities: Evidence from a medical school admissions testing program. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(3), 335-344. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90281-X

Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 21-36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.21

Szameitat, A. J., Hamaida, Y., Tulley, R. S., Saylik, R., & Otermans, P. C. J. (2015). “Women Are Better Than Men”–Public beliefs on gender differences and other aspects in multitasking. PLoS ONE, 10(10), e0140371. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0140371

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, mathematical, spatial and verbal intelligence: Four meta analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 493-504. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001

Page 145: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 130

Tao, H.-L., & Michalopoulos, C. (2017). Gender equality and the gender gap in mathematics. Journal of Biosocial Science, 1-17. doi:10.1017/S0021932017000141

Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (1998). Gender effects on Mexican-descent parents' questions and scaffolding during toy play: A sequential analysis. First Language, 18(53), 129-147. doi:10.1177/014272379801805301

Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and Animal Naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(2), 167-177. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4

Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245-1260. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245

Turner, M. A. (1983). Sex-role Identity Of Children And Its Relationship To The Sex-role Identity Of Parents And Teachers, And To Reading Interests, Attitudes Toward Reading, And Level Of Reading Proficiency. (Doctor of Education thesis), Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (Proquest ID 303270815)

Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Tanzman, M. S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. (pp. 279-332). Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Vernon, P. A. (1950). The structure of human abilities. New York: Wiley. Voracek, M., Pietschnig, J., Nader, I. W., & Stieger, S. (2011). Digit ratio (2D: 4D) and sex-

role orientation: Further evidence and meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 417-422. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.009

Voyer, D., & Jansen, P. (2017). Motor expertise and performance in spatial tasks: A meta-analysis. Human Movement Science, 54, 110-124. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2017.04.004

Voyer, D., Postma, A., Brake, B., & Imperato-McGinley, J. (2007). Gender differences in object location memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 23-38. doi:10.3758/BF03194024

Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. doi:10.1037/a0036620

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250-270. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250

Voyer, D., Voyer, S. D., & Saint-Aubin, J. (2017). Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 307-334. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1085-7

Wadnerkar, M. B., Cowell, P. E., & Whiteside, S. P. (2006). Speech across the menstrual cycle: A replication and extension study. Neuroscience Letters, 408(1), 21-24. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.07.032

Wallentin, M. (2009). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language, 108(3), 175-183. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.07.001

Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MA: Williams and Wilkins.

Weiss, E. M., Kemmler, G., Deisenhammer, E. A., Fleischhacker, W. W., & Delazer, M. (2003). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(4), 863-875. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00288-X

Page 146: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 131

Weiss, E. M., Ragland, J. D., Brensinger, C. M., Bilker, W. B., Deisenhammer, E. A., & Delazer, M. (2006). Sex differences in clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(04), 502-509.

West, R. L., Crook, T. H., & Barron, K. L. (1992). Everyday memory performance across the life span: effects of age and noncognitive individual differences. Psychology and Aging, 7(1), 72-82. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.7.1.72

Wilding, J., & Cook, S. (2000). Sex differences and individual consistency in voice identification. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91(2), 535-538. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.2.535

Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes : a multination study. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

Willingham, W., & Cole, N. S. (1997). Gender and fair assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Yairi, E., & Ambrose, N. (1992). Onset of stuttering in preschool children: Selected factors.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(4), 782-788. doi:10.1044/jshr.3504.782 Yarmey, A. D., Yarmey, A. L., Yarmey, M. J., & Parliament, L. (2001). Commonsense

beliefs and the identification of familiar voices. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(3), 283-299. doi:10.1002/acp.702

You, D., Maeda, Y., & Bebeau, M. J. (2011). Gender differences in moral sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Ethics & Behavior, 21(4), 263-282. doi: 10.1080/10508422.2011.585591

Young, R., & Sweeting, H. (2004). Adolescent bullying, relationships, psychological well-being, and gender-atypical behavior: A gender diagnosticity approach. Sex Roles, 50(7), 525-537. doi:10.1023/b:sers.0000023072.53886.86

Page 147: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 132

Chapter 3 – Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

This chapter provides a literature review on sex differences in visual-spatial

ability, and contains supplementary material to the general literature review in Chapter 2.

This work has been published as :

Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2017). Gender differences in spatial ability:

Implications for STEM education and approaches to reducing the gender

gap for parents and educators. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Visual-Spatial Ability:

Transforming Research into Practice (pp. 195-224). Switzerland: Springer

International.

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, Springer

Nature. In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship of this paper. I

acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Conducting literature review Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 148: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 133

Chapter 4 – Sex Differences in Mathematics and Science Achievement

This chapter reports on a meta-analysis of archival data from a large nationally

representative assessment of student achievement in the domains of mathematics and

science conducted in the United States. Student achievement in Grades 4, 8, and 12

have been periodically assessed across a sufficiently long time period that it is possible

to test for temporal trends such as the predicted decline in gender gaps with changes in

the relative status of men and women in society.

This chapter includes a co-authored paper that has been published as :

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and

science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress

assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662.

doi: 10.1037/edu0000012

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, American

Psychological Association. In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the

Responsible Conduct of Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship

of this paper. I acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 149: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 134

Chapter 5 – Sex Differences in Reading and Writing

This chapter reports on a meta-analysis of archival data from a large nationally

representative assessment of student achievement in reading and writing conducted in

the United States as part of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Student

achievement in Grades 4, 8, and 12 have been periodically assessed across a sufficiently

long time period that it is possible to test for temporal trends such as the predicted

decline in gender gaps with changes in the relative status of men and women in society.

Large and pervasive sex differences were found for writing achievement, especially

gender ratios at the tails. Some somewhat smaller sex differences were found for

reading, and there are twice as many boys as girls failing to attain basic literacy at the

lower left-tail of the ability distribution.

This chapter includes a co-authored paper that has been published as :

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2019). Gender differences in reading and

writing achievement: Evidence from the National Assessment of Educational

Progress (NAEP). American Psychologist. 74(4), 445-458. doi:

10.1037/amp0000356S

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, American

Psychological Association. In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the

Responsible Conduct of Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship

of this paper. I acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann Copyright (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 150: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 135

Chapter 6 – Cross-Cultural Patterns of Reading, Mathematics and Science Literacy

This chapter reports on a meta-analysis of student testing data from the 2009

wave of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), a large-scale

educational assessment of student’s reading, mathematics and science literacy across all

OECD members and a number of partner nations. This study reports data from 65

nations. Consistently across all nations, girls outperform boys in reading literacy,

d = -.44. Boys outperform girls in mathematics in the USA, d = +.22 and across OECD

nations, d = +.13. For science literacy, while the USA showed the largest gender

difference across all OECD nations, d = +.14, gender differences across OECD nations

were non-significant, and a small female advantage was found for non-OECD nations, d

= -.09. Across all three domains, these differences were more pronounced at both tails

of the distribution for low- and high-achievers. Considerable cross-cultural variability

was also observed, and national gender differences were correlated with gender equity

measures, economic prosperity, and Hofstede’s cultural dimension of power distance.

Educational and societal implications of such gender gaps are addressed, as well as the

mechanisms by which gender differences in cognitive abilities are culturally mediated.

It has been published as has been published as

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7),

e39904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Copyright statement :

It was published in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution (CC_BY)

license, and copyright was retained by the author.

Page 151: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 136

Chapter 7 – Meta-Analysis of Sex-Role Mediation Effect for Visual-Spatial Ability

This study reports a meta-analysis of the sex-role mediation effect for visual-

spatial ability. This chapter includes a co-authored paper that has been published as :

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-

analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521-535. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, Springer.

In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship of this paper. I

acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann

Page 152: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 137

Meta-Analysis Summary and Prelude to Empirical Studies

The previous set of chapters sought to examine whether the previously observed

sex differences in specific cognitive abilities (verbal and quantitative reasoning) would

still exist in contemporary samples, or alternatively, if they’d been eliminated as

claimed by Feingold (1988), Hyde (2005), as well as Caplan and Caplan (1997, 2016).

Additionally, it sought to contextualise that difference by evaluating the magnitude of

observed sex differences, and determine if they were large enough to have practical

importance. For quantitative reasoning, small but not trivial mean sex differences were

found for mathematics and more substantial gender gaps in high achievers. Somewhat

larger mean sex differences were found for science achievement, and again a sharp

disparity in the tail ratios for high achievers. For verbal and language abilities,

substantial sex differences were found for reading and writing with a developmental

trend observed with age/years of schooling. Examination of tail ratios also showed

substantial gender gaps in low- and high- achievers.

Collectively these studies provided a rationale for further investigation to

evaluate support for the sex-role mediation hypothesis. Signorella and Jamison (1986)

had conducted a meta-analysis on the association between masculinity and visual-

spatial ability, but the literature was now dated. For this reason, we produced a meta-

analysis of the association between masculine sex-role identification and visual-spatial

ability with more recently collected data. Additionally, this was useful in

contextualising the expected effect size for statistical power calculations in the

empirical study that follows.

Page 153: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 138

Chapter 8 – Empirical Study 1 – Sex and Sex-Role Differences in Specific

Cognitive Abilities

“If women are expected to do the same work as men, we must teach them the

same things.” – Plato, The Republic.

This study reports the empirical study into sex and sex-role differences in verbal

and visual-spatial abilities. Specifically it tests Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation

hypothesis in a modern sample, finding support for both predicted tranches (verbal and

visual-spatial ability) across a range of tasks. This has been published as:

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in

specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147-158. doi:

10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, Elsevier.

In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship of this paper. I

acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18

Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 154: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 139

Chapter 9 – Empirical Study 2 – Effect of Task-Labelling, Stereotype Threat, and

Sex-Role Identification on Cognitive Performance

“We see the world, not as it is, but as we are──or, as we are conditioned to see it.”

Stephen R. Covey, The 7 Habits of Highly Effective People

This study reports an empirical study investigating the effect of task labelling,

stereotype threat induction, and sex-role identification on cognitive performance in

visual-spatial and verbal ability tasks in a sample of 150 women. It also concurrently

measures sex-role identification in participants, in order to replicate the findings of

Chapter 8.

Page 155: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 140

“What does my performance on this test say about me?” : Effect of Task-Labelling, Stereotype Threat, and Sex-Role Identification on Cognitive Performance

Abstract

Sex differences in cognitive ability are explained by the sex-role mediation

hypothesis as arising from the development of sex-typed personality traits and

behaviors. Other researchers claim sex differences in latent ability do not exist,

but instead reflect diminished performance in the face of stereotype threat or

gender conformity pressures. A sample of 150 women was recruited to investigate

the effect of task-labelling, stereotype threat and sex-role identification on

cognitive performance. Initially the women were randomly assigned to either a

masculine or feminine task-labelling condition before completing a spatial

visualization task. Next, the women were randomly assigned to either a stereotype

threat or control condition, and then completed a mental rotation and verbal

fluency task. Results on visual-spatial tasks showed effects of task-labelling and

stereotype threat, as well as sex-role differences consistent with the sex-role

mediation hypothesis. Additionally, sex-role differences were found for a verbal

fluency task but there was no effect of stereotype lift. The results suggest that the

sex-role mediation effect observed in previous studies for visual-spatial tasks

reflects an enduring trait, but can be moderated by task-labelling and salience of

gender stereotypes.

Keywords: sex differences, stereotype threat, sex-role mediation hypothesis, spatial ability, verbal ability, gender priming

Page 156: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 141

“What does my performance on this test say about me?” : Effect of Task-Labelling, Stereotype Threat, and Sex-Role Identification on Cognitive Performance

The topic of sex differences in cognitive abilities has commanded the interest of

psychologists and researchers since the beginning of our field. It has also captured the

curiosity of parents, educators, and the media, due to the important educational and

social implications (Eagly, 1996; Halpern, 1997). The social significance of sex

differences include the underrepresentation of women in science and technology fields

(Carli, Alawa, Lee, Zhao, & Kim, 2016; Halpern et al., 2007), disparities between boys

and girls on standardized tests of reading and writing (Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Reilly,

2012; Reilly, Neumann & Andrews, 2018), as well as putative claims that men and

women are inherently “different” and would benefit from single-sex education

environments (for a critical review of evidence see Halpern et al., 2011).

While sex differences in most types of cognitive ability are relatively small in

magnitude (Hyde, 2005), two exceptions to this general rule are verbal and visual-

spatial abilities (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974).On average females score higher on tasks

involving language and verbal ability while males tend to score higher on tasks of

visual-spatial ability (Halpern, 2011; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). However, there is

vigorous debate amongst researchers over the extent to which the observed differences

reflect biological and psychosocial factors, necessitating the need for further research.

Additionally, it is unclear whether observed differences reflect actual sex differences in

latent ability or rather instead are an artefact of the testing environment and situational

factors (Massa, Mayer, & Bohon, 2005), such as stereotype threat (Flore & Wicherts,

2015; Steele, 1998).

One line of enquiry into the causes of sex differences highlights the role of

individual differences in sex-role identification on the performance of gender-typed

Page 157: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 142

cognitive tasks. The sex-role mediation hypothesis proposes that sex differences

between males and females on cognitive tests stem from the development of

stereotypically masculine and feminine personality traits and behaviours (Nash, 1979;

Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2016). Although the early socialization experiences of

boys and girls typically differ (Leaper & Friedman, 2007), there is also considerable

individual variation in the degree to which children acquire stereotypically masculine

and feminine personality traits, beliefs, and behaviours – a process referred to as sex-

typing (Kohlberg, 1966; Martin & Ruble, 2004). Highly sex-typed persons are

motivated to keep their behaviour and self-concept consistent with traditional gender

norms (Bem & Lenney, 1976), including performance in academic domains (Carli et al.,

2016; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Steffens & Jelenec, 2011). Others may

integrate aspects of both masculine and feminine identification into their personality,

termed psychological androgyny (Bem, 1984; Spence & Buckner, 2000).

Many persons acquire stereotypically masculine or feminine sex-roles consistent

with their biological gender, resulting in group differences between males and females

being observed. However the sex-mediation hypothesis also offers an explanation for

individual differences in performance, which is important because within-gender

variability on cognitive tasks is often greater than between-gender variability (Halpern

et al., 2007). Several studies have investigated sex-role identity and cognitive

performance (e.g. Saucier, McCreary, & Saxberg, 2002). Few studies have examined

interactions between sex-role identification and situational factors of the testing

environment such as the perceived sex-role appropriateness of a task, or activation of

stereotype threat. The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of sex-role

identification, and its relationship to these two intrapersonal factors on women’s

cognitive performance.

Page 158: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 143

Overview of Gender Differences in Specific Cognitive Abilities

While it is generally regarded that men and women do not differ in general

intelligence (Jensen, 1998; Neisser et al., 1996), sex differences do exist for more

specific cognitive abilities. The largest sex differences are found in visual-spatial ability

where males as a group perform better than females (Voyer et al., 1995). But there are

also appreciable differences in verbal fluency (Gauthier, Duyme, Zanca, & Capron,

2009; Weiss, Kemmler, Deisenhammer, Fleischhacker, & Delazer, 2003) and language

tasks such as reading and writing (Berninger, Nielsen, Abbott, Wijsman, & Raskind,

2008; Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Reilly, 2012), where females score significantly higher than

males on average.

These two domains are important to cultivate because visual-spatial ability

predicts later development of mathematical and scientific aptitude (Duffy, Sorby,

Nozaki, & Bowe, 2016; Kersh, Casey, & Young, 2008; Verdine, Golinkoff, Hirsh-

Pasek, & Newcombe, 2017; Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009), while verbal and

language proficiency are crucial skills for occupational success and higher

socioeconomic status (Kutner et al., 2007; Ritchie & Bates, 2013). Females on average

score slightly lower in standardized tests of mathematics and science (Ganley &

Lubienski, 2016; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2015), with more pronounced gender

gaps in the proportion of high achievers at the upper-right-tail of the ability distribution

(Wai, Cacchio, Putallaz, & Makel, 2010). Males also score lower than females on

standardized tests of reading and writing (Lietz, 2006; Willingham & Cole, 1997). But

there is also considerable variability in performance within both males and females as

groups. For example, some men score poorly on tests of mathematics and science, while

some women score considerably higher than their male peers on such tests. Halpern et

al. (2007) observed that within-gender variability is, in fact, greater than between-gender

Page 159: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 144

variability. Any theoretical explanation for the origin of sex differences in cognitive

ability should thus consider the role that individual differences play in the emergence of

group differences between males and females.

Sex-Role Mediation as an Explanation for Sex differences

Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis proposes that within-gender and

between-gender variability arises as a result of differences in sex-role identification

when performing stereotypically masculine or feminine cognitive tasks. Specifically,

Nash (1979) theorized that masculine identification leads to the cultivation of visual-

spatial ability, while feminine identification promotes the acquisition of verbal and

language abilities (see Figure 9.1). Sex-role identification occurs early during childhood

and represents an enduring personality trait that is relatively stable over time (Hyde,

Krajnik, & Skuldt-Niederberger, 1991; Martin & Ruble, 2004). While there have been

significant changes in the relative roles of men and women (Donnelly et al., 2015),

recent research shows the stability of gendered stereotypes over time (Haines, Deaux, &

Lofaro, 2016).

Figure 9.1. Sex-role mediation theory of cognitive abilities. Masculine sex-role identification

is associated with increased spatial experiences while feminine sex-role identification

provides additional opportunities to develop verbal and language proficiency.

Feminine sex-roles (expressive/communal traits)

Sex

Masculine sex-roles (instrumental/agentic traits)

Differential practice and

sex-typing of activities

Visual-spatial ability

Verbal ability

Differential practice and

sex-typing of activities

Page 160: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 145

Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis was based on earlier work by

Sherman (1967) and others into how sex-role identification can result in differential

learning and practice experiences in early childhood, as well as self-selection of hobbies

and interests that promote either visual-spatial or verbal ability. It also reflected research

into the self-concept and gender-schema theory, which argued that rigidly sex-typed

individuals are highly motivated to keep their actions consistent with their self-concept.

Thus they may show less motivation to persevere in the face of difficult and challenging

content when performing a sex-typed cognitive task that is incompatible with their self-

concept, and may be more susceptible to negative gender stereotypes about ability and

overt gender bias (LaCosse, Sekaquaptewa, & Bennett, 2016; Robnett, 2015). In doing

so, Nash extended Sherman’s work to place cognitive development in a social context,

where appraisal of task characteristics, prior practice and experience with the task, and

knowledge of gender stereotypes about ability may all contribute to sex-typed cognitive

abilities.

The sex-role mediation hypothesis also predicts individual differences in actual

ability, due to self-selection of stereotypically masculine and feminine leisure activities

and intellectual interests. For example, engagement in stereotypically masculine leisure

activities such as construction block play, video gaming, and model-making in

childhood and adolescence is correlated with adult performance on visual-spatial tasks

(Baenninger & Newcombe, 1995). Feminine sex-role identification is associated with

more positive attitudes to reading (McGeown, Goodwin, Henderson, & Wright, 2011),

and to engagement in activities such as reading for leisure, creative writing and journal

keeping (Athenstaedt, Mikula, & Bredt, 2009; McHale, Kim, Whiteman, & Crouter,

2004). These activities provide further opportunities to practice and cultivate specific

Page 161: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 146

cognitive skills. Individual differences in sex-role identification may lead to differential

levels of practice and experience with sex-typed cognitive tasks.

Several decades have passed since Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis

was first proposed. There have been changes in the status and roles of women during

this period as well as changes in cultural perceptions of masculinity and femininity

(Auster & Ohm, 2000). These changes raise the question of whether earlier research

findings will be replicated with modern samples (Eagly & Diekman, 2003). Signorella

and Jamison (1986) found a positive association between masculine sex-roles and

visual-spatial performance in a meta-analysis of research findings. Subsequently, a

meta-analysis by Reilly and Neumann (2013) examined the contribution of masculine

sex-roles to mental rotation performance and found the relationship still holds with

modern samples. However, few studies had examined the contribution of femininity to

verbal ability and language tasks.

More recently an empirical study by Reilly, Neumann, and Andrews (2016)

tested the sex-role mediation hypothesis across a range of visual-spatial and verbal

language tasks. Masculinity was associated with higher performance for visual-spatial

tasks, while femininity predicted performance on verbal and language tasks. The

researchers employed tests of statistical mediation, finding that sex-role identification

was a mediator between sex and performance on cognitive tasks. While such research

documents the sex-role mediation effect in modern samples, it leaves unanswered the

question of whether other factors might also contribute to the observed sex differences

in performance. Two situational factors previously shown to affect performance on sex-

typed cognitive tests are task-labelling and stereotype threat.

Page 162: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 147

Sex-typing of Cognitive Tests and Task-labelling

One explanation for reduced performance may be a perceived conflict between

the sex-typing of a cognitive task as inherently masculine or feminine, and the

individual’s sex-role identification. Kagan (1964) noted that from an early age, children

begin to classify everyday tasks and behaviours as being either masculine or feminine.

Reading is stereotypically regarded as feminine while visual-spatial and mathematical

tasks are perceived as masculine (Hyde & Lindberg, 2007; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood,

Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002). When attempting to complete a sex-typed task that is

incompatible with one’s sex-role identification, the test-taker may be less motivated and

may be easily discouraged when encountering difficult material.

Two studies have attempted to manipulate individuals’ appraisals of cognitive

tasks through labelling and to examine the effect on performance. Brosnan (1998) had

participants complete the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT; Witkin, 1971), after

providing them with one of two sets of instructions – one that emphasized the visual-

spatial nature of the task, and the other that described it as a test of empathy and

perspective-taking. The ambiguous nature of the test and novelty of the stimuli gave

such descriptions face validity. Although there was no effect of labelling in males,

females in the empathy condition performed significantly better than those in the spatial

condition. A second study by Massa, Mayer, and Bohon (2005) replicated the labelling

effect on GEFT performance in a female college-aged sample, and also found that

motivation was higher when the test was portrayed as being feminine in nature.

Gender Stereotypes, and Stereotype Threat

While the sex-role appropriateness of the task may well be a factor in general

performance (e.g. Massa et al., 2005), an alternate explanation for reduced performance

is that by describing the task as measuring visual-spatial ability, knowledge of gender

Page 163: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 133

Chapter 4 – Sex Differences in Mathematics and Science Achievement

This chapter reports on a meta-analysis of archival data from a large nationally

representative assessment of student achievement in the domains of mathematics and

science conducted in the United States. Student achievement in Grades 4, 8, and 12

have been periodically assessed across a sufficiently long time period that it is possible

to test for temporal trends such as the predicted decline in gender gaps with changes in

the relative status of men and women in society.

This chapter includes a co-authored paper that has been published as :

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and

science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress

assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662.

doi: 10.1037/edu0000012

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, American

Psychological Association. In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the

Responsible Conduct of Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship

of this paper. I acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 164: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

195© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2017 M.S. Khine (ed.), Visual-spatial Ability in STEM Education, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-44385-0_10

Chapter 10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability: Implications for STEM Education and Approaches to Reducing the Gender Gap for Parents and Educators

David Reilly , David L. Neumann , and Glenda Andrews

10.1 Introduction

10.1.1 Overview of Gender Differences

The existence of gender differences in cognitive ability is a controversial topic. Nevertheless, researchers in psychological and the social sciences widely acknowl-edge that males and females differ in spatial ability (Halpern and Collaer 2005 ; Kimura 2000 ). Indeed, it is one of the most robust and consistently found phenom-enon of all cognitive gender differences (Halpern 2011 ; Voyer et al. 1995 ). While there is individual variability within each gender, on average males score higher than females on tests that measure visual-spatial ability. However, there is consider-able debate over just how large the differences between males and females are. Researchers also differ in their perspectives on the origins of the gender differences, including the relative contributions of biological, social and cultural factors. This chapter provides an overview of the research literature, as well as covering the developmental and educational implications for children.

Many researchers posit that early expertise in spatial ability in children lays down a foundation for the development of quantitative reasoning, a collective term encompassing science and mathematics. These researchers argue that the early dif-ferences in spatial ability have important implications for student achievement in STEM (science, technology, engineering and mathematics) subjects, and may par-tially explain the underrepresentation of women in science. However, while some

D. Reilly (*) Griffi th University , Southport , QLD , Australia e-mail: d.reilly@griffi th.edu.au

D. L. Neumann • G. Andrews Griffi th University , Southport , QLD , Australia

Menzies Health Institute Queensland , Southport , QLD , Australia

Page 165: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

196

children may be naturally gifted in spatial ability, there is a large body of research showing that spatial profi ciency can be improved through relatively brief interven-tions. A growing number of educational psychologists have argued that early educa-tion of spatial intelligence is necessary as a matter of equity for all students, and that it may offer substantial benefi ts for the later development of mathematical and sci-entifi c skills across all ability levels (Halpern et al. 2007 ). We review interventions aimed at increasing spatial aptitude, and the role of parents and teachers in encour-aging the development of these abilities.

10.1.2 What Is Spatial Ability?

The term “spatial ability” (also referred to in some research as visuospatial or visual-spatial ability) encompasses a range of different skills and operations, so it is important to clearly defi ne the term. Laypeople can sometimes use the term very loosely, covering anything from block building assembly to reading maps and navi-gating one’s way around the city streets. Such tasks often incorporate additional (non-spatial) processes, including memory and general problem solving skills. Psychologists and cognitive researchers apply the term spatial ability to tasks that are intended to measure specifi c cognitive processes in isolation. Linn and Petersen ( 1985 , p. 1482) defi ned spatial ability as the “skill in representing, transforming, generating and recalling symbolic, non-linguistic information”. More generally, it is the ability to perceive and understand spatial relationships, to visualize spatial stim-uli such as objects, and to manipulate or transform them in some way – such as mentally rotating an object to imagine what it might look like viewed from a differ-ent angle or perspective. Spatial ability is crucial to a wide variety of traditional occupations including architecture, interior decorating, drafting, aviation, as well as a growing number of new and emerging occupations in the science and technology fi elds.

Spatial ability encompasses a broad range of cognitive processes, with the size of gender differences varying depending on the type of task (Voyer et al. 1995 ). When measuring spatial ability, some tasks measure global spatial skills such as wayfi nd-ing and navigation in virtual environments or outside the laboratory (Lawton and Kallai 2002 ). More commonly, specially designed tasks are employed to tap one or more spatial components in isolation. Linn and Petersen ( 1985 ), in a pioneering review of the literature, outlined three distinct categories of spatial ability. Firstly, we have spatial perception , which involves perceiving spatial relationships. A com-monly employed task of spatial perception is Piagetian Water Level Task, which requires individuals to draw the waterline on a variety of containers or bottles that have been tilted a certain number of degrees (see Fig. 10.1 ). Another is the Judgment of Line Angle and Position test (JLAP), which requires subjects to correctly judge the orientation of a series of tilted lines (see Fig. 10.2 ).

The second category of spatial tasks is mental rotation . Tasks measuring mental rotation involve requiring individuals to mentally rotate spatial objects to see how they would look from a different angle or perspective (see Fig. 10.3 ). Mental rotation

D. Reilly et al.

Page 166: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

197

tasks usually involve three dimensional stimuli (Kimura 2000 ), though some tasks use less complex two dimensional stimuli (Prinzel and Freeman 1995 ).

The third category of spatial ability is spatial visualization which involve more complicated multistep manipulations of spatial information in order to reach a solu-tion. These tasks often incorporate some element of spatial perception and mental rotation. They are distinguished by having multiple solution strategies for reaching a solution. Common tests of spatial visualization include the Embedded Figures

Fig. 10.1 In the Piaget water level task (Vasta and Liben 1996 ), subjects are presented with a container of liquid ( left ), with varying quantities of fl uid. The container is then tilted adjacent to the horizontal plane. Subjects must then draw a line to indicate the probable water line in each of these containers

1

2

3 7

8

9

64 5

Fig. 10.2 Representative stimuli for judgement of the Judgment of Line Angle and Position test (JLAP; Collaer et al. 2007 ). Subjects must match the orientation of stimuli lines ( left ) to a refer-ence array ( right ). The correct answers from left to right are 2, 4 and 9

Fig. 10.3 Sample stimuli from the Vandenberg mental rotation task (Vandenberg and Kuse 1978 ). Subjects must locate both instances of the target shape ( left ) amongst the four possible choices. Two of the choices are mirror image distractors. To answer the question correctly, both targets must be located. The correct answer is 1 and 3 (From Peters and Battista ( 2008 ). Used by permission)

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 167: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

198

Test (EFT; see Fig. 10.4 ), which requires individuals to search for a target shape within a more complex picture of geometric shapes and to ignore distracting visual information. Another task is the Paper Folding task, which requires individuals to visualize how a sheet of paper would appear if it were folded in a certain way and then one or more holes were punched through the folded sheet. Individuals must indicate how the unfurled paper would appear and indicate the position of dots from a series of possible answers (see Fig. 10.5 ).

Some researchers have proposed a fourth category called spatiotemporal ability , which involves making time-to-arrival judgments or tracking the movement of an object through space (Hunt et al. 1988 ). Such tasks are computer administered in order to accurately measure response times and determine whether there are dis-crepancies between projected and actual arrival time (see Fig. 10.6 ). Other tasks involve directing the path of multiple objects concurrently (see Fig. 10.7 ; Contreras et al. 2001 , 2007 ). However, it is unclear whether the gender difference observed with these tasks is necessarily spatial in nature, because there is some evidence that

Target shape Stimuli Item

Target shape Stimuli Item

Fig. 10.4 Spatial visualization items representative of those used in embedded fi gures tasks (Witkin 1971 ). Subjects are asked to locate a target shape (shown on the left ) within a more com-plex picture ( right )

D. Reilly et al.

Page 168: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

199

males are more accurate in time perception generally (Hancock and Rausch 2010 ; Rammsayer and Lustnauer 1989 ).

10.1.3 Statistical Methods for Evaluating Gender Differences in Research

Experiments in psychology make heavy use of sampling, as it would be impractical to collect a measurement from every member of a given target population.

When a suffi ciently large number of people are recruited, statistical tests can be performed to determine the probability that the observed group differences are due to chance, or whether they are likely to be found again if the experiment was repeated. If the probability that the results of the study occurred by chance is very low, the result is said to be statistically signifi cant . Because research involves vol-unteer participants giving up their valuable time, and the time of the investigator to supervise data collection, researchers generally seek to minimise the number of participants involved. When extremely small sample sizes are recruited for a study

Fig. 10.5 Representative stimuli for a paper folding task (French et al. 1963 ). On the left, we have a blank sheet of paper with the fold line indicated ( top-left ). A hole is punched through the folded sheet of paper ( bottom-left ), and then subjects are asked to indentify which of the choices would represent the unfurled paper. Correct answer is d)

Fig. 10.6 An example of dynamic spatial ability task proposed by Hunt et al. ( 1988 ) requires subjects to judge the velocity of a target object as it moves behind an obscured view, and to press a key when they believe the object will emerge

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 169: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

200

it may be lacking in statistical power (the ability to detect a statistically signifi cant effect in a given sample, if indeed the effect in question is genuine). Furthermore, samples may differ in important characteristics, such as age, socioeconomic status, level of education, which may affect the study outcomes, serving to increase or diminish the magnitude of any group differences between males and females. By pooling the data from many studies, statistical power is increased and the researcher can arrive at a more reliable estimate of the true size of a given effect than could be reached from any individual study.

Meta-analysis is a statistical technique employed to summarize research fi ndings across studies. Meta-analysis uses statistical methods to quantify effects across studies in an open and transparent manner, rather than simply comparing the tally of positive to negative studies (referred to as ‘vote counting’) or presenting a subjec-tive interpretation of the scientifi c literature. For example, a selective review of spatial literature by Caplan et al. ( 1985 ) made the surprising claim that gender dif-ferences in spatial ability were diminishing and were no longer reliably found. A subsequent meta-analysis by Linn and Petersen ( 1985 ) provided strong quantitative evidence in a review of the entire published literature of the time that refuted such claims. Statistical techniques and software have advanced suffi ciently in recent times so that it is now possible to test additional hypotheses about potential modera-tors, such as whether gender differences are diminishing in size across decades, or

Target

Turn left Turn right Turn left Turn right

Black DotControls

White DotControls

Fig. 10.7 Dynamic spatial ability requires subjects to steer two concurrently moving objects to a fi xed destination point by clicking on the turn left and turn right buttons. Arrows show motion path of the black and white dots . Representative of the Spatial Orientation Dynamic Test – Revised (SODT-R; Contreras et al. 2007 )

D. Reilly et al.

Page 170: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

201

whether gender differences are present at certain developmental ages (such as child-hood and adolescence).

When comparing two groups (such as males and females), the size of the effect in question is represented using a metric. A commonly used metric is Cohen’s d , which represents the mean difference between two groups divided by the pooled standard deviation. The use of a common metric facilitates comparisons across dif-ferent types of tests and samples, in a way that just reporting the mean difference could not. Cohen ( 1988 ) offered a set of guidelines for interpreting the magnitude of these group differences, suggesting that an effect size of d < 0.20 could be consid-ered a “small” effect, values of approximately 0.50 could be considered medium in size, and values of 0.80 or greater would be considered large in magnitude. These benchmarks offer even the non-statistician assistance in determining whether the effect in question is practically signifi cant , holding research to a higher standard than statistical signifi cance alone.

10.1.4 How Large Are Gender Differences in Spatial Ability?

The meta-analytic review conducted by Voyer et al. ( 1995 ) represented the most comprehensive meta-analysis of the research on gender differences in spatial ability published at that time. The review categorised tasks by age, comparing children (under 13 years), adolescents (13–18 years), and adults (over 18 years). Mental rotation tasks showed the largest gender differences ( d = 0.33 for children, d = 0.45 for adolescents and d = 0.66 in adults) followed by spatial perception ( d = 0.33 for children, d = 0 .43 for adolescents and d = 0.48 in adults). Spatial visualization showed the smallest gender differences ( d = 0.02 in children, growing to 18 for ado-lescents and d = 0.23 in adults). By Cohen’s guidelines, these would be medium- sized gender differences for mental rotation and spatial perception and in the case of spatial visualization tasks, relatively small. Contrary to earlier claims (e.g. Caplan et al. 1985 ), there is little substantive evidence that gender differences in visual spatial ability have greatly diminished over time though. Furthermore the gender differences follow a developmental progression from relatively small gender differ-ences in childhood towards much larger gender differences in adolescence and adulthood. Though a meta-analysis has not yet been conducted on the type of spatial task called spatiotemporal ability, effect sizes in such studies typically fall in the medium to large range also (Halpern 2000 ).

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 171: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

202

10.1.5 When Are Gender Differences in Spatial Ability First Observed?

Gender differences in spatial ability are observed early. Children in primary school show meaningful differences across a range of spatial tasks including mental rota-tion and spatial transformation (Lachance and Mazzocco 2006 ; Levine et al. 1999 ). Indeed, some studies have even observed small sex differences in young infants when simplifi ed tests of spatial reasoning are employed (Moore and Johnson 2008 ; Quinn and Liben 2008 ). However, the gender gap in spatial ability does appear to widen around the time of puberty, which some had claimed supported arguments for a biological and hormonal contribution. Correlation by itself does not necessarily prove causation though, as there may be other factors that co-vary with puberty. For example, as developmental researchers would also point out, this is time of increased gender conformity and strengthening of sex-roles (Ruble et al. 2006 ), as well as greater gender differentiation in play and leisure activities which provide opportuni-ties to practise spatial skill (Baenninger and Newcombe 1989 ). Even after puberty the gender gap continues to widen, with somewhat larger effect sizes found in adults than adolescents. There is evidence that input and practice is required to fully develop spatial ability (Baenninger and Newcombe 1995 ), and the increase noted in puberty and in later adulthood may refl ect the accumulation of social infl uences across time rather than the infl uence of hormonal changes.

10.2 Spatial Ability and Quantitative Reasoning

Spatial ability is thought to underpin the development of quantitative reasoning skills such as mathematics and science (Nuttall et al. 2005 ; Uttal et al. 2013b ), which are important educational objectives. Factor analysis (a statistical technique used to investigate the relationship between tests) of cognitive ability tests show high loading for mathematical performance against a spatial factor (Bornstein 2011 ; Carrol 1993 ; Halpern 2000 ). Wai et al. ( 2009 ) note that a large body of research over the course of over 50 years has established that spatial ability plays a crucial role in stimulating the development of quantitative reasoning skills. For example, spatial reasoning is important for understanding diagrams of complex scientifi c concepts and principals, but individual differences in spatial ability predict learning outcomes with such media in physics and chemistry (Höffl er 2010 ; Kozhevnikov et al. 2007 ; Wu and Shah 2004 ). When engaging in complex problem-solving tasks in science and mathematics, students who use spatial imagery and diagrams perform better than students using verbal strategies (Spelke 2005 ), and growth in spatial working memory is positively correlated with mathematics profi ciency (Li and Geary 2013 ).

Furthermore, performance on measures of spatial ability are predictive of future scholastic achievement in mathematics and science, even many years later (Uttal et al. 2013b ). Shea et al. ( 2001 ) reported the results of a 20 year longitudinal study

D. Reilly et al.

Page 172: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

203

that followed children from seventh grade through to the age of 33. They found that individual differences in spatial ability measured in adolescence predicted educa-tional and vocational outcomes two decades later, even after controlling for pre- existing mathematical and verbal abilities.

Another study by Casey et al. ( 1995 ) examined a large sample of U.S. adoles-cents preparing to sit the Mathematics Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT-M) for college entry, an important prerequisite for entry into further education in mathematics and science. Performance on the Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task successfully pre-dicted SAT-M entrance scores, even after controlling for general scholastic ability (Casey et al. 1995 ). Although still signifi cant for males, the relationship between spatial ability and mathematics achievement was stronger for females suggesting that girls may be particularly disadvantaged by defi cits in spatial reasoning. Casey et al. suggest that spatial ability acts as an important mediator in the gender gap in STEM achievement. Furthermore, they found that higher spatial ability was associ-ated with greater self-effi cacy beliefs about learning mathematics (Casey et al. 1997 ). Attitudes may exert a powerful infl uence on whether students decide to undertake further classes in mathematics and science (Ferguson et al. 2015 ; Simpkins et al. 2006 ), suggesting that there may be motivational effects as well as cognitive effects when spatial competencies are improved.

10.2.1 Importance of Spatial Ability for STEM

Educators, scientists, and policy makers acknowledge the importance of increasing mathematical and science literacy profi ciencies for students generally. There is also evidence to suggest that the early gender differences in spatial ability may contrib-ute to the later emergence of gender differences in mathematics and science (Ceci et al. 2009 ; Wai et al. 2009 ). Examination of historical scholastic achievement scores in the U.S. by Hedges and Nowell ( 1995 ) found that males, on average, have higher achievement scores in mathematics and science. Furthermore, when we examine the extreme right tail of the ability distribution, the gender gap is consider-ably larger. More recently, studies on data from the federal National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) in the United States replicated these fi ndings. For example, Reilly et al. ( 2015 ) observed small but stable mean gender differences in mathematics and science achievement and that at the higher levels of achievement boys outnumber girls by a ratio of 2:1 (Reilly et al. 2015 ). However gender gaps in maths and science are not inevitable. International assessments of educational achievement fi nd that in some countries, females actually outperform males to a signifi cant degree in mathematics and science (Else-Quest et al. 2010 ; Guiso et al. 2008 ; Reilly 2012 ).

A number of researchers have proposed that in order to address the gender gap in mathematics and science achievement, it is necessary to fi rst address the gender gap in spatial ability (Halpern 2007 ; Newcombe 2007 ). Fortunately spatial ability is not a fi xed and immutable trait (see the section “Interventions for Training of Spatial Ability”). In a review of educational research on gender difference, Hyde and

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 173: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

204

Lindberg ( 2007 ) argued that even a mild increase in spatial ability might have “mul-tiplier effects in girls’ mathematical and science performance” (Hyde and Lindberg 2007 , p. 29). This is an important goal as a matter of gender equity, but we can also see substantial improvements of training for males as well. In a review of the devel-opmental and educational research on spatial ability and STEM and the American educational system, Uttal et al. ( 2013b ) argue that including spatial thinking in the science curriculum could substantially increase the number of students capable of pursuing STEM careers. Given that in many developed countries there are shortages within STEM occupations, addressing spatial profi ciency in early education may be an important tool for improving overall mathematics and science literacy.

10.3 Theoretical Perspectives on Origins of Gender Differences

Halpern and Collaer ( 2005 ) described gender differences in spatial ability as some of the largest found for any cognitive task, raising the important question as to its developmental origins. Why do males on average outperform females on spatial tasks? Past approaches to this question have emphasized biological factors as well as social factors, cultural infl uences, and life experiences. It is unlikely that there is one single factor that can adequately explain the magnitude of the gender gap for spatial ability. Most gender difference researchers would acknowledge both biologi-cal and social forces contribute to their development, embracing a biopsychosocial model of gender differences (Halpern and Tan 2001 ; Hyde 2014 ). While there may be biological factors that predispose an individual to greater or lesser profi ciency on spatial tasks, it must be remembered that they are not immutable. Full development of such skills requires practice and experience, and both males and females can make signifi cant gains with training.

10.3.1 Evolutionary and Genetic Factors

Evolutionary psychology seeks to make sense of gender differences in human cog-nition by considering the role of evolutionary selection arising from the division of labour between men and women in traditional hunter-gatherer societies (Eagly and Wood 1999 ; Geary 1995 ). Men would be required to travel long distances in order to track and hunt animals, a task requiring strong spatial perception and navigation skills (Buss 1995 , 2015 ). In contrast, women fulfi lled the role of the gatherer of more local food and assumed childrearing duties. This role had less need for spatial profi ciency but emphasized other adaptive traits such as nurturing and fi ne-motor skills. Over successive generations, evolutionary forces may have developed sex- specifi c profi ciencies in spatial ability, giving males a strong advantage over females with such tasks (Buss 2015 ; Jones et al. 2003 ).

D. Reilly et al.

Page 174: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

205

Support for the position of evolutionary psychology comes from cross-cultural studies of cognitive gender differences. Unlike language and quantitative reasoning which shows substantial variation across countries and cultures (Else-Quest et al. 2010 ; Lynn and Mikk 2009 ; Reilly 2012 ), a large body of research has shown that spatial differences are consistently found in all countries (Janssen and Geiser 2012 ; Peters et al. 2006 ). Furthermore intelligence – including spatial ability – is a highly heritable trait (Bratko 1996 ; Sternberg 2012 ), meaning that it can be passed down from one generation to the next. Nevertheless, some researchers question the valid-ity of evolutionary and genetic factors (Hyde 2014 ), arguing that at the genetic level men and women are identical with the exception of the sex chromosome. Such argu-ments do not take into account other biological differences. For instance, the expres-sion of sex hormones might be an important factor linked to genetic and evolutionary gender differences (Hines 2015a ; Sherry and Hampson 1997 ).

10.3.2 Contribution of Sex Hormones to Spatial Ability

Sex hormones such as androgens and estrogens have been proposed as a biological explanation for observed gender differences in spatial ability (Kimura 1996 , 2000 ; Sherry and Hampson 1997 ). While both males and females produce these sex hor-mones to some degree, greater androgen production is typically found in males while greater estrogen and progesterone production is present in females. Such a difference starts early, with differences in testosterone concentration of foetuses found as early as 8 weeks gestation (Hines 2010 ). Production of sex hormones greatly increases with the onset of puberty (Spear 2000 ), and is associated with a range of psychological and behavioural changes as well as differences in brain development (Berenbaum and Beltz 2011 ; Sisk and Zehr 2005 ).

Even before birth, sex hormones contribute to the organisation and development of the brain with lasting effects on behaviour and interests for children (Hines 2015a ). Girls exposed to higher than normal levels of androgenic hormones prena-tally, either due to a genetic disorder such as congenital adrenal hyperplasia or because androgenic hormones were prescribed to mothers during pregnancy, show increased male-typical play, behaviour, and interests as young children (Auyeung et al. 2009 ; Hines 2010 ). Furthermore, they perform at a higher level on tasks of spatial ability than their same-sex peers (Puts et al. 2008 ). Because spatial ability requires environmental input for development, toys and play can be an important source of spatial experiences. Many stereotypically masculine activities such as construction blocks and model building promote spatial development (Caldera et al. 1989 ; Caplan and Caplan 1994 ), and gender differences in sex hormones may infl u-ence boys and girls play preferences.

Sex hormones also play an activational role in human behaviour and cognition after the onset of puberty (Berenbaum and Beltz 2011 ; Spear 2000 ), which coin-cides with a widening of the gender gap in spatial ability (Kimura 2000 ; Voyer et al. 1995 ). There is an intuitive appeal to considering hormones as explaining part or all

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 175: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

206

of the gender gap in spatial ability, but correlation by itself does not prove causation. Hormonal effects also coincides with increased gender conformity pressures for adolescents (Ruble et al. 2006 ) which may limit the interests and leisure activities that boys and girls pursue. These, in turn, may provide greater exposure to spatial experiences for boys than girls, thereby exacerbating gender differences.

To establish the causal effects of hormones would require an experiment whereby androgens were administered, which would be both impractical and unethical in developing children. There are instances where researchers have observed the effect of atypical levels of sex hormones (either reduced or increased levels) that are asso-ciated with certain medical conditions. Spatial ability in men diagnosed after puberty with hypogonadism is lower than in those with normal testosterone levels (Alexander et al. 1988 ; Hier and Crowley Jr. 1982 ), while men receiving hormone replacement therapy later in life showed signifi cant improvements in spatial perfor-mance after treatment (Janowsky et al. 1994 ). In otherwise healthy individuals, some studies have also found a contribution of endogenous testosterone in the bloodstream to spatial performance in both genders (Davison and Susman 2001 ; Hausmann et al. 2009 ; Hromatko and Tadinac 2007 ), as well as fl uctuations across the menstrual cycle in girls (Hausmann et al. 2000 ; Kimura and Hampson 1994 ). However, not every study fi nds robust associations (Puts et al. 2010 ), and the activa-tional role that these hormones play may explain a much smaller proportion of vari-ance in spatial ability than their earlier contribution to brain development (Falter et al. 2006 ).

10.3.3 Different Socialisation Experiences Between Boys and Girls

While biological contributions to spatial ability may explain some of the gender gap, many researchers argue that gender differences in early socialization experi-ences of boys and girls also play a signifi cant role. Although there is certainly a contribution of biology, many theorists note that gender is socially constructed. From infancy and throughout childhood and adolescence, boys and girls experience the world differently, and are subject to different pressures and expectations (Lytton and Romney 1991 ; Martin and Ruble 2004 ). Boys and girls receive different mes-sages about the suitability of particular toys from their parents, and elicit different styles of interaction during shared play with their parents, caregivers and siblings (Caldera et al. 1989 ). Children also acquire messages about gender expectations from their peers, and from their teachers and instructors once they have entered the educational system (Jacobs et al. 2002 ).

There are many different theoretical perspectives on the socialization of gender. For example, social-role theory proposes that psychological differences between men and women arise from gender segregation in men and women’s social roles (Eagly and Wood 1999 ), while the social cognitive theory of gender development posits that gender development is the result of learned experiences that teach gender roles through a system of observation, reinforcement, and punishment (Bussey and

D. Reilly et al.

Page 176: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

207

Bandura 1999 ). An exhaustive coverage of the many other theoretical perspectives on gender is beyond the scope of this chapter, so we highlight only those relating specifi cally to spatial ability.

10.3.4 Sex-Role Mediation Theory of Spatial Ability

As children develop, they acquire stereotypically masculine or feminine traits, behaviours and interests, a developmental process referred to as sex-typing (Kohlberg and Ullian 1974 ; Martin and Ruble 2010 ). However, there is also wide variability across individuals in the degree to which people integrate masculine and feminine traits into their self-concept and sex-role identity (Bem 1981 ; Spence and Buckner 2000 ). Highly sex-typed individuals are motivated to keep their behaviour and self-concept consistent with traditionally gender norms, including the expres-sion of intellectual abilities (Bem 1981 ; Steffens and Jelenec 2011 ). Others may integrate aspects of both masculine and feminine identifi cation into their self- concept, termed androgyny.

The sex-role mediation hypothesis proposes that a masculine or androgynous sex-role identity promotes the development of spatial ability (Nash 1979 ). This theory proposes a number of mechanisms, including self-selection of play and lei-sure activities throughout childhood and adolescence, self-effi cacy beliefs and moti-vation to practise tasks that encourage spatial competency, and sex-role conformity pressures (Reilly and Neumann 2013 ). This hypothesis has been tested a number of times over the decades, and two meta-analyses have been conducted (Reilly and Neumann 2013 ; Signorella and Jamison 1986 ). Both fi nd support for sex-role medi-ation on the most prominently tested visual spatial task of mental rotation, but the scope of such reviews are limited by the shortage of studies testing other compo-nents of spatial ability. More recently an empirical study by Reilly, Neumann and Andrews ( 2016 ) tested support for the sex-role mediation hypothesis across a range of visual-spatial tasks, including mental rotation, spatial perception and spatial visu-alization. Masculine sex-role identifi cation signifi cantly predicted performance in both males and females.

10.3.5 Gender Stereotypes About Intelligence and Spatial Ability

Children begin to exhibit cultural stereotypes about what constitutes “masculine” or “feminine” by their early school years (Blakemore 2003 ; Ruble et al. 2006 ). This extends to characterising particular scholastic subjects and intellectual interests as masculine or feminine. For example, mathematics and geometry (which encourage development of spatial ability) is seen as masculine while language and arts are seen as feminine (Nosek et al. 2002 ). Boys also report greater interest and higher motivation in mathematics – a fi nding that is replicated cross-culturally (Goldman and Penner

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 177: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

208

2014 ). Such stereotypes infl uence the way that men and women see themselves in relation to intellectual domains generally (Nosek et al. 2002 ), as well as their motiva-tion to persevere when they encounter obstacles to learning (Meece et al. 2006 ).

While gender stereotypes may infl uence interest and motivation, they also shape perceptions of our abilities and self-effi cacy. Despite there being no scientifi c evi-dence for gender differences in general intelligence, parents typically believe their sons are more intelligent than daughters (Furnham 2000 ; Furnham and Akande 2004 ; Furnham et al. 2002 ; Furnham and Thomas 2004 ). These gender stereotypes are quickly incorporated into children’s own self-beliefs and persist into adulthood. A consistent fi nding cross-culturally is that when asked to rate their own level of general intelligence, males tend to estimate their intelligence level considerably higher than do females (for a meta-analysis see Szymanowicz and Furnham 2011 ). The effect size of this gender difference is not insubstantial, d = 0.34. Males also rate themselves as more spatially competent than females, d = 0.43, which is again a moderately sized effect.

Popular cultural stereotypes (e.g. Pease and Pease 2001 ) that women can’t read maps or navigate without asking for directions do women a real disservice. Males in general are seen as more capable at performing spatial tasks by a signifi cant degree (Halpern et al. 2011 ; Lunneborg 1982 ), and gender stereotypes can become self-fulfi lling prophecies that undermine both interest in such tasks as well as per-formance (Steele 1997 ). Recognizing that spatial ability is not immutable, but that it can improve with learning and instruction is an important fi rst step for any tar-geted intervention aimed at eliminating the gender gap and ensuring gender equity.

10.3.6 Differential Practice of Spatial Skills by Boys and Girls

Piaget ( 1951 ) was one of the earliest scholars to suggest that play is an important part of child development, helping to develop childrens’ motor skills and spatial abilities. Boys and girls are typically encouraged by parents to engage in stereotypi-cally masculine and feminine play consistent with their gender (Eccles et al. 1990 ), but boys and girls also express preferences for different types of toys themselves (Hines 2015b ). For example, boys tend to show a preference for vehicles and weap-ons while girls show more interest in dolls. The effect size for this gender difference is extremely large, with one study in children aged 4–10 years fi nding an effect size of d = 2.0 (Pasterski et al. 2005 ). While there is considerable gender segregation in the types of toys marketed to boys and girls (Blakemore and Centers 2005 ), it is diffi cult to separate how much these choices are culturally directed and how much of the preference is biologically based. Recall that early androgen exposure prena-tally has been associated with male-typical toy and play preferences (Auyeung et al. 2009 ; Hines 2010 ), suggesting at least some infl uence on boys’ and girls’ choices. Indeed, this strong effect is even found amongst non-human primates divorced of human cultural traditions. Male primates express greater interest and play longer with stereotypically masculine toys such as balls, cars, and trucks while female

D. Reilly et al.

Page 178: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

209

primates preferred dolls and plush animals (Alexander and Hines 2002 ; Hassett et al. 2008 ).

Caplan and Caplan ( 1994 ) have argued that many stereotypically masculine toys and activities encourage the practice and development of spatial skills, while tradi-tionally feminine play reinforces other culturally valued traits like communication and cooperation. For example, construction blocks and model assembly requires children to read 2D depictions of 3D objects and then fi nd the correct spatial orien-tation of small and similar looking parts, while carpentry involves precise measure-ment of spatial relations and manipulation of parts. At earlier ages, toys like cars and trucks offer hands-on practice in visually tracking a moving object and judging the correct angle and speed to cause collisions. Girls play less on average with spatial toys than do males (Jirout and Newcombe 2015 ), and thus have less opportunities to practise these skills. Even if the effect of differential practice of spatial skills offers only a modest initial advantage to boys, the effect may grow larger as children enter adolescence and begin to self-select leisure activities and hobbies that they enjoy and are competent at performing. Activities such as carpentry, mechanics, models, and computer games would further enhance visual spatial skills.

There is strong evidence to support the theory that gender differences in spatial ability are at least partially infl uenced by differential levels of practice between boys and girls. Surveys and questionnaires measuring participation in spatial activities are positively correlated with performance on a range of spatial tests (Baenninger and Newcombe 1989 ; Chan 2007 ). However, it is equally plausible that people with high spatial ability may be the ones who want to engage in spatial activity in the fi rst place (Baenninger and Newcombe 1989 ). It does seem likely that spatial activity experiences may be developmentally important in children (Doyle et al. 2012 ), and that differential levels of practice make some contribution.

10.4 Interventions for Training of Spatial Ability

A considerable body of evidence attests to the malleability of visuospatial reason-ing, and that peak spatial ability is only reached with suffi cient environmental input and experience (Baenninger and Newcombe 1995 ; Caplan and Caplan 1994 ). While biological and social factors may result in males starting with a modest initial advantage over females in spatial ability, it is important to remember that it is an acquired skill; people do not emerge de novo and become Tetris grand masters. There is an old joke that starts with the question “How do you get to Carnegie Hall?” – the punchline of course is “practice, practice, practice”. Like any other learned skill, if we receive training and do appropriate practice we can improve spatial abilities over time.

A large number of studies have examined the effects of brief training interven-tions to improve spatial ability. While there is wide variation in effectiveness, almost all such interventions show some improvement in spatial ability. With the large num-ber of studies, training types, and choices of samples, the technique of meta- analysis

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 179: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

210

can provide an objective quantitative assessment. But before turning to these reviews, theoretical issues need to be considered.

There are four important theoretical questions. First, does spatial training benefi t all recipients equally, or are there differential rates of improvement for males and females? If spatial training was only effective in those who already have a moderate level of profi ciency, its usefulness in addressing the gender gap would be limited. Second, do the effects of training transfer to all spatial tasks (thereby indicating an improvement in latent spatial ability), or only to tasks that are very similar or indeed identical to those used in training? Sims and Mayer ( 2002 ) have questioned whether the effect of spatial training might simply be the result of practice and familiarity, rather than genuine improvement in latent ability. For interventions to be genuinely useful, training effects must generalise to novel and unfamiliar spatial tasks. Third, do the improvements to spatial ability persist over time or are they short-lived? Fourth, do all types of training interventions work, or do characteristics such as the type and intensity of training matter?

Two meta-analyses have investigated the effect of brief spatial instruction and training interventions. The fi rst, by Baenninger and Newcombe ( 1989 ) investigated the effects of training in studies that used a repeated measures design (i.e. subjects’ initial performance on a spatial test is measured, a brief training intervention is offered, and then spatial performance is tested a second time). Their review included studies spanning a considerable range of years from the 1940s to the 1980s. They found that substantial improvements could be made to spatial ability after training, with an impressive effect size of d = 0.70 when tested on the same spatial measure that they were trained on, and a more modest effect size of d = 0 .49 when more general spatial tasks were administered. This is an important distinction, because it shows that the effects of spatial training generalize well to other spatial tasks rather than being simply familiarity with the test content arising from repeated administra-tion. The researchers also sought to test whether there was evidence of differential improvement between males and females, but found no signifi cant gender differ-ences. What the researchers did not address though is whether the improvements to spatial ability persist over time. Instead the authors considered the intensity of the training intervention, fi nding that multiple sessions over several weeks delivered meaningful improvement and that extremely brief or single session interventions showed less substantive benefi ts.

While the review by Baenninger and Newcombe ( 1989 ) makes an important con-tribution to the literature, a number of researchers have argued that changes in men and womens’ roles over the past few decades should result in smaller gender differ-ence over time (Caplan and Caplan 1994 ). When research becomes too dated, it raises the question of whether it remains applicable to current generations. More recently, Uttal et al. ( 2013b ) conducted an extensive meta-analytic review of the empirical studies on spatial training from more recent years. Their meta-analysis also included a large number of unpublished studies (such as masters and PhD level theses). This is important because there might be a selection bias in the literature towards publishing only statistically signifi cant fi ndings while non-signifi cant fi nd-ings may be discarded, termed the fi le drawer effect in psychology (Ioannidis et al.

D. Reilly et al.

Page 180: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

211

2014 ; Rosenthal 1979 ). A genuine test of the effectiveness of training interventions would also need to consider fi ndings that might disconfi rm the hypothesis.

Uttal et al. ( 2013b ) considered a wide range of spatial training interventions, from explicit instruction and courses to playing video games and practising spatial tasks. The meta-analysis found that spatial training interventions were highly effec-tive, with an overall effect size of d = 0.47 which is a medium-sized effect. Consistent with the earlier meta-analysis by Baenninger and Newcombe there was no evidence for differential improvement between males and females. Both genders gained the same benefi ts from training. Moderator analysis also showed no difference in the type of training being offered, with similarly sized effects across interventions that offered spatial learning courses, practice on spatial tasks or practice on video games. Adults also showed similar rates of improvements as adolescents, and though there was a slight tendency for interventions with children to have larger effect sizes, this trend did not reach statistical signifi cance.

Another important research question about training interventions is whether the effects persist over time. Most studies that report the results of a spatial training intervention test subjects at the conclusion of the intervention, but a number of the studies evaluated in Uttal et al. ( 2013b ) introduced a short delay of a few weeks and some tested subjects after as long as several months (Terlecki et al. 2011 ). If there were genuine and lasting improvements to latent spatial ability, we should see simi-larly sized effects of improvement between studies that tested performance immedi-ately to those studies that included some latency. The meta-analysis found the effect of training to be durable, with no diminution of improvement for studies that intro-duced a delay before retesting.

To address the question of whether training interventions show generalisability to other types of spatial tasks, Uttal et al. ( 2013b ) compared studies that used very similar measures of spatial performance to that covered in training with studies that employed substantially different types of spatial tasks. Importantly, the meta- analysis showed no difference between these two categories, providing evidence of transfer to novel tasks.

The research outlined above provides strong evidence that regardless of gender, spatial ability is highly malleable with instruction and training. Furthermore these effects do transfer to other types of spatial tasks and persist over time. Even brief interventions seem to have some effect, but more intensive training over multiple sessions yields the strongest benefi ts. Importantly the effects of training generalise across tasks, and improvements can be delivered for practically any age group from children to older adults.

10.4.1 Spatial Training and STEM Outcomes

While spatial ability is important for many occupations, the most compelling ben-efi ts of spatial training are in improving mathematical and science achievement in students. Longitudinal studies have provided compelling evidence of an association

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 181: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

212

between spatial ability and profi ciency in mathematics and science (Wai et al. 2010 ), but to date only a limited number of studies have investigated whether spatial train-ing translates into tangible improvements in STEM achievement. Cheng and Mix ( 2014 ) conducted a randomized control trial of spatial training in a sample of 6- and 7-year old children, fi nding improvements in a test of basic calculation skills. A subsequent study by Krisztián et al. ( 2015 ) that taught spatial training with origami over a 10 week period in a sample of fi fth and sixth grade students found similar improvements in computation skills over a control group. At present there are no spatial training studies that have measured science learning outcomes though in children, and none with adolescents in high school.

Amongst college-aged young adult samples, only two studies have investigated whether increasing spatial ability translates to improvements in mathematics and science learning. Sanchez ( 2012 ) conducted a randomized control trial that offered an intervention to target spatial ability, and found that the spatial group outper-formed controls when tested on their learning from a short course on volcanoes and plate tectonics. In another study operating over a longer time period, Miller and Halpern ( 2013 ) recruited a sample of male and female fi rst-year college students and randomly assigned them to either a control group or a spatial training condition (consisting of six 2-h spatial training sessions over a 6 week period). The gender gap in spatial ability narrowed somewhat after spatial training. In addition, the grades in student coursework were examined at the end of the year (up to 10 months after training ended). Compared to the control group, those receiving the intervention achieved higher grades in their physics coursework ( d = 0.32) but not in other classes like chemistry or calculus. The study also found signifi cant correlations between students’ spatial ability and course GPA in the following sophomore year for a num-ber of STEM courses, including electricity and magnetism, biology, engineering, and differential equations. The conclusions of this study are limited though by the small sample size for the treatment group (14 women, 24 men) which resulted in a reduced statistical power.

10.5 Reducing Gender Differences by Promoting Spatial Ability in Children

With the link between spatial ability and development of mathematics and science skills, a number of prominent educational and gender researchers have argued for the importance of developing spatial competency ability as a foundation for profi -ciency in STEM subjects (Hyde and Lindberg 2007 ; Newcombe and Frick 2010 ; Wai et al. 2009 ). With competing interests in a crowded curriculum, teachers and principals might be understandably reluctant to allocate time for regular lessons on promoting spatial competency. However, the effect of even brief training interven-tions over several sessions has been found to be effective in reducing the gender gap in spatial ability (Uttal et al. 2013a ). Since both males and females can improve

D. Reilly et al.

Page 182: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

213

their spatial reasoning substantially, it might be applied broadly to all students, which avoids the potentially stigmatizing effects of singling out females as a group for special interventions.

While explicit training would benefi t older students such as those in high school or entering college, Newcombe and Frick ( 2010 ) advocate the importance of early education for spatial intelligence before the gender gap widens. One approach would be to integrate spatial learning with existing content in the STEM curriculum. In a report by the American National Research Council ( 2006 ), a range of practical strategies are outlined for engaging students to think spatially as part of mathemat-ics and science classes. Rich multimedia can present complex scientifi c concepts visually, and many electronic textbooks offer data visualizations that are interactive rather than being static displays. For example, force and motion concepts are diffi -cult to convey verbally or from a printed diagram. By showing the motion path of a physical object, a child can see the effects of physical phenomena.

Parents and caregivers might also gently encourage spatial learning outside of school by providing children with play and leisure activities (outlined in Table 10.1 ) that encourage spatial development through attention to spatial relationships (e.g., higher–lower; longer-shorter; wider-narrower). Games such as jigsaws, construc-tion blocks, and board games provide contexts that facilitate spatial learning. Newcombe and Frick also note that everyday conversation can also be an opportunity

Table 10.1 Summary of children’s play and leisure activities providing spatial experiences

Specifi c spatial abilities

Age category Play and leisure activity SP MR SV ST WF

Toy and play experiences for younger children

Construction blocks ● ● ● ‘Action-oriented’ toys such as cars and vehicles

● ●

Geometric shape toys ● ● Throwing and catching ball games ● ● Jigsaws ● ● ● Art and drawing activities ● ● Mazes and maps ● ●

Enrichment experiences for older children

‘Transforming’ toys appropriate to age

● ● ●

Advanced construction bricks such as Lego™

● ● ●

Model building ● ● ● Origami ● ● ● Computer games (action) ● ● ● ● Computer games (puzzle) ● ● ● Computer games (construction) ● ● ● Perceptual and motor skills training such as juggling

● ● ●

Organised sports ● ● ● SP spatial perception , MR mental rotation , SV spatial visualiztion , ST spatiotemporal , WF wayfi nd-ing and navigation

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 183: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

214

for parents to highlight the spatial properties of objects through questions and gen-tly introduce spatial language and concepts into the conversation (Ferrara et al. 2011 ). Indeed, many household experiences can be learning opportunities to dem-onstrate spatial concepts, such as measuring and transformation of solids and liq-uids when moving ingredients from one container to another during cooking, or imagining what shape will be made if we fold a sheet of paper diagonally. Educational toys that provide examples of geometric shapes can be a good way to extend spatial language further by learning the names of common objects such as triangles, squares, circles, and relationships before introducing more complex shapes and concepts (Newcombe and Frick 2010 ).

Children as young as 3 or 4 years of age can understand the concepts of maps and how they relate to the physical world if introduced at the right pace (Shusterman et al. 2008 ), while puzzles like mazes can offer further practice of spatial and navi-gational skills (Jirout and Newcombe 2014 ). In older children, enrichment activities like jigsaw puzzles and origami can also provide additional opportunities to encour-age spatial development (Boakes 2009 ; Taylor and Hutton 2013 ), particularly when parents and educators engage children in active conversation and provide guided assistance. Art and drawing activities can also provide practice in spatial perception and visualization skills (Calabrese and Marucci 2006 ). Age-appropriate toy robots that children can change into vehicles and back provides practice in learning com-plex multi-step transformations like that involved with spatial visualization, while a wealth of literature has shown that construction blocks provide opportunities to practise spatial perception and transformation skills (Caldera et al. 1999 ; Jirout and Newcombe 2015 ; Stannard et al. 2001 ). They also provide practice in interpreting two and three-dimensional diagrams, and then translating these diagrams into phys-ical steps.

Another promising enrichment activity that aids in practising spatial skills may be video games. Computer gaming has emerged as a popular leisure activity for children and can be an opportunity to practise spatial skills. While boys still report playing more computer games than girls, in recent years the gap has been diminish-ing (Terlecki et al. 2011 ). Additionally, the wider availability of gaming on mobile phones and tablets may see shifts in gender patterns of usage. Not every player will enjoy fi rst-person shooters or fast action games, and game developers are increas-ingly embracing other genres to entice non-game players into the market. However, not all games are equal, and some games may have greater educational potential than others. In a review by Spence and Feng ( 2010 ) on the contribution of video-game play to spatial cognition, action-based games and maze/puzzle genres emerged as the most likely to affect spatial cognition as they provide repeated practice in spatial perception, mental rotation, and navigation tasks. Indeed, a number of stud-ies have shown that even brief training with computer games may be effective as an intervention (as reviewed earlier).

Parental concerns over the use of videogames may need to be considered if they are to be recommended. Concerns over violence in some types of videogames or excessive amounts of time spent playing remain legitimate (Festl et al. 2013 ). However, when enjoyed in moderation with parental selection of content there is evidence that the benefi ts for spatial cognition outweigh the costs (Ferguson 2007 ;

D. Reilly et al.

Page 184: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

215

Uttal et al. 2013a ). Parents may also be more comfortable offering less violent and adversarial games to their children, such as the popular construction and building game “Minecraft” which is appealing to boys and girls equally and is already used by some educators (e.g. Short 2012 ). Spence and Feng propose that gaming might also be an opportunity to deliver more targeted educational interventions specifi -cally developed with the goal of raising spatial abilities in a similar fashion to com-mercial brain-training products.

There is also a strong link between the development of motor skills and spatial reasoning (Frick et al. 2009 ; Richter et al. 2000 ). Neuroimaging studies show that regions of the brain associated with motor skills are activated when performing mental rotation tasks (Halari et al. 2006 ; Richter et al. 2000 ). Interventions that con-sist of motor skills training have been shown to enhance mental rotation perfor-mance in children (Blüchel et al. 2013 ). Newcombe and Frick ( 2010 ) advocate that educators and parents should provide young children plenty of time for free play and physical action with objects like balls to provide practice in motor skills. By association, this should transfer into positive benefi ts for spatial ability.

Sporting activity and organised sports might also offer opportunities to more specifi cally develop spatial ability. While individual families may differ, sons typi-cally receive greater encouragement to pursue athleticism and organised sports than daughters (Leaper 2005 ), and greater media attention and funding is given to male professional sports stars (Gill and Kamphoff 2010 ). In contrast, girls have lower enrolment in organised sports and withdraw from sporting teams at a higher rate (Vilhjalmsson and Kristjansdottir 2003 ). But there is evidence that playing sports may help to develop spatial ability (Moreau et al. 2015 ). When children who play regular sport were compared to similar aged matches who did not, those who played sport performed better on tests of spatial performance (Notarnicola et al. 2014 ), with similar fi ndings in young adults (Lord and Leonard 1997 ; Moreau et al. 2011 ). Motor coordination is a signifi cant predictor of mental rotation ability even after controlling for the effect of gender (Pietsch and Jansen 2012 ), and two studies have found that learning and practising juggling skills increased mental rotation perfor-mance for both adults and children (Jansen et al. 2009 , 2011 ). Encouragement of sports activity within the context of the educational system and by parents may help to lessen the gender gap in spatial ability, in addition to the non-cognitive benefi ts (Moreau et al. 2015 ).

10.6 Directions for Future Research

Most researchers now endorse biopsychosocial models of gender differences in spa-tial ability (Halpern et al. 2007 ) rather than considering exclusively biological or social causes, and the debate has shifted towards their relative contributions. Whereas once spatial ability was considered fi xed and immutable, a considerable body of research has demonstrated that exposure to new spatial experiences through-out early childhood promotes growth in spatial profi ciency. Furthermore, spatial training interventions can produce substantial benefi ts that potentially could

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 185: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

216

translate to a reduction or even the elimination of the gender gap in mathematics and science achievement.

As reviewed earlier, only a limited number of spatial training studies have mea-sured subsequent outcomes in science and mathematics achievement outcomes however. To date though, there have been no spatial training interventions that have followed children longitudinally to follow their progress, and only a single study by Miller and Halpern ( 2013 ) has tracked the progress of college-aged students for a prolonged length of time. Arguments for spatial training interventions would be strengthened by further studies monitoring student progress over longer time peri-ods. It would also allow investigators to determine what types of spatial training and at what intervals, will best deliver changes in STEM-specifi c outcomes. While brief interventions may well yield long-term improvement, it is also possible that spatial training will require maintenance “booster” training at periodic intervals to deliver lasting educational improvements.

10.7 Summary and Conclusions

While individuals may differ, on average males score higher in tests of visual spatial ability. They also rate themselves as more spatially competent than females. Gender differences in spatial ability emerge from an early age. While clearly observable in children, the gender gap widens in adolescence and continues to grow into adult-hood where it is quite large. Gender differences are found for a variety of categories of spatial tasks, but the largest and most actively studied is mental rotation, followed by spatial perception and then spatial visualization skills. There are a range of theo-retical perspectives on why gender differences in spatial ability develop from biol-ogy to environmental causes, but one of the most frequently argued causes is differential levels of spatial learning and practice between males and females. This is supported by retrospective studies fi nding associations between childhood spatial experiences and spatial ability in adults.

Gender differences in spatial ability also precede the development of gender dif-ferences in mathematics and science, and longitudinal studies have found that early performance on spatial tasks can predict future performance in STEM, even many years later. There is also robust evidence demonstrating that spatial ability is not an immutable skill, and that even brief interventions can deliver impressively sized improvements. Such evidence makes a compelling argument for integrating spatial learning into early education, but parents can also provide additional learning opportunities for their children by engaging in spatial language, demonstrating spa-tial concepts within the home, and providing toys and games that encourage spatial practice. In older children, computer games can provide an opportunity to learn and practise spatial skills if they express an interest them, and organised sports has also been shown to improve spatial ability. The research supports the conclusion that concerted efforts by educators to address the gender gap in spatial ability in children and adolescents may translate into improvements in girls’ and boys’ mathematics

D. Reilly et al.

Page 186: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

217

and science achievement. However there is a need for longitudinal studies to deter-mine which types of training and at what intervals will best support students in this regard, and the extent to which this reduces the gender gap for STEM outcomes.

References

Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23 (6), 467–479. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00107-1 .

Alexander, G. M., Swerdloff, R., & Wang, C. (1988). Androgen-behavior correlation in hypogo-nadal and eugonadal men: Cognitive abilities. Hormones and Behavior, 33 (2), 85–88. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1439 .

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychological Science, 20 (2), 144–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02279.x .

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20 (5), 327–344. doi: 10.1007/BF00287729 .

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1995). Environmental input to the development of sex- related differences in spatial and mathematical ability. Learning and Individual Differences, 7 (4), 363–379. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(95)90007-1 .

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender Schema Theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88 (4), 354–364. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354 .

Berenbaum, S. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2011). Sexual differentiation of human behavior: Effects of prenatal and pubertal organizational hormones. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 32 (2), 183–200. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.03.001 .

Blakemore, J. E. O. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys. Sex Roles, 48 (9–10), 411–419.

Blakemore, J. E. O., & Centers, R. E. (2005). Characteristics of boys’ and girls’ toys. Sex Roles, 53 (9–10), 619–633. doi: 10.1007/s11199-005-7729-0 .

Blüchel, M., Lehmann, J., Kellner, J., & Jansen, P. (2013). The improvement in mental rotation performance in primary school-aged children after a two-week motor-training. Educational Psychology, 33 (1), 75–86. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.707612 .

Boakes, N. J. (2009). Origami instruction in the middle school mathematics classroom: Its impact on spatial Visualization and geometry knowledge of students. RMLE Online: Research in Middle Level Education, 32 (7), 1–12.

Bornstein, M. H. (2011). The mind of the preschool child: The intelligence-school interface. In O. A. Barbarin & B. H. Wasik (Eds.), Handbook of child development and early education: Research to pratice (pp. 123–142). New York: Guilford Press.

Bratko, D. (1996). Twin study of verbal and spatial abilities. Personality and Individual Differences, 21 (4), 621–624. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00091-8 .

Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50 (3), 164–168. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.50.3.164 .

Buss, D. M. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind . Boston: Pearson. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentia-

tion. Psychological Review, 100 , 676–713. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676 . Calabrese, L., & Marucci, F. S. (2006). The infl uence of expertise level on the visuo-spatial ability:

Differences between experts and novices in imagery and drawing abilities. Cognitive Processing, 7 (1), 118–120. doi: 10.1007/s10339-006-0094-2 .

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 187: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

218

Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & Marion, O. B. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60 (1), 70–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb02696.x .

Caldera, Y. M., Mc Culp, A. D., O’Brien, M., Truglio, R. T., Alvarez, M., & Huston, A. C. (1999). Children’s play preferences, construction play with blocks, and visual-spatial skills: Are they related? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23 (4), 855–872.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1994). Thinking critically about research on sex and gender . New York: Harper Collins.

Caplan, P. J., MacPherson, G. M., & Tobin, P. (1985). Do sex-related differences in spatial abilities exist? American Psychologist, 40 (7), 786–799. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.7.786 .

Carrol, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies . New York: Cambridge University Press.

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). The infl uence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31 (4), 697–705. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.697 .

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., & Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores: A comparison of spatial skills with internalized beliefs and anxiet-ies. Developmental Psychology, 33 (4), 669–680. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.669 .

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135 (2), 218–261. doi: 10.1037/a0014412 .

Chan, D. W. (2007). Gender differences in spatial ability: Relationship to spatial experience among Chinese gifted students in Hong Kong. Roeper Review, 29 (4), 277–282.

Cheng, Y.-L., & Mix, K. S. (2014). Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15 (1), 2–11. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.725186 .

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

Collaer, M. L., Reimers, S., & Manning, J. T. (2007). Visuospatial performance on an internet line judgment task and potential hormonal markers: Sex, sexual orientation, and 2D:4D. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36 (2), 177–192. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9152-1 .

Contreras, M. J., Colom, R., Shih, P. C., Álava, M. J., & Santacreu, J. (2001). Dynamic spatial performance: Sex and educational differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 30 (1), 117–126. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00015-5 .

Contreras, M. J., Rubio, V. J., Peña, D., Colom, R., & Santacreu, J. (2007). Sex differences in dynamic spatial ability: The unsolved question of performance factors. Memory and Cognition, 35 (2), 297–303. doi: 10.3758/BF03193450 .

National Research Council. (2006). Learning to think spatially: Geographic Information Systems (GIS) as a support system in the K-12 curriculum . Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Davison, K. K., & Susman, E. J. (2001). Are hormone levels and cognitive ability related during early adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25 (5), 416–428. doi: 10.1080/016502501316934842 .

Doyle, R. A., Voyer, D., & Cherney, I. D. (2012). The relation between childhood spatial activities and spatial abilities in adulthood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33 , 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2012.01.002 .

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54 (6), 408–423. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408 .

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents’ socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46 (2), 183–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x .

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136 (1), 103–127. doi: 10.1037/a0018053 .

D. Reilly et al.

Page 188: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

219

Falter, C., Arroyo, M., & Davis, G. (2006). Testosterone: Activation or organization of spatial cognition? Biological Psychology, 73 (2), 132–140. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.011 .

Ferguson, C. J. (2007). The good, the bad and the ugly: A meta-analytic review of positive and negative effects of violent video games. Psychiatric Quarterly, 78 (4), 309–316. doi: 10.1007/s11126-007-9056-9 .

Ferguson, A. M., Maloney, E. A., Fugelsang, J., & Risko, E. F. (2015). On the relation between math and spatial ability: The case of math anxiety. Learning and Individual Differences, 39 , 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.02.007 .

Ferrara, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Golinkoff, R. M., & Lam, W. S. (2011). Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5 (3), 143–151. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01122.x .

Festl, R., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction, 108 (3), 592–599. doi: 10.1111/add.12016 .

French, J. W., Ekstrom, R. B., & Price, L. A. (1963). Manual for kit of reference tests for cognitive factors . Princeston: Educational Testing Service.

Frick, A., Daum, M. M., Walser, S., & Mast, F. W. (2009). Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10 (1–2), 18–40. doi: 10.1080/15248370902966719 .

Furnham, A. (2000). Parents’ estimates of their own and their children’s multiple intelligences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18 (4), 583–594. doi: 10.1348/026151000165869 .

Furnham, A., & Akande, A. (2004). African parents’ estimates of their own and their children’s multiple intelligences. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 22 , 281–294.

Furnham, A., & Thomas, C. (2004). Parents’ gender and personality and estimates of their own and their children’s intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 37 (5), 887–903.

Furnham, A., Reeves, E., & Budhani, S. (2002). Parents think their sons are brighter than their daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimations and estimations of their children’s mul-tiple intelligences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163 (1), 24–39. doi: 10.1080/00221320209597966 .

Geary, D. C. (1995). Sexual selection and sex differences in spatial cognition. Learning and Individual Differences, 7 (4), 289–301. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(95)90003-9 .

Gill, D., & Kamphoff, C. (2010). Gender in sport and exercise psychology. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 563–585). New York: Springer.

Goldman, A. D., & Penner, A. M. (2014). Exploring international gender differences in mathemat-ics self-concept. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth , 1–16. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2013.847850 .

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320 (5880), 1164–1165. doi: 10.1126/science.1154094 .

Halari, R., Sharma, T., Hines, M., Andrew, C., Simmons, A., & Kumari, V. (2006). Comparable fMRI activity with differential behavioural performance on mental rotation and overt verbal fl uency tasks in healthy men and women. Experimental Brain Research, 169 (1), 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0118-7 .

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F. (2007). Science, sex, and good sense: Why women are underrepresented in some

areas of science and math. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 121–130). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F., & Collaer, M. L. (2005). Sex differences in visuospatial abilities: More than meets

the eye. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking (pp. 170–212). New York: Cambridge University Press.

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 189: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

220

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using a psychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brain and Cognition, 45 (3), 392–414. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1287 .

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8 (1), 1–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x .

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011). Beliefs about cognitive gender differ-ences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for size. Sex Roles, 64 (5–6), 336–347. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2 .

Hancock, P., & Rausch, R. (2010). The effects of sex, age, and interval duration on the perception of time. Acta Psychologica, 133 (2), 170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.11.005 .

Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy prefer-ences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54 (3), 359–364. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.008 .

Hausmann, M., Slabbekoorn, D., Van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & Gunturkun, O. (2000). Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114 (6), 1245–1250. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.6.1245 .

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences – A psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34 (3), 389–401. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.019 .

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269 (5220), 41–45. doi: 10.1126/science.7604277 .

Hier, D. B., & Crowley, W. F., Jr. (1982). Spatial ability in androgen-defi cient men. New England Journal of Medicine, 306 (20), 1202–1205. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198205203062003 .

Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14 (10), 448–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005 .

Hines, M. (2015a). Early androgen exposure and human gender development. Biology of Sex Differences, 6 (1), 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s13293-015-0022-1 .

Hines, M. (2015b). Gendered development. In R. M. Lerner & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Handbook of child development and developmental science (7th ed.). Hoboken: Wiley.

Höffl er, T. N. (2010). Spatial ability: Its infl uence on learning with visualizations—a meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 22 (3), 245–269. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9126-7 .

Hromatko, I., & Tadinac, M. (2007). Testosterone levels infl uence spatial ability: Further evidence for curvilinear relationship. Review of Psychology, 13 (1), 27–34.

Hunt, E., Pellegrino, J. W., Frick, R. W., Farr, S. A., & Alderton, D. (1988). The ability to reason about movement in the visual fi eld. Intelligence, 12 (1), 77–100. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(88)90024-4 .

Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology, 65 (1), 373–398. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057 .

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about the nature of gender differ-ences and the implications for gender equity. In S. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equity through education (2nd ed., pp. 19–32). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Ioannidis, J. P. A., Munafò, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., & David, S. P. (2014). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive science: Detection, prevalence, and prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18 (5), 235–241. 0.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010.

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfi eld, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73 (2), 509–527. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00421 .

Janowsky, J., Oviatt, S., & Orwoll, E. (1994). Testosterone infl uences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108 (2), 325–332. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.108.2.325 .

Jansen, P., Titze, C., & Heil, M. (2009). The infl uence of juggling on mental rotation performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40 (2), 351–359.

D. Reilly et al.

Page 190: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

221

Jansen, P., Lange, L., & Heil, M. (2011). The infl uence of juggling on mental rotation performance in children. Biomedical Human Kinetics, 3 , 18–22. doi: 10.2478/v10101-011-0005-6 .

Janssen, A. B., & Geiser, C. (2012). Cross-cultural differences in spatial abilities and solution strategies – An investigation in Cambodia and Germany. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43 (4), 533–557. doi: 10.1177/0022022111399646 .

Jirout, J. J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2014). Mazes and maps: Can young children fi nd their way? Mind, Brain, and Education, 8 (2), 89–96. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12048 .

Jirout, J. J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2015). Building blocks for developing spatial skills evidence from a large, representative US sample. Psychological Science, 26 (3), 302–310. doi: 10.1177/0956797614563338 .

Jones, C. M., Braithwaite, V. A., & Healy, S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117 (3), 403–411. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.403 .

Kimura, D. (1996). Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones infl uence human cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6 (2), 259–263. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80081-x .

Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kimura, D., & Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women is infl uenced by fl uctua-

tions in sex hormones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3 (2), 57–61. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769964 .

Kohlberg, L., & Ullian, D. (1974). Stages in the development of psycho-sexual concepts and atti-tudes. In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp. 209–222). New York: Wiley.

Kozhevnikov, M., Motes, M. A., & Hegarty, M. (2007). Spatial visualization in physics problem solving. Cognitive Science, 31 (4), 549–579. doi: 10.1080/15326900701399897 .

Krisztián, Á., Bernáth, L., Gombos, H., & Vereczkei, L. (2015). Developing numberical ability in children with mathematical diffi culties using origami. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 121 (1), 233–243. doi: 10.2466/24.10.PMS.121c16x1 .

Lachance, J. A., & Mazzocco, M. M. (2006). A longitudinal analysis of sex differences in math and spatial skills in primary school age children. Learning and Individual Differences, 16 (3), 195–216. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.12.001 .

Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfi nding strategies and anxiety about wayfi nding: A cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47 (9–10), 389–401. doi: 10.1023/A:1021668724970 .

Leaper, C. (2005). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 189–225). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35 (4), 940–949.

Li, Y., & Geary, D. C. (2013). Developmental gains in visuospatial memory predict gains in math-ematics achievement. PLoS ONE, 8 (7), e70160.

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56 (6), 1479–1498. doi: 10.2307/1130467 .

Lord, T., & Leonard, B. (1997). Comparing scores on spatial-perception tests for intercollegiate athletes and nonathletes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84 (1), 299–306. doi: 10.2466/pms.1997.84.1.299 .

Lunneborg, P. W. (1982). Sex differences in self-assessed, everyday spatial abilities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55 (1), 200–202. doi: 10.2466/pms.1982.55.1.200 .

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames, 13 (1), 3–13. doi: 10.3176/tr.2009.1.01 .

Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109 (2), 267–296. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267 .

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13 (2), 67–70. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x .

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 191: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

222

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender development. Annual Review of Psychology, 61 (1), 353–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100511 .

Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Burg, S. (2006). Gender and motivation. Journal of School Psychology, 44 (5), 351–373. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.004 .

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Can spatial training improve long-term outcomes for gifted STEM undergraduates? Learning and Individual Differences, 26 , 141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.012 .

Moore, D. S., & Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19 (11), 1063–1066. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02200.x .

Moreau, D., Mansy-Dannay, A., Clerc, J., & Guerrien, A. (2011). Spatial ability and motor perfor-mance: Assessing mental rotation processes in elite and novice athletes. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 42 (6), 525–547.

Moreau, D., Clerc, J., Mansy-Dannay, A., & Guerrien, A. (2015). Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice. Journal of Individual Differences, 33 (2), 83–88. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000075 .

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. In M. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263–302). New York: Academic.

Newcombe, N. S. (2007). Taking science seriously: Straight thinking about spatial sex differences. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 69–77). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatial intelligence: Why, what, and how. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4 (3), 102–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2010.01089.x .

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male, me= female, therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (1), 44–59. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44 .

Notarnicola, A., Maccagnano, G., Pesce, V., Tafuri, S., Novielli, G., & Moretti, B. (2014). Visual- spatial capacity: Gender and sport differences in young volleyball and tennis athletes and non- athletes. BMC Research Notes, 7 (57), 1–5. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-7-57 .

Nuttall, R. L., Casey, M. B., & Pezaris, E. (2005). Spatial ability as a mediator of gender differ-ences on mathematics tests: A biological-environmental framework. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 121–142). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76 (1), 264–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00843.x .

Pease, A., & Pease, B. (2001). Why men don’t listen and women can’t read maps: How we’re dif-ferent and what to do about it . London: Orion Books.

Peters, M., & Battista, C. (2008). Applications of mental rotation fi gures of the Shepard and Metzler type and description of a mental rotation stimulus library. Brain and Cognition, 66 , 260–264. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.09.003 .

Peters, M., Lehmann, W., Takahira, S., Takeuchi, Y., & Jordan, K. (2006). Mental rotation test performance in four cross-cultural samples (N=3367): Overall sex differences and the role of academic program in performance. Cortex, 42 (7), 1005–1014. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70206-5 .

Piaget, J. (1951). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. (C. Gattegno & F. M. Hodgson, Trans.). New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd.

Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2012). The relationship between coordination skill and mental rotation ability. In S. K. C. Stachniss & D. H. Uttal (Eds.), Spatial cognition VIII (pp. 173–181). Kloster Seeon: Springer.

Prinzel, L. J., III, & Freeman, F. G. (1995). Sex differences in visuo-spatial ability: Task diffi culty, speed-accuracy tradeoff, and other performance factors. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49 (4), 530–539. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.49.4.530 .

D. Reilly et al.

Page 192: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

223

Puts, D. A., McDaniel, M. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Spatial ability and prena-tal androgens: Meta-analyses of congenital adrenal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D: 4D) stud-ies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37 (1), 100–111. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9271-3 .

Puts, D. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Burriss, R. P., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58 (2), 282–289. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005 .

Quinn, P. C., & Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19 (11), 1067–1070. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02201.x .

Rammsayer, T., & Lustnauer, S. (1989). Sex differences in time perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68 (1), 195–198.

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7 (7), e39904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039904 .

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 68 (9), 521–535. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0 .

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science: A meta-analysis of national assessment of educational progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 (3), 645–662. doi: 10.1037/edu0000012 .

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in specifi c cogni-tive abilities. Intelligence, 54 , 147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004 .

Richter, W., Somorjai, R., Summers, R., Jarmasz, M., Menon, R. S., Gati, J. S., et al. (2000). Motor area activity during mental rotation studied by time-resolved single-trial fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (2), 310–320.

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The fi le drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86 (3), 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 .

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development (pp. 858–932). Hoboken: Wiley.

Sanchez, C. A. (2012). Enhancing visuospatial performance through video game training to increase learning in visuospatial science domains. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19 (1), 58–65. doi: 10.1080/0300443011670110 .

Shea, D. L., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2001). Importance of assessing spatial ability in intel-lectually talented young adolescents: A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 (3), 604–614. doi: 10.1037//0022-0663.93.3.604 .

Sherry, D. F., & Hampson, E. (1997). Evolution and the hormonal control of sexually-dimorphic spatial abilities in humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1 (2), 50–56. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01015-2 .

Short, D. (2012). Teaching scientifi c concepts using a virtual world: Minecraft. Teaching Science, 58 (3), 55–58.

Shusterman, A., Ah Lee, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Young children’s spontaneous use of geometry in maps. Developmental Science, 11 (2), F1–F7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00670 .

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitive perfor-mance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100 (2), 207–228. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207 .

Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longi-tudinal examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42 (1), 70–83. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.70 .

Sims, V. K., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specifi city of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16 (1), 97–115.

Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26 (3–4), 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003 .

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24 (4), 417–463. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2 .

10 Gender Differences in Spatial Ability

Page 193: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

224

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science?: A critical review. American Psychologist, 60 (9), 950–958. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950 .

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24 (1), 44–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x .

Spence, I., & Feng, J. (2010). Video games and spatial cognition. Review of General Psychology, 14 (2), 92–104. doi: 10.1037/a0019491 .

Stannard, L., Wolfgang, C. H., Jones, I., & Phelps, P. (2001). A longitudinal study of the predictive relations among construction play and mathematical achievement. Early Child Development and Care, 167 (1), 115–125. doi: 10.1080/0300443011670110 .

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and perfor-mance. American Psychologist, 52 (6), 613–629. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613 .

Steffens, M., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereotypes regarding math and language: Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, but not for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64 (5), 324–335. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x .

Sternberg, R. J. (2012). Intelligence. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3 (5), 501–511. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1193 .

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, math-ematical, spatial and verbal intelligence: Four meta analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 (5), 493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001 .

Taylor, H. A., & Hutton, A. (2013). Think3d!: Training spatial thinking fundamental to STEM education. Cognition and Instruction, 31 (4), 434–455.

Terlecki, M. S., Brown, J., Harner-Steciw, L., Irvin-Hannum, J., Marchetto-Ryan, N., Ruhl, L., et al. (2011). Sex differences and similarities in video game experience, preferences, and self- effi cacy: Implications for the gaming industry. Current Psychology, 30 (1), 22–33. doi: 10.1007/s12144-010-9095-5 .

Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., et al. (2013a). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139 (2), 352–402. doi: 10.1037/a0028446 .

Uttal, D. H., Miller, D. I., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013b). Exploring and enhancing spatial thinking links to achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22 (5), 367–373. doi: 10.1177/0963721413484756 .

Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spa-tial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47 (2), 599–604. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599 .

Vasta, R., & Liben, L. S. (1996). The water-level task: An intriguing puzzle. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5 (6), 171–177. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512379 .

Vilhjalmsson, R., & Kristjansdottir, G. (2003). Gender differences in physical activity in older children and adolescents: The central role of organized sport. Social Science & Medicine, 56 (2), 363–374. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00042-4 .

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (2), 250–270. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250 .

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifi es its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101 (4), 817–835. doi: 10.1037/a0016127 .

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., & Steiger, J. H. (2010). Accomplishment in science, technol-ogy, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) and its relation to STEM educational dose: A 25-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 (4), 860–871. doi: 10.1037/a0019454 .

Witkin, H. A. (1971). A manual for the embedded fi gures test . Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Wu, H.-K., & Shah, P. (2004). Exploring visuospatial thinking in chemistry learning. Science Education, 88 (3), 465–492. doi: 10.1002/sce.10126 .

D. Reilly et al.

Page 194: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Journal of Educational Psychology

Sex Differences in Mathematics and ScienceAchievement: A Meta-Analysis of National Assessment ofEducational Progress AssessmentsDavid Reilly, David L. Neumann, and Glenda AndrewsOnline First Publication, November 10, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012

CITATIONReilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2014, November 10). Sex Differences inMathematics and Science Achievement: A Meta-Analysis of National Assessment ofEducational Progress Assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance onlinepublication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012

Page 195: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Sex Differences in Mathematics and Science Achievement:A Meta-Analysis of National Assessment of Educational

Progress Assessments

David Reilly, David L. Neumann, and Glenda AndrewsGriffith University

Gender gaps in the development of mathematical and scientific literacy have important implications forthe general public’s understanding of scientific issues and for the underrepresentation of women inscience, technology, engineering, and math. We subjected data from the National Assessment ofEducational Progress to a meta-analysis to examine whether there were sex differences in mathematicsand science achievement for students in the United States across the period 1990–2011. Results show thatthere were small but stable mean sex differences favoring males in mathematics and science across thepast 2 decades, with an effect size of d � .10 and .13, respectively, for students in 12th grade.Furthermore, there were large sex differences in high achievers, with males being overrepresented by afactor of over 2:1 at the upper right of the ability distribution for both mathematics and science. Furtherefforts are called for to reach equity in mathematics and science educational outcomes for all students.

Keywords: sex differences, mathematics, science, education, meta-analysis

The issue of sex differences in science and mathematicsachievement continues to capture the interest of parents, educators,researchers, and policy makers and has implications for the waysin which children are educated and encouraged to pursue theirchosen careers (Halpern et al., 2007; Hyde & Lindberg, 2007).Although significant inroads have been made in recent decades,women continue to be underrepresented in fields related to science,technology, engineering, and math (STEM; Handelsman et al.,2005; National Science Foundation, 2011), even though morewomen than men now attend college (Alon & Gelbgiser, 2011).Predicted shortfalls in the number of science graduates for theUnited States relative to other developing nations carry seriouseconomic and social consequences (President’s Council of Advi-sors on Science and Technology, 2010) and will require broaden-ing the pool of new entrants into STEM fields to include morewomen in order to meet the growing demand. Though the exactcausal mechanisms that contribute to sex differences in enteringmathematics and science fields are yet to be fully understood (Ceci& Williams, 2011; Hanson, Schaub, & Baker, 1996), many re-searchers believe that early sex differences in achievement atschool shape attitudes toward STEM fields and self-efficacy be-liefs (Halpern et al., 2007; Newcombe et al., 2009; Wai, Lubinski,& Benbow, 2009; Wang, Eccles, & Kenny, 2013). Furthermore,

even if they choose not to pursue a STEM-related profession,students entering college and university are increasingly requiredto have more advanced technical and quantitative skills. For thisreason the emergence of sex differences in educational achieve-ment of students is of interest to educational psychologists.

A key component of any strategy to raise the representation ofwomen in STEM fields is to address gender gaps in mathematicsand science outcomes, but the existence and magnitude of thesedifferences are strongly contested (Gallagher & Kaufman, 2005;Halpern et al., 2007; Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990; Hyde &Linn, 2006; Spelke, 2005; Wai et al., 2009). Much of the empiricalresearch in this area is somewhat dated (e.g., Hyde et al., 1990).Furthermore, as Hedges and Nowell (1995) pointed out, with fewexceptions most empirical studies in this area are subject to selec-tion and sampling biases. Furthermore, as there are interactionsbetween gender and other sociocultural factors (Becker & Hedges,1988; Frieze, 2014; Hyde & Mertz, 2009; Nowell & Hedges, 1998;Spelke, 2005) these findings do not necessarily generalize well tothe wider population. Debate about educational issues such assex-segregated schooling (Halpern, Eliot, et al., 2011) or earlyintervention programs to boost mathematics and science literacy(Hyde & Lindberg, 2007; Newcombe & Frick, 2010) can only beserved by timely and accurate empirical research into the nature ofsex differences in science and mathematics achievement (Alberts,2010; Halpern, Beninger, & Straight, 2011). Additionally, if gen-der gaps are decreasing in response to cultural and educationalchanges (Auster & Ohm, 2000; Wood & Eagly, 2012), existingresearch on sex differences in educational achievement for math-ematics and science could quickly become dated and requireperiodic reassessment (Hyde & Mertz, 2009). We describe thefindings of prior research on sex differences in these domains andthen extend these findings by reporting a meta-analysis of sexdifferences in national science and mathematical achievementfrom the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) for

David Reilly, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University; DavidL. Neumann and Glenda Andrews, School of Applied Psychology andGriffith Health Institute, Griffith University.

This research was supported in part by a Griffith University Postgrad-uate Research Scholarship.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to DavidReilly, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Southport,Queensland 4222, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Journal of Educational Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 107, No. 1, 000 0022-0663/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012

1

Page 196: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

the years 1990–2011. First, we review the theoretical frameworksthat posit the emergence of sex differences in quantitative reason-ing.

Theoretical Perspectives on Sex Differences inQuantitative Reasoning

Although reviews of intelligence testing studies find no evi-dence for sex differences in general intelligence (Halpern &Lamay, 2000; Neisser et al., 1996), consistent patterns of sexdifferences have been observed for more specific components ofcognitive ability (Halpern, 2011; Kimura, 2000). For example,women show greater proficiency with verbal ability and languagetasks and men demonstrate higher performance on tasks that tapvisuospatial abilities (Halpern & Lamay, 2000). Sex differenceshave also been documented in quantitative reasoning (our presentfocus) in tasks that assess mathematical and scientific skills (Halp-ern et al., 2007; Wai et al., 2009). A number of theoreticalperspectives have been proposed by researchers to explain why sexdifferences in quantitative reasoning might emerge; these includeboth biological and psychosocial contributions. Although a fullcritique of all these theoretical perspectives is beyond the scope ofthis study, the most prominent and well-established perspectivesmay be categorized as biological, social/environmental, or psycho-biosocial theories.

Biological Theories of Sex Differences

Sex hormones have been proposed as an explanation for groupdifferences between males and females (Collins & Kimura, 1997;Kimura, 2000), because sex hormones exert an influence on theorganization and development of the human brain before birth(Hines, 2006) and play an activational role at different points inmaturation (Hines, 1990). Associations have been found betweendigit ratio—a marker of prenatal androgen exposure—and somecognitive tasks (Collaer, Reimers, & Manning, 2007), thoughevidence has been mixed. However, most research on biologicalcontributions to sex differences has focused on sex hormoneproduction, which increases with the onset of puberty. Becausethis increase coincides with a widening of the gender gap inquantitative reasoning during adolescence and early adulthood(Hyde et al., 1990), there is an intuitive appeal to such an expla-nation. Although initial interest by researchers into the contribu-tions of sex hormones such as androgens to sex differences inquantitative reasoning was high (Kimura & Hampson, 1994),research findings have found mixed support. Some studies havefound no association, and other studies have observed that endog-enous hormone levels explain very little variance in individualperformance (Halari et al., 2005; Puts et al., 2010).

Another purported biological contribution to sex differences inquantitative reasoning comes from evolutionary psychology. Dar-win (1871) first proposed that sexual selection as a result ofevolutionary pressures has led to a differentiation in the roles ofmen and women, a theme that has been expanded upon by evolu-tionary psychology to propose an alternate explanation for why sexdifferences in quantitative reasoning emerge (Archer, 1996; Geary,1996). In the past, it was adaptive for males to develop and honespatial skills for navigation and hunting (Buss, 1995), leading tothe development of greater visuospatial ability in males. This in

turn lays down the foundation for the development of quantitativereasoning through a variety of mechanisms including differingsocial roles and sex typing of children’s’ play activities (Caplan &Caplan, 1994; Geary, 1996, 2010). Furthermore, the traditionallyfeminine roles of caring for others and sensitivity to emotions mayhave been adaptive, resulting in a tendency for women to focus onpeople over things (Su, Rounds, & Armstrong, 2009), which Hyde(2014) argued may decrease motivation to acquire quantitativeskills and pursue a STEM-based career. A common theme in sucharguments is an interaction between biology and environment,rather than a strictly deterministic role of biology.

Social and Environmental Contributions

Although biological factors may make a modest contribution tosex differences, many theorists argue that psychological and socialfactors exert a greater influence over the course of a lifetime. Onesuch theory is Eagly and Wood’s social-role theory (Eagly, 1987;Eagly & Wood, 1999), which proposes that any psychological sexdifferences arise from the distribution of men and women’s rolesin society. The gendered division of labor between men andwomen encourages the development of instrumental andachievement-oriented traits in men and expressive and communal-oriented traits in women. Such a position is also compatible withgender schema theory (Bem, 1981), which proposes that childrendevelop an internal schema about the sex typing of interests andbehavior and that they are motivated to behave in a mannerconsistent with their internal sex-role identity (Martin & Ruble,2004). From an early age children learn to categorize things asinherently masculine or feminine (Kagan, 1964), including schoolsubjects like mathematics and science (Nosek et al., 2009). Theseform the foundation for sex typing of interests and activities, whichfacilitates the development of specific cognitive abilities. Nash(1979) formalized this as a sex-role mediation explanation forcognitive sex differences, theorizing that masculine identificationleads to cultivation of spatial, mathematical, and scientific skills(Reilly & Neumann, 2013; Signorella & Jamison, 1986).

Another prominent theory was put forward by Caplan andCaplan (1994), who argued that traditionally “masculine” playactivities promote the development of spatial ability by encourag-ing the practice and application of spatial skills (Serbin, Zelkowitz,Doyle, Gold, & Wheaton, 1990). Other theorists argue that genderconformity pressures also play an affective role in developingone’s talents. Highly sex-typed individuals are motivated to keeptheir behavior consistent with internalized sex-role standards andnorms, but those low in sex typing show greater cognitive andbehavioral flexibility (Bem, 1975; Martin & Ruble, 2004; Spence,1984). This has implications for success in academic domains thatare traditionally male dominated, such as science and mathematics(Eccles, 2007). Conversely, as we see changes in the segregationof men’s and women’s roles and increasing gender equality, wemight also see a diminishing of sex differences in these areas overtime (Hyde, 2014).

Psychobiosocial Theories of Sex Differences

Theorists may be divided over the relative share of nature andnurture in the emergence of sex differences in cognitive abilities,but there is a growing consensus that both make a meaningful

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

2 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 197: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

contribution and neither in isolation cannot explain sex differences(Wood & Eagly, 2013). Indeed, it may be impractical to separatea specific biological and social component and study them inisolation, as their effects are reciprocal in nature (Halpern, 2011).Many theorists have therefore adopted psychobiosocial models forexplaining the development of sex differences (Halpern & Tan,2001; Hausmann, Schoofs, Rosenthal, & Jordan, 2009); theseincorporate elements of biological, psychosocial, and socioculturalfactors to explain group differences between males and females atthe population level.

These theories offer perspectives on why sex differences inquantitative reasoning may be found, but it is also important toconsider the many ways in which males and females are alike.Hyde (2005) has proposed the gender similarities hypothesis,which argues that men and women are more similar than different.Specifically, it hypothesizes that sex differences in cognition areeither small in magnitude or nonexistent. Although this hypothesisis not supported for language (Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Stoet & Geary,2013) and spatial abilities (Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995), wheresex differences are moderately large, the gender similarities hy-pothesis may be compatible with the existence of sex differencesin quantitative reasoning, as these tend to be somewhat smaller inmagnitude (Hyde et al., 1990). However, the gender similaritieshypothesis would be incompatible with sex differences that aremoderate or large in magnitude, such as a gender imbalance in thesex ratio of high-achieving students in mathematics and science(Benbow, 1988; Hedges & Nowell, 1995). It is also a hypothesisthat is can easily be put to the test, by examining the performanceof men and women in tests that tap quantitative reasoning skills.

Previous Meta-Analyses of Sex Differences inMathematics and Science

Meta-analysis of national testing data by Hedges and Nowell(1995) from several decades of assessment (1960s–1990s) re-vealed small mean differences favoring males in mathematics andscience performance (ranging from d � .03 to d � .26 for math-ematics and d � .11 to d � .50 for science). Although mean sexdifferences might play an important role in the underrepresentationof women in STEM fields, other researchers have noted that thedistribution of performance in a number of cognitive domains ismore variable for males than for females (Feingold, 1992; Hyde,2005; Machin & Pekkarinen, 2008). Even if there were no differ-ences in the average performance of males and females on aspecific ability test, greater variance in the male group would resultin an overrepresentation in the extreme tails of the distribution(Feingold, 1992; Halpern et al., 2007; Turkheimer & Halpern,2009), such as the intellectually gifted from which many STEMresearchers hail (Wai, Cacchio, Putallaz, & Makel, 2010). Forexample, sex (male:female) ratios of students at the 95th percentilein the above-mentioned data sets ranged from 1.5 to 2.4 in math-ematics and 2.5 to 7.0 in science achievement across samples(Hedges & Nowell, 1995). This can translate to a disparity ineducational outcomes, and some researchers have argued that sexdifferences in variability may be more important than the meandifferences (Feingold, 1995; Humphreys, 1988; Machin &Pekkarinen, 2008).

The greater male variability hypothesis can be examinedthrough calculation of the variance ratio (VR), defined as the ratio

of male variance to female variance (Feingold, 1992; Hedges &Nowell, 1995; Turkheimer & Halpern, 2009). A variability ratio of1.00 indicates that males and females are equal in variance. VRvalues less than 1.00 indicate that females show more variabilitythan males, and VR values greater than 1.00 reflect greater malevariability (Priess & Hyde, 2010). Feingold (1994) argued thatvalues between 0.90 and 1.10 ought to be regarded as negligible(i.e., homogeneity of variance), and this practice is adopted herein.

More recently, Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, and Williams(2008) presented data from a subset of the National Assessment ofEducational Progress (NAEP), a nationally representative proba-bility sample drawn from all 50 U.S. states. The advantage of thissampling method is that national NAEP data provide a reliablepopulation-level estimate of student performance, reflecting thedemographic traits of the general population of students. Althoughindividual state and national performance data were not availableat the time, Hyde et al. (2008) obtained data from a selection of 10states across Grades 2 though 11. Mean sex differences were small(ds from �.02 to .06). Hyde (2014) has characterized these dif-ferences as “trivial” in size, and others have used this research toargue that sex differences are no longer found in modern samples(Hyde & Mertz, 2009; Lindberg, Hyde, Petersen, & Linn, 2010).

Although Hyde et al. (2008) conducted their analysis with themost recent information available at the time, a key limitation oftheir methodology is that only a 10-state subset of the national dataset was analyzed. Hedges and Nowell (1995) argued there arelimitations to the use of samples that show a selection bias,because the conclusions they yield may be erroneous if attemptingto generalize to the wider population (Becker & Hedges, 1988;Spelke, 2005; Stumpf, 1995). In particular, use of such samplesmay affect the magnitude of any observed gender gap, as literaturesuggests an interaction between student and socioeconomic back-ground for many cognitive abilities (Hanscombe et al., 2012;Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco, Newcombe, & Huttenlocher, 2005).National assessments of the NAEP are also drawn from bothpublic and private schools and thus may better reflect the demo-graphic composition of students enrolled in U.S. educational in-stitutions than analysis of only public school data.

The national test data from the NAEP are now publicly availablefor researchers, and they provide a broader sampling of studentsthan was available at the time to Hyde et al. (2008). We present ananalysis of national NAEP performance for boys and girls, allow-ing for an empirical test of claims of sex differences in mathemat-ics for U.S. students in the present day. Furthermore, because dataare now available across several decades, it is possible to examinetemporal trends across the year of assessment as well as develop-mental trends across grade level of students (Hyde et al., 2008;Lindberg et al., 2010). Although the NAEP assesses mathematicsmore regularly, periodic national testing of science performancemakes it possible to assess gender gaps in this domain as well. Sexdifferences in science achievement may also play a role in thedecision of individuals to pursue a science-related profession.

We focused on four key research questions for the domains ofmathematics and science. First, are there sex differences in overallmathematics and science achievement for modern samples ofstudents in the United States, and is the gap diminishing over time?Sociocultural theories of sex differences would predict a decline inthe magnitude of sex differences over time, but biological and

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

3SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 198: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

psychobiosocial theories would be compatible with stability ineffect sizes.

Second, do males show greater variability in performancethan females, as predicted by biological theories? Third, if thereare sex differences in means and in variance, what is theircombined contribution to the proportion of males and femalesattaining an advanced proficiency standard in mathematics andscience achievement? Finally, if there are sex differences inscience achievement, are they present for all of the three contentareas assessed (earth science, physical sciences, life sciences)?These research questions also provide a test of the sex differ-ences and similarities hypothesis, which would predict thateffect sizes are small in magnitude.

Method

National Assessment of EducationalProgress Data Source

The NAEP is a project of the National Center for EducationStatistics (NCES), part of the U.S. Department of Education.NAEP conducts assessments across a range of subjects, includingreading, writing, mathematics, history, civics, geography, and sci-ence. Each subject area is assessed periodically, and the mostfrequently assessed subjects are reading, mathematics, and science.National and state performance in each assessment are reportedpublicly in a series of documents titled “The Nation’s ReportCard.” These documents provide a review of major trends writtenin language accessible to parents, educators, and policy makers(http://nationsreportcard.gov/). These form part of the main NAEPassessment, which uses a modern mathematics and science curric-ulum with large sample sizes and frequent assessments. A second-ary category of assessment is the NAEP long-term trends (LTT)assessment of mathematics, which samples students on an earliercurriculum framework from the 1970s onward. The LTT assessesmore basic mathematical content, such as numbers, shapes, mea-surement, and probability; the main assessment also includes al-gebra, geometry, and problem solving. Additionally, the LTTrestricts students to hand calculations, which limits the depth ofcomplexity for assessment items. Although useful information canbe obtained from the long-term trend assessments, it fails toadequately assess students’ knowledge of more advanced mathe-matical content included in the main assessment frameworks and issampled less frequently than the main assessment. As such, it wasdeemed unsuitable for analysis, and only the main assessment datawere reported in the main article. However, published reports ofthe LTT long-term assessments show a consistent gender gap infavor of males in mathematics for students at age 13 and 17 thathas remained essentially unchanged since assessments began(Rampey, Dion, & Donahue, 2009).

The results of NAEP assessments are made freely available toresearchers for secondary analysis via the NAEP Data Explorer(http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/naepdata/).The target popu-lation for NAEP national assessments is made up of all students inany educational institution (from both private and public school-ing), currently enrolled in the target grade (4, 8, and 12). Schooland student responses are appropriately weighted to draw anestimate of the target population that reflects student demographics

(e.g., specific ethnic and socioeconomic groups). This may meanthat some students and schools will be oversampled or under-sampled, as appropriate. These weights are applied to draw anestimate of national student performance, reported through theNAEP Data Explorer. Additional information about sampling de-sign is available from the NAEP website (https://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/mathematics/sampledesign.asp).

Mathematics framework. The mathematics assessmentframework covers five key content areas, which have remained thesame since 1990. These are (a) number properties and operations;(b) measurement; (c) geometry; (d) data analysis, statistics, andprobability; and (e) algebra. Students are assessed at a grade-level-appropriate standard (for example, at Grade 8 the topic of algebraincludes linear equations, whereas at Grade 12 this topic is ex-tended to include quadratic and exponential equations). Assess-ment items vary in complexity level to accommodate a wide rangeof ability levels. This is important, as some research has notedgreater sex differences are present for complex problem-solvingitems (Hyde et al., 1990). Calculators are permitted for approxi-mately one third of the assessment, but the remaining questionsmust be completed without calculators. The mathematics frame-work for assessment of Grades 4 and 8 is comparable with that forearlier assessments, allowing student performance in more recentyears to be compared to those from earlier assessments. Althougha revised mathematics framework was instituted in 2005 for stu-dents in Grade 12, these assessments are comparable to thoseadministered previously, as they reflect similar content areas. Furtherinformation on the mathematics content areas can be found at theNAEP website (http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/mathematics/whatmeasure.aspx).

Science framework. Topic areas for science assessment aregrouped into the following three domains, which both form sepa-rate subscales and contribute to the overall science achievementscore:

• Physical sciences, including concepts related to properties andchanges of matter, forms of energy, energy transfer and conserva-tion, position and motion of objects, and forces affecting motion.

• Life sciences, including organization and development of cellsand organisms, matter and energy transformations, interdepen-dence, heredity and reproduction, evolution and diversity.

• Earth and space sciences, including concepts relating to ob-jects in the universe, the history of the Earth, material properties,tectonics and energy in Earth systems, climate and weather, andbiogeochemical cycles.

The science framework used for assessment was revised in2009, in response to revised national science education standards.Although the content areas remained the same (physical, earth, andlife science), they now include coverage of space science. Studentscompleted a range of multiple-choice and open-ended questions,including hands-on practical science tasks and interactive computer-administered tasks, from the 2009 assessment onward. For additionalinformation about the science framework and sample questions, seehttp://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/science/whatmeasure.aspx.

Reliability of the NAEP instrument. Multiple choice itemsare computer scored, and constructed response are marked byraters. Consistency across markers for the constructed responseitems was generally high for both mathematics and science (Co-hen’s � � .80). Item response theory is then employed by NCESto measure latent scores, which offers greater control over the

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

4 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 199: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

measurement characteristics of each question and ensures highreliability. (See http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/tdw/analysis/for additional information about reliability of measures.) Further-more, the NCES conducted a NAEP–Trends in International Math-ematics and Science Study (TIMSS) linking study to compare theassessment frameworks to international standards, finding themcomparable.

Schedule of Assessment

Mathematics and science assessments are conducted periodi-cally, in adherence with the NAEP schedule. Mathematics isassessed more frequently, roughly every two to three years (1990,1992, 1996, 2000, 2003, 2005, 2007, 2009, 2011). The schedule ofassessments gives greater coverage to Grades 4 and 8, which aredevelopmentally critical time periods for the acquisition of math-ematics and scientific skills (Newcombe & Frick, 2010). Grade 12assessment was not conducted in 2003 and 2011. Science isassessed every four to five years (1996, 2000, 2005, 2009, 2011)and with somewhat smaller samples of students than for themathematics assessments. Grades 4, 8, and 12 were all assessed inthe science target years, except for 2011.

In addition to achieving an overall test score, students areevaluated against fixed achievement levels in the NAEP, whichcategorize students at a basic, proficient, and advanced level.Sex differences in the percentage of students attaining theselevels are also available and were obtained from the NAEP DataExplorer. Although some researchers have examined sex dif-ferences in the extreme upper tail of mathematics and sciencedistributions (Benbow, 1988; Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Hyde etal., 2008; Nowell & Hedges, 1998; Wai et al., 2010), Hyde andMertz (2009) have questioned whether sex differences in ex-treme talent are a necessary requirement for pursuing STEM-related fields. When greater male variability is present, this maypresent an exaggerated picture of sex differences, particularly ifmore stringent cutoff points are examined (e.g., 99.9th percen-tile). Examining sex ratios in attainment of an advanced profi-ciency in science or mathematics represents a trade-off betweenselecting a cutoff point that is germane to the question ofunderrepresentation of women in STEM-related fields andseeking to avoid selecting an ability level that serves to exag-gerate sex differences.

Participants

National performance data in NAEP mathematics were exam-ined for the period 1990–2011, with a combined total sample sizeof almost 2 million students (see Table 1). Performance data inscience were examined for the period 1996–2011. Science wasassessed less frequently and with fewer students, with a combinedtotal sample size of over 800,000. Information on sample sizes wasobtained from annual reports of the NAEP, which in recent yearsfollowed the convention of rounding to the nearest hundred. Whenindividual numbers of males and females were not reported, theassumption of equal sample sizes was made. Additional informa-tion on the schedule of assessments and sample size of individualassessment years can be found in the Appendix.

Meta-Analytic Procedure

Mean math and science scores and standard deviation for malesand females were obtained from the Data Explorer website. TheNAEP Data Explorer provides summary statistics (i.e., mean,standard deviation) rounded off to whole numbers, which intro-duces measurement imprecision. It can also export more precisevalues in Excel format, which was the option used in this meta-analysis. The unit of analysis was group differences in perfor-mance of males and females at the national level, rather than forindividual states. Effect sizes are reported as the mean differencebetween males and females in standardized units (Cohen, 1988;Hedges, 2008), commonly referred to as Cohen’s d. By conven-tion, a positive value for d indicates higher male performance anda negative value indicates higher female performance (Hyde,2005).

Comprehensive Meta Analysis (CMA) V2 and Microsoft Excelsoftware were used to calculate the statistics. Meta-analysis typi-cally employs either a fixed-effects or a random-effects model forcombining study samples. As NAEP assessments span a number ofdecades recruiting from independent samples, and it was hypoth-esized that student characteristics may have changed across yearsof sampling, a random-effects model was chosen (Borenstein,Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). The random effects modelgives slightly wider confidence intervals than a fixed-effectsmodel, but it gives a more appropriate estimate of how muchvariability is present across samples (Hunter & Schmidt, 2000;Kelley & Kelley, 2012). The benefit of such an approach is that wecan have greater confidence in the population estimate of sexdifferences produced and that it is not the result of inflated Type Ierror. Using a random effects statistical model also adjusts forvariation in test content and student characteristics over time.

In addition to calculating effect size data for each grade level,we investigated whether the year of assessment was a potentialmoderator with the technique of meta-regression (Kelley & Kelley,2012). Meta-regression extends a conventional meta-analysis bydetermining whether a moderating variable accounts for variationin the magnitude of an observed effect (i.e., explains sources ofheterogeneity). Based on claims of diminishing gender gaps (e.g.,Hyde & Linn, 2006), a negative association with year of assess-ment was predicted. Although it is clear that sex differences inmathematics are smaller than systematic reviews had found in datafrom the 1960s–1980s (Hedges & Nowell, 1995), it is not apparentwhether such a trend would continue to the point at which males

Table 1Sample Size Information for Mathematics andScience Assessments

Content domain Grade N students assessed

Mathematics 4 974,7008 845,400

12 104,900Total 1,925,100

Science 4 352,1058 470,374

12 56,437Total 878,916

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

5SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 200: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

and females would perform equivalently (Caplan & Caplan, 1994)or whether it would plateau. We employed a random effects model(method of moments) for the meta-regression model to test if theyear of assessment acted as a moderator (Borenstein et al., 2009;Thompson & Higgins, 2002). Additionally, we performed sub-group analysis for individual grades with a random effects modelto examine whether sex differences change as students progressthrough their schooling, as indicated by previous research (Hyde etal., 1990).

Variance ratios (VR) for individual samples were calculatedfollowing the method of Feingold (1992). Estimates of overallmale and female variance ratios were combined across years ofsampling for each grade level. Some researchers have questionedwhether, when variance ratios are combined across samples, meanvariance ratios may be the most appropriate measure (Katzman &Alliger, 1992) and have advocated the use of medians or logtransformed means. These metrics are most appropriate if thedirection of variance ratios change across samples (i.e., greatermale variability is found in some samples, and greater femalevariability is found in others). Although this was not the case (seethe Appendix), by convention and for comparability with otherstudies the log transformed variance ratios were averaged acrosssample years and then transformed back into the Fisher’s varianceratio statistic. This statistic addresses whether males and femalesdiffer at the extreme tails of an ability distribution (e.g., the top 1%of gifted students) rather than focusing on the performance of the“average” students in the middle of the distribution (Priess &Hyde, 2010).

Additionally, the percentages of students for each gender whoachieved an advanced proficiency standard were obtained to in-vestigate the combined effect of sex differences in central ten-dency and variability. Sex ratios, defined as the relative risk ratio(RR) of male to female students, were calculated for mathematicsand science performance at the advanced level of proficiency. Thismethodology is a somewhat different than that followed in previ-ous studies. It represents a trade-off between selecting a cutoffpoint that fairly evaluates high-achieving students in their ability tosolve STEM problems and selecting an arbitrarily high cutoff (e.g.,99th percentile) that would serve to exaggerate sex differences.

Results

We conducted two separate meta-analyses on the NAEP samplefor mathematics and science, with population-level estimates ofsex differences partitioned by grade level (4, 8, 12). Althoughstatistically significant sex differences favoring males were foundin each grade (p � .001), emphasis is placed on effect size, as thisgives an indication of the magnitude and practical impact of theobserved differences (Hedges, 2008; Hyde, 2005). In a review ofmeta-analytic theory and practice, Hyde and Grabe (2008, p. 170)recommended a threshold for considering effect sizes in sex dif-ferences research a priori and argued that effect sizes smaller thand � .10 be considered “trivial” per Hyde’s (2005) gender similar-ities hypothesis. Accordingly we use this threshold herein forconsidering whether the observed sex differences are practicallymeaningful. Variance ratios and the sex ratio of students attainingthe advanced level of proficiency are also reported for math andscience. The original data used in this analysis are presented in theAppendix.

NAEP Assessment of Mathematics

National performance data in mathematics were examined forthe period 1990–2011 (see the Appendix for a schedule of assess-ment years). National sex differences are somewhat larger thanthose reported by Hyde et al. (2008) in their 10-state sample, witha weighted mean effect size of d � .07, Z � 12.07, p � .001.However there was considerable heterogeneity present in the dis-tribution of effect sizes, Q(23) � 251.57, p � .001, I2 � 90.86 (seeFigure 1). In order to better explain variability across assessments,we tested whether grade level and year of assessment were poten-tial moderators.

Grade level as a moderator. Table 2 presents comparisonsbetween males and females in math across the three grade levels.When effect sizes were partitioned across the three measured agegroups with subgroup analysis, there was a statistically significantdifference between grade levels, Q(2) � 23.15, p � .001. Al-though sex differences were extremely small in elementary andearly high school, they grew larger in the final year of high school(d � .10). The Grade 12 effect size is at the threshold of Hyde’s(2005) criterion for nontrivial sex differences.

Year of assessment as a moderator. Next we performed ameta-regression analysis to test for a declining gender gap inmathematics over time. Contrary to our hypothesis, there was nosignificant effect of assessment year, Z � �.10, b � �.0001,CI95% [�.0016, .0015], p � .923; nor was the interaction betweenyear and grade significant. This is consistent with other studies thatreported stability for mean sex differences in mathematics inrecent decades rather than a declining trend (McGraw, Lubienski,& Strutchens, 2006; Rampey et al., 2009).

Variance ratios. In line with previous research, the variabilityof males’ performance in mathematics was wider than that offemales across each age group (see Table 3) and exceeded Fein-gold’s (1994) threshold for nontrivial variance ratios. These vari-ance ratios were also stable across the time period examined, withno association with year of assessment or grade (p � .05).

Figure 1. Histogram of observed effect sizes in NAEP mathematicsassessments (1990–2011). NAEP � National Assessment of EducationalProgress.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

6 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 201: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender gaps in high achievers for mathematics. In order toevaluate the combined effect of mean differences and greater malevariability, we calculated the ratio of males:females attaining theadvanced proficiency standard for mathematics, RR � 1.51, Z �15.36, p � .001. As there was significant heterogeneity acrossassessments, Q(23) � 300.99, p � .001, I2 � 92.35, we calculatedrisk ratios separately for each grade level with subgroup analysis(see Table 3). There was a statistically significant difference in sexratios between grades, Q(2) � 61.74, p � .001. There was amoderate overrepresentation of high-achieving males in Grades 4and 8, but sex ratios increased considerably by Grade 12 to a ratioof 2.13 males to every female student. Although these ratios arestill smaller than reported from earlier decades (e.g., Benbow,1988; Hedges & Nowell, 1995), they remain important targets foreducational intervention to encourage and foster high achievement.

Additionally, we tested whether there was a decline in thegender gap for high achievers over time, finding a significantinteraction between grade and year of assessment (p � .05). Toinvestigate, we performed a meta-regression on year of assessmentfor each grade level. Although there was a tendency towardslightly smaller sex ratios for Grade 4 students over time,Z � �4.45, b � �.0247, CI95% [�.0355, �.0138], p � .001, therewas no association between year of assessment and high achieversin Grades 8 (Z � �.37, p � .711) and 12 (Z � �1.15, p � .249),indicating stability across the time period examined.

NAEP Assessment of Science

National performance data in science was examined for the period1996–2011 (see the Appendix for schedule). Overall, the sex differ-ence between males and females was small and comparable to sexdifferences in mathematics (d � .11, Z � 9.15, p � .001). Howeverthere was considerable heterogeneity across assessments, Q(11) �328.22, p � .001, I2 � 96.33 (see Figure 2). In order to better explainvariability across assessments, we tested whether grade level and yearof assessment were potential moderators.

Grade level as a moderator. Using subgroup analysis wepartitioned effect sizes across the three grade levels, reducingheterogeneity somewhat. Table 4 presents sex differences in sci-ence achievement across each grade level and shows significantdifferences favoring males across all grades. Although the ob-served effect sizes were small in magnitude, values for Grade 8and Grade 12 exceed Hyde’s (2005) criteria for negligible sexdifferences (d � .12 and .13, respectively).

Year of assessment as a moderator. Next we performed ameta-regression analysis to test the effect of assessment year as apotential moderator. Contrary to our hypothesis of a declininggender gap in science over time, there was no significant effect ofthe year of assessment on the magnitude of sex differences inscience, b � .00, CI95%[�.0039, .0057], Z � .37, p � .711; norwas there an interaction between year and grade.

Variance ratios. Consistent with previous research, the vari-ability of boys’ performance in science was larger than that ofgirls’ (see Table 5). Variance ratios across all grades exceededFeingold’s (1994) criterion for greater male variability and werecomparable to that found for mathematics. These variance ratioswere also stable across the time period examined, with no associ-ation with year of assessment or interaction with grade (p � .05).

Gender gaps in high achievers for science. The influence ofgreater male variability is most readily apparent when looking atsex ratios for attainment of an advanced proficiency standard inscience. We calculated the risk ratio of males:females attaining theadvanced proficiency standard for science, RR � 1.85, Z � 12.81,p � .001. As there was significant heterogeneity across assess-ments, Q(12) � 83.32, p � .001, I2 � 85.63, we calculated riskratios separately for each grade level using subgroup analysis (seeTable 5). This reduced heterogeneity somewhat. Sex ratios forstudents were modest in Grade 4 (1.56) but grew wider for olderstudents in Grade 8 (1.88) and Grade 12 (2.28). There was also asignificant difference in science gender gaps between grades, withbetween-groups heterogeneity, Q(2) � 9.05, p � .011.

Table 2Sex Differences in NAEP Mathematics Achievement for Grades 4, 8, and 12

Grade k Cohen’s d

95% confidence interval Test of null (2-tail)

HeterogeneityLower limit Upper limit Z p

4 9 .07 .06 .09 10.67 �.001 Q(8) � 90.37, p � .001, I2 � 91.158 9 .04 .03 .06 6.54 �.001 Q(8) � 28.52, p � .001, I2 � 71.95

12 6 .10 .08 .12 10.08 �.001 Q(5) � 10.71, p � ns

Note. k denotes the number of assessments conducted for each grade. Effect sizes that exceed Hyde’s (2005) criterion for nontrivial differences (d � .10)are highlighted in bold. NAEP � National Assessment of Educational Progress; ns � nonsignificant.

Table 3Sex Differences in Variability and Sex Ratios Attaining Advanced Proficiency in Mathematics

GradeVariance

ratioRiskratio

95% confidence interval Test of null (2-tail)

HeterogeneityLower limit Upper limit Z p

4 1.12 1.51 1.42 1.60 13.71 �.001 Q(8) � 94.30, p � .001, I2 � 91.518 1.12 1.30 1.23 1.37 9.27 �.001 Q(8) � 24.71, p � .002, I2 � 67.63

12 1.15 2.13 1.90 2.38 13.28 �.001 Q(5) � 8.77, p � ns

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

7SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 202: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Additionally, we tested whether there was a decline in thegender gap for high achievers over time or an interaction betweengrade and year. Although there was no significant association withyear of assessment overall (Z � 0.84, p � .401), the interactionwas significant (p � .05), and we examined effects of year for eachlevel of grade. There was no significant association with year ofassessments for Grades 4 (Z � �.13, p � .899) and 12 (Z � �.58,p � .557), but there was a significant trend toward slightly largerscience sex ratios in more recent years for students in Grade 8, Z �2.98, b � �.0260, CI95% [�.0009, .0431], p � .003.

Science domains. Overall science achievement shows onlypart of the picture, however. NAEP assesses science literacy acrossthree subject domains: physical sciences, earth sciences, and lifesciences (see Table 6). If group differences were present across allthree domains, sex differences in overall science literacy might bean appropriate target for intervention. However, this was not thecase. Although small sex differences were found in physical sci-ences (d � .13) and earth sciences (d � .17), there were nosignificant differences for life sciences. The absence of a statisti-cally significant sex difference in life sciences is consistent withthe findings of the National Educational Longitudinal Study(Burkam, Lee, & Smerdon, 1997) and the Trends in Mathematicsand Science Study (Neuschmidt, Barth, & Hastedt, 2008), whichreport finding no sex differences in the field of life sciences. We

note however that greater male variability was present for allcontent areas and grades.

There was also considerable heterogeneity of effect sizes acrossassessments, which may be due in part to the reduced coverage ofassessments conducted for science, as well as the smaller samplesizes employed (particularly for Grade 12). Accordingly, moder-ator analysis was also performed for each science content domainto determine if grade and year effects were present. There was noeffect of year of assessment across all three measures or interac-tions between grade and year of assessment. Although there wereno significant effects of grade level for earth and life sciences,there was a tendency for larger sex differences in physical sciencesfor older students.

Discussion

Our aim in this study was to evaluate the evidence for sexdifferences in mathematics and science achievement over a broadspan of years and to determine whether these were diminishingover time in response to educational advancements and culturalchanges in the roles of men and women (Auster & Ohm, 2000;Wood & Eagly, 2012). The NAEP data set provided an extremelylarge nationally representative sample of students collected over awide time span, and it affords a more accurate and reliable test ofsex differences in STEM achievement than can be obtained froma single sample. In doing so it extends coverage of the earlieranalysis by Hedges and Nowell (1995) to include the most recentlyavailable data (1990–2011).

Sex Differences in Means

In contrast to the analysis by Hyde et al. (2008), which found nodifference in a 10-state subset of the national assessment, analysisof the complete NAEP data set found a small but nontrivial meandifference in mathematics favoring males for students in their finalyear of year of schooling. Furthermore, we extended the analysisto include national testing of science achievement with similarfindings. These findings make the claim that sex differences inquantitative reasoning have been eliminated in modern samplessomewhat premature, but neither is there evidence of a widedisparity between the performance of the average male and femalestudent. It is also consistent with U.S. performance in internationaltests of science and mathematics, which have found only small sexdifferences (Else-Quest, Hyde, & Linn, 2010; Guiso, Monte, Sa-pienza, & Zingales, 2008; Reilly, 2012).

It is unclear exactly why the earlier meta-analysis by Hyde et al.(2008) on a small subset of testing data found no difference in

Table 4Sex Differences in NAEP Science Achievement for Grades 4, 8, and 12

Grade k Cohen’s d

95% confidence interval Test of null (2-tail)

HeterogeneityLower limit Upper limit Z p

4 4 .08 .04 .12 3.64 .001 Q(3) � 174.57, p � .001, I2 � 98.288 4 .12 .08 .16 6.39 �.001 Q(3) � 41.93, p � .001, I2 � 90.46

12 4 .13 .09 .18 6.05 �.001 Q(3) � 24.68, p � .001, I2 � 87.84

Note. Effect sizes that exceed Hyde’s (2005) criterion for nontrivial differences (d � .10) are highlighted in bold. NAEP � National Assessment ofEducational Progress.

Figure 2. Histogram of observed effect sizes in NAEP science assess-ments (1996–2011). NAEP � National Assessment of Educational Prog-ress.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

8 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 203: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

NAEP mathematics performance, although sex differences in thenational data set were somewhat larger. It may be due to educa-tional factors (inherent differences from state to state), from theinclusion of private and public institutions in the national data set,or that when a more representative sample and less selectivesample is collected greater sex differences emerge (Hyde et al.,1990). We also note that the magnitude of these mean sex differ-ences in the NAEP was smaller than similar assessments collectedin the decades prior to 1990 for mathematics and science (Hedges& Nowell, 1995), which would be consistent with changes pre-dicted by sociocultural perspectives. However, there was no asso-ciation between the magnitude of the sex difference observed ineach assessment and the assessment year, indicating that there wasstability across the period of time investigated (1990–2011). Thatno further change occurred over this time frame would be com-patible with biological and psychobiological perspectives. Stabil-ity is also consistent with the findings of McGraw et al., (2006),who found no change across a shorter time frame for NAEPmathematics performance. We found meaningful sex differences,but this does not necessarily preclude Hyde’s gender similaritieshypothesis as it posits that sex differences in cognitive ability areonly small in magnitude.

The data also indicated that there was a developmental trendacross both types of quantitative reasoning skills, with smallereffect sizes in elementary school and larger effect sizes in olderstudents. Sex differences in mathematics exceed Hyde’s criterionin Grade 12, whereas sex differences in science achievement reach

a nontrivial size in Grades 8 and 12. A prior meta-analysis (Hydeet al., 1990) also found larger sex differences are observed whencomplex problem-solving tasks are measured, and the mathematicsassessment framework increases in complexity during Grades 8and 12. This is also consistent with developmental literature re-porting a widening of the gender gap in quantitative reasoning ataround puberty and middle school (Fan, Chen, & Matsumoto,1997; Hyde et al., 1990; Robinson & Lubienski, 2011), when thesaliency of gender roles becomes more prominent as suggested bysociocultural perspectives on gender (Nash, 1979; Ruble, Martin,& Berenbaum, 2006). During adolescence and into early adult-hood, gender stereotyping about the sex typing of activities andinterests increases at both the explicit and implicit level (Halpern& Tan, 2001; Nosek et al., 2009; Steffens & Jelenec, 2011), whichhas implications for sex differences in achievement motivation andself-efficacy for mathematics and science (Priess & Hyde, 2010;Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006). How-ever, it also coincides with a time of increased hormonal changesas outlined by biological theories (Kimura, 2000), and offeringmore than speculation as to the origins of sex differences at thesedevelopmental periods is therefore difficult.

Of particular interest in our analysis is the observation that meansex differences were present for some, but not all, of the scientificdomains assessed by the NAEP. Despite the considerable samplesize there was no sex difference found for biology and life sci-ences, where males and females show equivalent performance(Neuschmidt et al., 2008). Reviews of the literature find that males

Table 5Sex Differences in Variability, and Sex Ratios Attaining Advanced Proficiency in Science

GradeVariance

ratioRiskratio

95% confidence interval Test of null (2-tail)

HeterogeneityLower limit Upper limit Z p

4 1.09 1.56 1.33 1.83 5.45 �.001 Q(3) � 19.20, p � .001, I2 � 84.378 1.12 1.88 1.64 2.16 8.95 �.001 Q(4) � 34.32, p � .001, I2 � 88.34

12 1.14 2.28 1.88 2.76 8.41 �.001 Q(3) � 4.77, p � ns

Table 6Sex Differences Across NAEP Science Domains for Grades 4, 8, and 12

Sciencedomain Grade

Varianceratio Cohen’s d

95% confidence intervalTest of null

(2-tail)

HeterogeneityLower limit Upper limit Z p

Earth science 4 1.08 .16 .12 .21 7.27 �.001 Q(3) � 147.58, p � .001, I2 � 97.978 1.10 .15 .11 .19 7.46 �.001 Q(3) � 57.84, p � .001, I2 � 93.08

12 1.15 .21 .17 .26 9.04 �.001 Q(3) � 63.84, p � .001, I2 � 95.30Overall .17 .13 .21 8.54 �.001 between groups, Q(2) � 4.16, p � .125

Physical science 4 1.11 .05 .02 .09 3.10 .003 Q(3) � 35.15, p � .001, I2 � 91.478 1.13 .17 .14 .20 10.94 �.001 Q(4) � 103.27, p � .001, I2 � 96.13

12 1.14 .18 .14 .22 9.64 �.001 Q(3) � 20.56, p � .001, I2 � 85.41Overall .13 .06 .21 3.31 �.001 between groups, Q(2) � 32.33, p � .001

Life science 4 1.06 .01 �.05 .06 0.22 .826 Q(3) � 367.40, p � .001, I2 � 99.188 1.10 .04 �.01 .09 1.61 .107 Q(4) � 37.26, p � .001, I2 � 89.27

12 1.10 .01 �.04 .07 0.46 .645 Q(3) � 18.37, p � .001, I2 � 83.67Overall .02 �.01 .05 1.38 .167 between groups, Q(2) � 0.94, p � .624

Note. Effect sizes that exceed Hyde’s (2005) criterion for nontrivial differences (d � .10) are highlighted in bold. NAEP � National Assessment ofEducational Progress.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

9SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 204: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

have greater overall interest in science than females do and ratetheir aptitude more highly (Osborne, Simon, & Collins, 2003;Weinburgh, 1995). But when inquiries are made regarding interestin specific scientific domains, biology and life sciences show nosignificant difference between males and females (Miller, Bless-ing, & Schwartz, 2006). Rather than indicating any inherent lack ofability, sex differences in certain but not all domains of sciencemay reflect different patterns of interest and motivation towardpeople-oriented fields (Su et al., 2009), or that other domains areseen as being less relevant to future career paths (Jones, Howe, &Rua, 2000; Miller et al., 2006). Alternately, the mathematicalrequirements of biology and life sciences may be lower than forthe physical sciences, or there may be reduced sex-typing stereo-types for this field of study.

High Achievers

Sex difference research often focuses on the performance of theaverage student, but considerably less attention is given to sexdifferences in the prevalence of high achievers and those factorsthat contribute to their success (Wai, Putallaz, & Makel, 2012).Although only small mean differences in mathematics and scienceachievement were found, consistent with prior research the per-formance of males showed consistently greater variability than thatof female students (Hedges & Nowell, 1995). Greater male vari-ability in performance is often associated with essentialist biolog-ical theories of sex differences (Feingold, 1992), but it is alsopredicted by differential social and learning experiences affordedto boys and girls as argued in sociocultural theories of gender. Thecombined effect of small mean differences and greater male vari-ability is then reflected in the sex ratios of students attaining thehigh proficiency standard of the NAEP in math and science.Although there are no established guidelines as to how to interpretthe magnitude of sex ratios, we would suggest that a sex ratio ofover 2:1 (i.e., over twice as many males as females reaching thesestandards) should be considered meaningful and nontrivial. Find-ing a large sex difference in high achievers for mathematics andscience may not be in keeping with a strict interpretation of Hyde’s(2005) gender similarities hypothesis, but it should be noted thatthe hypothesis as it was originally articulated considered onlymean sex differences (Hyde, 2005) and did not speak to genderimbalances in high achievers. Additionally, there was no overalleffect of year of assessment on tail ratios, though there was a slighttendency for change in Grade 4 mathematics and Grade 8 science.It may be the case that changes predicted by sociocultural perspec-tives operate over a longer time frame or that greater male vari-ability remains unchanged, as might be predicted by psychobio-logical theories.

Implications

Although mean sex differences in mathematics and science weresmall in magnitude, even small differences in ability level may beconsequential if experienced over time (Eagly, Wood, & Diekman,2000; Prentice & Miller, 1992; Rosenthal, 1986). In particular,they may serve to undermine self-efficacy and interest in tradi-tionally sex-typed subjects such as mathematics and science(Eccles, 2013; Else-Quest, Mineo, & Higgins, 2013). However,this is of less concern than the combined effect of small mean

differences and greater male variability, which leads to largegender gaps in high achievers for mathematics and science.

Further efforts may be warranted to encourage and cultivategirls’ interest and aptitude in these subject areas—particularly withstudents who have yet to realize their full potential. Many studentshave a stereotypically masculine image of mathematics and sci-ence (Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Smeding, 2012), andcountering deeply ingrained sex stereotypes is not easily achieved(Shapiro & Williams, 2012). Although all students receive instruc-tion in these areas through the school curriculum, parents canfacilitate development of mathematics and science interest andaptitude by providing early enrichment activities and sciencelearning experiences equally for daughters and sons (Newcombe &Frick, 2010). Boys report having more extracurricular experienceswith toys and games that promote science learning (Jones et al.,2000), and examination of parent–child interactions shows thatparents explain scientific concepts to boys more frequently than togirls (Crowley, Callanan, Tenenbaum, & Allen, 2001; Diamond,1994; Tenenbaum & Leaper, 2003). Parents also estimate theintelligence of sons as being higher than that of daughters, includ-ing their mathematics intelligence (Furnham, Reeves, & Budhani,2002), and parental expectations can profoundly impact the self-efficacy of children (Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990). Encourag-ing and supporting daughters who show interest or aptitude inscience to develop their potential may be critical for addressinggender gaps in high achievers.

The educational environment in which mathematics and scienceare taught at school can also have a profound impact on studentlearning outcomes (Gunderson, Ramirez, Levine, & Beilock,2012). Teachers have different beliefs about male and femalestudents in mathematics, have more frequent interactions withmale than with female students, and have higher expectations inthis field for boys (Li, 1999). Similar findings have been reportedfor science education, such as calling more frequently on malestudents to answer questions or provide a demonstration (Jones &Wheatley, 1990). Differential learning experiences for boys andgirls in the classroom are often subtle (Beaman, Wheldall, &Kemp, 2006) but may be contributing to the development of lowerself-efficacy and less interest in STEM for girls (for a review, seeGunderson et al., 2012). Individual differences in endorsement ofsex stereotypes about STEM can seriously undermine girls’achievement in these fields later in life (Schmader, Johns, &Barquissau, 2004), so it is important that educators send a positivemessage about the applicability of mathematics and science skillsto both genders.

A growing body of research also suggests that visuospatial skillsplay an important role in the development of quantitative reason-ing (Nuttall, Casey, & Pezaris, 2005) and that sex differences inspatial ability may be a mediator (Wai et al., 2009). However, evenbrief educational interventions can show marked improvements inthe development of spatial ability in both genders (Uttal et al.,2013), with evidence of transfer to other quantitative tasks. Manyresearchers have advocated for the inclusion of spatial learningwithin the school curriculum (Newcombe & Frick, 2010; Priess &Hyde, 2010), as this would provide benefits to all students and laydown a solid foundation for the later development of quantitativereasoning. Contrary to our hypothesis, mean sex differences andsex ratios of high achievers did not show a decline over the timeperiod analyzed. Despite societal changes in the roles of men and

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

10 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 205: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

women (Auster & Ohm, 2000), this has not translated into dimin-ishing sex differences over time as predicted by social and psy-chobiosocial perspectives. The present findings of stable sex dif-ferences give further weight to arguments that educationalinterventions are still required in the interest of gender equity.

Strengths and Limitations

The issue of sex differences in quantitative reasoning has beencontentious in recent decades, with some researchers arguing thatthere are considerable differences and others arguing that there arenone. By employing a large nationally representative sample suchas the NAEP, we can be more confident that the observed sexdifferences reflect the diversity of socioeconomic status and eth-nicity found in the United States, as well as the different educa-tional environments of each state. The statistical technique ofmeta-analysis makes it possible to aggregate findings from multi-ple waves of assessment, ensuring that the conclusion reached isnot idiosyncratic to a particular assessment year and student co-hort. As such it gives greater confidence in estimating the magni-tude of sex differences in mathematics and science in U.S. studentsunder the NAEP.

It has also offered the opportunity to test whether the magnitudeof said differences is declining and to establish that—at least forthe time period analyzed—these are stable across time. It alsodraws attention to the role that greater male variability can playand the critical importance of examining tail ratios of high-achieving students for a complete test of the gender similaritieshypothesis.

Although adding to the existing literature on sex differences,this study is not without limitations. First, it does not provide anyinformation on the causal factors that explain why sex differencesemerge. Although researchers have identified a number of biolog-ical, psychological, and social factors that contribute to sex differ-ences in quantitative reasoning (Halpern et al., 2007), many re-searchers agree that a variety of factors are ultimately responsibleand advocate a biopsychosocial model of sex differences (Halpern,2004; Halpern & Tan, 2001). Thus, the findings of a meta-analysiscan shed no light on why sex differences emerge and can onlydocument their existence.

Second, our study does not consider other factors, such associoeconomic background and ethnicity. There is some evidenceto show interactions between sex differences and ethnic back-grounds. For example, although sex differences are consistentlyfound for Caucasian and Hispanic students, some studies havefailed to find differences for African American samples (Fan et al.,1997; McGraw et al., 2006). Likewise, some studies have foundinteractions between socioeconomic status and sex differences inearly spatial development (Levine et al., 2005), which provides afoundation for quantitative reasoning. Teasing apart such theoret-ical contributions would be a useful addition to the literature.Finally, our analysis is limited by the test content being assessedby the NAEP. Previous studies (e.g., Hyde et al., 1990) have notedlarger sex differences are found in complex problem solving, butthe NAEP includes test items across a range of difficulty levels.International assessments of student ability, such as the Pro-gramme for International Student Assessment (PISA), includemore challenging test content and find somewhat larger sex dif-ferences in mathematics and science for U.S. students than found

under the NAEP (Guiso et al., 2008; Reilly, 2012). Although theseparallel lines of evidence provide a replication of sex differences,they do suggest that the NAEP may underestimate the true effectsize of such differences somewhat.

Summary

In the present study, we report a meta-analysis of sex differencesin mathematics and science achievement in the NAEP, a nationallyrepresentative sample of students drawn from public and privateinstitutions from across all states in the United States. Small meansex differences favoring males were observed in science andmathematics performance, making claims of their absence prema-ture. Further examination of male and female performance acrossthe three domains of science found that males and females wereequivalent in performance for life sciences but not for earth andphysical sciences. Contrary to our hypothesis, sex differences werenot moderated by the year in which students were tested, indicatingstability across time. Additionally we found that the performanceof males was more variable than that of females, which hasimplications for the proportion of males to females in the upper-right tail of the ability distribution. Greater male variability maycontribute to the disparity in educational outcomes in STEM-related fields, with males being overrepresented in attainment of anadvanced proficiency in mathematics and science by a ratio of over2:1. Further research into the psychological and social factorsunderpinning these gender gaps is required, as well as educationalinterventions and support services to help girls realize their fullpotential in mathematics and science achievement. Counteractingthe tendency for initially small sex differences in achievement tobe translated into larger sex differences in career choices is likelyto require concerted and sustained efforts at many levels.

References

Alberts, B. (2010, December 3). Policy-making needs science. Science,330, 1287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1200613

Alon, S., & Gelbgiser, D. (2011). The female advantage in college academicachievements and horizontal sex segregation. Social Science Research, 40,107–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.06.007

Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social roleand evolutionary explanations compatible? American Psychologist, 5,909–917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.909

Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in con-temporary American society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-RoleInventory. Sex Roles, 43, 499 –528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007119516728

Beaman, R., Wheldall, K., & Kemp, C. (2006). Differential teacher atten-tion to boys and girls in the classroom. Educational Review, 58, 339–366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00131910600748406

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection andvariability in studies of gender differences. Behavioral and Brain Sci-ences, 11, 183–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Bem, S. L. (1975). Sex role adaptability: One consequence of psycholog-ical androgyny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31,634–643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0077098

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sextyping. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354

Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability inintellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possible

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

11SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 206: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

causes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 169–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049244

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009).Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley.

Burkam, D. T., Lee, V. E., & Smerdon, B. A. (1997). Gender and sciencelearning early in high school: Subject matter and laboratory experiences.American Educational Research Journal, 34, 297–331. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00028312034002297

Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexualselection. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.3.164

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1994). Thinking critically about research onsex and gender. New York, NY: Harper Collins.

Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011). Understanding current causes ofwomen’s underrepresentation in science. Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences, USA, 108, 3157–3162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014871108.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Collaer, M. L., Reimers, S., & Manning, J. T. (2007). Visuospatial perfor-mance on an Internet line judgment task and potential hormonal mark-ers: Sex, sexual orientation, and 2D:4D. Archives of Sexual Behavior,36, 177–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9152-1

Collins, D. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). A large sex difference on a two-dimensional mental rotation task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 845–849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.111.4.845

Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001).Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientificthinking. Psychological Science, 12, 258 –261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347

Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12293-000

Diamond, J. (1994). Sex differences in science museums: A review.Curator: The Museum Journal, 37, 17–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2151-6952.1994.tb01003.x

Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-roleinterpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in humanbehavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psycholo-gist, 54, 408–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408

Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory ofsex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M.Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp.123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences inparticipation in physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.),Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evi-dence (pp. 199–210). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso-ciation.

Eccles, J. S. (2013). Gender and achievement choices. In E. T. Gershoff,R. S. Mistry, & D. Crosby (Eds.), Societal contexts of child develop-ment: Pathways of influence and implications for practice and policy(pp. 19–34). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes,expectancy effects, and parents’ socialization of gender differences.Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-nationalpatterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psycho-logical Bulletin, 136, 103–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018053

Else-Quest, N. M., Mineo, C. C., & Higgins, A. (2013). Math and scienceattitudes and achievement at the intersection of gender and ethnicity.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 293–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684313480694

Fan, X., Chen, M., & Matsumoto, A. R. (1997). Gender differences inmathematics achievement: Findings from the National Education Lon-gitudinal Study of 1988. Journal of Experimental Education, 65, 229–242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1997.9943456

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities:A new look at an old controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62,61–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543062001061

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in variability in intellectual abil-ities: A cross-cultural perspective. Sex Roles, 30, 81–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01420741

Feingold, A. (1995). The additive effects of differences in central tendencyand variability are important in comparisons between groups. AmericanPsychologist, 50, 5–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.1.5

Frieze, I. H. (2014). The importance of samples. Sex Roles, 70, 357–358.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0376-6

Furnham, A., Reeves, E., & Budhani, S. (2002). Parents think their sons arebrighter than their daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimationsand estimations of their children’s multiple intelligences. Journal ofGenetic Psychology, 163, 24 –39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221320209597966

Gallagher, A. M., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.). (2005). Gender differences inmathematics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Geary, D. C. (1996). Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematicalabilities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 229–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00042400

Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences(2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008, May 2008).Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320, 1164–1165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1154094

Gunderson, E., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. (2012). The roleof parents and teachers in the development of gender-related mathattitudes. Sex Roles, 66, 153–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2

Halari, R., Hines, M., Kumari, V., Mehrotra, R., Wheeler, M., Ng, V., &Sharma, T. (2005). Sex differences and individual differences in cogni-tive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormonesand gonadotropins. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 104–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.104

Halpern, D. F. (2004). A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex differences incognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13,135–139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00292.x

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., &Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in scienceand mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 1–51.

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011). Sex differencesin intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cam-bridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 253–272). New York, NY: Cam-bridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde,J., . . . Martin, C. L. (2011, September 23). The pseudoscience ofsingle-sex schooling. Science, 333, 1706 –1707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1205031

Halpern, D. F., & Lamay, M. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review ofsex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12,229–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009027516424

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using apsychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brainand Cognition, 45, 392–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1287

Handelsman, J., Cantor, N., Carnes, M., Denton, D., Fine, E., Grosz, B., . . .Sheridan, J. (2005, August 19). More women in science. Science, 309,1190–1191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1113252

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

12 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 207: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P.,Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) andchildren’s intelligence (IQ): In a UK-representative sample SES mod-erates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7(2),e30320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030320

Hanson, S. L., Schaub, M., & Baker, D. P. (1996). Gender stratificationin the science pipeline: A comparative analysis of seven countries.Gender & Society, 10, 271–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/089124396010003005

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009).Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitivesex differences—A psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinol-ogy, 34, 389–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.019

Hedges, L. V. (2008). What are effect sizes and why do we need them?Child Development Perspectives, 2, 167–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00060.x

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores,variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7604277

Hines, M. (1990). Gonadal hormones and human cognitive development.In J. Balthazar (Ed.), Hormones, brain and behaviors in vertebrates (pp.51–63). New York, NY: Karger.

Hines, M. (2006). Prenatal testosterone and gender-related behaviour.European Journal of Endocrinology, 155(Suppl. 1), S115–S121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1530/eje.1.02236

Humphreys, L. G. (1988). Sex differences in variability may be moreimportant than sex differences in means. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,11, 195–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049402

Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2000). Fixed effects vs. random effectsmeta-analysis models: Implications for cumulative research knowledge.International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 8, 275–292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-2389.00156

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychol-ogist, 60, 581–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review ofPsychology, 65, 373–398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences inmathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,107, 139–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139

Hyde, J. S., & Grabe, S. (2008). Meta-analysis in the psychology ofwomen. In F. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: Ahandbook of issues and theories (pp. 142–173). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about thenature of gender differences and the implications for gender equity. InS. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equity through edu-cation (2nd ed., pp. 19–32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C.(2008, July 25). Gender similarities characterize math performance.Science, 321, 494–495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1160364

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2006, October 27). Gender similarities inmathematics and science. Science, 314, 599–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1132154

Hyde, J. S., & Mertz, J. E. (2009). Gender, culture, and mathematicsperformance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,106, 8801–8807. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901265106

Jones, M. G., Howe, A., & Rua, M. J. (2000). Gender differences instudents’ experiences, interests, and attitudes toward science and scien-tists. Science Education, 84, 180 –192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(200003)84:2�180::AID-SCE3�3.0.CO;2-X

Jones, M. G., & Wheatley, J. (1990). Gender differences in teacher–studentinteractions in science classrooms. Journal of Research in ScienceTeaching, 27, 861–874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660270906

Kagan, J. (1964). The child’s sex role classification of school objects.Child Development, 35, 1051–1056.

Katzman, S., & Alliger, G. M. (1992). Averaging untransformed varianceratios can be misleading: A comment on Feingold. Review of Educa-tional Research, 62, 427– 428. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543062004427

Kelley, G. A., & Kelley, K. S. (2012). Statistical models for meta-analysis:A brief tutorial. World Journal of Methodology, 2(4), 27–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.5662/wjm.v2.i4.27

Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Kimura, D., & Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women

is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Current Directions inPsychological Science, 3, 57–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769964

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., &Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex differ-ence in spatial skill. Psychological Science, 16, 841–845. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01623.x

Li, Q. (1999). Teachers’ beliefs and gender differences in mathematics: Areview. Educational Research, 41, 63–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0013188990410106

Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Newtrends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psy-chological Bulletin, 136, 1123–1135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021276

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement.Trames, 13, 3–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.3176/tr.2009.1.01

Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008, November 28). Global sex differencesin test score variability. Science, 322, 1331–1332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1162573

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues:Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions inPsychological Science, 13, 67–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x

McGraw, R., Lubienski, S. T., & Strutchens, M. E. (2006). A closer lookat gender in NAEP mathematics achievement and affect data: Intersec-tions with achievement, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Jour-nal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37, 129–150.

Miller, P. H., Blessing, J. S., & Schwartz, S. (2006). Gender differences inhigh school students’ views about science. International Journal ofScience Education, 28, 363–381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500690500277664

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. InM. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cogni-tive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263–302). New York, NY:Academic Press.

National Science Foundation. (2011). Women, minorities, and persons withdisabilities in science and engineering: 2011. Arlington, VA: NationalScience Foundation.

Neisser, U. C., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N.,Ceci, S. J., . . . Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns.American Psychologist, 51, 77–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77

Neuschmidt, O., Barth, J., & Hastedt, D. (2008). Trends in gender differ-ences in mathematics and science (TIMSS 1995–2003). Studies in Ed-ucational Evaluation, 34, 56–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2008.04.002

Newcombe, N. S., Ambady, N., Eccles, J., Gomez, L., Klahr, D., Linn, M.,. . . Mix, K. (2009). Psychology’s role in mathematics and scienceeducation. American Psychologist, 64, 538–550. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014813

Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatial intelli-gence: Why, what, and how. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4, 102–111.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2010.01089.x

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

13SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 208: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math � male,me � female, therefore math not � me. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 83, 44–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.44

Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala,A., . . . Greenwald, A. G. (2009). National differences in gender-sciencestereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achieve-ment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106,10593–10597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809921106

Nowell, A., & Hedges, L. V. (1998). Trends in gender differences inacademic achievement from 1960 to 1994: An analysis of differences inmean, variance, and extreme scores. Sex Roles, 39, 21–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018873615316

Nuttall, R. L., Casey, M. B., & Pezaris, E. (2005). Spatial ability as amediator of gender differences on mathematics tests: A biological-environmental framework. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.),Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological ap-proach (pp. 121–142). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge Uni-versity Press.

Osborne, J., Simon, S., & Collins, S. (2003). Attitudes towards science: Areview of the literature and its implications. International Journal ofScience Education, 25, 1049 –1079. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0950069032000032199

Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive.Psychological Bulletin, 112, 160–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.160

President’s Council of Advisors on Science and Technology. (2010).Prepare and inspire: K-12 science, technology, engineering, and math(STEM). Washington, DC: Education for America’s Future.

Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender and academic abilities andpreferences. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook ofgender research in psychology (pp. 297–316). New York, NY: Springer.

Puts, D. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Burriss, R. P., Jordan, C. L.,& Breedlove, S. M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mentalrotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58,282–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005

Rampey, B. D., Dion, G. S., & Donahue, P. L. (2009). NAEP 2008 trendsin academic progress (NCES 2009–479). Washington, DC: NationalCenter for Education Statistics.

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoSONE, 7(7), e39904. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatialability: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 68, 521–535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Robinson, J. P., & Lubienski, S. T. (2011). The development of genderachievement gaps in mathematics and reading during elementary andmiddle school examining direct cognitive assessments and teacher rat-ings. American Educational Research Journal, 48, 268–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0002831210372249

Rosenthal, R. (1986). Media violence, antisocial behavior, and the socialconsequences of small effects. Journal of Social Issues, 42, 141–154.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1986.tb00247.x

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender devel-opment. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbookof child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-ment (6th ed., pp. 858–932). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of acceptinggender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’sexperience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50, 835–850. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:SERS.0000029101.74557.a0

Serbin, L. A., Zelkowitz, P., Doyle, A. B., Gold, D., & Wheaton, B. (1990).The socialization of sex-differentiated skills and academic performance:A mediational model. Sex Roles, 23, 613–628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00289251

Shapiro, J., & Williams, A. M. (2012). The role of stereotype threats inundermining girls’ and women’s performance and interest in STEMfields. Sex Roles, 66, 175–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0051-0

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androg-yny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulle-tin, 100, 207–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207

Smeding, A. (2012). Women in science, technology, engineering, andmathematics (STEM): An investigation of their implicit gender stereo-types and stereotypes’ connectedness to math performance. Sex Roles,67, 617–629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0209-4

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematicsand science? A critical review. American Psychologist, 60, 950–958.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950

Spence, J. T. (1984). Gender identity and its implications for masculinityand femininity. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium onMotivation: Psychology and gender (pp. 59–95). Lincoln: University ofNebraska Press.

Steffens, M., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereotypesregarding math and language: Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, but not for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64,324–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x

Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2013). Sex differences in mathematics andreading achievement are inversely related: Within- and across-nationassessment of 10 years of PISA data. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e57988. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057988

Stumpf, H. (1995). Gender differences in performance on tests of cognitiveabilities: Experimental design issues and empirical results. Learning andIndividual Differences, 7, 275–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(95)90002-0

Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women andpeople: A meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. PsychologicalBulletin, 135, 859–884. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017364

Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent–child conversations aboutscience: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Psychol-ogy, 39, 34–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34

Thompson, S. G., & Higgins, J. P. (2002). How should meta-regressionanalyses be undertaken and interpreted? Statistics in Medicine, 21,1559–1573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.1187

Turkheimer, E., & Halpern, D. F. (2009). Sex differences in variability forcognitive measures. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 612–614.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01169.x

Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren,C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: Ameta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352–402.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028446

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differ-ences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of criticalvariables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250

Wai, J., Cacchio, M., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Sex differencesin the right tail of cognitive abilities: A 30-year examination. Intelli-gence, 38, 412–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.006

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEMdomains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowl-edge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101,817–835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016127

Wai, J., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M. C. (2012). Studying intellectual outliers:Are there sex differences, and are the smart getting smarter? CurrentDirections in Psychological Science, 21, 382–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721412455052

Wang, M.-T., Eccles, J. S., & Kenny, S. (2013). Not lack of ability butmore choice: Individual and gender differences in choice of careers in

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

14 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 209: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Psychological Sci-ence, 24, 770–775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612458937

Weinburgh, M. (1995). Gender differences in student attitudes towardscience: A meta-analysis of the literature from 1970 to 1991. Journal ofResearch in Science Teaching, 32, 387–398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660320407

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., & Davis-Kean, P.(2006). Development of achievement motivation. In N. Eisenberg, W.Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social,

emotional, and personality development (pp. 933–1002). Hoboken, NJ:Wiley.

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differ-ences and similarities in behavior. Advances in Experimental SocialPsychology, 46, 55–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394281-4.00002-7

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2013). Biology or culture alone cannot accountfor human sex differences and similarities. Psychological Inquiry, 24,241–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.815034

Appendix

Archive of National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) Data Used for Analysis

Table A1Descriptive Statistics, Effect Sizes, and Variance Ratios for NAEP Mathematics

Year Grade

Male Female

Sample sizeVariance

ratio Cohen’s dM SD M SD

2011 4 241.41825 29.76624 239.92438 28.11948 209,000 1.12 0.052009 4 240.61765 29.50272 238.69442 27.86402 168,800 1.12 0.072007 4 240.79044 29.43191 238.62343 27.74161 197,700 1.13 0.082005 4 239.11030 28.92446 236.59788 27.81468 172,000 1.08 0.092003 4 236.37463 29.06977 233.41351 27.58066 190,000 1.11 0.102000 4 226.82131 32.34153 224.30827 30.05055 13,800 1.16 0.081996 4 223.73966 31.70661 223.27141 29.95813 6,600 1.12 0.021992 4 220.89259 32.52064 218.52010 30.80918 8,700 1.11 0.071990 4 213.54463 32.73525 212.54085 30.70411 8,900 1.14 0.032011 8 284.45084 37.21046 283.23397 35.12125 175,200 1.12 0.032009 8 283.94915 37.22430 281.85728 35.48711 161,700 1.10 0.062007 8 282.40116 37.40132 280.27550 34.62987 153,000 1.17 0.062005 8 279.61146 37.14541 278.01277 35.43316 162,000 1.10 0.042003 8 278.48139 37.18516 276.63517 35.21715 153,000 1.11 0.052000 8 273.91265 39.25296 272.27437 36.79607 15,800 1.14 0.041996 8 271.43222 38.25208 269.44691 36.62322 7,100 1.09 0.051992 8 268.09776 36.78734 268.70292 35.68133 9,400 1.06 �0.021990 8 263.20994 37.23174 261.87034 34.70190 8,900 1.15 0.042009 12 154.94494 34.89788 151.66908 32.47539 51,700 1.15 0.102005 12 151.31353 35.54736 148.78616 32.35334 15,100 1.21 0.072000 12 301.90598 37.44853 298.52331 33.72126 13,800 1.23 0.091996 12 302.94416 34.98625 300.34237 32.67684 6,900 1.15 0.081992 12 301.33159 34.71171 297.75355 33.04985 8,500 1.10 0.111990 12 297.08056 36.39719 291.48571 34.89335 8,900 1.09 0.16

Note. Effect sizes that are statistically significant at p � .05 are highlighted in bold. Variance ratios (VRs) above 1.00 indicate greater male variability;VRs below 1.00 reflect greater female variability. NAEP � National Assessment of Educational Progress.

(Appendix continues)

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

15SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 210: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table A2Percentage of Male and Female Students Attaining the Advanced Proficiency Level for Mathematics

Grade YearMale at advanced

or higherFemale at advanced

or higher Risk ratio

4 2011 7.576799 5.717962 1.332009 6.914833 4.945088 1.402007 6.625340 4.485612 1.482005 5.831723 4.180971 1.392003 4.891417 2.916470 1.682000 3.436696 1.760290 1.951996 3.054985 1.426135 2.141992 2.111726 1.334901 1.581990 1.685714 0.625990 2.69

Grade 4 ratio 1.508 2011 9.216519 7.266763 1.27

2009 8.801046 7.020203 1.252007 8.103602 5.876708 1.382005 6.731102 5.331733 1.262003 6.118808 4.649840 1.322000 5.917239 4.102844 1.441996 4.296899 3.341992 1.291992 3.172883 2.990450 1.061990 2.366303 1.571801 1.51

Grade 8 ratio 1.3012 2009 3.520143 1.808225 1.95

2005 3.062339 1.372165 2.232000 3.233790 1.363867 2.371996 2.538683 1.378109 1.841992 2.082941 1.065519 1.951990 2.287939 0.685097 3.34

Grade 12 ratio 2.13

Table A3Descriptive Statistics, Effect Sizes, and Variance Ratios for NAEP Science

Year Grade

Male Female

Sample sizeVariance

ratio Cohen’s dM SD M SD

2009 4 150.57607 35.71345 149.40869 34.21378 156,500 1.09 0.032005 4 152.52981 31.78684 148.65937 30.40344 172,500 1.09 0.122000 4 152.54469 35.28189 147.13105 33.46140 15,800 1.11 0.101996 4 150.85221 33.53692 149.13861 32.39515 7,300 1.07 0.042011 8 154.16130 35.05894 149.21276 33.25938 122,000 1.11 0.142009 8 151.98475 36.14538 147.99001 33.65030 151,100 1.15 0.112005 8 150.48951 36.21289 146.58535 34.31355 173,700 1.11 0.112000 8 154.35722 36.53652 147.34892 35.03792 15,800 1.09 0.171996 8 150.84548 34.83000 149.12814 32.90619 7,800 1.12 0.062009 12 152.87615 35.79328 147.15407 33.95205 11,100 1.11 0.162005 12 149.01019 34.95658 145.14886 32.58746 22,000 1.15 0.112000 12 147.66712 35.31678 145.17791 32.66081 15,800 1.17 0.071996 12 153.73568 34.34129 147.22612 32.31225 7,500 1.13 0.20

Note. Effect sizes that are statistically significant at p � .05 are highlighted in bold. Variance ratios (VRs) above 1.00 indicate greater male variability;VRs below 1.00 reflect greater female variability. NAEP � National Assessment of Educational Progress.

(Appendix continues)

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

16 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 211: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table A4Descriptive Statistics and Effect Sizes for Males and Females Across the Field of Science

Grade Year Subdomain

Male FemaleVariance

ratioEffect sizeCohen’s dM SD M SD

4 2009 Earth 151.98697 35.48140 147.94554 34.36776 1.07 0.12Physical 150.68020 35.76229 149.30413 34.14173 1.10 0.04Life 149.02011 35.45167 151.01676 34.48297 1.06 �0.06

2005 Earth 154.62696 34.23227 147.90774 32.83755 1.09 0.20Physical 152.88549 33.46338 150.37863 31.59997 1.12 0.08Life 150.07751 32.25717 147.69223 31.53551 1.05 0.07

2000 Earth 155.29923 37.11476 146.93308 35.06581 1.12 0.23Physical 151.73910 36.73263 147.29376 35.00875 1.10 0.12Life 150.59632 36.19456 147.16675 34.57285 1.10 0.10

1996 Earth 152.68547 35.27448 147.28560 34.50327 1.05 0.15Physical 150.40359 35.89103 149.59256 34.06300 1.11 0.02Life 149.46810 35.33133 150.53822 34.64733 1.04 �0.03

8 2011 Earth 153.92140 34.84762 147.80144 33.75392 1.07 0.18Physical 155.57074 35.11631 147.88813 32.92987 1.14 0.23Life 153.30020 35.25709 151.58303 33.69989 1.09 0.05

2009 Earth 152.87875 35.59819 147.08004 34.11659 1.09 0.17Physical 152.65646 36.30124 147.30709 33.38742 1.18 0.15Life 150.69328 35.95506 149.30199 33.95972 1.12 0.04

2005 Earth 152.17593 36.88717 147.55658 34.80591 1.12 0.13Physical 149.21860 39.26031 142.21619 37.12914 1.12 0.18Life 150.17797 36.55163 149.13386 35.07240 1.09 0.03

2000 Earth 155.05456 37.39703 148.12953 35.43240 1.11 0.19Physical 155.15257 38.90284 144.44899 37.61012 1.07 0.28Life 153.23782 36.96043 148.93848 35.64924 1.07 0.12

1996 Earth 151.69663 35.79566 148.25032 34.06708 1.10 0.10Physical 151.98523 35.94419 147.95250 33.87181 1.13 0.12Life 149.35236 35.93675 150.66815 33.98972 1.12 �0.04

12 2009 Earth 155.03106 35.30019 145.02064 33.96834 1.08 0.29Physical 153.45769 36.05441 146.57822 33.56864 1.15 0.20Life 150.91495 35.23967 149.09501 34.73195 1.03 0.05

2005 Earth 147.90490 35.52127 142.57167 33.07296 1.15 0.16Physical 151.21768 37.09497 144.72360 34.38433 1.16 0.18Life 147.90856 35.57997 148.15184 33.75475 1.11 �0.01

2000 Earth 146.87288 36.40061 142.34830 33.23612 1.20 0.13Physical 149.10605 37.42376 144.84199 35.29647 1.12 0.12Life 147.02286 35.56595 148.34385 32.84964 1.17 �0.04

1996 Earth 155.85416 35.50721 146.15392 33.06037 1.15 0.28Physical 154.14569 36.15524 146.04818 34.04841 1.13 0.23Life 151.20767 34.76459 149.47683 33.20536 1.10 0.05

Note. Effect sizes that are statistically significant at p � .05 are highlighted in bold.

(Appendix continues)

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

17SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE

Page 212: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Received February 5, 2014Revision received August 22, 2014

Accepted August 22, 2014 �

Table A5Percentage of Male and Female Students Attaining the Advanced Proficiency Level for Science

Grade YearMale at advanced

or higherFemale at advanced

or higherGenderratio

4 2009 0.682025 0.512587 1.332005 3.202503 1.853222 1.732000 4.792086 2.544751 1.881996 3.418698 2.695513 1.27

Grade 4 ratio 1.568 2011 2.203641 0.989545 2.23

2009 2.039891 0.967621 2.112005 4.038574 2.349279 1.722000 5.152212 2.799564 1.841996 3.553214 2.530816 1.40

Grade 8 ratio 1.8812 2009 1.998120 0.827877 2.41

2005 2.651007 1.266683 2.092000 2.760126 1.390137 1.991996 3.993961 1.346876 2.97

Grade 12 ratio 2.28

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

18 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 213: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 149

visual-spatial reasoning, and whether sex-role differences would still be present.

Thirdly, we employed a verbal fluency language task to examine whether there might an

increase in performance (stereotype-lift) in the stereotype priming condition.

The present study was conducted in two phases. In the first, women were

assigned to either a masculine or feminine labelling condition to complete the GEFT. It

was hypothesized that women in the masculine labelling condition would score lower

on the GEFT than those in the feminine labelling condition (Brosnan, 1998). Consistent

with the sex-role mediation hypothesis (Nash, 1979; Reilly & Neumann, 2013), it was

predicted that women high in masculine sex-role identification (masculine and

androgynous groups) would perform better than those low in masculine identification

(feminine and undifferentiated). A further question related to the potential interaction of

labelling and sex-role identification. It is plausible that the effect of labelling on GEFT

performance might be larger for women who identify as highly masculine than for

women who identify as less masculine.

In the second phase, women were assigned to either a stereotype-threat inducing

condition or a neutral control condition before completing a mental rotation task and a

test of verbal fluency. It was hypothesized that women in the stereotype-threat condition

would score lower on mental rotation performance than those in the control condition.

Consistent with past research (Reilly & Neumann, 2013), we also hypothesized that

masculine and androgynous women would score higher on the mental rotation task than

feminine and undifferentiated women. Although no a priori hypotheses were made, we

also sought to determine any interaction (if any) between sex-role category,

experimental condition and cognitive ability type. For the verbal fluency language task,

it was hypothesized that there would be a stereotype-lift effect, with more words

generated in the stereotype-priming condition than in the control condition. Consistent

Page 214: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender Differences in Reading and Writing Achievement: Evidence Fromthe National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP)

David ReillyGriffith University, Queensland, Australia

David L. Neumann and Glenda AndrewsGriffith University, Queensland, Australia, and Menzies Health

Institute Queensland, Australia

A frequently observed research finding is that females outperform males on tasks of verbaland language abilities, but there is considerable variability in effect sizes from sample tosample. The gold standard for evaluating gender differences in cognitive ability is to recruita large, demographically representative sample. We examined 3 decades of U.S. studentachievement in reading and writing from the National Assessment of Educational Progress todetermine the magnitude of gender differences (N � 3.9 million), and whether these weredeclining over time as claimed by Feingold (1988). Examination of effect sizes found adevelopmental progression from initially small gender differences in Grade 4 toward largereffects as students progress through schooling. Differences for reading were small-to-medium(d � �.32 by Grade 12), and medium-sized for writing (d � �.55 by Grade 12) and werestable over the historical time. Additionally, there were pronounced imbalances in genderratios at the lower left and upper right tails of the ability spectrum. These results areinterpreted in the context of Hyde’s (2005) gender similarities hypothesis, which holds thatmost psychological gender differences are only small or trivial in size. Language and verbalabilities represent one exception to the general rule of gender similarities, and we discuss theeducational implications of these findings.

Keywords: gender differences, reading, writing, literacy, sex differences

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000356.supp

The question of whether males and females differ incognitive abilities has been the focus of considerableresearch in recent decades. While there is a generalconsensus that males and females do not differ in generalintelligence (Halpern, 2000), gender differences are com-monly observed for more specific cognitive abilities suchas visual–spatial ability (Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995)and language (Miller & Halpern, 2014). However, Hyde(2005) had proposed the gender similarities hypothesis(GSH), which claimed that males and females “are sim-ilar on most, but not all, psychological variables. That is,men and women, as well as boys and girls, are more alikethan they are different” (p. 581). It holds that most genderdifferences are small or trivial (close to zero) in magni-

tude. One exception to this hypothesis may be the gendergap in reading achievement, which is found cross-culturally (Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Reilly, 2012) and ex-ceeds the threshold proposed by Hyde and Grabe (2008,p. 170) for nontrivial gender difference effect sizes (d �.10). In a recent review, Hyde (2014) remarked that it is“difficult to reconcile” (p. 382) the magnitude of thegender gap observed in reading with other domains ofverbal ability (e.g., vocabulary, anagrams), which Hydeand Linn (1988) claimed are typically much smaller.

While the issue of reading is received greater attention, thereis a growing body of evidence that males and females alsodiffer in writing ability (Camarata & Woodcock, 2006; Reyn-olds, Scheiber, Hajovsky, Schwartz, & Kaufman, 2015;Scheiber, Reynolds, Hajovsky, & Kaufman, 2015). Reynoldset al. (2015) noted that the issue of gender differences inwriting skills has been overlooked because it is less frequentlymeasured in educational assessments. In cases where writingability is assessed, researchers should examine gender differ-ences to determine if any meaningful differences occur. More-over, researchers should compare the size of any differences tothose observed with reading assessments when both domainsare examined in the same sample.

David Reilly, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University,Queensland, Australia; David L. Neumann and Glenda Andrews, School ofApplied Psychology, Griffith University, Queensland, Australia, and Men-zies Health Institute Queensland, Australia.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to DavidReilly, School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Southport,Queensland 4222, Australia. E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

American Psychologist© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 1, No. 999, 0000003-066X/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000356

1

Page 215: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Some researchers (e.g., Feingold, 1988) have claimed thatas a response to societal changes in the status and roles ofwomen, gender differences are declining (see the onlinesupplemental materials for a more detailed discussion ofthese issues). Gender role attitudes in the United States havechanged over time, giving boys the freedom to pursuelanguage-arts fields just as an increasing number of girlsnow pursue science, technology, engineering, and mathe-matics fields. Feingold analyzed educational data from 1947to 1980, showing a decline over time. More recently, Caplanand Caplan (1997, 2016) have questioned whether genderdifferences in verbal and language abilities even existed atall and were the product of selection bias in samples, whileHyde (2005) has claimed that most gender differences areeither small or trivial in size. The current study examineswhether historical patterns of gender differences in readingand writing are still present in modern samples and, if so, todetermine their magnitude. It presents a meta-analysis ofstudent reading and writing achievement drawn from theNational Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), alarge nationally representative sample of students from theUnited States conducted by the National Center for Educa-tional Statistics (NCES). Before turning our attention to thisdataset, we first present an overview of theoretical perspec-tives on gender differences in language ability.

Theoretical Perspectives on Gender Differences inLanguage Ability

In their pioneering text The Psychology of Sex Differ-ences, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) presented the first sys-tematic review of the psychological literature on gender

differences, arguing that gender differences in verbal abilityand language were “well established” (p. 351) and showeda developmental progression toward larger gaps with in-creasing age. Much of the literature they reviewed focusedexclusively on reading ability, rather than considering lan-guage proficiency more broadly with higher level tasks suchas writing, spelling, and grammar usage. But a number ofsubsequent studies have also reported gender differenceswith the largest being spelling and use of grammar (Reilly,Neumann, & Andrews, 2016; Stanley, Benbow, Brody,Dauber, & Lupkowski, 1992).

Theoretical explanations for the emergence of genderdifferences in reading and language proficiency have beenoffered. These center around biologically based or socio-cultural explanations for gender differences, or combina-tions of both (Eagly & Wood, 2013; Halpern & Tan, 2001):(a) differential rates of maturation, (b) gender differences inlateralization of brain function, (c) gender differences invariability, (d) gender differences in externalizing behaviorand language competence, and (e) gender-stereotyping ofreading and language as feminine traits. Each will be dis-cussed in detail next.

Differential Rates of Maturation

Girls have a faster rate of maturation and may thereforebe attaining greater proficiency than similarly aged boys(Dwyer, 1973), making reading easier and more enjoyable.Such an explanation holds that boys are merely delayed(developmental lag) and boys would attain an equivalentlanguage proficiency given sufficient time. However, thisclaim is inconsistent with studies showing gender differ-ences in reading that persist into adulthood (Kutner et al.,2007).

Gender Differences in Lateralization ofBrain Function

Some researchers have claimed that lateralization of brainfunction for language may differ between males and fe-males (Levy, 1969). It has been claimed that the regionsresponsible for language tasks are strongly lateralized to theleft cerebral hemisphere in right-handed males, but thatlanguage regions in females are more likely to be distributedacross both the left and right hemisphere (B. A. Shaywitz etal., 1995). Bilateral language function presumably affordssome benefits, which could explain the female advantageobserved on such tasks. However, empirical support for theLevy hypothesis is mixed (Kaiser, Haller, Schmitz, &Nitsch, 2009), with some neuroimaging studies showinggender differences in lateralization for language tasks (Bur-man, Bitan, & Booth, 2008; Clements et al., 2006), whileothers do not (Wallentin, 2009).

David Reilly

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

2 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 216: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender Differences in Variability

One explanation for lower reading and language profi-ciency in males is the greater male variability effect, whichstates that males show greater variability in cognitive per-formance across all cultures (Feingold, 1992; Machin &Pekkarinen, 2008). Even if there were no gender differencesin group means, the consequence of greater male variabilityis that males will be overrepresented at the extreme left tailof the ability distribution, which Hawke, Olson, Willcut,Wadsworth, and DeFries (2009) argued explains why gen-der ratios of poor readers favors females. Boys are alsooverrepresented in populations with reading impairment,dyslexia, attention disorders, and mental retardation sug-gesting that there may be a gender-linked neurologicalcontribution (Halpern, Beninger, & Straight, 2011). Whileexplanations for the greater male variability hypothesis inintelligence have been made by evolutionary psychologists(Geary, 2010), few specific evolutionary theories have beenproposed for verbal and language abilities (Geary, Win-egard, & Winegard, 2014), perhaps because these are morerecent in an evolutionary sense.

Gender Differences in Externalizing Behaviorand Language Competence

Other researchers have argued that gender differences inexternalizing behavior may also partly explain a greaterfemale language competence (Limbrick, Wheldall, & Mad-elaine, 2011). Clinicians identify more boys than girls withexternalizing behavior and attention disorders (McGee,Prior, Williams, Smart, & Sanson, 2002) which have both

been associated with reading and language impairment. Forexample, in a longitudinal sample from the United States,Rabiner and Coie (2000) reported that attention-impairmentand externalizing behavior measured in kindergarten pre-dicted later reading impairment in fifth grade. Other studieshave followed children over longer time frames. In a lon-gitudinal study of child development in Australia, Smart,Prior, Sanson, and Oberklaid (2001) found that externaliz-ing behavior problems at age 7 predicted the severity oflater reading and spelling difficulties at ages 13–14, evenafter controlling for intelligence and socioeconomic status.Although also present in girls, Smart et al. found that theassociation between externalizing behavior and reading im-pairment was significantly stronger in boys. Such an asso-ciation is not necessarily causal, and may well be reciprocalin nature. Within the context of the educational environ-ment, inattention and behavior problems may result in ad-ditional educational setbacks, as such problems can inter-fere with learning as well as lower academic motivation andrapport between teacher and student. But it is equally plau-sible that these conditions are related to a common neuro-biological factor (Berninger, Nielsen, Abbott, Wijsman, &Raskind, 2008).

Gender-Stereotyping of Reading and Language asFeminine Traits

Kagan (1964) first observed that children readily classifysocial behaviors and even intellectual tasks as either mas-culine or feminine in nature, based on shared cultural beliefsabout gender roles. Reading and language are generallyregarded as feminine in nature (Plante, de la Sablonnière,Aronson, & Théorêt, 2013), and gender stereotypes aboutlanguage are held by both males and females (Halpern,Straight, & Stephenson, 2011). The process by which achild acquires stereotypically masculine and feminine per-sonality traits is termed sex-typing (Bem, 1981). Highlysex-typed individuals are motivated to keep their behaviorand self-concept consistent with traditional gender norms(Martin & Ruble, 2010; Nash, 1979). The rigidity of sex-roles may translate into decreased reading interest and mo-tivation for some boys if there is a perceived incompatibilitybetween reading and masculine norms. Reading motivationis proposed as playing a strong role in later reading achieve-ment, with boys reporting lower reading motivation andinterest (Marinak & Gambrell, 2010; Mucherah & Yoder,2008). Lowered reading motivation is reflected in theamount of leisure time spent on reading (Moffitt & Wart-ella, 1991), leading to differential levels of practice betweenboys and girls. Girls in elementary school also report morepositive competence beliefs than boys for reading and lan-guage tasks (Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990).

David L.Neumann

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

3GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 217: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Large-Scale Assessments of Reading andWriting Achievement

One of the difficulties in evaluating research in the fieldof gender differences in cognitive ability comes from theuse of sampling methods, and the potential for selectionbias. It is not normally feasible to sample every male andfemale in a given population. Researchers thus often take asample group of participants and then use statistics andprobability to draw an inference about the underlying pop-ulation. Hedges and Nowell (1995) note that this approachcan be problematic for two reasons. First, as noted earlier,the greater male variability effect results in a greater numberof male high and low achievers at the top and bottom of theability distribution, respectively (Hawke et al., 2009;Machin & Pekkarinen, 2008). Greater variability may pres-ent a distorted picture of the underlying population which ismagnified in highly selected samples (Becker & Hedges,1988). Second, demographic factors such as socioeconomicstatus, ethnicity, and rural versus urban residence cangreatly influence cognitive ability (Fernald, Marchman, &Weisleder, 2013; Hanscombe et al., 2012), which may fur-ther limit the generalizability of a convenience sample.

For this reason, the gold standard for research is to recruita large sample that is representative of the population underinvestigation (Hedges & Nowell, 1995), in terms of gender,ethnicity, socioeconomic status, geographical region, and soforth. This approach increases confidence in the validity ofany conclusions made about specific groups, such as malesand females. Another reason why selection bias may beproblematic in the context of gender differences in readingand writing is that when investigating specific subgroups

(such as students that have been identified as poor readers),it is difficult to determine the underlying prevalence ofmales and females due to the issue of a gendered referralbias. Shaywitz, Shaywitz, Fletcher, and Escobar (1990)noted that more boys than girls are identified as poor readersby educational institutions, but when epidemiological stud-ies investigate reading impairment in the community girlsand boys approach an equal representation (Hawke et al.,2009; Jiménez et al., 2011). The implication here is that theprevalence of reading impairment in girls may simply justbe underreported, and that there may be a referral bias forboys. In order to test such a claim, a study would need torecruit a large, nationally representative sample and admin-ister a standardized reading assessment.

One such source is NAEP, which is conducted by NCES,part of the U.S. Department of Education. It has the addedadvantage that new waves of assessment have been con-ducted over several decades without major changes to thereading and writing frameworks so that temporal trends canbe investigated. Before turning our attention to this analysis,we first review previous studies that have recruited nation-ally representative samples of males and females to inves-tigate gender differences in reading and writing.

Gender Differences in Reading

Hedges and Nowell (1995) reported the largest study ofgender differences in achievement scores ever conducted,across a wide range of content areas using nationally rep-resentative samples from the United States. These includedstudent assessments of reading proficiency conducted byNAEP reported from 1971 to 1992. They found that girlsshowed significantly higher scores for tests of reading ineach year of assessment, with effect sizes ranging fromd � �.18 to �.30. Furthermore, they found that the per-formance of boys was more variable than that of girls withan average variance ratio (VR) of 1.12. This variabilityresulted in an overrepresentation of boys as poor readers.The researchers also examined data from a number of otherdata sets that recruited nationally representative stratifiedsamples. Across these other data sets, Hedges and Nowellfound similarly sized gender differences and greater malevariance. They also reported that the ratio of boys to girls inthe bottom 10% of reading comprehension (i.e., poor read-ers) ranged from 1.07 to 1.75, which paralleled that found inthe NAEP data. Thus, there were both mean gender differ-ences in reading ability and an overrepresentation of boyswho are poor readers.

While pioneering at the time it was published, a seriouslimitation of Hedges and Nowell’s (1995) analysis was thatthey only examined NAEP data from students near theend-point of their education, aged 17, and did not investi-gate whether gender differences were still present inyounger students. Developmental differences are an impor-

Glenda Andrews

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

4 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 218: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

tant consideration, as gender differences in literacy mightemerge at earlier ages. As they also reported substantialvariability across waves of assessment, the technique ofmeta-analysis ought to have been employed to aggregatefindings across waves to determine the overall trend. Addi-tionally, there is concern that with the passage of time suchresults may quickly become dated. A number of researcherssuch as Feingold (1988), or Caplan and Caplan (1997, 2016)have claimed that gender differences are disappearing (for afull overview see the online supplemental materials). NAEPcollects assessments of reading periodically, and there arenow numerous waves of data are unexamined. If genderdifferences in cognitive ability are disappearing, then suchan effect should be observable in U.S. children and adoles-cents over a sufficiently large timeframe.

Evidence for gender differences in reading proficiencymay also exist cross-culturally in large multinational assess-ments of student achievement (Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Reilly,2012). One such source is the Programme for InternationalStudent Assessment (PISA) conducted by the Organisationfor Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD)across member and partner nations. It seeks to assess stu-dent achievement in reading, mathematics, and science atage 15 (which is typically toward the end of compulsoryschooling in most countries). Lynn and Mikk (2009) foundappreciably sized gender differences across all nations inthe 2000, 2003, and 2006 waves of PISA assessment, whileReilly (2012) reached a similar conclusion with the PISA2009 dataset. There was also substantial variability acrossnations which researchers attribute to cultural factors suchas national levels of gender equality (Guiso, Monte, Sapi-enza, & Zingales, 2008; Reilly, 2015).

Though gender differences in reading have been found bymost studies that recruit sufficiently large and representativesamples, it is also important to acknowledge that there aresome rare exceptions. For example, Kaufman, Kaufman,Liu, and Johnson (2009) reported an analysis of the normingsample for the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement–Brief Form. The authors did not find significant genderdifferences in reading for adults, though significant genderdifferences were found in children as subsequently reportedby Scheiber et al. (2015) with this instrument. However, itis unclear whether this was the result of differences in testcontent across reading assessments, or if it was confoundedby historical effects of educational inequality in their cross-sectional sample (adults aged 22–90). Thus it is crucial toidentify under what contexts gender differences in readingmay be found, but their existence is not a foregone conclu-sion.

Gender Differences in Writing

As noted earlier, there are a limited number of studies thathave investigated gender differences in writing ability, and

the number of studies recruiting representative samples areeven fewer. Nowell and Hedges (1998) reported a moredetailed analysis of NAEP writing data from the period1984–1994, finding substantial gender differences in writ-ing (ranging from d � �.49 to �.55), greater male vari-ability, and that gender ratios for students falling in thebottom 10th percentile were between 2.6 and 3.3 males toevery female (Nowell & Hedges, 1998, p. 38). At present,there has been no subsequent meta-analysis published in-vestigating gender differences in NAEP writing assess-ments.

Two other prominent studies have investigated genderdifferences in writing with large representative samples.Camarata and Woodcock (2006) presented data from thenormative samples of the Woodcock-Johnson cognitive andachievement batteries, a large representative sample ofmales and females aged 5 through to 79. Females scoredsignificantly higher in writing achievement, with an averageeffect size across the life span of d � �.33. More recently,Scheiber et al. (2015) analyzed a large nationally represen-tative sample of adolescents and young adults completingthe Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement–SecondEdition Brief Form, which measures participants acrossreading, writing, and mathematics. While no difference wasfound in mathematics, females scored higher than males onthe tests of reading and writing ability. The effect size forreading was small (d � �.18), but the effect size for writing(d � �.40) was twice as large as that for reading. Given theappreciable gender differences found in these samples, itseems justifiable to expect a similarly sized effect in NAEPdata for writing tasks.

The Present Review

We sought to investigate whether the historical patterns ofgender differences in reading and writing reported byHedges and Nowell (1995) would be replicated for childrengrowing up in more recent decades. Consistent with previ-ous research, we hypothesized that gender differences inreading and writing achievement would be present. Basedon the claim made by Feingold (1988) and Caplan andCaplan (2016) that gender differences in cognitive abilityare decreasing, we also hypothesized that there would be asignificant negative association between year of assessmentand effect size, such that gender differences would show adecline over time. Given the large sample size employed byNAEP and that data from several decades of testing wereavailable, the analysis would have strong statistical power todetect an effect. Hyde and Grabe (2008) have advocated thata threshold of evidence higher than statistical significancebe adopted because although a very large sample size mightyield statistically significant differences, the actual size ofthe effects might be trivial. Therefore we adopted the re-search practice recommended by Hyde and Grabe (2008, p.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

5GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 219: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

170) and determined a priori that effect sizes smaller thand � .10 are characterized as trivial in size, even if they metthe threshold for statistical significance. We used Cohen’s(1988) recommendation that effect sizes around d � .20 beregarded as small, while around d � .50 medium.

Method

National Assessment of Educational ProgressData Source

The NAEP is a project of NCES, part of the U.S. Depart-ment of Education. The NAEP is used to track studentachievement over time in fourth-, eighth- and 12th-grade atthe state and national level of the United States. It measuresstudent achievement in reading, mathematics, science and avariety of other subject areas. National and state perfor-mances are reported annually in a series of reports titled“The Nation’s Report Card” (see http://nationsreportcard.gov/). This information is of use to parents, educators, andpolicymakers. However such reports only indicate that gen-der differences are statistically significant, without provid-ing any context about the size of such differences or genderratios of poor/advanced readers and writers.

NAEP data is also publically available so that it can beused by researchers to conduct secondary analysis, via theNAEP Data Explorer (http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/naepdata/). The sampling frame employed by NAEP is allstudents in the target grades (Grades 4, 8, and 12) in each ofthe 50 states of the United States, drawn from both publicand private educational institutions. School and studentresponses are appropriately weighted to draw a nationallyrepresentative estimate of the target population that reflectsstudent demographics such as socioeconomic status ofschool district, ethnicity, rural versus urban location andgender. For inclusiveness, the sampling frame also includesstudents with disabilities and English language learners,with the goal of reaching at least 85% of those identified asstudents with disabilities or English language learners. Ad-ditional information on the sampling methodology em-ployed is available from the NAEP website (http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/about/samplesfaq.aspx).

Content for the reading assessment includes reading com-prehension of a variety of different passages and genres(including information reports, stories, poetry and essays),as well as an understanding of vocabulary. Content for thewriting assessment includes persuasive, informative, andnarrative writing in response to stimuli material. Additionalinformation on reading and writing frameworks in eachgrade level is available from the NAEP website.

Schedule of Assessment

Reading and writing assessments are conducted periodi-cally, in adherence with the NAEP schedule. Reading as-

sessments are given greater priority than writing and occurevery 2 to 3 years (1988, 1990, 1992, 1994, 1998, 2000,2002, 2003, 2005, 2007, 2009, 2011, 2013, 2015), withgreater coverage given for students in Grades 4 and 8.Writing assessments occur approximately every 4 to 5 years(1998, 2002, 2007, 2011), and usually with a smaller samplesize than the reading assessments. We also included ar-chived data from the 1988, 1990, 1992, and 1996 writingassessments so that both dependent variables were assessedacross the same time frame. All assessments from 1988onward were included in the analysis.

Participants

National performance data for NAEP Reading assess-ments were examined from the period 1988–2015, with acombined total sample size of 3.035 million students. Test-ing data for the NAEP Writing assessments were examinedfor the period 1988–2011, with a combined total samplesize of 934,800. Students provided deemed consent throughtheir participation in each wave of assessment. This studyused published archival data and did not recruit participantsdirectly.

Meta-Analytic Procedure

Effect size statistics are presented as the mean differencebetween boys and girls in standardized units, commonlyreferred to as Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988). The meta-analysisemployed a random effects model. Heterogeneity acrosssamples was indicated by the I2 statistic, representing thepercentage of variation across samples attributed to genuineheterogeneity and not chance. We also investigated whetherthere were developmental differences in the magnitude ofthe gender gap across the three grade levels using subgroupanalysis, and whether the year of testing was a potentialmoderator using metaregression (Kelley & Kelley, 2012).Full details of the methodology employed in our analysisare reported in the online supplemental materials.

Results

Gender Differences in Reading Achievement

Girls showed significantly higher reading scores thanboys across every wave of assessment and in every grade,with an overall effect size of d � �.27, 95% confidenceinterval (CI) [–.29, �.25], Z � �26.08, p � .001 (seeFigure 1). Gender differences significantly exceeded thepredetermined cutoff (d � .10) advocated by Hyde andGrabe (2008) by a factor of 2.7. There was also significantheterogeneity in effect sizes, Q(36) � 2594.45, p � .001,I2 � 98.61, indicating considerable variation across assess-ments. To better explain the variability in effect sizes, we

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

6 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 220: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

investigated whether grade level or year of assessment werepotential moderators of the gender difference.

Grade level. Table 1 presents comparisons betweenmales and females in reading achievement across the threegrade levels assessed by NAEP. There was a statisticallysignificant difference between groups, Q(2) � 148.49, p �.001, with a tendency toward larger differences betweenboys and girls in older students. The initial gender differ-ence in reading achievement was small in Grade 4(d � �.19), but grew larger in Grade 8 (d � �.30) andGrade 12 (d � �.32).

Year of assessment. Next, we performed a metaregres-sion on reading achievement, using the year of assessmentas a predictor. There was no significant effect of year ofassessment, Z � .79, b � .0001, 95% CI [�.001, .003], p �.425, which is inconsistent with the hypothesis of a declin-ing gender difference over time.

Variance ratio. Consistent with previous research therewas greater male variability present in every sample tested,although it sometimes felt just short of Feingold’s threshold(VRs � 1.1) for individual years. Mean VR ratios werecalculated for each grade. All grades exceeded Feingold’s

Figure 1. Histogram of effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for the difference between boys and girls in readingachievement. All effect sizes fall to the left of the line of no effect and exceed Hyde’s criterion.

Table 1Gender Differences in National Assessment of Educational Progress Reading and Writing Achievement for Grades 4, 8, 12

95% Confidence interval Test of null (two-tail)

Outcome Grade k Cohen’s d Lower limit Upper limit Z value p VR Heterogeneity

Reading 4 14 �.19a,b �.21 �.18 �25.00 �.001 1.11 Q(13) � 47.69, p � .001, I2 � 72.748 13 �.30a �.32 �.29 �39.07 �.001 1.13 Q(12) � 157.06, p � .001, I2 � 92.36

12 10 �.32b �.34 �.30 �32.87 �.001 1.22 Q(9) � 245.01, p � .001, I2 � 96.33Writing 4 7 �.42a,b �.47 �.37 �17.55 �.001 1.01 Q(6) � 94.96, p � .001, I2 � 93.68

8 9 �.62a �.66 �.58 �29.94 �.001 1.06 Q(8) � 96.74, p � .001, I2 � 93.6812 9 �.55b �.59 �.51 �26.40 �.001 1.07 Q(8) � 256.73, p � .001, I2 � 96.88

Note. k � number of assessments conducted for each grade; VR � mean variance ratio. Boldface represents VRs that exceed Feingold’s threshold. Threeplanned contrasts between grades were conducted with a Bonferroni correction applied to control family-wise Type I error rate. Contrast C between Grade8 and 12 did not significantly differ with Bonferroni correction for reading and writing.a Grade 4 versus 8 significant. b Grade 4 versus 12 significant.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

7GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 221: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

critical value, with progressively higher variance in olderstudents.

Gender ratios for poor and gifted readers. In order toevaluate the combined effect of mean gender differencesand greater male variability on poor and gifted readers, weexamined the gender ratios of readers falling below the“basic” proficiency standard defined by NAEP as well asthose exceeding the “advanced” proficiency standard. Theanalysis examined the risk ratio of males to females attain-ing these levels. Equivalent proportions of boys and girls ata particular achievement level would be indicated by a riskratio of 1.00. Higher risk ratios (i.e., �1.00) would indicatean overrepresentation of boys attaining this standard, whilelower risk ratios (i.e., �1.00) would reflect an overrepre-sentation of girls at a particular standard.

The weighted risk ratio for poor readers was 1.39, 95% CI[1.34, 1.44], Z � 19.82, p � .001. The subgroup analysis isreported in Table 2. As can be seen, more boys than girlswere poor readers, which reached a ratio of 1.54 times asmany boys as girls falling below the minimum standard ofliteracy by Grade 12. The effect was reversed for advancedreaders, with more girls than boys achieving the advancedliteracy standard. Additionally the concentration of males inthe lower left tail of the distribution was higher than theconcentration of females at the upper right. The weightedrisk ratio for advanced readers was 0.55, 95% CI [0.52,0.59], Z � �17.28, p � .001, with subgroups also reportedin Table 2. Expressed in a metric that may be more intuitivefor nonstatisticians, by the time students reach Grade 12there are almost twice as many girls than boys that reach theadvanced standard of reading proficiency. Moderator anal-ysis showed a slight tendency toward smaller gender gaps inpoor readers over time (Z � �2.55, p � .010), but largergender gaps in advanced readers over time (Z � 2.31, p �.021).

Gender Differences in Writing Achievement

Next we examined the gender difference in writingachievement for the period 1988–2011. Overall, the gender

difference between males and females in writing was largerthan that found for reading, d � �.54, 95% CI [–.57, �.51],Z � �36.14, p � .001 (see Figure 2). Gender differences inwriting exceeded the predetermined cutoff (d � .10) advo-cated by Hyde and Grabe (2008) by a factor of 5.4. Therewas also significant heterogeneity in effect sizes, Q(24) �974.07, p � .001, I2 � 97.54, indicating considerable vari-ation across assessments. In order to better explain thevariability in effect sizes, we investigated whether gradelevel or year of assessment were potential moderators of thegender gap. An additional factor introducing heterogeneitymay be the changes in writing frameworks (new frame-works were introduced in 1988, 1998, and 2011) and themarked variability in sample sizes for more recent assess-ments.

Grade level. Table 1 presents comparisons betweenmales and females in writing achievement across the threegrade levels assessed by NAEP. The difference betweengrades was statistically significant, Q(2) � 42.01, p � .001,with a tendency toward a smaller initial gender difference inwriting proficiency for students in Grade 4. The initialgender difference in writing was medium-sized in Grade 4(d � �.42), but grew larger in Grade 8 (d � �.62) andGrade 12 (d � �.55).

Year of assessment. We performed a metaregressionon writing achievement, using the year of assessment as thepredictor. There was no significant effect of year,Z � �1.85, b � �.004, 95% CI [–.001, .001], p � .063,indicating stability in effect sizes across historical time.

Variance ratio. In examining the variance ratios pre-sented in Table 1, there was minimal support for greatermale variability with all grades falling short of Feingold’sthreshold.

Gender ratios for poor and gifted writers. In order toevaluate the joint effect of greater male variability and meangender differences on poor and gifted writers, we examinedthe gender ratios of readers below the ‘basic’ achievementas well as those exceeding the “advanced” achievementlevel. Writing proficiency levels attained were not pub-

Table 2Risk Ratio for Poor and Advanced Proficiency Level Readers and Writers, Across Grade Levels

Outcome Grade

Poor readers Advanced readers

Risk ratio

95% Confidence interval Test of null

Risk ratio

95% Confidence interval Test of null

Lower limit Upper limit Z p Lower limit Upper limit Z p

Reading 4 1.22a,b 1.20 1.24 25.55 �.001 .70a,b .68 .73 �19.64 �.0018 1.45a,b,c 1.43 1.48 44.84 �.001 .47a,b .45 .49 �36.82 �.001

12 1.54b,c 1.50 1.58 34.76 �.001 .52 .49 .55 �21.94 �.001Writing 4 2.01 1.55 2.60 5.31 �.001 .28 .20 .39 �7.40 �.001

8 2.27 1.89 2.72 44.84 �.001 .27 .21 .36 �9.66 �.00112 2.21 1.84 2.65 8.51 �.001 .39 .28 .54 �5.60 �.001

Note. Three planned contrasts between grades were conducted, with a Bonferroni correction applied to control family-wise Type I error rate.a Grade 4 versus 8 significant. b Grade 4 versus 12 significant. c Grade 8 versus 12 significant.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

8 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 222: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

lished for the archived reports (1988–1996), and weretherefore excluded from analysis.

The weighted risk ratio for poor writers was 2.19, 95% CI[2.00, 2.40], Z � 17.06, p � .001, with subgroup analysisreported for grades in Table 2. As can be seen by the table,there were twice as many boys falling into the category ofpoor writing than girls. However, the effect is reversed foradvanced readers with more girls achieving the advancedstandard for written expression, with a weighted risk ratio of0.30, 95% CI [.25, .36], Z � 13.29, p � .001. In otherwords, by the time students reach Grade 12 there are over2.54 times as many girls than boys that attain the advancedstandard of writing proficiency. Moderator analysis showedno change in gender ratios for poor writers over time (Z �.15, p � .881), or advanced (Z � 1.80, p � .071).

Discussion

Annual reporting of NAEP data had noted girls performedsignificantly higher than boys, but failed to provide esti-mates of how large these differences were. By calculatingan effect size, we can hold evidence of gender differences toa much higher standard than mere statistical significance byexamining whether the effect is practically significant.While a focus on mean gender differences is important, wealso considered its combined effect with greater male vari-

ability on the gender ratios at the lower left (poor readers/writers) and upper right (advanced readers/writers) tails ofthe ability distribution. Both measures (effect size for meangender differences, gender ratios of low/high achievers)provide a more comprehensive perspective than simplyexamining probability values. Further, the detailed recordskept for NAEP testing data offered a window into the pastto examine how boys and girls have fared in reading andwriting achievement over historical time (both developmen-tally across grades, and cohort effects across historicaltime).

Reading Proficiency

Girls significantly outperformed boys in reading abilityacross all grades, with a tendency toward larger effect sizesin high school than primary school (Grades 8, d � �.30 and12, d � �.32). These exceed Hyde’s criterion by a factor of3, and fall in the small-to-medium effect size categoryproposed by Cohen (1988). They are also comparable toeffect sizes for American students in international assess-ments such as PISA (Reilly, 2012) and the Progress inInternational Reading Literacy assessment (PIRLS; Mullis,Martin, Gonzalez, & Kennedy, 2003), where small to me-dium effect sizes were found. There was also no evidence ofa decline in the magnitude of effect sizes over time as had

Figure 2. Histogram of effect sizes for the difference between boys and girls in writing achievement. Alleffect sizes fall to the left of the line of no effect and exceed Hyde’s criterion for nontrivial gender differences.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

9GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 223: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

been hypothesized, though it is possible that this might bedetectable over a longer passage of historical time. Howeverto compare these results to other standardized tests of read-ing and writing would be problematic, and introduce amethodological confound in test content, level of difficulty,and sampling size.

How ought we interpret the practical impact of suchgender differences in reading? Rosenthal and Rubin (1982a)developed the binomial effect size display (BESD) to illus-trate the practical impact of such differences for nonstatis-ticians (such as parents and educators), especially for stu-dents falling near the middle of the distribution. This metricshows the percentage of males and females that meet orexceed an average score. Represented in the BESD format,the likelihood of being average or higher in reading abilityfor a student at the end of high school increases from 42.1%for boys to 57.8% for girls, a not insubstantial amount.

We can also contextualize this by considering the size ofgender differences for other types of cognitive ability, suchas science, technology, engineering, and mathematicsachievement. While considerable research focuses on thegender gap in mathematics and science, the effect sizes forreading are more substantial—over twice the size as thatfound in comparable NAEP assessments of mathematics(McGraw, Lubienski, & Strutchens, 2006; Reilly, Neu-mann, & Andrews, 2015). Thus, is it is important to ac-knowledge female strengths as well as those areas wheremales perform higher. Claims by researchers such as Caplanand Caplan (1997, 2016) that cognitive differences aredisappearing are therefore premature, but neither does itsupport a claim that boys and girls are radically different inreading literacy and would benefit from gender-segregatedinstruction as is claimed by same-sex advocates.

However, effect size statistics only represent the typicallyperforming girl or boy. When we examined students that fallbelow the minimum proficiency standard, far more boysthan girls fall into this category across all grades and moreimportantly by the end of high school by a factor of 1.5.This imbalance in a representative sample contradicts theclaim made by Shaywitz et al. (1990) that a greater diag-nosis of reading impairment in boys is merely the result ofa gendered referral bias. A completely different pattern wasfound for advanced readers though, with far more girlsattaining this level of proficiency (by a factor of almost 2).The pattern of results shows that at all levels of the abilitydistribution, girls significantly outperform boys in readingachievement. Hyde (2005) had proposed the GSH, arguingthat most mean gender differences are small or trivial inmagnitude. A limitation of that hypothesis though is that itfocuses exclusively on mean gender differences and effectsizes, while ignoring evidence from the upper and lowertails of the ability distribution. Taken together, it wouldappear there are gender differences in reading favoring girlsacross all levels of ability distribution, with these being

small (and more similar) in the middle of the distributionbut much larger (and impactful) at the tails. Furthermore,these gender differences are found in younger students, aswell as older ones. We did not find strong support for thegreater male variability hypothesis because the larger num-ber of low scoring boys was offset by the higher number ofhigh scoring girls.

Writing Proficiency

As was expected from previous studies (e.g., Hedges &Nowell, 1995; Reynolds et al., 2015), girls significantly out-performed boys in writing ability across all grades and assess-ment waves. The magnitude of effect sizes was higher than thatfound for reading, with effect sizes falling into the medium sizerange by Cohen’s (1988) conventions. Comparisons betweenboys and girls were slightly smaller in Grade 4 (d � �.42), butthis gender difference widened for older students (Grades 8,d � �.62; Grades 12, d � �.55). Represented in the BESDformat, the likelihood of being average or higher in writingability for a student at the end of high school increases from36.7% for boys to 63.3% for girls (i.e., a minority of boysattain this standard, but the majority of girls do). Furthermore,when examining the association between effect size and yearof assessment, there was no decline in the magnitude of effectsizes as predicted.

At the lower end of the ability distribution, boys were greatlyoverrepresented by a factor of 2 or more which grew slightlylarger for older students. Just as with reading, there was areversal of gender ratios for students attaining an advancedwriting proficiency, with girls greatly overrepresented by afactor of 2 or more. These results are consistent with theposition held by Reynolds et al. (2015) who argued that agender difference in writing is an exception to the GSH andcannot be easily dismissed as a small or trivial difference.

Why might the effect size for writing be larger than theeffect size for reading? Writing represents a more challeng-ing task, and larger gender differences are typically found asthe complexity of the task increases. While reading is apassive task, writing is a generative task that draws on othercomponents of verbal and language abilities that typicallyshow larger gender differences. For example, it requirescareful organization of ideas and the production of materialthat is clearly expressed, and grammatically accurate. Re-search shows that females score significantly higher onstandardized tests of spelling and of grammar, withmedium-sized effects (Stanley et al., 1992). Finding theright words to express a particular concept or nuance is alsoa demanding task for writers, and draws on verbal fluency(where females also show significantly higher performancethan males). Halpern and Tan (2001) noted that effect sizesfor verbal fluency fall in the medium to large range. All ofthese verbal skills can be improved with sufficient practice

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

10 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 224: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

and instruction, however, highlighting the importance ofthese basic skills in a crowded educational curriculum.

Gender Similarities Hypothesis

Hyde (2005) has proposed the GSH, which claims thatmost—but not all—psychological gender differences are smallor trivial in size. Zell, Krizan, and Teeter (2015) used thetechnique of metasynthesis to test this claim, finding that mosteffect sizes were small. However, they also noted a number ofimportant exceptions (see Zell et al., 2015, Table 3). Thegender differences observed in the present study for reading(small-to-medium) and writing (medium-sized) also representexceptions that may have been overlooked because a meta-analysis on the NAEP dataset had yet to be published formodern samples. The identification of new areas where mean-ingful gender differences remain does not invalid the GSH, butdoes serve as a prompt for further investigation.

Educational Implications

What might be the educational implications of such a gendergap in reading and writing proficiency during primary and highschool years? While information in the classroom environmentis often presented verbally, students are expected to read text-books and literary material as independent reading. Difficultiesin reading would be a serious impediment, particularly ifreading takes boys longer or if they are unable to gain a deepunderstanding of the text. While it has been known for sometime that boys are overrepresented in reading disabilities andformal diagnoses of dyslexia (Berninger et al., 2008; Rutter etal., 2004), this pattern of results suggests a more generalreading deficiency for the typical male student. Written com-munication is also important during the high school years, asthis format is commonly adopted in the format of essays orlaboratory reports. While boys tend to perform better than girlson standardized tests, girls tend to achieve significantly highergrades during schooling (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Voyer& Voyer, 2014), and it is possible that the assessment format(exam vs. written assignment) may be a contributing factor.While the issue of gender differences in reading ability hasbeen the focus of much research, gender differences in writingability may have been previously underestimated by research-ers and educators. The magnitude of the gender gap in writingability is sufficiently large that it may warrant educationalinterventions and further research on etiology. It may also bereflective of a more general language deficiency (rather thanjust an issue with reading), as other studies have also reportedpronounced gender differences in grammar and language us-age (e.g., Stanley et al., 1992).

While the existence of a gender gap in reading andwriting during compulsory schooling is troubling, the edu-cational implications for students considering pursuing ter-tiary education are potentially compounded. In a review of

gender inequalities in education, Buchmann, DiPrete, andMcDaniel (2008) note that women enroll in college anduniversities at a much higher rate than their similarly agedmale peers, achieve higher grades on average than males,and have a higher rate of degree completion (Buchmann &DiPrete, 2006). This pattern is mirrored across most OECDcountries and is not confined to the United States (OECD,2016). The transition from secondary to tertiary educationcan be difficult for many students, because it involvesindependent learning and considerable hours of study out-side of classroom contact time. The ability to read textbooksand assigned readings is a crucial part of learning. Althoughespecially poor readers are less likely to pursue tertiarystudies, the gender gap in reading appears to be present inaverage students as well, though smaller. In addition, theability to communicate verbally in a written format takes onincreasing importance, as producing reports and essays area common form of student assessment. Systemic gendergaps in writing might leave male students significantlyunderprepared for tertiary admission, and offer a partialexplanation for why females on average achieve highergrades in their tertiary studies (Voyer & Voyer, 2014) andhave higher completion rates (Buchmann et al., 2008).

Parents, educators, and policymakers may wonder what tomake of gender differences in reading and writing, and whatchanges might be made to address them in the interests ofequality of educational outcomes. It would be a mistake to takeevidence from this study to argue that boys and girls learn infundamentally different ways, require different styles of teach-ing, or would benefit from same-sex schooling. Scientificliterature is clear about the negative effects of highlightinggender in this way (Halpern, Eliot, et al., 2011), and howtreating a particular demographic group (i.e., just boys) canserve to undermine their confidence and motivation to im-prove. While attention has been paid in the past to earlyintervention for reading, educational interventions for writingmay be warranted, and a greater focus on writing tasks in thecurriculum to provide additional opportunities to practice writ-ing skills and provide feedback to students. These should beoffered broadly to all students—while the findings of thisstudy suggest that boys would benefit from these initiatives,these results also suggest that many girls would similarlybenefit.

Future Directions for Research and Limitations

While this study documents the existence and magnitudeof gender differences in reading and writing, it cannot shedany light on their etiology and which biological and social-ization factors most contribute to their development (Eagly& Wood, 2013). Most researchers advocate a biopsychoso-cial model of gender differences (Halpern, 2000), but sometentative conclusions may be drawn from the generalizabil-ity of gender differences in language outcomes across sam-

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

11GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 225: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

ples, historical time periods, and cultures. Our study repli-cates findings reported by Hedges and Nowell (1995) forearlier decades, and the magnitude of gender differencesremains stable over historical time. Additionally, we founda developmental effect, such that gender differences werefound in much younger students but increased with addi-tional years of schooling. International studies of readingachievement find that greater female performance is founduniversally across all nations (Guiso et al., 2008; Reilly,2012), which would at least be consistent with biologicalfactors. Yet there is also substantial variability across na-tions in the size of the gender gap, with sociocultural factorssuch as nation’s level of gender equality and gender normsalso making a strong contribution (Reilly, 2015). Like manygender researchers, we advocate a broad biopsychosocialmodel of factors that contribute to gender differences (Halp-ern, 2000), rather than any single cause.

Despite the strengths that a large demographically represen-tative sample like NAEP offers, the chief limitation is that it islimited to students from the United States, and to the way inwhich reading and writing skills are measured. Educationalpractices and frameworks clearly differ from country to coun-try. International assessments of students’ reading achievementsuch as PIRLS and PISA (Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Reilly, 2012)have found that the gender difference in reading is universal(i.e., all countries find girls significantly and meaningfullyoutperform boys). However, it is unclear whether a similarlysized gender difference in writing skills would exist interna-tionally. Additionally due to limitations of the dataset (such aslack of subgroup sample sizes in the publicly available data), itwas not possible to investigate Gender � Ethnicity interactionsor socioeconomic status differences, a limitation shared byother analyses of NAEP data (e.g., Reilly et al., 2015). Onefactor that cannot be controlled in this dataset though is studentdropout rates. As compulsory schooling extends in most U.S.states up to age 16, there should not be any meaningful attritionbetween Grades 4 and 8. While high school completion rateshave been steadily increasing over the historical time periodexamined, the gender differences reported for Grade 12 do notinclude young adults that leave before this time (and presum-ably may have poorer reading and writing proficiency). Asmore girls complete high school than boys, this may underes-timate the extent of gender differences in reading and writingin the general population. Additionally, it only presents asnapshot of students enrolled in U.S. schools—children thatare unable to attend formal schooling due to other factors suchas intellectual disability would likewise not be measured.

Conclusion

Gender differences in reading and writing achievement werefound across all levels of the ability spectrum. Girls outper-formed boys in mean reading and writing achievement, andcontrary to our hypothesis these gender differences do not

appear to be declining over the time period analyzed (1988–2015). Furthermore, there were pronounced differences in gen-der ratios for poor readers/writers, with boys greatly overrep-resented. This pattern was reversed for those students attainingan advanced proficiency standard, with significantly more girlsthan boys. Our study also examined gender differences inyounger students than those reported by Hedges and Nowell(1995), finding a developmental effect toward larger gaps asstudents progress through their schooling. These findings holdeducational implications for students’ academic success duringprimary and high school, as well as academic readiness toembark on further college studies. A challenge for researchersis to identify the precise nature of gender differences in readingand writing so that educators can design targeted interventionsto improve children’s reading and writing skills.

References

Archer, J., & Lloyd, B. (2002). Sex and gender (2nd ed.). Cambridge,United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139051910

Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in con-temporary American society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-RoleInventory. Sex Roles, 43, 499–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007119516728

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection andvariability in studies of gender differences. Behavioral and Brain Sci-ences, 11, 183–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sextyping. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354

Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind,W. (2008). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading dis-abilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 151–172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.007

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009).Introduction to meta-analysis. West Sussex, United Kingdom: Wiley.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470743386

Buchmann, C., & DiPrete, T. A. (2006). The growing female advantage incollege completion: The role of family background and academicachievement. American Sociological Review, 71, 515–541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000312240607100401

Buchmann, C., DiPrete, T. A., & McDaniel, A. (2008). Gender inequalitiesin education. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 319–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.34.040507.134719

Burman, D. D., Bitan, T., & Booth, J. R. (2008). Sex differences in neuralprocessing of language among children. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1349–1362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.021

Camarata, S., & Woodcock, R. (2006). Sex differences in processingspeed: Developmental effects in males and females. Intelligence, 34,231–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.12.001

Caplan, J. B., & Caplan, P. J. (2016). Thinking critically about research onsex and gender (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1997). Do sex-related cognitive differencesexist, and why do people seek them out? In P. J. Caplan, M. Crawford,J. S. Hyde, & J. T. E. Richardson (Eds.), Gender differences in humancognition (pp. 52–80). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195112917.003.0003

Clements, A. M., Rimrodt, S. L., Abel, J. R., Blankner, J. G., Mostofsky,S. H., Pekar, J. J., . . . Cutting, L. E. (2006). Sex differences in cerebrallaterality of language and visuospatial processing. Brain and Language,98, 150–158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.007

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

12 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 226: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Conger, D., & Long, M. C. (2010). Why are men falling behind? Gendergaps in college performance and persistence. The Annals of the Ameri-can Academy of Political and Social Science, 627, 184–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002716209348751

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Self-discipline gives girlsthe edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores.Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 198–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198

Dwyer, C. A. (1973). Sex differences in reading: An evaluation and acritique of current theories. Review of Educational Research, 43, 455–467. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543043004455

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in humanbehavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psycholo-gist, 54, 408–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2013). The nature–nurture debates: 25 years ofchallenges in understanding the psychology of gender. Perspectives onPsychological Science, 8, 340 –357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691613484767

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes,expectancy effects, and parents’ socialization of gender differences.Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x

Else-Quest, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2018). The psychology of women: Halfthe human experience (9th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. Amer-ican Psychologist, 43, 95–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities:A new look at an old controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62,61–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543062001061

Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences inlanguage processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months.Developmental Science, 16, 234–248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019

Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences(2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12072-000

Geary, D. C., Winegard, B., & Winegard, B. (2014). Reflections on theevolution of human sex differences: Social selection and the evolution ofcompetition among women. In V. Weekes-Shackleford & T. K. Shack-elford (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychologyand behavior (pp. 393–412). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0314-6_20

Gignac, G. E., & Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for indi-vidual differences researchers. Personality and Individual Differences,102, 74–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Diversity:Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320, 1164–1165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1154094

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011). Sex differencesin intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cam-bridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 253–272). New York, NY: Cam-bridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511977244.014

Halpern, D. F., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde,J., . . . Martin, C. L. (2011). Education: The pseudoscience of single-sexschooling. Science, 333, 1706–1707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1205031

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011). Beliefs aboutcognitive gender differences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for

size. Sex Roles, 64, 336–347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using apsychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brainand Cognition, 45, 392–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1287

Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P.,Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) andchildren’s intelligence (IQ): In a U.K.-representative sample SES mod-erates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7,e30320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030320

Hawke, J. L., Olson, R. K., Willcut, E. G., Wadsworth, S. J., & DeFries,J. C. (2009). Gender ratios for reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 15, 239–242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.389

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores,variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7604277

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychol-ogist, 60, 581–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review ofPsychology, 65, 373–398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057

Hyde, J. S., & Grabe, S. (2008). Meta-analysis in the psychology ofwomen. In F. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: Ahandbook of issues and theories (pp. 142–173). Westport, CT: Praeger.

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: Ameta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.104.1.53

Jiménez, J. E., de la Cadena, C. G., Siegel, L. S., O’Shanahan, I., García,E., & Rodríguez, C. (2011). Gender ratio and cognitive profiles indyslexia: A cross-national study. Reading and Writing, 24, 729–747.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9222-6

Kagan, J. (1964). The child’s sex role classification of school objects.Child Development, 35, 1051–1056.

Kaiser, A., Haller, S., Schmitz, S., & Nitsch, C. (2009). On sex/genderrelated similarities and differences in fMRI language research. BrainResearch Brain Research Reviews, 61, 49–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.03.005

Kaufman, A. S., Kaufman, J. C., Liu, X., & Johnson, C. K. (2009). Howdo educational attainment and gender relate to fluid intelligence, crys-tallized intelligence, and academic skills at ages 22–90 years? Archivesof Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 153–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp015

Kelley, G. A., & Kelley, K. S. (2012). Statistical models for meta-analysis:A brief tutorial. World Journal of Methodology, 2, 27–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.5662/wjm.v2.i4.27

Kutner, M., Greenberg, E., Jin, Y., Boyle, B., Hsu, Y.-c., & Dunleavy, E.(2007). Literacy in everyday life: Results from the 2003 National As-sessment of Adult Literacy. Washington, DC: National Center for Edu-cation Statistics.

Levy, J. (1969). Possible basis for the evolution of lateral specialization ofthe human brain. Nature, 224, 614–615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/224614a0

Limbrick, L., Wheldall, K., & Madelaine, A. (2011). Why do more boysthan girls have a reading disability? A review of the evidence. Austral-asian Journal of Special Education, 35, 1–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/ajse.35.1.1

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement.Trames, 13, 3–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.3176/tr.2009.1.01

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences.Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Assessment: Global sex differencesin test score variability. Science, 322, 1331–1332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1162573

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

13GENDER DIFFERENCES IN READING AND WRITING

Page 227: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Marinak, B. A., & Gambrell, L. B. (2010). Reading motivation: Exploringthe elementary gender gap. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49,129–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19388070902803795

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender development.Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353–381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100511

McGee, R., Prior, M., Williams, S., Smart, D., & Sanson, A. (2002). Thelong-term significance of teacher-rated hyperactivity and reading abilityin childhood: Findings from two longitudinal studies. Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 43, 1004–1017.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00228

McGraw, R., Lubienski, S. T., & Strutchens, M. E. (2006). A closer lookat gender in NAEP mathematics achievement and affect data: Intersec-tions with achievement, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Jour-nal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37, 129–150.

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sexdifferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 37–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011

Moffitt, M. A. S., & Wartella, E. (1991). Youth and reading: A survey ofleisure reading pursuits of female and male adolescents. Reading Re-search and Instruction, 31, 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19388079209558075

Mucherah, W., & Yoder, A. (2008). Motivation for reading and middleschool students’ performance on standardized testing in reading. Read-ing Psychology, 29, 214 –235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02702710801982159

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E., & Kennedy, A. M. (2003).PIRLS 2001 International Report: IEA’s study of reading literacy inprimary schools in 35 countries. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLSInternational Study Center.

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. InM. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cogni-tive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263–302). New York: Ac-ademic Press.

National Center for Education Statistics. (2016). Digest of EducationalStatistics. Retrieved from: https://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/

Nowell, A., & Hedges, L. V. (1998). Trends in gender differences inacademic achievement from 1960 to 1994: An analysis of differences inmean, variance, and extreme scores. Sex Roles, 39, 21–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018873615316

Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD).(2016). Education at a Glance 2016: OECD Indicators. Paris: Author.

Plante, I., de la Sablonnière, R., Aronson, J. M., & Théorêt, M. (2013).Gender stereotype endorsement and achievement-related outcomes: Therole of competence beliefs and task values. Contemporary EducationalPsychology, 38, 225–235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.03.004

Rabiner, D., & Coie, J. D., & The Conduct Problems Prevention ResearchGroup. (2000). Early attention problems and children’s reading achieve-ment: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of the American Academy ofChild & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 859–867. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200007000-00014

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoSOne, 7(7), e39904. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Reilly, D. (2015). Gender differences in reading from a cross-culturalperspective: The contribution of gender equality. Paper presented at theInternational Convention of Psychological Science, Amsterdam, Neth-erlands. http://dx.doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.18218.72647

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences inmathematics and science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment ofEducational Progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology,107, 645–662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-roledifferences in specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147–158.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Reynolds, M. R., Scheiber, C., Hajovsky, D. B., Schwartz, B., & Kaufman,A. S. (2015). Gender differences in academic achievement: Is writing anexception to the gender similarities hypothesis? The Journal of GeneticPsychology, 176, 211–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2015.1036833

Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982a). Further meta-analytic proceduresfor assessing cognitive gender differences. Journal of Educational Psy-chology, 74, 708–712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.5.708

Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982b). A simple, general purpose displayof magnitude of experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychol-ogy, 74, 166–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.2.166

Rutter, M., Caspi, A., Fergusson, D., Horwood, L. J., Goodman, R.,Maughan, B., . . . Carroll, J. (2004). Sex differences in developmentalreading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. Journalof the American Medical Association, 291, 2007–2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.291.16.2007

Scheiber, C., Reynolds, M. R., Hajovsky, D. B., & Kaufman, A. S. (2015).Gender differences in achievement in a large, nationally representativesample of children and adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 52,335–348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.21827

Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Constable, R. T., Skudlarski,P., Fulbright, R. K., . . . . (1995). Sex differences in the functionalorganization of the brain for language. Nature, 373, 607–609. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/373607a0

Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990).Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of the Con-necticut Longitudinal Study. Journal of the American Medical Associ-ation, 264, 998 –1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1990.03450080084036

Smart, D., Prior, M., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2001). Children withreading difficulties: A six year follow-up from early primary school tosecondary school. Australian Journal of Psychology, 53, 45–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00049530108255121

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits,trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychologyof Women Quarterly, 24, 44–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x

Stanley, J. C., Benbow, C. P., Brody, L., Dauber, S., & Lupkowski, A.(1992). Gender differences on eighty-six nationally standardized apti-tude and achievement tests. In N. Colangelo, S. G. Assouline & D. L.Ambroson (Eds.), Talent development: Proceedings from the 1991Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on TalentDevelopment (pp. 42–65). Unionville, NY: Trillium Press.

Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholasticachievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1174–1204.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036620

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differ-ences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of criticalvariables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250

Wallentin, M. (2009). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities andlanguage cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language, 108, 175–183.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.07.001

Zell, E., Krizan, Z., & Teeter, S. R. (2015). Evaluating gender similaritiesand differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist, 70, 10–20.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038208

Received January 10, 2018Revision received May 22, 2018

Accepted May 29, 2018 �

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

14 REILLY, NEUMANN, AND ANDREWS

Page 228: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 135

Chapter 6 – Cross-Cultural Patterns of Reading, Mathematics and Science Literacy

This chapter reports on a meta-analysis of student testing data from the 2009

wave of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), a large-scale

educational assessment of student’s reading, mathematics and science literacy across all

OECD members and a number of partner nations. This study reports data from 65

nations. Consistently across all nations, girls outperform boys in reading literacy,

d = -.44. Boys outperform girls in mathematics in the USA, d = +.22 and across OECD

nations, d = +.13. For science literacy, while the USA showed the largest gender

difference across all OECD nations, d = +.14, gender differences across OECD nations

were non-significant, and a small female advantage was found for non-OECD nations, d

= -.09. Across all three domains, these differences were more pronounced at both tails

of the distribution for low- and high-achievers. Considerable cross-cultural variability

was also observed, and national gender differences were correlated with gender equity

measures, economic prosperity, and Hofstede’s cultural dimension of power distance.

Educational and societal implications of such gender gaps are addressed, as well as the

mechanisms by which gender differences in cognitive abilities are culturally mediated.

It has been published as has been published as

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7),

e39904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Copyright statement :

It was published in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution (CC_BY)

license, and copyright was retained by the author.

Page 229: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender, Culture, and Sex-Typed Cognitive AbilitiesDavid Reilly*

School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Southport, Queensland, Australia

Abstract

Although gender differences in cognitive abilities are frequently reported, the magnitude of these differences and whetherthey hold practical significance in the educational outcomes of boys and girls is highly debated. Furthermore, when gendergaps in reading, mathematics and science literacy are reported they are often attributed to innate, biological differencesrather than social and cultural factors. Cross-cultural evidence may contribute to this debate, and this study reports nationalgender differences in reading, mathematics and science literacy from 65 nations participating in the 2009 round of theProgramme for International Student Assessment (PISA). Consistently across all nations, girls outperform boys in readingliteracy, d = 2.44. Boys outperform girls in mathematics in the USA, d = .22 and across OECD nations, d = .13. For scienceliteracy, while the USA showed the largest gender difference across all OECD nations, d = .14, gender differences acrossOECD nations were non-significant, and a small female advantage was found for non-OECD nations, d = 2.09. Across allthree domains, these differences were more pronounced at both tails of the distribution for low- and high-achievers.Considerable cross-cultural variability was also observed, and national gender differences were correlated with genderequity measures, economic prosperity, and Hofstede’s cultural dimension of power distance. Educational and societalimplications of such gender gaps are addressed, as well as the mechanisms by which gender differences in cognitiveabilities are culturally mediated.

Citation: Reilly D (2012) Gender, Culture, and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities. PLoS ONE 7(7): e39904. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Editor: Sonia Brucki, University Of Sao Paulo, Brazil

Received February 29, 2012; Accepted May 28, 2012; Published July 10, 2012

Copyright: � 2012 David Reilly. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permitsunrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Funding: This research was supported in part by a Griffith University Postgraduate Research Scholarship. The funders had no role in study design, data collectionand analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing Interests: The author has declared that no competing interests exist.

* E-mail: [email protected]

Introduction

Rightly or wrongly, the topic of gender differences in cognitive

abilities appears perennial, holding curiosity not only for social

scientists but also for the general public and media [1–4].

Intelligence is multifaceted [5–10], and comprises a range of

culturally-valued cognitive abilities. While there is almost unan-

imous consensus that men and women do not differ in general

intelligence [11–14], there are several domains where either males

or females as a group may show an advantage, such as visuospatial

[15–16] and verbal abilities [17–18] respectively. However, gender

differences in quantitative abilities [19], such as science and

mathematics, remain contentious. Researchers are divided be-

tween arguing for small but still influential differences in

quantitative reasoning [9–11], and claiming that any observed

differences in maths are so small, in fact, that they can be

categorised as ‘trivial’ [12–14].

A key limitation of research in this area is that it is largely US-

centric, and does not speak to gender differences between males

and females raised under different social and educational

environments in other cultures. Additional lines of evidence are

required, and one such source is international testing of students.

Secondly, research primarily focuses on mean gender differences,

and fails to address gender differences in the tails of distributions

which Hyde, et al. [20] argues may forecast the underrepresen-

tation of women in the science, technology, engineering and

mathematics (STEM) related professions.

To this aim, I present findings from the 2009 OECD

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), which

to my knowledge has not yet been widely discussed in psychology

journals. This information provides a snapshot of current gender

differences and similarities in reading, mathematics and science

across 65 nations. It also highlights the wide degree of cultural

variation between nations, and examines the role that social and

environmental factors play in the development of gender

differences. Before reviewing the PISA findings, I will briefly

discuss the advantages that national and cross-national testing

have to offer the debate on the nature of gender differences in

cognitive abilities.

Advantages of Nationally-representative Samples forAssessing Gender Differences

Large national and international samples can provide a

‘yardstick’ estimate of gender differences within a given region,

at a given point in time. By drawing from a broad population

of students, national and international testing provide us with

stronger evidence for gender similarities or differences than

could be found from smaller, more selective samples. It is

common practice for gender difference studies to use conve-

nience samples drawn from psychology student subject pools

[21], as well as from groups of high performing students such as

gifted and talented programmes [22] – conclusions drawn from

such samples may not be generalizable to wider populations.

There is evidence to suggest that the performance of males is

more widely distributed, with a greater numbers of high and

low achievers [23]. This has been termed the greater male

variability hypothesis [10,15–16], and presents a problem for

researchers recruiting from only high achievers – even though

mean differences between males and females may be equal, if

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 230: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

the distribution of male scores is wider than females, males will

be overrepresented as high-achievers in a selective sample. This

may lead to the erroneous conclusion that gender differences

exist in the population of males and females.

A good example of this in practice comes in the form of the

Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT) used for assessing suitability of

students for college entry within the United States. Males

consistently outperform females on the mathematical component

[22,24–25]. Gender differences in SAT-M are extremely robust

across decades, see Figure 1. On the basis of this evidence

alone, one might erroneously conclude that the gender gap in

mathematics is pervasive unless consideration is given to the

demographics of the sample. Students considering college

admission are motivated to undertake the SAT, and this is

largely a self-selected sample that may differ on important

characteristics such as socioeconomic status, and general ability

level. Additionally many more girls sit the SAT than boys

[24,26], reflecting the higher admission rate of women in

college [27]. Thus the sample of males is more selective, while

the sample of females is more general. One cannot rule out the

possibility that the male sample includes a greater proportion of

high achieving students and that the female sample may have

included students of more mediocre mathematical ability,

lowering mean performance.

This does not mean, necessarily, that one should discount any

finding of gender differences in the SAT-M as being invalid. Data

from the SAT may be extremely useful in estimating gender

differences in the population of students considering further

education. This is a very narrow, quite specific theoretical

question. But such findings cannot be easily generalised to the

general population, which is what researchers and laypersons alike

would seek to test.

Another source of information on gender differences comes

from experimental research carried out in the laboratory, under

tightly controlled conditions. Equal numbers of males and females

can be recruited using random selection. When large samples are

randomly drawn from the general population, the scores of both

high and low achievers are included in measurements of gender

differences. Such studies are time-consuming and expensive to

conduct, however. More commonly, gender difference studies use

much smaller convenience samples, such as a subject pool of

college students which also introduces the problem of selection

bias [21]. College subject pools differ from the general population

across many different characteristics [28], such as socioeconomic

status, general intelligence, and prior educational experiences.

Since the scores of males are more variable [12,18–19], a

convenience sample that draws from only the upper-tail of ability

will be skewed with a greater frequency of high performing males

than females, thus exaggerating any gender difference that is

found.

Additionally, many cognitive abilities show an interaction

between gender and socioeconomic status [1,25–28]. Studies

that selectively recruit from college subject pools in medium- to

high- socioeconomic status regions would therefore be more

likely to find gender differences than those recruiting from lower

socioeconomic regions, as there will be greater differentiation

between high and low ability levels. Likewise, samples drawing

from a college pool may find greater gender differences than if

they were recruited from a high school sample, or from the

general population. Potentially, this could give a distorted

picture of actual gender gaps when generalising from these

selective samples to the wider population of males and females.

Large national samples allow researchers to investigate

objectively the existence and magnitude of gender differences

or similarities. We can be more confident that any observed

differences are reflective of what we would find in the general

population of boys and girls, and are not simply due to

sampling bias. As additional waves of testing are conducted

using similar measurement instruments, we can also begin to

track any changes over time. It allows us to evaluate efforts

aimed at reducing gender differences, and to see areas where

further progress must be made. Such data may also be of

benefit to policy makers and educational institutions in

advocating for educational change, and in support of programs

aimed at addressing inequalities.

Figure 1. Gender differences in SAT-M performance. On average, boys score higher than girls on the SAT-M exam (approximately one third ofa standard deviation). The pattern of scores is consistent across years and does not appear to be diminishing, contrary to other lines of evidence thatshow gender differences in mathematics are small [51].doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.g001

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 231: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender Differences in Mathematics and Science withinthe United States

For the United States, one such program is the National

Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), a federal assessment

of educational achievement. The NAEP is conducted for all states

within the United States and since participation is both

comprehensive and not self-selected, is ideally suited to answering

the question of whether males and females differ in mathematical

ability (a type of quantitative reasoning). Hyde [20] and colleagues

examined gender differences between boys and girls in mathe-

matics from grades 2 through 11, drawing on a sample of students

from ten states which amounted to a sample of over seven million

students. Hyde, et al. [20] reported an effect size for gender

differences in each grade that approached zero, and categorised

differences between males and females as ‘‘trivial’’ [29].

While this evidence seems quite compelling, one must be

cautious about generalising the conclusion of ‘no difference’ in

maths performance on the NAEP to maths performance in all

areas of mathematics. As Hyde and Mertz [29] acknowledge, the

test content of the NAEP does not include complex test items,

making it impossible to investigate gender differences in this area.

Complex and novel mathematical problem-solving is a prerequi-

site skill for success in many academic areas but most particularly

in STEM-related fields. With increased affordability and access to

calculators and computers, basic computation skills have become

less important than the ability to understand complex problems

and find strategies to solve them. A comprehensive meta-analysis

conducted by Hyde, Fennema and Lamon [30] found small to

medium sized differences in complex problem solving favoring

males (d = .29). Assessment that includes these types of mathemat-

ical problems, therefore, should presumably show larger gender

differences and might not necessarily support the gender

similarities hypothesis. Evidence from the NAEP may exhibit a

ceiling effect, as test content hasn’t adequately provided the

opportunity for differentiation between high and low ability levels

in complex reasoning. This would make the distribution of scores

largely homogenous, preventing us from adequately testing the

gender differences/gender similarities hypothesis.

International Sampling of Science and MathematicalAbility

Another source of evidence for evaluating claims of gender

differences comes from international testing of students’ educa-

tional attainments as part of the OECD’s Programme for

International Assessment (PISA). Beginning in 2000 and conduct-

ed every three years, participating nations assess the educational

attainment of students using a standardized exam that allows their

performance to be compared globally. PISA aims to assess the

educational progress of students as they reach the end of

compulsory education, at age 15, across three skill areas: these

being reading literacy, mathematical literacy, and science literacy.

Samples are stratified random probability samples, selected from a

range of public and private institutions across geographical

regions, and weighted so as to be nationally representative [31].

This overcomes the selection-bias of tests such as the SAT-M

[24,26], as well as providing a more valid assessment of the general

population of boys and girls at that age than could be found in

college-bound students.

Additionally, the test content of PISA is somewhat different to

that of other national testing assessments, such as the NAEP. PISA

assesses both knowledge and problem-solving skills, reflecting the

type of real-world content and skills required to be an informed

and capable information consumer and citizen. It assesses a

student’s reading, mathematical and scientific literacy, their ability

to solve problems and to apply their knowledge and skills across

each of these three domains. This is in contrast to tests that require

primarily memory of learned material from the curriculum,

allowing for greater differentiation between high and low ability

levels. As such, it taps higher level cognitive skills than may be

found in testing schemes like NAEP, which Hyde and colleagues

have reported show small or trivial gender differences in science

and mathematics [20]. The test content is sufficiently demanding

that only 1.9% of US students are classified as attaining the highest

proficiency level in mathematics, and only 1.3% of US students in

science. While this makes it ideal for testing for gender differences

or similarities within a given country such as the US, it also affords

the opportunity to study them cross-culturally.

Cross-national Variation in Cognitive AbilitiesCross-national variation in the magnitude of gender differences

can provide useful information about the environmental condi-

tions that foster, or inhibit, gender differences in domains such as

mathematics. While gender differences in mathematics are

frequently found at a national level, they are not found universally

across all nations [32]. Social roles for women vary greatly from

culture to culture, with some cultures promoting higher standards

of gender equality and access to education than others [33]. Even

those nations that have progressive attitudes towards women may

still have strongly-held cultural stereotypes that narrowly constrain

them [34–38]. Cultural stereotypes that girls and women are less

able than boys and men in mathematics and science still endure

[39–40], and these stereotypes have damaging consequences for

the self-efficacy of young girls [41].

Cross-cultural comparisons of the performance of males and

females might help answer some theoretical questions about the

origins of any observed gender differences. When we see consistent

gender differences across many or all nations, and when they are

large enough in magnitude to have a practical impact on the

educational and occupational aspirations of boys and girls, then we

might reasonably conclude some systematic process is responsible

– be this biological or institutional. When we see changes in the

magnitude and the direction of gender differences, as is the case for

science performance reported below, then we might reasonably

conclude that either cultural or environmental influences are

strong moderators in the development of cognitive ability - gender

differences are not an inevitable consequence of biology. Finally, if

we were to see more similarities than differences in the performance

of boys and girls, then this would also be useful information for

shaping public policy and educational practices such as continuing

support for coeducation [42].

A number of previous studies have examined the size of

gender differences in cognitive abilities cross-culturally in an

attempt to shed light on the underlying causes of such variation.

Baker and Jones [43] reported strong correlations between

measures of gender equity (such as percentage of females in

higher education and the occupational status of women in

society) and gender differences in mathematics. Gender differ-

ences in mathematics were smaller in more gender-equal nations

than in less-equal nations. Though the precise mechanism by

which this occurs is unclear, these findings have been replicated

by a number of researchers [31–32,43]. This suggests that two

factors influencing the cognitive abilities of women are the

gender stereotypes that a culture holds, and the gender-roles for

women in a society [29,32]. This has been referred to in the

literature as the gender stratification hypothesis [33,43], which

argues that gender differences are more pronounced when the

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 232: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

roles of men and women are tightly controlled into separate

spheres and duties [35,37,44–45].

Mathematics is not the only cognitive domain where we see an

influence of gender-equality and gender stereotypes on cognitive

performance. The female advantage in reading and language,

while universal, also differs in magnitude between nations. Guiso,

et al. [32] examined data from the PISA 2003 round of testing,

replicating the finding of Baker and Jones for mathematics as well

as finding an association between gender equity and the gender

gap in reading. Although this might be expected given that

correlations between mathematics performance and reading

overlap, the direction of the association differed. Instead of finding

reduced gender differences in reading for countries fostering

greater gender-equality, the gender gap between boys and girls

actually increased. One possibility for this seemingly paradoxical

finding is that whatever natural advantage girls may have for

reading is suppressed in more restrictive countries, but that under

favorable conditions is allowed to flourish to its full potential.

However, further replication of these findings with subsequent

waves of testing is required to determine whether this association is

stable across time.

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA)2009

Cross-cultural evidence of gender differences or similarities

provides a stronger foundation for understanding the role of social

and biological factors in the development of sex differences, as

noted above. The aim of this study was to explore sociocultural

factors that promote, or inhibit, the development of gender gaps in

highly sex-typed academic domains of reading, mathematics and

science [46]. It presents findings from international assessment of

student abilities as part of the Programme for International

Student Assessment (PISA), conducted by the Organisation for

Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). The study

uses data from the most recent round of testing to calculate

national and international gender gaps in reading, mathematics,

and science literacy.

In addition to presenting data on national gender differences, it

uses meta-analytic techniques to calculate global gender differ-

ences to examine evidence for Hyde’s gender similarities hypothesis

[47], which posits there are no meaningful gender differences in

cognitive performance. The study also seeks to replicate the

findings of past researchers for the gender stratification hypothesis

[27,38,43–44], using several measures of gender equity and

occupational segregation. A number of other sociocultural

constructs are also examined to determine the extent to which

gender differences are culturally mediated by factors other than

biology.

One hypothesised influence is the economic prosperity of a

nation [39–41], which reflects two mechanisms. Firstly, greater

economic prosperity allows for a greater proportion of national

resources to be spent on education, resulting in a higher quality of

education and emphasis on skills such as mathematics and science.

Secondly, skills in these technical areas are in greater demand, and

represent a pathway to a higher standard of living. This may result

in greater competition for these occupations, and such competition

may not always be helpful to the career aspirations of women

wishing to enter male-dominated fields. While increases in gender

equity are strongly associated with economic prosperity (and hence

should be associated with smaller gender gaps), these may be

partially offset by increased occupational stratification and

stronger cultural stereotypes associating maths and science with

gender roles [27,32–33,44–45]. Thus increased gender differences

are not purely the result of increased spending on education and

also reflect social processes.

A second mechanism by which gender differences may be

culturally mediated is through the attitudes, values, and beliefs of a

nation. While beliefs about the role of women in society vary

considerably from nation to nation, there are few instruments

available that have wide global coverage of gender stereotypes and

attitudes towards women [38,48–49]. One of most widely used

cultural instruments is Hofestede’s [50] five cultural dimensions.

One of these is theoretically relevant to cultural mediation of

gender differences in cognitive ability, the dimension of power

distance.

The dimension power distance describes the ways in which

societies address the issue of human inequality, and the ways in

which social groups are segregated [50]. In a lower power distance

culture, there are reduced distinctions between social classes,

between employees and employers, between students and teachers,

and between genders. Higher power distance cultures have greater

social division, and a compensatory strategy for those who are

lower in power is to acquire culturally valued skills through

education. Girls may have increased motivation to learn maths

and science and pursue higher status occupations as a way of

overcoming social inequity.

HypothesesBased on prior research and theoretical perspectives, it was

hypothesised that:

1) Gender differences in the domains of mathematics, and

science would be found for the United States, and these

would be larger than those reported by Hyde [51]. These

would reflect gender stereotypes associating these domains

with masculinity and males [39]. However gender differ-

ences cross-culturally would be much smaller, in partial

support of a global gender similarities hypothesis.

2) Gender differences in reading performance in favor of girls

would be found in reading for the United States and cross-

culturally, reflecting an inherent biological disposition that is

only weakly influenced by cultural environment.

3) Measures of national gender equity would be associated with

smaller gender gaps in mathematics and science, in support

of the gender stratification hypothesis. Furthermore, in-

creased gender equity would be weakly associated with

wider reading gaps in favor of girls.

4) Economic prosperity would be associated with wider gender

gaps in mathematics and science than in less prosperous

nations, reflecting increased spending on education, in-

creased demand for these skills, and heightened competition

by males. Such competition may not be helpful to the career

aspirations of women, but will not influence reading

performance which is less malleable to social and cultural

influences.

5) Countries that score highly on Hofestede’s power distance

dimension have greater segregation and foster inequalities,

particularly for women. A compensatory strategy for women

is to acquire culturally-valued skills such as science and

mathematics. High power distance nations would be

associated with smaller gender gaps or a slight female

advantage in these domains. Boys may have increased

motivation to develop reading and writing proficiency in

high power distance cultures, resulting in smaller gender

gaps for reading literacy.

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 233: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Methods

ParticipantsPerformance data for students accessed under PISA is offered as

a publicly accessible archive for researchers. Additionally,

aggregate national performance profiles are published as separate

male and female subgroups [31], which were used for analysis.

PISA 2009 included 34 OECD countries, as well as 31 additional

partner nations. This amounts to a total participant size of 480,405

students (50.6% female) drawn from across 65 nations. This

represents the most recent round of testing, as well as providing

performance data for a broader range of nations than earlier PISA

assessments.

AnalysisNational performance profiles in reading, mathematics and

science literacy were obtained from OECD [31], which reports the

assessment of boys and girls separately. Because of the large

sample sizes involved in national testing, even slight or trivial

differences between boys and girls may be deemed statistically

significant, even though it may have no practical significance. For

this reason, an effect size is presented in the form of Cohen’s d, the

mean standardized difference. This allows the reader to draw his

or her own conclusions as to the practical significance of reported

gender differences.

The computation is calculated as the mean difference between

male and female scores, divided by the pooled within-gender

standard deviation. By convention, female scores are subtracted

from male scores, so that a positive d indicates higher scores for

males while a negative d reflects higher scores for females. This

convention is observed for readability reasons only, and the

interested reader may choose to rephrase the equations so that

male scores are subtracted from female scores simply by inverting

the sign of any effect size given.

Conventional criteria for labelling effect sizes as ‘‘small’’,

‘‘medium’’, or ‘‘large’’ have many limitations and should be used

with great caution [52–53]. Cohen [53] offered a rule of thumb

that an effect size of d #.20 could be considered a ‘‘small’’ effect

for the purpose of estimating statistical power, and that many

legitimate psychological phenomena studied are in fact small

effects. The label of small is perhaps an unfortunate one as some

researchers have mistakenly taken small to be of no practical

significance, a practice Rosenthal and Rubin [54] caution against.

However Hyde, et al. [20] have argued that effect sizes as small as

d = .04 should be regarded as trivial, a cut-off which seems sound

practice. Hyde [47] has also suggested that d #. 10 should be

actually be regarded ‘‘as close to zero’’ (p.581), a cut-off which is

overly conservative and dismisses what are legitimate, albeit very

small, between-group differences. Accordingly, Cohen’s conven-

tions for labelling are followed for reporting. Additionally, gender

differences are presented using Rosenthal and Rubin’s [54–55]

Binomial Effect Size Display (BESD) which presents results in a

metric that represents effect size in a format suitable for

interpretation by non-statisticians [56].

In order to test the gender similarities hypothesis, national

gender gaps in reading, mathematics, and science were combined

using meta-analysis. Comprehensive Meta Analysis (CMA) V2

software was used for the calculation of statistics [57]. A random-

effects model was chosen [58] due to the high degree of cross-

cultural variability, which would make a fixed-effects model

unsuitable [56,59]. Such a method is more conservative in

estimating error terms and produces wider confidence intervals,

giving us greater assurance that the true effect size falls within this

range.

Favreau [60] argues against the use of null hypothesis testing for

evaluating claims of gender difference because it may be overly

sensitive, and does not present a clear picture of how differences

are distributed across groups. Accordingly, data is presented

showing high and low-achievers, as well as effect sizes. Even when

a mean gender difference may be regarded as ‘small’ by Cohen’s

[53] conventions, or ‘trivial’ by Hyde [47], a more pronounced

difference may be found at the tails of a distribution in high and

low-achieving students, resulting in quite disparate educational

outcomes.

Moderation effects of sociocultural factors were examined to test

the gender stratification hypothesis for national gender gaps using

correlational analysis. Although past researchers [32,61] have

examined the gender stratification hypothesis for mathematics and

reading, exploration of the relationship with science has gone

largely untested. Multiple measures of gender equity were used, as

each instrument operationalises the construct of gender equity

differently, and prior research has shown that they vary in their

predictive validity for educational and social outcomes. Other

moderators tested include economic prosperity, as measured by

GDP, and Hofstede’s power distance dimension.

Gender gap index. For comparability with Guiso, et al.’s

findings, the Gender Gap Index (GGI) produced by the World

Economic Forum was selected as one measure of gender equity

[62]. Data for the calendar year of PISA testing was used. This

measure assesses four areas: economic participation, educational

attainment, political empowerment, and health and survival.

While the first three are theoretical relevant to the gender

stratification hypothesis, health and survival (which measures

differences in male and female life expectancy, as well as sex ratio)

may reflect other - largely biological – factors, thus lowering

predictive validity of this measure. An additional criticism of this

measure is that the economic participation component emphasises

male to female participation across various sectors, but gives less

emphasis to income disparities.

Relative status of women. As an alternative conceptualisa-

tion of gender equity, the Relative Status of Women (RSW)

measures gender differences across educational attainment, life

expectancy, and women’s share of income [63]. This reflects a

stronger economic and educational component in estimation of

gender stratification, with wage inequality playing a greater

weighting.

Women in research. Else-Quest, et al. [61] argued that

domain-specific indicators of gender equity may play an important

role in the development of gender differences, with those related to

gender stratification in educational outcomes showing strong

predictive validity. One such marker is the relative share of

research positions held by women. Data for this measure was

obtained from the UNESCO Institute for Statistics, and supple-

mented by data from the National Science Foundation and

Statistics Canada. Data was selected for the calendar year 2009

when possible, or earlier if not available. Women’s relative share of

research positions was available for forty one nations.

Gross domestic product (GDP). Economic data was

obtained from the World Economic Outlook database produces

by the International Monetary Fund. Archived information for the

calendar year 2009 was obtained for sixty-one nations.

Hofstede’s power distance index. National power distance

scores are published in Hofstede’s text ‘‘Culture’s consequences’’ [50],

which ranks nations across this dimension. Data was unavailable

however for many of the non-OECD partner nations, and several

European countries, and was supplemented by national profiles

published online (http://geert-hofstede.com). This provided

coverage of fifty two nations.

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 234: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Statistical PowerWhile the sample size represented by the PISA 2009 was

extremely large, when examining gender differences at the country

level (n = 65) for correlation analysis the sample size is relatively

small. Additionally, data for gender equity measures and for

Hofstede’s cultural dimension of power distance was unavailable

for many non-OECD nations reducing sample size even further.

With a reduced sample size, correlations may lack sufficient power

to detect relationships that are relatively weak in nature [53].

Given that hypotheses were directional (e.g. greater gender

equality would be associated with a reduced gender gap in

mathematics), a decision to make correlation tests one-tailed would

often have allowed such a correlation to be deemed statistically

significant (as probability values are halved). For this reason exact

probability values are given, along with the size of the correlation

coefficient, so that the reader can decide whether to make the

appropriate adjustment. All tests report two-tailed correlations

unless otherwise specified. Data for one nation, Colombia,

represented both a univariate and multivariate outlier, and was

excluded from all correlational analysis.

Results

Although assessing qualitatively different abilities, there was a

strong overlap between national gender differences in reading,

mathematics and science. The quantitative abilities of mathemat-

ics and science showed the greatest overlap. Table 1 presents

intercorrelations between national gender differences in these

domains, while Table 2 gives correlations between national

predictor variables. Tables 3 and 4 present national sample size

and calculated effect sizes across the three domains for OECD and

partner nations respectively.

Reading LiteracyTable 5 presents summary statistics for reading achievement.

Within the United States, girls outperformed boys in overall

reading, Cohen’s d = 2.26 which is just over a quarter of a

standard deviation. By comparison, the OECD gender difference

in reading was larger, d = 2.42. Examining performance data for

the US sample further, boys were overrepresented at the lowest

level of reading proficiency, with approximately 4.5 boys to every

girl. When we consider the vocational and economic outcomes

associated with poor literacy, such a large disparity is alarming.

Such findings are consistent with previous findings on reading

literacy assessed by PISA [32] and gender differences in the

prevalence of reading difficulties [64–65]. When we look at

students attaining the highest level of reading proficiency (Level 6),

the trend is reversed with over twice the number of girls than boys

achieving the highest standard. Thus boys are overrepresented at

the lower end of the spectrum, while girls are overrepresented at

the highest end.

Overall, across all sixty-five nations the gender difference in

reading literacy favored girls, d = 2.44 [95%CI = 2.41, 2.46],

Zma = –31.04, p,.001, with a similar gender difference also being

found for OECD nations only as a group. Additionally, statistically

significant gender differences in reading favoring girls were found

in every nation surveyed, and have since the first assessment in 2000

[66]. These effect sizes ranged from 2.11 to 2.68, from a small- to

a medium- sized difference in reading literacy.

To investigate the gender stratification hypothesis, I examined

correlations between gender equity and the gender gap in reading.

Partial support was found for the gender stratification hypothesis.

National scores on the Relative Status of Women (RSW) measure

were negatively correlated with reading, r = 2.33, p = .018, such

that increased gender equality was associated with larger reading

gaps favoring females. Additionally, the educational measure of

women in research (WIR) was associated with larger reading gaps,

r = 2.38, p = .016. Surprisingly though, there was no association

between the gender gap index (GGI) and reading ability, r = 01.

Examination of the scatterplot showed no discernable pattern, and

the result was not driven by outliers.

Stronger support for the gender stratification hypothesis was

found when examining gender differences in the percentage of

students attaining the highest level of reading. Improvements in

national gender equity was associated with a wider gender gap in

high achieving girls, RSW, r = 2.32, p = .021; GGI, r = 2.41,

p = .002, which is consistent with the findings of Guiso, et al. [32].

Somewhat surprisingly, however, the educational measure of

gender equity showed a strong positive association, with increases

in the percentage of women in research associated with smaller

gender gaps, r = .57, p,.001. While the role of women in higher

education may make a contribution to the mean performance of

girls and boys in basic reading literacy, it may be the case that for

high-achieving reading comprehension skills, boys and girls benefit

equally from female role-models in higher learning.

No association between GDP and gender differences in reading

was found, r = .04, consistent with predictions. However, a strong

association with economic prosperity was found for reading high

achievers, r = 2.43, p,.001 with a greater ratio of female to male

high achievers as GDP increased. This suggests an interaction

between gender and GDP, with girls benefiting more from

economic prosperity than boys. Furthermore, while no association

was found between power distance and mean reading literacy

scores of boys and girls, a strong positive association with the

gender gap in high achievers was found as hypothesized, r = .40,

p = .003 with gender ratios approaching more equal representation

as power distance increased. Cultural mediation through econom-

ic prosperity and power distance was not found for mean male and

female performance, only for gender ratios in high achievement.

Mathematics LiteracyTable 6 presents summary statistics for mathematics literacy.

Within the United States, boys scored higher on mathematical

literacy than girls, d = .22 which is a small but non-trivial effect

size. Additionally, the size of the gender differences was almost

twice that of the OECD average. This is in contrast to previous

studies examining national mathematics performance by Hyde,

et al. [20] which had found a gender gap that approached zero. At

the lower end of ability level for the US sample, the difference in

prevalence between girls and boys was extremely slight; however

at the highest ability levels there were just over twice as many boys

than girls reaching this proficiency level.

Table 1. Correlations between National Gender Differencesfor PISA Reading, Mathematics, and Science Performance (AllNations).

Reading Mathematics Science

Reading 1.00 .75*** .78***

Mathematics 1.00 .81***

Science 1.00

*p,.05,**p,.01,***p,.001.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t001

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 235: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

As the distribution of gender differences differed somewhat

between OECD and partner nations, they are reported separately.

Overall, across all 34 OECD nations, there was a significant

gender difference favoring males on mathematical literacy,

Cohen’s d = .13 95%CI [.11,.15], Zma = 11.22, p,.001. While

this is a small effect size, it does exceed the criteria set forth by

Hyde and Linn [51] for trivial gender differences. Gender

differences across PISA partner nations also favored males,

Cohen’s d = .07 95%CI [.02,.11], Zma = 3.10, p = .001 although

this difference was somewhat smaller.

While statistically significant differences were found in most

countries, they showed considerable variability ranging from

d = 2.12 to d = .43 (see Figure 2). For many nations the gender gap

is negligible, while others show small to medium sized differences.

Additionally the direction of the gender gap was sometimes

reversed, with girls outperforming boys in many nations. Under

different social and educational environments, a gender advantage

supporting either males or females emerges. This would be

inconsistent with Hyde’s [47] gender similarities hypothesis; rather,

gender differences or similarities in mathematics are strongly

mediated by cultural factors.

To explore the gender stratification hypothesis, correlations

between gender equity measures and the gender gap in maths

were examined. As hypothesized there was a strong negative

relationship between the educational measure of women in

research and the gender gap in mathematics, r = 2.38, p = .014.

Greater representation of women in research was associated with

smaller gender gaps or a female advantage, consistent with the

findings of Else-Quest, et al. [61]. However, only a weak

association was found between gender equity measure of RSW,

r = 2.14, and no association was found between GGI and maths,

in contrast to the findings of Guiso, et al. [32].

Since the PISA 2009 dataset includes a much broader range of

partner nations than was examined by Guiso, et al. [32], the

strength of the gender equity association may have been obscured

by additional noise reflecting developed/developing nationhood.

When restricting analysis to OECD nations only, the hypothesized

gender equity association was found for the relative status of

women (RSW) measure, r = 2.42, p = .020, as well as a weak

association with GGI, r = 2.21 that fell short of statistical

significance. While gender equity plays an important role in the

development of gender differences in mathematical literacy for

developed nations, it may be the case that there are more

proximate needs for girls in developing nations (such as access to

schooling, parental support, freedom from work and home duties)

that these gender equity measures do not assess.

A similar pattern of associations was found for gender

differences in high achieving mathematics students across all

nations. There was a strong association between women in

research educational measure, r = 2.63, p,.001, with increased

representation of women in research positions associated with a

smaller gender difference in high achievers approaching zero (see

Figure 3). However no association was found between the gender

gap in high achievers and other gender equity measures, nor was

this found when restricting to OECD nations only.

Support was also found for the economic prosperity hypothesis.

Mean gender differences in mathematics literacy were larger in

more economically prosperous nations, r = .31, p = .015. This

relationship was stronger for high achievement, r = .53, p,.001

with a greater number of males attaining this level of proficiency.

Examining the relationship between Hofsetede’s power distance

cultural dimension and mathematics literacy, support was also

found for cultural mediation. There was a strong negative

relationship between power distance and mean gender differences

in mathematics, r = 2.28, p = .044, as well as for gender ratios in

high achievement, r = .233, p = .019. Gender differences were

smaller in nations with greater tolerance for inequality, suggesting

a compensatory strategy to acquire culturally and economically

valued skills in mathematics.

Science LiteracyTable 7 presents summary statistics for science literacy

achievement scores. For the United States, a gender difference

of d = .14 was found. Furthermore, the United States showed the

largest gender difference across all OECD countries. Although

statistically significant, the difference between the average boy and

girl is small, but neither is it of a trivial magnitude either. Boys in

the US scored higher than boys internationally, while girls scored

lower than their international peers. Additionally, at both ends of

the ability level spectrum, gender differences were more

pronounced – there are approximately 1.5 boys to every girl

achieving the highest level of science proficiency. Thus while the

mean difference between males and females may be ‘‘small’’ by

Cohen’s [53] effect size conventions, it may have more of an

impact than one might assume from that label.

In contrast to US performance, across OECD countries there

was no difference between boys and girls, d = .00 95%CI

[2.03,.03], Zma = 0.10, p = .919. However there was a large

degree of cultural variability, with gender differences favoring

both boys and girls. Indeed, statistically significant differences in

favor of boys were only found in nine countries, and only three

were higher than the ‘close-to-zero’ criterion suggested by Hyde

of d ,.10. Figure 4 shows mean standardized effect sizes.

(Cohen’s d) for gender-gaps in science across OECD nations.

Table 2. Correlations between Measures of Gender Equity, Economic Prosperity, and Hofestede’s Power Distance Index.

Gender Gap Index(GGI)

Relative Status ofWomen (RSW)

Relative Share of Womenin Research (WIR)

Gross Domestic Product(GDP) per capita, 2009

Hofstede’s PowerDistance Index (PDI)

GGI 1.00 .43** 2.09 .43** 2.59***

RSW 1.00 2.11 .05 2.38*

WIR 1.00 2.60*** .42**

GDP 1.00 2.58***

PDI 1.00

*p,.05,**p,.01,***p,.001.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t002

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 236: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Gender similarities, rather than differences, were the norm which

is consistent with the findings of Hyde and Linn [51]. Somewhat

surprisingly, there were also five nations where girls outperformed

boys to a statistically significant degree (the largest being Finland,

d = 2.17). One of the advantages of cross-cultural comparisons in

national testing is that it highlights just how powerfully cultural

and environmental influences can be in either promoting - or

inhibiting - the cognitive development and learning of a child.

A markedly different picture of gender differences in science can

be found across the 31 non-OECD nations. In general, females

scored higher in science literacy than males across most nations.

Overall, across non-OECD nations surveyed there was a

statistically significant difference in science literacy favoring girls,

d = 2.09 95%CI [2.14, 2.04], Zma = 23.44, p = .001. For some

nations, the gender difference was trivial or favored boys, but these

were the exception; this is in contrast to the gender similarities in

science noted above for OECD nations.

When both OECD and non-OECD nations were combined,

there was a statistically significant difference in favor of girls,

d = 2.04 [95%CI2.070,2.013] Zma = 22.84, p = .005. This effect

size would fall into the trivial size by Hyde’s [67] conventions, but

a focus on the combined sample overlooks the pattern of gender

differences at a national level where girls show small but

meaningful gains over boys in science literacy across large parts

of the world. Given that women are underrepresented in science,

particularly in the United States [68] such findings call into

question the validity of cultural stereotypes that associate science

Table 3. National Gender Differences in Reading,Mathematics, and Science Literacy for Countries within theOECD.

Sample size Effect sizes (Cohen’s d)

Country Males Females Reading Mathematics Science

Australia 7020 7231 20.37 0.11 20.01

Austria 3252 3338 20.41 0.20 0.08

Belgium 4345 4156 20.27 0.21 0.06

Canada 11431 11776 20.38 0.14 0.05

Chile 2870 2799 20.27 0.26 0.11

Czech Republic 3115 2949 20.53 0.05 20.05

Denmark 2886 3038 20.34 0.19 0.13

Estonia 2430 2297 20.53 0.11 20.01

Finland 2856 2954 20.64 0.03 20.17

France 2087 2211 20.38 0.16 0.03

Germany 2545 2434 20.42 0.16 0.05

Greece 2412 2557 20.50 0.15 20.11

Hungary 2294 2311 20.42 0.13 0.00

Iceland 1792 1854 20.46 0.04 0.02

Ireland 1973 1964 20.41 0.09 20.03

Israel 2648 3113 20.38 0.08 20.03

Italy 15696 15209 20.48 0.16 20.02

Japan 3126 2962 20.39 0.10 20.12

Korea 2590 2399 20.45 0.04 20.03

Luxembourg 2319 2303 20.38 0.20 0.07

Mexico 18209 20041 20.29 0.17 0.08

Netherlands 2348 2412 20.27 0.19 0.04

New Zealand 2396 2247 20.44 0.08 20.06

Norway 2375 2285 20.52 0.06 20.04

Poland 2443 2474 20.56 0.04 20.07

Portugal 3020 3278 20.44 0.13 20.04

Slovak Republic 2238 2317 20.57 0.03 20.01

Slovenia 3333 2822 20.60 0.01 20.15

Spain 13141 12746 20.33 0.21 0.08

Sweden 2311 2256 20.46 20.02 20.04

Switzerland 6020 5790 20.42 0.20 0.08

Turkey 2551 2445 20.52 0.12 20.15

United Kingdom 6062 6117 20.26 0.23 0.10

United States 2687 2546 20.26 0.22 0.14

Note: Significant gender differences are highlighted in bold.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t003

Table 4. National Gender Differences in Reading,Mathematics, and Science Literacy for PISA Partner Countries.

Sample size Effect sizes (Cohen’s d)

Country Males Females Reading Mathematics Science

Albania 2321 2275 20.62 20.12 20.33

Argentina 2183 2591 20.34 0.11 20.08

Azerbaijan 2443 2248 20.31 0.13 20.10

Brazil 9101 11026 20.30 0.19 0.04

Bulgaria 2231 2276 20.54 20.04 20.19

Colombia 3711 4210 20.11 0.43 0.26

Croatia 2653 2341 20.58 0.12 20.10

Dubai (UAE) 5554 5313 20.47 0.02 20.26

Hong Kong-China 2257 2280 20.39 0.15 0.03

Indonesia 2534 2602 20.55 20.02 20.13

Jordan 3120 3366 20.63 20.01 20.39

Kazakhstan 2723 2689 20.47 20.01 20.10

Kyrgyzstan 2381 2605 20.54 20.07 20.24

Latvia 2175 2327 20.59 0.02 20.09

Liechtenstein* 181 148 20.39 0.28 0.18

Lithuania 2287 2241 20.68 20.07 20.20

Macao-China 3011 2941 20.45 0.13 20.03

Montenegro 2443 2382 20.57 0.14 20.14

Panama 1936 2033 20.33 0.06 20.02

Peru 3000 2985 20.23 0.20 0.05

Qatar 4510 4568 20.44 20.05 20.25

Romania 2378 2398 20.47 0.04 20.13

Russian Federation 2623 2685 20.50 0.03 20.03

Serbia 2680 2843 20.47 0.13 20.01

Shanghai-China 2528 2587 20.50 20.01 20.01

Singapore 2626 2657 20.32 0.05 20.01

Chinese Taipei 2911 2920 20.43 0.05 20.01

Thailand 2681 3544 20.52 0.05 20.16

Trinidad andTobago

2283 2495 20.51 20.08 20.17

Tunisia 2359 2596 20.37 0.16 0.01

Uruguay 2810 3147 20.42 0.13 20.01

Note: Significant gender differences are highlighted in bold.*Although effect sizes are large, caution must be taken interpreting due tosmall sample size.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t004

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 237: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

with masculinity [69], and highlight the need for further efforts at

challenging these damaging cultural stereotypes.

Examining mean gender differences in science literacy, partial

support for the gender stratification hypothesis was found. There

was a strong correlation between national GGI scores and science,

r = .29, p = .035, with greater gender equity associated with smaller

gender gaps approaching zero. However, only a weak non-

significant association was found for the RSW, r = .14.

Additionally, there was a strong negative correlation between

the percentage of women in research and gender gaps in science,

r = 2.39, p = .011, with increased representation of women being

associated with a stronger female advantage over males in science.

Thus increased gender equity was associated with more equal

science performance, but this was offset by higher female

performance as the share of women in research positions

increased.

Only weak support for the gender stratification hypothesis was

found for gender differences in high achievement in science.

Increased gender equity as measured by the percentage of

researchers who are women was associated with smaller gender

gaps in the number of high achievers, r = 2.57, p,.001 (see

Figure 5). While positive female role models are certainly

important for challenging gender stereotypes about women in

science generally, they may be even more so for encouraging

young women to excel in science and pursue it as a career path. In

contrast to this finding, there was no association between the

relative status of women measure, r = .12 and a slight positive

correlation with gender equity as measured by the GGI, r = .29,

p = .029, with increased gender equity associated with more male

high achievers than female which is contrary to predictions. This

anomalous association may be at least partly explained by the

underlying construct measured by the GGI. It incorporates a

strong economic component in its formula, with a correlation of

r = .43 between national GGI scores and economic productivity as

measured by GDP. When controlling for economic productivity,

the association between GGI and science high achievers becomes

non-significant, r = .12, p = .373.

Strong support was also found for culturally mediation of

gender differences in science. Positive relationships were observed

between GDP and gender differences in mean science scores,

r = .42, p = .001, as well as for gender ratios in high achievement,

r = .27, p = .036, as hypothesised. In contrast, a negative relation-

ship was found between the power-distance dimension and mean

gender differences in science, r = 2.39, p = .005 with gender

differences favoring girls in high power-distance nations. This

effect was even stronger for gender ratios in high achievement,

r = 2.45, p = .001.

Discussion

Does the size of gender differences in reading, mathematics, and

science from PISA assessment merit further research into the social

and cultural factors that promote, or inhibit, differential educa-

tional outcomes for boys and girls? Evidence presented for the

United States shows that there are meaningful gender gaps across

all three domains. Furthermore, they are larger than those found

in most OECD nations placing the US among the highest gender

gaps in mathematics and science in the developed world, but

somewhat smaller than other nations in reading literacy. However,

quite different patterns are found when examining gender gaps

globally. US performance is reviewed first, followed by a

discussion of cross-cultural evidence.

Reading LiteracyWhile a small-to-medium sized gender difference in reading was

found for US students d = 2.26, this was comparatively smaller

than that found in other OECD nations. However, gender

differences were strikingly different at both tails of the distribution,

with boys overrepresented in the lowest level of reading

proficiency and girls overrepresented in the highest. PISA

sampling allows for exclusion of students with limited language

proficiency, so it is likely that this result reflects poorer reading

ability generally rather than male overrepresentation in reading

difficulties students. This pattern is consistent with existing

research on gender ratios for reading difficulties [64–65].

Cross-culturally, a medium sized gender difference (d = 2.44)

was found for reading literacy, which would be inconsistent with

Hyde’s gender similarities hypothesis [47]. Expressed in the BESD

Table 5. Reading Ability for Girls and Boys for the USA and OECD nations.

Girls Boys Standard Deviation Effect Size (d)

United States 513 488 (97) 2.26

OECD Average 513 474 (93) 2.42

% students at lowest ability level, USA 0.2% 0.9% 4.5 boys : 1 girl

% at highest ability level, USA 2.1% 0.9% 2.4 girls : 1 boy

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t005

Table 6. Mean Mathematical Ability for Girls and Boys for the USA and OECD nations.

Girls BoysStandardDeviation Effect Size (d)

United States 477 497 (91) .22

OECD Average 490 501 (92) .12

% students at lowest ability level, USA 9.5% 6.8% 1.40 girls : 1 boy

% at highest ability level, USA 1.2% 2.5% 2.12 boys : 1 girl

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t006

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 238: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

format, the likelihood of being average or higher in reading ability

increases from 39% for boys to 61% for girls. Reading

performance was higher for girls than boys across every nation,

but also showed considerable between-nation variation. Though

the direction of gender differences would be consistent with a

biological explanation, it appears at least partially malleable by

social and cultural factors. While there was no support for cultural

mediation through economic prosperity and power distance in

mean gender differences, contrary to predictions associations were

found for high achievers in reading literacy.

It has been a common research finding that boys are generally

poorer readers and writers than girls [70], and considerable effort

has been made to address the gender gap over recent decades with

focus on early identification and intervention for reading

difficulties. Basic literacy is an essential life skill for all children,

and for full participation as a citizen. While much attention is

given to the issue of math and science gender gaps, gender gaps in

reading are in fact much larger and favor girls at both tails of the

distribution. While gender gaps in reading literacy for the USA

were smaller than those found internationally, the need for further

progress remains. Enrolments of women outnumber men in

college, with higher female GPA and completion rates than their

male peers [16,65–66]. Raising the educational aspirations of boys

who experience difficulties in reading literacy, and continuing

support for early intervention is critical as a matter of gender

equity.

Mathematics LiteracyGender differences in mathematics literacy were comparatively

larger for the United States than those found across other OECD

nations. These findings are consistent with student test data

reported by Hedges and Nowell [23], as well as findings from

PISA 2003 [32,61] that a small gender difference in mathematics

exists, but is also inconsistent with findings of no difference

reported by Hyde and colleagues using data from the NAEP [20].

How are we to reconcile this discrepancy?

As reviewed earlier, problem-solving for complex and novel

mathematics tasks show a small to medium sized male advantage

[30], and PISA assessment of mathematical literacy is somewhat

different to that of the NAEP. This may allow for greater

differentiation between high and low ability students if a ceiling-

effect is present, and may provide a more thorough test of the

gender similarities hypothesis. It may well be the case that gender

differences in basic mathematical literacy are trivial in size [71],

but that gender differences can be found in more complex tasks

[30] requiring more than just curriculum knowledge.

Gender differences were observed for US performance, d = .22,

which is small in size by Cohen’s [53] conventions and non-trivial

by Hyde’s [47] criteria. When expressed in the BESD format, the

likelihood of being average or higher in mathematics increases

from 44.5% for girls to 55.5% for boys. One should be careful not

to make too much, or too little, of this gender difference. As Hyde

[47] points out, the degree of overlap between male and female

performance is large for effect sizes in the small range, with many

girls performing at or above the male average in mathematics.

This perspective does not diminish the observation that a gender

Figure 2. Histogram of gender difference effect sizes in mathematics literacy across OECD nations.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.g002

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 10 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 239: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

gap exists. As can be seen from the cross-cultural evaluation of

mathematics, gender gaps in mathematics are not an inevitability,

with many countries in fact showing higher female performance.

This difference is most apparent when examining student

attainment of the highest proficiency level in mathematics, with

double the amount of boys than girls reaching this stage. Benbow

[22] argued that gender differences in high-achievement for

mathematics could be at least partially explained by greater male

variability and a combination of biological and environmental

factors. It is likely that greater male variability explains at least part

of the gender difference in high achievement, but that sociocul-

tural factors also play a role in the development of mathematics at

the extreme tails of the distribution. While general proficiency in

mathematics is an important life goal for all students, attainment of

an advanced level of mathematics is an important prerequisite for

pursuing more technical degrees in STEM-related fields [72]. A

growing body of research suggests that self-efficacy and confidence

in mathematics play an important part in the decision making

process of women to pursue STEM-related careers or direct their

talents elsewhere [23,62–64]. Increasing self-confidence in math-

ematics and instilling a sense of mastery may be a crucial

component any educational intervention, as well as challenging

negative cultural stereotypes about women’s ability in mathematics

[41,69]. At least for students within the USA, gender differences in

mean and high achievement for mathematics have not been

eliminated, and highlight the need for further progress.

While cross-culturally, gender differences favored males across

OECD and partner nations, the magnitude of this difference

(d = .13) was also small in size and subject to wide cultural

variation. The likelihood of being average or higher in

mathematical ability increases from 46.7% for girls to 53.2%

for boys, a small but non-trivial difference. Unlike reading

Figure 3. Relationship between women in research and gender ratios of high-achievers in mathematics literacy.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.g003

Table 7. US National Science performance for girls and boys, including high and low achievers.

Girls BoysStandardDeviation Effect Size (d)

United States 495 509 (98) .14

OECD Average 501 501 (94) .00

% students at lowest ability level, USA 4.6% 3.8% 1.20 girls : 1 boy

% at highest ability level, USA 1.0% 1.5% 1.52 boys : 1 girl

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.t007

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 11 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 240: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

literacy, there were a number of countries which had non-

significant gender differences, which would be inconsistent with

strong biological differences between boys and girls in mathe-

matical reasoning [11,15,65–66]. It may be the case that

whatever slight advantage boys have is magnified by social and

cultural reinforcement, to produce gender differences in some

countries but that other nations raise girls and boys to

equivalent performance.

A parallel may be also drawn between cross-cultural support

for gender differences in mathematics, and similar evidence for

gender differences in spatial ability [67–70]. Many theorists

have argued that spatial ability provides a foundation for later

development of mathematical ability [13,73–76]. Although

gender differences are consistently found across all cultures

favoring males, the magnitude of spatial differences is subject to

cultural variation. In particular, Lippa, Collaer and Peters [77]

compared national measures of gender equality and economic

development with gender differences in spatial performance for

a fifty-three nation sample, finding strong positive correlations

with both measures. These findings are correlational, not causal,

but taken together may change the way in which we think

about the development of cognitive differences. It would appear

Figure 4. Distribution of effect sizes for gender differences in science literacy across OECD nations.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.g004

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 12 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 241: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

that gender differences in number of cognitive abilities are at

least partially influenced by social and cultural influences such

as gender equality and the status of women [32,61]. While

parental, teacher and peer influences also play a part [78–83],

the influence of wider cultural influences at the macro-level may

be important considerations for any biopsychosocial models of

gender difference.

Science LiteracyWhile the effect size for gender differences in science literacy for

the USA was relatively small compared to that of reading and

mathematics, it stands out as the largest effect size across all

OECD nations, d = .14. This is a small effect size, but also not a

trivial one by Hyde’s [47] conventions. Represented in the BESD

format, the likelihood of being average or higher in science literacy

increases from 46.5% for girls to 53.5% for boys. Additionally,

boys were slightly overrepresented in attaining the highest level of

science proficiency, but not to the same degree as for mathematics.

Of all the domains assessed, science literacy appears to be the most

variable cross-culturally, with many countries showing no differ-

ence whatsoever, and many showing a female advantage. This is a

promising sign, and a benchmark to which the USA can aspire.

This pattern of results was consistent with the gender similarities

hypothesis.

Gender Stratification HypothesisIn order to test the gender stratification hypothesis, this study

examined the relationship between national measures of gender

equity and gender gaps in reading, mathematics and science

literacy. While some support for the gender stratification

hypothesis was found, the predictive validity of gender equity

measures varied across instruments and domains. In particular,

relationships between the Gender Gap Index instrument were

often weak, and in the case of science literacy high achievers in a

direction contrary to hypotheses. This failure to support the

gender stratification hypothesis using all gender equity measures

should not be interpreted as a refutation of the hypothesis, but

means that one should evaluate the hypothesis carefully. Each

instrument taps different aspects of the underlying gender equity

construct, and it is likely that some elements of equity have greater

bearing on educational outcomes than others. A consistent finding

across all three domains, and across both mean performance and

high achievers, was that the relative share of women in research

accurately predicted the presence or absence of gender differences.

Figure 5. Relationship between women in research and gender ratios of high-achievers in science literacy.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.g005

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 13 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 242: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

However, composite measures of gender equity showed weaker or

inconsistent associations.

It may be the case that measures more closely related to

education, such as gender differences in relative share of research

and science positions, may more accurately measure the under-

lying social and cultural conditions that foster or inhibit the

development of gender differences in reading, mathematics and

science literacy. None of the instruments directly measure attitudes

towards women in STEM-related fields, or gender stereotypes

about the relative abilities of males and females [69,84]. Instead,

the composite measures relate to the role of women in society in

general, which may lack the specificity required to consistently

predict gender differences in learning outcomes. Although

increased gender equity generally may be associated with the

presence or absence of gender gaps in reading, mathematics and

science, it may not be the direct cause.

The relative share of women employed in scientific research

may be more directly related to societal attitudes about the role of

women in technical fields, and to gender stereotypes about the

capabilities of males and females in sex-typed achievement

domains. Girls growing up in a society that praises the scientific

and technical achievements of men but lacks equivalent female

role models may perceive that women are less capable in this area,

or that their skills are not culturally valued. They may instead be

motivated to develop other talents, such as high proficiency in

language, and to pursue careers in less-segregated professions.

Conversely, if girls grow up in a social environment where they see

progression into further education and specialisation in STEM-

related fields is not only possible but also commonplace, they may

be more motivated to acquire and master mathematics and science

skills. In such a culture, encouragement from parents and teachers

may be higher, and they may show greater confidence and

improved self-efficacy in these domains than children from other

cultures. While mean gender differences are smaller (or favor

females) in such nations, this also translates to increased female

representation in high achievers as well. This provides for stronger

support of the gender stratification hypothesis.

Economic ProsperityMean gender differences were larger for mathematics and

science in economically prosperous nations as hypothesised but

were largely unrelated to reading literacy. This likely reflects both

increased educational spending for economically prosperous

nations, as well as increased emphasis being placed on mathe-

matics and science skills. Student achievement in less prosperous

nations may be more homogenous with smaller gender differences,

and there may be a reduced focus on teaching of these skills. It

may also be the case that there is greater competition by males to

achieve in these masculine sex-typed domains. These associations

were also found for gender ratios in high achievement. Addition-

ally, gender ratios for high achievers in reading literacy were also

related to economic prosperity, which was unexpected.

Power DistanceHofstede [50] argued that cultures differed in their tolerance for

inequality, with some cultures observing social class distinctions

more strongly than others. Such cultures may place greater

emphasis on social roles and stratification, but one way of

overcoming inequity is the pursuit of culturally valued skills and

traits. As a compensatory strategy, girls may seek out higher social

status positions by obtaining education in mathematics and

science, and this may help to explain the female advantage for

science observed for non-OECD nations. As hypothesised, these

associations were found for mean gender differences in mathe-

matics and science as well as for gender ratios of high achievers.

Lesser support was found for cultural mediation in reading

literacy, with no association for mean gender differences but a

positive association for gender ratios in high achievement.

Social ImplicationsThe question of whether gender differences exist in cognitive

abilities has important implications for parents, educators, and

policy-makers [20,47,72,82–83]. Yet great caution must be taken

when interpreting empirical evidence - Hyde [47] raises a

legitimate concern that inflated claims of wide gender difference

might contribute to increased gender segregation in education and

the workforce, and that the potential of girls may be overlooked by

parents and teachers [78–82]. This study finds evidence of gender

similarities rather than differences cross-culturally but also that

meaningful gender gaps in maths and science remain and are

related to cultural factors.

Society as a whole also has a vested interest in this question,

both directly and indirectly. We as citizens rely on the services and

advancements that a highly skilled science and technology

workforce provide, with direct benefits for our health and lifestyle,

and for an economy that depends on the brightest and most

innovative of minds entering these fields to sustain an interna-

tionally competitive advantage. There are also indirect benefits

from having a society that is at least partially scientifically literate –

making decisions through the political process and personal

choices about issues such as the use of stem-cell technologies,

vaccination of children against disease, or evidence of climate

change. When students, particularly girls, disengage with science

learning there are costs to the individual, in the form of reduced

security and income, but also to the wider society. While not every

child may have the ability or interest to pursue a scientific career, a

basic scientific literacy is required for full participation in society.

The underrepresentation of women in science is a serious social

issue, and considerable resources are being expended to address

this problem [72,83–84]. Recognising that a gender gap exists is

the first step towards changing it, while cross-cultural evidence of

gender similarities provides strong evidence that the gender gaps

in maths and science are not inevitable. STEM-related careers can

be a pathway to a higher standard of living and job security, and

girls deserve the same encouragement as boys to pursue these

professions as a matter of social justice. Newcombe et al. [85]

argues that psychology can make a positive contribution to

changing the social and educational environments that curtail the

potential of all students in mathematics and science.

Strengths and LimitationsThe broader coverage of nations included in the PISA 2009

round of assessment makes for a stronger test of research

hypotheses than was previously possible. Additionally, many of

the partner nations would be categorised as lower in human

development, with reduced access to the educational advantages

found in other nations. While researching educational outcomes

for large and economically prosperous nations like the United

States is important, debate about gender differences is often

shaped by evidence from relatively affluent samples. In less

advantaged nations, provided girls and boys are still afforded the

same access to education, performance in maths and science

literacy is more homogenous giving greater support to the gender

similarities hypothesis. However, there is still substantial cultural

variability in gender differences, and much of this is driven by

cultural variation in gender equality. For a large portion of the

world, the strongest predictor of gender differences in educational

outcomes is equivalent access to education, occupational segrega-

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 14 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 243: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

tion, and representation of women in technical and research

professions. If priority were to be given to improving these

globally, substantial improvements in female literacy in maths and

science could be realised.

While support for research hypotheses were generally observed,

availability of data for cross-cultural correlations meant reduced

statistical power to detect relatively weak correlations. It may well

be the case that the hypothesised associations with mean gender

differences across reading, maths, and science could have been

detected with expanded coverage of Hofstede’s cultural dimen-

sions [50]. There are likely many other cross-cultural correlates of

gender differences that remain unexplored, such as gender

stereotypes about cognitive abilities, and cultural variations in

attitudes towards women in society. Such research is limited by the

need to obtain wide coverage of these constructs across nations.

SummaryEvidence from national testing for the United States shows that

there are meaningful gender gaps to be addressed in academic

achievement across reading, mathematical and science literacy.

Furthermore, these are larger than that found cross-culturally,

where evidence for the gender similarities hypothesis is stronger.

Globally, there is a small gender difference in mathematics literacy

favoring males, and a small difference in science literacy favoring

girls in non-OECD nations. However, a consistent finding for

reading literacy is that girls outperform boys both in mean

differences overall and gender ratios in attaining high reading

achievement. Correlational analyses show that economic prosper-

ity, gender equity, and the dimension of power distance are good

predictors of global gender differences in cognitive abilities.

Author Contributions

Analyzed the data: DR. Wrote the paper: DR.

References

1. Eagly AH (1995) The science and politics of comparing women and men.

American Psychologist 50: 145–158.

2. Halpern DF, Lamay M (2000) The smarter sex: A critical review of sex

differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review 12: 229–246.

3. Caplan PJ, Caplan JB (1997) Do sex-related cognitive differences exist, and why

do people seek them out? In: Caplan PJ, Crawford M, Hyde JS, Richardson

JTE, editors. Gender differences in human cognition. New York: Oxford

University Press. 52–80.

4. Reilly D (2012) Exploring the science behind sex and gender differences in

cognitive abilities. Sex Roles. doi: 10.1007/s11199-012-0134-6.

5. Gardner H (1985) Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences: Basic

books.

6. Horn J (1994) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In: Sternberg RJ,

editor. Encyclopedia of human intelligence. New York: Macmillan. 443–451.

7. Gardner H (1999) Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st

century: Basic Books (AZ).

8. Neisser U (1967) Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

9. Carroll JB (1997) The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities. In: Flanagan

DP, Harrison PL, editors. Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests

and issues. New York: Guilford. 69–76.

10. Sternberg RJ (1994) Encyclopedia of human intelligence. New York:

MacMillan.

11. Neisser UC, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJ Jr, Boykin AW, Brody N, et al. (1996)

Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist 51: 77–101.

12. Hyde JS, Lindberg SM (2007) Facts and assumptions about the nature of gender

differences and the implications for gender equity. In: Klein SS, editor.

Handbook for achieving gender equity through education. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 19–32.

13. Halpern DF (2007) Science, sex, and good sense: why women are

underrepresented in some areas of science and math. In: Ceci SJ, editor. Why

aren’t more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence.

Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. 121–130.

14. Jensen AR (1998) The g factor: the science of mental ability. London: Praeger.

15. Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial

abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological

Bulletin 117: 250–270.

16. Linn MC, Petersen AC (1985) Emergence and characterization of sex differences

in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development 56: 1479–1498.

17. Hyde JS, Linn MC (1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis.

Psychological Bulletin 104: 53–69.

18. Halpern DF (2000) Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

19. Maccoby EE, Jacklin CN (1974) The psychology of sex differences. Stanford:

Stanford University Press.

20. Hyde JS, Lindberg SM, Linn MC, Ellis AB, Williams CC (2008) Gender

similarities characterize math performance. Science 321: 494–495.

21. Sears DO (1986) College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow

data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology 51: 515–530.

22. Benbow CP (1988) Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in

intellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possible causes.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 169–232.

23. Hedges LV, Nowell A (1995) Sex differences in mental test scores, variability,

and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science 269: 41–45.

24. Halpern DF (2011) Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

25. Gallagher AM, Kaufman JC, editors (2005) Gender differences in mathematics.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

26. Spelke ES (2005) Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics andscience?: a critical review. American Psychologist 60: 950–958.

27. Alon S, Gelbgiser D (2011) The female advantage in college academic

achievements and horizontal sex segregation. Social Science Research 40:

107–119.

28. Henry PJ (2008) College sophomores in the laboratory redux: Influences of a

narrow data base on social psychology’s view of the nature of prejudice.

Psychological Inquiry 19: 49–71.

29. Hyde JS, Mertz JE (2009) Gender, culture, and mathematics performance.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 8801–8807.

30. Hyde JS, Fennema E, Lamon SJ (1990) Gender differences in mathematics

performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 107: 139–155.

31. OECD (2010) PISA 2009 Results: What students know and can do - Studentperformance in reading, mathematics and science (Volume I).

32. Guiso L, Monte F, Sapienza P, Zingales L (2008) Culture, gender, and math.

Science 320: 1164–1165.

33. Riegle-Crumb C (2005) The cross-national context of the gender gap in mathand science. In: Hedges LV, Schneider B, editors. The scoial organization of

schooling. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. 227–243.

34. Eccles JS (1987) Gender roles and women’s achievement-related decisions.Psychology of Women Quarterly 11: 135–172.

35. Eagly AH (1987) Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation.

Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

36. Eagly AH (1987) The interpretation of sex differences in social behavior. In:Eagly AH, editor. Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation.

London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

37. Eagly AH, Mitchell AA (2004) Social role theory of sex differences andsimilarities: Implications for the sociopolitical attitudes of men and women. In:

Paludi MA, editor. Praeger guide to the psychology of gender. Westport, Conn.:Praeger Publishers. 183–206.

38. Best DL, Williams JE (1997) Sex, gender, and culture. In: Berry JW, Segall MH,

Kagitcibasi C, editors. Handbook of cross-cultural psychology Volume 3: Social

behavior and applications. 2nd ed. Needleham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.163–212.

39. Halpern DF, Straight CA, Stephenson CL (2011) Beliefs about cognitive gender

differences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for size. Sex Roles 64: 336–347.

40. Eccles JS, Jacobs JE, Harold RD (1990) Gender role stereotypes, expectancy

effects, and parents’ socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues46: 183–201.

41. Steele CM (1997) A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity

and performance. American Psychologist 52: 613–629.

42. Halpern DF, Eliot L, Bigler RS, Fabes RA, Hanish LD, et al. (2011) Thepseudoscience of single-sex schooling. Science 333: 1706–1707.

43. Baker DP, Jones DP (1993) Creating gender equality: Cross-national gender

stratification and mathematical performance. Sociology of Education 66: 91–103.

44. Eagly AH, Wood W (1999) The origins of sex differences in human behavior:

Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist 54: 408–423.

45. Eagly AH, Wood W, Diekman AB (2000) Social role theory of sex differencesand similarities: A current appraisal. The developmental social psychology of

gender: 123–174.

46. Nash SC (1979) Sex role as mediator of intellectual functioning. In: Wittig MA,Petersen AC, editors. Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Develop-

mental issues. New York: Academic Press. 263–302.

47. Hyde JS (2005) The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist 60:

581–592.

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 15 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 244: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

48. Williams JE, Best DL (1990) Measuring sex stereotypes : a multination study.

Newbury Park: Sage Publications.49. Triandis HC (2004) The many dimensions of culture. The Academy of

Management Executive (1993–2005) 18: 88–93.

50. Hofstede GH (2001) Culture’s consequences : Comparing values, behaviors,institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage

Publications.51. Hyde JS, Linn MC (2006) Gender similarities in mathematics and science.

Science 314: 599–600.

52. Hedges LV (2008) What are effect sizes and why do we need them? ChildDevelopment Perspectives 2: 167–171.

53. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale,NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

54. Rosenthal R, Rubin DB (1982) A simple, general purpose display of magnitudeof experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychology 74: 166–169.

55. Rosenthal R, Rubin DB (1982) Further meta-analytic procedures for assessing

cognitive gender differences. Journal of Educational Psychology 74: 708–712.56. Rosenthal R, DiMatteo MR (2001) Meta-analysis: Recent developments in

quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology 52:59–82.

57. Borenstein M, Rothstein HR (1999) Comprehensive meta-analysis: A computer

program for research synthesis. Englewood, NJ: BioStat.58. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR (2009) Introduction to

Meta-Analysis. West Sussex. UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.59. Field AP (2001) Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo

comparison of fixed-and random-effects methods. Psychological methods 6:161–180.

60. Favreau OE (1997) Sex and gender comparisons: Does null hypothesis testing

create a false dichotomy? Feminism & Psychology 7: 63–81.61. Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Linn MC (2010) Cross-national patterns of gender

differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 136: 103–127.

62. World Economic Forum (2009) The Global Gender Gap Report 2009.

63. Dijkstra AG, Hanmer LC (2000) Measuring socio-economic gender inequality:Toward an alternative to the UNDP Gender-Related Development Index.

Feminist Economics 6: 41–75.64. Hawke JL, Olson RK, Willcut EG, Wadsworth SJ, DeFries JC (2009) Gender

ratios for reading difficulties. Dyslexia 15: 239–242.65. Shaywitz SE, Escobar MD, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Makuch R (1992)

Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of

reading ability. New England Journal of Medicine 326: 145–150.66. OECD (2010) PISA 2009 Results: Learning trends: Changes in student

performance since 2000 (Volume V).67. Hyde JS (2007) New directions in the study of gender similarities and differences.

Current Directions in Psychological Science 16: 259–263.

68. National Science Foundation (2011) Women, minorities, and persons withdisabilities in science and engineering: 2011. Arlington, VA: National Science

Foundation.69. Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG (2002) Math = male, me = female,

therefore math? me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83: 44–59.

70. Dwyer CA (1973) Sex differences in reading: An evaluation and a critique of

current theories. Review of Educational Research 43: 455–467.71. Hyde JS, Fennema E, Ryan M, Frost LA, Hopp C (1990) Gender comparisons

of mathematics attitudes and affect. Psychology of Women Quarterly 14: 299–

324.72. Hanson SL, Schaub M, Baker DP (1996) Gender stratification in the science

pipeline: A comparative analysis of seven countries. Gender and Society 10:271–290.

73. Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP (2009) Spatial ability for STEM domains:

Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies itsimportance. Journal of Educational Psychology 101: 817–835.

74. Nuttall RL, Casey MB, Pezaris E (2005) Spatial ability as a mediator of genderdifferences on mathematics tests: A biological-environmental framework. In:

Gallagher AM, Kaufman JC, editors. Gender differences in mathematics: Anintegrative psychological approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University

Pres. 121–142.

75. Casey MB, Nuttall R, Pezaris E, Benbow CP (1995) The influence of spatialability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across

diverse samples. Developmental Psychology 31: 697–705.76. Geary DC, Saults SJ, Liu F, Hoard MK (2000) Sex differences in spatial

cognition, computational fluency, and arithmetical reasoning. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology 77: 337–353.77. Lippa R, Collaer M, Peters M (2010) Sex differences in mental rotation and line-

angle judgments are positively associated with gender equality and economicdevelopment across 53 nations. Archives of Sexual Behavior 39: 990–997.

78. Eccles JS, Blumenfeld P (1985) Classroom experiences and student gender: Arethere differences and do they matter? In: Wilkinson L, Marrett CB, editors.

Gender influences in classroom interaction. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. 79–

114.79. Eccles JS, Wigfield A, Harold RD, Blumenfeld P (1993) Age and gender

differences in children’s self- and task perceptions during elementary school.Child Development 64: 830–847.

80. Frome PM, Eccles JS (1998) Parents’ influence on children’s achievement-

related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 435.81. Furnham A, Hosoe T, Tang T (2001) Male hubris and female humility? A cross-

cultural study of ratings of self, parental, and sibling multiple intelligence inAmerica, Britain, and Japan. Intelligence 30: 101–115.

82. Furnham A, Petrides K (2004) Parental estimates of five types of intelligence.Australian Journal of Psychology 56: 10–17.

83. Furnham A, Reeves E, Budhani S (2002) Parents think their sons are brighter

than their daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimations and estimationsof their children’s multiple intelligences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 163:

24–39.84. Nosek BA, Smyth FL, Sriram N, Lindner NM, Devos T, et al. (2009) National

differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in

science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy ofSciences 106: 10593–10597.

85. Newcombe NS, Ambady N, Eccles JS, Gomez L, Klahr D, et al. (2009)Psychology’s role in mathematics and science education. American Psychologist

64: 538.

Gender, Culture and Sex-Typed Cognitive Abilities

PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 16 July 2012 | Volume 7 | Issue 7 | e39904

Page 245: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 136

Chapter 7 – Meta-Analysis of Sex-Role Mediation Effect for Visual-Spatial Ability

This study reports a meta-analysis of the sex-role mediation effect for visual-

spatial ability. This chapter includes a co-authored paper that has been published as :

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-

analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521-535. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, Springer.

In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship of this paper. I

acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann

Page 246: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

1 23

Sex RolesA Journal of Research ISSN 0360-0025 Sex RolesDOI 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Gender-Role Differences in Spatial Ability:A Meta-Analytic Review

David Reilly & David L. Neumann

Page 247: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

1 23

Your article is protected by copyright and all

rights are held exclusively by Springer Science

+Business Media New York. This e-offprint is

for personal use only and shall not be self-

archived in electronic repositories. If you

wish to self-archive your work, please use the

accepted author’s version for posting to your

own website or your institution’s repository.

You may further deposit the accepted author’s

version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s

request, provided it is not made publicly

available until 12 months after publication.

Page 248: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Gender-Role Differences in Spatial Ability: A Meta-AnalyticReview

David Reilly & David L. Neumann

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Abstract Although gender-related differences in highlygender typed cognitive abilities are of considerable interestto educators and cognitive researchers alike, relatively littleprogress has been made in understanding the psychologicalprocesses that lead to them. Nash (1979) proposed a gender-role mediation hypothesis for such differences, with partic-ular emphasis on spatial ability. However, changes in genderequality and gender stereotypes in the decades since merit are-examination of whether a gender-role association stillholds (Feingold 1988). A meta-analysis of 12 studies thatexamined gender-role identity and mental rotation per-formance was conducted. These included studies fromthe United Kingdom, Canada, Poland, Croatia, and theUnited States of America. The mean effect size formasculinity was r = .30 for men and r = .23 forwomen; no association was found between femininityand mental rotation. This effect size was slightly largerthan that found previously by Signorella and Jamison(1986), and exceeds many other factors known to influ-ence spatial ability. The implications of gender-role me-diation of gender differences are discussed and futureresearch directions are identified.

Keywords Gender differences . Spatial ability . Gender-rolemediation . Gender roles . Mental rotation . Meta-analysis

Introduction

Though progress has been made in closing gaps in recentdecades, women still remain underrepresented in science, tech-nology, engineering and mathematics (STEM)-related fields inthe United States with fewer women entering these fields intertiary education (National Science Foundation 2011).Concerns about the underrepresentation of women are alsopresent in many other countries, including Britain (Brosnan1998) and Australia (Bell 2010). Although exceptions exist forpsychology and medical sciences (Hyde 2007b), in generalwomen are underrepresented in the sciences at a graduate level,as well scoring lower in tests of mathematics and scienceachievement at school within the U.S. (Gallagher andKaufman 2005; Hedges and Nowell 1995). These findingsare also supported by more recent reviews of mathematicsand science literacy in large international assessments of stu-dent achievement such as the Programme for InternationalStudent Achievement (Else-Quest et al. 2010; Guiso et al.2008; Reilly 2012), which assesses students worldwide as theyreach the end of compulsory schooling. Much of the researchin this area, however, draws on samples from America, and allstudies cited herein are U.S.-based unless otherwise noted.

A consensus statement issued by major researchers in thearea of gender-related cognitive differences identified re-search into the sources of individual differences in STEMachievement as an important priority (Halpern et al. 2007).When men and women are compared at the populationlevel, reviews find no evidence of gender differences ingeneral intelligence (Halpern and Lamay 2000; Neisser etal. 1996). However, researchers have frequently observedgender differences in more specific components of cognitiveability (Boyle et al. 2010a, b; Neumann et al. 2007, 2010).The size of such differences ranges from small to large, as afunction of the cognitive component under investigation(Halpern et al. 2011). The largest and most consistent genderdifferences are found in spatial ability (Halpern 2011; Kimura

D. Reilly (*) :D. L. NeumannSchool of Applied Psychology, Griffith University,Southport, Queensland 4222, Australiae-mail: [email protected]

D. L. NeumannBehavioural Basis of Health Program, Griffith Health Institute,Queensland, Australiae-mail: [email protected]

Sex RolesDOI 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Author's personal copy

Page 249: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

2000; Maccoby and Jacklin 1974), where reviews find effectsizes ranging frommedium to large (Linn and Petersen 1985;Voyer et al. 1995). Gender differences in spatial abilityare also found cross-culturally in large internationalstudies with young-adult samples (Peters et al. 2006;Silverman et al. 2007).

The present review explores one such contribution to thedevelopment of spatial ability, that of gender roles. Thisterm has been previously referred to in the literature as sexroles (Bem 1981; Constantinople 1973), but the term genderroles is preferred as it is broader and encompasses sociocul-tural factors as well as biological explanations for observeddifferences (Frieze and Chrisler 2011). Relationships be-tween spatial ability, quantitative reasoning and gender rolesare discussed before reviewing empirical support forgender-role associations with spatial ability. All studies citedherein are U.S.-based unless otherwise noted.

Spatial Ability and Quantitative Skills

Many researchers (e.g. Wai et al. 2009) have proposed thatspatial ability provides a foundation for the development ofquantitative reasoning such as science and mathematics(Nuttall et al. 2005; Serbin et al. 1990). Factor analyses ofcognitive ability tests show high loadings for mathematicalperformance against a spatial factor (Carrol 1993; Halpern2000). Furthermore, measures of spatial ability have predictivevalidity, in that they can predict future performance in quanti-tative fields (Williams andCeci 2007). For example, Shea et al.(2001) followed a large group of intellectually talented boysand girls over a 20 year longitudinal study, from seventh gradeuntil age 33. They found that individual differences in spatial,verbal, and quantitative reasoning in adolescence predictededucational and vocational outcomes two decades later.Further, spatial ability made a significant unique contributioneven after controlling for verbal and mathematical ability(Shea, et al. 2001). Spatial ability is also predictive of collegemathematical entrance scores (Casey et al. 1995, 1997), whichare an important prerequisite for entry to further education inscience and mathematics disciplines (Ceci et al. 2009).

Factors that influence spatial ability during developmenthold promise for educational interventions that seek to reducethe gender gap in science and mathematics in adulthood(Halpern 2007; Newcombe 2007). Hyde and Lindberg (2007,p. 29) argued that even mild improvement in spatial abilitymay have “multiplier effects in girls’mathematical and scienceperformance”. Additionally, higher levels of spatial ability areassociated with attitudinal changes towards mathematics andself-confidence in mathematical ability from elementaryschool (Eccles et al. 1993) to high school and college (Eccles1987; Eccles et al. 1990). Thus the contribution of spatialability to later cognitive development may be in part social aswell as intellectual (Crawford et al. 1995; Nash 1979).

Academic domains where one feels competent and are seenas being socioculturally valued for one’s gender are more likelyto be pursued than those that are not (Eccles et al. 1990).

Although medium to large gender differences in spatialability performance are found in most reviews of studies(Linn and Petersen 1985; Voyer et al. 1995), Hyde (2005)notes that within-gender variation is larger than between-gen-der differences. Since gender alone explains only a portion ofindividual variation in spatial ability (Caplan and Caplan1994), identifying other developmental factors which promotespatial ability is an important research goal (Halpern et al.2007; Hyde and Lindberg 2007). Neisser et al. (1996, p. 97)argued that understanding the source of such differences iscritical, and that such questions are “socially, as well asscientifically important”. One potential source of individualdifferences is that of gender-role identity.

Although the exact mechanisms contributing to the emer-gence of gender differences in spatial ability are debated (seeCaplan and Caplan 1994 and Halpern 2011 for a discussion)they are believed to be influenced by a network of biologicaland sociocultural contributions (Ceci et al. 2009; Crawford etal. 1995; Eagly and Wood 1999; Halpern and Tan 2001). Onesuch contribution is that of gender-role identity.

Though boys and girls typically differ in early socialisationexperiences (Eccles et al. 1990; Emmott 1985; Lytton andRomney 1991), there is considerable individual variation inthe degree to which they develop and acquire stereotypicallymasculine and feminine personality traits, behaviors and in-terests (Bem 1974; Constantinople 1973; Kagan 1964a). Thisprocess is referred to as gender typing (Kohlberg 1966;Kohlberg and Ullian 1974), and holds implications for thedevelopment of gender-role identity and integration of mas-culinity and femininity into an individual’s self-concept andgender schema (Bem 1981; Knafo et al. 2005; Spence 1993).Highly gender typed individuals are motivated to keep theirbehavior and self-concept consistent with traditional gendernorms (Bem1975; Bem and Lenney 1976; Maccoby 1990;Martin and Ruble 2004), and this also applies to academicdomains (Nosek et al. 2002; Oswald 2008; Steffens andJelenec 2011). Others may integrate aspects of both masculineand feminine identification into their self-schema, termedandrogyny (Bem1984; Spence 1984).

Gender-Role Mediation of Spatial Ability

Nash (1979) proposed a gender-role mediation explanationfor gender differences in which it is argued that gender-roleidentity can either promote or inhibit optimum developmentof cognitive ability in highly gender-typed domains, such asspatial and verbal ability. Specifically, Nash (1979) theo-rized that masculine identification leads to cultivation ofspatial, mathematical, and scientific skills, whereas feminineidentification facilitates verbal and language abilities.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 250: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

In a review of gender-role influences on cognitive ability,Nash (1979, p. 263) wrote “For some people, cultural mythsare translated into personality beliefs which can affect cog-nitive functioning in gender-typed intellectual domains”.This argument was based on earlier work by Sherman(1967) into differential learning and practice experiencesof boys and girls. In doing so, Nash extended Sherman’stheory by placing cognitive development of spatial ability ina social context, where gender-role identity encourages ordiscourages optimum development of spatial potential. Nashidentified several mechanisms that contribute to spatial de-velopment, including gender typing of intellectual domains,gender-role conformity and self-efficacy beliefs.

Differential Spatial Experiences

Sherman (1967) hypothesized a causal explanation for thepresence of gender differences in spatial ability, based on achild’s differential opportunities to develop and refine spa-tial skills through play and recreational activities. Boys andgirls typically differ in their socialisation experiences, andare encouraged by parents to engage in either stereotypicallymasculine or feminine play appropriate to their gender(Eccles et al. 1990; Lytton and Romney 1991). However,play is also an opportunity for active engagement and cog-nitive development (Piaget 1968). Caplan and Caplan(1994) argued that traditionally “masculine” typed activitiespromote the development of spatial ability by encouragingthe practice and application of spatial skills (Connor andSerbin 1977). In contrast, traditionally “feminine” activitiesdo not require the use of spatial skills, but reinforce othersocially valued skills (Lever 1976).

What distinguishes Sherman’s (1967) explanation fromother explanations (such as Caplan and Caplan 1994) is thatit focuses specifically on gender roles, rather than solely onbiological gender, as explaining individual differences inspatial ability. Differential practice of skills promoting spa-tial development occur through gender typing of activitiesand interests (Serbin and Connor 1979; Serbin et al. 1990).Rather than assuming that the lives of boys and girls do notoverlap, or that all boys engage in a high level of activityand receive equal opportunities to practise and developspatial ability, it accounts for individual differences andgender typing. There is evidence to support this argument.Retrospective studies have shown that an association existsbetween spatial ability and activity preferences in youngadult college-level samples (Baenninger and Newcombe1989; Signorella et al. 1989).

Gender Typing of Intellectual Domains

Kagan (1964b) noted that objects in the everyday world,social activities, and even intellectual pursuits become gender

typed as either masculine or feminine, based on shared con-sensual beliefs that emerge very early in childhood. For ex-ample, reading and language is regarded as being feminine(Dwyer 1973, 1974), whereas mathematics, science and tech-nology are regarded as masculine (Li 1999; Nash 1975). Bothat an implicit (Lane et al. 2012; Nosek et al. 2009; Steffens andJelenec 2011) and an explicit level (Benbow 1988; Halpernand Tan 2001), cultural beliefs about specific cognitive tasksas being inherently masculine or feminine prevail - even forgenerations growing up with increased gender equality (Libenet al. 2002). Recently, Halpern et al. (2011) showed that laybeliefs about cognitive gender differences in student andcommunity samples were firmly entrenched across bothmen and women. Although these stereotypes are not anaccurate reflection of reality, Nash (1979) argued theyhave the potential to shape the self-concepts of boysand girls, and how they see themselves in relation tothese academic domains (Hyde and Lindberg 2007).

Gender-role Conformity Pressures

Gender roles and associated stereotypes describe differencesbetween men and women, and prescribe how they shouldbehave in social and occupational settings (Eagly andMitchell 2004). Highly gender typed persons are motivated tokeep their behavior consistent with internalised gender-rolestandards and norms (Bem and Lenney 1976), whereas thoselow in gender typing or for whom gender-role identity is lesssalient show greater cognitive and behavioral flexibility(Arbuthnot 1975; Bem 1975; Stein and Bailey 1973).Conformity cues as to who should engage in certain behaviors,and what activities are permissible for boys or girls, come frompeers, parents, and the media (Martin and Ruble 2004;Matthews 2007), and this has implications for intellectual do-mains that are masculine or feminine dominated (Eccles 2007).

Nash (1979) argued that the increased saliency of genderand gender typing of academic subjects in adolescence maylead to a conflict between the “ideal” image a student holdsof himself or herself, and the activities he or she chooses toperform well in and values. Perceived incompatibility be-tween being “feminine” and succeeding in stereotypically“masculine” domains can hinder academic achievement(Rosenthal et al. 2011; Schmader 2002). Thus there is alsoan attitudinal and motivational component to developmentof intellectual abilities (Nash 1979).

Self-efficacy Beliefs and Gender Stereotypes

During childhood when gender-role saliency is low, boys andgirls show relatively little difference in intellectual abilities,and what differences exist often favors girls (Halpern 2000;Nash 1979). However, gender typing of intellectual pursuitsquickly emerges in adolescence (Dwyer 1974; Kagan 1964b),

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 251: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

and leads to several negative psychological consequences forsome children (Nash 1979). Firstly, girls and boys receivedifferent messages about occupational aspirations and theusefulness of specific academic skills (Fennema andSherman 1977; Hyde and Lindberg 2007). Secondly, as notedearlier, gender typing of intellectual tasks is often seen asbeing incompatible with a feminine gender-role identity at atime when conformity pressure increases (Eccles 2007;Hoffman 1972; Rosenthal et al. 2011). This can result inlowered self-esteem and reduced self-efficacy beliefs forgender-typed tasks (Pajares and Miller 1994). Gender stereo-types suggest that men and women are better at some tasksthan others, and this is reflected in self-estimations of intelli-gence in gender-typed domains (for a review seeSzymanowicz and Furnham 2011). Additionally, a large bodyof research has observed that a feminine gender typing isassociated with considerably lower self-esteem than mascu-line or androgynous individuals (Spence et al. 1975; Whitley1983, 1988), including academic self-esteem (Alpert-Gillisand Connell 1989; Lau 1989; Robison-Awana et al. 1986).

Evidence for a Spatial-Gender-Role Association

A prior meta-analysis by Signorella and Jamison (1986) foundsupport for Nash’s hypothesis in spatial ability. However therehave been major and potentially relevant changes in genderroles and stereotypes in the intervening decades (Auster andOhm 2000; Hyde and Lindberg 2007) which Feingold (1988)has argued are responsible for declining gender differences incognitive ability. This view is supported byHyde (2005, 2006,2007) and colleagues across a range of intellectual abilities(Hyde 2007a; Lindberg et al. 2010). These changes questionthe validity of Nash’s theory in contemporary society andwhether such gender-role associations still exist today. Forthis reason, we aimed to conduct a meta-analysis of studiespublished since Signorella and Jamison’s (1986) review, to seewhether the gender-role mediation hypothesis still holds.Although these studies are primarily based on researchconducted in the USA, studies from other nations (e.g.,Poland, Croatia, United Kingdom, Canada) are also examinedfor a broader test of Nash’s theory.

Meta-analysis provides researchers with a way to criticallyevaluate the cumulative evidence of empirical evidence(Rosenthal 1984), and the technique is becoming increasinglycommon in psychology (Hyde 1990; Rosenthal and DiMatteo2001). Although individual studies taken in isolation mightshow that a relationship between factor X on ability Y may bepresent or absent, factors such as random sampling error andlack of statistical power may result in erroneously rejecting thenull hypothesis (Type I error) or failing to detect an effect thatis real (Type II error). The technique of meta-analysis allowsone to draw firmer conclusions about the existence of anassociation (Rosenthal and DiMatteo 2001), as well to arrive

at an estimate of its size that is more accurate and reliable thancould be determined from a single empirical study.

A requirement of meta-analysis is that empirical studiesmeasure a similar construct drawn from similar samples (R.Rosenthal 1984, 1995), and that there are a sufficient numberof studies to make meaningful conclusions. Spatial ability isnot a unitary construct; it encompasses at least three separateprocesses – spatial perception, visualisation, and mental rota-tion (Linn and Petersen 1985). Mental rotation is one of themost widely researched areas of cognitive gender differences(Halpern and Lamay 2000), due in part to the fact comparisonsof men and women in mental rotation show the largest effectsizes of all spatial tasks (Voyer et al. 1995). Some researchersregard mental rotation to be a representation of general spatialreasoning (Casey et al. 1995; Halpern 2000; Vandenberg andKuse 1978), and there is evidence that performance in mentalrotation prospectively predicts later development of quantita-tive reasoning (Casey et al. 1997; Nuttall et al. 2005).Therefore this review is confined to studies that investigatedperformance in mental rotation tasks. In addition, gender dif-ferences are larger after late adolescence when gender rolesbecome particularly salient (Nash 1979). There are also issuesof reliability and validity when assessing gender roles in youn-ger samples. For this reason, only studies using high school,college or young adult samples were considered for inclusionin the reported meta-analysis. Studies using younger samples,such as that by Titze et al. (2010), were not considered.

In sum, the present review involved a meta-analysis ofstudies that have investigated gender-role associations withmental rotation task performance. It was hypothesized(Hypothesis 1) that masculinity would be positively associat-ed with greater mental rotation performance in men andwomen. The influence of femininity was also investigated asa research question. It was hypothesised (Hypothesis 2) thatthere would be a negative association between femininity andmental rotation performance for both genders. Since the mag-nitude of gender differences typically varies with the type andlevel of difficulty of mental rotation task (Voyer et al. 1995),we also examined the type of mental rotation instrument as apotential moderator. Similarly, because there have been de-bates over which measures of masculinity and femininity arethe best predictor of behavior (Bem 1984; Spence andBuckner 2000), we examined the type of gender-role instru-ment as a potential moderating variable.

Method

Search Strategy

To access as many studies as possible, a number of searchstrategies were used. Firstly, a Web of Science citationsearch for articles citing either Nash (1979) or Signorella

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 252: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

and Jamison (1986) was performed, as any study publishedthat is relevant to the meta-analysis would be likely to citethese key articles. Secondly, GoogleScholar and PsycINFOsearches were performed for studies containing the keywords“spatial ability” or “mental rotation” and any combinationwith the keywords “masculine”, “masculinity”, “androgy-nous”, or “androgyny”. This second method identified a num-ber of additional studies that were not specifically testing agender-role mediation hypothesis, but merely included agender-role measure and mental rotation task as part of alarger battery of neuropsychological tests (e.g., Rahman etal. 2004). Furthermore, an attempt to locate unpublished stud-ies was made by searching the Dissertation Abstracts andERIC databases for studies, locating one additional study.The search was performed in September, 2012.

Selection Criteria

The following inclusion criteria were used:

& peer-reviewed empirical studies published after 1986 orunpublished manuscripts and reports dated after 1986

& gender-role identity was measured using a psychometrical-ly valid and reliable gender-role instrument, such as theBemSexRole Inventory (BSRI; Bem 1974) or the PersonalAttributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence et al. 1974)

& participants sampled were either an adult or high schoolaged adolescent, from a non-clinical sample

Requests to authors (n = 5) for additional information weremade where a masculinity and mental-rotation association wasnot explicitly tested or reported. Three studies could not beincluded due to insufficient information to determine an effectsize (Evardone and Alexander 2009; Tuttle and Pillard 1991;Vonnahme 2005). One practice sometimes adopted is to con-sider all studies missing an effect size to have an associationwith an absolute value of zero, a practice that Rosenthal (1995)considers overly conservative and leads to inaccurate esti-mates. This practice was considered at length by Hedges andBecker (1986) who caution against missing value substitution.Accordingly the decision was made to exclude these missingstudies. Following application of the selection and exclusioncriteria, there were 12 available studies examining mentalrotation and gender roles. However it should be noted thatthe possibility of unpublished null studies (commonly termedthe “file drawer problem”) is addressed using meta-analytictechniques that test for publication bias (Orwin 1983;Rosenthal 1979).

Sample Characteristics

The characteristics of all studies identified in the litera-ture search are presented in Table 1. Several of the

studies recruited participants from different countries,making for a broader test of Nash’s hypothesis thanwould be possible if analysing only data from theUSA. It should be noted that in most studies, sampleswere drawn almost exclusively from student subjectpools, limiting generalisability somewhat to a young-adult, college-level educated sample.

Procedure

Comprehensive Meta Analysis (CMA) V2 software wasused for the calculation of statistics (Borenstein andRothstein 1999). A random-effects model was chosen(Borenstein et al. 2009) because spatial ability is subjectto a large number of psychosocial moderators, and avariety of different gender-role instruments and mentalrotation tasks were used over multiple decades. Therandom effects model gives slightly wider confidenceintervals than a fixed-effects model (Field 2001;Rosenthal and DiMatteo 2001), but gives a more appro-priate estimate of how much variability is present inempirical studies (Kelley and Kelley 2012).

The focus of the review was the relationship betweengender-role identity and mental rotation, which can be rep-resented by Pearson’s product moment correlation, r.Gender-role instruments offer separate masculinity and fem-ininity scales, allowing us to consider the effect of mascu-linity independently of femininity, and to test both for amental rotation association.

Where the direct product–moment correlation betweengender-role masculinity scale and mental rotation wasreported, this was used because it represents the directassociation independent of a subject’s femininity scale.However, two studies reported only the mean values formasculine, feminine, and androgynous groups. Since an-drogyny represents a “special case”, and some theoristsargue that such participants cannot be legitimately combinedwith either the masculine or feminine group (Taylor andHall 1982), the androgynous participants were excluded asper Signorella and Jamison’s (1986) recommendation.Such an approach is the most conservative strategyavailable, and may lead to an underestimation of thetrue effect size in cases where androgynous participants(high masculinity, high femininity) score higher thantheir masculine or feminine counterparts (e.g. Hamilton1995). By doing so, however, it affords a simple com-parison between masculine and feminine participants only,allowing for the use of Cohen’s d and then conversion to ras the common effect size unit using the formula givenby Rosenthal (1984). Several studies recruited male orfemale participants only, and in several cases examinedonly masculinity associations. Calculations were performedusing the CMA software.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 253: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Tab

le1

Characteristicsof

thestud

iesidentifiedfrom

theliteraturesearch

onmentalrotatio

nperformance

andgend

erroles

Study

Cou

ntry

Sam

pleTyp

eSam

ple

Age

NGender

Role

Mental

Rotation

Males

Fem

ales

Masculin

ity(r)

Fem

ininity

(r)

Masculin

ity(r)

Fem

ininity

(r)

Jamison

andSigno

rella

(198

7)USA

Highscho

olstud

ents

8thgrade

10females

BSRI

CRT

.45*

.04

.27

.14

19males

Signo

rella

etal.(198

9)USA

Sub

ject

pool

n/r

132females

BSRI

CRT

.08

.01

.24*

.05

156males

Gilg

erandHo(198

9)USA

Sub

ject

pool

M=19

.052

females

BSRI

TSRT

.00

−.17

.00

−.17

38males

Voy

erandBryden(199

0)a

Canada

Sub

ject

pool

M=21

.065

females

BSRI

VMRT

.50*

*-

.21

-65

males

Tuttle

andPillard(199

1)USA

Com

mun

ityRange

25–40

88females

CPI

TSRT

n/r

101males

New

combe

andDub

as(199

2)USA

Lon

gitudinal

1661

females;attrition

rate

=29

%PA

QTSRT

--

.15

-.10

Ham

ilton

(199

5)b

UnitedKingd

omCom

mun

ity,scho

olandcollege

M=18

.012

2females

BSRI

SMRT

.12

−.12

.14

−.14

54males

JagiekaandHerman-

Jeglinska(199

8)ae

Poland

Sub

ject

pool

n/a

30males

BSRI

SMRT

.34*

--

-

Saucier

etal.(200

2)Canada

Sub

ject

pool

M=22

.854

females

PAQ

VMRT

.45*

*−.02

.45*

**

−.02

41males

Rahman

etal.(200

4)de

UnitedKingd

omCom

mun

ityRange

18–40

120females

EPP

VMRT

.41*

**

-.23*

*-

120males

Ritter

(200

4)c

UnitedKingd

omSub

ject

pool

M=21

.037

females

BSRI

SMRT

.34*

−.26

−.18

−.14

42males

Scarbroug

handJohn

ston

(200

5)USA

Sub

ject

pool

M=19

.641

females

BSRI

CSMRT

--

.40*

*.00

Von

nahm

e(200

5)USA

Sub

ject

pool

M=21

.246

males

BSRI

CMRT

n/r

n/r

Hromatko

etal.(200

8)Croatia

Unspecified

M=24

.826

females

BSRI

TSRT

--

.64*

**

.03

Evardon

eandAlexand

er(200

9)USA

Sub

ject

pool

M=20

.052

females

BSRI

VMRT

n/r

n/r

n/r

n/r

58males

aCalculatedfrom

pvalue

bAnd

rogy

nous

(highmasculin

ity,high

femininity

)elim

inated

*p<.05;

**p<.01;

***p<.001

two-tailed

cCalculatedfrom

mediansplit

ofmasculin

ity/fem

ininity

dDataprov

ided

byauthor

eDataon

lyavailableformasculin

ityn/r=data

notrepo

rted

CPICaliforniaPsycholog

ical

Inventory;

EPP

Eysenck

PersonalityProfile

(EPP;Eysenck

etal.19

96);CRTCardRotationTest(Frenchet

al.19

63);TSR

TThu

rstone

SpatialRelations

Test

(Thu

rstone

1958

);SM

RTShepard

andMetzlerMentalR

otationTest(Shepard

andMetzler19

71);VMRTVandenb

ergMentalR

otationTest(Vandenb

ergandKuse19

78);CMRTCoo

perandShepard

MRT(Coo

perandShepard

1973

)

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 254: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Meta-analytic Results

Study characteristics and effect sizes are presented in Table 1.Since empirical studies using gender roles frequently findgender × gender-role interactions, the associations with mas-culinity and femininity are reported separately for men andwomen. Forest plots are provided when gender-role associa-tions are statistically significant. A forest plot conveys a visualrepresentation of the effect size estimates of individual studiesand their variability (Lewis and Clarke 2001); one can see theamount of variation between individual studies as well as theoverall trend. In the centre of each study’s confidence intervalis a square; the size of the square corresponds to the samplesize used in each study. The diamond symbol represents theoverall estimate of the sample, with the centre of the diamondbeing the point estimate and its horizontal tips representing theconfidence interval.

Girls and Women

Figure 1 presents a forest plot of the association betweenmasculinity and mental rotation performance for girls andwomen, and effect sizes are given in Table 1. Hypothesis 1predicted that masculinity would be positively associated withgreater mental rotation performance. As shown in Fig. 1, moststudies with female samples were in a direction consistent withthis hypothesis with the exception of two studies: Gilger andHo (1989) found no association, whereas Ritter (2004) found aweak negative association. The distribution of effect sizesacross studies was heterogenous, Q(10) = 21.13, p = .020,I2 = 52.67 indicating moderate variability across studies. It isalso noteworthy that the two largest associations were found inthe non-USA samples of Croatia (r = .64) andCanada (r = .45).However, the size of the correlation is unlikely to be culturallyrelated given that the third largest association was found in aUSA sample (r = .40) and that small associations were also

found in non-USA samples (e.g., r = −.18 for Ritter 2004;r = 14 for Hamilton 1995).

In support of Hypothesis 1, the combined masculinityeffect size for women was r = .23 (95 % CI lower = .11,upper = .34), Zma = 3.72, p < .001. This correlation forwomen was only slightly larger than that found bySignorella and Jamison (1986), who found a significant asso-ciation of r = .19 between masculinity and mental rotation forgirls and women using androgyny measures. To put thesefindings into perspective, we employed Rosenthal’sBinomial Effect Size Difference (BESD; R. Rosenthal andRubin 1982), a metric that represents effect size in a formatsuitable for interpretation by non-statisticians (R. Rosenthaland DiMatteo 2001). Represented in the BESD format, thelikelihood of being average or higher in mental rotation per-formance increases from 38.5 % for feminine women to61.5 % for masculine or androgynous women.

The possibility of unpublished null studies (referred to asthe “file drawer problem”) was also addressed by the calcula-tion of Orwin’s Fail-Safe N, which estimates the number ofnull studies required to reduce mean effect sizes to a specificcutoff-point (Borenstein et al. 2009; Orwin 1983). EmployingOrwin’s calculation, it would take only two more null studiesto reduce the association to that found previously bySignorella and Jamison (1986); therefore the stronger associ-ation in these studies should be taken only tentatively.

Hypothesis 2 predicted that there would be a significantnegative association between femininity and mental rotationperformance. This hypothesis was not supported, r = −.05, p =n.s. Such a finding is also consistent with the findings ofSignorella and Jamison (1986) who failed to find any associ-ation between femininity and mental rotation performance.

Boys and Men

The forest plot of the association between masculinity andmental rotation performance for boys and men is shown inFig. 2 and it presents the second test of Hypothesis 1. As can

Fig. 1 Forest plot of masculinity association with mental rotationperformance for girls and women. Positive associations indicate bettermental rotation performance as masculinity increases. The combinedeffect size is represented as a diamond shaped correlation

Fig. 2 Forest plot of masculinity association with mental rotationperformance for boys and men. Positive associations indicate bettermental rotation performance as masculinity increases. The combinedeffect size is represented as a diamond shaped correlation

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 255: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

be seen from the figure, the scores of men were slightly widerin variability than for women, with many studies showingrelatively large associations while three showed relativelyweak or non-significant correlations. Similar to the resultsfor women, there did not appear to be a strong relationshipbetween the country the study was conducted in and the sizeof the association. The largest association was found in asample of men from Canada (r = .50), but the equal secondlargest association was found in a USAmale sample (r = .45).However, it is noteworthy that the two remaining studies withUSA samples did not find any significant association (r = .08and r = .00). The distribution of effect sizes across all studieswas heterogenous, Q(8) = 17.92, p = .022, I2 = 55.36, indi-cating moderate variability between studies.

In support of Hypothesis 1, the association between mas-culinity and mental-rotation performance for men was signif-icant, r = .30, (95 % CI lower = .16 upper = .42), Zma = 4.25,p < .001. Again, the association is slightly larger than thatestimated by Signorella and Jamison (1986), who reported anr = .15 between masculinity and mental rotation performancefor boys and men. Orwin’s Fail-safe N showed that it wouldtake an additional eight unpublished studies with a meanassociation of zero to reduce this correlation to the size foundin the earlier review (r = .15). Represented in the BESDformat, the likelihood of being average or higher in mentalrotation performance increases from 35 % for feminine boysand men to 65 % for those with a masculine or androgynousgender-role identity. Finally, in contrast to Hypothesis 2, noassociation was found between femininity and mental rotationfor boys and men, r = −.06, p = n.s.

Moderating Variables

Since there was moderate between-study heterogeneity inthe masculinity association for both men and women, it isimportant to determine potential moderators that may beresponsible such as the type of gender-role instrument usedto classify participants, or the nature of the mental rotationtask. Alternately, instruments might vary in their predictivevalidity for men and women, and this information might beuseful in planning future research. Accordingly, effect sizesand heterogeneity were examined for men and women sep-arately across gender-role instrument.

Tables 2 and 3 present associations across type of gender-role instrument for men and women respectively. While theBSRI was used most frequently, the strongest gender-roleassociations were found with the PAQ for both men andwomen. However with an insufficient number of studiesemploying gender-role measures other than the BSRI, anyconclusions made about the predictive validity of these in-struments are tentative.

Another potential source of heterogeneity is the nature of themental rotation task employed. Meta-analytic reviews havefound that the magnitude of gender differences differs acrossinstruments (Voyer et al. 1995). It seems likely, therefore, thatsimilar variation would be present when considering gender-role associations. Table 4 presents effect sizes for studiesgrouped by mental rotation instrument. Instruments weregrouped into four categories. These groupings reduced hetero-geneity, suggesting that much of the variability observed acrossstudies was the result of using different instruments for mea-suring mental rotation. It should also be noted that theVandenberg instrument also produced the highest gender-roleeffect size of any mental rotation task. This may reflect theincreased difficulty of this instrument which allows for greaterdifferentiation between high and low ability (Voyer et al. 1995).

Discussion

The present meta-analysis examined evidence for Nash’s(1979) gender-role mediation hypothesis of spatial ability,as measured by performance on mental-rotation tasks. In aprevious review, Signorella and Jamison (1986) found asmall but statistically significant association between genderrole and mental rotation performance. The present resultssupport the conclusions drawn by Signorella and Jamison(1986). There is a significant and medium sized associationbetween masculinity and mental rotation in researchconducted in the past 25 years. The size of the associationdid not appear to be strongly related to the country in whichthe study was conducted, although there was some evidencethat the type of mental rotation task and gender-role measureused in the study was a factor. The present meta-analysisalso showed that there was no association between feminin-ity and mental rotation performance.

Table 2 Effect size and heterogeneity by gender-role instrument for men

Type of instrument N of studies Effect size (r) Zma, p-value Heterogeneity

Bem Sex-Role Inventory 7 .25 Z = 3.07, p = .002 Q(6) = 11.73, p = n.s.

Personal Attributes Questionnaire 1 .45 Z = 2.20; p = .028 N/A

Eysenck Personality Profiler 1 .41 Z = 2.50; p = .013 N/A

Total heterogeneity within-groups, Q (6) = 11.74, p = .068; between-groups, Q(2) = 6.18, p = .045

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 256: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

The results of this meta-analysis demonstrate three impor-tant things. Firstly, it upholds the claims made by Nash (1979)that, at least for mental rotation tasks, masculine gender rolescontribute to the development of spatial ability. Although onlycorrelational in nature, the inclusion of the longitudinal studybyNewcombe and Dubas (1992) shows that gender roles havepredictive validity for later development of spatial ability.Secondly, this review demonstrates the persistence of genderroles over a larger span of time, in that studies reviewed aredrawn from three decades of research; it would appear that theempirical findings of Nash and others were not a statisticalquirk, or an artefact of prevailing gender inequalities of thepast. Thirdly, the review shows that the magnitude of thegender-role association may be somewhat larger than previ-ously thought by researchers, especially for men.

A possible explanation for finding a stronger associationbetween gender roles and spatial ability than Signorella andJamison (1986) is the quality of instruments used acrossstudies. Many of the earlier studies reviewed by Signorellaand Jamison (1986) used instruments that operationalisedmasculinity and femininity as bipolar opposites of a uni-dimensional construct (Constantinople 1973) rather than or-thogonal aspects of gender-role identity (Bem 1981). This leadsto misclassification of masculine, feminine, and androgynousparticipants (Bem 1974, 1977) and an attenuation of effect sizedue to imprecision (Cooper 1981). It is difficult to suggest atheoretical reason why gender roles might influence cognitivedevelopment more strongly now in men than in previous de-cades, but this possibility cannot be ruled out entirely.

A growing trend in empirical research is a move awayfrom levels of statistical significance towards evaluations ofthe magnitude of effect sizes (Wilkinson 1999), to assess

their practical impact and importance. Cohen (1988) pro-vides a good rule of thumb to gauge associations by: corre-lations of .10 or higher are regarded as small, .30 or higheras medium, and correlations higher than .50 are consideredlarge. Frequently these yardsticks are used rather rigidly,and some researchers regard differences that are “small” as“trivial” or non-existent (Hyde 1996, 2005). Cooper (1981)warns against this practice, as the magnitude of effects thatmay be found can differ greatly from one field of psycho-logical research to another. Similarly, in a review of effectsizes and practical importance for research with children,McCartney and Rosenthal (2000) advise against such yard-sticks, and caution that effect sizes should be compared tothose found in that particular research domain. For thisreason, comparisons to a range of other effects deemedpreviously to be influential in spatial ability may bebetter able to put the results of this meta-analysis incontext (Hyde 1990).

The present results showed a gender-role association ofr = .30 for men and r = .23 for women. Two areas previouslydocumented to contribute to spatial ability are prior spatialactivity preferences in childhood (Signorella et al. 1989) andsocioeconomic status (Levine et al. 2005). A meta-analysisby Baenninger and Newcombe (1989) produced an r = .10between spatial activity preferences and spatial ability.Levine et al. (2005) found that spatial ability differencesare found between low, medium, and high socioeconomicstatus groups for adolescents with an effect size r = .23 formental rotation. When compared to these factors, whichresearchers have previously argued to be important and havea meaningful impact on spatial ability, the contribution ofgender role and mental rotation is greater, and may go some

Table 3 Effect size and heterogeneity by gender-role instrument for women

Type of instrument N of studies Effect size (r) Zma, p-value Heterogeneity

Bem Sex-Role Inventory 8 .21 Z = 2.43, p = .015 Q(7) = 17.07, p = .017

Personal Attributes Questionnaire 2 .30 Z = 1.97; p = .049 Q(1) = 3.02, p = n.s.

Eysenck Personality Profiler 1 .23 Z = 1.17; p = n.s. N/A

Total heterogeneity within-groups, Q (8) = 20.09, p = .010; between-groups, Q(2) = 1.04, p = n.s.

Table 4 Effect size and heterogeneity by mental rotation instrument

Type of instrument N of studies Effect size (r) Zma, p-value Heterogeneity

Card Rotations Task (French et al. 1963) 2 .22 Z = 1.81, p = .071 Q(1) = 1.44, p = n.s.

Thurstone Spatial Relations (Thurstone 1958) 3 .21 Z = 1.83; p = .058 Q(2) = 10.38, p = .006

Vandenberg MRT (Vandenberg and Kuse 1978) 3 .38 Z = 4.29; p < .001 Q(2) = 1.41, p = n.s.

Generic Mental Rotation Tasks 4 .22 Z = 2.43; p = .015 Q(3) = 3.91, p = n.s.

Total heterogeneity within-groups, Q (8) = 17.14, p = .029; between-groups, Q(3) = 10.47, p = .015

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 257: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

way to explaining existing gender differences in spatialability (Nash 1979; Sherman 1967).

Implications for Spatial Development

One possible intervention being considered for individualsmost at risk of forestalled spatial development is that of spatialtraining. In a review article, Newcombe and Frick (2010)stress the importance of early intervention in the developmentof spatial abilities during early childhood. Although it wouldbe desirable to offer spatial instruction and training for allstudents to address this gender-gap (Hyde and Lindberg 2007),competing interests in an ever crowded curriculum make thelikelihood of this practice being adopted rather bleak; indeedfew schools incorporate spatial ability specifically into thecurriculum during elementary school (Mathewson 1999;Newcombe and Frick 2010). A more practical measure mightbe for limited intervention programs to target at-risk students,in the same way that reading and literacy interventions areoffered for students struggling in these areas.

While screening directly for spatial deficits may be possible,large gender differences do not typically emerge until adoles-cence (Linn and Petersen 1985; Voyer et al. 1995). Earlyintervention is desirable before such differences emerge(Newcombe and Frick 2010). The assessment of gender rolesmight serve as amore useful risk factor to consider than gender,and it has the advantage of not necessarily being limited to onegender. Nuttall et al. (2005) describe gender-role appropriateintervention programs that develop spatial expertise, but as ofyet, there are no longitudinal studies of such programs.Educators may wish to be mindful to include a range ofopportunities that encourage spatial development as well asstressing their importance and relevance to both boys and girls.

Newcombe and Frick (2010) also advocate early inter-vention by parents, in providing children with activities andopportunities outside the classroom to develop spatialawareness, perception and visualisation. Rigidly held gen-der roles restrict children’s self-selection of activities (Rubleet al. 2006; Tracy 1987), and parents may wish to encouragea broader repertoire in their children including sports andtoys that encourage spatial development (Doyle et al. 2012).The continuing failure to find a negative relationship be-tween femininity and spatial ability for both genders is alsonoteworthy. Feminine identification should not be discour-aged in order to develop spatial and quantitative ability.

Future Directions for Research

Although gender differences in cognitive ability are frequentlydebated, many researchers note there is greater within-gendervariability than between men and women (Hyde 1990; Priessand Hyde 2010). Gender-role identity appears to be an impor-tant, but previously underestimated contributor to these

individual differences in spatial ability, which in turn is a keyfoundation for higher-level quantitative skills such as mathe-matics (Casey et al. 1997; Delgado and Prieto 2004) andSTEM related fields (Halpern 2007; Newcombe 2007).Indeed, Halpern (2007, p. 125) has claimed that spatial abilityis “essential” for success in STEM-related subjects. As such,the emergence of gender roles as a factor that meets or exceedsother factors that contribute to spatial ability is important, bothas a potential diagnostic indicator for interventions as well as afocus for future investigation. By better understanding thepsychosocial processes associated with gender roles and intel-lectual development, one might be able to identify strategies -such as self-efficacy training or challenging of gender stereo-types - that would help negate performance impairments.

Additionally, this meta-analysis affirms the merit of con-sidering gender roles, rather than just biological gender, instudies of individual differences in cognition. Though thisreview was confined to only mental-rotation, it remains tobe seen whether the results can be generalised more widelyto other spatial ability tasks such as spatial perception andvisualisation (Linn and Petersen 1985). For example, isthere something specific about a masculine or androgynousgender role that leads to improved ability to perceive spatialobjects and mentally rotate them, or can it be generalised toother spatial tasks? This would allow us to test whethergender-role differences in perception are chiefly responsi-ble, or whether there are differences in the actual cognitiveprocesses underlying such tasks, for example a generalcognitive style (Arbuthnot 1975; Milton 1957). A limitednumber of studies with adolescents and young adults haveconsidered the Piaget water-level task (Jamison andSignorella 1980; Kalichman 1989; Popiel and De Lisi1984; Signorella and Jamison 1978) or the EmbeddedFigures Test (Bernard et al. 1990; Brosnan 1998; Hamilton1995), with some inconsistencies, but larger studies arerequired. Furthermore, as Signorella and Jamison (1986)note, Nash’s (1979) hypothesised associations betweengender-role identity and verbal ability remain largelyuntested, which future studies should pursue.

Conclusion

We have seen many changes in society’s beliefs aboutgender equality in the intervening decades since Nash(1979) proposed her gender-role mediation hypothesis ofintellectual development. However, for spatial ability atleast, this association seems as relevant today as when theclaim was first made. The results from our meta-analysissupport Nash’s hypothesis for the development of spatialability, and this provides strong support for calls to conductfurther research in this area to investigate the cognitive andsocial processes that underlie the association betweengender-roles and cognitive abilities.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 258: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Acknowledgments This research was supported in part by a GriffithUniversity Postgraduate Research Scholarship. Thanks go to Dr HeatherGreen, Dr Michael Steele, Dr Elizabeth Conlon, and Dr MargaretSignorella for early revisions of this manuscript.

References

Alpert-Gillis, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Gender and sex roleinfluences on children’s self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 57,97–114. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00762.x.

Arbuthnot, J. (1975). Sex, sex-role identity, and cognitive style. Perceptualand Motor Skills, 41, 435–440. doi:10.2466/pms.1975.41.2.435.

Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity incontemporary American society: A reevaluation using the BemSex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 43, 499–528. doi:10.1023/A:1007119516728.

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1989). The role of experience inspatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–344. doi:10.1007/BF00287729.

Bell, S. (2010). Women in science: The persistence of gender inAustralia. Higher Education Management and Policy, 22, 1–19.doi:10.1787/hemp-v22-art3-en.

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.doi:10.1037/h0036215.

Bem, S. L. (1975). Sex role adaptability: One consequence of psycho-logical androgyny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,31, 634–643. doi:10.1037/h0077098.

Bem, S. L. (1977). On the utility of alternate procedures for assessingpsychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology, 45, 196–205. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.45.2.196.

Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender Schema Theory: A cognitive account of sextyping. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354.

Bem, S. L. (Ed.). (1984). Androgyny and gender schema theory: Aconceptual and empirical integration. Lincoln: University ofNebraska Press.

Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex typing and the avoidance ofcross-sex behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,33, 48–54. doi:10.1037/h0078640.

Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoningability in intellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, ef-fects, and possible causes. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11,169–232. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00049670.

Bernard, M. E., Boyle, G. J., & Jackling, B. F. (1990). Sex-role identityand mental ability. Personality and Individual Differences, 11,213–217. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(90)90234-I.

Borenstein, M., & Rothstein, H. R. (1999). Comprehensive meta-analysis: A computer program for research synthesis. Englewood:BioStat.

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R.(2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. West Sussex: John Wiley& Sons, Ltd.

Boyle, G. J., Neumann, D. L., Furedy, J. J., & Westbury, H. R. (2010a).Combining the methods of differential and experimental psychol-ogy to study sex differences in human cognitive psychologicalfunctions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110, 392–410.doi:10.2466/pms.110.2.396-410.

Boyle, G. J., Neumann, D. L., Furedy, J. J., & Westbury, H. R. (2010b).Sex differences in verbal and visual-spatial tasks under differenthemispheric visual-field presentation conditions. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 110, 396–410. doi:10.2466/pms.110.2.396-410.

Brosnan, M. J. (1998). The implications for academic attainment ofperceived gender-appropriateness upon spatial task performance.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 203–215.doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1998.tb01284.x.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1994). Thinking critically about researchon sex and gender. New York: Harper Collins.

Carrol, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). Theinfluence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematicscollege entrance test scores across diverse samples. DevelopmentalPsychology, 31, 697–705. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.697.

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., & Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of genderdifferences in mathematics college entrance test scores: A com-parison of spatial skills with internalized beliefs and anxieties.Developmental Psychology, 33, 669–680. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.669.

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrep-resentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations.Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218–261. doi:10.1037/a0014412.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine-andfeminine-activity-preference scales for preschoolers: Correlates withother classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development,48, 1411–1416. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1977.tb03947.x.

Constantinople, A. (1973). Masculinity-femininity: An exception to afamous dictum? Psychological Bulletin, 80 , 389–409.doi:10.1037/h0035334.

Cooper, H. M. (1981). On the significance of effects and the effects ofsignificance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,1013–1018. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.5.1013.

Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of therotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual informa-tion processing (pp. 75–176). Oxford: Academic.

Crawford, M., Chaffin, R., & Fitton, L. (1995). Cognition in socialcontext. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 341–362.doi:10.1016/1041-6080(95)90006-3.

Delgado, A. R., & Prieto, G. (2004). Cognitive mediators and sex-related differences in mathematics. Intelligence, 32, 25–32.doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00061-8.

Doyle, R. A., Voyer, D., & Cherney, I. D. (2012). The relation betweenchildhood spatial activities and spatial abilities in adulthood.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33, 112–120.doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.01.002.

Dwyer, C. A. (1973). Sex differences in reading: An evaluation and acritique of current theories. Review of Educational Research, 43,455–467. doi:10.2307/1170076.

Dwyer, C. A. (1974). Influence of children’s sex role standards onreading and arithmetic achievement. Journal of EducationalPsychology, 66, 811–816. doi:10.1037/h0021522.

Eagly, A. H., &Mitchell, A. A. (2004). Social role theory of sex differencesand similarities: Implications for the sociopolitical attitudes of menandwomen. InM. A. Paludi (Ed.),Praeger guide to the psychology ofgender (pp. 183–206). Westport: Praeger Publishers.

Eagly, A. H., &Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in humanbehavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. AmericanPsychologist, 54, 408–423. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408.

Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement-relateddecisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172.doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987.tb00781.x.

Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences inparticipation in physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci(Ed.),Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debatethe evidence (pp. 199–210). Washington, D.C.: AmericanPsychological Association.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 259: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender rolestereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents’ socialization of gen-der differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201.doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x.

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993).Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task percep-tions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847.doi:10.2307/1131221.

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-nationalpatterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127. doi:10.1037/a0018053.

Emmott, S. (1985). Sex differences in children’s play:Implications for cognition. International Journal of Women’sStudies, 8, 449–456.

Evardone, M., & Alexander, G. (2009). Anxiety, sex-linked behaviors,and digit ratios (2D:4D). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 442–455. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9260-6.

Eysenck, H. J., Wilson, G. D., & Jackson, C. (1996). EysenckPersonality Profiler. Surrey: PSI Press.

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing.American Psychologist, 43, 95–103. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95.

Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. A. (1977). Sex-related differences inmathematics achievement, spatial visualization and affective fac-tors. American Educational Research Journal, 14, 51–71.doi:10.3102/00028312014001051.

Field, A. P. (2001).Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: AMonte Carlocomparison of fixed-and random-effects methods. PsychologicalMethods, 6, 161–180. doi:10.1037//1082-989X.6.2.161.

French, J. W., Ekstrom, R. B., & Price, L. A. (1963). Manual for kit ofreference tests for cognitive factors. Princeston: EducationalTesting Service.

Frieze, I. H., & Chrisler, J. C. (2011). Editorial policy on the use of theterms “Sex” and “Gender”. Sex Roles, 64, 789–790. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9988-2.

Gallagher, A. M., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.). (2005). Gender differencesin mathematics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gilger, J. W., & Ho, H.-Z. (1989). Gender differences in adultspatial information processing: Their relationship to pubertaltiming, adolescent activities, and sex-typing of personality.Cognitive Development, 4, 197–214. doi:10.1016/0885-2014(89)90015-4.

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture,gender, and math. Science, 320, 1164–1165. doi:10.1126/science.1154094.

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.).Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F. (2007). Science, sex, and good sense: Why women areunderrepresented in some areas of science and math. In S. J. Ceci(Ed.), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchersdebate the evidence (pp. 121–130). Washington, D.C.: AmericanPsychological Association.

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.).Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F., & Lamay, M. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical reviewof sex differences in intelligence. Educational PsychologyReview, 12, 229–246. doi:10.1023/A:1009027516424.

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using apsychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences.Brain and Cognition, 45, 392–414. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1287.

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., &Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences inscience and mathematics. Psychological Science in the PublicInterest, 8, 1–51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x.

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011). Sex differ-ences in intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.),

The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 253–272).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011). Beliefsabout cognitive gender differences: Accurate for direction,underestimated for size. Sex Roles, 64, 336–347. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2.

Hamilton, C. J. (1995). Beyond sex differences in visuo-spatial pro-cessing: The impact of gender trait possession. British Journal ofPsychology, 86, 1–20. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02542.x.

Hedges, L. V., & Becker, B. J. (1986). Statistical methods in the meta-analysis of research on gender differences. In J. S. Hyde & M. C.Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 14–50). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental testscores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals.Science, 269, 41–45. doi:10.1126/science.7604277.

Hoffman, L. W. (1972). Early childhood experiences and women’sachievement motives. Journal of Social Issues, 28, 129–155.doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1972.tb00022.x.

Hromatko, I., Tadinac, M., & Vranic, A. (2008). Femininity andmasculinity across the menstrual cycle: A relation to mate value.Collegium Antropologicum, 32, 81–86.

Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender dif-ferences. Signs, 16, 55–73. doi:10.1086/494645.

Hyde, J. S. (1996).Where are the gender differences?Where are the gendersimilarities? In D.M. Buss&N.M.Malmouth (Eds.), Sex, power andconflict (pp. 107–118). New York: Oxford University Press.

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. AmericanPsychologist, 60, 581–592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581.

Hyde, J. S. (2006). Gender similarities still rule. AmericanPsychologist, 61, 641–642. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.6.641b.

Hyde, J. S. (2007a). New directions in the study of gender similaritiesand differences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16,259–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00516.x.

Hyde, J. S. (2007b). Women in science: Gender similarities in abilitiesand sociocultural forces. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.),Why aren’t more womenin science?: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 131–145).Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about thenature of gender differences and the implications for genderequity. In S. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook for achieving gender equitythrough education (2nd ed., pp. 19–32). Mahwah: LawrenceErlbaum Associates.

Jagieka, A., & Herman-Jeglińska, A. (1998). The effect of masculinityand sex hormones on a mental rotation task in right-and left-handed males. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30,111–112. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(98)90282-3.

Jamison, W., & Signorella, M. L. (1980). Sex-typing and spatialability: The association between masculinity and success onPiaget’s water-level task. Sex Roles, 6, 345–353. doi:10.1007/BF00287356.

Jamison, W., & Signorella, M. L. (1987). Relations of masculinity andfemininity in self-concept to spatial performance in adolescents.The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 148, 249–251. doi:10.1080/00221325.1987.9914554.

Kagan, J. (1964a). Acquisition and significance of sex-typing and sex-roleidentity. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of childdevelopment research (Vol. 1, pp. 137–168). NewYork: Russell Sage.

Kagan, J. (1964b). The child’s sex role classification of school objects.Child Development, 35, 1051–1056. doi:10.2307/1126852.

Kalichman, S. C. (1989). Sex roles and sex differences in adult spatialperformance. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 93–100.doi:10.1080/00221325.1989.9914579.

Kelley, G., & Kelley, K. (2012). Statistical models for meta-analysis: Abrief tutorial. World Journal of Methodology, 2, 27–32.doi:10.5662/wjm.v2.i4.27.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 260: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Knafo, A., Iervolino, A. C., & Plomin, R. (2005). Masculine girls and

feminine boys: Genetic and environmental contributions to atyp-ical gender development in early childhood. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 88, 400–412. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.400.

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’ssex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby & R. G.D’Andrade (Eds.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–172). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Kohlberg, L., & Ullian, D. (1974). Stages in the development ofpsycho-sexual concepts and attitudes. In R. C. Friedman, R. M.Richart, & R. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp.209–222). New York: Wiley.

Lane, K., Goh, J., & Driver-Linn, E. (2012). Implicit science stereo-types mediate the relationship between gender and academicparticipation. Sex Roles, 66, 220–234. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-0036-z.

Lau, S. (1989). Sex role orientation and domains of self-esteem. SexRoles, 21, 415–422. doi:10.1007/BF00289600.

Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. SocialProblems, 23, 478–487. doi:10.1525/sp.1976.23.4.03a00100.

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., &Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sexdifference in spatial skill. Psychological Science, 16, 841–845.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01623.x.

Lewis, S., & Clarke, M. (2001). Forest plots: Trying to see the wood andthe trees. BMJ, 322, 1479–1480. doi:10.1136/bmj.322.7300.1479.

Li, Q. (1999). Teachers’ beliefs and gender differences in mathematics:A review. Educational Research, 41, 63–76. doi:10.1080/0013188990410106.

Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2002). Language at work:Children’s gendered interpretations of occupational titles. ChildDevelopment, 73, 810–828. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00440.

Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Newtrends in gender and mathematics performance: A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1123–1135. doi:10.1037/a0021276.

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterizationof sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. ChildDevelopment, 56, 1479–1498. doi:10.2307/1130467.

Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socializationof boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109,267–296. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267.

Maccoby, E. E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender con-stancy in sex-differentiated development. New Directions forChild and Adolescent Development, 1990, 5–20. doi:10.1002/cd.23219904703.

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sexdifferences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues:Cognitive perspectives on gender development. CurrentDirections in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.

Mathewson, J. H. (1999). Visual-spatial thinking: An aspect of scienceoverlooked by educators. Science Education, 83, 33–54. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(199901)83:1<33::AID-SCE2>3.0.CO;2-Z.

Matthews, J. (2007). Hidden sexism: Facial prominence and itsconnections to gender and occupational status in popularprint media. Sex Roles, 57, 515–525. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9276-3.

McCartney, K., & Rosenthal, R. (2000). Effect size, practical impor-tance, and social policy for children. Child Development, 71, 173–180. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00131.

Milton, G. A. (1957). The effects of sex-role identification uponproblem-solving skill. Journal of Abnormal and SocialPsychology, 55, 208–212. doi:10.1037/h0044103.

Nash, S. C. (1975). The relationship among sex-role stereotyping, sex-role preference, and the sex difference in spatial visualization. SexRoles, 1, 15–32. doi:10.1007/BF00287210.

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as mediator of intellectual functioning. InM. A. Wittig & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences incognitive functioning: Developmental issues (pp. 263–302). NewYork: Academic.

National Science Foundation. (2011). Women, minorities, and personswith disabilities in science and engineering: 2011. Arlington:National Science Foundation. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/wmpd/pdf/wmpd2011.pdf.

Neisser, U. C., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N.,Ceci, S. J., et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns.American Psychologist, 51, 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77.

Neumann, D. L., Fitzgerald, Z. T., Furedy, J. J., & Boyle, G. J. (2007).Sexually dimorphic effects of acute nicotine administration onarousal and visual-spatial ability in non-smoking human volun-teers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 86, 758–765.doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.001.

Neumann, D. L., Sturm, A. C., Boyle, G. J., & Furedy, J. J. (2010).Effects of nicotine administration via a sublingual tablet on arous-al and verbal ability in non-smokers. Australian Journal ofPsychology, 62, 75–81. doi:10.1080/00049530902795458.

Newcombe, N. S. (2007). Taking science seriously: Straight thinkingabout spatial sex differences. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren’t morewomen in science?: top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 69–77). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Newcombe, N. S., & Dubas, J. S. (1992). A longitudinal study of pre-dictors of spatial ability in adolescent females. Child Development,63, 37–46. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03593.x.

Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatialintelligence: Why, what, and how. Mind, Brain, and Education,4, 102–111. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2010.01089.x.

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male,me= female, therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 83, 44–59. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44.

Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T.,Ayala, A., et al. (2009). National differences in gender–sciencestereotypes predict national sex differences in science and mathachievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,106, 10593–10597. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809921106.

Nuttall, R. L., Casey, M. B., & Pezaris, E. (2005). Spatial ability as amediator of gender differences on mathematics tests: Abiological-environmental framework. In A. M. Gallagher & J.C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An inte-grative psychological approach (pp. 121–142). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis.Journal of Educational Statistics, 8, 157–159.

Oswald, D. L. (2008). Gender stereotypes and women’s reports ofliking and ability in traditionally masculine and feminine occupa-tions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 196–203. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00424.x.

Pajares, F., & Miller, D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-conceptbeliefs in mathematical problem solving: A path analysis. Journalof Educational Psychology, 86, 193–203. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.193.

Peters, M., Lehmann, W., Takahira, S., Takeuchi, Y., & Jordan, K.(2006). Mental rotation test performance in four cross-culturalsamples (N= 3367): Overall sex differences and the role of aca-demic program in performance. Cortex, 42, 1005–1014.doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70206-5.

Piaget, J. (1968). Six psychological studies. New York: Vintage Books.Popiel, E. M., & De Lisi, R. (1984). An examination of spatial ability

in relation to factors from the Bem Sex-Role Inventory.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 131–136.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 261: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender and academic abilitiesand preferences. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.),Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 297–316).New York: Springer.

Rahman, Q., Wilson, G. D., & Abrahams, S. (2004). Biosocialfactors, sexual orientation and neurocognitive functioning.Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 867–881. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00154-9.

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities.PLoS One, 7, e39904. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.

Ritter, D. (2004). Gender role orientation and performance on stereo-typically feminine and masculine cognitive tasks. Sex Roles, 50,583–591. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000023077.91248.f7.

Robison-Awana, P., Kehle, T. J., & Jenson, W. R. (1986). But whatabout smart girls? Adolescent self-esteem and sex role percep-tions as a function of academic achievement. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 78, 179–183. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.78.3.179.

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for nullresults. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638.

Rosenthal, R. (1984). Meta-analytic procedures for social research.Beverly Hills: Sage.

Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. PsychologicalBulletin, 118, 183–192. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183.

Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent develop-ments in quantitativemethods for literature reviews.Annual Review ofPsychology, 52, 59–82. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59.

Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purposedisplay of magnitude of experimental effect. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 74, 166–169. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.74.2.166.

Rosenthal, L., London, B., Levy, S., & Lobel, M. (2011). The roles ofperceived identity compatibility and social support for women ina single-sex STEM program at a co-educational university. SexRoles, 65, 725–736. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9945-0.

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender devel-opment. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.),Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 3. Social, emotional,and personality development, pp. 858–932). Hoboken: Wiley.

Saucier, D. M., McCreary, D. R., & Saxberg, J. K. J. (2002). Doesgender role socialization mediate sex differences in mental rota-tions? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1101–1111.doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00106-4.

Scarbrough, P. S., & Johnston, V. S. (2005). Individual differences inwomen’s facial preferences as a function of digit ratio and mentalrotation ability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 509–526.doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.002.

Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threateffects on women’s math performance. Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 38, 194–201. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1500.

Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children’splay preferences and patterns of cognitive performance.Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 315–316. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(79)90050-X.

Serbin, L. A., Zelkowitz, P., Doyle, A. B., Gold, D., & Wheaton, B.(1990). The socialization of sex-differentiated skills and academicperformance: A mediational model. Sex Roles, 23, 613–628.doi:10.1007/BF00289251.

Shea, D. L., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2001). Importance ofassessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adoles-cents: A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of EducationalPsychology, 93, 604–614. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.93.3.604.

Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703. doi:10.1126/science.171.3972.701.

Sherman, J. A. (1967). Problem of sex differences in space perceptionand aspects of intellectual functioning. Psychological Review, 74,290–299. doi:10.1037/h0024723.

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1978). Sex differences in thecorrelations among field dependence, spatial ability, sex roleorientation, and performance on Piaget’s water-level task.Developmental Psychology, 14, 689–690.

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androg-yny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. PsychologicalBulletin, 100, 207–228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207.

Signorella, M. L., Jamison, W., & Krupa, M. H. (1989). Predictingspatial performance from gender stereotyping in activity prefer-ences and in self-concept. Developmental Psychology, 25, 89–95.doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.89.

Silverman, I., Choi, J., & Peters, M. (2007). The Hunter-Gatherertheory of sex differences in spatial abilities: Data from 40 coun-tries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 261–268. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6.

Spence, J. T. (1984). Gender identity and its implications for mascu-linity and femininity. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), NebraskaSymposium on Motiuation: Psychology and Gender (pp. 59–95).Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Spence, J. T. (1993). Gender-related traits and gender ideology: Evidencefor a multifactorial theory. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 64, 624–635. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.624.

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits,trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: What do they signify?Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 44–62. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x.

Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The PersonalAttributes Questionnaire: A measure of sex role stereotypes andmasculinity-femininity. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents inPsychology, 4, 127.

Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1975). Ratings of self andpeers on sex-role attributes and their relation to self-esteem andconceptions of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 32, 29–39. doi:10.1037/h0076857.

Steffens, M., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereo-types regarding math and language: Implicit ability stereotypesare self-serving for boys and men, but not for girls and women.Sex Roles, 64, 324–335. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x.

Stein, A. H., & Bailey, M. M. (1973). The socialization of achievementorientation in females. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 345–366.doi:10.1037/h0035261.

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, mathematical, spatial and verbal intelligence:Four meta analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21,493–504. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001.

Taylor, M. C., & Hall, J. A. (1982). Psychological androgyny:Theories, methods, and conclusions. Psychological Bulletin, 92,347–366. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.347.

Thurstone, T. G. (1958). SRA Primary Mental Abilities (test). Chicago:Science Research Associates.

Titze, C., Jansen, P., & Heil, M. (2010). Mental rotation performancein fourth graders: No effects of gender beliefs (yet?). Learningand Individual Differences, 20, 459–463. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.04.003.

Tracy, D. M. (1987). Toy-playing habits, spatial ability, science andmathematics achievement: Are they related? Sex Roles, 17, 115–138. doi:10.1007/BF00287620.

Tuttle, G. E., & Pillard, R. C. (1991). Sexual orientation and cognitiveabilities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20, 307–318. doi:10.1007/BF01541849.

Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a grouptest of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 47, 599–604. doi:10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 262: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Vonnahme, P. (2005). Facial preferences of Caucasian males as afunction of 2D:4D digit ratio and mental rotation ability(Unpublished dissertation). New Mexico State University, LasCrues, New Mexico.

Voyer, D., & Bryden, M. (1990). Mental rotation: A study of genderdifferences and sex role identity. Canadian Psychology, 31, 253.

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sexdifferences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and considerationof critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.doi:10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250.

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability forSTEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psycho-logical knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 101, 817–835. doi:10.1037/a0016127.

Whitley, B. E. (1983). Sex role orientation and self-esteem: A criticalmeta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 44, 765–778. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.765.

Whitley, B. E. (1988). Masculinity, femininity, and self-esteem: Amultitrait-multimethod analysis. Sex Roles, 18, 419–431.doi:10.1007/bf00288393.

Wilkinson, L. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals:Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594–604. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594.

Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2007). Introduction: Striving forperspective in the debate on women and science. In S. J. Ceci(Ed.), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchersdebate the evidence (pp. 3–23). Washington, D.C.: AmericanPsychological Association.

Sex Roles

Author's personal copy

Page 263: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 137

Meta-Analysis Summary and Prelude to Empirical Studies

The previous set of chapters sought to examine whether the previously observed

sex differences in specific cognitive abilities (verbal and quantitative reasoning) would

still exist in contemporary samples, or alternatively, if they’d been eliminated as

claimed by Feingold (1988), Hyde (2005), as well as Caplan and Caplan (1997, 2016).

Additionally, it sought to contextualise that difference by evaluating the magnitude of

observed sex differences, and determine if they were large enough to have practical

importance. For quantitative reasoning, small but not trivial mean sex differences were

found for mathematics and more substantial gender gaps in high achievers. Somewhat

larger mean sex differences were found for science achievement, and again a sharp

disparity in the tail ratios for high achievers. For verbal and language abilities,

substantial sex differences were found for reading and writing with a developmental

trend observed with age/years of schooling. Examination of tail ratios also showed

substantial gender gaps in low- and high- achievers.

Collectively these studies provided a rationale for further investigation to

evaluate support for the sex-role mediation hypothesis. Signorella and Jamison (1986)

had conducted a meta-analysis on the association between masculinity and visual-

spatial ability, but the literature was now dated. For this reason, we produced a meta-

analysis of the association between masculine sex-role identification and visual-spatial

ability with more recently collected data. Additionally, this was useful in

contextualising the expected effect size for statistical power calculations in the

empirical study that follows.

Page 264: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 138

Chapter 8 – Empirical Study 1 – Sex and Sex-Role Differences in Specific

Cognitive Abilities

“If women are expected to do the same work as men, we must teach them the

same things.” – Plato, The Republic.

This study reports the empirical study into sex and sex-role differences in verbal

and visual-spatial abilities. Specifically it tests Nash’s (1979) sex-role mediation

hypothesis in a modern sample, finding support for both predicted tranches (verbal and

visual-spatial ability) across a range of tasks. This has been published as:

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in

specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147-158. doi:

10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Permission for inclusion of the final paper has been granted by the publisher, Elsevier.

In accordance with the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of

Research, a statement of contribution is provided for authorship of this paper. I

acknowledge the contribution of my supervisors to this manuscript.

My contribution involved: Data collection from archival sources Statistical Analysis Writing chapter (Signed) ______________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 David Reilly (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18

Primary Supervisor David L. Neumann (Countersigned) ________________________________ (Date) : 1/12/18 Associate Supervisor Glenda Andrews

Page 265: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Intelligence

Sex and sex-role differences in specific cognitive abilities

David Reilly a,⁎, David L. Neumann a, Glenda Andrews a,b

a School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University, Queensland, Australiab Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Queensland, Australia

⁎ Corresponding author at: School of Applied PsychologQueensland 4222, Australia.

E-mail address: [email protected] (D. Reilly).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.0040160-2896/Crown Copyright © 2015 Published by Elsevie

a b s t r a c t

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 18 August 2015Received in revised form 11 December 2015Accepted 14 December 2015Available online xxxx

Sex differences in cognitive abilities are a controversial but actively researched topic. The present study examinedwhether sex-role identity mediates the relationship between sex and sex-typed cognitive abilities. Threehundred nine participants (105 males and 204 females) were tested on a range of visuospatial and languagetasks under laboratory conditions. Participants also completed measures of sex-role identity, used to classifythem into masculine, feminine, androgynous and undifferentiated groups. While sex differences were foundfor some but not all measures, significant sex-role differences were found for all spatial and language measureswith the exception of a novel 2D Mental Rotation Task. Masculine sex-roles partially mediated the relationshipbetween sex and a compositemeasure of spatial ability, while feminine sex-roles fully mediated the relationshipbetween sex and a composite measure of language ability. These results suggest that sex-role identity may havegreater utility in explaining individual differences in cognitive performance than biological sex alone.

Crown Copyright © 2015 Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords:Sex differencesSex-role mediation hypothesisSpatial abilityVerbal ability

The topic of sex differences in cognitive abilities remains an activebut controversial research question because of its educational, socialand public policy implications (Eagly & Mitchell, 2004; Halpern,2014). While most reviews find that males and females do not differin general intelligence (Halpern, Beninger, & Straight, 2011; Jensen,1998; cf. Nyborg, 2015) sex differences are frequently found in specificcognitive abilities (Nisbett et al., 2012). Robust and sizeable sexdifferences are found for visuospatial ability (referred herein as spatialability) and verbal ability (Miller & Halpern, 2013). Overall, males dobetter on spatial tasks such as mental rotation and spatial perception(Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995), while females do better on languagetasks such as verbal fluency and grammar (Halpern & Lamay, 2000;Lynn, 1992). The effect sizes are moderately large, and are reflected inbeliefs about gender differences in cognitive ability (Halpern, Straight,& Stephenson, 2011).

Spatial and verbal skills are of particular interest to educational re-searchers for two reasons. Firstly, research suggests that spatial abilityforms the basis for the development of sex differences in quantitativereasoning such as mathematics and science (Newcombe & Frick, 2010;Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009). Despite significant progress in closingthe gender gap, meaningful sex differences in mathematics and scienceachievement persist, at least for students in the USA (McGraw,Lubienski, & Strutchens, 2006; Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews, 2015).This is an active area of research, given the underrepresentation ofwomen in science, technology, engineering and mathematics(collectively referred to as STEM) fields (National Science Foundation,

y, Griffith University, Southport,

r Inc. All rights reserved.

2011). Furthermore, international assessments of student achievementsuch as the OECD's Programme for International Student Assessment(PISA) also find sex differences in mathematics and science for some,but not for all, nations (Else-Quest, Hyde, & Linn, 2010; Guiso, Monte,Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008; Reilly, 2012). Secondly, verbal ability andlanguage competence are essential life skills required for full participa-tion in society and the workforce. Both within the United States, andcross-culturally, males consistently score significantly lower thanfemales on tests of reading and writing (Guiso et al., 2008; Klecker,2006; Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Reilly, 2012). Some researchers havespeculated that this contributes to the growing trend acrossmostWest-ern nations of fewermen thanwomen entering and completing tertiaryeducation (Alon&Gelbgiser, 2011; Buchmann&DiPrete, 2006). Thirdly,both spatial and verbal abilities are specific cognitive abilities that arefrequently investigated by sex researchers, and emerge as distinct sep-arate factors of intelligence (Johnson & Bouchard, 2007).

1. Theoretical perspectives on sex-typed cognitive abilities

When sex differences are observed by researchers, this raisesquestions regarding their origins (Wood & Eagly, 2000). Early researchinto sex differences in cognitive abilities focused primarily onbiologically-based explanations, including the contribution of hor-mones (Auyeung et al., 2009; Hines, 1990; Kimura & Hampson, 1994)and anatomical structures such as the corpus callosum (Hines, Chiu,McAdams, Bentler, & Lipcamon, 1992). One argument supporting sucha view is the observation of greater male variability (Feingold, 1992;Machin & Pekkarinen, 2008), leading to exaggerated sex differences atthe extreme tails of the ability distribution. While sex differences inthe extremely gifted is an important topic in its own right, as they

Page 266: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

148 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

related to a relatively small percentage of thepopulation, themajority ofsex difference research is concernedwithmean sex differences betweenmales and females as a group. Empirical studies into the effect of hor-mones on cognition find mixed support (cf. Halari et al., 2005; Kimura& Hampson, 1994), and that they explain only a small percentage ofvariance. In recent decades explanations have broadened to incorporatesociocultural factors, such as differences between boys' and girls' earlysocialization experiences (Lytton & Romney, 1991), differential parentalexpectations for sons and daughters (Eccles, Jacobs, & Harold, 1990;Furnham, Reeves, & Budhani, 2002), gender stereotypes (Archer,1992; Shapiro & Williams, 2012), and cultural beliefs (Guiso et al.,2008; Reilly, 2012). Most researchers now accept that sex differencesare influenced by a network of biological and sociocultural factors ratherthan any single factor (Ceci, Williams, & Barnett, 2009; Nisbett et al.,2012; Wood & Eagly, 2012).

2. Sex role mediation of cognitive abilities

While it is difficult to disentangle nature from nurture, a commonal-ity that is shared by both is that they contribute towards the develop-ment of an individual's sex-role identity or the degree to which anindividual embodies stereotypically masculine and feminine personali-ty traits, behaviors, and interests (Bem, 1981b; Spence & Buckner,2000). Though boys and girls as two distinct groups will differ in theirearly socialization experiences (Lytton & Romney, 1991; Martin &Ruble, 2004), there is considerable individual variationwithin each gen-der group in the degree to which a person acquires sex-typed traits.While some children become rigidly sex-typed, others incorporate ele-ments of both masculinity and femininity into their persona (Wood &Eagly, 2015). Highly sex-typed individuals are motivated to keep theirbehavior and self-concept consistent with traditional gender norms(Bem & Lenney, 1976; Martin & Ruble, 2004), including the sex-typingof specific skills, interests, and cognitive abilities.

Nash (1979) proposed the sex-role mediation hypothesis as one suchexplanation for the origins of sex differences in specific cognitiveabilities. Nash (1979, p. 263) wrote “For some people, cultural mythsare translated into personality beliefs which can affect cognitive func-tioning in sex-typed intellectual domains”. This argument was basedon earlier work by Sherman (1967) into differential learning andpractice experiences of boys and girls. Under the sex-rolemediation hy-pothesis, masculine identification promotes the development of spatialreasoning and mathematics, while feminine identification promotesverbal ability and language aptitude (see Fig. 1). Essentially, the sex-role mediation hypothesis proposes that group differences in cognitiveabilities emerge as a result of individual differences in sex-roleidentification (Durkin, 1987).

There is evidence to support sex-role mediation, at least for the de-velopment of spatial ability. Reilly and Neumann (2013) conducted ameta-analysis of the association between masculinity and mentalrotation (the most commonly used measure of spatial ability), findinga robust association for both males and females. However, it is unclearwhether such an association generalizes to other types of spatial ability

Fig. 1. Nash's (1979) sex-role mediat

such as spatial perception and visualization. An earlier review bySignorella and Jamison (1986) found an association with these typesof measures, but it is unclear whether a similar result would be foundin modern samples. Furthermore, few studies have investigated thesecond aspect of Nash's sex-role mediation hypothesis, namely thatfeminine identification promotes the development of reading andlanguage skills. Indeed, Signorella and Jamison noted that there was “apaucity of studies” (p. 219) that provide a test of sex-role mediationwith language tasks.

3. The present study

The aim of the present study is to test the sex-role mediation hy-pothesis across a broader range of spatial and verbal tasks than previ-ously used by researchers. There have also been considerable changesin the roles ofmen andwomenwith the passage of time, so it is arguablewhether historical conceptualizations ofmasculinity and femininity stillapply (Auster & Ohm, 2000; Hoffman & Borders, 2001). Furthermore,some researchers have claimed that the magnitude of sex differencesis diminishing in response to these social changes (Priess & Hyde,2010). However, implicit gender stereotypes about sex-typing of cogni-tive tasks as being either masculine or feminine remain strong (Martin& Ruble, 2004; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002), as do beliefs aboutcognitive sex differences (Halpern, Straight, et al., 2011). We set out todetermine whether previous experimental studies finding evidence ofsex-role mediation (e.g. Hamilton, 1995) would be replicated whenrecruiting from a modern sample of young adults.

Linn and Petersen (1985) categorized tests of spatial ability as fallinginto one of three domains: mental rotation, spatial perception, andspatial visualization. The largest sex differences are found in mental ro-tation, while spatial perception also shows appreciably large sex differ-ences (Voyer et al., 1995). However, the skill of spatial visualizationshows relatively small sex differences which are sometimes not statisti-cally significant, and so is less seldom included in a battery of cognitivemeasures. We selected measures from all three spatial domains (rota-tion, perception and visualization) so as to provide good content validityof spatial reasoning. We also employed a second test of mental rotationusing two dimensional objects as stimuli, as most mental rotation tasksemploy three dimensional objects at a cost of increased task difficulty.

The range of tasks available for measuring verbal ability is broad andless neatly defined than for spatial ability (Hyde & Linn, 1988). Sex dif-ferences in verbal fluency are apparent early in development (Halpern& Lamay, 2000), and are moderate in size (Hines, 1990). We selectedphonological verbal fluency for this purpose as it is a widely used cogni-tive measure in psychological research. We also included a synonymgeneration task, which requires participants to generate words thatare similar in meaning (associational fluency). Sex difference re-searchers have also found large sex differences in reading comprehen-sion and writing (Lynn, 1992), and so we also included a measure ofreading and grammatical skills known to produce moderately largesex differences (Stanley, Benbow, Brody, Dauber, & Lupkowski, 1992).

ion theory of cognitive abilities.

Page 267: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

149D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

Finally, we administered sex-role instruments to allow a test of thesex-role mediation hypothesis. The primary focus was on personalitytraits asmeasured by the Bem Sex-Role Inventory. This instrument pro-vides a masculine and feminine scale, which on the basis of a mediansplit classifies participants into one of four categories: masculine (M),feminine (F), androgynous (high M, high F) and undifferentiated (lowM, low F). We also included additional sex-role measures to determinewhich aspects of sex-role identity (personality traits, behavioral actions,or social identity) best predicted performance on the spatial and verbalability tasks.

4. Hypotheses

1. Consistent with past research, we hypothesized that males wouldperform higher than females on all spatial ability measures.

2. Participants with highermasculinity scores (masculine and androgy-nous groups) will perform better on spatial ability tasks than partic-ipantwith lowermasculinity scores (feminine and undifferentiated),consistent with the sex-role mediation hypothesis (Nash, 1979).

3. Regression analysis with the three sex-role measures will determinewhich aspect of sex-role identity (personality, behavior, or socialidentity) is the best predictor of spatial ability.

4. We hypothesized that sex differences in spatial ability are mediatedby masculine sex-role identity.

5. Consistent with past research, we hypothesized that females wouldperform higher than males on all verbal ability measures.

6. Participants scoring high on femininity (feminine and androgynousgroups) will perform better on verbal tasks than participants scoringlow in femininity (masculine and undifferentiated), consistent withthe sex-role mediation hypothesis.

7. Regression analysis with the three sex-role measures will determinewhich aspect of sex-role identity (personality, behavior, or socialidentity) is the best predictor of verbal ability.

8. We hypothesize that sex differences in verbal ability aremediated byfeminine sex-role identity.

5. Method

5.1. Participants

Three hundred and nine participants (105males, 204 females) wererecruited from a university subject-pool of psychology students, cur-rently completing a researchmethods course. Themajority of these stu-dents were enrolled in an undergraduate psychological science degree.The remainder were enrolled in other health or science programs inwhich the research methods course was required or recommended asan elective. The mean age was 25.46 years (SD = 8.03), and there wasno significant difference in age between male and female participants,t(304) = 1.21, p = .228. As the distribution of psychological sex-rolesis not even in college samples, recruiting a larger number of participantswas necessary to ensure a reasonable cell size for analysis in each of thefour sex-role categories. All participants provided informed consent to aprotocol approved by the Institutional Human Research EthicsCommittee.

5.2. Sex-role measures

The 30 item short form of the Bem Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem,1974, 1981a) was used to measure sex-role identity. The BSRI is a gen-eral personality inventory that incorporates 10 masculine, 10 feminine,and 10 neutral personality traits and items to detect social desirabilitybias. Each item is rated on a 7-point Likert scale (from “1 — never oralmost never true of me” to a midpoint of “4 — occasionally true” andto “7 — always or almost always true of me”). Separate masculine andfeminine scores are produced by averaging responses across scaleitems, resulting in two continuous variables for use in regression

analysis. Participants can also be categorized on the basis of a mediansplit into one of four sex-role categories, masculine, feminine, androgy-nous (high in masculinity and femininity) and undifferentiated (low inboth masculine and feminine traits).

With the passage of time since the original publication of the BemSex-Role Inventory, it is possible that prevailing gender norms andvalues may have shifted in the intervening period. If so, the genderednature of its test items may not reliably discriminate between the con-structs of masculinity and femininity for modern samples. Choi, Fuqua,and Newman (2007) investigated the factor structure of the BSRI in acollege sample, finding support for distinct masculine and femininefactors. Similar findings emerged in a confirmatory factor analysisacross both college and community samples, with the conclusion thatthe instrument remains valid with modern samples (Choi, Fuqua, &Newman, 2009). For a further discussion on the history and propertiesof this instrument, see Wood and Eagly's (2015) review.

A second measure of sex-role identity, the Personal AttributesQuestionnaire (PAQ; Spence, Helmreich, & Holahan, 1979) was alsoused. This is a 24-item self-report measure that includes a mixture ofeight stereotypically masculine and eight feminine personality traits.Participants rate themselves on a bipolar 5-point scale (e.g. “verypassive” versus “very active”). Although the BSRI and PAQ were highlycorrelated in our sample (r = .72 for masculinity, r = .83 for feminini-ty), they represent somewhat different conceptualizations of genderidentity and this distinction was used in regression analyses todetermine which measure was a stronger predictor of cognitive ability.

We also administered the identity subscale of the Collective Self-Esteem Scale (CSES; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992) which is a brief four-itemmeasure assessing gender-based social identity. Some researchersmake a distinction between gender identity based on sex-typedpersonality traits, and self-categorization by the individual (Wood &Eagly, 2015). Including a social identity measure makes it possible toexamine the relative strength of gender identity associations inpredicting intellectual performance. Items include “Overall, I feel thatthe gender group of which I am a member is not worthwhile” and “Ingeneral, I'm glad to be a member of the gender group I belong to”,with two items being negatively coded. Items are rated on a 7-pointLikert-type scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”.Higher scores indicate greater identification with one's biologicalgender.

5.3. Spatial cognitive measures

5.3.1. Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task (VMRT)A computer administered version of the mental rotation task was

employed. The stimuli were Vandenberg and Kuse's (1978) originalthree-dimensional (3D) stimuli which had been redrawn by Peterset al. (1995). On each trial, participants were presented with a target3D image and asked to select the two images (from the 4 options)that were rotated images of the target. Response times were measuredfrom onset of the target image until both selections were made. Thestandard Vandenberg scoring system awards a full mark for locatingboth of the rotated targets and no mark if one or none was identified(Peters et al., 1995). This scoring method discourages guessing and in-creases task difficulty. Participants completed a series of practice itemswith feedback. Participants were allocated 3 min to complete a blockof 12 items, followed by a brief rest period and then a second block of12 items. The time remaining was displayed for each block, and partic-ipants were instructed that accuracy was important, as an item wouldonly be scored correctly if both targets were located. The maximumscore for the test is 24 (Cronbach's α = .94 for the current sample).

5.3.2. 2D Mental Rotation Task (2DMRT)Previous research has used bar histograms as stimuli to test for sex

differences in mental rotation of two-dimensional (2D) stimuli. Weemployed the same stimuli used in Neumann, Fitzgerald, Furedy, and

Page 268: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table 1Distribution of sex-roles in the sample.

Sex-role classification

Gender Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated

Males 38 9 23 35(36.2%) (8.6%) (21.9%) (33.3%)

Females 44 44 58 58(21.6%) (21.6%) (28.4%) (28.4%)

150 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

Boyle (2007) as a computer administered task, recording reaction time(in ms) and accuracy. Participants were presented with two bar histo-grams, and given up to 5 s to correctly identify whether they matchedby pressing a key (an error was recorded in the event of non-response). Participants were given practice session containing 15items, followed by a randomized sequence of 40 actual test items (halfof which were matching, half of which did not match). The dependentvariable was the accuracy rate expressed as a percentage (current sam-ple Cronbach's α = .81).

5.3.3. Piaget Water Level Task (PWLT)Spatial perceptionwas assessed using a computer administered ver-

sion of the Piagetian Water Level Task (reviewed in Vasta & Liben,1996). Participants were presented with a 2D depiction of a containerin the centre of the screen, as well as a flat un-tilted table at the bottomof the screen to represent the horizontal plane. They were instructedthat the container would then be tilted (as shown on screen), and thatusing the computer mouse they should draw the waterline from astarting point on the right side of the container. The stimuli were depic-tions of equal-sized vessels containing varying volumes of liquid suchthat the vessel was 20%, 50% or 80% full. The vessels were tilted at anglesof 0, 20, 30, 40 or 50° from the horizontal. Participants were adminis-tered two trials of each angle (0°, 20°, 30°, 40°, 50°) in a randomsequence for a total of 10 trials, with varying heights of liquid (20%,50% or 80%). The dependent variable for this measure was the averageangular error from the horizontal plane across all trials (current sampleCronbach's α = .83). Additionally, participants rated their level ofconfidence on a 7-point scale for each trial ranging from “1 — notvery confident” to “7 — very confident” (current sample's Cronbach'sα = .95).

5.3.4. Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT)Spatial visualization was measured using the paper and pencil ver-

sion of the Group Embedded Figures Test (Witkin, Dyk, Faterson,Goodenough, & Karp, 1962). This task is thought to require respondentsto dis-embed a target object from a geometric background, and toisolate distracting stimuli (Linn & Petersen, 1985;Witkin, 2003). Partic-ipants are shown a series of complex geometric shapes and asked to lo-cate the target by tracing its outline. After completing a practice item,participants are given four minutes to complete a block of nine items.This was followed by a 1 min rest period and a second block of nineitems. Scoring was one point for a correct item, and zero for omittedor incorrect items, for a maximum score of 18 (Cronbach's α = .86 inthe current study).

5.4. Verbal cognitive measures

5.4.1. Phonological verbal fluencyParticipants are given a letter of the alphabet and asked to generate

andwrite down asmanywords as possible in 60 s. Theywere instructedthat only uniquewords were permitted (e.g. if run was given, then runsor running should not be given as answers), and that names of people,brand names, and places would be marked as incorrect. After a practicetrial, participants were given four letters in order of F, A, S, and C, one attime. Internal consistency was high for the sample (Cronbach's α =.89). The average number of words reported over the four trials wasthe dependent variable.

5.4.2. Synonym generation taskFollowing themethodology of Hines (1990) andHalpern andWright

(1996), participants were given stimulus words and asked to write asmany synonyms as possible within the time limit of 60 s per trial. Apractice trial with sample synonyms was presented first to ensure thatparticipants understood the task requirements. The six stimulus items(strong, dark, wild, sharp, turn and clear) were drawn from Hines(1990). Four online dictionaries (Oxford, Collins, Cambridge, and

Thesaurus.com) that offered comprehensive definitions of word mean-ings and lists of synonymswere used to determine whether the report-ed words were correct as synonyms of the stimulus words. One markwas awarded for each correct synonym. Internal consistency in thecurrent study was high, Cronbach's α = .90.

5.4.3. Differential aptitude test — language usage (DAT-L)This instrument measures an individual's ability to detect errors in

grammar, punctuation, and capitalization in written text. Participantsare presented with individual sentences of text and asked to identifywhere in the sentence the error is located. To discourage guessing,some items have no error present which the subject must also identifycorrectly. The test has a multiple-choice format with response options.It contains 30 items and a time limit of 10 min is imposed. As thereare regional differences between Australian and American English, theAustralian version of the DAT-L was used (Cronbach's α = .79 for thecurrent sample).

5.5. Procedure

Participants were advised that they were participating in a study oncognitive problem solving and personality traits and then undertook ei-ther the block of spatial (VMRT, 2DMRT, Piaget WLT, GEFT) or block ofverbal tasks (phonological verbal fluency, synonym generation task,DAT-L) with the presentation order of the spatial and verbal task blockscounterbalanced. A rest period of 4 min was given between task blocksto prevent fatigue. In order tominimize gender priming effects, the sex-role personality inventories and demographic information question-nairewere administered after the cognitive testinghad been completed.Participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation.

Statistical analysis was conducted using a series of factorial ANOVAs,and in order to avoid Type I error inflation resulting frommultiple com-parisons, a planned linear contrast was made based a priori on experi-mental hypotheses. Linear contrasts offer the advantage of increasedstatistical power by pooling two or more cells. When testing the effectof masculinity on spatial reasoning, a linear contrast compared highmasculinity groups (masculine and androgynous) with lowmasculinitygroups (feminine and undifferentiated). Similarly when testing theeffect of femininity on verbal reasoning, a linear contrast comparedthose scoring high on femininity (feminine and androgynous) withthose scoring low on femininity (masculine and undifferentiated).Regression analysis also investigated linear associations between sex-role measures and outcomes, overcoming the limitation of smallANOVA cell sizes. Mediation analysis was performed using the PROCESSmacro for SPSS (Preacher & Hayes, 2004).

6. Results

6.1. Sex-role classification

Participants were classified into one of four sex-role categories,based on a median-split of BSRI masculinity and femininity scores(Masculinity = 4.50, Femininity = 5.50). Table 1 presents the distribu-tion of males and females classified according to the four sex-roles,while Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for gender-related

Page 269: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table 2Mean and standard deviations across sex and sex-role groups for gender-related measures.

Measure Males Females

Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

BSRI masculinity 5.26 .51 3.98 .38 5.10 .39 3.88 .57 5.09 .44 3.68 .65 5.04 .42 3.83 .58BSRI femininity 4.48 .85 5.93 .26 6.01 .36 4.60 .87 4.67 .64 6.13 .39 6.07 .42 4.76 .58PAQ masculinity 3.93 .45 3.15 .71 3.86 .43 3.05 .66 3.69 .57 2.87 .72 3.58 .47 3.06 .59PAQ femininity 3.39 .60 4.25 .41 4.17 .31 3.59 .55 3.64 .52 4.41 .37 4.27 .34 3.75 .40Collective Self-Esteem Scale 6.51 .45 5.68 1.10 6.32 .87 5.71 1.23 6.03 1.13 5.56 1.71 6.24 1.05 5.92 .99

151D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

measures. A Chi-square test showed that sex-role classification wasdependent on sex of the participants, χ2(3) = 14.09, p = .003.

t-Tests for independent samples showed that masculinity scoreswere significantly higher for male than female participants, t(307) =2.44, p = .015, d = .25. As would be expected, females also scoredhigher on femininity scores than males, t(307) = −3.94, p b .001,d = −.40. A similar pattern of sex differences was found with the PAQmeasures for masculinity, t(307) = 3.14, p = .002, and for femininity,t(307) = −4.74, p b .001, which were highly correlated with BSRImasculinity (r = .72) and femininity (r = .83) scores.

Although there were no significant sex differences in CSES, t(307)=1.26, p= .210, there was a statistically significant difference across sex-role categories, F(3, 305)=5.76, p= .001, ηp2= .05,withmasculine andandrogynous participants scoring higher than feminine and undifferen-tiated participants, t(305) = 4.13, p b .001, d = .48.

6.2. Descriptive statistics for spatial and verbal measures

Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics partitioned across sex-rolecategories for all spatial measures, while Table 4 presents these forverbal measures. Bivariate correlations between all measures arereported in Table 5.

6.3. Visuospatial measures

6.3.1. Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task (VMRT)Performance on the VMRT was analyzed with a 2 (Sex) × 4 (Sex-

Role) factorial ANOVA (see Fig. 2). Significant main effects of sex, F(1,298) = 17.26, p b .001, ηp2 = .06, and of sex-role, F(3, 298) = 5.31,p = .001, ηp2 = .05, were found. The interaction between sex and sex-role was not significant, F(3, 298) = 1.09, p = .356. As can be seen inFig. 2, males scored higher in overall accuracy than females, t(304) =4.16, p b .001, d = .48. The planned linear contrast showed that partic-ipants classified as masculine and androgynous scored higher thanthose classified as feminine and undifferentiated, t(304) = 3.51,p b .001, d = .40.

To explorewhether the groupdifferenceswere the result of a speed–accuracy trade off, we also examined reaction timeswith a 2 (Sex)× 4 ×(Sex-Role) factorial ANOVA. There was no significant main effect of sex,F(1, 298)= 3.17, p= .076, ηp2 = 01. Males and females did not differ inthe amount of time spent on items. However, there was a statisticallysignificant main effect of sex-role, F(3, 298) = 4.65, p = .003, ηp2 =.05 (see Fig. 3). Contrary to that expected by a speed–accuracytrade off, feminine and undifferentiated participants took more timeon the items than the masculine and androgynous participants,t(304) = −3.35, p = .001, d = −.39. The interaction was notsignificant.

6.3.2. Mental Rotation Task (2D)We investigated the percentage accuracy and reaction time (RT)

across trials for the Mental Rotation Task (2D) with separate 2(Sex) × 4 (Sex-Role) factorial ANOVAs. On inspection of the histogramfor percentage accuracy, there appeared to be evidence of a ceilingeffect with a large percentage of participants making few or no errors

in judgment (median = 90%). As the distribution was extremelynegatively skewed, a logarithmic reflectionwas applied. Contrary to ex-pectations, there was no significantmain effect of sex, F(1, 298)= 1.11,p = .292, or sex-role, F(3, 298) = .28, p = .839, nor was there asignificant interaction. Likewise, there were no group differences forreaction times, and no further analysis was undertaken for this task.

6.3.3. Piaget Water Level Task (PWLT)Angular error and confidence on the PWLT were analyzed with 2

(Sex) × 4 (Sex-Role) factorial ANOVAs. The distribution of angular er-rors was positively skewed with some heterogeneity of variance, andaccordingly a square root transformation was applied. However as theoutcome of all ANOVA tests did not differ, the untransformed dataare reported. As predicted there was a significant main effect of sex,F(1, 298) = 4.62, p = .032, ηp2 = .02, with less angular error for males.There was also the predicted main effect of sex-role, F(3, 298) = 9.89,p b .001, ηp2 = .09 (see Fig. 3). The interaction between these factorswas not significant, F(3, 298) = 1.16, p = .325. The planned contrastshowed that masculine and androgynous participants showed lessangular error than feminine and undifferentiated participantst(305) = −5.01, p b .001, d = −.58.

Additionally, males reported greater confidence in estimating theangle than females, F(1, 298) = 11.94, p = .001, ηp2 = .04, and therewas also a significant main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 298) =5.13, p = .002, ηp2 = .05. The interaction was non-significant, F(3,298) = .39, p = .753. The planned linear contrast showed thatmasculine and androgynous participants had higher self-confidenceratings on the Piaget task than feminine and undifferentiatedparticipants, t(305) = 3.59, p b .001, d = .41.

6.3.4. Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT) performancePerformance on the GEFT was analyzed with a 2 (Sex) × 4 (Sex-

Role) factorial ANOVA. Surprisingly, therewas no significantmain effectfor sex, F(1, 300) = .05, p = .828, ηp2 = .00. As predicted, there wassignificant main effect of sex-role identity, F(3, 300) = 4.75, p = .003,ηp2 = .05. Results of the planned contrast showed masculine andandrogynous participants scored higher overall for the GEFT thanfeminine and undifferentiated participants, t(306) = 3.43, p = .001,d = .39. The interaction between sex and sex-role was non-significant, F(3, 300) = .85, p = .467, ηp2 = .01 (see Fig. 4).

6.3.5. Sex-role mediation of spatial abilityIn order to perform a more detailed regression analysis of the sex-

role mediation hypothesis and minimize the need for multiplecomparisons, we first converted scores on each of the spatial tasksinto standardized z-scores. Next, because all spatial performancemeasures were significantly correlated (see Table 5), we calculatedthe mean standardized score for each participant. The resulting com-posite score was used as the criterion variable for spatial ability.

We then performed a hierarchical multiple regression on spatialability (see Table 6). Sex was entered as the sole predictor in Step 1,followed by the two sex-role measures (BSRI and PAQ) for masculinityand femininity, as well as the CSES in Step 2. Although only masculinitywas hypothesized to make a contribution to spatial performance,

Page 270: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table3

Meanan

dstan

dard

deviations

across

sexan

dsex-role

grou

psforsp

atialm

easu

res.

Spatialm

easu

reMales

Females

Mascu

line

Feminine

And

rogy

nous

Und

ifferen

tiated

Mascu

line

Feminine

And

rogy

nous

Und

ifferen

tiated

MSD

MSD

MSD

MSD

MSD

MSD

MSD

MSD

VMRT

Score

13.84

4.37

9.67

6.12

12.24

4.85

10.83

5.10

10.21

3.19

7.39

3.80

9.52

4.63

9.65

4.36

VMRT

RT(m

s)16

,163

3454

21,924

8144

17,807

4752

17,443

5165

18,404

5057

22,391

8616

18,312

5248

20,229

7685

2DMRT

Score

89.40

11.17

87.00

12.96

89.14

13.25

86.74

12.19

85.74

12.06

83.17

13.57

86.89

15.37

88.72

10.59

2DMRT

RT(m

s)22

3466

224

3080

521

7664

126

3372

524

0367

526

7798

124

6368

923

3758

1PW

LTan

gle

3.45

3.97

10.20

11.78

3.97

4.35

9.85

9.20

7.49

4.94

10.25

7.41

7.19

5.49

10.58

8.58

PWLT

confi

denc

e5.77

1.09

5.30

1.67

5.91

1.01

5.14

1.24

5.10

.90

4.65

1.38

5.38

.94

4.84

1.23

GEF

TScore

11.95

3.99

10.44

5.13

11.09

4.55

9.20

5.24

11.73

4.12

8.64

4.09

11.67

4.26

10.14

4.17

Note:

VMRT

=Van

denb

ergMen

talR

otationTa

sk;2

DMRT

=2D

Men

talR

otationTa

sk;P

WLT

=Piag

etW

ater

Leve

lTask;

GEF

T=

Group

Embe

dded

Figu

resTe

st.

152 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

femininity was included to rule out the possibility of a significantnegative association. Assumptions of normality of residuals, linearityof associations, absence of multi-collinearity and homoscedasticitywere met. At Step 1, sex made a significant contribution to spatial abil-ity, Fchg(1, 306) = 14.82, p b .001, accounting for 4.6% of the variance inspatial ability. Step 2 introduced the sex-role measures, Fchg(6, 301) =14.53, p b .001, Rchg2 = .19, explaining a total of 23.2% of the variancein spatial ability. While both BSRI and PAQ masculinity had significantbivariate correlations, only BSRI made a significant unique contribution(β= .33, p b .001), with a large portion of the variance in spatial abilitybeing shared by the BSRI and PAQ masculinity measures. Furthermore,there was no significant association between femininity scores andspatial performance. Additionally, there was a small contribution ofcollective self-esteem to spatial ability, but this failed to achieve statisti-cal significance (β = .10, p = .068).

Because we had established that masculinity made a significantcontribution even after controlling for the effect of biological sex, wenext sought to test whether sex-role identity was acting as a statisticalmediator. Baron and Kenny (1986) offered a formal set of criteria fortesting statistical mediation. Firstly there should be a significant associ-ation between the predictor variable (biological sex) and the outcome(spatial ability) (β=−.22, p b .001). Secondly the relationship betweenthe predictor and the mediator (masculine sex-roles) was significant(β = −.14, p = .015, Path A). Thirdly the association between themediator and the outcome should still be significant after controllingfor the effect of sex (β = −.41, p b .001, Path B), as shown in Fig. 5.We further tested this model using the Sobel test whichwas also signif-icant, Z=−2.25, p= .025 and calculation of the bootstrapped estimateof the indirect effect showed that it differed significantly from zero inthat the confidence intervals did not span zero (95%CI=−.16 to−.02).

Having established these necessary preconditions for mediation, wetested whether the relationship was partially or fully mediated. In a fullmediation model, the association between predictor variable and theoutcome will become zero and non-significant after controlling for theeffect of themediator (Path C). If the predictor variable still makes a di-rect contribution to the outcome even after controlling for themediator,it can be said to be only partially mediated. Though the beta weight wassignificantly diminished after controlling for the mediator, there wasstill a significant association between biological sex and spatial ability(β=−.16, p= .002). In support of the sex-role mediation hypothesis,the relationship between sex and spatial ability was partially mediatedby sex-roles, but sex also made a direct contribution to spatial ability.

6.4. Verbal and language measures

6.4.1. Phonological verbal fluency taskThemean number of words reported across trials was analyzedwith

a 2 (Sex) × 4 (Sex-Role) factorial ANOVA (see Fig. 6). We did not findthe expected main effect of sex in our sample, F(1, 298) = .07, p =.785, ηp2 = .00. However, the predicted main effect of sex-role wasfound, F(3, 298) = 5.57, p = .001, ηp2 = .05. Planned contrasts showedthat participants classified as having masculine and undifferentiatedsex roles wrote fewer words than those classified as having feminineand androgynous sex-roles, t(304) = −4.03, p b .001, d = −.46. Theinteraction between sex and sex-role fell short of statistical significance,F(3, 298) = 2.46, p = .063, ηp2 = .02.

6.4.2. Synonym generation taskThe total number of synonyms generated across trials was analyzed

with a 2 (Sex) × 4 (Sex-Role) factorial ANOVA (see Fig. 7). There wasslight positive skewness, so a square root transformation was appliedbefore analysis. This did not change the outcome, so the results of theANOVA on the untransformed data are reported. As with the phonolog-ical fluency task, the predicted main effect of sex was not found, F(1,298) = .29, p = .592, ηp2 = .00. However, there was the expectedmain effect of sex-role category, F(3, 298) = 5.65, p = .001, ηp2 = .05,

Page 271: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table 4Mean and standard deviations across sex and sex-role groups for verbal measures.

Males Females

Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated

Verbal measure M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Phonological Verbal Fluency 10.92 3.36 14.42 1.84 13.91 3.02 11.66 3.65 12.26 2.82 13.35 3.95 12.56 3.17 12.26 3.10Synonym Generation 3.26 1.33 4.94 2.08 3.76 1.90 3.34 1.61 3.50 1.24 4.66 1.94 4.01 2.24 3.64 1.39Language Usage 19.71 5.60 23.67 4.44 22.65 2.93 19.51 5.80 21.67 4.18 23.28 3.92 22.67 3.59 22.55 3.68

153D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

with no interaction between sex and sex-role, F(3, 298)= .23, p= .875,ηp2 = .00. Feminine and androgynous participants generated more syn-onyms thanmasculine and undifferentiated participants, t(304)=3.89,p b .001, d = −.45.

6.4.3. Differential aptitude test — language usageOverall performance on the DAT was analyzed with a 2 (Sex) × 4

(Sex-Role) factorial ANOVA (see Fig. 8). As predicted, there weresignificant main effects of sex, F(1, 298) = 3.97, p = .047, ηp2 = .01,and of sex-role, F(3, 298) = 4.91, p= .002, ηp2 = .05. Planned contrastsshowed females had higher scores than males, t(304) = −1.99, p =.047, d = −.23, while feminine and androgynous participantsscored higher than masculine and undifferentiated participantst(304) = −3.80, p b .001, d = −.44. The interaction between sex andsex-role fell short of statistical significance, F(3, 298) = 2.12, p = .097,ηp2 = .02.

6.4.4. Sex-role mediation of verbal abilityIn order to test statistical mediation for verbal ability, we converted

performance on verbal measures into standardized z-scores. As all ver-bal measures were significantly correlated (see Table 4), we calculatedthemean standardized score for each participant across the three verbalmeasures and used this as the criterion variable. Next we performed ahierarchical regression analysis to determine which measures of sex-role identity best predicted performance on language tasks (seeTable 7). Assumptions of normality of residuals, linearity of associations,absence of multicollinearity and homoscedasticity were met. Sex wasentered as the sole predictor in Step 1, Fchg(1, 306) = 7.82, p = .005,

Table 5Bivariate correlations between sex, sex-roles, and cognitive measures of spatial and verbal abil

Measure 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

1. Biological sex – −.14⁎ .22⁎⁎⁎ −.18⁎⁎ .26⁎⁎⁎ −.07 −.29⁎⁎⁎

2. BSRI masculinity – −.01 .72⁎⁎⁎ −.13⁎ .25⁎⁎⁎ .29⁎⁎⁎ −3. BSRI femininity – −.07 .83⁎⁎⁎ .03 −.13⁎

4. PAQ masculinity – −.14⁎ .33⁎⁎⁎ .26⁎⁎⁎ −5. PAQ femininity – −.02 −.13⁎

6. Collective Self–EsteemScale

– .17⁎⁎ −

7. VMRT Score – −8. VMRT time –9. Piaget WLT AngularError

10. Piaget WLTConfidence Rating

11. Group EmbeddedFigures Test

12. Verbal fluency13. Synonym generation14. Differential aptitudetest— language usage

15. Spatial compositescore

16. Verbal compositescore

⁎ p b .05.⁎⁎ p b .01.⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

explaining approximately 2.5% of the variance in language ability. AtStep 2, we added the BEM and PAQ measures of sex-roles, as well asCSES. Although only femininity was hypothesized to make a contribu-tion to language performance, masculinity was included in the regres-sion model to rule out the possibility of a significant negativeassociation. The revised model explained 23% of the variance inlanguage ability, Fchg(5, 301)=16.05, p b .001, Rchg2 = .21. The BSRI fem-ininity scale (β= .25, p= .007) and the PAQ femininity scale (β= .20,p= .029) each made significant a unique contributions, although therewas a considerable overlap between these instruments. BSRI femininitywas the stronger predictor, so it was used in themediation analysis. Im-portantly,masculinity did not contribute to language ability. Additional-ly, the CSES made a significant contribution (β=−.16, p= .004), withlower scores associated with better performance on the verbal tasks.

Having established that femininity made a significant contributionto verbal ability, we tested for statistical mediation using the BSRI fem-ininity measure (see Fig. 9). First, there was a significant association be-tween sex and verbal ability (β = .16, p = .005). Second, there was asignificant association between sex and the mediator variable of femi-ninity (β= .22, p b .001, Path A). Next, there was a significant associa-tion between the mediator (femininity) and verbal ability aftercontrolling for sex (β = .40, p b .001, Path B). Results of the Sobel testshow a statistically significant mediation model, Z = 3.48, p b .001,and calculation of the bootstrapped estimate of the indirect effectshowed that it differed significantly from zero (95% CI = .07 to .27).After controlling for themediator, the direct effect of sex on verbal abil-ity was no longer significant (β= .07, p= .195), yielding evidence for afull mediation model (see Fig. 9).

ities.

9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16.

.17⁎⁎ .17⁎⁎ −.21⁎⁎⁎ −.01 .06 .11 .21⁎⁎⁎ −.22⁎⁎ .16⁎⁎

.29⁎⁎⁎ −.39⁎⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎⁎ −.04 −.16⁎⁎ −.08 .43⁎⁎⁎ −.13⁎

.12⁎ .03 .03 .04 .33⁎⁎⁎ .36⁎⁎⁎ .29⁎⁎⁎ −.05 .42⁎⁎⁎

.22⁎⁎ −.38⁎⁎⁎ .45⁎⁎⁎ .24⁎⁎⁎ −.04 −.20⁎⁎ −.10 .39⁎⁎⁎ −.15⁎

.12⁎ .04 .00 .01 .32⁎⁎⁎ .37⁎⁎⁎ .32⁎⁎⁎ −.07 .43⁎⁎⁎

.18⁎⁎ −.18⁎⁎ .25⁎⁎⁎ .14⁎ −.16⁎⁎ −.27⁎⁎⁎ .03 .22⁎⁎⁎ −.17⁎⁎

.40⁎⁎⁎ −.37⁎⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎⁎ .01 .06 .13⁎⁎ .74⁎⁎⁎ .06.14⁎ −.19⁎⁎ −.12⁎ .21⁎⁎⁎ .09 .15⁎ −.29⁎⁎⁎ .19⁎⁎

– −.62⁎⁎⁎ −.35⁎⁎⁎ −.01 .08 −.07 −.77⁎⁎⁎ .01

– .34⁎⁎⁎ .03 −.09 .12⁎ .61⁎⁎⁎ .00

– .08 .05 .18⁎ .73⁎⁎⁎ .13⁎

– .61⁎⁎⁎ .43⁎⁎⁎ .05 .84⁎⁎⁎

– .37⁎⁎⁎ .01 .82⁎⁎⁎

– .17⁎⁎ .76⁎⁎⁎

– .08

Page 272: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Fig. 2.Mean performance of males and females on the Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task.Error bars indicate ±1 S.E.M.

Fig. 4. Mean performance on the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT) for males andfemales. Error bars indicate ±1 S.E.M.

154 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

7. Discussion

The present study provided a more comprehensive test of Nash's(1979) sex-role mediation hypothesis than has been previously con-ducted, using a range of measures tapping aspects of spatial reasoningand verbal ability. The expected sex differences emerged for some butnot all tasks. However sex-role differences were found for most spatialand all verbal tasks.Masculine and androgynous participants performedbetter on spatial tasks than feminine and undifferentiated participants.Feminine and androgynous participants performed better on languagetasks. Despite social and political changes in the roles of men andwomen in the intervening period, support for the sex-role mediationhypothesis was found in a modern sample of college students.

7.1. Spatial ability

Predicted sex differences in performance were found for 3D mentalrotation and spatial perception tasks, but not for spatial visualizationas assessed by the GEFT, nor for mental rotation as measured by the2D task. In a meta-analysis of sex differences in spatial ability, Voyeret al. (1995) found that spatial visualization effect sizes were consider-ably smaller than for other spatial tasks and not consistently foundacross all studies. Inclusion of the GEFT in future studies with larger

Fig. 3.Mean angular error on the PiagetWater Level Task formales and females. Error barsindicate ±1 S.E.M. graph.

samples may be warranted to determine whether sex differences inspatial visualization are still reliably found in modern samples.

The absence of a sex difference for the novel 2D rotation task wassurprising, as previous researchers had documented substantial sex dif-ferences in mental rotation tasks with similar stimuli (Collins & Kimura,1997; Prinzel & Freeman, 1995). We note, however, that a previousstudy by Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) found that by manipulatingthe task difficulty level, sex differences on rotation tasks can be substan-tially reduced or even eliminated. Therefore a failure to find any groupdifferences may well be due to the ease of our version and a failure todifferentiate between high- and low-ability as indicated by the ceilingeffect (median accuracy N90%). Sex differences in performance wereobserved in the 3D Mental Rotation Task, and males completed itemson this task more quickly than females. This may reflect greater confi-dence for males on spatial tasks, which was found for explicit confi-dence ratings on the Piaget Water Level Task.

While previous research had established support for the sex-rolemediation hypothesis with some types of spatial ability (e.g. mental ro-tation), it was unclear whether this effect would generalize to otherforms of spatial reasoning. Our study found robust sex-role differencesacross 3D mental rotation, spatial perception, and spatial visualizationtasks, consistent with Nash's hypothesis. Regression analysis of a com-posite spatial ability score confirmed a significant associationwithmas-culine sex-roles, and importantly, that a corresponding negativeassociation with feminine sex-roles was not present. When all threegender-related measures were entered into the regression model,BSRI masculinity emerged as the strongest predictor of spatial perfor-mance, though there was considerable overlap with other measures.Mediation analysis found that masculine sex-role identity was a signif-icant mediator of the relationship between sex and spatial ability. The

Table 6Hierarchical multiple regression of spatial ability (N = 308).

Variable β t p-Value sr2 R R2

Step 1 .21 .04Sex (0 = male) −.22 −3.85 b.001⁎⁎⁎ .05

Step 2 .49 .24Sex (0 = male) −.15 −2.93 .004⁎⁎ .02BSRI masculinity .33 4.23 b.001⁎⁎⁎ .05BSRI femininity −.11 −1.19 .237⁎ .00PAQ masculinity .10 1.38 .170 .01PAQ femininity .12 1.33 .186 .00CSES .10 1.83 .068 .01

⁎ p b .05.⁎⁎ p b .01.⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

Page 273: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Fig. 5. Indirect effect of sex on spatial ability,withmasculine sex-roles acting as amediator.Path C represents the direct effect of sex after controlling for the mediator. Note: *p b .05,**p b .01, ***p b .001.

Fig. 7.Mean number of words written for males and females on the synonym generationtask. Error bars indicate ±1 S.E.M.

155D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

results of regression analysis qualified that this relationship supported apartialmediationmodel, but that sex alsomakes a direct contribution tospatial performance independent of the mediator.

While the primary focus of this study was on cognitive performanceas reflected in accuracy and error rates, we alsomeasured reaction timefor rotation and confidence ratings for the Piaget Water Level Task.Some researchers have suggested that speed of processing is an impor-tant factor in explaining sex differences in mental rotation (Voyer,2011). However,males and females did not differ in the average amountof time spent on items. Interestingly, we did notice group differencesacross sex-role categories, with masculine and androgynous partici-pants completing problems faster. Confidence ratings for the PiagetWLT were also higher for males and those high in masculinity, in linewith the smaller angular error for these groups.

7.2. Verbal ability and language skills

In contrast to previous studies, we observed no sex differences inverbal fluency or synonym generation for our sample. This is surprising,given the appreciable effect sizes reported in past research (Halpern &Tan, 2001; Hines, 1990). However, significant differences across sex-role categories were found for these tasks, with those scoring high onfemininity (feminine and androgynous groups) generating significantlymore words than the masculine and undifferentiated groups.

We did, however, observe a meaningful sex difference in grammarand language usage. This is in line with previous research findingsmall to medium effect sizes (Stanley et al., 1992). Additionally, sex-role differences were considerably larger than the difference betweenmales and females. It also highlights the utility of examining the effectof sex-role identity on cognitive measures, as a comparison of males

Fig. 6.Mean number ofwords generated formales and females on the phonological verbalfluency task. Error bars indicate ±1 S.E.M.

and females alone would have been unable to detect any group differ-ences in performance for some verbal measures.

Regression analysis showed that all three sex-role measuresaccounted for unique variance in the composite measure of verbal abil-ity. However, the BSRI (which measures sex-typed personality traits)was the strongest predictor of verbal performance.We conductedmedi-ation analysis, which showed that the relationship between sex andverbal abilitywas fullymeditated by feminine sex-roles. As the distribu-tion of sex-role categories frequently varies from sample to sample, thismay explain fluctuations in the magnitude of sex differences acrossstudies. If our sample of males was somewhat lower in femininity andour females somewhat higher, we may well have found a significantsex difference in verbal fluency and synonym generation.

7.3. Implications and limitations

The sex-role mediation hypothesis proposes an additional develop-mental factor to explain the emergence of sex differences in spatialand verbal abilities: namely that the degree to which individuals identi-fy with masculine and feminine sex-roles may influence their acquisi-tion of spatial and verbal skills, respectively. This, in turn, mayinfluence broader psychological factors such as intelligence.

Somemales and females develop a gender-congruent sex-role iden-tity that leads to a restriction of their interests and behaviors (Bem,1981b;Martin& Ruble, 2004),while others incorporate an androgynous

Fig. 8. Performance on the DAT Language Usage subtest for males and females.

Page 274: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

Table 7Hierarchical multiple regression of verbal ability (N = 308).

Variable β t p-Value sr2 R R2

Step 1 .16 .03Sex (0 = male) −.16 −2.80 .005⁎⁎ .03

Step 2 .48 .23Sex (0 = male) −.03 .58 .563 .00BSRI masculinity −.05 −.64 .526 .00BSRI femininity .25 2.72 .007⁎⁎ .02PAQ masculinity −.01 −.14 .893 .00PAQ femininity .20 2.20 .029⁎ .01CSES −.16 −2.91⁎⁎⁎ .004⁎⁎ .02

⁎ p b .05.⁎⁎ p b .01.⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

156 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

sex-role identity affording greater cognitive and behavioral flexibility(boys and girls can do almost anything). While children receive mes-sages about the suitability and gender-appropriateness of behaviorfrom a variety of sources including peers and culture, parents and edu-cators can have profound influence on the socialization of sex-roles(Witt, 1997). The sex-role mediation hypothesis suggests that theremay be tangible benefits for cognitive development in nurturing the in-terests and talents of both boys and girls who have androgynous flexi-bility, while gently encouraging those that show a more restrictedrange of pursuits to diversify their interests. Childrenwhoexpress an in-terest in educational toys or leisure activities that promote spatial orlanguage competence should be encouraged (McGeown, Goodwin,Henderson, & Wright, 2011; Newcombe & Frick, 2010), even if thosetoys or activities are stereotypically associated with only one gender(Berenbaum, Martin, Hanish, Briggs, & Fabes, 2008).

A variety of theoretical explanations have been proposed for sex-role mediation effects, including sex roles being associated withselection of leisure activities and interest that promote spatial orlanguage development (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1995; McGeownet al., 2011), endorsement of gender stereotypes about the sex-typingof school subjects (Liben, Bigler, & Krogh, 2002), and reduced self-efficacy beliefs and achievement motivation for sex-typed cognitivetasks (Choi, 2004). Stereotype threat might also be a factor (Steele,1997; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002), with poorer performance onstereotypically masculine and feminine tasks. While the findings ofthis study provide support the theory that sex-roles act as a mediatorfor the development of sex differences in spatial and verbal abilities,the evidence presented is correlational which by itself cannot provedirect causation. Furthermore, one cannot rule out the possibility thatacquisition of sex-role identification may stem in part from children'sobservations of their own performance in sex-typed cognitive domainslike English and mathematics. Although research shows that childrenacquire considerable knowledge of sex-roles between 2 and 5 yearsold, a period that predates formal instruction in subjects likemathemat-ics and reading (Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, 2006), it remains possiblethat competencies for intellectual tasks help further refine one's sex-role identity, or that there are bidirectional links between sex-roleidentity and intellectual abilities.

Fig. 9. Indirect effect of sex on verbal ability, with feminine sex-roles acting as a mediator.

Further research is required to investigate the causalmechanisms bywhich sex roles mediate intellectual development, and to rule out thepossibility that sex role identity acts as a proxy for some as yet unspec-ified factor. While the study presents a test of sex-role mediation in asample of adult undergraduate students, such a sample may differfrom the general population in demographic characteristics such as so-cioeconomic status and level of education. There is tentative evidencethat socioeconomic status moderates the sex difference in visuospatialreasoning at least (Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco, Newcombe, &Huttenlocher, 2005), and replication of sex-rolemediation in communi-ty samples would represent a stronger test of such hypotheses. It mayalso capture a broader range of sex role attitudes, as cross-sex-typedsubjects such as feminine males occur less frequently in the generalpopulation and were small in number in our sample. A limitation ofthis study is the sample size (particularly of males) and the extent towhich the results can be generalized to the population of interest. Nev-ertheless the sex differences observed in our study (i.e., male advantagein spatial tasks and a female advantage on verbal tasks) are broadly inline with those observed in other studies that include large samples.

8. Summary

The findings of our study provide support for Nash's (1979) sex-rolemediation hypothesis for both spatial reasoning and verbal languageskills in a modern adult sample, despite the passage of time since itwas first proposed. Masculine sex-roles were associated with betterspatial ability for all three categories of spatial reasoning (mental rota-tion, spatial perception, and spatial visualization). Feminine sex-roleswere associated with better verbal ability (phonological verbal fluency,synonym generation, and grammar and language usage). Regressionanalysis showed that sex-typed personality traits were the strongestpredictor of spatial reasoning and language skills, and that sex-roleidentity mediated the relationship between sex and spatial/verbal rea-soning. It also highlights the utility of measuring sex-role identity forexplaining individual differences in specific cognitive abilities, as sex-role differences were found across measures evenwhen sex differenceswere absent. Further research is required to examine the causal mecha-nisms by which sex-roles mediate intellectual functioning.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by a Griffith University post-graduate research scholarship.

References

Alon, S., & Gelbgiser, D. (2011). The female advantage in college academic achievementsand horizontal sex segregation. Social Science Research, 40(1), 107–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.06.007.

Archer, J. (1992). Gender stereotyping of school subjects. The psychologist. 5. (pp. 66–69).Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in contemporary American

society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 43(7/8),499–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007119516728.

Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., et al.(2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior ingirls and in boys. Psychological Science, 20(2), 144–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02279.x.

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1995). Environmental input to the development ofsex-related differences in spatial and mathematical ability. Learning and IndividualDifferences, 7(4), 363–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(95)90007-1.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in socialpsychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting andClinical Psychology, 42(2), 155–162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036215.

Bem, S. L. (1981a). BEM Sex Role Inventory Manual: Manual. Instrument and scoring guide.Mind Garden.

Bem, S. L. (1981b). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. PsychologicalReview, 88(4), 354–364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354.

Page 275: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

157D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex typing and the avoidance of cross-sex behavior. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 33(1), 48–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0078640.

Berenbaum, S. A., Martin, C. L., Hanish, L. D., Briggs, P. T., & Fabes, R. A. (2008). Sex differ-ences in children's play. In J. B. Becker, K. J. Berkley, N. Geary, E. Hampson, J. P.Herman, & E. A. Young (Eds.), Sex differences in the brain: From genes to behavior(pp. 275–290). New York: Oxford University Press.

Buchmann, C., & DiPrete, T. A. (2006). The growing female advantage in college comple-tion: The role of family background and academic achievement. American SociologicalReview, 71(4), 515–541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000312240607100401.

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., & Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women's underrepresentation in sci-ence: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2),218–261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014412.

Choi, N. (2004). Sex role group differences in specific, academic, and general self-efficacy.The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 138(2), 149–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.138.2.149-159.

Choi, N., Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J. L. (2007). Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis ofthe Bem Sex Role Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 67(5),818–832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164406299106.

Choi, N., Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J. L. (2009). Exploratory and confirmatory studies of thestructure of the Bem Sex Role Inventory short form with two divergent samples.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69(4), 696–705. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164409332218.

Collins, D. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). A large sex difference on a two-dimensional mentalrotation task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111(4), 845–849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.111.4.845.

Durkin, K. (1987). Social cognition and social context in the construction of sex differ-ences. In M. A. Baker (Ed.), Sex differences in human performance (pp. 141–170).Chichester: John Wiley.

Eagly, A. H., & Mitchell, A. A. (2004). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities:Implications for the sociopolitical attitudes of men and women. In M. A. Paludi (Ed.),Praeger guide to the psychology of gender (pp. 183–206). Westport, Conn.: PraegerPublishers.

Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects,and parents' socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46(2),183–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x.

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differ-ences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 103–127.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018053.

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A new look at anold controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62(1), 61–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543062001061.

Furnham, A., Reeves, E., & Budhani, S. (2002). Parents think their sons are brighter thantheir daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimations and estimations oftheir children's multiple intelligences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(1),24–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221320209597966.

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science,320(5880), 1164–1165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1154094.

Halari, R., Hines, M., Kumari, V., Mehrotra, R., Wheeler, M., Ng, V., et al. (2005). Sex differ-ences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship toendogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1),104–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.104.

Halpern, D. F. (2014). It gets crowded with an elephant and an ape in the room: Teachingabout female and male cognitive differences and similarities. Teaching of Psychology,41(1), 88–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0098628313514187.

Halpern, D. F., & Lamay, M. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences inintelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 229–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009027516424.

Halpern, D. F., & Tan, U. (2001). Stereotypes and steroids: Using a psychobiosocial modelto understand cognitive sex differences. Brain and Cognition, 45(3), 392–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1287.

Halpern, D. F., & Wright, T. M. (1996). A process-oriented model of cognitive sex differ-ences. Learning and Individual Differences, 8(1), 3–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90003-5.

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011a). Sex differences in intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg, & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence(pp. 253–272). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011b). Beliefs about cognitive genderdifferences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for size. Sex Roles, 64(5–6),336–347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2.

Hamilton, C. J. (1995). Beyond sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: The impact ofgender trait possession. British Journal of Psychology, 86(1), 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02542.x.

Hines, M. (1990). Gonadal hormones and human cognitive development. In J. Balthazart(Ed.), Hormones, brain and behaviour in vertebrates (pp. 51–63). New York: Karger.

Hines, M., Chiu, L., McAdams, L. A., Bentler, P. M., & Lipcamon, J. (1992). Cognitionand the corpus callosum: Verbal fluency, visuospatial ability, and languagelateralization related to midsagittal surface areas of callosal subregions.Behavioral Neuroscience, 106(1), 3–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.3.

Hoffman, R. M., & Borders, L. D. (2001). Twenty-five years after the Bem Sex-Role Inven-tory: A reassessment and new issues regarding classification variability.Measurementand Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34(1), 39–55.

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 53–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.104.1.53.

Jansen-Osmann, P., & Heil, M. (2007). Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences inthe speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain and Cognition,64(3), 217–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.002.

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. London: Praeger.Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2007). Sex differences in mental abilities: g masks the

dimensions on which they lie. Intelligence, 35(1), 23–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.012.

Kimura, D., & Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced byfluctuations in sex hormones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(2), 57–61.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769964.

Klecker, B. (2006). The gender gap in NAEP fourth-, eighth-, and twelfth-grade readingscores across years. Reading Improvement, 43(1), 50–56.

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J.(2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex difference in spatial skill.Psychological Science, 16(11), 841–845. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01623.x.

Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. R. (2002). Language at work: Children's gendered in-terpretations of occupational titles. Child Development, 73(3), 810–828. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00440.

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences inspatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479–1498. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130467.

Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one'ssocial identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(3), 302–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167292183006.

Lynn, R. (1992). Sex differences on the differential aptitude test in British and Americanadolescents. Educational Psychology, 12(2), 101–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0144341920120201.

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames, 13(1), 3–13.http://dx.doi.org/10.3176/tr.2009.1.01.

Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents' differential socialization of boys and girls: Ameta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 267–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267.

Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Global sex differences in test score variability. Science,322(5906), 1331–1332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1162573.

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children's search for gender cues: Cognitive perspec-tives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2),67–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.

McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2011). Gender differences inreading motivation: Does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journalof Research in Reading, 35(3), 328–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x.

McGraw, R., Lubienski, S. T., & Strutchens, M. E. (2006). A closer look at gender in NAEPmathematics achievement and affect data: Intersections with achievement, race/eth-nicity, and socioeconomic status. Journal for Research inMathematics Education, 37(2),129–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/30034845.

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends inCognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011.

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. InM. A.Wittig, & A. C.Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues(pp. 263–302). New York: Academic Press.

National Science Foundation (2011).Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in sci-ence and engineering: 2011. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation (Retrievedfrom http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/wmpd/pdf/wmpd2011.pdf).

Neumann, D. L., Fitzgerald, Z. T., Furedy, J. J., & Boyle, G. J. (2007). Sexually dimorphic ef-fects of acute nicotine administration on arousal and visual-spatial ability in non-smoking human volunteers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 86(4),758–765. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.001.

Newcombe, N. S., & Frick, A. (2010). Early education for spatial intelligence: Why, what,and how. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4(3), 102–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2010.01089.x.

Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., et al. (2012). Intel-ligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2),130–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027240.

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = male, me = female, there-fore math ≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44.

Nyborg, H. (2015). Sex differences across different racial ability levels: Theories of originand societal consequences. Intelligence, 52, 44–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.005.

Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). Aredrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test—different versions and factorsthat affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28(1), 39–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1995.1032.

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect ef-fects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &Computers, 36(4), 717–731. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553.

Priess, H. A., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Gender and academic abilities and preferences. In J. C.Chrisler, & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology(pp. 297–316). New York: Springer.

Prinzel, L. J., III, & Freeman, F. G. (1995). Sex differences in visuo-spatial ability: Task dif-ficulty, speed–accuracy tradeoff, and other performance factors. Canadian Journal ofExperimental Psychology, 49(4), 530–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1196-1961.49.4.530.

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PloS One, 7(7), e39904.http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039904.

Page 276: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

158 D. Reilly et al. / Intelligence 54 (2016) 147–158

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521–535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0.

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and sci-ence: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments.Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645–662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012.

Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender development. In N.Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (6th ed.).Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, vol. 3. (pp. 858–932). Hoboken, NJ:Wiley.

Shapiro, J., & Williams, A. M. (2012). The role of stereotype threats in undermining girls'and women's performance and interest in STEM fields. Sex Roles, 66(3–4), 175–183.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0051-0.

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitiveperformance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 207–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207.

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes,and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1),44–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x.

Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive componentsof psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports ofneurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10),1673–1682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1673.

Stanley, J. C., Benbow, C. P., Brody, L., Dauber, S., & Lupkowski, A. (1992). Gender differ-ences on eighty-six nationally standardized aptitude and achievement tests. In N.Colangelo, S. G. Assouline, & D. L. Ambroson (Eds.), Talent development: Proceedingsfrom the 1991 Henry B. and JocelynWallace National Research Symposium on Talent De-velopment (pp. 42–65). Unionville, NY: Trillium Press.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity andperformance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613.

Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psy-chology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental SocialPsychology, 34, 379–440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0.

Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimension-al spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47(2), 599–604. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599.

Vasta, R., & Liben, L. S. (1996). The water-level task: An intriguing puzzle. CurrentDirections in Psychological Science, 5(6), 171–177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512379.

Voyer, D. (2011). Time limits and gender differences on paper-and-pencil tests of mentalrotation: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(2), 267–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0042-0.

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abili-ties: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin,117(2), 250–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250.

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligningover 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance.Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817–835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016127.

Witkin, H. A. (2003). A manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Menlo Park, CA: MindGarden.

Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., & Karp, S. A. (1962). Psycholog-ical differentiation: Studies of development. Wiley.

Witt, S. D. (1997). Parental influence on children's socialization to gender roles.Adolescence, 32(126), 253–259.

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Once again, the origins of sex differences. American Psy-chologist, 55(9), 1062–1063. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.9.1062.

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differences and similiaritiesin behavior. In J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in Experimental Social Psy-chology. vol. 46. (pp. 55–124). New York: Academic Press.

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2015). Two traditions of research on gender identity. Sex Roles,73(11), 461–473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0480-2.

Page 277: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 154

the neutral priming condition, a briefing statement was provided that described the wide

variability in performance levels on visual-spatial and verbal tasks, and that the

experiment would be investigating the contribution of personality traits to individual

differences. As before, multiple choice questions followed the briefing statement to

verify that the briefing statement had been read and understood, and in the event of

incorrect answers they were instructed to read the briefing statement again (0.8% of

participants). All participants completed the mental rotation and verbal fluency tasks

with the presentation order counterbalanced to avoid sequencing effects.

After completing the cognitive tasks, participants were presented with a gender

beliefs checklist and the BSRI personality inventory. These items were administered

last to avoid gender-priming in the neutral condition of the experiment, as previous

studies had found that even asking demographic questions about gender can lower the

performance of women on visual-spatial tasks (McGlone & Aronson, 2006; Ortner &

Sieverding, 2008).

Results

Labelling of Spatial Task

Performance on the Group Embedded Figures Test was analyzed using a 2 ×

(Condition: Spatial or Empathy labelling) × 4 (Sex-Role: Masculine, Feminine,

Androgynous, Undifferentiated) factorial ANOVA. Cell sizes for the spatial condition

were Masculine (11), Feminine (15), Androgynous (14), Undifferentiated (30), while

those for empathy condition were: Masculine (23), Feminine (13), Androgynous (24)

and Undifferentiated (20). The assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance

were met. As shown in Figure 2 and consistent with hypotheses, there was a significant

main effect of condition, F(1, 142) = 30.88, p < .001, ηp2 = .18, with a large effect size.

When the task was described as a test of empathy and perspective taking, women scored

Page 278: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 155

significantly higher on the spatial visualization task than when the task was described as

being visual-spatial in nature t(148) = 5.56, p < .001, d = .91. Furthermore there was a

significant main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 142) = 3.01, p = .032, ηp2 = .06. To

evaluate our hypothesis, we conducted a planned linear contrast to compare those

scoring high in masculine sex-role identification (masculine and androgynous groups)

with those scoring low in masculine identification (feminine and undifferentiated

groups). The planned contrast showed that masculine and androgynous participants

scored significantly higher than those classified as feminine and undifferentiated, t(148)

= 3.00, p = .003, d = .48. The interaction term approached but fell short of statistical

significance, F(3, 142) = 2.57, p = .057, ηp2 = .05.

Figure 9.2. Mean performance on the Group Embedded Figures Test across condition

and Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) sex-role categories. Error bars indicate ± 1 S.E.M.

Page 279: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 156

Stereotype-Threat

Performance on the mental rotation task was analyzed using a 2 × (Condition:

stereotype-threat or control) × 4 (Sex-Role: masculine, feminine, androgynous,

undifferentiated) factorial ANOVA. Cell sizes for the stereotype-threat condition were

Masculine (15), Feminine (18), Androgynous (17), Undifferentiated (24), while those

for the control condition were: Masculine (19), Feminine (10), Androgynous (21) and

Undifferentiated (26). The assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance were

met for the distributions. As shown in Figure 3, there was a significant main effect of

condition, F(1, 142) = 10.66, p = .001, ηp2 = .07, with women in the stereotype-threat

condition scoring significantly lower than women in the control condition t(148) = .

There was also a significant main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 142) = 7.34, p < .001,

ηp2 = .13. The planned linear contrast showed that masculine and androgynous women

scored higher than feminine and undifferentiated, t (148) = 3.29, p = .001, d = .54. The

interaction term between condition and sex-role was not significant, F(3, 142) = .94, p

= .423, ηp2 = .02.

Page 280: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 157

Figure 9.3. Mean performance on the Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task across condition

and Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) sex-role categories. Error bars indicate ± 1 S.E.M.

We also examined whether there was an effect of sex-role or condition on the

average completion time of all questions answered (displayed in Figure 4). Consistent

with previous studies there was a significant main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 142)

= 5.42, p = .001, ηp2 = .10, with the planned contrast showing that masculine and

androgynous women completed items faster than feminine and undifferentiated, t(145)

= 3.04, p = .003, d = .50. There was no main effect of condition on speed, F(1, 142) =

3.57, p = .061, ηp2 =.03. Interestingly, the interaction between condition and sex-role

category was significant, F(3, 142) = 3.53, p = .016, ηp2 = .07. In order to examine the

interaction further, two separate one-way ANOVA’s were conducted. For those

participants in the stereotype-threat condition, there was a significant difference across

Page 281: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 158

sex-role categories, F(3, 71) = 6.67, p < .001, η2 = .22, with the planned contrast

showing that masculine and androgynous participants were faster to complete questions

than feminine and undifferentiated participants, t(71) = -3.93, p < .001, d = -.93.

However there was no significant difference in speed across sex-role categories under

the control condition, F(3,71) = .81, p = .494, η2 =.03.

Figure 9.4. Mean completion time for the Vandenberg Mental Rotation Task items

across condition and Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) sex-role categories. Error bars

indicate ± 1 S.E.M.

Stereotype Lift

Verbal fluency scores were analyzed using a 2 × (Condition: stereotype-lift or

control) × 4 (Sex-Role: masculine, feminine, androgynous, undifferentiated) factorial

ANOVA. Cell sizes for the stereotype condition were Masculine (15), Feminine (18),

Page 282: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 159

Androgynous (17), Undifferentiated (24), while those for the control condition were:

Masculine (19), Feminine (10), Androgynous (21) and Undifferentiated (26). All

assumptions of the factorial ANOVA were met. As shown in Figure 5, and contrary to

the stereotype-lift hypothesis, women in the stereotype priming condition did not

generate more words than those in the control condition, F(3, 142) = .11, p = .739, ηp2 =

.00, nor was the interaction term between condition and sex-role significant, F(3, 142) =

.52, p = .667, ηp2= .01. However there was a significant main effect of sex-role

category, F(3, 142) = 3.20, p = .025, ηp2 = .06. The planned linear contrast showed that

feminine and androgynous women (high femininity) generated more words than

masculine and undifferentiated women (low femininity), t(148) = -2.91, p = .004, d = -

.47.

Page 283: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 160

Figure 9.5. Mean number of words generated in verbal fluency task across condition

and Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) sex-role categories. Error bars indicate ± 1 S.E.M.

Manipulation Check

As a manipulation check, we tested whether there were any differences in gender

beliefs about cognitive abilities between those receiving the stereotype-threat induction

and those in the control condition following the briefing. Those in the stereotype-threat

condition showed greater endorsement than those in the control condition for visual-

spatial ability, t(147) = 3.99, p < .001, d = .65, and for verbal language ability, t(147) =

2.99, p = .003, d = .49. In addition, to rule out the possibility of a carry-over effect of

the experimental manipulation of task-labelling on the GEFT from the first phase of the

experiment, we checked whether those assigned to the empathy condition differed in

their mental rotation and verbal fluency scores from those assigned to the spatial

Page 284: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 161

condition. Condition was dummy-coded with 0 being spatial condition and 1 being

empathy condition. There was no significant difference in mental rotation, t(148) = -

1.74, p = .083, or verbal fluency, t(148) = .62, p = .533, between conditions to confirm

that there were no carry-over effects.

Discussion

Previous studies on the role of sex-role identification have found differences in

performance between high and low masculinity participants on visual-spatial tasks

(Reilly & Neumann, 2013). However, the research literature was unclear on whether

this reflected enduring differences in sex-role identification (an enduring personality

trait), or was a more transient state resulting from situational and intrapersonal factors.

Two possible sources of temporary performance decrements were investigated in this

experiment, that of task-labelling and stereotype-threat.

Sex-typing of cognitive tasks and task-labelling

Firstly, we examined whether the way in which visual-spatial tasks were

described to test-takers might influence women’s performance on the GEFT. When the

GEFT was portrayed as requiring stereotypically ‘masculine’ traits of visual-spatial

reasoning, women scored lower than when the same task was portrayed as requiring

stereotypically ‘feminine’ traits of empathy and perspective taking. This is consistent

with the earlier study by Brosnan (1998). Brosnan (1998) attributed the effect to the

perceived sex-typing of the task and whether it was gender-appropriate. It is possible

that women in the empathy condition simply exerted more effort in the face of

challenging content than those who believed it to be a masculine sex-typed task. There

was also an independent effect of sex-role category in line with the sex-role mediation

hypothesis, and which is consistent with two previous studies employing the GEFT in

college-aged samples (Brosnan, 1998; Reilly et al., 2016). Regardless of how the spatial

Page 285: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 162

visualization task was described, masculine and androgynous women still scored

significantly higher than feminine and undifferentiated women. If the group differences

were solely a result of the perceived sex-typing of the task, there should be no

significant main effect of sex-role category. From these results, it would appear that task

labelling and sex-role identity both contributed to task performance, with task labelling

producing a temporary effect that added to the sex-role differences which occurred

regardless of condition.

Our results differed somewhat from that observed by Massa et al. (2005), who

found no overall main effects of task description or sex-role identity in women but did

observe a significant interaction between description and sex-role. In our sample, the

interaction term fell short of statistical significance, which may reflect sampling

variance or differences in study methodology. The procedure employed by Massa et al.

differed in that half their participants completed the Bem Sex Role Inventory before

attempting the spatial visualization task, and half completed the BSRI survey

afterwards. This may have led to an unanticipated gender priming effect in some

participants, particularly when using the longer form of the BSRI instrument where the

gendered nature of the personality traits is more readily apparent. It may also be due to

differences between the short and long form of the BSRI instrument, with the short form

reportedly possessing better psychometric properties than the longer one (T. Campbell,

Gillaspy, & Thompson, 1997; Choi, Fuqua, & Newman, 2009).

Stereotype Threat/Lift

We also investigated the effect of negative gender stereotypes about cognitive

ability by experimentally inducing stereotype threat. For the mental rotation task,

women in the stereotype-threat condition performed more poorly than those in the

control condition who were not briefed about patterns of sex difference research for

Page 286: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 163

visual spatial tasks. Stereotype threat is often considered a factor in high-stakes testing

(Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995), such as performance on standardized tests of

aptitude like the SAT for college admission. But our study found a significant

stereotype threat effect even for a relatively non-consequential task, in a situation where

anonymity was guaranteed. Consistent with past research (Reilly & Neumann, 2013),

there was also a significant main effect of sex-role category, with masculine and

androgynous groups scoring higher than feminine and undifferentiated groups on

mental rotation. There was also a significant interaction between sex-role and condition

for reaction times, with masculine and androgynous participants answering questions

faster than feminine and undifferentiated participants in the stereotype threat condition

but not in the control condition. This may be due to the additional performance pressure

caused for the stereotype threat condition.

Several recent studies have experimentally induced the stereotype threat effect

for visual-spatial tasks (S. M. Campbell & Collaer, 2009; Heil, Jansen, Quaiser-Pohl, &

Neuburger, 2012; McGlone & Aronson, 2006). McGlone and Aronson (Experiment 1a)

examined the effect of gender priming on mental rotation performance with the

Vandenberg MRT. They found that a manipulation designed to make gender category

salient led to poorer performance. They found a medium-sized effect comparable to that

observed in our study. A second experiment by Heil, Jansen, Quaiser-Pohl and

Neuburger (2012) briefed participants with one of three sets of instructions (either that

males were better at visual-spatial tasks, women were better, or that no sex differences

exist), followed by a mental rotation test. In their sample, women’s mental rotation

performance was poorer when led to believe that men score better on visual-spatial

tasks than the neutral condition. Additionally women scored higher than the neutral

condition when led to believe that women were better at visual-spatial tasks. Finally,

Page 287: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 164

Campbell and Collaer (2009) also found a stereotype threat effect for a novel

visuospatial task involving accuracy of judgment for angular line tasks. The present

study replicates these findings on stereotype threat induction, and extends such research

to consider the contribution of sex-role identity on performance.

We also sought to test whether the activation of a favourable stereotype (women

scoring higher than men on language tasks) might affect performance on a verbal

fluency task. When briefed about the strong female advantage of tasks of verbal ability

(positive stereotype), women did not generate any more words than those in the control

condition as would have been predicted by the stereotype lift hypothesis (Walton &

Cohen, 2003). It may be easier to decrease performance with negative gender

stereotypes than it is to increase performance with positively affirming stereotypes.

Predicted sex-role differences in word production were observed though, consistent

with those found previously (Nash, 1979; Reilly et al., 2016).

Implications and Limitations

Although a large body of research has demonstrated robust sex differences in

cognitive ability for both visual-spatial and verbal domains, researchers disagree on the

extent to which they reflect innate biological processes or sociocultural differences

between men and womens’ roles in society. Hyde (2005) has noted that within-sex

variation is often larger than the variability between males and females, and that

researchers should investigate factors associated with individual differences in cognitive

ability. The sex-role mediation hypothesis proposed by Nash (1979) offers an alternate

explanation grounded not in biological sex, but rather in the development of

stereotypically masculine and feminine personality traits. It was unclear though whether

observed performance differences are due to differences in sex-role identity or to more

transient factors associated with the testing situation such as the perceived sex-role

Page 288: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 165

appropriateness of the task or stereotype-threat. The present results for visual-spatial

ability suggest that although there are transient effects of the testing situation, there are

still significant sex-role differences reflecting an enduring trait. We also found

significant sex-role differences in a verbal fluency task, while no difference in fluency

was found between women reminded about the strong female advantage in such tasks

and the control group.

While many studies have documented the stereotype threat effect in the context

of high-stakes testing and educational assessment (such as tests of mathematical

achievement), only a few studies (e.g. Massa et al., 2005) have investigated it for other

domains such as visual-spatial ability. An important consideration is also the presence

of situational cues such as test instructions that might subtly impact on performance.

The present results highlight how beliefs about the skills required to complete a

challenging cognitive task can serve to boost or impair performance. Even subtle

situational cues may inadvertently prime test-takers to think about sex stereotypes, and

further research is needed to increase resiliency in such testing situations (Miyake et al.,

2010).

The present study recruited a female-only sample because gender priming and

stereotype-threat induction have only been demonstrated to exert an effect in women for

visual-spatial tasks. There is scant research on similar performance impairments in

males on tasks of verbal ability and language, but one recent study has found evidence

for stereotype-threat in boys when reading ability is tested (Pansu et al., 2016). Given

the moderately large sex differences present on language tasks such as reading, writing,

punctuation, and grammar (Halpern, 2011), the question of whether males are similarly

susceptible to stereotype threat and whether this interacts with sex role orientation

warrants further investigation. Also, while the primary outcome of interest in our study

Page 289: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 166

was performance on visual-spatial and verbal tasks, it is possible that group differences

in measured performance might also reflect factors such as motivation and self-efficacy

beliefs. Future studies might strengthen the validity of their findings by including such

measures.

Conclusions

The present study demonstrates the effect of three factors on visual-spatial

performance in a young-adult sample of women. Firstly the way in which participants

see the task can have a significant effect on their performance. In our experiment

women led to believe a task required stereotypically feminine skills of empathy

performed better than when instructed it was a test of visual-spatial reasoning.

Secondly, we demonstrated that knowledge and priming of gender stereotypes can also

lead to diminished performance for visual-spatial tasks, even in the absence of high-

stakes testing. In addition to these state effects, there were significant sex-role

differences with greater visual-spatial performance by masculine and androgynous

women, while feminine and androgynous women showed greater verbal fluency. This

pattern of results suggests the sex-role mediation effect observed in previous studies

exerts an effect on visual-spatial performance in women, but can be moderated by task-

labelling and salience of gender stereotypes.

Page 290: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 167

References

Athenstaedt, U., Mikula, G., & Bredt, C. (2009). Gender role self-concept and leisure

activities of adolescents. Sex Roles, 60(5), 399-409. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-

9543-y

Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in contemporary American

society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 43(7/8), 499-

528. doi: 10.1023/A:1007119516728

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. S. (1995). Environmental input to the development of

sex-related differences in spatial and mathematical ability. Learning and Individual

Differences, 7(4), 363-379. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(95)90007-1

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. doi: 10.1037/h0036215

Bem, S. L. (1981). BEM Sex Role Inventory Manual: Manual, Instrument and Scoring

Guide: Mind Garden.

Bem, S. L. (1984). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual and empirical

integration. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiuation:

Psychology and Gender (pp. 179–226). Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska

Press.

Bem, S. L., & Lenney, E. (1976). Sex typing and the avoidance of cross-sex behaviour.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(1), 48-54. doi:

10.1037/h0078640

Berninger, V. W., Nielsen, K. H., Abbott, R. D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008).

Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School

Psychology, 46(2), 151-172. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.007

Brosnan, M. J. (1998). The implications for academic attainment of perceived gender-

appropriateness upon spatial task performance. British Journal of Educational

Psychology, 68(2), 203-215. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1998.tb01284.x

Campbell, S. M., & Collaer, M. L. (2009). Stereotype threat and gender differences in

performance on a novel visuospatial task. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 33(4),

437-444. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01521.x

Campbell, T., Gillaspy, J. A., & Thompson, B. (1997). The factor structure of the Bem

Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI): Confirmatory analysis of long and short forms.

Page 291: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 168

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57(1), 118-124. doi:

10.1177/0013164497057001008

Carli, L. L., Alawa, L., Lee, Y., Zhao, B., & Kim, E. (2016). Stereotypes about gender and

science: Women ≠ Scientists. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 244-260.

doi: 10.1177/0361684315622645

Choi, N., Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J. L. (2007). Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis

of the Bem Sex Role Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement,

67(5), 818-832. doi: 10.1177/0013164406299106

Choi, N., Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J. L. (2009). Exploratory and confirmatory studies of

the structure of the Bem Sex Role Inventory Short Form with two divergent

samples. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69(4), 696-705. doi:

10.1177/0013164409332218

Donnelly, K., Twenge, J. M., Clark, M. A., Shaikh, S. K., Beiler-May, A., & Carter, N. T.

(2015). Attitudes toward women’s work and family roles in the United States,

1976–2013. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(1), 41-54. doi:

10.1177/0361684315590774

Duffy, G., Sorby, S., Nozaki, S., & Bowe, B. (2016, 12-15 Oct. 2016). Exploring the role

of spatial cognition in problem solving. Paper presented at the 2016 IEEE Frontiers

in Education Conference (FIE), Eria, PA: Institute of Electrical Engineers.

Eagly, A. H. (1996). Differences between women and men: Their magnitude, practical

importance, and political meaning. American Psychologist, 51(2), 158-159. doi:

10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.158

Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2003). The malleability of sex differences in response to

changing social roles. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology

of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive

psychology (pp. 103-115). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Flore, P. C., & Wicherts, J. M. (2015). Does stereotype threat influence performance of

girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis. Journal of School Psychology,

53(1), 25-44. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002

Ganley, C. M., & Lubienski, S. T. (2016). Mathematics confidence, interest, and

performance: Examining gender patterns and reciprocal relations. Learning and

Individual Differences, 47, 182-193. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.01.002

Gauthier, C. T., Duyme, M., Zanca, M., & Capron, C. (2009). Sex and performance level

effects on brain activation during a verbal fluency task: A functional magnetic

Page 292: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 169

resonance imaging study. Cortex, 45(2), 164-176. doi:

10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.006

Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are a-changing … or are

they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 40(3), 353-363. doi: 10.1177/0361684316634081

Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education.

American Psychologist, 52(10), 1091-1102. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1091

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A.

(2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological

Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1-51. doi: 10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x

Halpern, D. F., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde, J. S., . . . Martin,

C. L. (2011). The pseudoscience of single-sex schooling. Science, 333(6050), 1706-

1707. doi: 10.1126/science.1205031

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and

numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269(5220), 41-45. doi:

10.1126/science.7604277

Heil, M., Jansen, P., Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Neuburger, S. (2012). Gender-specific effects of

artificially induced gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learning and Individual

Differences, 22(3), 350-353. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.01.004

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-

592. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

Hyde, J. S., Krajnik, M., & Skuldt-Niederberger, K. (1991). Androgyny across the life

span: A replication and longitudinal followup. Developmental Psychology, 27(3),

516–519. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.516

Hyde, J. S., & Lindberg, S. M. (2007). Facts and assumptions about the nature of gender

differences and the implications for gender equity. In S. S. Klein (Ed.), Handbook

for achieving gender equity through education (2nd ed., pp. 19-32). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in

children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across

grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509-527. doi: 10.1111/1467-

8624.00421

Page 293: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 170

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor : the Science of Mental Ability. London: Praeger.

Kagan, J. (1964). The child's sex role classification of school objects. Child Development,

35(4), 1051-1056. doi: 10.2307/1126852

Kersh, J., Casey, B. M., & Young, J. M. (2008). Research on spatial skills and block

building in girls and boys: The relationship to later mathematical learning. In B.

Spodak & O. Saracho (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on Mathematics in Early

Childhood Education (pp. 233-253). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

Kutner, M., Greenberg, E., Jin, Y., Boyle, B., Hsu, Y.-c., & Dunleavy, E. (2007). Literacy

in everyday life: Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy.

Washington, DC.: National Center for Education Statistics.

LaCosse, J., Sekaquaptewa, D., & Bennett, J. (2016). STEM stereotypic attribution bias

among women in an unwelcoming science setting. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 40(3), 378-397. doi: 10.1177/0361684316630965

Lietz, P. (2006). A meta-analysis of gender differences in reading achievement at the

secondary school level. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 32(4), 317-344. doi:

10.1016/j.stueduc.2006.10.002

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences

in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479-1498. doi:

10.2307/1130467

Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). Sex differences in reading achievement. Trames, 13(1), 3-13.

doi: 10.3176/tr.2009.1.01

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford:

Stanford University Press.

Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children's search for gender cues : Cognitive

perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,

13(2), 67-70. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x

Massa, L. J., Mayer, R. E., & Bohon, L. M. (2005). Individual differences in gender role

beliefs influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual

Differences, 15(2), 99-111. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2004.11.002

McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2011). Gender differences in

reading motivation: does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journal of

Research in Reading, 35(3), 328-336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x

Page 294: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 171

McGlone, M. S., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial

reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 486-493. doi:

10.1016/j.appdev.2006.06.003

McHale, S. M., Kim, J. Y., Whiteman, S., & Crouter, A. C. (2004). Links between sex-

typed time use in middle childhood and gender development in early adolescence.

Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 868-881. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.868

Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T. A.

(2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom

study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234-1237. doi:

10.1126/science.1195996

Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. In M. A. Wittig & A.

C. Petersen (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental

issues (pp. 263-302). New York: Academic Press.

Neisser, U. C., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., . . .

Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist,

51(2), 77-101. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male, me= female,

therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44-59.

doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44

Ortner, T. M., & Sieverding, M. (2008). Where are the gender differences? Male priming

boosts spatial skills in women. Sex Roles, 59(3), 274-281. doi: 10.1007/s11199-

008-9448-9

Pansu, P., Régner, I., Max, S., Colé, P., Nezlek, J. B., & Huguet, P. (2016). A burden for

the boys: Evidence of stereotype threat in boys' reading performance. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 26-30. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.008

Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A

redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test-different versions and factors

that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28(1), 39-58. doi:

10.1006/brcg.1995.1032

Régner, I., Smeding, A., Gimmig, D., Thinus-Blanc, C., Monteil, J. M., & Huguet, P.

(2010). Individual differences in working memory moderate stereotype-threat

effects. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1646-1648.

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7),

e39904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Page 295: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 172

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-

analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521-535. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and

science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress

assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662. doi:

10.1037/edu0000012

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2018). Gender differences in reading and

writing achievement: Evidence from the National Assessment of Educational

Progress (NAEP). American Psychologist. doi: 10.1037/amp0000356

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in

specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147-158. doi:

10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Ritchie, S. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Enduring links from childhood mathematics and

reading achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychological Science, 24(7),

1301-1308. doi: 10.1177/0956797612466268

Robnett, R. D. (2015). Gender Bias in STEM fields. Psychology of Women Quarterly,

40(1), 65-79. doi: 10.1177/0361684315596162

Saucier, D. M., McCreary, D. R., & Saxberg, J. K. J. (2002). Does gender role

socialization mediate sex differences in mental rotations? Personality and

Individual Differences, 32(6), 1101-1111. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00106-4

Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women's

math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(2), 194-201.

doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1500

Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces

working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3),

440-452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440

Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender

differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women's experience in the math

domain. Sex Roles, 50(11), 835-850. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000029101.74557.a0

Sherman, J. A. (1967). Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of

intellectual functioning. Psychological Review, 74(4), 290-299. doi:

10.1037/h0024723

Page 296: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 173

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and

cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 207-228.

doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207

Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes,

and sexist attitudes: What do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(1),

44-62. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01021.x

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math

performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4-28. doi:

10.1006/jesp.1998.1373

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and

performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613-629. doi: 10.1037/0003-

066X.52.6.613

Steele, C. M. (1998). Stereotyping and its threat are real. American Psychologist, 53(6),

680-681. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.6.680

Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance

of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797-

811. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797

Steffens, M., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereotypes regarding math

and language: Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, but not

for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64(5), 324-335. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x

Verdine, B. N., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2017). Spatial

skills, their development, and their links to mathematics. Monographs of the

Society for Research in Child Development, 82(1), 7-30. doi: 10.1111/mono.12280

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial

abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological

Bulletin, 117(2), 250-270. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.117.2.250

Wai, J., Cacchio, M., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Sex differences in the right tail

of cognitive abilities: A 30 year examination. Intelligence, 38(4), 412-423. doi:

10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.006

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains:

Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its

importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817-835. doi:

10.1037/a0016127

Page 297: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 174

Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 39, 456-467. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00019-2

Weiss, E. M., Kemmler, G., Deisenhammer, E. A., Fleischhacker, W. W., & Delazer, M.

(2003). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Personality and Individual

Differences, 35(4), 863-875. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00288-X

Willingham, W., & Cole, N. S. (1997). Gender and fair assessment. Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Witkin, H. A. (1971). A manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., & Karp, S. A. (1962).

Psychological differentiation: Studies of development. New York: Wiley.

Page 298: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 175

Chapter 10 – Empirical Study 3 - Sex and Sex-Role Differences in Self-Estimated

Intelligence (SEI)

“Such is the nature of men, that howsoever they may acknowledge many others to be

more witty, or more eloquent, or more learned; yet they hardly believe there be many so

wise as themselves.” – Thomas Hobbes, English philosopher.

or “I’m smarter than the average bear” – Yogi Bear

Overview

The link between intelligence and academic achievement is fairly robust (Laidra,

Pullmann, & Allik, 2007; Rohde & Thompson, 2007), with correlations between

academic achievement and IQ in high school students ranging from .50 and .70 (Jensen,

1998). This effect is even stronger in younger students (Mayes, Calhoun, Bixler, &

Zimmerman, 2009). However, a great deal of educational success in post-secondary

education depends on non-cognitive factors, such as personality traits, academic

motivation, and self-efficacy beliefs. How we see ourselves intellectually – either as

smart, academically capable or possessing more mediocre abilities – can have a

profound impact on our academic engagement, motivation, and self-efficacy beliefs.

These, in turn, guide our intellectual interests and leisure activities, and ultimately the

academic and occupational paths we choose (or reject) in later life. But how is our

intellectual self-concept formed, and are there sex and sex-role differences in the

accuracy of these self-evaluations?

Much of the literature relating to this study has been presented in an earlier

chapter (see Sections 2.2.1-2.2.3), but to briefly recap there are several widely observed

research findings that have bearing on this issue. Firstly, many people see themselves as

being somewhat smarter than the average person, which has been termed the “above-

average effect” (Kruger & Dunning, 1999), and rarely do people rate themselves “below

average” (McCrae, 1990). The accuracy of self-judgements of intelligence is actually

Page 299: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 176

weak, with correlations between self-reports and psychometrically measured IQ

typically falling in the range of r = .20 to .25 in college samples (Paulhus, Lysy, & Yik,

1998). However a more recent meta-analysis by Freund and Kasten (2012) found

slightly higher average effect size of r = .33 when including non-academic samples

drawn from the community. Secondly, males and females typically differ in their self-

estimates, with males providing significantly higher self-estimates of intelligence than

females (despite the absence of sex differences in IQ for the general population). This

has been termed the “male hubris, female-humility” effect, and has been widely

replicated cross-culturally (Furnham, Hosoe, & Tang, 2001; Szymanowicz & Furnham,

2011). While the effect might at least partly reflect a social desirability bias, it is still

found in samples drawn from countries which emphasise communal traits of humility

and modesty. Thirdly, when rating the intellectual abilities of family members (parents,

spouses, and children), male relatives are still given higher scores on average than

female relatives by both sexes. Therefore there appears to be a genuine self-enhancing

bias in men for self-estimated intelligence and a self-derogatory bias in women

(Furnham et al., 2001). Less clear, though, are the mechanisms underlying sex

differences in self-estimated intelligence. Research has shown though that these sex

differences are found as early as fifth grade, where it is typically labelled as intellectual

self-concept (Gold, Brush, & Sprotzer, 1980; Marsh, 1989). The literature differentiates

between self-concept and self-esteem (Brinthaupt & Erwin, 1992), with self-concept

being the way an individual sees themselves and self-esteem referring to the affective

component. However they are strongly intertwined, and a negative intellectual concept

over time leads to reduced academic self-esteem.

In addition to overall impressions of global intelligence, sex differences in

intelligence are also found for more specific cognitive abilities, such as verbal or

Page 300: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 177

mathematical/scientific reasoning. A large number of studies have presented

descriptions of multiple intelligences after the fashion of Gardner (1983, 1999). It is not

necessary to accept the underlying premises of Gardner’s theory multiple intelligences,

but it does offer an insight into laypeople’s understanding of intelligence and cognitive

abilities. That sex differences in estimation of specific cognitive abilities exist may be

expected, because of cultural stereotypes associating specific cognitive abilities with

gender (Swim, 1994). For example, implicit associations between masculinity and

STEM and between femininity and arts/language are widely observed cross-culturally

(Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002). A meta-analysis by Syzmanowicz and Furnham

(2011) found robust sex differences in self-estimated abilities for mathematical/logical

(d = .44) and spatial intelligence (d = .43) for which there are strong prevailing gender

stereotypes. However, a significant but much smaller effect was found for self-estimates

of verbal intelligence (unweighted d = .12), due in part to the presence of four studies

where women provided higher estimates than males (ranging from d = -.38 to -.15).

This might reflect of prevailing gender stereotypes about female proficiency in language

having a moderating effect, or prior knowledge in the psychology subject pools sampled

of empirical research studies finding a general female advantage on verbal and language

abilities (see Section 2.2.1). Few consistent sex differences have been found for other

domains of Gardner’s multiple intelligences, though individual studies have reported

exceptions (Visser, Ashton, & Vernon, 2008; Yuen & Furnham, 2006).

While the male hubris/female humility effect has been widely documented, a

limitation of many previous studies is their reliance on self-reports without assessing

intelligence psychometrically. While there is near consensus in the literature that males

and females do not differ in intelligence, Becker and Hedges (1988) note that the

presence of greater male variability in the population and recruitment of non-randomly

Page 301: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 178

selected samples (such as from a psychology subject pool) may actually yield samples

with significant sex differences in intelligence. Without checking for differences in

intelligence in convenience samples, it is impossible to determine whether male

hubris/female humility effect is genuine or an artefact of sampling bias. Furthermore,

participants may be more truthful in providing estimates if they know that they are

going to complete an intelligence test, thus minimising social desirability bias.

The present study seeks to address such issues, by investigating sex differences

in self-estimated intelligence and concurrently administering the Cattell’s Culture Fair

IQ test. Additionally, it will explore whether sex-role identification might explain the

apparent differences between males and females, and whether these differences might

also be explained by participants’ general self-esteem. The rationale for considering

self-esteem was that many people with high self-esteem exaggerate their successes and

positive traits to themselves and to others, while people with low self-esteem focus

more strongly on failures and negative traits (Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs,

2003). Hansford and Hattie (1982) found a modestly sized correlation between self-

esteem and academic performance as measured by GPA, r = .34, so it does appear that

the way in which we see ourselves and intellectual achievement are related (if not

necessarily casual). To date, however, only one study has examined the relationship

between objectively measured intelligence and self-esteem. Gabriel, Critelli and Ee

(1994) found that people with high self-esteem rated themselves as more intelligent than

people with low self-esteem (r = .35), but the results of the IQ test did not justify such

rosy claims because there was no evidence for a relationship between self-esteem and

objectively measured intelligence. Additionally, the discrepancy between measured and

self-estimated intelligence was also positively correlated with participants self-esteem

scores (r = .38).

Page 302: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 179

Though not the primary the focus of this study, previous literature has also

identified associations between sex-role identification and self-esteem, with masculine

and androgynous participants reporting high levels of self-esteem and psychosocial

wellbeing when compared with feminine and undifferentiated groups (Alpert-Gillis &

Connell, 1989; Lau, 1989; Whitley, 1983). Males also report significantly higher levels

of general self-esteem than females (Kling, Hyde, Showers, & Buswell, 1999; Major,

Barr, Zubek, & Babey, 1999). On this basis it was reasoned that lower self-esteem

might partly explain sex and sex-role differences in self-estimated intelligence (SEI).

Hypotheses

1. Consistent with previous research, males will report higher SEI scores for

general intelligence than females, even in the absence of objectively

evaluated differences in intelligence.

2. Regardless of sex, high masculinity participants (i.e., masculine and

androgynous groups) will report higher SEI scores than low masculinity

ones (i.e., feminine and undifferentiated).

3. Males and high masculinity groups will report higher general self-esteem

and academic self-esteem than females and low masculinity groups

4. It is hypothesized that sex, masculinity, and general self-esteem will be

associated with SEI, even after controlling for psychometrically

measured intelligence.

5. Masculinity scores would act as a statistical mediator of the relationship

between sex and SEI scores.

6. Sex and sex-role differences will also be found in self-estimates of

multiple intelligences, following a similar pattern as observed with

general intelligence.

Page 303: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 180

Method

Participants

Two hundred and twenty-eight participants (103 male, 125 female) were

recruited from a university subject-pool of students completing a first-year research

methods and statistics course. As the distribution of psychological sex-roles is not even

in college samples, recruiting a larger number of participants was necessary to ensure a

reasonable cell size for analysis in each of the four sex-role categories. This subject pool

was chosen because it included psychology and non-psychology students in order to

draw from a broader pool of sex-role categories. Sex-role categories are not equally

represented in the general population (e.g., feminine-scoring males typically comprise

less than 12% of males, while a similar proportion is found for masculine sex-typed

females), and recruiting from a broader pool of candidates than just a single discipline

(psychology) maximises the likelihood of encountering sufficient numbers for each

sex/sex-role category. While the majority of these students were completing an

undergraduate psychological science degree (53.7%), a large proportion were enrolled

in exercise science or physiotherapy (30.7%), followed by health or biomedical sciences

(7.4%) and occupational therapy (4%). Only 3% were studying another type of degree

and had selected the subject as an elective. As is typical of university participants, the

distribution of ages was strongly positively skewed with a mean age of 22.62 (SD =

6.30, range = 18 to 47 years) and there was no significant difference in age between

males and females. Eighty-nine percent of the sample spoke English as their primary

language. All participants provided informed consent to a research protocol approved by

the Griffith University Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC).

Page 304: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 181

Procedure

Participants were informed that they were participating in a study on the

measurement of human intelligence, and the accuracy of self-estimates. They were

provided with a booklet containing the self-estimated intelligence (SEI) measures,

followed by the Cattell Cultural Fair IQ Test (CCFIT). To prevent intellectual fatigue,

rest periods were provided between each subtest of the CCFIT. Following test

administration, participants completed a number of personality surveys measuring self-

esteem, sex-role identification, and general demographic information. The personality

surveys were administered after the self-estimated intelligence survey and CCFIT, in

order to minimise gender priming effects on SEI and test performance. Participants

were tested in small batches (maximum 3 participants per session) so that compliance

with instructions could be monitored and that survey items were read and considered

before answering.

Measures

Self-estimated intelligence (SEI). Following the methodology of Furnham and

Rawles (1995), participants were provided with a simple one page sheet from the

booklet which explained in a brief paragraph that the distribution of intelligence in the

general population followed a bell curve (see Figure 10.1 for stimuli) that is normally

distributed, with the average IQ score being 100 with a standard deviation of 15. This

replicated following the methodology employed by Furnham and Rawles (1995) who

generously provided copies of stimulus materials, and has been used subsequently in

numerous self-estimated intelligence studies. The text of the paragraph was also read

aloud by the experimenter to ensure that written instructions were followed. While the

properties of the normal distribution were familiar to students in the statistics course,

labelled framing anchors were also provided to aid in estimation. Participants were

Page 305: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 182

asked to use this scale to provide an estimate of their intelligence relative to other

people, and to write this as a whole number.

Moderate Mild Below Average Above Gifted Exceptionally

Impairment Impairment Average Average Gifted

Figure 10.1 Stimulus material used for self-estimation of intelligence

On a subsequent page of the booklet, participants read several paragraphs

describing research of Gardner’s (1999) theory of multiple intelligences, which defined

intelligence more broadly than would be typically assessed by an IQ test. Gardner

subsequently revised his model of multiple intelligences to include a total of 9 separate

skills (Verbal and linguistic intelligence, Logical-mathematical intelligence, Spatial,

Musical, Bodily-kinaesthetic, Interpersonal, Intrapersonal, Naturalistic, and

Existential/Spiritual intelligence). Each skill was accompanied by a brief paragraph

description that had been pilot tested for readability. An issue identified in pilot testing

was that some participants completed the task extremely quickly with minimal variation

in scores across domains. So that participants gave considered and deliberated

responses, they were instructed to complete the task one definition at a time, and to

50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

IQ

Page 306: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 183

record a response only after the experimenter had read the paragraph aloud (on the

pretence ‘that some participants might come from a non-English background or have

reading impairments such as dyslexia, and we want to make sure instructions are clearly

understood’). This also ensured that participants had received the appropriate definition

for each task, even if they elected not to read the presented material. The definition of

existential / spiritual intelligence was phrased for inclusiveness so that it was clear to

subjects that this may include but does not require religious practice. Participants

responded by providing a numerical IQ score in the same format as for general

intelligence.

Cattell Culture Fair Test of Intelligence (CCFIT, Cattell, 1973). The CCFIT is a

non-verbal measure of fluid intelligence (gF), designed specifically to be as free of

culture and educational experiences as possible. This measure was selected for

inclusiveness, as it does not require a high standard of English language proficiency and

around 10% of the student body come from a background where English is not their

primary language. Additionally the student body also includes students who have

entered university from alternate pathways (5% of the current sample did not complete

high school), or who may have reading difficulties (estimates for Australia range from 5

to over 15% depending on the criteria and sampling methodology (Skues &

Cunningham, 2011). It has been designed to minimise cultural or educational biases

assumed to be present in the Weschler or Stanford-Binet IQ assessments, by excluding

items that require verbal and linguistic proficiency and general knowledge of a specific

culture. Additionally, prior research confirmed no sex bias in the CCFIT with

equivalent scores for males and females among adult high school graduates (Colom &

Garcıa-López, 2002).

Page 307: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 184

The specific instrument employed was CCFIT Scale 3, Form A intended for use

with adult participants. It contains very clear and simple instructions that are

administered in a spoken format by the tester, and requires only a multiple-choice

response. The test is also suitable for small group administration, which was desirable to

minimise testing time required if participants had been assessed sequentially. The test

includes practice items and safeguards to ensure understanding of and compliance with

instructions. The instrument also provides appropriate norms tables to allow for

conversion between raw scores and their equivalent IQ (centered around a mean of 100

with a standard deviation of 15), for direct comparability to SEI scores provided by

subjects. The CCFIT also shows strong convergent validity other tests of general

intelligence such as the WAIS with r = .72 (Cattell, Krug, & Barton, 1973), and loads

highly against more recently revised intelligence scales (Carroll, 1993).

The CCFIT assessment requires inductive reasoning about perceptual patterns,

and is comprised of four subtests (series completion, classification , matrices,

conditions/typology). Each subtest is completed under strict timing conditions, with

items of increasing level of difficulty such that less than 10% of subjects completed all

items in the current sample. Although there is no penalty for guessing, two of the

subtests require multiple correct responses for the item to be scored correctly. Individual

responses were recorded on response sheets that were transcribed and then computer

scored for accuracy of scoring. Reliability of the instrument for the current sample was

high across the four subtests (Cronbach’s α = .72).

General self-esteem. Participants completed the Rosenberg (1965) General Self

Esteem Scale, a brief 10 item rating scale that is widely used and demonstrates good

psychometric reliability and validity (Sinclair et al., 2010). Participants recorded a

response on a 4-point Likert-type scale (ranging from 1- “Strongly Agree”, to 4-

Page 308: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 185

“Strongly Disagree”). Sample items include “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”

and “All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure.”, with several items being

reverse coded (Cronbach’s α = .89 for sample).

Academic self-esteem. There were two measures. Subjects completed a seven-

item Academic Self-Esteem scale (see Appendix A3), adapted for this present study

from Johnson et al.’s (1983) Academic Self-Esteem subscale, and Bachman’s (1970)

Self-Concept of Ability Scale (SCAS). For comparability, subjects endorsed items on

the same 4 point scale used for the Rosenberg GSES. Sample items include “I feel

confident in my ability to complete university”, and “I am not doing as well at

university as I would like to” with negatively worded items that were reverse coded.

Subjects also completed the single item Rosenberg Academic Self-Esteem scale, which

asks “How do you rate yourself in academic ability compared with those studying your

degree” on a 5-point scale (ranging from 1- “Far below average” to 5- “Far above

average”). The final response variable incorporated both measures of academic self-

esteem, with high reliability (Cronbach’s α = .87) for the eight-item scale (see Appendix

A4).

Bem Sex-Role Inventory. The 30 item short form of the Bem Sex Role Inventory

(BSRI; 1974, 1981) was used as a measure of sex-role identification. The BSRI is a

general personality inventory that includes 10 masculine, 10 feminine as well as 10

neutral and filler items so that the gendered nature of the instrument is not transparent.

Traits are rated on a 7-point Likert scale (from “1 – Never or almost never true of me”

to a midpoint of “4 – Occasionally true” and ending in “7 – Always or almost always

true of me”). Separate masculinity and femininity scores were produced by averaging

responses across each scale, resulting in a continuous score suitable for regression

analysis. Participants were also categorised on the basis of a median split of their

Page 309: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 186

masculinity (Mdn = 4.60) and femininity (Mdn = 5.30) scores, to one of four sex-role

categories: masculine, feminine, androgynous (high masculinity and high femininity)

and undifferentiated (low in both masculine and feminine personality traits). Internal

consistency, as assessed by Cronbach’s α, was high in the present sample (masculinity

scale α = .81, femininity scale α = .85) and despite the passage of time since its

inception the BSRI remains a valid measure of sex-role identification in modern

samples (Choi, Fuqua, & Newman, 2007).

Results

Sex Role Classification

Table 10.1 presents the distribution of sex-role categories in our sample for

males and females. As would be expected from past research, an independent samples

t-test showed that males were significantly higher in BSRI masculinity scores than

females, t(225) = 3.04, p = .003, d = .41, and that females were significantly higher than

males in BSRI femininity, t(225) = -2.48, p = .014, d = -.33 than males.

Table 10.1

Distribution of Sex-Role Categories in Sample

Sex-Role Classification

Gender Masculine Feminine Androgynous Undifferentiated

Males 29 17 34 23

(28.2%) (16.5%) (33.0%) (22.3%)

Females 23 33 36 32

(18.5%) (26.6%) (29.0%) (25.8%)

Cattell’s Culture Fair Intelligence Test (CCFIT)

In order to examine the distribution of intelligence in our sample, I converted the

raw CCFIT scores to IQ scores using the norms outlined in the Cattell (1973) manual.

The distribution of IQ scores was approximately normally distributed (see Figure 10.2,

Page 310: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 187

with no significant skewness or kurtosis, Shapiro-Wilks p > .001) but did contain

several low-scoring outliers. A one-sample t-test revealed that our sample mean was

significantly higher than that of the general population t(223) = 11.83, p < .001, d =

1.57. An independent samples t-test confirmed that males and females in our sample did

not significantly differ in measured intelligence, t(226) = 1.27, p = .206. Any observed

sex difference in SEI could not, therefore, be explained by apparent differences in actual

intelligence resulting from sampling bias. Additionally, a 2 × (Sex) 4 × (Sex-Role

Category) factorial ANOVA confirmed no sex-role differences in measured intelligence,

nor any interaction, all Fs < 2.61, p > .05.

Figure 10.2. Distribution of measured IQ scores in the sample

Page 311: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 188

General and Academic Self-Esteem

I next examined sex and sex-role differences in general self-esteem, using a 2 ×

(Sex) 4 × (Sex-Role Category) factorial ANOVA (see Figure 10.3). The assumptions of

normality and homogeneity of variance were met. There was a significant main effect of

sex, F(1, 219) = 6.71, p = .010, η2 = .03, with males giving higher self-reports of general

self-esteem than females (d = .40). Additionally there was a significant main effect of

sex-role category, F(3, 219) = 7.88, p < .001, η2 = .10, but no interaction between these

terms. The effect of sex-role category was stronger than biological sex In line with

experimental hypotheses, a planned contrast confirmed that masculine and androgynous

subjects reported higher general self-esteem scores than feminine and undifferentiated,

t(225) = 4.62, p < .001, d = .62, which is a medium effect by Cohen’s (1988)

conventions.

Figure 10.3. Rosenberg General Self-Esteem scores across sex and sex-role categories.

Page 312: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 189

Next I examined the construct of academic self-esteem, which was hypothesized

as being more tightly coupled to a participant’s self-estimated intelligence score. A 2 ×

(Sex) 4 × (Sex-Role Category) factorial ANOVA was conducted on academic self-

esteem, and all assumptions were met. As was the case with general self-esteem, males

reported significantly higher academic self-esteem than females, F(1, 219) = 15.01, p <

.001, η2 = .06, as well as a significant main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 219) = 6.04,

p = .001, η2 = .08. However the interaction was not significant, and again the sex-role

identification effect was slightly stronger than biological sex. The planned contrast

demonstrated that participants with high masculinity (masculine and androgynous sex

roles) reported significantly higher academic self-esteem than participants with low

masculinity (feminine and undifferentiated sex roles), t(225) = 4.26, p < .001, d = .57,

which is a medium effect size.

Self-Estimated Intelligence (SEI) Scores

The distribution of self-estimated intelligence scores in our sample was

significantly negatively skewed (std. skewness = 2.19), with a tendency for participants

to rate their intelligence as “above average”, and a mean SEI of 107.55 (SD = 10.98).

Surprisingly, quite a number of participants (approximately 19%) rated their

intelligence as below average, with scores ranging from 70 IQ points to a maximum of

135. This was unexpected as the ‘above average’ effect is generally robust, and issue I

address further in the discussion.

A 2 × (Sex) 4 × (Sex-Role Category) factorial ANOVA4 was conducted on self-

estimated IQ scores (see Figure 10.4). Although mild negative skewness was present

(absolute standardized skewness = 2.23, p < .05), the ANOVA is robust against minor

4 A reflected log transformation was applied to the distribution and the analysis repeated, with no change in outcome. As the untransformed data was in a metric (IQ score) that was more meaningful, the untransformed data is reported. Additionally the analysis was run with CCFIT as a covariate with no change in outcome.

Page 313: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 190

violations of normality when variances are equal (Field & Wilcox, 2017). The

assumption of homogeneity of variance was met. As predicted by prior research, there

was a significant main effect of sex, F(1, 219) = 30.79, p < .001,

η2 = .12, with males (M = 112.12, SD = 9.20) reporting significantly higher estimated

IQ than females (M = 103.66, SD = 10.88), t(225) = 5.55, p < .001, d = .74, which

equates to a difference of approximately 8.5 IQ points. There was also a significant

main effect of sex-role category, F(3, 219) = 7.23, p < .001, η2 = .09. A planned linear

contrast compared the high masculinity participants (masculine + androgynous) to the

low masculinity participants (feminine + undifferentiated). As hypothesized masculine

and androgynous subjects gave higher self-estimates of IQ than feminine and

undifferentiated, t(225) = 4.65, p < .001, d = .62. Both effects were medium in size.

There was no significant interaction between sex and sex-role category.

Figure 10.4. Self-estimated IQ scores across sex-role categories, for males and females

Page 314: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 191

Bivariate Correlations

Bivariate correlations between all measures are reported in Table 10.2.

Directions of correlations were consistent with previous literature, with sex and

masculinity being significantly associated with self-estimated IQ, both measures of self-

esteem, and with IQ discrepancy scores.

Table 10.2

Bivariate Correlations between Sex and Sex-Role Measures, Self-Estimated

Intelligence, Measured Intelligence, General and Academic Self-Esteem (N = 228)

Measure 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.

1. Sex a - -.21** .16* -.38*** -.08 -.20** -.20** -.27***

2. BSRI masculinity - .02 .34*** .06 .19** .37*** .26***

3. BSRI femininity - -.04 -.07 .04 .11 -.04

4. Self-estimated IQ - .30*** .44*** .28*** .45***

5. Cattell IQ - -.72*** -.02 .08

6. IQ Discrepancy - .22** .25***

7. Rosenberg Self-Esteem - .54***

8. Academic Self-Esteem -

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

a Dummy coded variable; 0 = male, 1 = female

Predictors of Sex Differences in Self-Estimated Intelligence

Next I set out to explore possible explanations for the male hubris, female

humility effect. In the sample, the correlation between SEI and measured intelligence

was just at the cusp of being medium in strength r (228) = .30, p < .001, and the

scatterplot confirmed it was linear in nature. One possible explanation might be that

males and females greatly differ in the accuracy of their judgments of self-estimated

intelligence. To test this hypothesis, I calculated the bivariate correlation between SEI

Page 315: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 192

and measured intelligence for males and females separately. The correlation between

SEI and measured intelligence was slightly higher for males, r(103) = .33, p < .001,

than for females, r(124) = .26, p = .004 but both fell in the small to medium range of

effect sizes, and any difference most likely reflects sampling error. To confirm this,

Fisher’s r-to-z transformation was applied to assess the significance of the difference

between the two correlation coefficients rmale and rfemale, zdif = .57, p = .284 (1-tailed).

Thus I was able to rule out glaring differences in accuracy between males and females

(see Figure 10.5).

Figure 10.5. Scatterplot of association between self-estimated and psychometric IQ, for

males and females respectively.

Another plausible explanation for sex differences in SEI might be the

contribution of self-esteem. Reported in Table 10.2, there was a moderate positive

correlation between self-estimated intelligence and general self-esteem scores. However

Page 316: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 193

it is also plausible that having a high intellect also makes a positive contribution to

one’s general self-esteem, so I tested whether the correlation between SEI and general

self-esteem remained significant after controlling for scores on the Cattell Culture Fair

test. The positive correlation between SEI and Rosenberg General Self Esteem with

CCFIT scores partialled out was still statistically significant, r = .30, p < .001, and of

moderate strength (i.e. general self-esteem was associated with self-estimates of

intelligence). As might be expected, the correlation between SEI and academic self-

esteem was somewhat stronger, r = .45, though this is likely to be a reciprocal

relationship.

To explore the joint effects of biological sex, sex-role identification, and general

self-esteem, I performed a hierarchical multiple regression on self-estimated intelligence

scores (see Table 10.3). In order to control for individual differences in actual

intelligence, at Step 1 I entered Cattell IQ CCFIT scores as the sole predictor,

Fchg(1,223) = 22.71, p < .001, explaining approximately 9% of the variance in SEI. Next

in Step 2, I entered biological sex, as well as BSRI masculinity and femininity scores.

Although only sex and masculinity were hypothesized to make a significant

contribution to SEI scores, femininity was included to rule out the possibility of a

significant negative association. Together these factors resulted in an increased model

fit, Fchg(3,220) = 20.76, p < .001, explaining an additional 20% of variance in the

dependent variable. Both sex and masculinity scores were significant predictors. Finally

at Step 3, I entered General Self-Esteem scores to test the hypothesis that self-esteem

may be a contributing factor. This resulted in a small increase in model fit, Fchg(1,219) =

4.39, p < .001. The final model was statistically significant, F(5, 219) = 19.36, p < .001,

accounting for 31.7% of the variance in individual self-estimates of intelligence. As can

be seen from the table, even after controlling for individual differences in measured

Page 317: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 194

intelligence (β = .27), the three hypothesized predictors of biological sex (β = -.30),

masculinity (β = .21) and general self-esteem (β = .13) made significant and unique

contributions. Biological sex was the strongest predictor, followed by measured

intelligence, masculinity, and finally a smaller contribution of general self-esteem which

had considerable overlap with the other predictors.

Table 10.3

Hierarchical multiple regression of Self-Estimated Intelligence scores (N = 255)

Variable t p-value sr2 R R2

Step 1 30 .09

Cattell IQ .30 4.77 <.001*** .09

Step 2 .54 .29

Cattell IQ .26 4.61 <.001*** .06

Sex (0 = male) -.32 -5.46 <.001*** .10

Masculinity .26 4.42 <.001*** .06

Femininity .03 .58 .562 .00

Step 3 .55 .31

Cattell IQ .27 4.72 <.001*** .07

Sex (0 = male) -.30 -5.09 <.001*** .08

Masculinity .21 3.44 .001** .05

Femininity .02 .28 .780 .00

General Self-Esteem .13 2.10 .037* .02

* p < .05, ** p <.01, *** p <.001

Next I examined whether masculine sex-role identification (masculinity score as

a continuous variable) acted as a statistical mediator in the relationship between

biological sex and SEI scores (see Figure 10.6) to test hypothesis 5. Baron and Kenny

(1986) proposed three criteria for establishing statistical mediation. Firstly the predictor

(biological sex) should predict the dependent variable (SEI). Secondly, the predictor

must be correlated with the proposed mediator variable (masculine sex-role

Page 318: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 195

identification, shown as Path A). Thirdly the mediator must correlate with the

dependent variable (SEI) even after controlling for the contribution of the predictor

(shown as Path B). The Sobel test of statistical mediation was significant, Sobel z = -

2.55, p = .010, and calculation of the bootstrapped estimate of the indirect effect showed

that it differed significantly from zero [95% CI = -2.26 to -0.41], following the criteria

outlined in Preacher and Hayes (2004). As the mediation effect was significant, I then

tested whether the relationship was fully or only partially mediated (Baron & Kenny,

1986). In a full mediation model, the association between predictor and dependent

variable will no longer be statistically significant after controlling for the mediator (i.e.,

all of the effect of the predictor acts indirectly through the mediator, and does not make

a direct contribution). This relationship is represented by Path C in Figure 10.6. Though

diminished, the beta weight remained statistically significant, indicating that the

relationship was only a partial mediation. Though acting indirectly through masculine

sex-role identification, there was still a direct contribution of sex to SEI scores.

Figure 10.6. Indirect effect of sex on SEI, with masculine sex-roles acting as a mediator

on self-estimated intelligence. Path C represents the direct effect of sex after controlling

for the mediator.

Having identified in the multiple regression analysis that biological sex made a

slightly stronger contribution to SEI than measured intelligence, sex-role identification,

Sex Self-Estimated Intelligence

Masculine sex-role identification

Path A = -.21***

Path B = .27***

Path C = -.33***

Page 319: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 196

and general self-esteem, I sought to quantify how large the discrepancy between self-

estimates and measured intelligence was. A composite variable representing the

discrepancy between self-estimated and measured intelligence was created, with

positive values indicating higher SEI than measured intelligence. An independent

samples t-test on IQ discrepancy scores confirmed a significant sex difference, t(225) =

3.00, p = .003, d = .40. Visual inspection of the discrepancy scores showed that on

average, males in our sample demonstrated fairly sound judgement in appraising their

intelligence (M = -0.36, SD = 13.60), but that there was also wide variability with some

males greatly overestimating their intelligence and some males underestimating (range

= -27 to +38 IQ points). However females systematically undervalued their intellectual

capabilities by over 6 IQ points (M = -6.32, SD = 15.89), and for those female

participants who did offer inflated self-estimates, these were much smaller in size

(range = -41 to +25 IQ points).

To confirm our interpretation of the data, a categorical variable named accuracy

direction was created to measure whether participants had underestimated their

intelligence (discrepancy score < -5 IQ points), overestimated their intelligence

(discrepancy score > +5 points), or made an accurate assessment (in the range -5 to +5

IQ points discrepancy). Though arbitrary, this represents a discrepancy between

perceived and objectively assessed IQ of one third of a standard deviation or Cohen’s d

= .33. Chi-square analysis (see Table 10.4) showed that there were significant sex

differences in accuracy direction, χ2 (2, N = 228) = 7.26, p = .027. Inspection of the

adjusted standardized residuals showed that there were significantly more females

underestimating their intelligence than males (adj. z = 2.7, p = .004 1-tailed). In

particular, over half of the female participants significantly underestimated their

intellectual ability compared to only a third of the male participants. Consistent with the

Page 320: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 197

male hubris effect, there were also significantly more males than females overestimating

their intelligence (adj z = 1.8, p = .035 1-tailed). However, if a non-directional 2-tailed

comparison were used, the sex difference in overestimators would just fall short of

statistical significance.

Table 10.4

Accuracy direction of male and female participants

Accuracy direction

Sex Underestimating Accurate Overestimating

Male 35.9% 32.0% 32.0%

Female 53.6% 24.8% 21.6%

Self-estimates of multiple intelligences

Next, a 2 × (Sex) 4 × (Sex-Role Category) factorial MANOVA was performed on

the nine self-estimates of Gardner’s multiple intelligences. As the cell size differed

across sex-role category and Box’s M was significant (p < .001), Pillai’s trace was

selected as the more conservative estimate. Assumptions of normality and homogeneity

of variance were met. In line with previous research, there was a significant multivariate

effect of biological sex, F(9, 212) = 7.02, p < .001, η2 = .23 which is a medium to large

effect. There was also a significant multivariate effect of sex-role identification, F(27,

642) = 2.22, p < .001, η2 = .09, though there was no significant interaction F(27, 642) =

1.02, p = .437. As the overall multivariate effects were significant and of non-trivial

size, this justified examination of univariate effects without a need to apply a

Bonferroni correction (c.f. Huberty & Morris, 1989). For ease of comparison, sex and

sex-role differences are reported separately in Table 10.5 and 10.6 respectively. Five of

Page 321: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 198

the nine multiple intelligence domains showed significant differences between males

and females, with effect sizes ranging from small to large.

Table 10.5

Sex differences on self-estimated multiple intelligences

Domain Male Female F(1,220) p-value d

1. Verbal 106.45 (12.87) 107.07 (11.65) .73 .395 -.05

2. Logical-Mathematical 108.39 (16.68) 98.66 (13.43) 18.36 < .001*** .64

3. Spatial 109.80 (12.51) 98.54 (11.93) 40.79 < .001*** .92

4. Musical 102.64 (18.11) 99.50 (14.72) .64 .426 .19

5. Bodily-kinaesthetic 112.57 (14.26) 106.47 (14.74) 7.54 .007** .42

6. Interpersonal 112.69 (12.98) 112.86 (11.72) .20 .654 -.01

7. Intrapersonal 110.61 (12.63) 109.36 (12.79) .11 .742 .09

8. Naturalistic 104.43 (11.88) 99.10 (11.06) 10.36 .001** .46

9. Existential/spiritual 108.72 (16.92) 102.94 (12.84) 6.85 .009** .39

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001;

Table 10. presents sex-role differences across the nine multiple intelligence

domains. Although sex differences were not present for every domain (Table 10.4),

there were significant sex-role differences for each of the domains. Accordingly a

planned linear contrast was conducted comparing the high masculinity groups

(masculine + androgynous) with the low masculinity groups. Masculine persons

reported significantly higher self-estimates of multiple intelligences, with effect sizes

ranging from small to medium in size.

Page 322: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 199

Table 10.6

Sex-role Differences in Self-Estimated Multiple Intelligences

Domain Masc. Fem. Andr. Undif. F-ratio Planned Contrast

1. Verbal 110.35

(1.61)

104.51

(1.81)

106.74

(1.51)

104.54

(1.66)

2.78* t(226) = 2.44, p = .015, d = .33

2. Logical-Mathematical 106.66

(1.98)

98.41

(2.22)

104.38

(1.85)

103.23

(2.04)

2.68* t(226) = 2.32, p = .021, d = .32

3. Spatial 105.71

(1.59)

100.34

(1.79)

107.01

(1.49)

101.83

(1.64)

3.70* t(226) = 3.22, p = .001, d = .43

4. Musical 102.98

(2.12)

95.86

(2.38)

105.23

(1.99)

97.40

(2.18)

4.25** t(226) = 3.44, p = .001, d = .46

5. Bodily-kinaesthetic 112.18

(1.86)

106.22

(2.10)

114.00

(1.75)

103.96

(1.92)

6.47*** t(226) = 4.18, p < .001, d = .56

6. Interpersonal

113.82

(1.53)

112.36

(1.72)

117.87

(1.44)

105.83

(1.58)

10.82*** t(226) = 4.30, p < .001, d = .57

7. Intrapersonal 110.48

(1.66)

107.58

(1.87)

113.88

(1.56)

106.44

(1.71)

4.09** t(226) = 3.03, p = .003, d = .40

8. Naturalistic 101.83

(1.50)

100.49

(1.69)

104.20

(1.41)

99.12

(1.55)

2.15 t(226) = 2.09, p = .038, d = .27

9. Existential/spiritual 106.80

(1.93)

104.23

(2.17)

110.47

(1.81)

100.57

(1.99)

4.79** t(226) = 3.15, p = .002, d = .42

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001;

Discussion

The main aim of this study was to investigate factors that may explain why

males tend to provide higher self-estimates of their intelligence than females. A meta-

analysis by Szymanowicz and Furnham (2011) demonstrated that the male-hubris,

female humility effect was robust, with an average effect size of d = .37, but that this

was somewhat higher in samples drawn from psychology and social science subject

pools, d = .48. Furham and Rawles (1995) have attributed this effect to a misbelief held

by psychology students about the intellectual superiority of males. Still, sex differences

in SEI for our sample (d =.74) were somewhat higher in our sample than would be

Page 323: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 200

expected based on previous studies with psychology subject pools (Szymanowicz &

Furnham, 2011), falling into the medium to large range. Importantly I was also able to

rule out the possibility of an actual sex difference in measured intelligence in our

sample (arising due to sampling bias from a university educated subject pool). Group

differences between males and females in SEI were illusory and did not reflect actual

intelligence differences. Consistent with hypotheses there was also an independent

effect of sex-role identification in SEI, with masculine and androgynous subjects

reporting higher self-estimates than feminine and undifferentiated, d = .62 which is also

medium to large effect size.

Explanations for Sex Differences in Self-Estimated Intelligence

An important research question has been whether people hold realistic views of

their own intelligence, or whether they are distorted. This question has been of broader

interest to researchers (irrespective of gender), with a large number of studies

investigating whether people are accurate judges of their own ability (Kruger &

Dunning, 1999). The relationship between self-estimated and psychometrically

measured intelligence is generally fairly weak (Paulhus et al., 1998), suggesting that

while actual ability does contribute to the self-image that we hold, there are other

factors at play.

A limitation of the studies mentioned above is that they did not test whether

there were sex differences in accuracy of intellectual perceptions (for example, if males

held somewhat inflated perceptions while females were more pragmatic). Indeed

relatively few studies have explicitly tested the hypothesis that there might be dramatic

sex differences in accuracy of perceptions. Some studies have found weaker correlations

in females than males for the relationship between SEI and measured intelligence, while

other studies have found their accuracy to be comparable. For example, Borkenau and

Page 324: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 201

Liebler (1993) found the correlation between SEI and measured intelligence to be

equivalent for males and females. Similarly, Reilly5 and Mulhern (1995) found similar

correlations between SEI and measured intelligence for males and females. However, a

third study by Furnham and Rawles (1999) found that only males showed a significant

correlation between SEI and measured intelligence. Another study by Furnham and

Fong (2000) using the Raven’s Progressive Matrices as a measure of fluid intelligence

also provided mixed findings in a sample of British and Singaporean students. In the

British sample, the relationship between SEI and measured intelligence was significant

for males but not for females. However, both males and females showed a similar

degree of accuracy in the Singaporean sample with a medium effect size.

In our sample, the correlation between SEI and measured intelligence was

moderate in strength, r = .30. Furthermore, there was a significant association for both

males and females and no significant difference in correlation strengths (see Figure

10.5). Thus it is possible to rule out gross disparities in accuracy between males and

females as one possible explanation (though there were still discrepancies in direction,

with a self-enhancing bias in males and self-deriding bias in females). Next, I examined

the discrepancy scores and accuracy direction. Consistent with the male hubris, female

humility effect, chi-square analysis showed that there was a greater frequency of males

overestimating their intelligence than for females. Furthermore there were also

significantly more females than males underestimating their ability.

One reason for these directional differences this might be sex differences in

modesty norms (Rudman, 1998), and social pressure on women to demonstrate public

modesty. But given that sex differences in intellectual self-concept are seen so early in

childhood, another plausible explanation for sex differences in SEI might be the

5 No relation to the present author

Page 325: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 202

contribution of self-esteem, explored by Hypotheses 3 and 4. Although most people

tend to see themselves in a rosy light, as more honest, more attractive, more intelligent

than the average person (Kruger & Dunning, 1999), for people with low general self-

esteem, views of the self are decidedly negative, especially when making social

comparisons to others. Consistent with Gabriel, Critelli and Ee (1994), self-estimated

intelligence and general self-esteem were related with a moderately sized correlation.

Having a positive self-image of oneself generally leads to higher self-reports of

intelligence with a similarly sized contribution as psychometrically measured

intelligence. The analysis also found the discrepancy between SEI and measured SEI

was positively associated with self-esteem (i.e. people with high self-esteem showed a

tendency to overestimate their intelligence). Given the widely documented sex

differences in self-esteem (Kling et al., 1999), as well as literature on the contribution of

sex-role identification to self-esteem (Hirschy & Morris, 2002; Whitley & Gridley,

1993) and in light of the present findings, self-esteem warrants further investigation in

future SEI studies as one mechanism underlying sex differences in SEI. It also suggests

that sex differences in SEI are genuinely held beliefs, countering the argument that they

might be due to modesty norms and differ from actual self-concept (Rudman, 1998).

Next I used a hierarchical regression model to determine the joint effects of sex,

sex-role identification, and general self-esteem. Each made a significant and unique

contribution to overall SEI scores, even after statistically controlling for actual fluid

intelligence. Masculinity was positively correlated with higher SEI, but there was no

effect of femininity as a personality trait. The relationship between sex and SEI was

partially mediated by masculine sex-role identification, and met the criteria for

statistical mediation. Thus there was an indirect effect of sex on SEI scores mediated

through masculinity, as well as a direct effect. Much of the contribution of self-esteem

Page 326: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 203

overlapped with sex and sex-role identification (as would be expected given the

documented sex/sex-role effects in the literature) but it still made a significant and

unique contribution.

Self-estimates of multiple intelligences

Next, I examined sex and sex-role differences in estimates of multiple

intelligences. Only five of the nine multiple intelligences showed statistically significant

sex differences. Consistent with the meta-analysis by Szymanowics and Furnham

(2011), logical-mathematical and spatial showed the largest differences, but meaningful

effect sizes were also found for bodily-kinaesthetic, naturalistic and existential/spiritual

intelligence. Visser, Ashton and Vernan (2008) have also reported a moderately large

sex difference on self-estimates of bodily-kinaesthetic intelligence, but relatively few

studies have evaluated naturalistic and existential multiple intelligences as they were

only recently added to Gardner’s revised model. However Furnham and Ward (2001)

reported significantly higher estimates by males for naturalistic intelligence, and a study

by Yuen and Furnham (2006) found significant non-trivial sex differences for bodily-

kinaethetic, naturalistic, and existential/spiritual intelligence consistent with our pattern

of results. Intelligence domains that did not show significant differences in self-

estimates were verbal, musical, and the two aspects of emotional intelligence

(intrapersonal and interpersonal). Perhaps the reason is that gender stereotypes for these

domains generally favour women (Bennett, 2000), thus offsetting the tendency towards

higher estimation by for intelligence generally. It might also be the case that women are

less likely to underestimate their abilities here and more likely to underestimate in

domains associated with males, given popular gender stereotypes. A number of previous

studies have also found non-significant effects for these domains (Furnham, Clark, &

Bailey, 1999; Rammstedt & Rammsayer, 2002b), which would be consistent with the

Page 327: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 204

interpretation that gender stereotypes favouring women buffers against the trend

towards higher male self-estimates generally.

While sex differences were found for some, but not all, multiple intelligences

domains, the questionnaire was more sensitive to sex-role effects. Planned contrasts

revealed that the high masculinity groups (masculine + androgynous) reported

significantly higher self-estimates than the low masculinity groups (feminine +

undifferentiated), regardless of biological sex. Just as with general self-esteem and

academic self-esteem, masculinity seemed to have an enhancing effect on self-estimates

of intelligence in these domains. Several other studies have investigated sex-role

identification effects on the estimates of multiple intelligences with mixed findings.

Rammstedt and Rammsayer (2002a) used the BSRI in a German sample, finding sex-

role identification acted as a moderator for some but not all domains. Furnham, Clark

and Bailey (1999; Study 2) recruited a small sample of students (n = 80) using the PAQ

instrument to measure sex-role identification and did not find sex-role effects, but their

study was extremely underpowered. More recently, Szymanowicz and Furnham (2013)

recruited a British sample from the general population, finding significant sex-role

effects for verbal, social, emotional, and practical intelligence factors, but not the

expected effect for mathematical/logical and spatial intelligence. However subsequent

regression analyses that treated masculinity as a continuous variable showed a

significant positive association on all but the emotional intelligence factor. Thus our

study replicates their findings that masculinity leads to higher self-estimated

intelligence scores for multiple intelligence domains.

Implications and limitations

Our study expands on the existing body of literature on self-estimated

intelligence by ruling out dramatic sex differences in accuracy, but also showing that

Page 328: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 205

general self-esteem is as strongly correlated with SEI as is psychometrically measured

intelligence. Having a positive self-image is correlated with higher self-estimated

intelligence, though causality cannot be determined. Significant sex and sex-role

differences in self-estimated intelligence were found despite there being no difference in

psychometrically measured intelligence. In particular, masculinity acted as a protective

factor while there was no effect of femininity. The relationship between sex and self-

estimated intelligence was partially mediated by masculine sex-role identification, but

there remained a significant direct effect of sex as well. Additionally, significant sex-

role differences were found on all nine multiple intelligence domains, even in the

absence of sex differences.

The effect we were investigating has been termed by Furnham et.al (2001) as the

“male-hubris, female humility” effect. This phraseology stems from the observation that

males rate their intellectual abilities as being much higher than females, despite

compelling psychometric evidence that sex differences in general intelligence do not

exist (Halpern, 2011; Jensen, 1998). In our study, the inclusion of the Cattell Culture

Fair Intelligence Test afforded the opportunity to examine the discrepancy between SEI

and measured IQ, and the nature of sex differences. Although significant sex differences

in SEI were observed, especially for direction of the discrepancy, as noted earlier the

average discrepancy score in IQ for males approached zero with inflated scores being

largely offset by under-estimates. This finding seems peculiar, and requires further

investigation. It may have one of several explanations. It may be a property of the

sample recruited in the present study: our sample consisted of primarily first-year

university students, with a mixture of below-average IQ (15.5%) and mature-age

students who may be lacking in self-confidence and doubting their intellectual ability.

There was a moderately strong positive association between self-estimated IQ and

Page 329: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 206

academic self-esteem (as well as a significant correlation between academic self-esteem

and discrepancy scores), so our sample may differ from other student samples who have

been studying longer and are more settled. Alternately, it may be that college subject

pools drawn from American samples have higher self-esteem, as has been claimed by

some researchers (Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2005; Diener & Diener,

1995). Another explanation may be an unintended consequence of the informed consent

and briefing materials, in that participants were aware they would soon complete an

intelligence test to compare against their self-estimates. This knowledge might have

dampened self-estimated IQ scores somewhat, leaving some males to provide more

conservative estimates than they would otherwise without the knowledge that their

scores would be actually checked. However, as demonstrated by the chi-square analysis

of accuracy of estimates there were still a large number of males with inflated self-

estimates, as much as 38 IQ points – so if it did exert an effect it did not do so

uniformly.

Another possibility is that it might be a property of the Cattell Culture Fair Test

itself. Although an excellent measure of fluid intelligence, the instrument has not been

revised since 1973, and the provided norms tables may have resulted in inflated IQ

scores. The well documented Flynn effect of rising IQ scores across historical time may

have been an issue. A recent meta-analysis by Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) has found

that rises in intelligence vary depending on the domain, with fluid intelligence

(measured by the Cattell instrument) showing the greatest gains, approximately 0.41 per

annum. While the instrument remains a reliable and valid measure of nonverbal fluid

intelligence to include in a regression of self-estimated intelligence, the outdated norms

tables may have yielded an inflated IQ score for calculating discrepancy scores. Thus I

cannot rule out the possibility that, on average, males are still overestimating their level

Page 330: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 207

of intelligence in line with the male-hubris effect, but that the outdated norm table

resulted in a discrepancy score in our sample for males approaching zero. Nonetheless, I

did see a substantial underestimation for the females in our sample, consistent with the

female humility effect. So few studies investigate discrepancy scores, and it would be

interesting to see if there is still a pattern ‘male hubris’ when intelligence is measured

by other instruments that provide a Full-Scale IQ such as the WAIS or Stanford-Binet

tests.

Conclusions

How we see ourselves intellectually can have a profound influence on academic

engagement. Self-perceptions that are completely out of touch with reality are

problematic: when unrealistically high it may set students up for eventual failure and

disappointment, but when unrealistically low it may result in student disengagement,

lowered academic expectations, and a failure to pursue educational options that a

student is capable of achieving. Research has identified that academic self-concept is

strongly linked to academic achievement, and that this is a reciprocal relationship

(Marsh, 1990; Marsh, Trautwein, Lüdtke, Köller, & Baumert, 2005; Valentine, DuBois,

& Cooper, 2004)While the pattern of higher self-estimates of intelligence by males than

females is a robust effect replicated cross-culturally, relatively little progress had been

made in understanding why this effect exists. Our results point to two contributing

factors, firstly the influence of general self-esteem and secondly sex-role identification.

Issues of low self-esteem are generally considered in the context of mental health and

psychosocial wellbeing, but our results show that it may partly contribute to the so-

called ‘male hubris, female humility’ effect as well. Starting from adolescence onwards,

girls and women are overrepresented in depressive symptoms even in non-clinical

populations (Wang et al., 2016), and lowered self-esteem is one consequence of this.

Page 331: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 208

Our results suggest that lowered self-esteem underlies the male hubris, female humility

effect, and is an underappreciated risk factor for educational achievement. Though

dated, a meta-analysis by Hansford and Hattie (1982) reported a positive association

between self-esteem and academic achievement (r = .22), supporting such a conclusion

that a negative intellectual self-concept may translate to poorer educational outcomes.

Eccles (1994, 2013) expectancy-value model of achievement motivation suggests that a

crucial factor in achievement-related choices is their perceptions of their own abilities

and expectations of success.

Additionally a large body of research (e.g. Else-Quest, Mineo, & Higgins, 2013)

has reported lower female self-efficacy beliefs in mathematics and science, and

suggested that these may be far more influential than actual ability in contributing to the

underrepresentation of women in STEM. However these have almost always been

interpreted in light of cultural stereotypes associating STEM with masculinity (Nosek et

al., 2002), rather than considering the contribution of a negative intellectual self-image

more broadly. In planning educational interventions to raise STEM performance,

addressing academic self-esteem and intellectual self-image may be an important targets

for consideration. Some educational interventions such as values affirmation writing

exercises (Kost-Smith et al., 2012; Miyake et al., 2010) buffer against intellectual

stereotype threat, and may be particularly useful in an educational setting in combatting

lowered intellectual self-image (Martens, Johns, Greenberg, & Schimel, 2006). Also

while sex-role identification is not a desirable target for intervention, it may identify

students of either gender prone to underestimating their intellectual abilities, whom

parents and educators can be mindful of as needing further support and encouragement.

Page 332: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 209

References

Alpert-Gillis, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Gender and sex role influences on children's

self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 57(1), 97-114. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

6494.1989.tb00762.x

Bachman, A. M. (1970). The relationship between a seventh-grade pupil's academic

self-concept and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in

Mathematics Education, 1(3), 173-179. doi: 10.2307/748335

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical

considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-

esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier

lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 1-44. doi:

10.1111/1529-1006.01431

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2005). Exploding the

self-esteem myth. Scientific American Mind, 16(4), 50-57. doi:

10.1038/scientificamericanmind1205-50

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection and variability in studies

of gender differences. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 11(2), 183-184. doi:

10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. doi: 10.1037/h0036215

Bem, S. L. (1981). BEM Sex Role Inventory Manual: Manual, Instrument and Scoring

Guide: Mind Garden.

Bennett, M. (2000). Self-estimates and population estimates of ability in men and

women. Australian Journal of Psychology, 52(1), 23-28. doi:

10.1080/00049530008255363

Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and

intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(3), 546-553. doi: 10.1037/0022-

3514.65.3.546

Page 333: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 210

Brinthaupt, T. M., & Erwin, L. J. (1992). Reporting about the self: Issues and

implications. In T. M. Brinthaupt & R. P. Lipka (Eds.), The Self: Definitional

and methodological issues (pp. 137-171). New York: University of New York

Press.

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities : a survey of factor-analytic studies

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cattell, R. B. (1973). Cattell Culture Fair Intelligence Test. Champaign, IL: Institute for

Personality and Ability Testing.

Cattell, R. B., Krug, S. E., & Barton, K. (1973). Technical Supplement for the Culture

Fair Intelligence Tests, Scales 2 and 3. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality

and Ability Testing.

Choi, N., Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J. L. (2007). Hierarchical confirmatory factor

analysis of the Bem Sex Role Inventory. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 67(5), 818-832. doi: 10.1177/0013164406299106

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences (2nd ed.).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

Colom, R., & Garcıa-López, O. (2002). Sex differences in fluid intelligence among high

school graduates. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 445-451. doi:

10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00040-X

Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-

esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(4), 653. doi:

10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.653

Else-Quest, N. M., Mineo, C. C., & Higgins, A. (2013). Math and science attitudes and

achievement at the intersection of gender and ethnicity. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 37(3), 293-309. doi: 10.1177/0361684313480694

Field, A. P., & Wilcox, R. R. (2017). Robust statistical methods: a primer for clinical

psychology and experimental psychopathology researchers. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 98, 19-38. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.05.013

Freund, P. A., & Kasten, N. (2012). How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis

on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychological Bulletin,

138(2), 296-321. doi: 10.1037/a0026556

Furnham, A., Clark, K., & Bailey, K. (1999). Sex differences in estimates of multiple

intelligences. European Journal of Personality, 13(4), 247-259.

Page 334: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 211

Furnham, A., & Fong, G. (2000). Self-estimated and psychometrically measured

intelligence: A cross-cultural and sex differences study of British and

Singaporean students. North American Journal of Psychology, 2(2), 191-200.

Furnham, A., Hosoe, T., & Tang, T. (2001). Male hubris and female humility? A cross-

cultural study of ratings of self, parental, and sibling multiple intelligence in

America, Britain, and Japan. Intelligence, 30(1), 101-115. doi: 10.1016/S0160-

2896(01)00080-0

Furnham, A., & Rawles, R. (1995). Sex differences in the estimation of intelligence.

Journal of Social Behaviour & Personality, 10(3), 741-748.

Furnham, A., & Rawles, R. (1999). Correlations between self-estimated and

psychometrically measured IQ. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 405-

410. doi: 10.1080/00224549909598400

Furnham, A., & Ward, C. (2001). Sex differences, test experience and the self-

estimation of multiple intelligences. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 30(2),

52-59.

Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. (1994). Narcissistic illusions in self‐

evaluations of intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62(1),

143-155. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00798.x

Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. London, UK:

Fontana Press.

Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century.

New York: Basic Books.

Gold, A. R., Brush, L. R., & Sprotzer, E. R. (1980). Developmental changes in self‐

perceptions of intelligence and self‐confidence. Psychology of Women

Quarterly, 5(2), 231-239. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1980.tb00959.x

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Hansford, B. C., & Hattie, J. A. (1982). The relationship between self and

achievement/performance measures. Review of Educational Research, 52(1),

123-142. doi: 10.3102/00346543052001123

Hirschy, A. J., & Morris, J. R. (2002). Individual differences in attributional style: the

relational influence of self-efficacy, self-esteem, and sex role identity.

Personality and Individual Differences, 32(2), 183-196. doi: 10.1016/S0191-

8869(01)00003-4

Page 335: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 212

Huberty, C. J., & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus multiple univariate

analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 105(2), 302-308. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.105.2.302

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor : the Science of Mental Ability. London: Praeger.

Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., & Anderson, D. (1983). Social interdependence and

classroom climate. Journal of Psychology, 114(1), 135-142. doi:

10.1080/00223980.1983.9915406

Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences

in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 470-500. doi:

10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.470

Kost-Smith, L. E., Pollock, S. J., Finkelstein, N. D., Cohen, G. L., Ito, T. A., Miyake,

A., . . . Singh, C. (2012). Replicating a self-affirmation intervention to address

gender differences: Successes and challenges. Paper presented at the AIP

Conference Proceedings-American Institute of Physics.

Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in

recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121-1134. doi: 10.1037//0022-

3514.77.6.1121

Laidra, K., Pullmann, H., & Allik, J. (2007). Personality and intelligence as predictors

of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary

school. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 441-451. doi:

10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.001

Lau, S. (1989). Sex role orientation and domains of self-esteem. Sex Roles, 21(5), 415-

422. doi: 10.1007/BF00289600

Major, B., Barr, L., Zubek, J., & Babey, S. H. (1999). Gender and self-esteem: A meta-

analysis. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.), Sexism and

stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence (pp.

223-253). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept:

Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3),

417-430. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.417

Martens, A., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., & Schimel, J. (2006). Combating stereotype

threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance.

Page 336: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 213

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 236-243. doi:

10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.010

Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., Bixler, E. O., & Zimmerman, D. N. (2009). IQ and

neuropsychological predictors of academic achievement. Learning and

Individual Differences, 19(2), 238-241. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.09.001

McCrae, R. R. (1990). Traits and trait names: How well is Openness represented in

natural languages? European Journal of Personality, 4(2), 119-129. doi:

10.1002/per.2410040205

Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T.

A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A

classroom study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234-1237. doi:

10.1126/science.1195996

Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math= male, me= female,

therefore math≠ me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 44-

59. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44

Paulhus, D. L., Lysy, D. C., & Yik, M. S. M. (1998). Self-report measures of

intelligence: Are they useful as proxy IQ tests? Journal of Personality, 66(4),

525-554. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00023

Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-

analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909-2013). Perspectives on Psychological

Science, 10(3), 282-306. doi: 10.1177/1745691615577701

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating

indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behaviour Research Methods,

Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 717-731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553

Rammstedt, B., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2002a). Gender differences in self estimated

intelligence and their relation to gender role orientation. European Journal of

Personality, 16(5), 369-382. doi: 10.1002/per.454

Rammstedt, B., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2002b). Self-estimated intelligence: Gender

differences, relationship to psychometric intelligence and moderating effects of

level of education. European Psychologist, 7(4), 275-284. doi: 10.1027//1016-

9040.7.4.275

Reilly, J., & Mulhern, G. (1995). Gender differences in self-estimated IQ: The need for

care in interpreting group data. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(2),

189-192. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)00148-l

Page 337: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 214

Rohde, T. E., & Thompson, L. A. (2007). Predicting academic achievement with

cognitive ability. Intelligence, 35(1), 83-92. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: the costs and benefits

of counterstereotypical impression management. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 74(3), 629-645. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.629

Sinclair, S. J., Blais, M. A., Gansler, D. A., Sandberg, E., Bistis, K., & LoCicero, A.

(2010). Psychometric properties of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: overall

and across demographic groups living within the United States. Evaluation &

the Health Professions, 33(1), 56-80. doi: 10.1177/0163278709356187

Skues, J. L., & Cunningham, E. G. (2011). A contemporary review of the definition,

prevalence, identification and support of learning disabilities in Australian

schools. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 16(2), 159-180. doi:

10.1080/19404158.2011.605154

Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the

accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

66(1), 21-36. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.21

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of

general, mathematical, spatial and verbal intelligence: Four meta analyses.

Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 493-504. doi:

10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2013). Gender and gender role differences in self-

and other-estimates of multiple intelligences. The Journal of Social Psychology,

153(4), 399-423. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2012.754397

Visser, B. A., Ashton, M. C., & Vernon, P. A. (2008). What makes you think you're so

smart? Measured abilities, personality, and sex differences in relation to self-

estimates of multiple intelligences. Journal of Individual Differences, 29(1), 35-

44. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.29.1.35

Wang, K., Lu, H., Cheung, E. F., Neumann, D. L., Shum, D. H., & Chan, R. C. (2016).

“Female Preponderance” of depression in non-clinical populations: A meta-

analytic study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01398

Page 338: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 215

Whitley, B. E. (1983). Sex role orientation and self-esteem: A critical meta-analytic

review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(4), 765-778. doi:

10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.765

Whitley, B. E., & Gridley, B. E. (1993). Sex role orientation, self-esteem, and

depression: A latent variables analysis. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 19(4), 363-369. doi: 10.1177/0146167293194001

Yuen, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Sex differences in self‐estimation of multiple

intelligences among Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. High Ability Studies,

16(2), 187-199. doi: 10.1080/13598130600618009

Page 339: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 216

Chapter 11 - Discussion

The time has come, ’ the Walrus said, To talk of many things: Of shoes — and ships — and sealing-wax — Of cabbages — and kings…. Lewis Carroll: ‘The Walrus and the Carpenter’

This chapter serves as an overview and integration of the collected studies

reported in the thesis, and how they have addressed the four research questions outlined

in Chapter 1. The overarching goal of this program of research was to make progress on

a seemingly intractable problem – why do sex differences develop for specific cognitive

abilities at the population level, and are there alternative explanations (such as

psychological traits, self-concept) for the group differences? However, there was a lack

of consensus in the literature about whether sex differences still exist, and if so to what

extent are they present in the population.

The current thesis had two major aims. The first aim was to address identified

gaps in the literature about the existence and magnitude of sex differences with

contemporary samples, and to provide a firmer evidence base by using representative

samples and cross-cultural data-sources. Furthermore, it has been argued that cross-

cultural studies in particular offer the opportunity to test various theories about origins –

if sex differences are universal and show minimal variability, then it would at least be

consistent with a biological cause (Geary, 2010; Kenrick, Trost, & Sundie, 2004). If

they were universal but showed meaningful variability, it would suggest that there are

social and cultural practices that act as moderators. And if they were quite inconsistent

(either reversing direction, or varying greatly in magnitude), this pattern would support

psychosocial models but contradict claims of innate and immutable biological

differences.

Page 340: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 217

The second aim was to address the more difficult problem of why sex differences

are found, by exploring a variety of psychosocial theories: including Nash’s sex-role

mediation theory, the effect of situational factors like the testing environment on

performance (even in the absence of high stakes testing), and the contribution of self-

estimated intelligence. Using the information gleaned from studies addressing the

second aim, a refinement of existing psychobiosocial models is proposed, situating

distal and proximal factors. Each research question is addressed in turn.

11.1 Magnitude of sex differences in cognitive abilities

Much of the research on sex differences in cognitive abilities at the time of

starting this research program was dated, and there was the very legitimate question of

whether the research findings of the past would generalise to modern samples growing

up in more egalitarian times (didn’t we solve that whole ‘gender’ thing, right?).

Feingold (1988) made the bold claim that cognitive differences were disappearing,

while Hyde (2005) advanced the ‘gender similarities hypothesis’ which holds that most

sex differences are actually small or trivial in magnitude and that future research should

be framed in terms of gender similarities rather than gender differences. Based largely

on these two lines of evidence, Caplan and Caplan (2016) questioned whether sex

differences in verbal and language abilities existed, and suggested that the motives for

conducting further research were alpha bias or ideological in nature (Caplan & Caplan,

1997).

The gender similarities hypothesis in particular has exerted a strong influence on

subsequent literature in the field, and has to some degree constrained further scientific

research in this area (Eagly, 2018; Halpern, 2014b). But Hyde and Grabe (2008) also

made a compelling argument that the technique of meta-analysis (allowing one to

investigate effects over a range of studies, sample types, and time-points) can offer

Page 341: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 218

greater clarity in understanding the extent of sex differences or their absence. As

Rosenthal (1995) noted, meta-analytic reviews have much more to offer than merely

attesting to the size and robustness of an effect: they also afford the ability to examine

potential moderators such as developmental effects, and historical changes over time.

This is especially important in the field of sex difference research, as there is often

sampling bias due to non-representative convenience samples (Becker & Hedges, 1988)

which can distort the conclusions drawn (Wilkinson, 1999).

Take, for example, the issue of mathematical and scientific abilities (collectively

referred to as quantitative reasoning). In a high profile study published in Science,

Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, and Williams (2008) analysed state performance data

collected in the United States for the NAEP. They reported that the weighted mean sex

difference across ages was d = .0065 and essentially trivial in every grade. A limitation

of their methodology was that it was a convenience sample limited to only ten states,

and was a snapshot in time across a single year (unstated). Furthermore, it only

examined mean sex differences in mathematics rather than sex ratios at the tail of the

distribution, as reported by Hedges and Nowell (1995). Curiously, though, the paper

claimed gender similarities in mathematics and science without reporting analysis of

any science achievement (but is arguably germane to the issue of STEM). There are

various reasons why Hyde et al.’s data might not be representative of the nation as a

whole (state assessment data often draws only from public schools not private, and there

can be disparities in the educational standards and curricula not only at the local county

level in the United States, but also across states). However, public availability of

national testing data from NAEP assessments offered the opportunity to hold such a

claim to greater scrutiny (see Chapter 4).

Page 342: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 219

Chapter 4, reported as Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2015) revealed a more

complex and more nuanced picture than the conclusion of gender similarities found by

Hyde et al. (2008). Rather than an absolute magnitude of d = .00 reported by Hyde et

al., a small but stable sex difference in mathematics was observed with a developmental

trend towards a peak after adolescence in Grade 12 of around d = +.10. It pointed to

neither a substantial gap nor a trivial amount, especially when investigating sex ratios of

high achieving students attaining the ‘Advanced’ proficiency standard in maths. By the

end of compulsory schooling, the sex ratio was over twice as many males as females

(2.13) for mathematics – certainly a cause for concern and target for further study.

Importantly, there was no evidence for a decline in either the effect size or the sex ratio

over time for the period analysed (1990 - 2011) as had been claimed by Feingold (1988)

and Caplan and Caplan (2005).

Furthermore, Reilly et al. (2015) conducted the first analysis of science

achievement data from the NAEP since Hedges and Nowell’s (1995) pioneering review

over thirty years ago. Sex differences in science achievement were also relatively

modest, d = +.11, but showed a similar developmental trend towards larger differences

in older students. This effect varied by science discipline, with somewhat larger effects

found in earth and space sciences (d = +.21 by Grade 12) and physical sciences (d =

+.18 by Grade 12), and importantly no significant difference for biology and life

sciences - even given the appreciable sample size. These findings point not to any

inherent lack of ability (not that this was ever predicted!), but rather potential

differences in interest level and relevance to the type of careers. A comprehensive meta-

analysis by Su, Rounds and Armstrong (2009) of over half a million respondents

showed that men have greater interests in things, and women show greater interest in

people, with a large sex difference on the Things-People dimension (d = 0.93).

Page 343: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 220

Interestingly medicine and biological sciences are the only STEM fields where equal or

greater representation of women is reached (National Science Foundation, 2017).

Furthermore, by Grade 12 there were over twice as many males than females achieving

the ‘Advanced’ proficiency standard set by the NAEP (2.28), which has bearing on the

relative numbers of men and women seeking to pursue STEM careers. Voracek, Mohr,

and Hagmann (2013) have argued the importance of considering these tail ratios,

especially in the context of sex difference research but notes that they are scarcely

investigated.

My purpose in restating these findings is to observe that it revealed a very

different picture than had been previously offered by Hyde and colleagues (which

essentially amounted to endorsement of the null hypothesis). By asking the unasked

research questions (are there sex differences in high-achieving students, are there

developmental effects) new information was revealed. We also extended research to

consider the question of sex differences in science achievement, and important

moderators (developmental, and by scientific field) that had been overlooked in prior

research.

There is an expression in the legal profession termed the ‘chilling effect’ that

restrictions on freedom of speech or the threat of a lawsuit can have on subsequent

public discourse. Meta-analysis is imbued with a special power and respect in

psychology and the social sciences (when in doubt, consult a meta-analysis!) and so

carries greater evidentiary weight. But as Rosenthal and DiMatteo (2001) observed,

their power to shed light on research questions are limited by the quality of the data

used and the research questions asked. I would argue that a similarly ‘chilling effect’

can be present for subsequent scientific research when a highly visible, compellingly

written meta-analytic review concludes support for the null hypothesis and that the

Page 344: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 221

debate is now closed. The pattern of Hyde’s analyses were also inconsistent with

international studies of mathematics (Guiso, Monte, Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008;

Chapter 6), and so seemed ‘anomalous’. Becker and Hedges (1988) long ago argued the

importance of considering the effects of selection bias in recruitment of samples when

testing for sex differences, due to a variety of factors including interactions with

demographic variables such as socioeconomic status (Hanscombe et al., 2012;

Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D'Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003), rural versus regional

localities, and ethnicity (Else-Quest, Mineo, & Higgins, 2013).

Another domain of cognitive abilities where fairly firm support for the existence

of sex differences had been found by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) was that of verbal

and language abilities. Indeed, the existence of sex differences in verbal abilities had

‘been one of the tried and true “facts” of psychology for decades’ (Hyde & Linn, 1988,

p. 53). Yet in the meta-analysis conducted by Hyde and Linn, they had concluded that

year of publication was an important and previously overlooked moderator, such that

sex differences in verbal abilities were decreasing overtime. In studies published in

1973 or earlier, the effect size was d = -.23 – but in studies published after 1973 the

effect size was considerably smaller d = -.10. On this basis, Hyde and Linn concluded

that “the difference is so small that we argue that gender differences in verbal ability no

longer exist”, (p. 53). I have addressed the shortcomings of the Hyde and Linn meta-

analysis in Chapter 2 (succinctly a cherry-picking of literature which Stumpf (1995)

reviews) and how their conclusions differ starkly from other sex difference researchers

(Halpern, 2000, 2011; Kimura, 2000). Yet it is not uncommon to still find it cited as

conclusive evidence that sex differences in verbal abilities do not exist, and it formed

the basis for Caplan and Caplan’s (1997) assertion that sex differences in verbal and

language abilities had been eliminiated.

Page 345: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 222

This position was refuted by later reviews (such as Hedges & Nowell, 1995), but

it did ask an intriguing question – if not absent, were they at least diminishing in

response to changes in societal values or educational practices? Though many waves of

NAEP assessment data had been collected in the decades since Hedge and Nowell’s

review, it came as genuine surprise to me that there were no published analyses on the

dataset examining the extent of sex differences in reading and writing. As before with

the meta-analysis of mathematics and science achievement, it struck me as an unasked

research question (or if it had been asked and analysed, presumably gone unpublished6).

These claims though by two prominent sets of researchers now meant that there was a

lack of consensus in the literature (verbal differences were either “well established” or

entirely spurious). To their credit, Caplan and Caplan (1997) did raise a genuine

concern about methodological issues with studies, such as employing non-

representative convenience samples, and the vagaries of operational definitions of

verbal ability. Reading and writing proficiency were at least clearly well defined, and

the NAEP provided a vigorous assessment framework that remained stable over time.

Chapter 5, and subsequently published as Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2018)

put this research question to the test. Our study found compelling evidence of mean sex

differences in reading and writing, as well as in the sex-ratios of students attaining the

lowest and highest proficiency standards for both outcomes. There were also

developmental effects towards larger differences as students progress through

schooling, and contrary to Feingold’s claim there was no decline in magnitude over the

timespan investigated (27 years). I would argue that the difference from Hyde and Linn

6 During extensive rounds of peer review, a concern echoed by several reviewers was that there were danger in reporting sex differences, for fear that it might be misused by lay advocates of single-sex schooling or to support claims of biological determinism. The corollary to omitting them from the publication record is that it stifles research into their aetiology and educational interventions to reduce the size of the gender gap

Page 346: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 223

(1988) was selective inclusion/exclusion of certain types of verbal ability that showed

larger differences (e.g., verbal fluency, writing, spelling, grammar and language usage),

and the non-representative nature of their samples. Our findings concur with another

study published in the same month by Petersen (2018) on state-based NAEP

assessments, which found that sex differences in verbal ability are robust and generalise

to tasks other than just reading.

As mentioned above, cross-cultural research offers a stronger evidence base than

that drawn from a single country. So in Chapter 6, I examined cross-cultural patterns of

sex differences in reading, mathematics and science achievement. For reading, all

countries investigated showed significantly higher female performance (consistent with

a biological contribution), but interestingly there was also substantial variability in the

magnitude across countries, an effect first observed by Guiso et al. (2008). Countries

with greater gender equality showed larger sex differences in reading achievement, and

this observation has been replicated in all existing waves of PISA assessment (Reilly,

2015). Global sex differences were also found for mathematics, though the effect was

stronger in OECD nations and correlated with national levels of gender equality and a

country’s tolerance for wealth inequality. A somewhat different pattern of sex

differences was found for science achievement, with some countries showing

substantial sex differences favouring males and others favouring females. These were

correlated with gender- and wealth- inequality as well, and the reversal of direction for

sex differences primarily reflected cultural factors. In Western nations with more

egalitarian conditions, there was less pressure to pursue a STEM-based career for girls.

But in countries with relatively low gender equality, pursuing a STEM-career means

economic independence for women, and thus there is increased societal pressure to

excel in such fields. Although a replication with a subsequent PISA wave failed to

Page 347: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 224

support such conclusions and claimed that Reilly (2012) might represent a Type I error

(Stoet & Geary, 2015), a subsequent study Stoet, Bailey, Moore and Geary (2016) did

observe that countries with higher levels of gender equality showed larger sex

differences in mathematics – an apparent paradox that did not cite prior research finding

this effect (Guiso et al., 2008; Reilly, 2012). Nontheless it is taken as a replication, even

if not explicitly acknowledged.

Helgeson (2017) had argued that with the further passage of time since Hyde and

Linn’s meta-analysis on verbal ability, there was a need for a stronger evidence base for

testing such hypotheses. Helgeson reviewed the cross-cultural evidence presented in

Chapter 6 and published as Reilly (2012), concluding that it showed sex differences

remain robust for reading ability. Indeed, when reviewing this study Hyde (2014)

acknowledged that the universal pattern of higher female performance in reading in all

countries was “difficult to reconcile” (p. 382) with her earlier conclusions. That

concession made me question whether sex differences might be present with other areas

of verbal and language ability (leading to the analysis presented in Chapter 5). Miller

and Halpern (2013b) also reviewed the cross-cultural analysis of PISA data presented in

Chapter 6, devoting extended coverage of it and related studies, whereby they refined

their psychobiosocial model of sex differences to include the contribution of macro-

level cultural factors.

A recurring theme throughout this body of research is the words of sex

difference researcher Professor Diane F. Halpern, who has remarked that in the field of

sex differences “what you find depends on where you look” (Halpern, 1989, 2014a).

Any investigator brings to bear their own ideological biases (alpha bias maximises, beta

bias minimises), but Halpern has argued that ignoring the data, or neglecting to fully

investigate it, does not advance the field or help reduce actual sex differences in

Page 348: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 225

educational outcomes. Too often the focus of researchers has been in areas of male

advantage (such as visual-spatial ability and quantitative reasoning), so it would have

been remiss not to also investigate areas of potential female strengths such as reading

and writing. The debate over the nature of sex differences will continue, but it is

satisfying to have contributed to developing a stronger evidence base for evaluating

such claims.

11.2 Contribution of sex-role identification to cognitive performance

The existence of sex differences in specific cognitive abilities has been

examined, researched, and debated since the beginnings of psychometrics and

measurement of human intelligence (Eagly, 1995; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Yet

despite over a century of psychological research, the question of their origins seemed an

intractable enigma that has defied our best efforts to solve. The field had progressed

from purely biological explanations (sexual dimorphism in brain structures,

genetic/evolutionary contributions, and then endogenous sex hormones) to largely

psychosocial explanations (sex differences in early socialisation experiences,

differential treatment by parents and teachers, gender stereotypes). Archer (1996) has

called these “origin theories”, a term that has since been adopted by other authors (e.g.

Eagly & Wood, 1999). While these in isolation did not provide a satisfying explanation

(or even explain a large portion of variance in the gender gap), pioneers like Halpern

and Eagly brought the field towards embracing a biopsychosocial model of sex

differences. But quite rightly, critics such as Hyde argue that the overlap between males

and females is substantial, and rife with exceptions – males who perform poorly on

visual-spatial/quantitative tasks, females who perform poorly on verbal and language

tasks. How ought we explain these common exceptions to general rules about sex

differences?

Page 349: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 226

The sex-mediation hypothesis proposed by Nash (1979) had generated an initial

burst of research interest, with enough studies conducted that Signorella and Jamison

(1986) performed a meta-analysis to test the robustness of the effect for visual-spatial

reasoning. This effect was replicated a decade later by Hamilton (1995) for other

aspects of visual-spatial ability, including spatial visualization (GEFT). However few

studies tested the second tranche of Nash’s theory, which was that femininity was

associated with the cultivation of language and verbal abilities. Those studies that did

(e.g. Ritter, 2004) were often hampered by serious methodological limitations. These

included insufficient samples sizes for statistical power, employing only a single type of

verbal measure, and a failure to calculate effect sizes of the comparison between those

high/low in femininity in line with Nash’s hypothesis. Fortunately, such calculations

can easily be performed from descriptive statistics. Subsequent calculation of effect

sizes from Ritter’s reported descriptive statistics found that in the female sample,

feminine (d = -.77) and androgynous (d = -.33) participants scored significantly higher

than masculine ones. The effect size in the study reported by Ritter was compelling

enough that I believed it merited providing a comprehensive test of Nash’ sex-role

mediation hypothesis with verbal ability in a modern sample.

Before embarking on such an endeavour though, I wanted confidence (if only for

peace of mind) that at least the visual-spatial aspect of the hypothesis would hold up to

the passage of time. Chapter 7 and published as Reilly and Neumann (2013) offered

‘proof of concept’ that the effect was robust with the passage of time by conducting a

new meta-analysis for mental rotation tasks. Surprisingly, quite a number of studies had

measures of sex-role identification and mental rotation performance as part of a battery

of neuropsychological tasks, but never examined the correlation as they were unaware

of Nash’s hypothesis. A number of authors kindly reanalysed their data to provide these

Page 350: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 227

correlations, leading to a much broader pool of studies than those explicitly testing the

sex-role mediation hypothesis. With increased data, the effect seemed robust in both

males and females. The review went further as well, by outlining the link between

visual-spatial development and quantitative reasoning that more recent research had

identified. It also offered a stronger rationale for further research beyond just an

intellectual interest. Given the underrepresentation of women in STEM-fields other than

medicine and psychology (National Science Foundation, 2017), and the prominence that

encouraging visual-spatial reasoning was receiving in addressing this issue (Uttal,

Miller, & Newcombe, 2013), further understanding of factors contributing to visual-

spatial development has important practical implications.

Chapter 8, and subsequently published as Reilly, Neumann and Andrews (2016)

aimed to provide a “thorough” test of the sex-role mediation hypothesis by having

adequate statistical power to detect an effect. The experiment was designed to offer an

array of visual-spatial and verbal language measures (in part, because a single measure

could be easily dismissed as Type I error and chance, but also to examine the robustness

of the effect and if it would generalise across types of tasks). As other researchers have

noted, visual-spatial and verbal abilities are not unitary constructs (Halpern, 2011), and

generalisability across tasks is vital if we are trying to infer an association with latent

ability. Regardless of the experimental outcome, it was important that the second

tranche of Nash’s hypothesis would had been adequately tested with a modern sample –

if only to settle the question conclusively. For the domain of visual-spatial reasoning,

the previously reported association between masculine sex-roles and mental-rotation

was replicated (Reilly & Neumann, 2013). This also generalised to other types of

visual-spatial tasks: there was a significant association for spatial visualization (GEFT)

and spatial perception (Piaget WLT) tasks in both male and female participants,

Page 351: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 228

consistent with the earlier meta-analysis by Signorella and Jamison (1986). Further, an

association was also found between feminine sex-role identification and all three verbal

and language tasks (verbal fluency, synonym generation, and DAT language usage). In

reviewing the study, Petersen (2018) argued that a sex-role mediation effect may help

explain the apparent sex difference in reading and writing outcomes through restricting

or promoting activities that provide additional training opportunities for reading and

writing skills, as well as cultivating verbal fluency.

Having demonstrated support for a sex-role mediation model for both verbal and

language ability, and for visual-spatial ability, it remains the task of future research to

elucidate the underlying mechanisms by which the sex-role mediation effect is realised.

One such mechanism for which there exists a robust body of research is that sex

differences in visual-spatial development are the result of differential levels of practice

and training between males and females, arising from the sex-typing of many leisure

activities and interests (e.g., model-making, sports, and computer games like Tetris,

Minecraft, etc.). As reviewed in Chapter 3, additional opportunities for spatial

development and enrichment lead to a substantial increase in visual-spatial abilities for

both males and females. While there is evidence from retrospective recollections in

adults of an association between visual-spatial ability and childhood spatial experiences

(Signorella, Jamison, & Krupa, 1989), it is likely that there are multiple mechanisms

involved such as situational factors of the testing environment and sex-role conformity

pressures (see Section 11.3). One would hypothesise that a similar effect of an

association between feminine sex-role identification and verbal and literary facilitating

experiences might also be found, but only a limited number of studies have tested this

mechanism directly. It is however consistent with McGeown et al., (2011) who found

that in children, a feminine sex-role identity was a better predictor of reading motivation

Page 352: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 229

and reading ability than biological sex. More research is needed to see if this extends to

other verbal and language tasks in children, including writing and vocabulary.

11.3 Contribution of situational factors to cognitive sex differences

While additional training opportunities may be one mechanism underlying the

sex-role mediation effect, another equally plausible possibility is that the effects

observed in Reilly et al. (2016) were not solely the result of differences in latent ability

but also the product of situational factors of the testing situation itself. There were

additional mechanisms outlined in Nash’s original theory concerning the perceived sex-

typing of cognitive tasks (as either a masculine or a feminine one and hence requiring

those traits stereotypically associated with either gender), and the knowledge of gender

stereotypes about intellectual abilities. Might these situational factors better explain

these observed results rather than actual ability?

This is, of course, not a new question – it is one that has been in the minds of sex

difference researchers in other domains for some time, given the test/grades discrepancy

for quantitative reasoning (see Section 2.2.3). Else-Quest et al. (2013) have argued that

psychological traits (such as attitude, self-efficacy beliefs, self-concept) might also

contribute to cognitive performance on standardised tests, as well as the decision to

pursue, (or to not) STEM careers. Classical test theory (CTT) holds that performance on

a test (raw score) is a function of true ability and measurement error. We often consider

the fluctuations in psychological states during a testing session to be a source of

transient error (Chmielewski & Watson, 2009; Schmidt, 2003; R. L. Thorndike, 1951);

all things being equal systematic bias from these fluctuations will cancel out, and that

the only challenge it poses for the purposes of research is increased “noise” and a

challenge to statistical power. Measurement error does become problematic though at

the individual level when high-stakes testing is used such as standardised tests of

Page 353: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 230

educational achievement (e.g., entry tests of placement and admission to university or

college). A common example of this is test-anxiety, whereby internal psychological

states and excess physiological arousal have a deleterious effect on cognitive

performance. However, there is a more insidious type of state effect that can impact

measurement precision. This is when mood and state become a source of systematic

error, in that they affect one demographic group disproportionately through mechanisms

such as stereotype threat (Pennington, Heim, Levy, & Larkin, 2016; Spencer, Steele, &

Quinn, 1999; Steele, 1997). So beyond testing whether a sex-role mediation effect was

still present after controlling for state effects, an additional goal of Chapter 9 was to test

whether situational factors (such as the perceived sex-typing of a cognitive task) could

exert an influence on performance - even in a low-stakes testing environment where the

participant was reassured that there was no reward or penalty for performance and data

collection was anonymous.

The results of the study reported in Chapter 9 replicated earlier findings by

showing that the way test content is perceived can substantially increase or decrease

performance. When test content was portrayed as being a measure of empathy and

perspective taking, women scored substantially higher (d = .91) on the spatial

visualization task than when it was portrayed as being visual-spatial in nature – despite

sitting the same test content, under the same timing conditions, in the same room. This

replicates the original study by Brosnan (1998) and a more recent study by Massa,

Mayer and Bohon (2005) in a college-aged sample of women. Support for the sex-role

mediation hypothesis was also found, with masculine and androgynous (high

masculinity) women scoring significantly higher than feminine and undifferentiated

(low masculinity) women. Concurrent support suggests that while situational

perceptions of task content do exert an effect, there is still a meaningful contribution of

Page 354: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 231

sex-role identification that reflects latent ability. Thus, sex-role identification can help

to explain within-sex variability as an individual differences factor, as well as being a

driving force behind between-sex variability.

While previous studies (Brosnan, 1998) had found an effect of task-labelling on

female subjects performance on the GEFT spatial visualization task, there was the

possibility that the effect would not replicate with a contemporary sample given

changes in sex-roles and gender stereotypes. Further the Brosnan study had recruited

primary school students, and the task-labelling of the GEFT as a measure of empathy

and perspective taking may have been implausible with an adult sample. This is why the

study included a second component on explicit priming of knowledge of gender

stereotypes in the event that task labelling was too subtle. Contrary to expectations, the

effect size difference between gender priming and control groups was somewhat lower

for the mental rotation task (d = .54) than for spatial visualization, but the tasks are not

directly comparable because mental rotation reflects different aspects of spatial ability

and is also a more complex task. Again there was also a replication of the sex-role

mediation effect for mental rotation observed in Reilly et al., (2016) in the sample of

women, mounting stronger evidence for actual differences in ability.

Additionally, the study provides a cross-replication of the sex-role mediation

effect for verbal and language abilities, at least in a sample of women. For the verbal

fluency task, the high femininity groups generated significantly more words than the

low femininity groups (d = .47). However, we did not see the predicted stereotype lift

on the verbal fluency task – it may be easier to diminish performance through negative

stereotypes than it is to raise performance through positive stereotypes (Walton &

Cohen, 2003). It is a hypothesis seldom explored (e.g., Keller, 2007), and a direction

future research might pursue is whether a similar stereotype threat exists for adolescent

Page 355: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 232

and adult males who are completing verbal and language tasks. Two studies have found

small stereotype threat manipulation effects for reading, but only in extremely young

children (Hartley & Sutton, 2013; Pansu et al., 2016). The stereotype that men are less

proficient in verbal and language tasks is widely held in adult samples (Halpern,

Straight, & Stephenson, 2011; Swim, 1994), but to my knowledge no study has yet

convincingly demonstrated stereotype threat for adult males on language tasks. It is an

untested question that, had time permitted, I would have liked to address in a further

study.

Another contribution of this study is to show that performance on cognitive tasks

(even in low-stakes testing conditions) can be easily manipulated by situational factors

such as the way the test is perceived or explicit priming of gender stereotypes. In that

regard it has broader relevance to the question of how much of the gender gap in

experimental studies and standardised tests can be explained by situational factors of the

testing environment that affect women disproportionately to men, especially with high-

stakes tests such as tertiary entry exams (Leiner, Scherndl, & Ortner, in press). It also

helped strengthen my conviction that we need to move beyond simply studying sex

differences on actual cognitive abilities to also consider what psychological trait and

state variables explain individual differences in performance.

11.4 Contribution of sex role identification to self-estimated intelligence

Chapter 10 set out to explore a fundamentally different research question to that

examined in the thesis so far, asking not about actual differences in intellectual ability

but rather the way an individual perceives themselves to be different, especially relative

to their peers. The power of expectations was illustrated in Rosenthal and Jacobson’s

(1968) classic ‘Pygmalion in the Classroom’ experiment, where experimentally

manipulated teacher expectations of the intelligence of randomly selected students

Page 356: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 233

(identified as ‘gifted’) translated into higher psychometrically-measured IQ growth for

those students over the course of several years. Most of the major reviews of sex

difference research have focused primarily on actual ability (do differences exist, if so

how large are they), but neglected to consider the way in which a person sees

themselves – either as intellectually capable and on par or somewhat above their peers

in intellectual ability, or instead as lacking the same intellectual prowess of those

around them. What might that do, over the course of a child’s intellectual and

educational development to their sense of intellectual self-competency, self-efficacy

beliefs, and their patterns of academic interests? Research into academic motivation

shows that perceptions of competence (Stipek & Gralinski, 1991, 1996), as well as

beliefs about the rigidity or malleability of academic success, have important

implications for how individuals progress in their education, especially in sex-typed

intellectual domains such as STEM (Wang, Eccles, & Kenny, 2013).

Though sex differences in self-estimated intelligence (SEI) are frequently

reported (Szymanowicz & Furnham, 2011), relatively few studies have attempted to

examine their developmental trajectory. Some studies have investigated whether there

are sex differences in the “intellectual self-concept” of children, and found either that

very young girls and boys share similar self-concepts of their intellectual ability, or that

girls start with slightly higher self-concepts (Aaron et al., 2005; Eccles, Wigfield,

Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993). Yet as early as fifth grade we can see demonstrable sex

differences in intellectual self-concept, with boys seeing themselves as brighter (Gold,

Brush, & Sprotzer, 1980; Marsh, 1989). By the time these children reach high school

we see the robust sex difference in self-estimated intelligence (Steinmayr & Spinath,

2009), growing still larger in adulthood with college-aged samples. Intellectual self-

concept and self-estimates of ability in academic domains plays an important role in

Page 357: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 234

shaping achievement-related decision making (Ackerman & Wolman, 2007; Eccles et

al., 1993; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002), so understanding of the

antecedents of sex differences in self-estimated intelligence is critical for developing

educational and motivational interventions.

However, the bulk of literature attests to the robustness of the effect (across

types of samples, ages, socioeconomic groups, and cultures), but not its developmental

antecedents. While a full investigation into this phenomenon would be best served by a

longitudinal study (before sex differences in intellectual self-concept emerge) some

information can still be gleaned by looking at the developmental end-point (young

adults) to explore two hypotheses – firstly that the self-estimated intelligence might be

explained by low self-esteem (which past research suggests is more prevalent in

females), and secondly that it might be explained by patterns of sex-role identification.

Following the established protocol used by Furnham and Rawles (1995),

participants were asked to provide an estimate of their intelligence (expressed as an IQ

score), and then estimate their ability for specific cognitive domains as per Gardener’s

multiple intelligences. A point of difference in this study is that participants also

completed the Cattell’s Culture Fair Intelligence Test (CCFIT) as a measurement of

psychometric intelligence, followed by measures of self-esteem and sex-role identity

(BSRI).

The inclusion of an objective measurement of intelligence allowed us to rule out

two possible explanations for what has been termed by Furnham et al., (2001) as the

male hubris/female humility (MHFH) effect. Firstly, we could rule out the possibility of

actual sex differences in intelligence due to convenience sampling from a student

subject pool as one possible explanation, which had been a limitation of most previous

studies. Secondly, by examining the correlation between objective and self-estimated

Page 358: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 235

intelligence we were able to rule out a gross discrepancy in the accuracy rate of self-

assessments: if, for example, males held utterly unrealistic evaluations of their intellect

(no association) but females presented more realistic impressions. The results showed

that the correlation strength was comparable across men and women, but what differed

was the direction of reporting. More women underestimated their intelligence whereas

more males overestimated. Interestingly this effect was not universal in that some males

also underestimated their intellectual capabilities. Thus, the phraseology chosen by

Furnham et al. as male hubris may be inapt, and the picture is more nuanced than first

assumed.

Significant sex-role differences were also observed for self-estimated

intelligence, with high masculinity participants reporting significantly higher IQ

estimates than low masculinity groups, regardless of biological sex. Consistent with past

literature (Whitley, 1984; Whitley & Gridley, 1993), there were also sex and sex-role

differences in self-esteem for our sample. I had hypothesised that this might be one

mechanism resulting in lower self-estimations of intelligence in women. Regression

analysis showed that both masculine sex-role identification and self-esteem contributed

to self-estimated intelligence even after controlling for psychometric intelligence. Of

these, masculine sex-role identification exerted a stronger force. Mediation analysis

confirmed that masculine sex-role identification acted as a mediator between sex and

self-estimated intelligence scores, but there was still a significant direct effect of sex as

well (partially mediated).

Sex differences were also found for many, but not all, estimations of the

different components of Gardener’s multiple intelligences. The strongest effects were

found for logical-mathematical and spatial intelligences, followed by bodily-

kinaesthetic, naturalistic and existential intelligence. Significant differences were not

Page 359: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 236

found for verbal intelligence (traditionally regarded as feminine), musical intelligence,

and the two emotional intelligences (again, traditionally regarded as feminine). Thus the

pattern of results seemed to be driven more by commonly regarded gender stereotypes

(Swim, 1994), or what are lay intuitions about the direction of empirically observed

cognitive sex differences (Halpern, Straight, et al., 2011). There was stronger evidence

though for sex-role differences in specific multiple intelligences, with larger effect sizes

and in more domains.

One question that remains somewhat unresolved in my mind is the extent to

which sex differences in self-estimated intelligence are genuinely held, or whether they

are the product of a gender-specific social desirability bias to either boast (in the case of

males) or downplay (in the case of females) intelligence. The moderately-sized bivariate

correlation between self-estimated intelligence, general and academic self-esteem offers

some evidence that these are deeply held though. However, a follow-up study might

consider including some measurement of what has been termed in the sex-role literature

as felt pressure to conform to sex-role expectations (and in this case either enhance or to

downplay personal intelligence). Some studies of mate preference have found that

across cultures women hold a preference for partners that are smarter (Buss & Barnes,

1986; Shackelford, Schmitt, & Buss, 2005), but that men on average do not (Park,

Young, Eastwick, Troisi, & Streamer, 2016). At a cultural level, a sex-specific pattern

of continual self-enhancement/self-deprecation of intellectual ability may later transfer

to genuinely held beliefs.

Finally, the study has identified sex/sex-role differences in self-esteem, and in

particular academic self-esteem, as important targets for further study in understanding

the factors contributing to sex differences in self-estimated intelligence. Given the

abundant evidence that there is no support for actual sex differences in intelligence, and

Page 360: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 237

that a cluster of males in our study also under-estimated their intelligence, educational

interventions aimed at raising intellectual self-concept might be necessary for raising

the educational aspirations of both genders.

11.5 Collective findings and implications for theory building

Although each of the individual studies comprising this thesis makes a relatively

specific contribution, when viewed collectively they have broader implications for

theory building and extending existing psychobiosocial models of sex differences in

cognitive abilities. One of the most widely accepted models was that offered by Halpern

(2004, 2011) as a biopsychosocial model emphasising the reciprocal relation between

nature and nurture in the development of sex differences. This model was later endorsed

in a consensus statement by many of the prominent sex/gender researchers in the field

(Halpern et al., 2007). Though the model has its advantages (chiefly that it does not

attribute weighting to any particular element, and thus has been better embraced by

researchers who openly disagree about the relative emphasis of biological and social

processes), it is light on details about the precise mechanisms by which sex differences

develop. It also fails to explain why some types of cognitive tasks show higher

performance in females but others show the reverse pattern of higher performance in

males. These are themes that Halpern would return to in later works (Halpern et al.,

2007; Halpern, Beninger, & Straight, 2011). Additionally, at the time it did not

explicitly acknowledge the contributions of cultural factors (see Chapter 6), as an

additional factor over and above one’s direct social environment. Subsequently, the

model was refined by Miller and Halpern (2013b) to acknowledge culture as a pathway,

which included extended coverage of this issue on pages 40 and 41 of their review

including my own study published as Reilly (2012).

Page 361: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 238

Another prominent model, and one that I have drawn from, is the biosocial

model of sex differences proposed by Wood and Eagly (2012), which is based on

Eagly’s (1987) social role theory. It highlights the social construction of gender through

sex-roles, which serve to restrict and regulate thought and behaviour. At the heart of this

model is an emphasis on the way that individuals self-regulate their behaviour to

conform to sex-role stereotypes (which in turn are socially regulated through conformity

pressures, rewards and sanctions). As such it is highly compatible with a sex-role

mediation explanation for sex differences. Where my proposed model differs is that it

omits the activational effect of sex hormones as a regulatory process, due to the

relatively weak effect sizes and inconsistencies observed in the literature. While some

researchers contend sex steroids such as testosterone and estrogen exert an activational

effect on cognitive processes (e.g., Hampson, 2018), studies are frequently inconsistent

and to some degree context-dependent to certain environmental cues (Halari et al.,

2005; Hampson, 2018; Hausmann, Schoofs, Rosenthal, & Jordan, 2009; Puts et al.,

2010), so this pathway has been intentionally omitted from the model at this time.

Collectively, the studies in this thesis can shed light on some of those

mechanisms and can contribute to a more detailed psychobiosocial theory. One of the

most powerful criticisms levelled at theories of sex differences (and their practical

impact) comes from Hyde (2005, 2014) who has opined that within-sex variability is

larger than between-sex variability, and that theorists would be better served explaining

those individual differences rather than focusing on biological-based sex factors.

Though forcefully conveyed in her work, it actually stems from an argument first made

by Thorndike’s (1914) work into sex group differences and the role of individual

differences factors in educational psychology. Any theory attempting to explain why

sex differences emerge at the population level ought also to be able to explain sex-

Page 362: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 239

related contributions to individual performance and within-sex variability. Thorndike

was also the first to suggest that many of the causes of group differences were not due

to innate biological differences, but rather to differential levels of training and practice

arising from sex differences in activities and interests.

Nash (1979) had proposed a sex-role mediation theory of sex differences, but it

was somewhat dated and focused primarily on social mechanisms. In proposing a

revision of this theory, I have taken into consideration the two biopsychosocial models

of sex differences described above, as well as more recent work on social and cultural

factors.

Figure 11.1 presents a proposed biosociocultural model outlining such

mechanisms. It emphasises how, initially very small, biological contributions as

identified in the literature make a contribution to early brain development as relatively

distal factors. The biological contributions include evolutionary pressures (see Section

2.3.1.3) manifesting as genetic predispositions differentiating the sexes, as well as the

contribution of prenatal hormonal exposure. Collectively they exert a weak and indirect

force on brain development as well as contributing to later psychosocial behaviour.

More proximal is the contribution of early socialisation experiences which typically

differ between boys and girls, but which are also subject to wide individual differences.

Contributing to the acquisition of sex-role identification also are sociocultural factors,

such as gender segregation in the division of occupational and family roles in society,

the propagation of gender stereotypes (explicit and implicit), and the level of gender

inequality in the society in which a child is raised. The relative contribution of

biological, social and cultural factors is idiosyncratic to the individual resulting in

individual differences in acquisition of masculine and feminine sex-role identification,

but with enough commonalities differentiating the sexes that there are observable group

Page 363: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 240

Figure 11.1 Biosociocultural model of sex-role identification acquisition, recognising the contribution of biological factors, early socialisation experiences arising from differential treatment of boys and girls, as well as cultural factors such as stratification in the roles of men and women in society, propagation of gender stereotypes and sex-typing of activities/intellectual interests

Distal Contributions

Cultural factors

Brain development

Prenatal hormonal exposure

Genetic predispositions

Evolutionary pressures

Sex-role identification Early socialization

experiences

Page 364: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 241

differences between males and females as well in society. Thus the effect of sex (both as

a biological factor, and a social category) and culture are mediated through sex-role

identification. At earlier stages of development, environmental factors (such as

availability of sex-typed toys, the type and quality of amount of communication and

parental facilitation of verbal skills) may hold greater influence as parents, caregivers

and teachers exert tight control over the types of experiences available to a child. But as

children grow in autonomy, they start to gain greater control over their environment and

begin to self-select activities and interests based on their personality and interests

(niche-picking). Upon reaching adolescence - when conformity pressures increase –

their own sex-role identification and personality traits can manifests as either a broad or

narrow sex-typed repertoire of academic interests and leisure pursuits. Some elements

are under the control of the child, and some elements (such as interactions with parents

and teachers) reflect cultural values and gender-norms.

Figure 11.2 illustrates a sex-role mediation theory for the sex-typing and

acquisition of cognitive skills. Like its predecessor, the model highlights the association

between masculinity and development of visual-spatial ability and outlines a causal

mechanism through differential exposure to spatial experiences and training, as well as

sex-role conformity pressures. Visual spatial development is important not just as a skill

in itself, but also because it lays down a foundation for the development of quantitative

reasoning skills in mathematics and science. At the beginning of this course of research,

the association between visual-spatial ability and quantitative reasoning skills was

largely correlational in nature (e.g. Wai, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009). However, recently

published studies of educational interventions providing spatial training have found it

delivers increased mathematics and science self-efficacy and more importantly, transfer

Page 365: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 242 Figure 11.2 – Sex-role mediation theory of cognitive development.

Sex-role identification

Masculine sex-role (Instrumental/Agentic traits)

Visual-Spatial Ability

Differential exposure to spatial experiences

Verbal Ability

Sex-typed regulation of affect, behaviour and cognition

Enhanced self-appraisal of intelligence

Feminine sex-role (Expressive/Communal traits)

Perceived sex-typing of spatial activities as masculine

Differential exposure to reading and language experiences

Perceived sex-typing of verbal tasks as feminine

Higher self-esteem, and self-concept as intellectual

Quantitative reasoning

Page 366: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 243

effects in the form of improved grades in college and university students (Miller &

Halpern, 2013a; Sorby, Veurink, & Streiner, 2018). One study has also demonstrated

similar outcomes as early as elementary school for mathematics (Cheng & Mix, 2014).

As evidence for the pathway between development of visual-spatial ability and

quantitative reasoning now stands on surer footing, and for this reason has been

incorporated into the model.

Chapter 8 demonstrated support for a sex-role mediation effect with visual-

spatial ability, with the evidence from Chapter 9 supporting both a difference in latent

ability (arising from differential training from exposure to spatial experiences) as well

as an effect of perceived sex-typing of spatial tasks. Additionally, the revised model in

Figure 11.2 also identifies a new pathway that was not present in Nash’s original theory,

which is the association between masculinity and self-estimated intelligence. This

aspect of the model is based on the findings outlined in Chapter 10. This outcome is

important, because of the role that perceptions of intellectuality play in voluntary course

selection of more challenging academic content such as STEM subjects, as well as

buffering against negative cultural gender stereotypes (outlined in Eccles’s (2007, 2013)

expectancy-value model of academic achievement related choices).

Also presented in Figure 11.2 is the association between femininity and the

cultivation of verbal and language abilities. It was originally postulated by Nash, but

only a handful of studies had investigated this tranche of the theory. Chapters 8 and 9

investigated sex-role differences in several types of verbal fluency tasks, as well as

grammar and language usage, finding support for the model. However verbal abilities

are not a unitary construct, and incorporate a diverse range of tasks (see Section 2.2.1),

and observance of a sex-role mediation effect with other tasks including reading and

writing would further support the model.

Page 367: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 244

To illustrate the utility of this revised model for explaining both between- and

within-sex differences, I will give two hypothetical cases (one male, one female) to

show how it might work in practice. Consider the case of James, a young boy of above

average intellect who is born into a family with traditional beliefs about sex-roles and

endorsement of gender stereotypes. In early childhood, James is provided with a variety

of traditionally masculine toys and games – from cars and trucks that illustrate force and

motion, to construction blocks that cultivate visual-spatial development. James’s mother

talks with him often, but not to the same extent as his sisters about the same topics :

there is more talk about practical things and activities and less about emotional feelings

that support the scaffolding of social development and verbal aptitude (which given his

moderately high IQ, he has the potential to excel in). Upon entering school, James finds

he is called upon less to answer questions in language arts classes but more often in

mathematics and science classes (his teachers hold high expectations for him, as he

demonstrated an initial aptitude…. but there may be other similarly talented girls who

have been overlooked because of gender stereotypes). Like many of his male peers he

struggles with reading – it doesn’t come naturally to him, and his friends see it as a

‘girlie task’.

Already cultural and environmental factors are exerting an effect on his

intellectual development. His father takes him to museums though and encourages him

to learn coding, and slowly James finds himself acquiring an interest in STEM. In fact,

it is something that he enjoys, and begins to self-select activities that suit his interests.

Being strongly masculine sex-typed now though, James holds very little interest in

literature; reading is viewed as necessary for school but not a source of intellectual

stimulation and pleasure. Consequentially, James reads less often than does the typical

child his age. As sex-role conformity pressures increase as he enters high school, he

Page 368: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 245

holds expectations that he will probably pursue a career that draws on his strengths in

science. He also has great confidence in his intellectual ability – his parents think James

smart and tell him so all the time, and James feels he is much brighter than the typical

student. He does well in standardized tests, as he goes into them with confidence.

The totality of his experiences up to that point have shaped both his personality

and interests, magnifying his potential in some areas such as mathematics and science

but blunting it in others such as verbal and language abilities. There were opportunities

for his trajectory to diverge though: for example, if he’d been provided with toys and

games that encouraged language development, if his parents had given him comic books

to scaffold Jame’s early literary development followed by sci-fi or adventure novels,

which might buffer against the gender-conformity pressures he would later experience

in adolescence – well, James might equally have become a great writer and perhaps

combined his love of STEM into a career of science journalism. Ultimately his strong

masculine sex-typing might have limited his choices, but a greater variety of

experiences might have tempered this.

Consider another hypothetical example, a girl named Sarah born to a different

family but of equal innate intellectual potential as James. Sarah’s parents are both

middle-class workers who realise the value in attaining an education, regardless of a

child’s gender. Both her mother and father encourage her verbal communication skills

which manifest slightly earlier than Jame’s in line with developmental effects. This

gives Sarah a headstart with vocabulary development and Sarah’s parents both read to

her nightly, scaffolding her emerging literacy skills. Together Sarah’s parents make a

conscious effort to provide her with a range of educational toys and games, even ones

that are traditionally masculine. Even though the toyshop they buy from is highly

gender-segregated into separate boys and girls sections, Sarah’s father will often take

Page 369: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 246

her shopping for Lego, especially the new ‘Friends’ line which holds a broader appeal.

Tentatively, he also introduces her to age-appropriate transforming robots, and then

scales them up in difficulty level as she grows. These encourage her visual-spatial

development as well as cultivating a love for robotics and technology (she acquires the

attitude that robots are ‘cool’). Although often seen as a masculine field, Sarah’s father

encourages in her a love of science just as Jame’s did. She’s also exposed to a broader

repertoire of experiences than other girls her age, encouraging a less rigid mindset about

stereotypically masculine/feminine fields. She reads avidly, and like many girls her age

enjoys writing – her teachers actively encourage this. She struggles with mathematics

though, and feels that ‘it just comes naturally to boys’.

Sarah’s mother buys her a book on mathematics – not on how to do mathematics

but on the important contributions made by women to the field throughout history. She

falls in love with the tale of Countess Ada Lovelace, an exceptional mathematician who

was the world’s first computer programmer. While mathematics in primary school is

still a challenge, Sarah tells herself “if Ada could do maths, then I can too”. In high

school her advanced visual-spatial skills relative to her female peers allow her to really

come into her own (within-sex variability), facilitating the more advanced mathematics

topics like geometry and trigonometry that make it possible to branch into studying

physics and later chemistry. Compared to other girls her age, Sarah holds a greater

interest in science, and is more open to the possibility of exploring further studies in a

STEM field.

Sarah could become many things – a writer, an artist, a scientist or a doctor. She

has attained a healthy blend of masculine and feminine personality traits (androgyny),

and with that the consequence of behavioural flexibility in interests and talents. Her

innate intellectual potential in STEM was not curtailed like so many of her female

Page 370: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 247

peers, but she also has strong verbal and language talents that will compliment whatever

career choice she chooses. She also has greater confidence - though still holds doubts -

in her intellectual potential than many women her age (with a higher self-estimated

intelligence in line with her IQ). Again, her trajectory could have been very different

growing up in a more restrictive social environment, or if her own sex-typing had led

her down different paths.

11.6 Directions for future research and limitations

While the literature and experimental studies presented in this thesis support the

proposed sex-role mediation model of cognitive abilities, a chief limitation is that the

experimental studies examined cognitive abilities at the developmental end-point

(young adulthood). Definitive evidence of a mediation effect would require either cross-

sectional or longitudinal studies to establish the effect of sex-role identity in younger

students. Given that the meta-analyses presented in Chapters 4, 5 and 6 show that sex

differences in educational outcomes have not yet been eliminated in modern samples,

further research into their antecedents is sorely needed, and the optimal developmental

stage would be before the gender gap in educational outcomes widens after puberty.

However, in the case of self-estimated intelligence and what developmental

psychologists term intellectual self-concept, this begins even earlier with differentiation

between males and females appearing as early as fifth grade (Gold et al., 1980; Marsh,

1989). The psychological mechanisms behind this particular timing are not yet clear,

and investigating whether nascent sex-role identities and endorsement of gender

stereotypes (explicit and implicit) are responsible with younger children would be a

useful research goal, as well as any intermediary constructs/processes.

Another important research goal is to further test the underlying mechanisms

behind sex differences in verbal and language abilities. The proposed sex-role mediation

Page 371: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 248

model outlines two primary processes that are responsible: namely differential levels of

exposure to verbal and language tasks, as well as sex-typing. Meta-analyses of parent-

child communication studies have shown that mothers engage in more talk with

daughters than sons (Leaper, Anderson, & Sanders, 1998), girls are slightly more

talkative than boys (Leaper & Smith, 2004), and that girls show much higher reading

motivation in primary school (Marinak & Gambrell, 2010) and greater time spent

reading for leisure than boys in high school which is positively correlated with reading

achievement (Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006). Thus there is evidence for differential

levels of practice and training between the sexes as one mechanism underlying sex

differences, but further evidence is needed to document a sex-role mediation effect in

children and adolescents. At present, few studies have investigated associations between

sex-role identification, perceived sex-typing of language tasks, and performance on

reading and writing tasks with younger age-groups. McGeown, Goodwin, Henderson

and Wright (2011) found that feminine sex-role identification was a better predictor of

reading motivation than biological sex, while Pajares and Valiante (2001) found the

same pattern in a sample of primary school students for writing ability, motivation and

self-efficacy. Importantly none of the studies found a negative association with

masculinity. But replication of this effect with other samples would be desirable, as well

as for other types of verbal and language abilities. This highlights sex-role identification

as a useful construct in understanding individual differences in performance.

Another as yet unfinished task is to further investigate cross-cultural

contributions to sex differences in educational outcomes. Chapter 6 reported as Reilly

(2012) examined the impact of national levels of gender inequality on reading,

mathematics and science, and a subsequent conference paper by Reilly (2015) replicated

those findings for reading with subsequent PISA waves. However, further analysis

Page 372: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 249

needs to be conducted to test whether the association with gender inequality replicates

for mathematics and science achievement. There may be other, as yet unexamined

cultural factors that explain the high degree of heterogeneity in effect sizes globally for

mathematics and science, and why some nations show higher male performance but

others show a reversal with higher female performance. There is tentative evidence that

stark inequality leads to greater pressure on females to seek STEM careers, especially in

non-Western nations. As PISA and TIMSS continue to integrate addition partner

nations from the developing world, it will provide a good testing ground for these

research questions.

11.7 Practical Implications for Childhood Education

The research contained herein raises some important practical implications for

educational practice and the development of potential interventions. Firstly, it shows

that substantial sex differences remain in verbal and language abilities, and that

educators and researchers may have underestimated just how large a gap exists for

writing tasks. This has important implications for boys’ preparedness to pursue tertiary

education, and highlights the need for greater concentration on writing practice in the

curriculum. Just as a greater focus on science and mathematics is important for

encouraging girls in these domains, so too is a focus for literacy, the mechanics of

grammar, and practice on writing tasks important for equality of outcomes with boys.

Secondly, this body of research demonstrates that males and females are not

homogenous groups, and that sex-role identification (and the behavioural consequences

thereof, in terms of differential training and conformity pressures) may explain a

meaningful portion of the within-sex variability noted by earlier researchers such as

Thorndike (1914) and Hyde (2005). For children who are highly sex-typed and have a

narrowly constrained range of experiences and interests, there may be some merit in

Page 373: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 250

gently attempting to broaden their experiences. Examples of which would include

introducing stereotypically masculine spatial promoting toys and games, encouraging

more verbal communication and social play, and scaffolding early literacy skills.

Parents and caregivers are the initial gateway through which such experiences are

provided, but as children grow in autonomy they begin to self-select; if they have

previously been exposed to only a narrow range then their outlook may be similarly

restricted and fall along stereotypically masculine/feminine lines.

Differential levels of practice through play and leisure activities is only one

aspect of the sex-role mediation theory, however. Another important element is sex-role

conformity pressures (which intensify in adolescence). Children acquire gender-

stereotyped beliefs early, and they take cues from many sources – including parents,

teachers, media and peers. Parental attitudes to reading and writing (or mathematics and

science) convey important messages about the sex-typing of these pursuits, as does the

way they are taught in school and which students are called on in class. Parental and

teacher attitudes may be equally appropriate targets for interventions, as well as those of

children. If children see either language or STEM as equally appropriate (indeed,

expected) for both boys and girls, then this might influence the attitudes they bring to

bear in adolescence and entering high school. While educational interventions often

focus on increasing ability, equally important are student attitudes to the sex-typing of

fields and showing them that they may be relevant to their future career interests.

Thirdly, the issue of sex differences in intellectual self-concept (as measured by

self-estimated intelligence, and self-efficacy beliefs) really has not received enough

research attention. Many – but not all – males significantly overestimate their

intellectual abilities, while a substantial portion of females underestimate it. The reasons

are not yet clear (e.g. differential levels of social desirability of intelligence between the

Page 374: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 251

sexes, effects of child-gender on parental estimates of intelligence, cultural stereotypes),

but our research suggests that sex differences in general self-esteem may play a

substantial role. While there were group differences between males and females in our

study, sex-role identification mediated this effect, with a masculine sex-role

identification buffering against low self-esteem and leading to higher self-estimates of

intelligence. This effect was present in both men and women. Males with low masculine

identification are at particular risk, as are non-androgynous females. A holistic approach

to reducing sex differences in educational outcomes should also target intellectual self-

concept, as research has shown this strongly influences course selection in high school

and career aspirations.

One issue that I feel important to address as well is the question of whether sex

differences are an inevitable outcome of society, and whether attempts to influence

childrens’ play experiences and sex-role beliefs constitute social engineering. Tackling

the first question, cross-cultural research demonstrates that there is substantial

variability in mathematics and science outcomes. In some nations, there are strong

patterns of higher male performance, in others there are no significant sex differences,

and in others substantially higher female performance is evident. The latter are often

countries where gender and income inequality are substantial, and STEM represents a

pathway to economic independence. None of these findings suggest sex differences in

quantitative reasoning is inevitable. The picture is less optimistic for reading

achievement. Substantial sex differences are present in all participating nations, but

these is also substantial heterogeneity in their magnitude. We simply do not have

sufficient data to make conclusions about cross-cultural patterns of writing, but it is

likely to be the same. It may always be the case that some sex differences in verbal and

language abilities will exist, but as a matter of equity, I believe that parents, educators

Page 375: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 252

and policy-makers should make concerted efforts to mitigate against them. Not every

child will become a wordsmith or avid reader, but we may be able to raise the literary

skills of the typical boy with educational interventions that have broader targets

(spelling, grammar, verbal fluency, and writing) than just reading alone. And it may

better prepare them for an uncertain future in an age of automation, where reskilling and

further education will be expected of them.

The second issue may be seen as more problematic, as it has at its core elements

of public policy and ideology that are contentious. Does society have a right – or even,

an obligation – to attempt to engineer a child’s masculine and feminine identification

with the aim of minimising sex differences in educational outcomes? Society is best

served by diversity, and a plurality of different perspectives, with no sex-role category

being more worthy or legitimate than another. And given that sex-role identification is

determined, to a large degree, by biological forces and personality, will attempts at

‘modification’ be futile anyway? Even in studies with non-human primates, toy

selection preferences are often innately driven and devoid of cultural influence. But

certain combinations of sex-role identification have behavioural consequences, such as

restricted or broadened interests, leisure pursuits, and academic attitudes. Here, parents

and educators can make a contribution to the reduction of gender stereotypes and sex-

typing of intellectual pursuits, without seeking to change a child’s temperament. Just as

sports are now recognised as being equally important for girls as for boys, encouraging

verbal and language proficiency in boys and STEM skills in girls may be important

mechanisms for mitigating sex differences in educational outcomes. And building self-

confidence and encouraging realistic intellectual self-concepts will be beneficial for any

child: masculine, feminine or androgynous.

Page 376: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 253

11.8 Final Conclusions

Sex differences in specific cognitive abilities is an actively researched but

contentious topic, which has important social, occupational and public policy

implications for the ways in which children are educated and supported (both formally

in schools and informally in the home). The meta-analyses presented in this thesis have

addressed a long-standing research question about the magnitude of sex differences,

extending those of earlier researchers. Legitimate concerns had been raised that sex

differences may have been declining or even eliminated in modern samples, but to

paraphrase Mark Twain rumours of their death have been greatly exaggerated.

The notion that there might be inequality of educational outcomes for males and

females rankles at our sense of basic fairness. Yet designing educational interventions

or changes to teaching pedagogy requires a clearer understanding of why sex differences

develop (origin theories). This has been a seemingly intractable research question asked

since the beginning of psychometrics and intelligence testing. The sex-role mediation

explanation examined herein identifies and validates an individual differences factor for

explaining cognitive performance – both group differences between males and females,

as well as within-sex variability argued by Thorndike (1914) and Hyde (2005). Rather

than biological sex exerting direct influences, it is an individual’s combination of

masculinity and femininity that shapes the development of cognitive abilities through

differential exposure to stereotypically masculine or feminine past-times and intellectual

interests. It also highlights how other (non-intellectual) factors such as self-estimated

intelligence may contribute to disparities in educational outcomes, and how these are

also associated with sex-role identification. The information gleaned in the present

research may prove useful for designing educational interventions (such as targeting

perceptions of sex-typing, or addressing differential levels of training). This thesis may

Page 377: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 254

be a modest small step forward in the context of the enormity of the sex difference

debate, but the road to eliminating sex differences in educational outcomes will be long

and winding. In this respect, this thesis may allow researchers to travel around the

corner of mere debate about the presence of sex differences to head down a straight to

better understand how sex differences appear.

Page 378: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 255

References

Aaron, M. W., Courtney, L. K., Julie, D. F., Lori, A. K., Jamie, R. M., Kunal, S., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2005). College students lack knowledge of standard drink volumes: Implications for definitions of risky drinking based on survey data. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(4), 631-638. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000158836.77407.E6

Ackerman, P. L., & Wolman, S. D. (2007). Determinants and validity of self-estimates of abilities and self-concept measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13(2), 57-78. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.13.2.57

Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary explanations compatible? American Psychologist, 51(9), 909-917. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.51.9.909

Bachman, A. M. (1970). The relationship between a seventh-grade pupil's academic self-concept and achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 1(3), 173-179. doi:10.2307/748335

Becker, B. J., & Hedges, L. V. (1988). The effects of selection and variability in studies of gender differences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(2), 183-184. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00049256

Brosnan, M. J. (1998). The implications for academic attainment of perceived gender-appropriateness upon spatial task performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68(2), 203-215. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1998.tb01284.x

Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 559-570. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.50.3.559

Caplan, J. B., & Caplan, P. J. (2005). The perseverative search for sex differences in mathematics ability. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 25-47). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Caplan, J. B., & Caplan, P. J. (2016). Thinking critically about research on sex and gender (3rd ed.). New York: Routledge.

Caplan, P. J., & Caplan, J. B. (1997). Do sex-related cognitive differences exist, and why do people seek them out? In P. J. Caplan, M. Crawford, J. S. Hyde, & J. T. E. Richardson (Eds.), Gender differences in human cognition (pp. 52-80). New York: Oxford University Press.

Cheng, Y.-L., & Mix, K. S. (2014). Spatial training improves children's mathematics ability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 2-11. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.725186

Chmielewski, M., & Watson, D. (2009). What is being assessed and why it matters: The impact of transient error on trait research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 186-202. doi:10.1037/a0015618

Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 382-393. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.382

Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50(3), 145-158. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.50.3.145

Page 379: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 256

Eagly, A. H. (2018). The shaping of science by ideology: How feminism inspired, led, and constrained scientific understanding of Sex and Gender. Journal of Social Issues. doi:10.1111/josi.12291

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54(6), 408-423. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408

Eccles, J. S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in physical science and engineering. In S. J. Ceci (Ed.), Why aren't more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 199-210). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Eccles, J. S. (2013). Gender and achievement choices. In E. T. Gershoff, R. S. Mistry, & D. Crosby (Eds.), Societal Contexts of Child Development: Pathways of Influence and Implications for Practice and Policy (pp. 19-34). New York: Oxford University Press.

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differences in children's self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64(3), 830-847. doi:10.2307/1131221

Else-Quest, N. M., Mineo, C. C., & Higgins, A. (2013). Math and science attitudes and achievement at the intersection of gender and ethnicity. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(3), 293-309. doi:10.1177/0361684313480694

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43(2), 95-103. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95

Furnham, A., Hosoe, T., & Tang, T. (2001). Male hubris and female humility? A cross-cultural study of ratings of self, parental, and sibling multiple intelligence in America, Britain, and Japan. Intelligence, 30(1), 101-115. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00080-0

Furnham, A., & Rawles, R. (1995). Sex differences in the estimation of intelligence. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 10(3), 741-748.

Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female : the evolution of human sex differences (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gold, A. R., Brush, L. R., & Sprotzer, E. R. (1980). Developmental changes in self‐perceptions of intelligence and self‐confidence. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5(2), 231-239. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1980.tb00959.x

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320(5880), 1164-1165. doi:10.1126/science.1154094

Halari, R., Hines, M., Kumari, V., Mehrotra, R., Wheeler, M., Ng, V., & Sharma, T. (2005). Sex differences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 104-117. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.104

Halpern, D. F. (1989). The disappearance of cognitive gender differences: What you see depends on where you look. American Psychologist, 44(8), 1156-1158. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.44.8.1156

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F. (2004). A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex differences in cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(4), 135-139. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00292.x

Halpern, D. F. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 380: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 257

Halpern, D. F. (2014a). Gender Differences in Cognition and Their Implications for Undergraduate and Graduate STEM Education [Lecture]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=3dB2PWjps3U

Halpern, D. F. (2014b). It gets crowded with an elephant and an ape in the room: Teaching about female and male cognitive differences and similarities. Teaching of Psychology, 41(1), 88-93. doi:10.1177/0098628313514187

Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8(1), 1-51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x

Halpern, D. F., Beninger, A. S., & Straight, C. A. (2011). Sex differences in intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 253-272). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Straight, C. A., & Stephenson, C. L. (2011). Beliefs about cognitive gender differences: Accurate for direction, underestimated for size. Sex Roles, 64(5-6), 336-347. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9891-2

Hamilton, C. J. (1995). Beyond sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: the impact of gender trait possession. British Journal of Psychology, 86(1), 1-20. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02542.x

Hampson, E. (2018). Regulation of cognitive function by androgens and estrogens. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 23, 49-57. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.03.002

Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e30320. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030320

Hartley, B. L., & Sutton, R. M. (2013). A stereotype threat account of boys' academic underachievement. Child Development, 84(5), 1716-1733. doi:10.1111/cdev.12079

Hausmann, M., Schoofs, D., Rosenthal, H. E. S., & Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences - A psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(3), 389-401. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.019

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269(5220), 41-45. doi:10.1126/science.7604277

Helgeson, V. S. (2017). Psychology of Gender (5th edition). New York: Routledge. Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6),

581-592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581 Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology,

65(1), 373-398. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115057 Hyde, J. S., & Grabe, S. (2008). Meta-analysis in the psychology of women. In F.

Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: a handbook of issues and theories (pp. 142-173). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science, 321(5888), 494-495. doi:10.1126/science.1160364

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 53-69. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.104.1.53

Page 381: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 258

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73(2), 509-527. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00421

Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., & Anderson, D. (1983). SOCIAL INTERDEPENDENCE AND CLASSROOM CLIMATE. Journal of Psychology, 114(1), 135.

Keller, J. (2007). When negative stereotypic expectancies turn into challenge or threat: The moderating role of regulatory focus. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 66(3), 163-168. doi:10.1024/1421-0185.66.3.163

Kenrick, D. T., Trost, M. R., & Sundie, J. M. (2004). Sex-roles as adaptions: An evolutionary perspective on gender differences and similarities. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Psychology of Gender. New York: Guildford Press.

Kimura, D. (2000). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on

parents' talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3-27. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3

Leaper, C., & Smith, T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children's language use: talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 993-1027. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.993

Leiner, J. E., Scherndl, T., & Ortner, T. M. (in press). How do men and women perceive a high-stakes test situation. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02216

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Marinak, B. A., & Gambrell, L. B. (2010). Reading motivation: Exploring the elementary gender gap. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49(2), 129-141. doi:10.1080/19388070902803795

Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept: Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 417-430. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.417

Massa, L. J., Mayer, R. E., & Bohon, L. M. (2005). Individual differences in gender role beliefs influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(2), 99-111. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2004.11.002

McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2011). Gender differences in reading motivation: does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 328-336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013a). Can spatial training improve long-term outcomes for gifted STEM undergraduates? Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 141-152. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.012

Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2013b). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37-45. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011

National Science Foundation. (2017). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2017. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation Retrieved from https://www.nsf.gov/statistics/2017/nsf17310/.

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motivation and achievement of middle school students: A function of gender orientation? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(3), 366-381. doi: 10.1006/ceps.2000.1069

Page 382: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 259

Pansu, P., Régner, I., Max, S., Colé, P., Nezlek, J. B., & Huguet, P. (2016). A burden for the boys: Evidence of stereotype threat in boys' reading performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 26-30. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.008

Park, L. E., Young, A. F., Eastwick, P. W., Troisi, J. D., & Streamer, L. (2016). Desirable but not smart: Preference for smarter romantic partners impairs women's STEM outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46(3), 158-179. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12354

Pennington, C. R., Heim, D., Levy, A. R., & Larkin, D. T. (2016). Twenty years of stereotype threat research: A review of psychological mediators. PLoS ONE, 11(1), e0146487. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146487

Petersen, J. (2018). Gender difference in verbal performance: a meta-analysis of United States state performance assessments. Educational Psychology Review, 30(4), 1269-1281. doi:10.1007/s10648-018-9450-x

Puts, D. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Burriss, R. P., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58(2), 282-289. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005

Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e39904. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039904

Reilly, D. (2015). Gender differences in reading from a cross-cultural perspective- the contribution of gender equality. Paper presented at the International Convention of Psychological Science, Amsterdam, Netherlands. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.1.1879.2488

Reilly, D., & Neumann, D. L. (2013). Gender-role differences in spatial ability: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 68(9), 521-535. doi:10.1007/s11199-013-0269-0

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645-662. doi:10.1037/edu0000012

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2016). Sex and sex-role differences in specific cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 54, 147-158. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.12.004

Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2018). Gender differences in reading and writing achievement: Evidence from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). American Psychologist. doi:10.1037/amp0000356

Ritter, D. (2004). Gender role orientation and performance on stereotypically feminine and masculine cognitive tasks. Sex Roles, 50(7), 583-591. doi:f10.1023/B:SERS.0000023077.91248.f7

Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118(2), 183-192. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183

Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: Recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 59-82. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59

Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Schmidt, F. L. (2003). Beyond alpha: An empirical examination of the effects of different sources of measurement error on reliability estimates for measures of individual-differences constructs. Psychological methods, 8(2), 206-224. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.2.206

Page 383: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 260

Shackelford, T. K., Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), 447-458. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.023

Signorella, M. L., & Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 207-228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.2.207

Signorella, M. L., Jamison, W., & Krupa, M. H. (1989). Predicting spatial performance from gender stereotyping in activity preferences and in self-concept. Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 89-95. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.1.89

Sorby, S., Veurink, N., & Streiner, S. (2018). Does spatial skills instruction improve STEM outcomes? The answer is ‘yes’. Learning and Individual Differences, 67, 209-222. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2018.09.001

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 4-28. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52(6), 613-629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613

Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). What Explains Boys’ Stronger Confidence in their Intelligence? Sex Roles, 61(9), 736-749.

Stipek, D. J., & Gralinski, J. H. (1991). Gender differences in children's achievement-related beliefs and emotional responses to success and failure in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 361-371. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.361

Stipek, D. J., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children's beliefs about intelligence and school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 397-407. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.3.397

Stoet, G., Bailey, D. H., Moore, A. M., & Geary, D. C. (2016). Countries with higher levels of gender equality show larger national sex differences in mathematics anxiety and relatively lower parental mathematics valuation for girls. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0153857. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153857

Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2015). Sex differences in academic achievement are not related to political, economic, or social equality. Intelligence, 48, 137-151. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.006

Stumpf, H. (1995). Gender differences in performance on tests of cognitive abilities: Experimental design issues and empirical results. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(4), 275-287. doi:10.1016/1041-6080(95)90002-0

Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: a meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 859–884. doi:10.1037/a0017364

Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 21-36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.21

Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, mathematical, spatial and verbal intelligence: Four meta analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 493-504. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001

Thorndike, E. L. (1914). Educational Psychology, Vol. 3: Mental work and fatigue and individual differences. New York: Teachers College.

Page 384: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 261

Thorndike, R. L. (1951). Reliability. In E. F. Lindquist (Ed.), Educational Measurement. Washington, DC: American Council on Education.

Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623-628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x

Uttal, D. H., Miller, D. I., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). Exploring and enhancing spatial thinking links to achievement in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 367-373. doi:10.1177/0963721413484756

Voracek, M., Mohr, E., & Hagmann, M. (2013). On the importance of tail ratios for psychological science. Psychological Reports: Measures and Statistics, 112(3), 872-886. doi:10.2466/03.PR0.112.3.872-886

Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 817-835. doi:10.1037/a0016127

Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 456-467. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00019-2

Wang, M.-T., Eccles, J. S., & Kenny, S. (2013). Not lack of ability but more choice: Individual and gender differences in choice of careers in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Psychological Science, 24(5), 770-775. doi:10.1177/0956797612458937

Whitley, B. E. (1984). Sex-role orientation and psychological well-being: Two meta-analyses. Sex Roles, 12(1), 207-225. doi:10.1007/bf00288048

Whitley, B. E., & Gridley, B. E. (1993). Sex role orientation, self-esteem, and depression: A latent variables analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(4), 363-369. doi:10.1177/0146167293194001

Wilkinson, L. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54(8), 594-604. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differences and similiarities in behavior. In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 46, pp. 55-124). New York: Academic Press.

Page 385: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX-ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 262

Appendices

Appendix A1 – SAT Mathematics Meta-Analysis

Male Female Variance Ratio

(VR)

Cohen’s

d

p-value

Year Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.)

1996 527 115 492 107 1.16 0.32 <.001 ***

1997 530 114 494 108 1.11 0.32 <.001 ***

1998 531 114 496 108 1.11 0.32 <.001 ***

1996 531 115 495 110 1.09 0.32 <.001 ***

1997 533 115 498 109 1.11 0.31 <.001 ***

1998 533 115 498 113 1.04 0.31 <.001 ***

1999 534 116 500 110 1.11 0.30 <.001 ***

2000 537 116 503 111 1.09 0.30 <.001 ***

2001 537 116 501 110 1.11 0.32 <.001 ***

2002 538 116 504 111 1.09 0.30 <.001 ***

2003 536 117 502 111 1.11 0.30 <.001 ***

2004 533 116 499 110 1.11 0.30 <.001 ***

2005 533 118 500 111 1.13 0.29 <.001 ***

2006 534 118 499 112 1.11 0.30 <.001 ***

2007 534 118 500 112 1.11 0.30 <.001 ***

2008 531 119 500 113 1.11 0.27 <.001 ***

2009 532 119 499 113 1.11 0.28 <.001 ***

2010 531 121 499 114 1.13 0.27 <.001 ***

2011 530 123 499 114 1.16 0.26 <.001 ***

2012 527 124 496 115 1.16 0.26 <.001 ***

2013 524 126 494 116 1.18 0.25 <.001 ***

2014 527 115 492 107 1.16 0.32 <.001 ***

2015 530 114 494 108 1.11 0.32 <.001 ***

2016 531 114 496 108 1.11 0.32 <.001 ***

Point estimate d = +.30 [95% CI = .29 to .30], with males scoring higher than females

Datasource: The College Board Archived SAT Reports

Page 386: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 263

Appendix A2 – SAT Verbal Meta-Analysis

Male Female Variance Ratio

(VR)

Cohen’s

d

p-value

Year Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.)

1996 507 110 503 110 n/a 0.04 <.001 ***

1997 507 111 503 111 n/a 0.04 <.001 ***

1998 509 111 502 111 n/a 0.06 <.001 ***

1996 509 111 502 111 n/a 0.06 <.001 ***

1997 507 111 504 111 n/a 0.03 <.001 ***

1998 509 111 502 111 n/a 0.06 <.001 ***

1999 507 111 502 111 n/a 0.05 <.001 ***

2000 512 111 503 111 n/a 0.08 <.001 ***

2001 512 112 504 112 n/a 0.07 <.001 ***

2002 513 112 505 112 n/a 0.07 <.001 ***

2003 505 114 502 111 n/a 0.03 <.001 ***

2004 503 114 500 111 1.05 0.03 <.001 ***

2005 502 114 499 110 1.05 0.03 <.001 ***

2006 502 114 497 110 1.07 0.04 <.001 ***

2007 504 114 498 111 1.07 0.05 <.001 ***

2008 500 116 495 112 1.05 0.04 <.001 ***

2009 498 116 493 112 1.07 0.04 <.001 ***

2010 499 117 494 112 1.07 0.04 <.001 ***

2011 499 118 495 113 1.09 0.03 <.001 ***

2012 497 119 493 113 1.11 0.03 <.001 ***

2013 495 120 493 114 1.11 0.02 <.001 ***

2014 507 110 503 110 1.11 0.04 <.001 ***

2015 507 111 503 111 1.05 0.04 <.001 ***

2016 509 111 502 111 1.05 0.06 <.001 ***

n/a indicates male/female standard deviation data not available. Effect size calculated from pooled SD reported in

College Board Archived SAT reports

Point estimate d = +.045 [95% CI = .038 to .053], with males scoring slightly higher than

females. Note that this direction is contrary to the generally reported sex differences in

verbal and language abilities (see Chapter 2, Section 2.2.1 and Chapters 5 and 6).

Page 387: sex and sex-role differences in cognitive abilities

SEX AND SEX ROLE DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES 264

Appendix A3 – Academic Self-Esteem Measures

The following items were used as a measure of academic self-esteem, in Chapter

10. They were adapted from items in Johnson et al.’s (1983) Academic Self-Esteem

subscale, and Bachman’s (1970) Self-Concept of Ability Scale (SCAS), with wording

changed from high school to university. Several items were reverse-coded (indicated by

an asterisk) to minimise acquiescence bias.

Academic Self-Esteem Strongly

Agree Agree Disagree Strongly

Disagree 1. I feel confident in my academic

abilities SA A D SD

2. I am not doing as well at university as I would like to*

SA A D SD

3. Coursework is fairly easy for me SA A D SD 4. I sometimes feel lost in lectures

and reading textbooks* SA A D SD

4. Whenever I take a test I am afraid I will fail or do badly*

SA A D SD

5. I feel confident in my ability to complete university

SA A D SD

For comparability, the same response format as the Rosenberg Self-Esteem

Scale that participants completed on the previous page of their booklet was employed.

Additionally, we administered the single-item Rosenberg Academic Self-Esteem Scale

which asks respondents to compare themselves to other students enrolled in their

degree.

1. How do you rate yourself in academic ability compared with those studying your degree? (CIRCLE ONE)

Far below average

Slightly below

About average

Slightly better

Far above average