Top Banner
Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar but not uniform Molly Babel 1 , Grant McGuire 2 1 Department of Linguistics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia 2 Department of Linguistics, University of California at Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California [email protected], [email protected] Abstract This study reports on three populations’ ratings of vocal attractiveness for 30 male and 30 female voices producing isolated words. Equal numbers of male and female listeners were recruited from three dialect areas: northern California, western Canada, and Minnesota. Attractiveness ratings across dialects were highly correlated, particularly for female voices. To determine the acoustic features which influenced listener ratings, detailed acoustic analyses of vowel quality and voice quality were conducted. These measures were entered into separate principal component analyses to reduce the dimensionality. Principal components and additional measures of duration and f0 were entered into models to assess which acoustic features predict attractiveness ratings across dialects. The results indicate that despite the highly correlated ratings across dialects, listener populations differed slightly in the phonetic features used to make attractiveness judgments. Listeners from the more similar dialect groups (California and western Canada) used similar acoustic features in their judgments, supporting the hypothesis that vocal attractiveness involves community-specific preferences. These results support a theory of vocal attractiveness which considers community-specific norms in assessing vocal preferences. Index Terms: vocal attractiveness, perception, PCA, cross- dialect comparisons 1. Introduction The voice is a rich source of information for listeners. Beyond its role as the medium of communication in oral language, the human voice has the ability to convey biological information like sex [1] and age [2]; physiological details such as height and weight for men [3]; social classifications such as race [4]; and emotional states [5]. The perceived attractiveness of a voice could be wrapped up in several of these perceivable qualities. Previous work on vocal attractiveness has used a small selection of acoustic-phonetic measures that are related to talker size to predict listeners’ judgments of attractive voices. In this study, we employ a larger range of phonetic measures related to both the talkers’ laryngeal source and supralaryngeal cavity and non-physiological stylistic aspects of spoken language measurable from the signal to study the subjective vocal attractiveness ratings of sixty talkers in three dialect regions of North American English. Most previous research has focused on acoustic features theoretically related to sexual dimorphism, e.g., fundamental frequency and formant dispersion [6, 7]. The former of these two features has been well established in its relationship with vocal attractiveness with the consensus that an average or slightly higher-than-average overall f0 is considered more attractive for female voices and that an average or slightly lower-than-average voice is more attractive in male talkers [8, 9, 10, 11, 12]. This finding is assumed to be an exaggeration of the average laryngeal differences between males and females. Note, however, that cross-culturally the degree of apparent size and actual size difference between males and females varies [13, 14]. Previous research seems to underplay the performative aspects of spoken communication speech is learned and used in a way that reflects identity construction, part of which might involve the use of more prescriptive gender norms, which echoes sexual dimorphic traits. Unlike studies of visual attractiveness, it is impossible to fully remove cultural artifacts from speech stimuli as they are fundamental to the linguistic signal. Moreover, even highly dimorphic traits such as f0 [15] and formant frequencies [16] vary considerably depending on the language and cultural context. The goal of the present study is twofold. The first is to explore additional acoustic features beyond f0 and formant spacing to others that are known to vary between males and females, namely duration, vowel quality, and voice quality. The second goal is to compare listeners’ judgments from three different dialects of English, two closely related and a third that is more divergent. To our knowledge, no one has compared listeners’ judgments of attractiveness to a single set of voices across dialects. Uncovering differences in how listeners from different dialect backgrounds assess vocal aesthetics would support the hypothesis that local community preferences moderate judgments of vocal attractiveness. 2. Listener judgments of attractiveness 2.1. Voices This study used a corpus of 30 female and 30 male native speakers of American English reading a list of monosyllabic low-frequency words each containing one of the vowels /i ɑ u/. Females (mean age 24.2) and males (mean age 24.1) did not differ in age [t(51) = 0.05, p = ns]. The majority of speakers were from the western United States. Recordings were made at 44.1kHz using a head-mounted microphone. 2.2. Listener judgments of attractiveness 2.2.1. Participants Three sets of thirty listeners judged the stimuli (total n = 90). These sets were recruited from three different university communities, and recruitment was restricted to those who had been raised in the dialect region since toddlerhood. One group of listeners was run at the University of California, Santa Cruz, and these students were from northern California; a second group of participants was run at the University of British Columbia these listeners were from British Columbia and Alberta; a final group of listeners was run at the University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, and these listeners were from Minnesota and Wisconsin. We refer to these groups as the California, western Canadian, and Minnesota groups, PREPRESS PROOF FILE CAUSAL PRODUCTIONS 1
5

Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

Sep 06, 2018

Download

Documents

buitruc
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar but not uniform

Molly Babel 1, Grant McGuire

2

1 Department of Linguistics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia

2 Department of Linguistics, University of California at Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, California

[email protected], [email protected]

Abstract

This study reports on three populations’ ratings of vocal

attractiveness for 30 male and 30 female voices producing

isolated words. Equal numbers of male and female listeners

were recruited from three dialect areas: northern California,

western Canada, and Minnesota. Attractiveness ratings across

dialects were highly correlated, particularly for female voices.

To determine the acoustic features which influenced listener

ratings, detailed acoustic analyses of vowel quality and voice

quality were conducted. These measures were entered into

separate principal component analyses to reduce the

dimensionality. Principal components and additional measures

of duration and f0 were entered into models to assess which

acoustic features predict attractiveness ratings across dialects.

The results indicate that despite the highly correlated ratings

across dialects, listener populations differed slightly in the

phonetic features used to make attractiveness judgments.

Listeners from the more similar dialect groups (California and

western Canada) used similar acoustic features in their

judgments, supporting the hypothesis that vocal attractiveness

involves community-specific preferences. These results

support a theory of vocal attractiveness which considers

community-specific norms in assessing vocal preferences.

Index Terms: vocal attractiveness, perception, PCA, cross-

dialect comparisons

1. Introduction

The voice is a rich source of information for listeners. Beyond

its role as the medium of communication in oral language, the

human voice has the ability to convey biological information

like sex [1] and age [2]; physiological details such as height

and weight for men [3]; social classifications such as race [4];

and emotional states [5]. The perceived attractiveness of a

voice could be wrapped up in several of these perceivable

qualities. Previous work on vocal attractiveness has used a

small selection of acoustic-phonetic measures that are related

to talker size to predict listeners’ judgments of attractive

voices. In this study, we employ a larger range of phonetic

measures related to both the talkers’ laryngeal source and

supralaryngeal cavity and non-physiological stylistic aspects

of spoken language measurable from the signal to study the

subjective vocal attractiveness ratings of sixty talkers in three

dialect regions of North American English.

Most previous research has focused on acoustic features

theoretically related to sexual dimorphism, e.g., fundamental

frequency and formant dispersion [6, 7]. The former of these

two features has been well established in its relationship with

vocal attractiveness with the consensus that an average or

slightly higher-than-average overall f0 is considered more

attractive for female voices and that an average or slightly

lower-than-average voice is more attractive in male talkers [8,

9, 10, 11, 12]. This finding is assumed to be an exaggeration

of the average laryngeal differences between males and

females. Note, however, that cross-culturally the degree of

apparent size and actual size difference between males and

females varies [13, 14].

Previous research seems to underplay the performative

aspects of spoken communication – speech is learned and used

in a way that reflects identity construction, part of which might

involve the use of more prescriptive gender norms, which

echoes sexual dimorphic traits. Unlike studies of visual

attractiveness, it is impossible to fully remove cultural artifacts

from speech stimuli as they are fundamental to the linguistic

signal. Moreover, even highly dimorphic traits such as f0 [15]

and formant frequencies [16] vary considerably depending on

the language and cultural context.

The goal of the present study is twofold. The first is to

explore additional acoustic features beyond f0 and formant

spacing to others that are known to vary between males and

females, namely duration, vowel quality, and voice quality.

The second goal is to compare listeners’ judgments from three

different dialects of English, two closely related and a third

that is more divergent. To our knowledge, no one has

compared listeners’ judgments of attractiveness to a single set

of voices across dialects. Uncovering differences in how

listeners from different dialect backgrounds assess vocal

aesthetics would support the hypothesis that local community

preferences moderate judgments of vocal attractiveness.

2. Listener judgments of attractiveness

2.1. Voices

This study used a corpus of 30 female and 30 male native

speakers of American English reading a list of monosyllabic

low-frequency words each containing one of the vowels /i ɑ u/. Females (mean age 24.2) and males (mean age 24.1) did

not differ in age [t(51) = 0.05, p = ns]. The majority of

speakers were from the western United States. Recordings

were made at 44.1kHz using a head-mounted microphone.

2.2. Listener judgments of attractiveness

2.2.1. Participants

Three sets of thirty listeners judged the stimuli (total n = 90).

These sets were recruited from three different university

communities, and recruitment was restricted to those who had

been raised in the dialect region since toddlerhood. One group

of listeners was run at the University of California, Santa

Cruz, and these students were from northern California; a

second group of participants was run at the University of

British Columbia – these listeners were from British Columbia

and Alberta; a final group of listeners was run at the

University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, and these listeners were

from Minnesota and Wisconsin. We refer to these groups as

the California, western Canadian, and Minnesota groups,

PREPRESS PROOF FILE CAUSAL PRODUCTIONS1

Page 2: Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

respectively. The Californian and Canadian groups share more

dialect features than either do with the Minnesotan group.

2.2.2. Procedure

For all groups the procedure was the same. Fifteen stimuli

from one talker were presented after which the listener rated

the voice on a 1-9 scale where 1 = very unattractive and 9 =

very attractive. Talker order and word order for each talker

were randomized for each listener.

Figure 1: Correlations of ratings for female and male

voices from Californian raters (x-axis) and Western

Canadian raters (y-axis).

Figure 2: Correlations of ratings for female and male

voices from Californian raters (x-axis) and

Minnesotan raters (y-axis).

2.2.3. Results

Average attractiveness ratings for each talker were averaged

across listeners within each dialect group for male and female

voices. To assess the relationships between listeners

judgments of attractiveness across dialect regions, we

conducted a series of correlations.

Correlations were high across the board, but generally

higher for female voices than male voices. For female voices,

Californians’ and Canadians’ ratings were very highly

correlated [t(28)=14.72, r=0.94, p<0.001], as were

Californians’ and Minnesotan’s ratings [t(28)=14.7, r=0.94,

p<0.001], and Minnesotans’ and Canadians’ ratings

[t(28)=13.19, r=0.93, p<0.001).

For the male voices, the correlations were also high.

Minnesotans’ and Canadians’ ratings were most highly

correlated for the male voices [t(28)=9.58, r=0.87, p<0.001],

followed by Californians’ and Minnesotans’ ratings

[t(28)=8.9, r=0.86, p<0.001]. The correlation between

Californians’ and Canadians’ ratings were lower, but still very

high [t(28)=6.54, r=0.78, p<0.001]. These correlations are

shown in Figures 1-3.

Figure 3: Correlations of ratings for female and male

voices from Western Canadian raters (x-axis) and

Minnesotan raters (y-axis).

2.3. Acoustic measures

2.3.1. Vowel quality measures

Following previous studies, F0 and formant dispersion were

measured for each talker. These values were averaged across

all tokens for each talker and the standard deviation of F0 was

also calculated from these measures. F0 and formant

frequency measures were made using Gaussian windows with

a 2.5 ms step size. Values were calculated separately for male

and females where five formants within a 0-5kHz range for the

males and 0-5.5kHz range for females. F1-F4 were used in

calculating formant dispersion following [17].

2.3.2. Voice quality measures

In addition to these vowel measures we also gathered several

measures of spectral tilt. Spectral tilt is a measure of voice

quality [18, 19]. In general, higher values of tilt indicate

breathier voices while lower values indicate creakiness.

Several measures were taken using VoiceSauce [20]: the short

distance tilt measure of the amplitude of the first harmonic

minus the amplitude of the second harmonic (H1-H2) and the

longer distance measure of the first harmonic minus the peak

amplitude of the first, second, and third formants (H1-A1, H1-

A2, H1-A3, respectively).

2

Page 3: Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

Other global voice quality measures taken were the

Harmonic-to-noise ratio (HNR), Cepstral Peak Prominence

(CPP), energy, jitter, and shimmer. HNR was collected in the

0-3.5kHz range for each voice, following [21]. Jitter is a local

measure of deviation in periodicity – the averaged deviation of

subsequent pitch periods. Shimmer is a measure of variation in

amplitude – the averaged deviation in amplitude of subsequent

pitch periods. Both jitter and shimmer are measures that can be

described subjectively as voice smoothnesss.

We also measured whole word duration. Male productions

typically have shorter durations than females [22,23].

2.4. PCA

Many of our acoustic measures may be highly correlated with

one another. To eliminate such confounds in the analysis and

to reduce the dimensionality of the data, we conducted

principal components analyses (PCA) for the vowel quality

and voice quality measures. The vowel quality PCA was

performed on the full dataset which included both male and

female speakers. The PCA for voice quality was done

separately for male and female voices.

2.4.1. Vowel quality PCA

The vowel PCA used the F1-F3 Bark-transformed values for

each vowel. Table 1 summarizes the standard deviation (row

1), the proportion of variance accounted (row 2), and the

cumulative proportion accounted for (row 3) for each principal

component. Table 1 also provides the relative weightings for

each component, which are needed to interpret each

component in terms of the acoustic measures. The formant

dispersion measure was highly correlated with PC1; to avoid

colinearity in the model we use PC1 in the analyses below.

Table 1. The cumulative proportion of variance

accounted for and loadings from the PCA of vowel

quality across female and male voices.

PC1 PC2 PC3 PC4 PC5 PC6

SD 1.54 0.76 0.46 0.34 0.26 0.14

Proportion 0.70 0.17 0.06 0.04 0.02 0.01

Cumulative 0.70 0.88 0.94 0.97 0.99 1.00

Loadings

F1 /ɑ/ 0.34 -0.56 -0.18 -0.06 -0.72 -0.11

F1 /i/ 0.22 -0.12 0.12 0.61 0.23 -0.70

F1 /u/ 0.24 -0.09 0.04 0.68 0.01 0.69

F2 /ɑ/ 0.23 -0.53 -0.45 -0.23 0.63 0.09

F2 /i/ 0.44 -0.17 0.81 -0.32 0.15 0.09

F2 /u/ 0.73 0.60 -0.31 -0.11 -0.03 -0.04

2.4.2. Voice quality PCA

The second PCA analysis included all of the voice quality and

voice smoothness measures, i.e. H1-H2, H1-A3, HNR, CPP,

energy, jitter, and shimmer. Given the differences in vocal fold

vibration for males and females, we conducted separate PCA

for male and female voices. Tables 2 and 3 summarize

information necessary to interpret the principal components.

The analyses for male and female voices used 9 principal

components, but in the interest of space, we only present those

necessary to account for 100% of the cumulative variance.

Table 2. The cumulative proportion of variance

accounted for and loadings from the PCA of voice

quality for female voices.

PC1 PC2 PC3 PC4 PC5 PC6

SD 6.05 3.75 1.75 1.18 0.95 0.61

Proportion 0.65 0.25 0.05 0.02 0.02 0.01

Cumulative 0.65 0.90 0.95 0.97 0.99 1.00

Loadings

PC1 PC2 PC3 PC4 PC5 PC6

H1H2u 0.26 -0.03 -0.94 0.13 -0.12 0.14

H1A1u 0.26 0.13 0.10 0.73 -0.14 -0.58

H1A2u 0.68 -0.01 0.30 -0.11 -0.57 0.34

H1A3u 0.62 -0.02 0.06 -0.07 0.76 -0.04

HNR35 0.05 0.92 -0.07 -0.31 -0.01 -0.11

CPP -0.16 0.35 0.09 0.56 0.13 0.61

Energy 0.01 -0.06 0.08 0.17 0.21 0.38

Jitter 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00

Shimmer 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00

Table 3. The cumulative proportion of variance

accounted for and loadings from the PCA of voice

quality for male voices.

PC1 PC2 PC3 PC4 PC5 PC6 PC7

SD 6.72 3.22 2.13 1.77 1.05 0.76 0.59

Proportion 0.69 0.16 0.07 0.05 0.02 0.01 0.01

Cumulative 0.69 0.85 0.92 0.97 0.99 0.99 1.00

Loadings

PC1 PC2 PC3 PC4 PC5 PC6 PC7

H1H2u 0.24 0.06 -0.18 0.72 -0.20 -0.29 -0.52

H1A1u 0.42 0.18 -0.07 0.48 0.23 0.28 0.66

H1A2u 0.65 -0.08 0.05 -0.32 0.58 -0.28 -0.23

H1A3u 0.56 -0.14 0.27 -0.19 -0.63 0.38 -0.09

HNR35 -0.02 -0.93 -0.01 0.17 -0.02 -0.20 0.24

CPP -0.18 -0.11 0.72 0.29 0.35 0.38 -0.28

Energy 0.02 0.23 0.61 0.03 -0.22 -0.66 0.32

Jitter 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00

Shimmer 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00

2.5. Predicting listener judgments

In order to determine which factors were the most important in

listeners’ judgments of vocal attractiveness, two linear

regression models were calculated to predict the attractiveness

ratings. The independent variables in the regression models

were the principal components from each analysis which

brought the percentage of variance accounted for near 95%, in

addition to duration, and mean and standard deviation of F0.

The variables for the final models were chosen using a

backwards selection procedure with a criterion of p < 0.15.

3

Page 4: Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

Following this procedure, the two final models were then

calculated with the remaining predictors.

We first report the models for the females. The model for

the Californian listeners was significant and accounted for

54% of the variance in listener responses [F(4,25)=9.39,

p<0.001]. The model for Canadian listeners for female voices

was very similar [t(5,24)=7.73, p<0.001], also accounting for

54% of the variance. The model for the Minnesota listeners

performed worse, with the model accounting for only 44% of

the variance [F(4,25)=6.78, p<0.001]. The significant

contributors to the models for the female voices are reported in

Table 4. Factors with vPC are principal components from the

vowel PCA and those with tfPC are the spectral tilt female

PCA (tmPC = spectral tilt male PCA).

Table 4. Summaries significant contributors in

regression models predicting attractiveness judgments

for the female voices.

California

β SE t-value

Intercept 7.93 1.57 5.05***

Average F0 -0.02 0.01 -2.21*

vPC1 0.48 0.17 2.85**

vPC3 0.52 0.28 1.82

tfPC3 -0.26 0.09 -2.95**

Canada

β SE t-value

Intercept 10.2 1.8 5.66***

Average F0 -0.03 0.01 -3.09**

vPC1 0.62 0.19 3.24**

vPC3 0.63 0.32 1.97

vPC4 0.54 0.39 1.34

tfPC3 -0.26 0.1 -2.6*

Minnesota

β SE t-value

Intercept 7.53 1.45 5.2***

Average F0 -0.01 0.01 -1.93

vPC1 0.45 0.16 2.9**

vPC2 0.53 0.26 2.02

tfPC3 -0.14 0.08 -1.78

For the male voices, the model for Californian listeners

accounted for 60% of the variance [F(7,22)=7.23, p<0.001].

The model based on judgments from Canadian listeners

accounted for 48% of the variance [F(3,26)=9.78, p<0.001].

Finally, the model based on Minnesota listeners’ judgments of

male voices accounted for 51% of the variance [F(4,25)=8.68,

p<0.001]. These models are summarized in Table 5.

3. Discussion

The above results demonstrate cross-dialect differences and

similarities in the acoustic features that listeners prefer.

Overall, Californian and Western Canadian listeners are the

most similar in their preferences with the Minnesota group

being somewhat divergent.

Models for female voices were quite similar, particularly

those of the Californian and Canadian listeners. vPC1 is

suggestive of apparent size, but was dominated by F2 of /u/;

this suggests that it may be an indicator of dialect-specific

vowel positioning. More fronted productions of /u/ are rated as

more attractive. Breathier female voices are judged as more

attractive in the Californian and Canadian models. Also in

these two models, female voices with lower average F0 were

rated as more attractive.

Table 5. Summaries significant contributors in

regression models predicting attractiveness judgments

for the male voices.

California

β SE t-value

Intercept 5.67 0.63 8.96***

vPC1 -0.31 0.18 -1.75

vPC2 0.25 0.13 1.98

vPC3 -0.37 0.25 -1.52

vPC4 -1.75 0.33 -5.27***

tmPC1 0.03 0.02 1.85

tmPC4 -0.07 0.05 -1.46

Duration -4.10 1.26 -3.26**

Canada

β SE t-value

Intercept 7.33 0.77 9.52***

vPC4 -1.62 0.42 -3.87***

tmPC1 0.05 0.02 2.2*

Duration -5.06 1.67 -3.03**

Minnesota

β SE t-value

Intercept 5.94 0.58 10.19***

vPC2 0.21 0.11 1.92

vPC4 -1.44 0.32 -4.51***

tmPC1 0.03 0.02 1.66

Duration -2.49 1.26 -1.97

Male voices used a different set of factors, and while a

larger number of factors passed the criterion for model

selection, few were significant predictors in the final models.

vPC4 seems to indicate aspects of talker size, with apparently

larger male vocal tracts being more attractive. Duration was a

significant predictor for both Canadian and Californian

listeners; this suggests that sounding like an average or typical

male voice contributes to listeners’ assessments of vocal

attractiveness. tmPC1 suggests that breathier male voices were

more attractive to Canadian listeners.

4. Conclusion

The data presented in this paper suggest that the acoustic

information listeners use to make their assessments of vocal

attractiveness varies somewhat across dialects. The strong

correlations in our perception results suggest that despite this

use of different aspects of the acoustic signal, listeners across

dialects may exhibit high levels of uniformity in their

judgments. To further explore this future analysis of the data

will involve using clustering analyses to examine whether

groups of listeners within a dialect population behave in small-

group defining ways.

5. Acknowledgements

Thanks members of the Speech in Context Lab at UBC and the

Phonetics Lab at UCSC. This research has benefited from

discussion with Joseph King, Teresa Miller, Eric Vatikiotis-

Bateson, Martin Oberg, and Alexandre Bouchard-Côté.

Thanks to Benjamin Munson for collecting the Minnesota

data.

4

Page 5: Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects is similar …speechincontext.arts.ubc.ca/static/media/2013Perceived... · 2013-09-19 · Perceived vocal attractiveness across dialects

6. References

[1] Lass, N., Hughes, K., Bowyer, M., Waters, L., & Bourne, V.

(1976). Speaker sex identification from voiced, whispered, and filtered isolated vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 59 (3), 675-678.

[2] Ptacek, P. H., & Sander, E. K. (1966). Age recognition from voice. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 9(2), 273-277.

[3] van Dommelen, W. & Moxness, B. (1995). Acoustic parameters

in Speaker Height and Weight Identification: Sex-Specific Behaviour. Language and Speech, 38, 3, 267-287.

[4] Walton, J. & Orlikoff, R. (1994). Speaker race identification

from acoustic cues in the vocal signal. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 37, 4, 738-745.

[5] Scherer, K., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. (2001). Emotional

inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 1, 76-92.

[6] Feinberg, D.R., Jones, B.C., Little, A. C., Burt, D.M., & Perrett,

D.I. (2005). Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices.

Animal Behaviour, 69, 561-568.

[7] Feinberg, D.R., DeBruine, L.M., Jones, B.C., & Perrett, D.I. (2008). The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in

aesthetic judgments of women’s voices. Perception, 37, 615-

623. [8] Tuomi, S. K. & Fisher, J.E. (1979). Characteristics of Simulated

Sexy Voice. Folia phoniatrica, 31, 4, 242-9 [9] Apple, W., Streeter, L. A. & Krauss, R. M. (1979). Effects of

pitch and speech rate on personal attributions. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 715-727. [10] Zuckerman, M. & Driver, R. (1989). What sounds beautiful is

good: The vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of Nonverbal

Behavior, 13 (2), 67-82. [11] Riding, D., Lonsdale, D., & Brown, B. (2006). The Effects of

Average Fundamental Frequency and Variance of Fundamental

Frequency on Male Vocal Attractiveness to Women. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 30, 55-61.

[12] Saxton, T., Caryl, P., & Roberts, S. C. (2006). Vocal and Facial

Attractiveness Judgments of Children, Adolescents and Adults: the Ontogeny of Mate Choice. Ethology, 112, 1179- 1185.

[13] Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross

- language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41, 1-16. [14] Ohara, Y. (1992). Gender dependent pitch levels: A comparative

study in Japanese and English. In K. Hall, M. Bucholtz, & B.

Moonwomon (Eds.), Locating power: Proceedings of the Second Berkeley Women and Language Conference, Vol. 2, 468-477.

[15] Van Bezooijen, R. 1995. Sociocultural aspects of pitch

differences between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and Speech, 38, 253–65.

[16] Johnson, K. (2006). Resonance in an exemplar-based lexicon:

the emergence of social identity and phonology. Journal of

Phonetics, 43, 485–99.

[17] Reby, D. & McComb, K.. (2003). Anatomical constraints

generate honesty: acoustic cues to age and weight in roars of red deer stags. Animal Behaviour, 65 (3), 519-530.

[18] Gordon, M. & Ladefoged, P. (2001). Phonation types: a cross-

linguistic overview. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 383-406. [19] Keating, P.A. & Esposito, C.M. (2007). Linguistic voice

quality. Proceedings of the Eleventh Australasian International

Conference on Speech Science and Technology 2006, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand, December 6-8 2006.

[20] Shue, Y.-L., Keating, P., Vicenik, C., and Yu, K. (2011).

“VoiceSauce: A program for voice analysis,” in Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences XVII, edited by

W.-S. Lee and E. Zee (City University of Hong Kong, Hong

Kong), pp. 1846–1849. [21] Bruckert, L., Bestelmeyer, P., Latinus, M., Rouger, J. Charest, I.

Rousselet, G.A., Kawahara, H., & Belin, Pascal. (2010). Vocal

Attractiveness Increases by Averaging, Current Biology, 20, 1116-120.

[22] Byrd, D. (1994). Relations of sex and dialect to reduction.

Speech Communication, 15, 39-54.

[23] Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M. and Pisoni, D. B. (1996).

Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication,

20, 255-272.

5