Top Banner
Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature: Identifying study biases
60

Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Dec 19, 2015

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Rita Popat, PhDClinical Assistant Professor

Division of EpidemiologyStanford University School of Medicine

August 7, 2007

Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Identifying study biases

Page 2: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

What is critical appraisal?

Balanced assessment of benefits and strengths of research against its flaws and weaknesses

Assessment of research process and results

To be undertaken by all health professionals as part of their work

Page 3: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Why should we critically appraise?

Published research is not always valid – we cannot take conclusions for granted, even if the article is published in a peer-reviewed journal.

Published research is not always relevant – the abstract may indicate relevance but you will need to read the complete article to judge its applicability to your own practice/circumstances.

To improve clinical effectiveness, we need a systematic framework to interpret research, rather than relying on a haphazard or casual approach.

3

Page 4: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Key Steps To Effective Critical Appraisal

1. Are the results valid?

2. What are the results?

3. How will these results help me work with my patients?

4

Focus of today’s lecture

Page 5: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Outline

Quick review of study designs

What is validity?

Identifying study biases that can threaten

internal validity

5

Page 6: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Observational vs. Experimental Studies

Experimental studies – the investigator tries to control the environment in which the hypothesis is tested (the randomized, double-blind clinical trial is the gold standard)

Observational studies – the population is observed without any interference by the investigator

6

Page 7: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Why Observational Studies?

Cheaper Faster Can examine long-term effects Hypothesis-generating Sometimes, experimental studies are not

ethical (e.g., randomizing subjects to smoke) Sometimes, experimental studies are not

possible – examples… randomizing subjects to gestational

diabetes studying natural progression of a disease studying long term effects of drugs

7

Page 8: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

THE LANCET • Vol 359 • January 5, 2002

Page 9: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Cohort Studies

Target population

Exposed (E+)

Not Exposed (E-)

Disease-free cohort

Disease (IE+)

Disease-free

Disease (IE-)

Disease-free

TIME

9

Page 10: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

a b

c d

E

E

I

I

dc

cba

a

RR

  Outcome-

Exposure +

Exposure -

Outcome+

a + b

c + d

Incidence (probability) of outcome among exposed

Incidence (probability)of outcome among unexposed

10

Measure of association in cohort studies:

Relative Risk (RR)

Page 11: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

376 12560

985 23957

73.00395.0

029.0

24942985

12936376

RR

  T2DM-

Active †

Inactive

T2DM+

12936

24942

11

Interpretation: Active women are 27% less likely to develop T2DM compared to inactive women

†Energy expenditure was at least 1000 kcal/wk.

Page 12: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Cohort Studies: Advantages & Disadvantages

Advantages Allows you to measure true rates and risks of disease

for the exposed and the unexposed groups.

Temporality is correct

Can be used to study multiple outcomes.

Prevents bias in the ascertainment of exposure that may occur after a person develops a disease.

Disadvantages

Can be lengthy and costly!

Loss to follow-up is a problem (especially if non-random).

12

Page 13: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Target population

Exposed in past

Not exposed

Exposed in the past

Not Exposed

Case-Control Studies

Cases

(outcome +)

Controls

(outcome -)

13

Page 14: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

  Disease (D+)Cases

No disease(D-)Controls

 

Exposure (E+) a b

No exposure(E-) c d

 

14

Measure of Association in case-control studies: Odds Ratio (OR)

OR = Odds of exposure among controls

Odds of exposure among cases )|(

)|(

DEP

DEP

=

)|(

)|(

DEP

DEP=

caccaa

dbddbb

=

bc

ad

a+c b+d

Page 15: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

15

  Cases Controls  

NSAIDS+ 561 971

NSAIDS- 71 74

 

15

60.0)971)(71(

)74)(561(OR

Interpretation: NSAIDs use is associated with a 40% reduction in the risk of colon cancer

Page 16: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Case-Control Studies: Advantages & Disadvantages Advantages:

• Cheap and fast• Great for rare diseases

Disadvantages:• Exposure estimates are subject to

• recall bias (those with the disease are searching for reasons why they got sick and may be more likely to report an exposure)

• interviewer bias (interviewer may prompt a positive response in cases).

• Temporality is a problem (did exposure cause disease or disease cause exposure?)

16

Page 17: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Intervention Studies

Target population

Intervention group

No intervention group

Disease-free cohort

Disease

Disease-free

Disease

Disease-free

TIME

17

Page 18: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Eligible participants

RandomizedStandard lifestyle recommendations

Intensive Metformin PlaceboLifestyle(n = 1079) (n = 1073) (n = 1082) 18

Page 19: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

DPP trial: Primary Outcome - Diabetes

19

Page 20: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Intervention Studies: Advantages & Disadvantages

Advantages:• Allows randomization (controls for confounding)• Allows double-blind assessment (controls bias)

Disadvantages:• Can be lengthy and costly! • Loss to follow-up is a problem (especially if non-

random).• Ethical limitations

20

Page 21: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Causation in human health & disease

Association does not prove causation

• If a putative risk factor and the occurrence of an outcome are strongly associated with each other it does not provide evidence that the risk factor causes the disease, only implies that it is correlated with outcome

• Non-causal explanations may cause a spurious association – study biases (measurement error, selection bias, confounding, sampling error)

21

Page 22: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Study validity

INTERNAL VALIDITY

EXTERNAL VALIDITY

22

Do we believe the results?

Can the results be applied to the target population i.e., beyond the subjects in the study?

Page 23: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Threats to Internal validity: Non-causal explanations due to study biases

Confounding

Selection bias

Misclassification bias (measurement error)

23

Page 24: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Confounding

Confounding is the defined as a distortion of an exposure-outcome association brought about by the association of another factor with both the outcome and exposure

exposure outcome

Confounding factor

24

Page 25: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Research question - Is physical activity associated with risk of T2DM?

Physical activity T2DM

BMI++

25Potential confounder

JAMA. 2004;292:1188-1194

Page 26: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Why worry about confounding?

Spurious association

Exaggerate an association (over-estimate)

Attenuate an association (under-estimate)

Obscure an association

26

Page 27: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Methods for controlling confounding

Design phase• Randomization• Matching• Restriction

Analysis phase: statistical adjustment for confounders

27

Page 28: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Controlling confounding by randomization

Page 29: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Identifying potential confounders

Subset of Table 1 from JAMA. 2004;292:1188-1194

Page 30: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Statistical adjustment for confounding

Is BMI a confounder of the relationship b/w physical activity and T2DM?

Compare the crude hazard ratio (HR) to the adjusted HR.

Weinstein et al. JAMA 292:1188-94

Crude HR =2.9/3.9 = 0.74

Page 31: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Statistical adjustment for confounding Weinstein et al. JAMA 292:1188-94

10% rule for identifying confounders:(Crude HR - Adjusted HR) X 100 10%

Crude HR

Page 32: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

True or false

A randomized clinical trial design cannot be affected by bias due to confounding?

Answer: False (if randomization is not done appropriately, then can introduce bias due to confounding)

32

Page 33: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Identifying bias due to confounding in a RCT

Check the randomization procedure in the methods section (e.g., blocked randomization schemes when sample size is small)

Check Table 1 see if groups are balanced If not, how was it handled?

Was intention to treat analysis used?

33

Page 34: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

JAMA. 2006;296:2441-2450

Page 35: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

JAMA. 2006;296:2441-2450

Page 36: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Selection bias

A form of sampling bias due to systematic differences between those who are selected for study (or agree to participate) and those who are not selected (or refuse to participate).

36

Page 37: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Selection bias Improper selection of cases or controls in a case-

control study Subjects lost to follow-up varies according to both

the exposure of interest and the outcome (e.g., in prospective cohort studies and clinical trials)

Can affect any study design, although case-control studies more prone to selection bias

Selection bias can cause either overestimates or underestimates of the true associations between the exposure and disease in the underlying population

37

Page 38: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Identifying Selection bias What are the response rates?

Was follow-up complete? Ideally should have follow-up for at least 80% of

the initial sample/cohort

Does drop out differ in the groups being compared (e.g., treatment and placebo groups)?

38

Page 39: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Selection bias example:

In this case-control study, response rate was ~80% in cases and ~60% in controls.

Could there be selection bias…especially among controls?

39

Page 40: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Selection bias example:

So observed proportion of NSAID users among controls (92.9%) is greater than true proportion of exposure

Hence observed odds ratio of 0.54 is an overestimate (i.e., true odds ratio is greater than 0.54)

[Note that Odds ratio = p1* (1- p2) / p2 *(1- p1)]

Scenario: NSAID users in the base population were more likely to participate in this study than were non-NSAID users

p1 p2

40

Page 41: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Some strategies for minimizing selection bias

Careful enumeration and thorough attempts at recruiting all cases within the source population

High standards for methods of control selection (population-based ideal)

Minimizing non-response and refusals

Minimizing loss to follow-up (in cohort and RCTs)

41

Page 42: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Information bias (aka measurement error)

Misclassification of outcome Misclassification of exposure status

42

Page 43: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Information bias (measurement error)

Imperfect definitions of study variables (outcome or predictors) or flawed data collection procedures

Erroneous classification of – outcome– exposure

43

Page 44: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Information bias misclassification

Study

Gold-standard

b+da+c

c+dd

TN

c

FNOutcome -

a+bb

FP

a

TPOutcome +

Outcome

-

Outcome

+

Sensitivity = a / (a+c)

Specificity = d / (b+d)

44

Page 45: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

45

 Outcome + Outcome -

 

Exposure + (treatment)

a b

Exposure –(placebo)

c d

 

45

Misclassification of the outcome

Non-differential misclassification occurs when the degree of misclassification of outcome is independent of exposure status Tends to bias the association toward the null Occurs when the sensitivity and specificity of the

classification of outcome are same for exposed and non-exposed groups but less than 100%

Page 46: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

46

 Outcome + Outcome -

 

Exposure + (treatment)

a b

Exposure –(placebo)

c d

 

46

Misclassification of the outcome

Differential misclassification occurs when the degree of misclassification differs between the groups being compared. May bias the association either toward or away from

the null hypothesis Occurs when the sensitivity and specificity of the

classification of outcome differ for exposed and non-exposed groups

Page 47: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

47

  Cases Controls  

NSAIDS+ 561 971

NSAIDS- 71 74

 

47

Most likely scenario in this study

Misclassification of the outcome: NSAIDs and colon cancer example

Is misclassification of outcome likely to be non-differential or differential with respect to exposure?

Page 48: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Study Measures

The primary measures were the Medical Outcomes Study 36-Item Short- Form Health Survey (SF-36) bodily pain and physical function scales and the American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons MODEMS version of the Oswestry Disability Index (ODI).

Secondary measures included patient self-reported improvement, work status, and satisfaction with current symptoms and with care. Symptom severity was measured by the Sciatica Bothersomeness Index (range, 0-24; higher scores represent worse symptoms).

JAMA. 2006;296:2441-2450

Is misclassification of outcome likely to be non-differential or differential with respect to exposure?

Page 49: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:
Page 50: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

50

  Cases(outcome +)

Controls(outcome -)

 

Exposure + a b

Exposure - c d

 

50

Misclassification of the exposure

Non-differential misclassification occurs when the degree of misclassification of exposure is independent of outcome/disease status Tends to bias the association toward the null Occurs when the sensitivity and specificity of the

classification of exposure are same for those with and without the outcome but less than 100%

Page 51: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

51

  Cases(outcome +)

Controls(outcome -)

 

Exposure + a b

Exposure - c d

 

51

Misclassification of the outcome

Differential misclassification occurs when the degree of misclassification differs between the groups being compared. May bias the association either toward or away from the

null hypothesis Occurs when the sensitivity and specificity of the

classification of exposure differ for those with and without the outcome

Page 52: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

52

  Cases Controls  

NSAIDS+ 561 971

NSAIDS- 71 74

 

52

Non-differential: poor recall in cases and controls

Differential: cases recall NSAID use better than controls?

Misclassification of the exposure: NSAIDs and colon cancer example

Is misclassification of exposure likely to be non-differential or differential with respect to outcome?

Page 53: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Some strategies for minimizing misclassification bias Test reliability and validity of instruments used to

determine outcome and exposure

Similar methods for determining outcome and exposure in all study subjects

Train interviewers, blind interviewers to outcome status and study hypothesis ( interviewer bias)

Blind subjects to study hypothesis ( recall bias)

Use incident cases in case-control studies, not prevalent ( recall bias)

Try to use objective measures (e.g., pharmacy records vs. self-report use of medications)

53

Page 54: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Summary : Study designs and biases

54

Threats to internal validity

Case-control Cohort RCT

Confounding Generally present Generally present Not likely (due to randomization)

Selection bias Likely (e.g., when low response

rates)

May occur due to differential loss to

follow up

May occur due to differential loss to

follow-up

Misclassification of exposure

More likely to be differential

Generally non-differential

Not likely; if exists then non-differential (e.g., drop-in/drop out)

Misclassification of outcome

Most likely non-differential

Most likely non-differential

Most likely non-differential

Page 55: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Once you are satisfied the study findings are valid…

You can now ask whether the association causal?

Evaluate positive features of causation Temporality Strength of the association Dose-response relationship Consistency of findings Biologic plausibility

55

Page 56: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Experiment/RCT

Prospective cohort

Retrospective cohort

Case-control

Correlation/x-section

Case series

Case report

Proofof cause

Cost andease

Best

Best

56

Page 57: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Critical appraisal tools

Assist with systematic critique of published research papers

Several tools available

One of my favorite… http://www.muhc-ebn.mcgill.ca/EBN_tools.htm#evidence and look for link to “CASP Appraisal Tools”

57

Page 58: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Critical appraisal of an article about therapy or prevention

Primary guides: Was the assignment of patients to treatment groups randomized?

Were all patients who entered the trial properly accounted for at the conclusion?

Was follow up complete?

Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were randomized, that is, was an intention to treat analysis used?

58

Page 59: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

Secondary guides: Were patients, health workers and study personnel blinded to

treatment?

Does the study provide evidence that blinding was effective?

Were the groups similar at the start of the trial?

Was there an adequate Table 1?

If not, were adjustments made for differences?

Aside from the experimental intervention, were the groups

treated equally?

59

Critical appraisal of an article about therapy or prevention

Page 60: Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 Rapid appraisal of the literature:

What were the results? How large was the treatment effect? How precise was the estimate of the treatment effect? Were confidence intervals given? Will the results help me with my patients?

60

Will discuss these aspects on August 9th, 2007!

Critical appraisal of an article about therapy or prevention