Top Banner
/79 /No, e6O7 PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented to the Graduate Council of the University of North Texas in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE By Caroline Curlin, B.A. Denton, Texas May, 1990
93

PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

Dec 24, 2019

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

/79

/No, e6O7

PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS

USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY

THESIS

Presented to the Graduate Council of the

University of North Texas in Partial

Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

MASTER OF SCIENCE

By

Caroline Curlin, B.A.

Denton, Texas

May, 1990

Page 2: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

Curlin, Caroline. Prediction of Verbal Dominance

Behaviors Using Constructivist Theory. Master of Science

(Clinical Psychology), May, 1990, 94 pp., 20 tables, 44

references.

This study assessed how Constructivist theory accounts

for verbal dominance. Conversations of rotating dyads were

tape recorded, then coded for measures of dominance.

Subjects completed a trait dominance scale and a

constructivist personality test. Interpersonal rankings of

dominance were found to be more consistent with observed

behavior than trait dominance scores. Extreme trait

dominance scores were associated with a constructivist

measure indicating maladjustment. Dyads identified as more

resistant to change were found to use fewer verbal control

strategies; male/male dyads were characterized by direct,

functional interactions. Dyads that were highly comfortable

with one another utilized fewer verbal control methods.

Lastly, interactions in which participants reported

unfamiliar self-experiencing utilized higher levels of

verbal control. Implications for group processing,

assessment of dominance and sex differences are discussed.

Page 3: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES . .

CHAPTER

I. INTRODUCTION

II. METHOD

SubjectsProcedureMeasures of resistanceDependent measures

III. RESULTS . . . . . . . . . .

IV. DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . .

APPENDIX. ........ .....

REFERENCES... .... ...... .

... 31

to change

38

51

. . 60

. .83

iii

.v

Page 4: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Interrater Reliabilities of ObservationalMeasures . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36

2. Means and Standard Deviations of Independentand Dependent Variables: Monadic . . . . . . 38

3. Means and Standard Deviations for DependentVariables: Dyadic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39

4. Correlation Matrix of Verbal Measures andDominance Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40

5. Correlations Between Peer Ranking of Dominanceand Verbal Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

6. Regression Results for Deviation of Dominanceand FIC . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43

7. Regression Results for Total Talk Rate . . . . 78

8. Regression Results for Differential Talk Rate . 79

9. Regression Results of Total Interruption Rate . 79

10. Regression Results of Differential InterruptionRate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79

11. Regression Results of Total Question Rate . . . 80

12. Regression Results of Differential QuestionRate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80

13. Regression Results for Total Ah Rate . . . . . 81

14. Regression Results for Differential Ah Rate . . 82

15. Regression Results for Total Silence Rate . . . 82

16. Regression Results for Differential SilenceRate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82

17. Correlations Between Resistance to Change,Sex, and Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . . 46

iv

Page 5: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

LIST OF TABLES--CONTINUED

Table Page

18. Correlations Between Resistance to Change,Dominance Scores and Proportion of IndependentSelves Brought to Interactions. ., . . . . . . 47

19. Regression Results for Total Comfort . . . . . 49

20. Correlations Between Comfort andPredictor Variables ........................50

V

Page 6: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

When two people begin to develop a relationship,

apparently insignificant statements and behaviors have great

consequence for future structure and development of that

dyad. Each question, suggestion or gesture is a message

regarding the nature of the relationship that will exist.

Individuals who believe that their relationships evolve

without influences from one or both partners are unaware of

the process by which preferences and expectations are subtly

expressed in the initial stages. They may be aware -that the

relationship is platonic, not sexual, that practical jokes

do not occur or that one member usually decides where they

will eat dinner. However, it is not uncommon for

participants to be unable to describe the behavioral and

verbal communications by which such "rules" were

established.

Each time two people are engaged in an interpersonal

situation, communications are exchanged, both verbally and

nonverbally. Some theorists posit that it is impossible not

to communicate in the presence of others; even silence

expresses some meaning (Haley, 1972; Watzlawick, Beavin, &

Jackson, 1967). Danzinger (1976). argued that "a verbal

Page 7: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

2

message is never merely a neutral transmission of

information about the world outside, it is always also a

communication about the relationship between the speaker and

his audience" (p. 26). Researchers have regarded such

behavioral and verbal messages as requests (Haley, 1972),

attempts to impose behavioral change (Reusch & Bateson,

1951) or inquiries into the anticipated state of the

relationship (Kelly, 1955). Communications serve to

monitor, suggest, reply and delineate what future

interactions will entail. As each communication is reponded

to, either positively or negatively, the relationship is

further defined and structured. For example, if one member

talks animatedly about a recent baseball game, the other

person can either join in the conversation, gesturing

frequently, or can yawn and have nothing to say. In any

event, the message has been responded to, either directly or

indirectly, and the relationship further defined.

Several researchers have posited ways to conceptualize

this process. Foa and Foa (1980) developed a system to

classify what types of resources are exchanged, defined

along dimensions of concreteness and particularism (whether

any person can fulfill the need or if a particular person is

required). Reusch & Bateson (1951) emphasized the

interpretation or "codification" of messages and events that

take place in a relationship; such codification defines the

role and status of participants. Haley, 1972,

MUM

Page 8: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

3

conceptualized this process as answering two questions; 1)

what kinds of behaviors will take place and 2) who decides

what takes place. The first question refers to the content

of the behaviors existing in the relationship, the latter to

the structure, or the relative influenceability of each

member in determining what occurs between them. This does

not refer to control of the other person's behavior, but

rather the attempt to control the relationship's definition.

Interpersonal communication has also been divided into

similar poles. The content of messages has been refered to

as semantic (Danzinger, 1976) or denotative (Keisler &

Bernstein, 1974), that is, the actual verbal content used to

communicate information to the other participant. All that

is necessary to understand messages at this level is a

command of the language. Messages regarding the structure

of the dyad have been referred to as pragmatic

communications, "hidden agendas" regarding the way in which

the members interact with one another (Danzinger, 1976).

They are also known as connotative messages conveying

information regarding the attitudes or affect of one of the

participants (Keisler & Bernstein, 1974).

Although the denotative words spoken may indicate a

straightforward statement, connotative meanings may indicate

a quite different intent. Sarcasm is usually expressed by

tone of voice or gestures, and if interpreted by the

denotative content alone, would be greatly misunderstood.

Page 9: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

4

At this level, more than linguistic knowledge is necessary

for interpretation, for social norms and idiosyncratic

gestures and terms are utilized. Connotative messages

constitute much of the process of establishing the future

structure of relationships. By interpreting words merely at

the denotative level, as common in behavioral theory, one

may disregard important statements essential to resistance

and transference in therapy (Keisler & Bernstein, 1974), as

well as the everyday process of structuring relationships.

The focus of this study is upon this process of

connotatively communicating the differential influence of

participants, or the structure, of dyadic relationships.

Relationships can be structured in two ways according

to Haley (1972): complementary or symmetrical. A

complementary relationship is one in which both persons are

exchanging different types of behavior; this is typical in a

professor/student or physician/patient relationship. One

member has more influence in determining what shall occur

than the other. Symmetrical relationships are characterized

by an exchange of the same type of behavior, as is common in

friendships or peer relationships. Both members have

similar amounts of influence, and status differences between

the two are minimized. Watzlawick, Beavin & Jackson (1967)

posited that "all communicational interchanges are either

symmetrical or complementary, depending upon whether they

are based on equality or differences" (p. 70).

IMF-,AWA I , I I 11,111, m lim PrA-P .. .. -- -- -

Page 10: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

5

Entering a relationship with societally defined roles

lessens the need for establishing the structure of the

relationship. When one visits a physician, one expects that

the doctor will make the proper inquiries, label the

disorder and prescribe medicine, and, that the patient will

obediently conform to those suggestions. Generally, the

physician controls what occurs. Most individuals are aware

of societal expectations and so conform to physicians'

suggestions. It is unusual for a patient to diagnose the

illness and to suggest treatment to the physician, although

this may occur. In such a case of misunderstood roles, the

physician is likely to re-establish the fact that he or she

has attended medical school and is licensed to prescribe

medicine, not the patient. In this manner conflicting

expectations about appropriate behavior are resolved. When

individuals enter relationships where role expectations are

not prescribed, they must negotiate to define the structure

of influenceability. This process of arranging influence in

socially unstructured relationships is the focus of this

paper.

According to Haley (1972) the process of defining the

structure or level of influenceability of dyadic members is

mutual; "two people inevitably work out together what kind

of relationship they have by mutually indicating what kind

of behavior is to take place between them" (p. 10). The

process of equal participation in determining differential

Page 11: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

6

levels of influence may appear to be conceptually

incompatible. This process is one in which participants

mutually determine their relative influenceability. It

would seem logical that the more influential person

communicate their status to the other, whether acceptable or

not. Haley (1972) argued that it is impossible for one

person to define the relationship exclusively; for one

person to dominate, the other person must agree. It is just

as theoretically inconceivable for a passive person to have

no influence in determining their relative position. Haley

states (1972) "whenever a person tries to avoid controlling

the definition of a relationship he must at a more general

level be controlling what type of relationship this is to

be--one where he is not in control" (p. 10). Similarly,

Leary (1957) posited an interpersonal theory rejecting any

possibility of responding in a vacuum, for the behavior of

one person simultaneously affects the behavior of the other.

He classified behavior along two dimensions:

dominance/submission and love/hate. Behavior in one

quadrant "pulls" for or draws complementary behavior in the

opposite quadrant. In this manner, passive behavior both

verbally and nonverbally places expectations for dominant

behavior upon others.

The process by which both members mutually negotiate

their relative levels of dominance occurs via "maneuvers,"

communications that put the current status of the4

- pill-No, I i Mil

Page 12: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

7

relationship into question (Haley, 1972). Stable

relationships are those which have reached a state of mutual

agreement; thus, relatively few maneuvers are executed.

Unstable relationships occur when one or both persons wish

to change the existing system and behave in a manner so as

to suggest, question, or demand new roles. Each maneuver

must be responded to, either negatively, in which case

instability continues, or positively, leading to a stable

relationship once more. Typically beginning relationships

are characterized by instability, but relationships of any

length can exist in this unstable state.

This process of interpersonally maneuvering to define

the nature of interactions exists among participants in

larger social units as well as in dyads. Yalom (1970)

described several phases that groups must undergo to reach a

mature, profitably working state. The first stage is the

cocktail stage wherein group members as strangers attempt to

become familiar with one another, establish roles for

themselves and search for meaning in therapy. Groups at

this stage are typified by stereotyped, innocuous, social

chit-chat oriented to establish a sense of belonging and

understanding. The second stage consists of conflict and

competition for dominance. By expressing hostility toward

the leader and other members, each person strives to

establish a certain amount of influence in the group. Until

a mutually agreeable, stable dominance hierarchy is

Page 13: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

8

established, the group cannot progress to the third stage,

characterized by cohesiveness and intimacy, where most of

the valuable therapeutic work is accomplished. Just as

members must struggle, compete and negotiate the hierarchy

of influence in groups, they must also do so in dyads.

Similarly, Duck (1972) has characterized this struggle

for dominance in dyads as evolving from a superficial to a

more psychological basis. He reported that during the

formation of dyads, the salient factor in predicting

friendship development was similarity of concrete

constructs, those regarding physical attractiveness or other

superficial characteristics. As the relationship

progressed, the similarity of psychological constructs

influenced its evolution. As dyad members reach a cohesive

state, negotiation of more personally meaningful constructs

becomes central.

In addition to using the methodology of self-report

measures, researchers have attempted to determine behavioral

indicators of the process of maneuvering for influence.

Several fairly established behavioral measures of the level

of interpersonal dominance have been reported in current

literature. While many types of communication concomitant,

unorderly, or unstructured, conversation is considered by

communication theorists as a unique subset of sequential

communication defined as an "orderly exchange of speaking

turns" (Argyle, 1975, cited in Roger & Schumacher, 1983).

". l.,. -. , - -t wo"POINOWN"m

Page 14: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

9

The control of the flow and sequence of conversation has

implications for the relative influence and value of each

participant's input. Mishler and Waxler (1968)

differentiated two techniques of controlling conversation:

attention control and person control. Attention control is

a technique used to influence the course of interaction

while maintaining attentional focus on the speaker him- or

herself. One can influence the sequence and flow of

conversation while keeping the floor by directing statements

to one particular participant, participating frequently, or

by speaking in long, uninterrupted sequences. The second

technique, person control, an attempt to manipulate the

conversation by controlling the other person's behavior.

This is accomplished either by interrupting the person

holding the floor or by questioning. Mishler and Waxle

(1968) categorized person control strategies into direct and

indirect attempts at control: direct attempts control by

"explicit confrontation of another person in an attempt to

stop the speaker before he has finished his idea, to prevent

his being heard, or, more indirectly, to control the

direction and content of his interaction [by questioning]"

(p. 140); indirect attempts to attention control might

consist of a child's throwing a temper tantrum, or speaking

more loudly than others to draw attention. Mishler and

Waxler's hypothesized that more functional behavior is

Page 15: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

characterized by direct rather than indirect attempts at

influence, whether by person or attention strategies.

In dyads in which a dominance hierarchy has already

been established according to cultural or occupational

status, role differentiation of dominance behaviors is

readily seen. Wiens, Thompson, Matarazzo, Matarazzo, and

Saslow (1965) found that the interactional pattern described

above vary with status; those nurses in higher status

interrupted other nurses more, spoke in longer segments and

defended their positions more with physicians than lower

status nurses interacting with the same physicians.

Similarly, studies of interactional patterns of families

with schizophrenic children have assessed dominance by the

number of successful interruptions and the amount of

influential members' talking time. (Jacob, 1975; Riskin,

1972, cited in Ferguson, 1977). Mishler and Waxler (1968)

reported that in comparison to families with schizophrenic

children, "normal" families evidenced more role

differentiation in measures of verbal dominance between

parents and children.

Measures of verbal dominance behaviors have also been

used to determine the types of structure established in

relationships in which roles have not been culturally

defined. Nicola Ferguson (1977) transcribed seven-and-a-

half hours of spontaneous conversation between one

undergraduate and fifteen of her friends. Ferguson

Page 16: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

11

correlated several measures of verbal interruptions with two

measures of dominance within each of the fifteen dyads. The

two measures of dominance included a self-rating personality

questionnaire and the main subject's rating of her friends'

level of dominance. Ferguson concluded that only two

measures of interruptions (overlaps [unsuccessful

interruptions in which the first speaker finishes her

utterance], and silent interruptions [switches of speakers

during silence]) correlated significantly with either

measure of dominance.

Rogers and Jones (1975) studied 18 dyads composed of

one high dominant person and one low dominant person as

measured by Cattell's 16PF and Gough's self-descriptive

adjective checklist. Subjects were given a cooperative

problem-solving task to complete. Results indicated that

high dominant individuals talked more and attempted more

interruptions compared to their low dominant partners. In

addition, high dominant subjects appeared to have more

success at interrupting their partners, although this

comparison fell short of statistical significance. In a

similar study, Roger and Schumacher (1983) measured the

number of successful and unsuccessful interruptions by 18

male and 18 female dyad members when discussing issues on

which they were known to disagree. Subjects were grouped

into three dyads based on their scores on the Dominance

Scale of the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule; thus

Page 17: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

12

high-high, high-low and low-low dominant dyads were formed.

The researchers found that the rate of successful

interruptions increased over time for high-high dyads and

that significantly greater opinion change occurred in the

low-low dyads than the high-high dyads. Unsuccessful

interruption rate was reported to be an un reliable measure.

Aries, Gold and Weigel (1983) studied a trait scale of

dominance's ability to predict verbal and behavioral

measures of dominance in small groups of either the same or

mixed sex. Results indicated that in same sex groups, while

the personality measure accounted for an average of 10% of

the variance in single behavioral measures, 40% was

accounted for using all of the measures. However, in mixed

sex groups, personality-behavior correlations were

significantly reduced.

Fruge (1973) studied 32 males interacting in rotating

dyads to examine differences in self-report measures,

behavioral measures and peer rankings of dominance. His

attempt to predict overall dominance behaviors using

personality measures was unsuccessful; pretest measures of

influence were no better than chance in predicting future

behavior. However, dyadic analysis of data produced much

more discriminative data than monadic analysis. Also, the

subjects' use of indirect control strategies such as

questioning was accompanied by the subjects' mutually low

opinions of influenceability as well as low impressions of

Page 18: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

13

comfort and enjoyment. In particular, dyads in which one

individual exercised a proportionally larger amount of

indirect person control (questioning) reported the most

dissatisfaction. Fruge argued that questioning was an

attempt to influence the flow of conversation without

accepting responsibility. This corroborates Mishler and

Waxler's (1968) hypothesis that direct attempts to influence

conversation are more functional. As Fruge stated, dyads

who accept responsibility for directly influencing change

produced "well coordinated, spontaneous exchanges" (p. 37).

In mixed sex dyads, men have been observed to be much

more likely to interrupt than women. Zimmerman and West

(1975) reported that in mixed sex dyads, men contributed

100% of the overlaps (attempted interruptions) and 96% of

the interruptions. It appears that males are much more

likely to exert direct person control when interacting with

females than with males. Both Rogers and Jones (1975) and

Roger and Schumacher (1983) studied same sex dyads. Neither

found significant sex differences in interruption attempts

or successes between male or female dyads; Rogers and Jones

found no difference in talking time. However, Roger &

Schumacher found that females gave significantly more back

channel utterances, brief interjections such as "uh-huh"

that convey agreement and encouragement to continue

speaking. It appears that males are much more likely to

iM1+'^ ' ' .,.. ., .. ., .. , . zr ,i+t + k fi1sF At k :r., r:...:. ... :::.:

Page 19: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

14

exert direct person control when interacting with females

than with males.

Current literature has predominantly used trait theory

to explain the occurrence of dominance behavior.

Psychologists and lay people alike have relied on trait

theory to explain and predict behavior for years (Allport,

1937). Psychologists' study of personality has been based

upon the existence of enduring traits in human behavior

(Wiggins, 1979, cited in Aries, 1983), dispositions to act

in a certain manner that are consistent, stable and evident

across time (Mangusson & Endler, 1977). Unfortunately, the

reliance on such consistencies has not been demonstrated to

be of empirical value. The highest consistent correlations

between personality measures and subsequent behavior has

been .30 (Mischel, 1968). Mischel (1968) has been an

outspoken proponent of the use of situational variables,

rather than personological variables to predict and explain

behavior. In Personality and Assessment, 1968, Mischel

argued that the concept of traits has been proved

"untenable," that behavior is highly situationally specific,

and that social learning theory could be a better approach

to personality. Epstein and O'Brien (1985) retorted that

while individual item measures of behavior have little

reliability regarding trait measures, aggregating

observations over many occasions will result in a more

accurate measure. Epstein and O'Brien (1985) state that "the

Page 20: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

15

major value of traits lie, however, not in their usefulness

in predicting specific behavior, but in their value as

predictors of aggregated behaviors, that is, of behavior in

the long haul averaged over many situations, occasions and

responses" (p. 532). Thus researchers have attempted to

measure a variety of behavioral traits across varying

temporal and spatial settings.

Mischel (1973) posited that instead of viewing

behavioral variablity as evidence of human inconsistency and

unreliability, it could be considered a discriminative

ability necessary for coping. Variable behavior results

from people's sensitivity to situational cues and their

ability to adapt functionally to fluctuating demands and

contingencies. In fact, behavior that is extremely "trait,"

that is, not varying in response to situational variables,

may be maladaptive. Mischel (1973) further states that

"indiscriminate responding (i.e., 'consistent' behavior

across situations) tends to be displayed more by

maladaptive, severely disturbed, or less mature persons than

by well functioning ones" (p. 258). Pathology can also

exist in the other extreme; persons who have few consistent

ways of behaving or who cannot meaningfully relate

situations to ways of behaving may act chaotically. This

will be described in further detail later.

Constructivist theory was used in this study to predict

dominance structure in dyads. Kelly (1955) first proposed

Page 21: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

16

his view of humans as scientists, hypothesizing meanings in

the world and then validating, adapting and

reconceptualizing these constructs. One metaphor employed

within constructivist theory is the view of humanity

existing within a "community of selves." Contrary to

Western culture which values individuality and individual

consciousness, humans may be understood by considering the

self as "an organised grouping of sub-selves or tendencies"

(Mair, 1977a, p. 137). Each self has differing goals,

values, potentials and motivations, thus each regulates

behavior differently (Cantor, Markus, Niedenthal, & Nurius,

1986). The self that is most influential in each particular

situation determines the attitudes and behavior of the

individual.

Some theorists have attempted to describe the same

constellation of selves for each person. Breckler and

Greenwald (1986) listed four motivational facets of the

self:, the Diffuse self interested in hedonistic

gratification, the Public self, the Private self, and the

Collective self. Situational conditions determine which

self engages; the Diffuse self may become active under the

influence of drugs, the Public self when powerful others are

present, the Private self when alone, and the Collective

self when reference group issues are important. However, it

is a tenet of this research that the community of selves

varies substantially from person to person, according to

Page 22: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

17

each one's idiographic constellation (Gergen, 1971; Cantor

et al., 1986). Mair (1977b) gave the example of Peter who

determined that among his characters of selves existed the

"Conversationalist" who loved interacting and relaxing with

others with no other goal than enjoyment. Peter's

"Businessman" self was very organized, constructive, and did

not like to waste time. Usually these two selves could

cooperatively alternate in dominance, but occasionally one

or the other would become greedy and "hog the limelight" (p.

135), causing Peter's behavior to become one-sided. Selves

also vary in the extent to which they are salient; some

selves may be applicable in only a few situations while

others are used in the majority of one's interactions.

Selves may represent a single period in one's past or future

(Cantor et. al., 1986). Individuals may also vary in how

they differentiate between selves. Just as one person can

differentiate between two sizes of objects, large or small,

another may distinguish fine gradations between several

sizes.

Scott (1988) proposed that individual variations in

constructing and organizing one's community of selves

influence propensity for constructs which admit new elements

into the system. Scott based some of his thoughts upon

Hinkle (1965), who determined that the range of implications

in changing a construct (the amount of connectedness) and

the superordinance, or personal importance of the construct

Page 23: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

18

to be changed, influenced how likely that construct was to

permit change. Kelly's (1955) original thesis stated that

the purpose of constructs was to predict and understand the

world around us. Awareness of imminent reduction in the

predictive utility of one's constructs (threat) or awareness

of an existing state of lack of predictive implications

within the system (anxiety) are noxious states, thus are

generally avoided. Logically, if construct change leads to

either threat or anxiety, such change is unlikely to occur.

Scott (1988) expanded and modulated Hinkle's hypothesis to

determine four circumstances that predict the likelihood for

selves to accommodate new behavior based upon this

principle.

Kelly's Modulation Corollary (1955) defines

permeability as a construct's capacity to assimilate new'

elements. Taking in new information may or may not alter a

construct (Kelly, 1955). Moreover, if two constructs

predict incompatible events, the constructs are considered

inferentially incompatible (Fragmentation Corollary). When

this occurs, the degree of incompatiblity of the event with

the pre-existing construct determines how much the construct

will have to change maintain the new element. If a

construct is applied in a rigid, inflexible manner, it will

more likely be threatened by incompatible external events.

Changes that reduce the predictive utility of a construct,

particularly those that permit little flexibility, produce

Page 24: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

19

threat or anxiety, so that further changes will be less

likely to occur.

In addition to the measure described above, Scott

(1988) also proposed that "the more superordinate a self is

and the greater the number of selves with which it is

functionally dependent, the greater implications of any

change" (p. 11). That is, for a superordinate construct to

change, it would have to influence many of its dependent

constructs. Similarly, the more connected (functionally

dependent) the construct, the greater the number of

dependent constructs must also change. Just as

inferentially incompatible constructs increase threat and

anxiety because they cannot serve to predict consequences,

superordinate and connected constructs resist change- as

well.

The last characteristic proposed is in relation to

propensity for change is predominance. It is hypothesized

that a self's range of convenience will more likely be

explored and delineated if that self is frequently utilized.

When an opportunity for change arises, predominant selves

are more likely to undertake the new role because their more

familiar ranges of convenience have already been extended to

include role changes. In summary, the four construct

measures hypothesized to predict propensity for change as

modified from Scott, 1988, are constriction,

Page 25: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

20

superordinateness, number of functionally dependent selves,

and predominance.

In a similar vein, Kelly (1955) proposed that to an

extent, use of propositional constructs, those that are

relatively loose and unrelated to other dimensions, is more

adaptive than using rigidly related constellatory

constructs. Bieri (1955, cited in Landfield & Barr, 1976)

first coined the term "cognitive complexity" to refer to the

use of many unrelated, independent constructs. A person

displaying cognitive complexity would have a choice of ways

to conceptualize information, thus making sense of a greater

variety of conflicting, confusing events. A person with

only few available uncomplicated constructs could merely

deal effectively with situations congruent with his or her

existing system. Anything outside that person's limited

range of convenience would be seen as complicated,

mysterious and too different to be understood. On the other

hand, too much complexity without a method for organizing

constructs' interaction would result in a very original and

creative way of seeing things, but in an inability to

effectively process the knowledge. Schizophrenics

demonstrate extreme dysfunction in this manner: they may

have sufficiently complex, varied ways of construing their

experience, but cannot adequately order, integrate or

connect their thoughts within the myriad of their

unorganized, separate constructs. These individuals lack

Page 26: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

21

what Kelly (1955) described in his Organization and

Fragmentation Corollaries as ordinal relationships and

hierarchical organization. Assigning superordinate and

subordinate status to constructs produces essential,

functional orderliness and organization within the system.

Thus an individual with adequately complicated and diverse

constructs may be unable to use the system practically

because his or her constructs are not interrelated. Another

individual may not have such a range of complexity, but

because of adequate hierarchical organization, is able to

effectively use his or her available constructs.

Landfield and Barr (1976) define hierarchical

organization; or ordination, as the ability to differentiate

and meaningfully use gradients within a construct; that is,

when evaluating acquaintances, a person has the ability to

finely distinguish high and low measures of the descriptor.

The ability to discriminate levels of meaningfulness within

a construct is one task necessary to structure, compare and

meaningfully order, that is, to hierarchically organize

constructs. An individual may have many complex constructs,

but to be unable to make discriminations within the system.

Landfield and Barr (1976) categorized subjects into

four quadrants based upon their cognitive complexity, or

number of Functionally Independent.Constructs (FIC), and

their ordination (0). Subjects classified into Quadrant 1

had low integration and low differentiation, thus would have

Page 27: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

22

few ways of understanding their worlds and would have

restricted levels and ranges of meaningfulness. These

subjects are thought to be easily understood by others due

to their simplicity, but they can only meaningfully construe

a limited range of information. Subjects classified into

Quadrant 2 have high integration and low differentiation.

They are thought to have limited ways of understanding

others but have more levels and more deeply explored ranges

of meaningfulness. These persons would be easily understood

by others due to their simple constructs, but would be

better able to understand others and could expand their

existing system because of their greater sensitivity to

using their existing constructs. Subjects in Quadrant 3

have low integration and high differentiation, and are

thought to have a variety of types of meaning but limited

levels and ranges of meaningfulness. Because of their

inability to integrate, they would be unable to understand

others or be understood themselves. Schizophrenics and

persons with thought disorders or personal confusion would

be placed in this quadrant. Quadrant 4 consists of subjects

with both high integration and high differentiation, and

would have the greatest construct complexity and the highest

level and range of meaningfulness so they could comprehend

others easily. However, due to their complexity, others may

find them less easy to understand.

Page 28: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

23

Landfield and Barr (1976) supported these hypotheses in

a study of four ten-person groups who interacted for several

sessions. The authors concluded that simplicity and

restriction of construction leads to impairment in

meaningfully understanding those more complicated or diverse

than oneself but concurrently produces little difficulty in

being understood by others. Similarly, those who were

complicated enough to understand others were difficult to

understand themselves. Of particular interest was the

finding that those in Quadrant 3 exhibited a "fragmenting

orientation to people" (p. 25) and even had difficultly

understanding themselves.

Researchers have used constructivist measures of

ordination and complexity of construction to explore and

explain group therapy processing. Landfield and Rivers

(1975) found a decrease in FIC and 0 scores at the end of

twenty-week group sessions. Barr (1977, cited in Landfield,

1978) also reported a gradual lessening construction

complexity as the sixteen-session group progressed. The

level of meaningfulness also decreased at the half-way mark

but increased somewhat by the end of group. Landfield

(1978) concluded that in the initial stages of group

therapy, members tend to assume they have a greater

understanding of other members than is accurate. As

continued interaction invalidates those assumptions,

constructs begin to have less meaning; a reconsolidation

Page 29: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

24

process may eventually occur leading to increased

meaningfulness of interactions with others.

Viewed in a context of the first of several group

meetings, this study may provide insight into the process of

gaining an initial useful understanding of others and

negotiating for positions of dominance that occur in group

therapy. The use of rotating dyads, such as employed here,

is commonly used in the initial stages of long-term group

process or as an introduction to topics later discussed more

completely (Landfield, 1978). Rotating dyads has been found

to help members become more familiar and to increase

interaction levels in later large meetings (Neimeyer, 1978,

cited in Landfield, 1978; Fairbairn, 1978, cited in

Landfield, 1978). Additionally, information about how

easily group members cope with change suggested by other

members, how they assimilate new constructs encountered in

dissimilar individuals, how they adjust to a previously

existing structure of dominance and make sense out of new,

challenging ways of viewing the world can aid in both

explaining and predicting group process. One may be able to

better predict which members can acclimate to an existing

atmosphere, which members might be able to terminate

prematurely, or which ones may be better suited for a

particular group. In their study of encounter groups,

Lieberman, Yalom and Miles (1973) reported that the group

members' individual needs and conflicts could be used to

Page 30: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

25

predict assimilation into pre-existing groups. For example,

those who have an unmet need for intimacy may not adjust to

a group led by an aloof, impersonal leader. However, while

similarity is necessary for meaningful understanding of

others, a certain degree of uniqueness provides the new

perspective needed for change to occur. Landfield (1975)

suggested that although congruence of content seems to aid

communication and that lack of construct congruency between

therapist and client may lead to premature termination, some

amount of incompatibility in organization facilitates the

emergence of new ideas. It seems that a balancing act with

the degree of similarity is required for proper match of

patient to therapist and/or group.

The purpose of this study was to examine how

constructivist theory accounts for behavioral measures of

the dominance structure established in dyads in comparison

to a replication of recent literature utilizing trait

theory. Six general hypotheses from research on dominance

behavior in dyads, trait and constructivist theory shall be

described with a brief synopsis following.

Hypothesis One: Trait theory assumes that an individual

will behave in a consistent manner throughout most of his or

her interactions, particularly if many behavioral measures

are used instead of one. Accordingly, subjects who score

high or low dominant on a pre-test measure should behave

consistently in most interactions. Although some studies

'dYrb 2& ... ,n l't GS3wcA'. i: ;...,.-,,; «:N,.:n. -. ,. .. _ .5. irn.M..,. .,.

Page 31: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

26

have not used level of questioning as a measure of

dominance, Mishler & Waxier (1968) and Fruge (1973)

classified questioning as a way to influence the flow of

conversation without taking responsibility. If a person is

dissatisfied with the existing dyad structure but is

hesitant to appear influential to others, he or she is

likely use a numerous questions in an attempt to control the

dyad without receiving such an attribution.

Hypothesis One: High dominant individuals were predicted to

display more interruptions, longer time talked and fewer

questions; low dominant individuals were predicted to

display the opposite: fewer interruptions, a shorter time

talked, and more questions.

Hypothesis Two: According to Mischel, individuals who are

extremely "trait" in their behavior, rigidly responding in a

consistent manner without regard to situational cues, are

more likely to be maladaptive. Both extremely high and low

dominant individuals demonstrate inflexible and rigid

behavior, despite circumstantial demands. If these

individuals are indeed maladaptive, they would be classified

in Landfield's system as Quadrant 3. However, the dynamics

underlying this coping inability are not clearly delineated.

Although their dysfunction may be due to an inability to

effectively respond to cues they pick up socially (as would

be the case of Quadrant 3), it may also be due to a basic

inability to screen those cues. The latter case is better

Page 32: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

27

described by individuals classified into Quadrant 1--they

have few ways of understanding the world around them thus

have difficulty understanding anyone or anything outside

their limited range of convenience. Perhaps high dominant

persons are Quadrant 1, unable to understand cues

incompatible with their limited systems and low dominant

persons are Quadrant 3, picking up cues but unable to modify

their behavior because of a lack of understanding.

Hypothesis Two: Individuals with extremely deviant

dominance scores were predicted to evidence more extreme FIC

and Ordination scores than those with average dominance

scores.

Hypothesis Three: In constructivist theory, using questions

rather than interrupting or talking for long stretches was

hypothesized to be more characteristic of those who were

highly resistant to change. Mishler and Waxler (1968) and

Fruge (1973) considered direct attempts to control, whether

person or attention control, to be more interpersonally

adaptive. Rigid individuals are unable to assimilate those

behaviors, even if they are more functional. Individuals

who are resistant to change within their internal structure

may also be resistant to changing external structure, and so

may prefer to maintain the status quo in relationships.

These individuals were hypothesized to use indirect rather

than direct control strategies to communicate their needs.

Similarly, highly flexible individuals would not have an

Page 33: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

28

aversion to accepting responsibility for the status of

structure and would utilize more direct, functional

strategies of control.

Hypothesis Three: Dyads identified as highly resistant to

change were predicted to utilize fewer interruptions, more

questions and shorter speech durations than dyads identified

as permeable to change.

Hypothesis Four: Permeability to change was hypothesized to

be related to the number of functionally independent selves

subjects reported after each dyadic interaction.

Individuals permeable to change would more easily adjust

those selves brought to different dyads. As flexible dyadic

members present different constructs and selves in their

interactions, these members would have less difficulty

bringing selves that were appropriate for each separate

interaction. Similarly, individuals resistant to change

would have been more likely to present the same selves

regardless of their partner.

Hypothesis Four: Individuals classified as resistant to

change were expected to bring fewer independent selves to

interactions in comparison to those more permeable to

change.

Hypothesis Five: According to Leary's (1957) theory,

interpersonal demands or expectations placed on dyadic

partners influence behavior. Behavior in one category such

as dominance naturally "pulls" for complementary behavior

Page 34: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

29

such as submission. When the behavior of others does not

conform to what is expected and requested, discomfort,

ineffectiveness and tension result. As Fruge (1973)

reported, interpersonal dissatisfaction, disrespect for

another and discomfort correspond to the use of indirect

control methods. When one is unable for whatever reason to

meaningfully construe an interaction that interaction is

experienced as unpleasant, thus more dysfunctional, indirect

methods of control predominate. In addition, verbal

expressions indicative of tension would also increase.

Occurrences of "Ah's," non-meaningful utterances, and

silence in conversations have been found to reflect tension

and discomfort (Sigmund & Pope, 1968; Doster, 1975).

Because silences were hypothesized to be due to the next

speaker's unability or unwillingness to continue the current

line of thought or to elaborate another line, silences were

attributed to the next speaker following the pause.

Hypothesis Five: Dyads describing themselves as

uncomfortable were predicted to exhibit greater rates of

"ah's," silences, and questions, fewer interruption rates

and shorter speech durations than dyads with comfortable

self-descriptions.

Hypothesis Six: The same experience of anxiety, discomfort,

and inability to directly control were expected to occur

when persons were not able to behave in well-established,

familiar ways. When confronted with another whose thinking

Page 35: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

30

is very alien to our own, according to the Sociality

Corollary (Kelly, 1955) we are unable to meaningfully

construe the behavior or constructs of another person and

find it difficult to bring an appropriate self to that

interaction. Instead of meaningfully understanding the

interpersonal process and therefore behaving in an

appropriate way, we merely respond without understanding.

As discussed regarding the similarity of therapist and

client affecting treatment, some uniqueness may expand one's

current system, but a large amount of dissimilarity hinders

communication. When for some reason one is unable to behave

in familiar, understandable ways, then anxiety, tension and

inability to utilize direct control strategies were

hypothesized to occur.

Hypothesis Six: Self-descriptions considered non-prototypic

were predicted to accompany greater rates of silences,

"ah's," questions, lesser interruption rates, and shorter

speech durations than those evidenced by prototypic selves.

Page 36: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

CHAPTER II

METHOD

Subjects

Forty-one subjects were recruited from an introductory

psychology course in return for extra credit. Informed

consent was obtained from all subjects (see Appendix A)

before testing began.

Procedure

In the first testing session, subjects were pre-tested

as a group using the Dominance Scale of the California

Psychological Inventory and the Community of Selves Repgrid.

The Dominance Scale of the California Psychological

Inventory consists of 44 empirically derived true/false

items. Scores were converted to standard scores using

separated norms for sex. A high scorer is described as

confident, self-reliant, independent and displaying

leadership potential. Low scorers are reported to be

unassuming, retiring and indifferent. Gough (1957) reports

test-retest correlations for this scale ranging from .72 to

.80. Several cross-validations studies indicate acceptable

amount of validity. Standard scores ranged from 19 to 68.

The Community of Selves Repgrid (Doster & Watson, 1987)

consists of two sections. First, constructs are elicited by

31

. ...

Page 37: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

32

Landfield's (1971) modification of Kelly's (1955) Role

Construct Repertory Grid (see Appendix B). Secondly, these

constructs are applied to a series of potentially relevant

self-experiences, including the "Usual me" in order to gain

information regarding the self most predominant within the

construct system (see Appendix C).

At the second testing session, subjects participated in

pre-arranged groups of eight to nine persons, arbitrarily

assigned. The last four digits of their social security

numbers identified them. Subjects successively interacted

one-on-one with. other group members for five minutes until

each possible pair had interacted, the pairing having been

determined randomly. All interactions were tape recorded.

After each five minute conversation, subjects rated their

self experiencing for that interaction on their constructs

elicited from the Rep-test. After all pairs had interacted,

group members ranked one another according to perceived

influenceability and the degree to which they felt

comfortable with each partner (see Appendix E).

Peer rankings of dominance, consisting of seven or

eight scores (depending upon the group size) were averaged

for each person. Higher numerical rankings indicate lower

perceived dominance by their peers. Scores ranged from 2.00

to 7.88. Total and differential comfortability were

regarded in a dyadic manner. These measures consisted

respectively of the total or absolute difference of each

Page 38: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

33

participant's ranking of self-comfortability within the dyad

divided by the total group membership, to control for

variance due to unequal sizes. Higher scores indicated that

greater unease was experienced. Total comfortability scores

ranged from .29 to 2.0; differential comfortability ranged

from .00 to .88.

Measures of Resistance to Change

Four factors hypothesized to be related to propensity

to change were derived from the Rep-test. Monadically, each

of the four measures were computed before adding dyadic

information, obtaining an overall measure of the subjects'

propensity for resistance to change. This monadic

resistance to change measure was computed by separately

ranking the scores of all thirty-seven subjects for each of

the four measures. The rankings were then computed to

achieve a total resistance to change score. Scores varied

from 33.60 to 112.50, with lower scores indicating less

resistance to change.

Each of the four indicators had dyadic permutations as

well, consisting of conceptually similar indicators specific

to each interaction, seven or eight per person. These four

factors were ranked separately for all 284 available

incidences; thus each dyad had two separate rankings for

each of its four factors: both total and differential

resistance to change. Total resistance to change was

derived by totalling all eight scores ranging from 264.50 to

Page 39: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

34

1,548.00. Differential resistance to change consisted of

the total of the absolute difference between each of the

four sets; scores ranged from 22.50 to 628.50.

Constriction. A construct or self is thought to be

constricted or rigid when it is rated in extremes, either

positively or negatively. This implies that there is no

maleability or flexibility in that self, but either-or

thinking. Such selves or constructs would be impermeable to

external influence. Specifically, a construct is determined

as constricted monadically either when one rating level was

used eight or more times or when two levels together were

used eleven or more times. Dyadically, each self rating

after interactions was defined as constricted either if a

zero or six rating was used eight or more times or if the

total was eleven or more.

Superordinateness. Landfield and Barr (1976) proposed

that superordinateness was the ability to differentiate

between rating levels used in describing a self. This

superordinateness score was computed by multiplying the

number of different rating levels used to describe each self

by the difference between the lowest and highest rating.

This measure was computed monadically with original rep-test

data and dyadically for each of the seven or eight elicited

selves.

Connectedness. A self is thought to be connected

within a system when using that self affects others. It is

Page 40: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

35

operationalized monadically as the number of functionally

independent selves for the self-rep, and dyadically as the

number of selves functionally dependent upon each of the

seven or eight elicited selves.

Predominance. To tap the frequently used self, a usual

self was provided on the Rep-test. Predominance was defined

monadically as the degree of functional dependence of each

of the seven elicited selves with the usual self, totalled

across all selves. Dyadically the information was not

totalled but maintained separately for each of the elicited

selves.

Dependent Measures

Two independent raters examined a sample of tape

recordings for total time talked, number of interruptions,

and number of questions for each subject. Raters were given

definitions of each measure based upon Aries, Gold & Weigel

(1983) and Fruge (1973) (see Appendix D). All measures met

acceptable standards of interrater reliability except for

interruptions, which was initially .68. Coding

discrepancies were reevaluated by the raters and the rating

criteria solidified. Subsequent interrater reliability of

interruptions met acceptable levels ( r =.92). After coding

standards were thus established, the principle examiner

completed the rest of the coding. Interrater correlations

of observational measures are available in Table 1 below.

Page 41: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

36

Table 1

Interrater reliabilities of Observational Measures

Variables Correlation

Speech .92

Interruptions .92

Questions .92

Ahs .81

Silences .83

Because dyads varied in conversational time allowed,

observational measures were converted to obtain more

proportionally representative scores (Fruge, 1973). In

addition, to utilize both numerator and denominator in the

same scale, all time-related measures such as speech

duration, total and differential talk time, plus both

monadic and dyadic silence rates were all converted to

minutes. For monadic analysis, speech duration was computed

by dividing time talked by the total interactional time of

the dyad, including pauses; thus a score of .50 would

reflect that the subject talked for 50% of available

interaction time. A Pearson-moment correlation conducted

between the actual speech time of a dyad and the total

interactional time was .99, indicating that the proportional

measure of speech duration was not inflated due to excessive

Page 42: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

37

silences. Silence rate was computed by dividing the

combined length of silences by the total interactional time

of the dyad. Interruption rate, question rate, and "ah"

rate consisted of the incidence of each variable divided by

the total available time of that dyad.

Measures for dyadic analyses were computed separately.

Total and differential talk time were computed respectively

by totalling and subtracting the speech duration of both

participants, excluding silences, and dividing the results

by the total interaction time. Total and differential

interruption, question, "ah," and silence rates were

similarly obtained by dividing the total or the absolute

difference of each occurrence by the total interaction time.

Combined length of silences was used for dyadic silence

rates.

Page 43: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

CHAPTER III

RESULTS

The means and standard deviations of the monadic self

report and observational measures are listed in Table 2;

dyadic means and standard deviations are listed in Table 3.

Table 2

Means and Standard Deviations of Independent and

Dependent Variables: Monadic

Males Females

(30) (11)

M SD M SD

Dominance 44.81 11.17 47.91 10.01

Speech Duration .50 .08 .47 .09

Interruption Rate .65 .53 .94 .60

Question Rate 1.39 1.44 1.39 1.10

Ah Rate 1.01 .48 .89 .50

Silence Rate .02 .01 .03 .03

Resis. To Change 64.31 14.24 65.99 19.76

Peer Rankings 3.91 1.18 4.71 1.29

of Dominance

38

. w. _:. :.. , --

Page 44: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

39

Table 3

Means and Standard Deviations for Dependent Variables: Dyadic

M/M M/F F/F

(77) (55) (10)

Variables M SD M SD M SD

Comfort

Total

Differential

Speech Duration

Total

Differential

Question Rate

Total

Differential

Interruption Rate

Total

Differential

Ah Rate

Total

Differential

Silence Rate

Total

Differential

.17

.:29

.41

.21

.98 .03

.23 .15

.:L4

.51

.99

.59

1.13

.29

.99

.22

1.43

.70

.51 .49

.24 .30

.04

.45

.02

.64

.42

.28

.01

.16

1.05

.79

.76 .56

.31 .26

.90 .65

.36 .34

.01 .01

.47 .55

.65

.36

.03

.66

.85 .40

.28 .25

.99 .02

.24 .16

1.26 .94

.79 .88

.89 .64

.31 .37

.56 .41

.29 .25

.01 .02

.35 .61

Page 45: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

40

Table 4

Correlation Matrix of Verbal Measures and Dominance Scale

Variables'z B C D E F G

A .28* -.33* -.11 .10 .00 -.15

B -. 50*** -. 39** .07 -. 39** .16

C .81***-.29* .08 -.24+

D -. 32* -.01 .02

E -. 09 .10

F -.07

'A = Dominance Score; B = Speech Duration; C = Interruption

Rate; D = Question Rate; E = Ah Rate; F = Silence Rate;

G = Sex.

+ 2 < .10; * 2 < .05; ** 2 < .01; *** P < .001.

The first hypothesis proposes that persons higher on

the dominance scale would evidence higher levels of

interruptions, longer speech duration, and fewer questions.

Pearson product moment correlations were performed upon

dominance scores, sex, and verbal behaviors based upon

subjects' interactions. The dominance scale correlated

significantly with speech duration (r (36) = .28, 2 <.05)

and interruption rate (r (36) = -.33, 2 <.05). More

dominant individuals spoke longer and were interrupted less.

Sex was not significantly correlated with dominance or any

-Now.

Page 46: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

41

of the dependent measures, although there was a tendency for

females to have a higher rate of interruptions (r (41) =

-.25, <.10). Pearson correlation coefficients for these

measures are available in Table 4. These results gave

conflicting support for the hypothesis.

Additionally, peer rankings of influenceability were

used to predict the same characteristic verbalizations.

Other Pearson product-moment correlations were computed

between rankings of influentiability and the dependent

variable rates. These correlations are available in Table

5. Speech duration was negatively correlated (r (41) =

-.47, 2<.001), silence rate positively correlated (r (41) =

.50, <Q.001), and "ah" rate negatively correlated (r (41)

=-.36,2 < .01) with peer rankings of dominance.

Individuals characterized by their peers as being more

influential talked for relatively longer periods of time,

had fewer silences, but more "ah's." Sex was not

significantly related to peer rankings of dominance although

there was a tendency for females to be ranked as more highly

dominant. This provides greater support for the hypothesis,

although again, the sex correlate was not predicted.

Lastly, to compare peer opinion with self-rating, a

Spearman's rank order correlation was computed between

ranking according to the dominance scale and ranking of

influentiability by subjects. These two measures were

highly positively correlated (r (36) = .45, <.01); those

n 't k uS JSdi x ki*m ijii;+.; .. :.i n.:n., ;,r:.. .. : - .. ,::.. : z .:; l, c.: ;u

Page 47: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

42

considered dominant by their peers usually experienced

themselves as dominant on the trait scale.

Table 5

Correlations Between Peer Ranking of Dominance and Verbal

Measures

Variables' B C D E F G

A -.47*** .19 -.13 -.36** .50*** -.16

B -.50*** -. 39** .06 -.39** .14

C .81*** -.29* .08 -.21+

D -.33* -.01 .01

E -. 09 .11

F -. 08

'A = Average Peer Rankings of Dominance; B = Speech

Duration; C = Interruption Rate; D = Question Rate; E = Ah

Rate; F = Silence Rate; G = Sex.

* 2 < .05; ** < .01; *** 2 < .001.

It was next hypothesized that extreme trait dominance

scores, whether very high or low, would indicate pathology

as evidenced by extreme FIC and/or Ordination scores

(Landfield & Barr, 1976). A forward multiple regression

analysis was conducted to determine the ability of FIC,

Ordination scores, and sex to predict deviation of

dominance. The absolute value of the difference between

. ,_ :.::.,.. ..,.ter,,,.,. , , ;

Page 48: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

43

dominance scores and the overall mean of the sample was used

as the dominance deviation score. FIC significantly

predicted dominance deviation ( F (1, 58)=4.07, 2 < .05, R

2=.06). Individuals who reported dominance levels deviating

significantly from the mean, whether high or low, evidenced

lower FIC scores. Ordination did not reach minimal

tolerance levels and thus was not entered. This partially

supported the hypothesis.

Table 6

Regression Results for Deviation of Dominance and FIC

Predictor Value Beta Value R2

FIC -.25* .06

Ordination -- --

* 2 < .05.

Individuals highly resistant to change were expected to

favor questioning over interrupting or holding the floor to

direct conversation flow. Separate multiple regressions

analyses were run between resistance to change and sex for

each of the dependent variable rates. This analysis was

done at the dyadic level, so that the total and differential

resistance to change scores between each couple were treated

as a case, rather than as individuals. The cross product of

the total and difference scores was used as a predictor to

Page 49: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

44

determine if there was a significant interaction between the

two separate factors. The criterion variable rates were

also computed as total and difference rates within each

interaction. Gender was partialled into male/male,

male/female and female/female dyads. There was a

significant difference in ratio: 77 male/male dyads; 55

female/male dyads; and 10 female/female dyads. Dummy coding

of sex was used to separate dyads so that when one sex was

considered as a predictor variable, the other two sex groups

were combined for comparison. For example, the criterion

variables were predicted by male/male dyads in comparison to

combined male/female and female/female groups. The

regression analyses were done in a forced entry manner so

that Total Resistance to change, the Differential Resistance

to change and the cross-product were entered sequentially.

Results are contained in Tables 7 through 16 and are

available in Appendix G. All tables report R 2 as the

increment of explained variance for each unique variable,

and the significance level of Beta values refer to the

significance of the added predictability of that variable,

not of the overall model. Correlations between predictor

variables and criterion variables are available in Table 17.

Total resistance to change was a significant predictor

of total interruption rate (F (1,113) = 8.39, 2 <.005, R 2 =

.07), total question rate (F (1, 113) = 20.45, <.001, R 2

= .15), and the difference in question rate (F (1,113) =

Page 50: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

45

6.08, p <.05, R 2 = .05). Dyads evidencing high total

resistance to change were associated with lower overall

interruption rates, and with both members asking few

questions. The differential resistance to change and the

product between total and differential resistance to change

did not contribute substantial increments in predictability.

Overall it seems that interruption and question rate were

valid predictors, accounting for a combined 27% of the total

of the total variance. These results partially support the

hypothesis.

Sex was also a significant predictor of control

methods. Differential resistance to change and the cross-

product resistance to change caused suppression of the

explanatory model for each criterion variable, so the

analyses were re-examined without these two variables. Beta

values and R2 reported in Appendix G refer to analyses

performed without these two factors entered into the model.

Male/male dyads were characterized by lower total rates of

silences (F (2,112) = 6.47, p <.001, R 2 = .10), higher

total rates of interrupting (F (2,112) = 7.58, p <.05, R 2 =

.05) and longer overall speech duration (F (2,112) = 6.67, p

<.001, R 2 = .10). Overall, 25% of the variance was

predicted using these variables. Female/female and mixed

sex dyads were not predicted by any of the variables.

t auf a : r. t¬; - " . , - .. _.... -si iW+Meiti^,' . - i :.::.,-..YZ .:,cc,..,: cAGR g.

,..;,v: _

Page 51: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

46

Table 17

Correlations Between Resistance to Change, Sex and

Criterion Variables

Variables 1 A B C D E

Total Talk Rate .01 .04 .01 .32 -.27

Differential Talk Rate .10 .03 .05 .03 -.02

Total Interruption Rate -.26 -.07 -.17 .26 -.19

Differential Interruption -.16 .11 .04 .13 -.12

Rate

Total Question Rate -.39 -.06 -.20 .15 -.15

Differential Question Rate -.23 -.08 -.15 .10 -.06

Total Ah Rate .05 .10 .14 -.16 .05

Differential Ah Rate .03 .09 .11 -.14 .09

Total Silence Rate -.01 -.04 -.01 -.32 .27

Differential Silence Rate -.09 -.06 -.05 -.16 .11

'A = Total Resistance to Change; B = Differential Resistance

to Change; C = Cross-product Resistance to Change;

D = Male/male dyads; E = Male/female dyads.

Individuals identified as more resistant to change were

predicted to display fewer independent selves across

interactions. A Pearson-moment correlation was conducted

between the monadic resistance to change score, the ratio of

functionally independent selves brought to the interactions,

_ it82k illwinFa"+i 'F t -... - .-. +,zx:ar +. :-,r:- , . ._ ... ... ,. :. tia.+ s::,i ., .. rn ::.,w;.

Page 52: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

47

and the dominance scores. The number of functionally

independent selves was divided by the number of group

members to control for variance in the number of different

selves assessed. Resistance to change was not significantly

correlated to the occurrence of independent selves but was

positively correlated to dominance (r (34) = .35, p <.05).

These results are available in Table 18. Individuals

identified as highly resistant to change appeared more

dominant on the trait measure but did not vary significantly

in the proportion of independent selves evidenced. This

does not support the hypothesis except by inference; highly

"trait" individuals were considered rigid according to the

constructivist measure.

Table 18

Correlations Between Resistance to Change, Dominance Scores

and Proportion of Independent Selves Brought to Interactions

Variables' B C

A -.19 .35*

B -. 18

'A = Resistance to Change; B = Ratio of Functionally

Independent Selves; C = Dominance.

* 2 <.05.

Comfortability of a dyad was hypothesized to be

evidenced verbally by "ah's" and silences. A multiple

Page 53: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

48

regression was computed for how ably each of the verbal

measures predicted the reported comfort of the dyad. These

analyses were computed in a dyadic manner; the total and

difference levels of comfort within each interaction were

compared to the total and differential rates of speech

duration, questioning, interruptions, silences and "ah's."

This model was significant in predicting total

comfortability using the total interruption rate (R 2 = .07,

F (1,139) = 10.66, 2 <.01) and the difference in question

rate (R 2 = .10, F (2,138) = 7.69, 2 <.001). Only these two

factors correlated highly enough to be entered into the

equation. Table 19 contains a listing of these results;

Table 20 contains correlations between total comfort,

differential comfort, the cross-product comfort and the

predictor variables. The factors correlated such that

higher total comfort was associated with fewer total

interruptions and mutual levels of questions. Differential

and cross-product comfort were not significantly predicted

by any of the variables. Although silences and " ah' s " did

not vary consistently, there appears to be some influence

upon direct control strategies by combined comfort.

. .'R;:YYf e V179V vA - iUir7r ".:: «4}iK? ifiblNt. i: ._: ._: .:.:.. .. t .. : :r::;r ;.w:r. -. ?b7Yx' __ -

Page 54: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

49

Table 19

Regression Results for Total Comfort

Predictor Variables Beta Values R2

Total Interruption Rate -.27** .07

Differential Question Rate .18* .03

* 2 < .05; ** 2 < .01.

Interpersonal incidences of anxiety, discomfort and

inability to exercise direct control strategies were

hypothesized to accompany nonprototypic, or nonfamiliar,

self-experiencing. Ratings of self characterizations after

each conversation were separated into those that were

functionally dependent to selves elicited previously and

those that were unique. A one-way MANOVA was computed

between these two levels of selves with the five verbal

measures and self-reported comfort after each interaction.

The overall main effect was significant (F (6,236) = 3.34, ?

<.01) and subsequent univariate analysis indicated that

question rate (F (1,241) = 11.23, 2 <.001) and interruption

rate, (F (1,241) = 4.68, 2 <.05) were significantly higher

in nonprototypic selves. The hypothesis was questionably

supported; while higher question rate was supportive, the

higher interruption rate was incongruent, and additional

Page 55: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

50

indices of higher total rate of silences and " ah' s " were

missing.

Table 20

Correlations Between Comfort and Predictor

Variables

Variables1 A B C

Total Talk Rate .09 .06 .06

Differential Talk Rate .03 -.04 -.05

Total Interruption Rate -.27 -.04 -.10

Differential Interruption - .22 -.02 - .09

Rate

Total Question Rate -.02 .06 .04

Differential Question Rate .07 .08 .07

Total Ah Rate -.01 .05 .04

Differential Ah Rate -.00 .07 .05

Total Silence Rate -. 10 -.06 -.06

Differential Silence Rate .05 .01 .04

1 A = Total Comfort; B = Differential

product Comfort.

Comfort; C = Cross-

Page 56: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

CHAPTER IV

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to assess how useful

constructivist techniques proved in predicting verbal

indicators of dominance in briefly interacting dyads. Six

hypotheses were proposed and tested. The first hypothesis,

of correlating scores on a trait measure of dominance with

verbal indicators based on subjects' interactions, -elicited

conflicting support. Higher trait-dominant subjects

evidenced longer speech durations and lower interruption

rates while interpersonally ranked dominant subjects spoke

for longer periods of time and evidenced fewer silences and

more "ah's." Interpersonal measures of dominance

demonstrated a more consistent behavioral characterization

than did the trait predictor.

Secondly, subjects who appeared highly consistent or

rigid in a trait manner were hypothesized to be less well

adjusted in constructivist terms. A multiple regression

analysis was computed using Ordination, FIC, and sex of

subjects to predict more extreme trait indicators of

dominance. This analysis was partially supported. Extreme

trait dominance was significantly associated with lower FIC

scores, corroborating Mischel, 1973. These individuals

51

- OW-klk*W,

Page 57: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

52

would be classified in Quadrants 1 or 2 of Landfield and

Barr's (1976) quadrant system, depending upon their

ordination scores. Such individuals are described as being

limited in conceptualizing the world and uncomfortable with

situations outside of their viewpoint. Such persons could

easily be considered rigid interpersonally, constantly

behaving either deferentially or autocratically despite

mediating situational variables. Conceptually similar is

Phillips' (1981) study which reported that people

hospitalized with schizophrenia who showed extreme FIC

scores, either high or low, evidenced more extreme thought

disorders. This current study found maladjustment at one

end of this continuum. Perhaps difficulty of the opposite

kind, instability and erratic behavior, would be

characteristic of normals at the opposite extreme. Phillips

(1981) did not utilize Ordination as a predictive variable.

Individuals who were highly resistant to change were

thought more likely to use indirect methods of influencing

their verbal interactions. Several multiple regression

analyses were conducted using resistance to change to

predict verbal measures of subjects' interactions which

supported the hypothesis. Dyads similarly higher in

combined resistance to change were found to have lower rates

of interruptions and mutually low levels of questioning.

Resistance to change was successful in predicting reluctance

to utilize not only direct interventions, but indirect

Page 58: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

53

methods as well. It would be useful to discover how dyads

resistant to change expressed their conversational

preferences, if at all. Sex was even a more significant

predictor of control strategies. Male/male dyads had fewer

silences, more interruptions and longer speech duration for

both participants, the prototype of assertive, direct verbal

interaction. Sex of dyads was more predictive of verbally

dominant behavior than either trait or constructivist

theories.

Individuals more resistant to change were expected to

display fewer independent selves across their series of

interactions. A Pearson Product-moment correlation was

computed between these two factors, without resulting

significance. It may prove less difficult to elicit

autonomous selves in future tests if conversations were

allocated more time. However, those subjects more resistant

to change were associated with higher dominance levels,

giving added support to the second hypothesis: highly

"trait" individuals may be interpersonally inflexible.

Dyads reportedly less comfortable with one another were

suggested to evidence more indirect methods of control as

well as verbal indices of discomfort. A multiple regression

analysis was conducted using the verbal measures to predict

comfort. Dyads with higher combined comfort had fewer

interruptions and mutual levels of questions, partially

corroborating this hypothesis. This replicates Fruge's

Page 59: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

54

(1973) finding that dyadic dissatisfaction rises when one

person asks a disproportionate number of questions. These

dyads seem to be avoiding direct interventions but utilizing

indirect methods in proportion to their partners, passively

following their lead. It seems unusual that "ah's" did not

rise with comfort. Perhaps this is due to the experience of

discomfort as one of tolerable incongruence or unease with

the other person's overall perceptions, rather than an

agitated, "state" experience of emotional unease.

Lastly, interactions in which participants were unable

to utilize a familiar role were expected to evidence

indirect control strategies. Results of the MANOVA were

that nonprototypic selves were associated with higher

questioning and interrupting, providing some degree of

endorsement. It seems apparent that nonprototypic

experiences were not merely confusing or anxiety provoking,

but that they motivated participants to become more involved

in their processing. This seems somewhat similar to what

uncomfortable dyads were evidencing. While these

experiences do not produce typical anxiety, they seem to be

discomforting and activating at a behavioral level.

One of the assumptions in this study was that direct

and indirect control strategies were identifiable, had

different functions in conversation and would differentiate

the types of individuals who prefered one type of strategy

over the other, based upon Mishler and Waxler, 1968.

Page 60: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

55

However, this study did not support this distinction;

interruptions and questions were primarily used in a similar

manner. Interruptions and questions correlated at a very

high level, r (41) = .81, < .001 (see Tables 4 or 5).

Rarely did interruption rate rise when question rate did not

follow, particularly in dyadic analyses. When dyads were

less resistant to change, uncomfortable or were unable to

utilize familiar ways of behaving, both questioning and

interrupting rose. It may be more useful to collapse direct

and nondirect categories of control strategies into the

existence of any verbal methods at all. When verbal

manipulations are not used, nonverbal messages may become

more influential. Eye contact, tone of voice, or posture

could convey interest level, dissatisfaction or aloofness.

Similarly, analyzing the content of questions and

interruptions could further delineate the intent of such

strategies. Questioning to introduce a new topic of

conversation seems to be qualitatively different from

further delving into a topic one's partner introduced.

Similarly, skillful questioning can perhaps convey

disinterest as deftly as interrupting can. Due to the

insignificance and unreliability of differentiating

successful from unsuccessful interruptions in the literature

(Roger & Schumacher, 1983), this distinction was not

employed. If a more reliable method of separating the two

were available, such a measure might be meaningful in

Page 61: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

56

delineating truly dominant individuals from those who are

merely uncomfortable with their relative influenceability.

The finding that interruptions were negatively

correlated with trait dominance is very difficult to

explain. It is in direct contradiction to past literature.

Trait dominance seems to be measuring a construct

qualitatively different from interpersonal dominance,

although the two were very highly correlated. The

behavioral correlates of interpersonal dominance were more

clearly related to "text book" definitions of dominant

behavior, long speech duration and fewer silences (see Table

5). Trait dominance somewhat resembled resistance to change

(lower interruption rates) which was also linked to

maladaptive rigidity.- As is common in self-report measures,

there is often a discrepancy between self-presentation and

actual behavior. Perhaps trait dominance was tapping into

more inflexible persons' wishes to be characterized as

influential. In addition, there were other factors found to

covary with verbal behaviors, dyadic comfortability and

prototypic experiencing. These variables could have

moderated the appearance of dominance as a trait. It has

also been suggested that self monitoring may mediate the

expression of control strategies (Soldow & Thomas, 1984), so

that the particular social circumstances underlying each

situation would affect whether direct or indirect methods

are employed.

Page 62: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

57

The high correlation ( r (36) = .45, p < .01) between

interpersonally ranked dominance and trait dominance has

implications for measurement of personality variables. It

seems evident that whatever process individuals use to

screen and interpret behavior is a fairly accurate one.

Since there was an indirect correlation between those

rankings and pathological rigidity, future research might

elaborate more directly the ability of laypeople to detect

dysfunctional behavior in others. Distinguishing the exact

mechanisms that persons use in impression formation might be

used to improve existing trait measures.

Resistance to change seems to have made a fairly good

showing in this study. Except for its inability to predict

the number of independent selves actually observed, it

behaved as expected with regard to verbal control methods.

This variable may prove useful in psychotherapy.

Individuals who exhibit few questions and/or interruptions,

who avoid initiating change in the status quo, electing to

mirror the amount of control mechanisms their peers use, may

be considered resistant to change. Such clients may be

anticipated to be resistant to therapeutic intervention, so

that less offensive, incongruent strategies may be chosen to

gain initial access to inflexible schemas. In a group

psychotherapy setting, this construct may be used to

identify potential peer leaders that would be active during

Stage II rebellion (Yalom, 1970), or those who would be

Page 63: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

58

unable to assimilate into an existing group structure,

especially those considerably foreign to the group's

perceived socio-economic status, marital status or gender,

as well as more intrapsychic differences. There are several

cautions, however. Sex findings indicate that such a

construct may have more meaning for male/male dyads. Women

appear to be disadvantaged in this respect, appearing more

rigid and inflexible perhaps due to sociological rather than

personological causes. Similarly, these verbal indicators

also may rise temporarily due to uncomfortable or unfamiliar

settings. Such situations might temporarily increase

control mechanisms due to internal emotional states rather

than permeability to change.

Discouraging to the utility of constructivist or trait

theory was the superior utility of dyadic sex in predicting

verbal behaviors. Male/male dyads more fully characterized

dominant interactions than any monadic analyses or dyadic

contributions of resistance to change. Female/female dyads

or mixed sex dyads were not as easily predictable, as Aries

et al., 1983, reported. The factors contributing to the

emergence of functional, direct conversation in males has

usually been attributed to socialization (Zimmerman & West,

1975). It would be interesting to consider if this type of

verbal asset could explain instances of "networking" that

help men climb the business ladder. Not only may male

subordinates have common backgrounds with their superiors,

Wi t,.-ti6'e h:5w. aSka.-, ,,i.n.,,. ....-. ,. i, ix _.,;. -v: i!m"n

Page 64: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

59

but they may also be able to communicate in a more direct,

functional manner than male bosses do with their female

subordinates.

It is recommended that further research consider

nonverbal indicators of dominance as well as verbal ones.

Although it may be more tedious and logistically difficult

to obtain and code behavioral measures, there is a wealth of

information overlooked at that level. In particular, which

person is addressed more frequently could be a meaningful

indicator of perceived interpersonal dominance. In

addition, further personality measures such as self-

monitoring may further elucidate sex differences and other

inconsistencies.

Page 65: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX A

INFORMED CONSENT

60

Page 66: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

61

Name of Subject:

Principle Investigator: Caroline Curlin

The purpose of this study is to obtain information

regarding problem-solving and individual differences.

Possible benefits include positive interpersonal experiences

with group members and a further knowledge of the experience

of topics of psychology.

I understand that I will be asked to fill out a number of

personal questionnaires related to topics of psychology

discussed in class. Group discussions will be led by

graduate students in the Psychology department, and each of

the sessions will last approximately fifty minutes. The

first meeting will differ from the other four sessions in-

that in order to facilitate getting acquainted, each member

will talk one-on-one with every other group member for ten

minutes. Group meetings shall be tape recorded or video-

taped and the tapes will be erased after the necessary data

has been collected.

The results of this study will hopefully be published in

a psychological journal, but the identity and information

about individual participants will not be revealed.

I have heard a clear explanation and understand the

nature of this procedure. I understand that there are few

if any potential risks involved. I have heard a clear

Page 67: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

62

explanation and understand the benefits to be expected. If

I have any questions about this study, the investigator or

the instructor will be glad to answer them for me. I

understand that I am free to withdraw my consent for the

utilization of my data at any time without any negative

consequences.

I have read the above and understand it and hereby

consent to the procedures described above.

Date

Subject

Page 68: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX B

INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE ROLE CONSTRUCT TEST

63

:x'k ?" A+X 75'.:1uctGrA+.F '.; ti F<nd i . ... .. :: ,.:.. .,w"w .. ,. "y",,t, ..+w'.,..' a

Page 69: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

64

First Step:

Find the slanted lines in the upper left-and corner of the

RESPONSE SHEET.

1. Write the first name of your mother or the person who

has played the part of .your mother where it says mother.

2. Write the first name of your father or the person who

has played the part of your father where it says father.

3. Write the name of your brother nearest your own age,

or the person who has played the part of such a

a brother.

4. Write the name of your sister nearest your own age,

or the person who has played the part of such a

sister.

5. The happiest person you know personally.

6. The teacher whose point of view you have found most

acceptable.

7. Your sister nearest your own age, or the person who

has played the part of such a sister.

8. Your closest present friend of the same sex.

9. The unhappiest person you know personally.

10. The person you have met whom you would most like to

know better.

11. The most unsuccessful person you know personally.

12. The person with whom you usually feel most uncomfortable.

13. Your father or the person who has played the part

of your father.

Page 70: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

65

14. The most successful person you know personally.

15. Your husband (wife) or closest present boyfriend

(girlfriend).

16. Re-enter the name of the person list in #1.

Below your list of names, find row A. Notice that row

A has two highlighted squares. Think carefully about

yourself and about these two people. As you think about the

three of you, try to find one way in which two of you are

alike and different from the other.

Write the way in which two of these three people are

alike in the space provided at the right under Similarity.

Write the way in which one of these three people is

different from the two who are alike in the space provided

at the right under Different. If you cannot - see a

similarity or a difference among the three people leave

blanks.

After you finish row A, complete row B, row C, etc.

Follow the same instructions given above.

Page 71: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX C

INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE COMMUNITY OF SELVES REP-TEST

66

Page 72: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

67

Turn the answer sheet over to the reverse side. Notice

the dotted line that extends across the top of the page.

Fold the top of the page along the dotted line. When you

turn the answer sheet back to the front page, a new list of

titles should cover the names of your acquaintances.

The new titles are a list of the various ways you may

have of experiencing yourself. At times you may find

yourself to be "vulnerable" person, at times you may find

yourself to be a "self-critical" person, and so on.

The descriptions you have written under comparisons can

now used as rating scales to describe the various ways you

do experience yourself. Notice that between your

descriptions is a rating scale. You can use the rating

scale to describe moments in which you experience yourself

as silly and moments in which you experience yourself as

serious, for example.

Start with column 1 -the vulnerable me. Try to recall

times in your life when you have experienced yourself as a

vulnerable person. Can you get a picture in your mind of

some of those times when you felt vulnerable? Sometimes,

closing your eyes and making a mental picture can be

helpful. Now try to describe what you are like when you are

a vulnerable person. using each of the 15 rating scales

under comparisons, describe yourself as a vulnerable person.

Ratings should appear in each square under column 1.

Page 73: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

68

Use a zero (0) rating when neither description fits

your experience of yourself or when you are unsure about

your experience.

Continue on to Column 2 -the spiritual me. Try to

recall times in your life when you have experienced yourself

as a spiritual person. Can you get a picture in your mind

of some of those times when you experiences yourself as a

spiritual person? Again, closing you eyes and making a

mental image can be helpful. Now try to describe what you

are like when you are a spiritual person. Use each of your

15 rating scales under comparisons to describe yourself as a

spiritual person. Ratings should appear in each square

under column 2.

Continue on to each of the other columns. Try to

describe the various ways you may have of experiencing

yourself.

Column sixteen has been left blank for you to fill in

you "usual me", or your predominant self-experience.

Page 74: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX D

INSTRUCTIONS FOR SCORING VERBAL MEASURES

69

Page 75: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

70

Time Talking

The number of seconds a subject actually engaged in

talking during the dyadic discussion including overlaps with

other speakers but excluding time spent laughing and time

associated with single-word affirmations of statements.

Silences longer than two seconds were excluded.

Interruptions

The number of separate instances in which a subject

began to speak while another member of the group was still

speaking, excluding single-word affirmations or negations.

Questions

The number of statements that pose a question, whether

actually demanding an answer or rhetorical.

Ah's

Any verbal nonfluency or incoherent sounds that have no

inherent meaning that indicate a break in speech or thought.

Silences

Any pause measured in seconds that lasts longer than

two seconds.

Page 76: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX E

DOMINANCE AND COMFORTABILITY RANKINGS OF PEERS

71

Page 77: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

72

ID Code:

Rank each of the people your talked with today as to how

comfortable you felt with each. Put "1" besides the person

with whom you felt the most relaxed, comfortable, or at ease

and put up to "7", "8" or 911, depending upon the number of

persons in your group for the person with whom you felt the

least relaxed, comfortable or at ease.

Rank each of the people you talked with today as to how dominant

they appeared while interacting with you. Put "1" besides the

person you consider to be the most dominant, influential,

assertive, aggressive or strong and put up to "7", "8" or "9",

depending upon the number of people in your group, for the

person you consider to be the most shy, retiring, timid, quiet

or bashful.

Page 78: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX F

DOMINANCE SCALE OF CALIFORNIA PERSONALITY INVENTORY

73

Page 79: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

74

ID CODE:

For each item, if you agree with a statement, or feel

that it is true about you, answer TRUE. If you disagree

with a statement, or feel that it is not true about you,

answer FALSE.

1. I think I would enjoy having authority over other

people.

2. I find it hard to keep my mind on a task or job.

3. 1 have sometimes stayed away from another person

because I feared doing or saying something that

I might regret afterwards.

4. When in a group of people I have trouble thinking

of the right things to talk about.

5. School teachers complain a lot about their pay,

but it seems to me that they get as much as they

deserve.

6. I don't blame anyone for trying to grab all he

can get in this world.

7. Every citizen should take the time to find out

about national affairs, even if it means giving

up some personal pleasures.

8. I should like to belong to several clubs or

lodges.

9. I am certainly lacking in self-confidence.

10. When I work on a committee I like to take charge

of things.

Page 80: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

75

If given the chance I would make a good leader of

people.

Sometimes at elections I vote for men about whom I

know very little.

I very much like hunting.

A person does not need to worry about other people

if only he looks after himself.

I can honestly say that I do not really mind paying

my taxes because I feel that's one of the things

I can do for what I get from the community.

When prices are high you can't blame a person for

getting all he can while the getting is good.

In school I found it very hard to talk before

the class.

I am a better talker than a listener.

I would be willing to give money myself in order

to right a wrong, even though I was not mixed up

in it in the place.

We should cut down on our use of oil, if necessary,

so that there will be plenty left for the people

fifty or a hundred years from now.

When the community makes a decision, it is up to

a person to help carry it out even if he had been

against it.

I would rather have people dislike me than look

down on me.

22.

Page 81: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

76

23. I must admit I try to see what others think before

I take a stand.

24. People should not have to pay taxes for the schools

if they do not have any children.

25. In a group, I usually take the responsibility for

getting people introduced.

26. I would be willing to describe myself as a pretty

"strong" personality.

27. There are times when I act like a coward.

28. I must admit I am a pretty fair talker.

29. 1 have strong political opinions.30. I think I am usually a leader in my group.

31. I seem to do things that I regret more often than

other people do.

32. Disobedience to any government is never justified.

33. I would rather not have very much responsibility

for other people.

Page 82: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

APPENDIX G

TABLES 7-16

77

Page 83: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

78

Table 7

Regression Results for Total Talk Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change .01 .00

Differential Resistance to Change .04 .00

Cross-product Resistance to Change -.74 .02

Male/male Dyads .32*** .10

Male/female Dyads -.03 .00

* ** p_< .001.

Table 8

Regression Results for Differential Talk Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change .10 .01

Differential Resistance to Change .04 .00

Cross-product Resistance to Change -.29 .00

Male/male Dyads .05 .00

Male/female Dyads -.02 .00

Page 84: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

79

Table 9

Regression Results of Total Interruption Rates

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change -.26** .07

Difference Resistance to Change -.09 .01

Cross-product Resistance to Change -.43 .01

Male/Male Dyads .23* .05

Male/Female Dyads .15 .01

* <.05; ** 2 <.01.

Table 10

Regression Results for Differential Interruption Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change -.16+ .03

Differential Resistance to Change .10 .01

Cross-product Resistance to Change -.21 .00

Male/male Dyads .11 .01

Male/female Dyads .01 .00

+ .10.

Page 85: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

80

Table 11

Regression Results of Total Question Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change -. 39*** .15

Difference Resistance to Change -.09 .01

Cross-Product Resistance to Change -.28 .00

Male/Male Dyads .10 .01

Male/Female Dyads .02 .00

*** <.001.

Table 12

Regression Results for Differential Question Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change -.23* .05

Differential Resistance to Change -1.05 .01

Cross-product Resistance to Change .07 .00

Male/male Dyads .07 .01

Male/female Dyads .15 .01

* 2 < .05.

Page 86: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

81

Table 13

Regression Results for Total Ah Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change .05 .00

Differential Resistance to Change .10 .01

Cross-Product Resistance to Change 1.21 .01

Male/Male Dyads -.15 .02

Male/Female Dyads -.28 .02

Table 14

Regression Results for Differential Ah Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change .03 .00

Differential Resistance to Change .09 .01

Cross-product Resistance to Change .32 .00

Male/male Dyads -. 14 .02

Male/female Dyads -.11 .00

Page 87: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

82

Table 15

Regression Results for Total Silence Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance To Change -.01 .00

Differential Resistance to Change -.04 .00

Cross-product Resistance to Change .74 .02

Male/male Dyads -. 32*** .10

Male/female Dyads .03 .00

*** 2 < .001.

Table 16

Regression Results for Differential Silence Rate

Beta Change in

Predictor Values Values R2

Total Resistance to Change -.09 .01

Differential Resistance to Change -.07 .01

Cross-product Resistance to Change .94* .04

Male/male Dyads -.17+ .03

Male/female Dyads -.04 .00

+2 < .10; * 2 < .05"

Page 88: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

83

REFERENCES

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality, A psychological

interpretation. New York: Holt.

Aries, E. J., Gold, C., & Weigel, R. H. (1983).

Dispositional and situational influences on dominance

behavior in small groups. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 44, 779-786.

Breckler, S. J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1986). Motivational

Facets of the self. in R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins

(Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (p.

145-164). New York: Guilford Press.

Cantor, N., Hazel, M., Niedenthal, P., & Nurius, P. (1986).

title. in R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),

Handbook of motivation and cognition (p. 96-121). New

York: Guilford Press.

Danzinger, K. (1976) Interpersonal Communication. New York:

Pergamon Press.

Doster, J. (1975). Individual differences affecting

interviewee expectancies and perceptions of self-

disclosure. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, (3),

192-198.

Page 89: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

84

Doster, J. & Watson, W. (1987, August). The complexity of

Selves. Paper presented at the Seventh International

Congress of Personal Construct Psychology, Memphis, TN.

Duck, S. W. & Spencer, C. (1972). Personal constructs and

friendship formation. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 23, 40-45.

Epstein, S. & O'Brien, E.J. (1985). The person-situation

debate in historical and current perspective.

Psychological Bulletin, 98, 513-537.

Ferguson, N. (1977). Simultaneous speech, interruptions and

dominance. British Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 16, 295-302.

Fruge, E. (1973). A Multimethod investigation of

influential, dominant, or assertive behavior. Unpublished

manuscript.

Gergen, K. J. (1971). The concept of self. New York: Holt,

Rinehart & Winston.

Gough, H. C. (1957). Manual for the California Psychological

Inventory. Palo Alto, California: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Haley, J. (1972). Strategies of psychotherapy. New York:

Grune and Stratton.

Hinkle, (1965). The change of personal constructs from the

viewpoint of a theory of construct implications.

Unpublished manuscript, Ohio State University.

Page 90: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

85

Jacob, T. (1975). Family interaction in disturbed and normal

families: A methodological and substantive review.

Psychological Bulletin, 82. 33-65.

Keisler, D. J. & Bernstein, A. J. (1974). A Communications

critique of behavior therapies. Unpublished manuscript.

Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs.

New Your: W. W. Norton.

Landfield, A. W. (1975) The Complaint: a confrontation of

personal urgency and professional construction. In ???,

(Eds.). Issues and Approaches in Psychological

Therapies. London: Wiley.

Landfield, A. W. (1975). Exploring socialization through the

interpersonal transaction group. Unpublished manuscript.

Landfield, A. W. & Barr, M. A. (1976). Ordination: a New

measure of concept organization. Unpublished manuscript.

Landfield, A. W. & Rivers, P. C. (1975). An introduction to

interpersonal transaction and rotating dyads.

Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 12,

365-373.

Leary, (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality:_a

Functional theory and methodology for personality

assessment. New York: Ronald Press.

Magnusson, D. & Endler, N. S. (1977). Interactional

Psychology: present status and future prospects. in D.

Magnusson & N. S. Endler, (Ed.), Personality at the

Page 91: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

86

crossroads: Current issues in interactional Psychology

(p. 3-31). New York: Wiley and Sons.

Mair, (1977a). Psychology and psychotherapy: some common

concerns. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 50,

(1), 21-25.

Mair, J. M. M. (1977b). The community of self. In D.

Bannister (Ed.), New perspectives in personal construct

psychology (p. 125-149). London: Academic Press.

Markus, H. , & Smith, J. (1981). The influence of self-

schemata on the perception of others. In N. Cantor & J.

F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social

interaction (p. 233-262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York:

Wiley and Sons.

Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning

reconceptualization of personality. Psychological

Review, 80, 252-283.

Mishler, E. G. & Waxler, N. E. (1968). Interaction in

families: Experimental study of family processes and

schizophrenia. New York: Wiley and Sons.

Mortensen, C. C. (1972). Communication: The study of human

interaction. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Phillips, W. M. (1981). The minimax problem of personal

construct organization and schizophrenic thought

Page 92: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

87

disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 4,

692-698.

Reusch, J. & Bateson, G. (1951). Communication: the Social

matrix of Psychiatry. New York: Dorton.

Riskin, J., & Faunce, E. E. (1972). An evaluative review of

family interaction research. Family Processes, 11,

365-455.

Roger, D. B., & Schumaker, A. (1983). An evaluative review

of family interaction research. Family Processes, 11,

365-455.

Rogers, W. T., & Jones, S. E. (1975). Effects of dominance

tendencies on floor holding and interruption behavior in

dyadic interaction. Human Communication Research, 1,

113-122.

Scherer, K. R. (1979). Voice and speech correlates of

perceived social influence in simulated juries. In H.

Giles & R. St. Clair (Eds.), Language and social

psychology (]. 255-286). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Scott, G. (1988). Permeability of selves and compliance

with therapeutic homework tasks. Unpublished manuscript,

University of North Texas.

Siegman, A. W. & Pope, B. (1968). Interviewer warmth in

relation to interviewee verbal behavior. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 588-595.

Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. (1967).

Pragmatics of human communication. New York: Norton.

Page 93: PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING .../67531/metadc500956/m2/1/high_res_d/1002779072...PREDICTION OF VERBAL DOMINANCE BEHAVIORS USING CONSTRUCTIVIST THEORY THESIS Presented

88

Wiens, A., Thompson, S., Matarazzo, J., Matarazzo, R., &

Saslow, G. (1965). Interview interaction behavior of

supervisors, head nurses, and staff nurses. Nursing

Research, 23, 322-329.

Wiggins, G. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-

descriptive terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 395-412.

Yalom, I. D. (1970). The theory and practice of group

psychotherapy. New York: Basic books.

Zimmerman, D. & West, C. (1975). Sex roles, interruptions

and silences in conversation. In B. Thorne, & N. Henley

(Ed.), Language and sex: Differences and dominance (p.

105-129). Rowley, Mass; Newbury House.