Top Banner
Accepted Article This article has been accepted for publication and undergone full peer review but has not been through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi: 10.1111/ibi.12587 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. DR. ROMINA CLARA SCARDAMAGLIA (Orcid ID : 0000-0002-5400-6083) DR. JUAN CARLOS REBOREDA (Orcid ID : 0000-0001-5136-4574) Article type : Original Paper Editor: Sumasgutner, Petra Running-head: Roosting behaviour in brood parasitic cowbirds Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood parasitic cowbirds ROMINA C. SCARDAMAGLIA, 1 * ALEX KACELNIK 2 & JUAN C. REBOREDA 1 1 Departamento de Ecología, Genética y Evolución & IEGEBA-CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Pabellón II Ciudad Universitaria, C1428EGA Buenos Aires, Argentina 2 Department of Zoology, South Parks Road, University of Oxford, OX1 3PS, United Kingdom *Corresponding author: [email protected]. Shiny Cowbirds Molothrus bonariensis and Screaming Cowbirds M. rufoaxillaris are closely related brood parasites, but the former is socially polygynous or promiscuous and an extreme host generalist, while the latter is socially monogamous and parasitizes almost exclusively one host. Females of both species lay in relative darkness, before dawn, relying
26

Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Feb 21, 2023

Download

Documents

Khang Minh
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article has been accepted for publication and undergone full peer review but has not been through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi: 10.1111/ibi.12587 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

DR. ROMINA CLARA SCARDAMAGLIA (Orcid ID : 0000-0002-5400-6083) DR. JUAN CARLOS REBOREDA (Orcid ID : 0000-0001-5136-4574)

Article type : Original Paper

Editor: Sumasgutner, Petra

Running-head: Roosting behaviour in brood parasitic cowbirds

Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood parasitic cowbirds

ROMINA C. SCARDAMAGLIA,1* ALEX KACELNIK2 & JUAN C. REBOREDA1

1Departamento de Ecología, Genética y Evolución & IEGEBA-CONICET, Facultad de

Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Pabellón II Ciudad

Universitaria, C1428EGA Buenos Aires, Argentina

2Department of Zoology, South Parks Road, University of Oxford, OX1 3PS, United Kingdom

*Corresponding author: [email protected].

Shiny Cowbirds Molothrus bonariensis and Screaming Cowbirds M. rufoaxillaris are closely

related brood parasites, but the former is socially polygynous or promiscuous and an

extreme host generalist, while the latter is socially monogamous and parasitizes almost

exclusively one host. Females of both species lay in relative darkness, before dawn, relying

Page 2: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

for host nest location on previous days’ prospecting activity, or possibly on following better-

informed roost associates. We studied the temporal and spatial patterns of roosting

behaviour in these species to test the hypothesis that roosting behaviour of cowbirds is

related to their breeding strategy (brood parasitism) and reflects differences in strategies

between species. We recorded fidelity to a roost, location fidelity within a roost, inter-

individual spatial associations, and timing of roost departures and parasitic events, using

tagged individuals. Female Shiny Cowbirds and both sexes of Screaming Cowbirds showed

marked fidelity in roosting location, and roost departures occurred both during and after the

known time window for parasitism, with earlier departures probably corresponding to laying

days. Screaming Cowbird females and males that were trapped together and showed high

levels of association during the day also showed high levels of association in the roost. We

describe the spatial and temporal patterns of a relatively poorly known aspect of avian

ecology in general and of brood parasites’ behaviour in particular.

Keywords: bird assemblage, space use, site fidelity, radio-tracking, Molothrus.

It is common in many bird species for individuals to gather in groups ranging from a few to

many thousands to spend the overnight resting period together (Dhondt et al. 2006, Winkler

2006, Laughlin et al. 2014). Though a widespread behaviour, the functional significance of

communal roosting remains poorly understood, and may have different explanations across

taxa. Functions proposed for roosts include thermoregulation, reduction in probability of

predation, and information exchange, and some studies have provided direct or indirect

evidence for each of these factors in specific systems (see Yom-Tov 1979, du Plessis et al.

1994, Paquet et al. 2016). An influential hypothesis elaborated by Ward and Zahavi (1973)

proposed that bird assemblages such as roosts could act as information centres, where

individuals that do not have information on the location of good feeding sites can obtain such

Page 3: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

knowledge from individuals that do, but the empirical evidence is inconclusive and unlikely to

apply equally across communally roosting species (Weatherhead 1987).

Only a few studies have analysed the activity of birds in roosts, and among those

most have focused mainly on two aspects: roost site fidelity (Morrison & Caccamise 1985,

Conklin et al. 2007, Béchet et al. 2010, Laughlin et al. 2014) and roost-use in relation to

feeding sites (Morrison & Caccamise 1985, 1990, Conklin et al. 2007, Adams 2011). These

studies have shown great variability in roost site fidelity both between and within species. In

Common Starlings Sturnus vulgaris, for example, some individuals remain highly faithful to a

roost, whereas others frequently switch between different roosts in the area (Morrison &

Caccamise 1985). Tree Swallows Tachycineta bicolor return to the roost previously used

about 60% of the time, and when they do switch roosts, roost significantly closer to their

original roosting site than would be expected if they were choosing randomly among the

roosts in the area (Laughlin et al. 2014). However, Dunlin Calidris alpina use roosts that are

closer to their last foraging locations than roosts that are also known to them and in which

they have roosted before (Adams 2011). In addition, and contradicting expectations from the

‘information centre’ hypothesis for roost function (Ward & Zahavi 1973), Common Starlings

were found to be more faithful to their diurnal feeding ranges than to their communal

roosting sites, with birds commuting from up to five different communal roosts to a stable

diurnal range (Morrison & Caccamise 1985).

Brood parasites, such as cuckoos and cowbirds, exploit the parental care of other

species (hosts) by laying their eggs in their nests. The use of roosts in relation to brood

parasites’ reproductive strategy has not yet been studied, except for a few secondary or

anecdotal mentions in studies analysing home-ranges and activity patterns in the Brown-

headed Cowbird Molothrus ater (Thompson 1994, Gates & Evans 1998, Hahn et al. 1999)

and one study that addressed roost departure times in Shiny Cowbirds M. bonariensis

(Feare & Zaccagnini 1993).

Page 4: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Females of the host generalist Shiny Cowbird and the host specialist Screaming

Cowbird M. rufoaxillaris lay their eggs around dawn (Gloag et al. 2013, Scardamaglia et al.

2017) in nests that they visited on previous days (Scardamaglia et al. 2017). It is known that

cowbirds roost communally (Cruz et al. 1990, Feare & Zaccagnini 1993), and evidence

suggests that they fly directly from their roost to target nests (Scardamaglia & Reboreda

2014). In addition to differences in the degree of specialization in host use, Shiny and

Screaming Cowbirds differ in their mating systems: Shiny Cowbirds are socially polygynous

or promiscuous and females search for nests without the assistance of the male, while

Screaming Cowbirds are socially monogamous and males accompany females when they

search for nests (Mason 1987, Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014). Social behaviour varies

throughout the day: during the morning individuals are found alone or in small groups, while

in the afternoon they gather to feed in larger flocks (Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014, Kattan

et al. 2016), grouping in communal roosts towards sunset. A similar behaviour has been

observed in the Brown-headed Cowbirds Molothrus ater, a closely related species, in which

males and females spend the morning in small groups in areas rich in host nests, joining

large feeding flocks in the afternoon and roosting communally at night (Thompson 1994,

Gates & Evans 1998).

In this study we test the hypothesis that roosting behaviour in cowbirds is related to

their breeding strategy (brood parasitism) and reflects differences in strategies between

species. The differences between the mating systems of Shiny and Screaming Cowbirds led

us to expect differences in roosting behaviour between the males of the two species, but not

between the females. We studied roosting site use at two different scales: first, we studied

fidelity between roosts (i.e. whether individuals joined one or more roost communities), and

second, their location fidelity within roost (i.e. whether individuals settled randomly within

their roost each night or showed preference for particular locations). Because female Shiny

Cowbirds search for nests in daytime and must reach the same nests in the following

morning twilight, they face the navigational problem of reaching a different nest location

Page 5: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

every laying day in the dark. One might expect this to affect their choice of roosting site, but

how is not obvious. Since roosting may be constrained to dark, dense forest sites for

predation reasons, Shiny Cowbirds may benefit from roosting at consistent locations in order

to start laying expeditions in well-known areas. Host nests are typically in relatively open

sites, where twilight navigation may be easier. On the other hand, if navigation towards host

nests were not a problem, they could reduce the distance travelled for each laying trip by

changing roosting site between nights. The same reasoning can be applied to both sexes of

Screaming Cowbirds, which jointly visit host nests, but not to male Shiny Cowbirds, that do

not participate in nest scouting or dawn-laying trips. Given their spatial association during

the day and joint nest-searching, we expected Screaming Cowbird pairs to roost in close

proximity. Furthermore, because laying occurs mostly during the dawn twilight, when poor

light intensity is unsuited to searching for nests or foraging, early roost departures may only

occur on the fraction of days on which females are ready to lay.

METHODS

Study site and species

The study was conducted at the reserve ‘El Destino’ (35.141ºS, 57.393ºW), near the town of

Magdalena, in Buenos Aires Province, Argentina, during the breeding seasons (October-

February) 2010/11 to 2013/14. The area comprises ~500 ha of flooding grasslands with

interspersed woodland patches dominated by two low, spiny trees, spiny hackberry Celtis

ehrenbergiana and coronillo Scutia buxifolia. Screaming and Shiny Cowbirds are year-round

residents in the area. At this field site Shiny Cowbirds intensely parasitize the Chalk-browed

Mockingbird Mimus saturninus (frequency of parasitism 70-80%, Fiorini & Reboreda 2006;

Gloag et al. 2012) and to a lesser degree other species such as the House Wren

Troglodytes aedon (frequency of parasitism: 50%, Tuero et al. 2007). Chalk-browed

Mockingbirds breed from mid-September until mid-January and build open-cup nests in

Page 6: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

shrubs or trees with dense foliage, at a height of 1.5–2.5 m. They lay 3–5 eggs per clutch,

and if a nest fails, the same pair can attempt re-nesting several times (Fraga 1985).

Screaming Cowbirds parasitize exclusively the Greyish Baywing Agelaioides badius. The

frequency of parasitism is 80-90% and most nests are parasitized multiple times (Fraga

1998, Mason 1980, De Mársico et al. 2010). Greyish Baywings rarely build their own nest.

Instead, they breed in domed nests built by other species, secondary cavities, or nest boxes

(Friedmann 1929, Hoy & Ottow 1964, Fraga 1998, De Mársico et al. 2010). They breed from

early December to late February and are cooperative breeders. Assisted pairs generally

have one to three helpers, which are recruited after egg hatching (Fraga 1991, Ursino et al.

2011).

Radio-telemetry

We trapped 36 Shiny Cowbirds (27 females and 9 males) and 32 Screaming Cowbirds (27

females and 5 males) during the breeding seasons (October-February) 2010/11 to 2013/14

using walk-in funnel traps baited with millet, or mist-nets. The difference between the sexes

in the number of birds trapped and tagged was due to the fact that in a concurrent project

(Scardamaglia et al. 2017) we were more interested in female behaviour, resulting in a

greater number of tagged females. Each bird was marked with a unique combination of

colour plastic leg bands and a numbered aluminium band and was fitted with a radio-tag.

Two Shiny Cowbird females were tagged twice, in 2010/11 and 2013/14, and one female in

three breeding seasons, in 2010/11, 2011/12 and 2013/14. We used two different types of

radio-tags because this study was carried out simultaneously with two other projects

(Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014, Scardamaglia et al. 2017). During the breeding seasons

2010/11 and 2011/12 the animals were fitted with 1.2 g glue-on beeper-VHF radio-

transmitters (model PicoPip Ag392 from Biotrack, Wareham, UK or model A2455 from

Advanced Telemetry Systems, Isanti, Minnesota, USA). We glued the radio-transmitters to

Page 7: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

the bird’s back using cyanoacrylate adhesive and an activator (Loctite 401 and Loctite 770,

Henkel, respectively). During the 2012/13 and 2013/14 breeding seasons the animals were

fitted with 1.0 g backpack coded radio-tags (model NTQB-4-2, Lotek Wireless, Ontario,

Canada) using Teflon Ribbon (Bally Ribbon Mills, Philadelphia, USA) and the Rappole

harness technique (Rappole & Tipton 1991). The procedure of marking the cowbird and

fitting the radio-transmitter lasted less than 15 minutes. These two different techniques

yielded differences in the time the birds carried the transmitters. Birds fitted with glue-on

radio-tags carried them for 35.8 ± 21.0 days (mean ± sd, range = 6 - 68 days, n = 34, see

Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014), while those fitted with backpack tags carried them for the

whole duration of tag life (tags were designed to run for 79 days). We found that the

variables we measured (see below) did not differ significantly between tagging methods (see

Online Supporting Material for more details). The radio-tags had no obvious effects on

cowbirds’ behaviour, since females visited and parasitized nests as soon as 18 h after tag

deployment (see SM1 in Scardamaglia et al. 2017).

To determine roost locations, we tracked cowbirds on foot, using the homing

technique (White & Garrott 1990). Cowbirds were monitored around dusk using hand-held

radio-tracking receivers (beeper tags: model Sika, Biotrack, Wareham, UK; coded tags:

model SRX-400A, Lotek Wireless, Ontario, Canada). The coded tags hand-held receiver

range was limited: it required for the distance between receiver and tag to be no more than

20 m for proper code reading. Because some of the Screaming Cowbirds roosted in a deep

forest area where access was difficult in the dark and impeded reliable identification of the

birds, we present fewer data for this species. We recorded departure times from roosts using

a hand-held receiver each morning from 04:30 h to 06:30 h in 47, 34 and 29 days in the

breeding seasons 2010/11, 2011/12 and 2013/14 respectively. A departure was detected

when the tag signal was recorded moving away (and eventually lost) from the fixed location

the bird had maintained in the roost the previous night. During the day, we tracked cowbirds

Page 8: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

on foot to check that the radio-tags were still active and that the birds remained in the area

(and that any absence from the roost was not due to a faulty or lost radio-tag).

To detect parasitic events from tagged females, in all seasons we video-recorded

activity in mockingbird nests using a camera (Handykam 420 CCD colour micro-camera)

suspended above the nest and connected to a digital video recorder (Lawmate PVR1000 or

PVR500 ECO) placed at the base of the tree. We did not video record baywing nests since

they are prone to desert if disturbed during the pre-laying or laying period (M. C. De Mársico

pers. comm.). Additionally, during the seasons 2012/13 and 2013/14, we recorded tagged

Shiny and Screaming Cowbird visits to host nests using digitally encoded proximity data-

loggers (DataSika, Biotrack, Wareham, UK) that were connected to an omnidirectional

antenna (Biotrack, Wareham, UK) and to a 12 V car battery. The data-loggers continually

'listened' for nearby tags and recorded the tag identity code, date and time whenever a

tagged cowbird came within detection range, ~30 m from the antenna (see Scardamaglia et

al. 2017 for more details on recording of visits of tagged birds to host nests). We placed the

data loggers on the ground directly below 29 mockingbird and 16 baywing nests (Fig. 1) that

were in the construction or pre-laying stages (defined as the time lapsed since nest lining is

completed and the host lays its first egg). We monitored nest activity continuously, from the

moment that the data logger was deployed until 3–4 days after the onset of incubation or

nest failure (i.e. abandonment or depredation), whichever occurred first. Overall, we

monitored 156 nest-days of mockingbirds and 150 nest-days of baywings.

Data analysis

We defined a roost operationally as an area where a group of birds gather to spend the

night. Because there were clear breaks in the distribution of birds (see Results), we used

these breaks to delimit discrete roosts. We used home-range data previously published for

the cowbirds tracked during the first two breeding seasons (Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014)

Page 9: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

to determine the distance between the individuals’ resting locations and their diurnal

locations (i.e. where they search for and locate host nests). To do this, we used the

gCentroid function in rgeos package (Bivand & Rundel 2016) in R 3.3.1 (R Core Team 2017)

to calculate both the diurnal ranges centroids and roost centroids, and estimated the

distance between these points for each individual.

Site fidelity within a roost

We defined fidelity to a roost as the percentage of nights on which a bird was searched for at

a given roost that it was detected. For this estimate we used data from birds that were

monitored on at least four nights (maximum available 23 nights).

Fidelity within a roost

To determine whether birds were faithful to the locations they used within a discrete roost,

we compared the observed mean distance between night locations of each individual to the

mean distance between locations of a simulated distribution of birds that settled within the

roost at random each night. The simulation consisted of a Monte Carlo randomization

procedure (n = 1000 runs) where bird locations along a linear roost could take integer values

ranging from 0 m (the bird located in the same place in two different nights) to a maximum

determined by the distance between opposite ends of the roost, with a 1 m resolution (see

Online Supporting Material for more details of this simulation). We considered the observed

distance to be significantly shorter than expected by chance if it fell below the 95th percentile

of the distribution generated by the simulation.

Page 10: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Association between Screaming Cowbird pairs

To investigate whether pairs of Screaming Cowbirds maintain their diurnal association

during the night, we used data from three females and three males that had been captured

in pairs (i.e. individuals caught together in the same trap and showing high levels of

association during the day, Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014). As a control, we recorded the

frequency of association in the roost of two males and two females that were caught

separately (i.e. in the same trap but at different times) and that did not show association

during the day.

Timing of roost departures and parasitism events

We analysed the timing of both departure of females from the roost and parasitism events by

generating a frequency distribution of the timing of both sets of events at five-minute

intervals, and compared these between the two species using Mann-Whitney U-tests. The

times of departures and parasitic events were standardized relative to sunrise on the day of

recording for proper comparison, to account for variation in sunrise times through the season

(http://www.usno.navy.mil/). Because it was not logistically possible to record departure

times for all tagged females each morning, and record activity at all potential target nests,

the data presented here are not direct observations of females departing from the roost and

arriving at a host nest to parasitize on the same day.

Page 11: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

RESULTS

Characteristics of roosting locations

We recorded a total of 240 locations corresponding to 25 tagged Shiny Cowbird females (the

other two tagged females were not detected in the area after tagging), 10 locations

corresponding to five tagged Shiny Cowbird males (the other four tagged males were

detected in the area during the morning but never around dusk, so they must have roosted

outside our study area), 97 locations corresponding to 18 tagged Screaming Cowbird

females (we did not record any roosting location for the other nine tagged females due to

difficulty in accessing deep-forest locations), and 33 locations corresponding to the five

tagged Screaming Cowbird males.

Radio-tracked Shiny Cowbird females and both sexes of Screaming Cowbird mainly

used one roost, which was maintained throughout the breeding season (see below). Tagged

Shiny Cowbird males were rarely found in roosts in the study area. We detected a ‘main

roost’ for Shiny Cowbirds, where a few hundred birds roosted each night and where 93% of

locations of tagged birds were recorded. The distance between the opposite ends of this

roost was 755 m and it was located along a band of trees next to a low-use road, adjacent to

the diurnal home-ranges of the tagged birds (Fig. 1a). Other smaller roosts (of only a few

individuals) were recorded up to 4029 m apart. Screaming Cowbird roost locations were

distributed in a band of forest (Fig. 1b). The distance between opposite ends of this roost

was 1311 m.

The median distance between the roost centroid and the diurnal range centroid was

494.9 m (range = 219-889 m, n = 13) for Shiny Cowbird females, 966.7 m (range = 628-

1646 m, n = 8) for Screaming Cowbird females and 974.5 m (range = 554-1710 m, n = 5) for

Screaming Cowbird males.

Page 12: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Fidelity to a roost

Female Shiny Cowbirds were faithful to one roost throughout the breeding season. On

average, females returned to the same roost 82 % ± 25 % of the nights (mean ± sd, range =

17 – 100%, n = 22 females). The two females tracked during the seasons 2010/11 and

2013/14 used the same roost across years (Female 1: 100% fidelity in both seasons; female

2: 100% fidelity in season 1 and 89% fidelity in season 2), while there was variability in the

use of the roost for the female tracked across three breeding seasons (29% fidelity in

season 1; 80% fidelity in season 2; 84% fidelity in season 3).

For Screaming Cowbirds, precise data were available only for the birds tagged with

beeper radio-tags in seasons 2010/11 and 2011/12. Both males and females showed 100%

fidelity to their roosting site (n = 5 females, n = 3 males).

Fidelity within a roost

Shiny Cowbird females moved on average 62.3 ± 28.8 m (mean ± sd, range = 20-102 m, n =

15) between nights within a roost, Screaming Cowbird females moved 57.8 ± 35.5 m (range

= 16-122 m, n = 11) and Screaming Cowbird males moved 44.6 ± 18.9 m (range = 23-58 m,

n = 3) (see Supporting Online Material Fig. S1). The distance between opposite ends in the

distribution of birds, which defined the maximum values in our simulations, were 755 m and

1311 m for Shiny and Screaming Cowbirds respectively. Monte Carlo simulations showed

that movements between nights were significantly shorter than expected by chance (i.e. if

the birds located at random in the roost each night) for both Shiny (Fig. 2) and Screaming

Cowbirds.

Page 13: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Association between Screaming Cowbird pairs

Screaming Cowbird males and females that were trapped together and showed high levels

of association during the day showed higher levels of association in the roost than control

‘pairs’. The three pairs tracked were recorded associated in the roost in 12/12, 11/12 and 3/4

of the records respectively, while the two control ‘pairs’ did not show any association in any

of the records (0/4 and 0/1 respectively).

Timing of roost departures and parasitism events

Screaming Cowbird females left the roost earlier than Shiny Cowbird females (Mann-

Whitney U-test, U = 41.5, P < 0.001, Shiny Cowbirds n = 19, Screaming Cowbirds n = 11,

Online Supporting Material Table S1). Parasitic events occurred in a short time window in

both species, earlier in Screaming than Shiny Cowbirds (Mann-Whitney U-test, z = 4.6, P <

0.001, Shiny Cowbirds n = 21, Screaming Cowbirds n = 11, Online Supporting Material

Table S1).

In Shiny Cowbirds, 86% of parasitic events occurred before sunrise, with 59% of

roost departures coinciding with the parasitism time window (Fig. 3A). In Screaming

Cowbirds 100% of parasitism events occurred well before sunrise, and 22% of roost

departures occurred during the time window of parasitism (Fig. 3B).

DISCUSSION

We presented data on the dynamics of roost use by two species of brood parasitic cowbirds.

Radio-tagging birds allowed us to analyse the individual temporal and spatial patterns of

roost use. Females of both Shiny and Screaming Cowbirds mainly used a communal roost

near their diurnal home-ranges throughout the breeding season. Males of Screaming, but

Page 14: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

not Shiny Cowbirds shared the females’ communal roost, and pairs of Screaming Cowbirds

maintained their association in the roost at night. Females of both species left the roost

before and after sunrise, with a fraction of departures coinciding with the time window of

parasitism. Screaming Cowbird females departed from the roost and parasitized nests

earlier than Shiny Cowbird females.

Most radio-tagged cowbirds were found roosting in large groups. Similar to other

communally roosting species (reviewed by Beauchamp 1999), the benefits of roosting

communally for cowbirds may include a lower predation probability (through dilution of risk,

increased anti-predator vigilance or predator confusion) and higher probability of finding a

mate (Bijleveld et al. 2010). Furthermore, Ward and Zahavi (1973) proposed that bird

assemblages such as roosts could work as information centres, where individuals that do

not have information on the location of good feeding sites can obtain such knowledge from

individuals that do. In cowbirds, the possibility that females may eavesdrop, acquiring

information not on feeding sites but on the location of suitable target nests, seems a likely

additional benefit to roosting communally. Indirect evidence for active following has been

recorded in the Shiny Cowbird, where females frequently arrive at host nests in tandems,

probably due to uninformed females ‘tailing’ those that fly determinedly towards a previously

prospected target (Gloag et al. 2013). As with the foraging version of the Ward-Zahavi

hypothesis, this scenario does not identify benefits for the informed animals, but only for

those that follow. In fact, since cowbird females typically destroy eggs before laying, being

followed must be a significant liability. Identifying whether information transfer about

potential host nests occurs, and how the balance of costs and benefits combine to determine

roosting behaviour, remains a challenge for further research.

Shiny Cowbird females and Screaming Cowbirds of both sexes used roosts within

close range of their diurnal home-ranges, namely the areas where they search for host nests

(Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014), and showed high levels of fidelity both between and

within roosts. These findings may be related to the fact that these brood parasites must

Page 15: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

leave their roosting place and navigate towards host nests within a limited time window and

in conditions of very low light intensity. Navigating from a well-known area may therefore be

more important than reducing the distance between roosting-site and target nest. The fact

that we did not find male Shiny Cowbirds roosting in the area near their diurnal home-ranges

is consistent with this idea because they do not search for or relocate host nests and

consequently do not need to navigate in the dark. It is possible that Shiny Cowbird males

roost near areas of higher food availability and move during the morning to areas where

females search for host nests to mate. In a previous study, Scardamaglia and Reboreda

(2014) showed that Shiny and Screaming Cowbird females maintain relatively stable diurnal

ranges on consecutive days, which would allow females to monitor host nests’ progress and

in this way, be able to synchronize their laying with that of the host. This study also provides

some evidence that cowbird females fly directly from the roost to target host nests at dawn,

with females having little chance of locating suitable target nests on laying days, other than

by following informed females. In a related study, Scardamaglia et al. (2017) showed that

Shiny and Screaming Cowbird females conducted prospecting visits to host nests the days

before parasitism. Moreover, in this study we show that cowbird females are faithful to their

nocturnal roosting sites in addition to the constant diurnal home-ranges and prospecting

areas shown in previous studies (Scardamaglia & Reboreda 2014, Scardamaglia et al.

2017).

The strong roosting association found for pairs of Screaming Cowbirds shows that

diurnal social monogamy (Scardamaglia and Reboreda 2014) extends to the roost as well.

While it has been proposed that male and female Screaming Cowbirds search for and

relocate nests together (Friedmann 1929, Mason 1987), since they travel in pairs throughout

the day, it remains unclear whether males actively participate in these tasks or if their

presence alongside females near host nests is simply the result of mate guarding. The

relative enlargement of the hippocampus (an area of the brain involved in encoding spatial

information, O’Keefe & Nadel 1978) in both sexes in this species, as opposed to the sex

Page 16: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

differences found between males and females in Shiny Cowbirds (Reboreda et al. 1996),

where females search for and later relocate host nests alone, is consistent with the idea that

male and female Screaming Cowbirds share the nest-searching and relocating tasks.

Sharing the roost would be adaptive for Screaming Cowbird pairs if males just guard

females or assist them in nest searching and relocation. This socially monogamous mating

strategy is unusual among brood parasites (Hauber & Dearborn 2003). Several hypotheses

regarding avian mating systems predict that the absence of parental care should lead to an

increase in numbers of mates for both females and males (Hauber & Dearborn 2003), as

illustrated by the social promiscuity of Shiny Cowbirds (Mason 1987, Scardamaglia &

Reboreda 2014), also confirmed by our observations on roosting. While the genetic mating

systems still need corroboration, and more information is necessary about the daily routines

and copulatory behaviour of both species, it is reasonable to expect that the marked

difference in mating systems responds to differences in parasitic specialization, an aspect of

mating that to our knowledge has not been explored in any detail to date.

Parasitic cowbird females, as opposed to nest-building species, are constrained to

leave their roosting place and reach target nests within a limited time window, between

completion of egg formation and daylight. However, on days when they do not have an egg

to lay, and the morning will be spent only on feeding and searching for nests, they can leave

the roost later. Our data support this expectation. We found that 59% of Shiny Cowbird and

22% of Screaming Cowbird roost departures overlapped with the parasitism time window.

Thus, although we cannot rule out that some roost departures that occurred during the

laying time window may have not been associated with egg-laying, most departures

occurring afterwards must have been non-parasitic visits or females leaving the roost to

forage. The timing of departure from the roost may be a predictor of whether a female is on

a laying day or not, and consequently help to estimate the number of eggs laid by female

cowbirds during the breeding season. The difference observed between species in the

percentage of roost departures that overlap in timing with the distribution of parasitism

Page 17: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

events may be due to an underestimate of early departures by Screaming Cowbirds. Data

for departures from earlier than 50 min before sunrise are missing (there are parasitic events

recorded in the -55 to -50 min time bin but no roost departures recorded for the same period

or earlier). This may be attributed to the different methods that were used to record each

type of event: parasitism events were recorded by automatic data-loggers in the nests that

worked 24 h a day, while departures from the roost were recorded by a researcher with a

hand-held tracking receiver in the roost area. Hence, there was a limit on the recording of

times and access to roost locations, especially for Screaming Cowbirds. This leads to a

potential underestimate of the percentage of departures from the roost that coincide with the

parasitism time window in Screaming Cowbirds.

To summarize, we showed that throughout the breeding season females of Shiny

and Screaming Cowbirds roosted near the area at which they searched for and parasitized

host nests and that the timing of roost departures can potentially predict parasitic behaviour.

Our study illustrates how the use of radio-tracking technologies may help to understand

relatively poorly known aspects of the behaviour of brood parasites.

We thank Fundación Elsa Shaw de Pearson for allowing us to conduct the study in Reserve

El Destino and Daniel Campioni for help in preparation of Figure 1.

Funding statement: R.C.S was supported by a scholarship from the Consejo Nacional de

Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET). J.C.R is a Research Fellow of

CONICET. This work was supported by grants from Agencia Nacional de Promoción

Científica y Tecnológica and University of Buenos Aires. None of our funders had any

influence on the content of the submitted or published manuscript. None of our funders

require approval of the final manuscript to be published.

Ethics statement: All work complied with Argentinean Law.

Page 18: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Authors contribution: R.C.S., A.K. and J.C.R. conceived the idea, design and formulated

hypothesis, R.C.S. collected the data, R.C.S, A.K. and J.C.R. wrote the paper, R.C.S.

analysed the data.

LITERATURE CITED

Adams, T. L. (2011). Roost site seleciton of Dunlin (Calidris alpina) on Arcata Bay, Humboldt

Bay, Humboldt County, California. M.S. Thesis. The Faculty of Humboldt State

University.

Beauchamp, G. (1999). The evolution of communal roosting in birds : origin and secondary

losses. Behav. Ecol. 10:675–687. doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.6.675

Béchet, A., J.-F. Giroux, G. Gauthier, and M. Bélisle (2010). Why roost at the same place?

Exploring short-term fidelity in staging snow geese. Condor 112:294–303. doi:

10.1525/cond.2010.090050

Bijleveld, A. I., M. Egas, J. A. van Gils, and T. Piersma (2010). Beyond the information

centre hypothesis: Communal roosting for information on food, predators, travel

companions and mates? Oikos 119:277–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17892.x

Bivand, R., and C. Rundel (2016). rgeos: Interface to Geometry Engine - Open Source

(GEOS). R package version 0.3-20. [Online.] Available at https://cran.r-

project.org/package=rgeos.

Conklin, J. R. J., M. a Colwell, and D. B. Lank (2007). Diurnal and nocturnal roost site fidelity

of Dunlin (Calidris alpina pacifica) at Humboldt Bay, California. Auk 124:677–689. doi:

10.1642/0004-8038(2007)124[677:DANRSF]2.0.CO;2

Cruz, A., T. Manolis, and R. Andrews (1990). Reproductive interactions of the Shiny Cowbird

Molothrus bonariensis and the Yellow-hooded Blackbird Agelaius icterocephalus in

Page 19: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Trinidad. Ibis 132:436–444.

De Mársico, M. C., B. Mahler, M. Chomnalez, A. Di Giácomo, and J. C. Reboreda (2010).

Host Use by Generalist and Specialist Brood-Parasitic Cowbirds at Population and

Individual Levels. Adv. Study Behav. 42:83–121. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(10)42003-3

Dhondt, A. A., M. J. L. Driscoll, and E. C. H. Swarthout (2006). House Finch Carpodacus

mexicanus roosting behaviour during the non-breeding season and possible effects of

Mycoplasmal conjunctivitis. Ibis 149:1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2006.00588.x

du Plessis, M. a., W. W. Weathers, and W. D. Koenig (1994). Energetic benefits of

communal roosting by Acorn Woodpeckers during the nonbreeding season. Condor

96:631–637. doi: 10.2307/1369466

Feare, C., and M. Zaccagnini (1993). Roost departure by Shiny Cowbirds (Molothrus

bonariensis). Hornero 13:292–293.

Fiorini, V. D., and J. C. Reboreda (2006). Cues used by shiny cowbirds (Molothrus

bonariensis) to locate and parasitise chalk-browed mockingbird (Mimus saturninus)

nests. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 60:379–385. doi: 10.1007/s00265-006-0175-3

Fraga, R. M. (1985). Host-parasite interactions between Chalk-browed Mockingbirds and

Shiny Cowbirds. Orn. Monogr. 36:829–844.

Fraga, R. M. (1991). The social system of a communal breeder, the bay-winged

cowbird Molothrus badius. Ethology 89:195–210. doi:10.1111/j.1439-

0310.1991.tb00304.x

Fraga, R. M. (1998). Interactions of the parasitic Screaming and Shiny Cowbirds (Molothrus

rufoaxillaris and M. bonariensis) with a shared host, the Bay-winged Cowbird (M.

badius). In Parasitic birds and their hosts. Studies in coevolution (S. I. Rothstein and S.

K. Robinson, Editors). Oxford University Press, New York, NY, pp. 173–193.

Page 20: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Friedmann, H. (1929). The Cowbirds. A Study in the Biology of Social Parasitism (C.

Thomas, Editor). Springfield, Illinois, pp. 57–144.

Gates, J. E., and D. R. Evans (1998). Cowbirds breeding in the Central Appalachians:

spatial and temporal patterns and habitat selection. Ecol. Appl. 8:27–40.

Gloag R., V. D. Fiorini, J. C. Reboreda, and A. Kacelnik (2012) Brood parasite eggs

enhance egg survivorship in a multiply parasitized host. Proc. R. Soc. B

279:1831–9. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2047

Gloag, R., V. D. Fiorini, J. C. Reboreda, and A. Kacelnik (2013). The wages of violence:

mobbing by mockingbirds as a frontline defence against brood-parasitic cowbirds.

Anim. Behav. 86:1023–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.007

Hahn, D., J. Sedgwick, I. Painter, and N. Casna (1999). A spatial and genetic analysis of

cowbird host selection. Stud. Avian Biol. 18:204–217.

Hauber, M. E., and D. C. Dearborn (2003). Parentage without parental care: what to look for

in genetic studies of obligate brood-parasitic mating systems. Auk 120:1–13.

Hoy, G., and J. Ottow (1964). Biological and oological studies of the molothrine cowbirds

(Icteridae) of Argentina. Auk 81:186–203.

Kattan, G. H., A. Posada, D. F. Arenas, J. L. Moreno, and Á. Barrera (2016). Flocking

behavior of Shiny Cowbirds (Molothrus bonariensis) at feeding areas during the daily

cycle. Wilson J. Ornithol. 128:441–445. doi: 10.1676/1559-4491-128.2.441

Laughlin, A. J., D. R. Sheldon, D. W. Winkler, and C. M. Taylor (2014). Behavioral drivers of

communal roosting in a songbird: a combined theoretical and empirical approach.

Behav. Ecol. 25:734–743. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru044

Mason, P. (1980). Ecological and evolutionary aspects of host selection in cowbirds. D. Biol.

Thesis, University of Texas, Austin.

Page 21: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Mason, P. (1987). Pair formation in cowbirds: evidence found for Screaming but not Shiny

Cowbirds. Condor 89:349–356.

Morrison, D. W., and D. F. Caccamise (1985). Ephemeral roosts and stable patches – A

radiotelemetry study of communally roosting starlings. Auk 102:793–804.

Morrison, D. W., and D. F. Caccamise (1990). Comparison of roost use by three species of

communal roostmates. Condor 92:405–412.

O’Keefe, J., and L. Nadel (1978). The Hippocampus As A Cognitive Map. Oxford University

Press, Oxford.

Paquet, M., C. Doutrelant, M. Loubon, F. Theron, M. Rat, and R. Covas (2016). Communal

roosting, thermoregulatory benefits and breeding group size predictability in

cooperatively breeding sociable weavers. J. Avian Biol. 47:749-755. doi:

10.1111/jav.00916

Rappole, J. H., and A. R. Tipton (1991). New harness design for attachment of radio

transmitters to small passerines. J. Field Ornith. 62:335–337.

R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R

Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Online.] Available at https://www.r-project.org/.

Reboreda, J. C., N. Clayton, and A. Kacelnik (1996). Species and sex differences in

hippocampus size in parasitic and non-parasitic cowbirds. Neuroreport 7:505–508.

Scardamaglia, R. C., and J. C. Reboreda (2014). Ranging behavior of female and male

Shiny Cowbirds and Screaming Cowbirds while searching for host nests. Auk 131:610–

618. doi: 10.1642/AUK-14-54.1

Scardamaglia, R. C., V. D. Fiorini, A. Kacelnik, and J. C. Reboreda (2017). Planning host

exploitation through prospecting visits by parasitic cowbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

71:23. doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2250-8

Page 22: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Thompson, F. (1994). Temporal and spatial patterns of breeding Brown-headed Cowbirds in

the midwestern United States. Auk 111:979–990.

Tuero, D. T., V. D. Fiorini, and J. C. Reboreda (2007). Effects of shiny cowbird parasitism on

different components of house wren reproductive success. Ibis 149, 521–527.

Ursino, C. A., M. C. De Mársico, M. Sued, A. Farall, and J. C. Reboreda (2011). Brood

parasitism disproportionately increases nest provisioning and helper recruitment in a

cooperatively breeding bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65:2279–2286. doi:

10.1007/s00265-011-1238-7

Ward, P., and A. Zahavi (1973). The importance of certain assemblages of birds as

“information-centres” for food-finding. Ibis 115:517–534. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-

919X.1973.tb01990.x

Weatherhead, P. J. (1987). Field tests of information transfer in communally roosting birds.

Anim. Behav. 35(2):614–615.

White, G. C., and R. A. Garrott (1990). Chapter 4: Estimating Animal Locations. In: Analysis

of Wildlife Radio-Tracking Data. Academic Press, San Diego, California.

Winkler, D. W. (2006). Roosts and migrations of swallows. Hornero 21:85–97.

Yom-Tov, Y. (1979). The disadvantage of low positions in colonial roosts: an experiment to

test the effect of droppings on plumage quality. Ibis 121:331–333.

Figure 1. Study area with area used during the day (polygon), roosting locations centroids

and locations of data-loggers (i.e. monitored host nests) with marked parasitism events for

(a) Shiny Cowbirds and (b) Screaming Cowbirds.

Page 23: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.

Figure 2. Observed mean distance between the location of each female within the roost for

a sample of 15 female Shiny Cowbirds and frequency distribution of expected observations

of mean distances if females settled within the roost at random each night. The observed

distance between nights falls below the 95th percentile of the distribution, indicating that

females located significantly closer to the locations they used previously than expected by

chance.

Figure 3. Distribution of the proportion of roost departures and parasitic events that occurred

at different time intervals for Shiny Cowbird (A) and Screaming Cowbird (B) females. Shiny

Cowbird roost departure times (n = 97) correspond to 19 Shiny Cowbird females in 1-14

different days. Shiny Cowbird parasitism events (n = 21) were recorded either by video-

recordings or the automated telemetry system at Chalked-browed Mockingbird nests and

correspond to a maximum of 18 different females (some females could not be reliably

identified in video-recordings) in 20 different nests. Screaming Cowbird roost departure

times (n = 28) correspond to 11 Screaming Cowbird females in 1-6 different days. Parasitism

events (n = 11) were recorded by the automated telemetry system at Baywing nests and

correspond to 7 different females in 7 different nests.

Page 24: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

Figure 1

Page 25: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

Figure 2

Page 26: Roosting behaviour is related to reproductive strategy in brood ...

Acc

epte

d A

rtic

le

Figure 3