Top Banner
Today - Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes - Frequency code - Effort code
14

Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

May 01, 2023

Download

Documents

Khang Minh
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Today- Paralanguage vs. language

- Biological codes- Frequency code- Effort code

Page 2: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

ParalinguisticsAspects of vocal communication that are clearly meaningful, but not organized along linguistic lines

Often paralinguistic phonetic cues to:- Speakers’ attitudinal stances e.g. aggression, appeasement,

solidarity, condescension- Emotional states e.g. fear, surprise, anger, joy, boredom

Produced in parallel to linguistic messages, and sometimes in tight coordination e.g. nod on stressed syllable

Page 3: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Paralanguage vs. language Ladd 2008:37

Main difference has nothing to do with:

- Acoustic properties (e.g. VOT: linguistic vs. intensity: paralinguistic)

- Kinds of meaning being conveyed (propositional: linguistic vs. attitudinal: paralinguistic)

But rather with how form and meaning map onto each other

Page 4: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Paralanguage vs. language Ladd 2008:37

Paralanguage involves gradient signaling: semantic continua are matched by phonetic continua

e.g. if raising one’s voice indicates anger or surprise, the more one raises their voice, the more anger or surprise is conveyed

Language involves categorical signaling:fin and thin are closer in phonetic space than fin and sin. But their meanings are not!

Page 5: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Biological codes of paralinguistic meaning as applied to pitch Gussenhoven 2002

Paralinguistic form-meaning relations arise from the way biological factors affect speech production, with some fine-tuning by the cultural and linguistic contexts

These form–meaning relations are based on metaphorical interpretations of the effects of anatomical and physiological factors on speech

At least three biological codes codes can be identified:- The Frequency Code- The Effort Code- The Respiratory Code

If you’re interested:https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Agonistic_behaviour

Page 6: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

The Frequency Code Ohala 1983

large larynx ~ low pitch ~ large creature ~ male adult ~ …

Association of “small” meanings with high pitch yields interpretations like submission, friendliness, uncertainty, vulnerability, etc.

Association of “big” meanings with low pitch to dominance, aggressiveness, certainty, protectiveness, etc.

Page 7: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Rising statements vs. questions in French Torreira & Valtersson 2015

In French, statements with final rising intonation can indicate incomplete statements and yes-no questions:

25Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Phonetic and Visual Cues to Questionhood in French Conversation

Freq (H

z)

0

5,000

F0

(Hz)

700

100Time (s) 0.6720

t a t t pε̃ ʁ i m eu

Fig. 1. Waveform, s p ectro-gram, and pitch track for the polar question T’as tout imprimé ‘Have you printed everything?’ spoken by female speaker F05L in the NCCFr.

Freq (H

z)

0

5,000

F0

(Hz)

700

100

Time (s)0.6640

t a ʁ ʁi v a l i

Fig. 2. Waveform, spectro-gram, and pitch track for the continuation statement T’arrives à lire ‘You can read (it)’ spoken by female speaker F06R in the NCCFr. The utter-ance was produced as part of the larger turn Parce que tu tu lis le truc, t’arrives à lire, mais euh t’as pas la compréhension ‘Because you you read the thing, you can read (it), but uhm you don’t get the meaning’.

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

25Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Phonetic and Visual Cues to Questionhood in French Conversation

Freq (H

z)

0

5,000

F0

(Hz)

700

100Time (s) 0.6720

t a t t pε̃ ʁ i m eu

Fig. 1. Waveform, s p ectro-gram, and pitch track for the polar question T’as tout imprimé ‘Have you printed everything?’ spoken by female speaker F05L in the NCCFr.

Freq (H

z)

0

5,000

F0

(Hz)

700

100

Time (s)0.6640

t a ʁ ʁi v a l i

Fig. 2. Waveform, spectro-gram, and pitch track for the continuation statement T’arrives à lire ‘You can read (it)’ spoken by female speaker F06R in the NCCFr. The utter-ance was produced as part of the larger turn Parce que tu tu lis le truc, t’arrives à lire, mais euh t’as pas la compréhension ‘Because you you read the thing, you can read (it), but uhm you don’t get the meaning’.

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

Q S

Page 8: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Rising statements vs. questions in French Torreira & Valtersson 2015

Pitch measurements at points a, b, c, and d:

Page 9: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Results

32 Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Torreira/Valtersson

These figures show that, excepting the final pitch maximum at the end of the utterance in the case of female speakers, questions generally exhibited a higher average pitch scaling than continuation statements. Moreover, the difference in final minimum pitch appears to be greater for males than for females. A series of regression models with each of our pitch measures as dependent variable, utterance type and gender as fixed predictors, and speaker as a random factor confirmed the statistical validity of these observations. We obtained statistically significant interactions between utterance type and speaker gender both for final minimum pitch (utterance type: β = 2.31, t = 3.31, p < 0.0005; gender × utterance type: β = 2.33, t = 2.45, p < 0.05) and final maximum pitch (gender: β = –4.31, t = –6.24, p < 0.0001; gender × utterance type: β = 2.47, t = 3.04, p < 0.005). Main effects of utterance type were also observed for initial pitch (β = 0.83, t = 2.87, p < 0.005) and for the pitch value measured in the penultimate syllable of the utterance (β = 1.87, t = 5.66, p < 0.0001). In contrast with our findings for final pitch values, no effect of gender or interac-tion between gender and utterance type was observed for the non-final pitch measures.

Male speakers

Q

C

Firstsyllable

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–4

–2

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Penultsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Fig. 8. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C) and polar ques-tions (Q) of more than two syl-lables spoken by male speakers (n = 116).

Female speakers

Firstsyllable

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–4

–2

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Penultsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Q

CFig. 9. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C) and polar ques-tions (Q) of more than two syllables spoken by females (n = 79).

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

32 Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Torreira/Valtersson

These figures show that, excepting the final pitch maximum at the end of the utterance in the case of female speakers, questions generally exhibited a higher average pitch scaling than continuation statements. Moreover, the difference in final minimum pitch appears to be greater for males than for females. A series of regression models with each of our pitch measures as dependent variable, utterance type and gender as fixed predictors, and speaker as a random factor confirmed the statistical validity of these observations. We obtained statistically significant interactions between utterance type and speaker gender both for final minimum pitch (utterance type: β = 2.31, t = 3.31, p < 0.0005; gender × utterance type: β = 2.33, t = 2.45, p < 0.05) and final maximum pitch (gender: β = –4.31, t = –6.24, p < 0.0001; gender × utterance type: β = 2.47, t = 3.04, p < 0.005). Main effects of utterance type were also observed for initial pitch (β = 0.83, t = 2.87, p < 0.005) and for the pitch value measured in the penultimate syllable of the utterance (β = 1.87, t = 5.66, p < 0.0001). In contrast with our findings for final pitch values, no effect of gender or interac-tion between gender and utterance type was observed for the non-final pitch measures.

Male speakers

Q

C

Firstsyllable

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–4

–2

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Penultsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Fig. 8. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C) and polar ques-tions (Q) of more than two syl-lables spoken by male speakers (n = 116).

Female speakers

Firstsyllable

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–4

–2

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Penultsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Q

CFig. 9. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C) and polar ques-tions (Q) of more than two syllables spoken by females (n = 79).

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

Page 10: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

The Effort Code Gussenhoven 2002

more articulatory effort/precision ~ greater arousal ~ greater significance

Across languages and cultures, wider pitch excursions, loud speech, careful speech tend to be associated with:

- Surprise- Anger- Cooperativeness (e.g. Infant-directed speech)- …

Page 11: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

Rising statements vs. questions in French Torreira & Valtersson 2015

35Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Phonetic and Visual Cues to Questionhood in French Conversation

behaviour. Frowns only occurred in 6 cases, nods in 8 cases, upward head movements in 12 cases, and raised eyebrows were observed on 13 occasions. The percentage of polar questions in cases with raised eyebrows was over twice as high as when no eye-brow action was present (69.2 vs. 30.4%). This difference was statistically significant despite the poor statistical power of the test due to the low number of data points (β = 2.2, z = 2.88, p < 0.005). The distributions in the case of head movements were very clear. All nods and all upward head movements occurred in questions.

Gaze towards the addressee was present in our data in most of the utterances (in 203 out of 260 cases coded for this variable). As expected from previous studies, we found that utterances which were accompanied by gaze towards the addressee tended to be polar questions more frequently than utterances which were not (42.4 vs. 22.8%). This differ-ence was statistically significant in a regression model with utterance type as the response, gaze as fixed predictor, and speaker as random factor (β = 1.46, z = 3.49, p < 0.0005). Regarding manual gestures, we found that topic gestures, which were not infrequent in

Fig. 10. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C), polar questions displaying surprise (Q + s), and polar questions not displaying surprise (Q – s) for utterances of more than two syllables spo-ken by male speakers.

–6

–2

2

6

10

14

18

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

CQ – s

Firstsyllable

Penult syllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Q + s

Female speakers

Male speakers

Q + sQ – s

C

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–6

–2

2

6

10

14

18

Penultsyllable

Firstsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Fig. 11. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C), polar questions displaying surprise (Q + s), and polar questions not displaying surprise (Q – s) for utterances of more than two syllables spo-ken by female speakers.

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

35Phonetica 2015;72:20–42DOI: 10.1159/000381723

Phonetic and Visual Cues to Questionhood in French Conversation

behaviour. Frowns only occurred in 6 cases, nods in 8 cases, upward head movements in 12 cases, and raised eyebrows were observed on 13 occasions. The percentage of polar questions in cases with raised eyebrows was over twice as high as when no eye-brow action was present (69.2 vs. 30.4%). This difference was statistically significant despite the poor statistical power of the test due to the low number of data points (β = 2.2, z = 2.88, p < 0.005). The distributions in the case of head movements were very clear. All nods and all upward head movements occurred in questions.

Gaze towards the addressee was present in our data in most of the utterances (in 203 out of 260 cases coded for this variable). As expected from previous studies, we found that utterances which were accompanied by gaze towards the addressee tended to be polar questions more frequently than utterances which were not (42.4 vs. 22.8%). This differ-ence was statistically significant in a regression model with utterance type as the response, gaze as fixed predictor, and speaker as random factor (β = 1.46, z = 3.49, p < 0.0005). Regarding manual gestures, we found that topic gestures, which were not infrequent in

Fig. 10. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C), polar questions displaying surprise (Q + s), and polar questions not displaying surprise (Q – s) for utterances of more than two syllables spo-ken by male speakers.

–6

–2

2

6

10

14

18

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

CQ – s

Firstsyllable

Penult syllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Q + s

Female speakers

Male speakers

Q + sQ – s

C

Nor

mal

ized

pitc

h (s

emito

nes)

–6

–2

2

6

10

14

18

Penultsyllable

Firstsyllable

Minimum infinal vowel

Maximum infinal vowel

Fig. 11. Speaker-normalized mean pitch values (in semi-tones) for four pitch measure-ments in continuation statements (C), polar questions displaying surprise (Q + s), and polar questions not displaying surprise (Q – s) for utterances of more than two syllables spo-ken by female speakers.

Dow

nloa

ded

by:

Uni

vers

ity T

oron

to L

ibr.

142.

150.

190.

39 -

6/5/

2015

4:1

3:30

PM

Page 12: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

The Respiratory/Production CodeBy default, F0 tends to decrease during a breath group as subglottal pressure decreases (although speakers can control/cancel this process):

high pitch ~ utterance beginning ~ new topic ~ unfinished turn ~ …

low pitch ~ utterance end ~ end of topic ~ finished turn ~ ….

Page 13: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes

ExerciseFind one plausible instances of each biological code in the following video excerpt:

- Frequency code

- Effort code

- Respiratory code

Page 14: Paralanguage vs. language - Biological codes