Top Banner
677 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 http://www.actaps.com.cn This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 30770691), Beijing Municipal Government for Advancement of Sciences, and Capital Medical University for Innovation Awards. * Corresponding author. Tel: +86-10-83911514; E-mail: [email protected] Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain Andrew CN CHEN * Center for Higher Brain Functions, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China Abstract: In the past two decades, pain perception in the human brain has been studied with EEG/MEG brain topography and PET/ fMRI neuroimaging techniques. A host of cortical and subcortical loci can be activated by various nociceptive conditions. The activation in pain perception can be induced by physical (electrical, thermal, mechanical), chemical (capsacin, ascoric acid), psychological (anxiety, stress, nocebo) means, and pathological (e.g. migraine, neuropathic) diseases. This article deals mainly on the activation, but not modulation, of human pain in the brain. The brain areas identified are named pain representation, matrix, neuraxis, or signature. The sites are not uniformly isolated across various studies, but largely include a set of cores sites: thalamus and primary somatic area (SI), second somatic area (SII), insular cortex (IC), prefrontal cortex (PFC), cingulate, and parietal cortices. Other areas less reported and considered important in pain perception include brainstem, hippocampus, amygdala and supplementary motor area (SMA). The issues of pain perception basically encompass both the site and the mode of brain function. Although the site issue is delineared to a large degree, the mode issue has been much less explored. From the temporal dynamics, IC can be considered as the initial stage in genesis of pain perception as conscious suffering, the unique aversion in the human brain. Key words: human pain perception; brain measures; nociceptive pain; pathological pain; anatomy-physiology; genesis 1 Histological perspectives Since the dawn of evolution, pain has been inflicted in ani- mals and humans. Originally, pain serves a crucial surviv- ing function for warning of bodily injuries in acute pain and/or persistent pain. Nevertheless, it becomes maladapted that pathological dynamics may modify neural organiza- tion and pain becomes a disease condition in chronic pain. Therefore, perception of pain in the human brain has to be examined in this dynamic context. So far, A large majority of research has touched mainly on the experimental acti- vation of the nociception system in acute condition, largely on the healthy subjects and some on the patients inflicted with pain suffering. Our knowledge of pain perception can be summarized as: (1) Large amount comes from nocice- ptive pain in healthy subjects; (2) Some amount comes from nociceptive pain modulated by the pathological conditions; (3) Few amount comes from the pathological pain in pain patients per se. It seems we are advanced with research on “sensitivity” of experimental pain over “valid- ity” of pathological pain. Certainly, ease of study dictates our priority of scientific knowledge. To begin with, it is worthwhile to list the milestones of review papers published in the past 15 years regarding pain perception in the human brain, on a chronological clarity. This chronological order is often related to technical prow- ess in defining the scientific knowledge. Non-invasive EEG/ MEG topography (measuring electric/magnetic and source of brain activity) and PET/fMRI tomography (measuring receptors in PET, metabolite-blood flow in both) are the main techniques in studying pain perception in the human brain. Three periods can be divided to characterize the scientific progress. Only those pertinent to the activation, but not modulation, of human pain perception in the brain are cited here. I. Early period (1990-1999): Chen (1993): EEG/MEG mapping of human pain [1] . Chen (1993): PET/fMRI neuroimaging of human pain [2] . Casey (1996): Neuronal determinants of pain [3] . Jones(1996): Inflammatory pain [4] . Kramer (1997): EEG and headache [5] . Frishberg (1997): Neuroimaging in headache disorders [6] . Bromm (1998): Neurophysiological evaluation of pain [7] . Ingvar (1999): Pain and functional imaging [8] . Casey (1999): Forebrain mechanisms of nociception and Review
9

Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

Feb 19, 2018

Download

Documents

vominh
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

677Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685http://www.actaps.com.cn

This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 30770691), Beijing MunicipalGovernment for Advancement of Sciences, and Capital Medical University for Innovation Awards.

*Corresponding author. Tel: +86-10-83911514; E-mail: [email protected]

Pain perception and its genesis in the human brainAndrew CN CHEN*

Center for Higher Brain Functions, Capital Medical University, Beijing 100069, China

Abstract: In the past two decades, pain perception in the human brain has been studied with EEG/MEG brain topography and PET/fMRI neuroimaging techniques. A host of cortical and subcortical loci can be activated by various nociceptive conditions. The activationin pain perception can be induced by physical (electrical, thermal, mechanical), chemical (capsacin, ascoric acid), psychological(anxiety, stress, nocebo) means, and pathological (e.g. migraine, neuropathic) diseases. This article deals mainly on the activation, butnot modulation, of human pain in the brain. The brain areas identified are named pain representation, matrix, neuraxis, or signature. Thesites are not uniformly isolated across various studies, but largely include a set of cores sites: thalamus and primary somatic area (SI),second somatic area (SII), insular cortex (IC), prefrontal cortex (PFC), cingulate, and parietal cortices. Other areas less reported andconsidered important in pain perception include brainstem, hippocampus, amygdala and supplementary motor area (SMA). The issuesof pain perception basically encompass both the site and the mode of brain function. Although the site issue is delineared to a largedegree, the mode issue has been much less explored. From the temporal dynamics, IC can be considered as the initial stage in genesis ofpain perception as conscious suffering, the unique aversion in the human brain.

Key words: human pain perception; brain measures; nociceptive pain; pathological pain; anatomy-physiology; genesis

1 Histological perspectives

Since the dawn of evolution, pain has been inflicted in ani-mals and humans. Originally, pain serves a crucial surviv-ing function for warning of bodily injuries in acute painand/or persistent pain. Nevertheless, it becomes maladaptedthat pathological dynamics may modify neural organiza-tion and pain becomes a disease condition in chronic pain.Therefore, perception of pain in the human brain has to beexamined in this dynamic context. So far, A large majorityof research has touched mainly on the experimental acti-vation of the nociception system in acute condition, largelyon the healthy subjects and some on the patients inflictedwith pain suffering. Our knowledge of pain perception canbe summarized as: (1) Large amount comes from nocice-ptive pain in healthy subjects; (2) Some amount comesfrom nociceptive pain modulated by the pathologicalconditions; (3) Few amount comes from the pathologicalpain in pain patients per se. It seems we are advanced withresearch on “sensitivity” of experimental pain over “valid-ity” of pathological pain. Certainly, ease of study dictatesour priority of scientific knowledge.

To begin with, it is worthwhile to list the milestones of

review papers published in the past 15 years regarding painperception in the human brain, on a chronological clarity.This chronological order is often related to technical prow-ess in defining the scientific knowledge. Non-invasive EEG/MEG topography (measuring electric/magnetic and sourceof brain activity) and PET/fMRI tomography (measuringreceptors in PET, metabolite-blood flow in both) are themain techniques in studying pain perception in the humanbrain. Three periods can be divided to characterize thescientific progress. Only those pertinent to the activation,but not modulation, of human pain perception in the brainare cited here.

I. Early period (1990-1999):Chen (1993): EEG/MEG mapping of human pain[1].Chen (1993): PET/fMRI neuroimaging of human pain[2].Casey (1996): Neuronal determinants of pain[3].Jones(1996): Inflammatory pain[4].Kramer (1997): EEG and headache[5].Frishberg (1997): Neuroimaging in headache disorders[6].Bromm (1998): Neurophysiological evaluation of pain[7].Ingvar (1999): Pain and functional imaging[8].Casey (1999): Forebrain mechanisms of nociception and

Review

Page 2: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685678

pain[9].Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials from

parasylvian cortex[10].Treede (1999): Cortical representation of pain[11].

II. Middle period (2000-2004):Schnitzler (2000): Neurophysiology/neuroanatomy of

pain perception[12].Treede (2000): Cortical representation of pain near lat-

eral sulcus[13].Kakigi (2000): Pain-related SEP[14].Derbyshire (2000): Pain neuromatrix[15].Casey (2000): Concept of pain mechanisms[16].Wiech (2000): Neuroimaing of chronic pain[17].Aziz (2000): Neuroimaging of visceral sensation[18].Cutrer (2000): Functional neuroimaging of migraine

pathophysiology[19].Bradley (2000): Neuroimaging in fibromyalgia[20].Chen (2001): New perspective of EEG/MEG brain map-

ping and PET/fMRI neuroimaging of human pain[21].Bromm (2001): Brain images of pain[22].Goadsby (2001): Neuroimaging in headache[23].Rainville (2001): Representation of acute and persistent

pain[24].Price (2002): Pain and consciousness[25]

Rainville (2002): pain affect and pain modulation[26].Kakigi (2003): C-fiber in human pain[27].Garcia-Larrea (2003): Laser evoked potentials[28].Lorenz (2003): Attentional and cognitive factors in LEP[29].May (2003): Neuroimaging and headache[30].Derbyshire (2003): Visceral afferent pathways and brain

imaging[31].Mackey (2004): Functional imaging of chronic pain[32].Mechtler (2004): Pathogenesis of primary headache[33].Sánchez del Rio (2004): Functional neuroimaging of head-

aches[34].Chiapparini(2004): Neuroradiological findings of head-

ache[35].May (2004): Neuroimaging in primary headache[36].Davis (2004): Neuroimaging of pain[37].

III. Recent period (2005-2008):Kakigi (2005): Electrophysiological studies on human pain

perception[38].Lorenz (2005): Imaging acute vs pathological pain[39].Apkarian (2005): Human brain mechanisms of pain per-

ception and regulation in health and disease[40].Cohen (2005): Functional neuroimaging of primary head-

ache disorders[41].

May (2005): Imaging in the pathophysiology and diag-nosis of headache[42].

Tracey (2005): Nociceptive processing in the humanbrain[43].

de Leeuw (2005): Neuroimaging techniques for orofacialpain[44].

Favier (2005): Chronic cluster headache[45].Rossi (2005): Central pain modulation in chronic head-

aches[46].Treede (2006): Nociceptive network of the human brain[47].Aurora (2006): Brain dysfuncion in migraine[48].May (2006): Functional anatomy of headache [49].Mee (2006): Psychological pain[50].Cohen (2006): Primary headache disorders[51].May (2006): Functional imaging in headache[49].Derbyshire (2006): Brain in pain[52].Anderson (2006): Plasticity of pain-related neuronal ac-

tivity in the human thalamus[53].Schweinhardt (2006): Imaging pain in patients[54].Borsook (2006): Migraine pathophysiology[55].Detsky (2006): Migraine neuroimaging[56].Williams (2006): FMRI findings in fibromyalgia[57].Jackson (2006): Pain empathy[58].Mayer (2006): Brain-gut axis[59].Duquette (2007): Pain and emotion[60].DaSilva (2007): Neuroimaging findings in cluster head-

ache[61].May (2007): Functional and structural imaging in mi-

graine[62].Cook (2007): Imaging pain of fibromyalgia[63].May (2007): Visualising the brain in pain[64].Leone (2007): Functional neuroimaging and headache

pathophysiology[65].Moisset (2007): Brain imaging of neuropathic pain[66].Tracey (2007): The cerebral signature for pain percep-

tion and its modulation[67].Linder (2007): Post-traumatic headache[68].Veldhuijzen (2007): Imaging central pain syndromes[69].Derbyshire (2007): Modeling pain circuits[70].Derbyshire (2007): Imaging visceral pain[71].Wood (2008): Central dopamine in pain and analgesia[72].Tracey (2008): Imaging pain[73].

2 Sites of pain activation in the HUMAN brain

2.1 Research in healthy subjects: nociceptive painCasey[3] has early stated that the principle “Multipleperipheral, segmental, and supraspinal neuronal activitiescontrol nociceptive processing at all levels of the neuraxis”,

Page 3: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

679Andrew CN CHEN: Pain Perception and its Genesis in the Human Brain

and later emphasized the complexity mechanisms in per-ception also involved pain-inhibitory and pain-facilitatingpathways linked to cognitive, emotional, and stress-responsesystems[9]. Among the complexities, the forebrain mecha-nisms act in all levels of neuraxis[9]. The major determi-nant factors can be empirically examined: gender, the typeof noxious stimuli, and origin of nociceptive input. Thebrain structures invoked by noxious stimulatoin across thevarious factors emerged are: the bilateral thalamus, thecontralateral insula and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC),premotor cortex, and the cerebellar vermis. The corticalrepresentation of pain was early summarized by Treede etal.[11]: primary somatosensory cortex (SI), secondary so-matosensory cortex (SII) and its vicinity in the parietaloperculum, insular cortex (IC), ACC and prefrontal cortex(PFC).

In first set of meta-analysis of pain perception in thehuman brain[15, 74], Derbyshire[15] has emphasized theneuromatrix of pain activation invoked by a noxious eventcan involve the sensory, affective, cognitive, motor,inhibitory, and autonomic responses, while there is scantevidence that any particular region or circuit exclusivelyacts in pain perception. Also, the responses in patients areless clear in a whole, but a reduction in cerebral responseshas been noted. Peyron et al.[74] have made the followingpoints: (1) Cerebral blood flow (CBF) increases to noxiousstimuli are almost constantly observed in SII and insularregions, and in ACC. (2) The responses are slightly lessconsistent in the contralateral thalamus and SI. (3) Activa-tion of the lateral thalamus. SI, SII and insula are thoughtto be related to the sensory-discriminative aspects of painprocessing. (4) The thalamic response is bilateral, prob-ably reflecting generalized arousal in reaction to pain. (5)ACC appears to participate in both the affective andattentional concomitants of pain sensation, as well as inresponse selection. (6) Increased blood flow in the poste-rior parietal and prefrontal cortices is thought to reflectattentional and memory networks activated by noxiousstimulation. (7) Less noted but frequent activation con-cerns motor-related areas such as the striatum, cerebellumand supplementary motor area (SMA), as well as regionsinvolved in pain control such as the periaqueductal grey atbrainstem.

The affective dimension of pain perception was articu-lated by Price[75], and delineated the central network con-sisting of (1) a direct spinal pathways to limbic structuresand medial thalamic nuclei provide direct inputs to brainareas involved in affect, (2) an indirect spinal pathways tosomatosensory thalamic and cortical areas and then through

a cortico-limbic pathway integrates nociceptive input withcontextual information and memory to provide cognitivemediation of pain affect, and (3) both direct and cortico-limbic pathways converge on the same anterior cingulatecortical and subcortical structures whose function may beto establish emotional valence and response priorities.

Based on the knowledge up to 2000, Chen[21] introduceda general model of pain perception in the human brain (seeFig. 1 in the section on mode of pain perception). The painimpulse transmission at brain stem, thalamus, SI and SIIfor ascending activation in sensory-discriminative func-tion and descending regulation. The transaction of aver-sion and affect memory in perception take place at insular,hippocampus, and amygdale nuclei. The translation of cog-nitive attention and execution in pain perception resides atACC for attention/autonomic regulation and posterior cin-gulate cortex (PCC) for pain memory, at the parietal cor-tex for spatial vigilance and PFC for executive function ofcognitive-evaluative function. This model is fairly consis-tent with that articulated by Apkarian et al.[40] from a meta-analysis taking into account of variation in noxious stimuli(electrical, thermal, mechanical), measurement techniques(EEG, PET, fMRI), and sample categories (healthyvolunteers, patients). Acute pain network is shown to in-clude thalamus, SI, SII, ACC, IC and PFC.

In addition to the above brain responses by physical ac-tivation of pain perception, chemical activation in the healthysubjects by ascorbic acid is worthy notice[76]. The tonicnoxious chemical stimulation by ascorbic acid activatesthe cingulate cortex and the putative foot representationarea of SI. Pain-related activation in the majority of sub-jects is shown in SI, cingulate, motor, and premotor cortex,while negative correlations can be found in medial parietal,perigenual cingulate, and medial prefrontal regions.Furthermore, anticipation to pain per se activates distrib-uted cortical parietal, cingulate and frontal regions, prob-ably reflecting the dynamic anxiety in pain activation.

Finally, pain perception in the human brain can be in-voked by psychological activation. Anticipation of chemi-cal nociception[77] enhanced the putative representation areaof the contralateral SI but decreased during anticipation inother portions of the contralateral and ipsilateral SI, and inthe anteroventral cingulate cortex. A top-down process byanticipation seems to modulate cortical systems involvedin sensory and affective components of pain even in theabsence of actual noxious input. It suggests that cognitivefactors can directly influence the activity of cortical noci-ceptive networks.

Another controlled type of psychological activation is

Page 4: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685680

studied by imaged pain[78]. Imaged pain by hypnoticsuggestion, without actual painful stimulation, revealed sig-nificant changes during hypnotically induced pain experi-ence within the thalamus, ACC, IC, and PFC. The findingscompare well with the activation patterns during pain fromknown experimental nociceptive activations.

As an antidote to placebo, nocebo[79, 80] by negative ver-bal suggestions or subjective thoughts can induce antici-patory anxiety about the impending pain increase, whichmay trigger the activation of cholecystokinin (CCK). Inturn, CCK facilitates pain transmission. And CCK-antago-nists have been found to block this anxiety-inducedhyperalgesia.2.2 Experimental activation in patients with pain:pathological sensitizationMost experimental stimuli conducted on the pain patientsinvolve physical activation, some chemical activation, andfew psychological activation. The study goals are simu-lated to probe pathological condition in alteration of experi-mental pain in the patient brain. Thus, it allows isolationand identification of brain of pathological changes in patients.

To begin with, the brain of pain patients may be some-times maladaptive to the persist or even chronic pain con-ditions in response to peripheral and central nervous sys-tem injury, as in allodynia behavior. Abnormal pain evokedby innocuous stimuli in allodynia has been associated withincrease of the thalamic, insular and SII responses, but aparadoxical CBF decrease in ACC[39]. Allodynia is usualphysiological effect, and can induce hyperalgesia in re-sponse to non-noxiously tactile or thermal stimulation inhealthy subjects. Brain responses in allodynia by tactilemechanical stimulation differ slightly from those by ther-mal stimulation. Both of them excite SI, SII, IC and frontalcortices, but add cingulate and somatosensory cortices inthermal hyperalgesia.

In studying neuropathic pain of patients due to periph-eral or central neurological lesions with exhibition ofallodynia, Moisset and Bouhassira[66] from meta-analysisindicate: (1) spontaneous pain is associated with changesof thalamic activity in the medial pain system in relation topain affect, (2) allodynia activation can be varied due toboth intrinsic and extrinsic factors, and (3) there is no uniquepain matrix or allodynia matrix. The brain areas activatedin allodynia of neuropathic pain are seemed well tocicomscribe the general pain matrix: thalamus, and SI, SII,IC, ACC and PFC.2.3 Research in pain patients: fibromyalgesia, head-ache

Recently, the default mode network of brain function[81],i.e. resting brain function devoid of any activation, has beenwell identified in various conditions of brain diseases.Likewise, it becomes necessary to identify the default modenetwork of clinical pain conditions. In this context, thechronic low back pain patients[82] exhibit reduced deactiva-tion in several key default mode network regions, which issuggested to underlie the cognitive and behavioral impair-ments accompanying chronic pain. However, pain per-ception studied in fibromyalgesia patients has been investi-gated to explore probable maladapted brain regions. But,little gross abnormality in brain activities is yet defined[57].

Likewise, studies of patients respectively with severecluster headache, migraine, and headache of persistentnature have come to a general profile[62, 65]. Cluster head-ache is related with the activation of the posterior hypotha-lamic grey matter, migraine with the brainstem area, attime of cortical hyperexcitability and interacts with trigeminalnociceptive system[61].

3 Modes of pain activation in the human brain

It is almost granted that knowledge of the sites of brainactivation is fully necessary, but has to be regarded as notsufficient, in the understanding of pain processing in thehuman brain. After nearly 15 years of intensive pain inves-tigation in neuroimaging, we now have to turn to our spareknowledge in understanding the mechanisms, i.e. mode ofaction, in human pain perception. Perception is singularpsychological construct, with a complex physiologicalphenomenon in the brain, as that of consciousness. Themode of action can involve in several stages: sensory trans-duction and transmission, affective translation, and cogni-tive transaction. The Chen model (Fig. 1) provides a goodspatial and temporal illustration in conception, but is notgood enough to understand how the hierarchy organiza-tion of information processing in parallel function. We havelittle knowledge of the input/output linkage of electrical sig-nals and chemical mediators in this regard at the site ofbrain areas identified for pain processing in the human brain.

The mode of pain perception can be defined in threedistinct conceptual stages and functional neuraxis setsshown in Fig. 1. (1) Sensory transmission at the brainstemarea, also acting in descending regulation, and the thalamicrelay nuclei, while sensory discrimination of spatial/tem-poral features in SI cortex. (2) Affect transaction at theSII, IC, amygdala and hippocampus cortices for aversivelearning, visceral nociception, and autonomic effect. (3)

Page 5: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

681Andrew CN CHEN: Pain Perception and its Genesis in the Human Brain

Fig. 1. A general model of pain perception in the human brain. Transmission of sensory nociception is largely considered as serial processingfrom brainstem (BS), thalamus (T), SI and SII. This sensory-perceptual system participates in the cerebral functions of detection, localization,timing, learning, relay, integration, ascending transmission and descending regulation of pain. The transaction of painful emotion is probablyconsidered as parallel processing at sites of insular cortex (IC), amygdala (A), hippocampus (Hip), hypothalamus (H) and cingular cortex (Cg).This affective-motivational system participates in the functions of affective reaction, visceral activation, multi-sensory integration, homeosta-sis regulation, fear and memory. Translation of painful cognition is assumed to be carried out by the integration of several cortical areas inprefrontal cortex (PFC), primary motor cortex (MI), supplementary motor cortex (SMA) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). This cognitive-evaluation system participates in the functions of executive control, co-ordination of action/intention and spatial/bodily attention. Nevertheless,the biophysical and physiological mechanisms of these pain processings in the brain still remain unknown[7].

Cognitive attention at the ACC, PFC, as well in responseselection or action planning. This extensive network, widelydistributed in the human brain, in pain perception may op-erate in hierarchy or in parallel under both spatial-temporaldynamics. The mode of action in pain perception is con-jectured after empirical verification at present.

In terms of time scale at the initial stage of pain activa-tion in the brain, the bilateral areas are near sylvian featurecommand special attention and considered as the first cor-tical relay station in the central processing of noxious stimuli[13].Using somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in responseto intracutaneous galvanic stimulations, the measured cor-tical topographic maps display clear different activationmaps between non-painful and painful stimuli. At the middlelatency phase (70-140 ms), the anatomical substrates areisolated and identified. In fact, this recent study (Chen andTheuvenet, paper in submission) has identified both tem-poral differentiation and spatial segregation of SII and IC.The contralateral SII source at 93 ms and the N114 IC areseparated by 21 ms in time, and segregated by a Euclidianspace of 23.2 mm in distance. The model of the IC as the

generator of pain perception in the discrete noxious activa-tion in the brain is illustrated in Fig. 2, and the dipole iden-tifications in Fig. 3.

4 Conclusions

In 15 years of pain research of pain perception in the hu-man brain by non-invasive EEG/MEG tomography and PET/fMRI tomography, a network of wide distributed brainareas is identified as sites in processing noxious activation.These core areas include thalamus relay in sensorytransmission, the somatotopic organization of SI serves insensory discrimination of spatial and temporal nociceptiveevent, with SII in recognition, memory, and learning ofpain information. The IC involves in affective-motivationfunction to aversion and autonomic effect. The ACC par-ticipates mainly in attention and response selection. ThePCC consolidates affective memory and emotional displays.The PPC acts in sensory registration and selectedawareness. The PFC commands in executive control overcognitive-evaluative aspect of pain perception. The sites

Page 6: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685682

of pain perception are well replicated in studies of healthysubjects. In patients of fibromyalgesia and neuropathic pain,discrete brain mal-adaption in noxious processing has yetto be specifically identified. Nevertheless, cluster migraineof enhanced activation in the brainstem area and clusterheadache at the posterior hypothalamus grey matter are

considered to be pain-related disease specific. Given themode of function, subscribed than empirically verified ineach brain area for pain perception, much less is reallyknown in the virtual mechanisms of pain processing withinthe nociceptive neural network. By controlled discrete in-tracutaneous galvanic stimulation, we contend that IC at

Fig. 2. Scalp field potentials at three stages (73 ms, 93 ms, 114 ms) in middle latency. At the peak 73 ms in the middle latency period, a maincontralateral centro-medial negativity was focalized. At the peak 93 ms and 114 ms stages, three main focal negativities (contralateral centro-medial site, contralateral left temporal site, ipsilateral right temporal site) were isolated along with the associated medial-frontal positivity atthe forehead. At 114 ms, the somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) recorded at the focal electrodes showed the largest activity (note the 2-fold increase in scale bars) and was located at the centro-medial site. While the SEPs at the contralateral site was larger than the ipsilateral site(redlines denote the study time at 114 ms), the ipsilateral peak latency lagged 13 ms compared to the contralateral peak (Chen and Theuvenet,paper in submission).

Page 7: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

683Andrew CN CHEN: Pain Perception and its Genesis in the Human Brain

Fig. 3. Correlation of scalp potentials, dipole sites and insular structure.Three main dipoles are depicted as the generators for the respectivetopographic maxima in Fig. 2, and the anatomical substrates identi-fied are respectively the ipsilateral lentiform cortex, vertex SMA andcontralateral main insular generator in response to painful galvanicstimulation. The inlet image displays a real post-mortem brain, withthe lateral cortex dissected to reveal the shape, size, and position ofthe insular cortex (courtesy of Digital Anatomist Project, Universityof Washington, Seattle, USA). The virtual relation of the left insulardipole is indicated by red arrow. This supports the role of the insularcortex in the genesis of pain perception in human (Chen andTheuvenet, paper in submission).

the middle latency stage (70-140 ms) may be a valid can-didate for the generator of pain perception in the humanbrain.

REFERENCES

1 Chen AC. Human brain measures of clinical pain: a review. I.Topographic mappings. Pain 1993; 54 (2): 115-132.

2 Chen AC. Human brain measures of clinical pain: a review. II.Tomographic imagings. Pain 1993; 54 (2): 133-144.

3 Casey KL. Match and mismatch: identifying the neuronal deter-minants of pain. Ann Intern Med 1996 124 (11): 995-998.

4 Jones AK, Derbyshire SW. Cerebral mechanisms operating inthe presence and absence of inflammatory pain. Ann Rheum Dis1996; 55 (7): 411-420.

5 Kramer U, Nevo Y, Harel S. Electroencephalography in theevaluation of headache patients: a review. Isr J Med Sci 1997; 33(12): 816-820.

6 Frishberg BM. Neuroimaging in presumed primary headache

disorders. Semin Neurol 1997; 17 (4): 373-382.7 Bromm B, Lorenz J. Neurophysiological evaluation of pain.

Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1998; 107 (4): 227-253.8 Ingvar M. Pain and functional imaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond

B Biol Sci 1999; 354 (1387): 1347-1358.9 Casey KL. Forebrain mechanisms of nociception and pain: analy-

sis through imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999; 96 (14): 668-674.

10 Treede RD, Vogel H, Rios M, Krauss G, Lesser RP, Lenz FA.Pain-related evoked potentials from parasylvian cortex in humans.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 1999; 49: 250-254.

11 Treede RD, Kenshalo DR, Gracely RH, Jones AK. The corticalrepresentation of pain. Pain 1999; 79 (2-3): 105-111.

12 Schnitzler A, Ploner M. Neurophysiology and functional neu-roanatomy of pain perception. J Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 17(6): 592-603.

13 Treede RD, Apkarian AV, Bromm B, Greenspan JD, Lenz FA.Cortical representation of pain: functional characterization ofnociceptive areas near the lateral sulcus. Pain 2000; 87 (2): 113-119.

14 Kakigi R, Watanabe S, Yamasaki H. Pain-related somatosensoryevoked potentials. J Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 17 (3): 295-308.

15 Derbyshire SW. Exploring the pain “neuromatrix”. Curr RevPain 2000; 4 (6): 467-477.

16 Casey KL. Concepts of pain mechanisms: the contribution offunctional imaging of the human brain. Prog Brain Res 2000; 129:277-287.

17 Wiech K PH, Birbaumer N. Neuroimaging of chronic pain: phan-tom limb and musculoskeletal pain. Scand J Rheumatol Suppl2000; 113: 13-18.

18 Aziz Q, Schnitzler A, Enck P. Functional neuroimaging of vis-ceral sensation. J Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 17 (6): 604-612.

19 Cutrer FM, O’Donnell A, Sanchez Del Rio M. Functionalneuroimaging: enhanced understanding of migrainepathophysiology. Neurology 2000; 55 (9 Suppl 2): S36-S45.

20 Bradley LA, McKendree-Smith NL, Alberts KR, Alarcón GS,Mountz JM, Deutsch G. Use of neuroimaging to understandabnormal pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia. Curr Rheumatol Rep2000; 2(2): 141-148.

21 Chen AC. New perspectives in EEG/MEG brain mapping andPET/fMRI neuroimaging of human pain. Int J Psychophysiol2001; 42(2): 147-159.

22 Bromm B. Brain images of pain. News Physiol Sci 2001; 16:244-249.

23 Goadsby PJ. Neuroimaging in headache. Microsc Res Tech 2001;53 (3): 179-187.

24 Rainville P, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. Representation of acuteand persistent pain in the human CNS: potential implications forchemical intolerance. Ann NY Acad Sci 2001; 933: 130-141.

25 Price DD, Barrell JJ, Rainville P. Integrating experiential-phenomenological methods and neuroscience to study neural

Page 8: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685684

mechanisms of pain and consciousness. Conscious Cogn 2002; 11(4): 593-608.

26 Rainville P. Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation.Curr Opin Neurobiol 2002; 12 (2): 195-204.

27 Kakigi R, Tran TD, Qiu Y, Wang X, Nguyen TB, Inui K, WatanabeS, Hoshiyama M. Cerebral responses following stimulation ofunmyelinated C-fibers in humans: electro- and magneto-encephalographic study. Neurosci Res 2003; 45 (3): 255-275.

28 Garcia-Larrea L, Frot M, Valeriani M. Brain generators of laser-evoked potentials: from dipoles to functional significance.Neurophysiol Clin 2003; 33 (6): 279-292.

29 Lorenz J, Garcia-Larrea L. Contribution of attentional and cogni-tive factors to laser evoked brain potentials. Neurophysiol Clin2003; 33 (6): 293-301.

30 May A. Headache: lessons learned from functional imaging. BrMed Bull 2003; 65: 223-234.

31 Derbyshire SW. Visceral afferent pathways and functional brainimaging. ScientificWorldJournal 2003; 3: 1065-1080.

32 Mackey SC, Maeda F. Functional imaging and the neural sys-tems of chronic pain. Neurosurg Clin N Am 2004; 15 (3): 269-288.

33 Mechtler L. Role of neuroimaging in our understanding of thepathogenesis of primary headaches. Curr Pain Headache Rep2004; 8 (5): 404-409.

34 Sánchez del Rio M, Alvarez Linera J. Functional neuroimaging ofheadaches. Lancet Neurol 2004; 3 (11): 645-651.

35 Chiapparini L, Ciceri E, Nappini S, Castellani MR, Mea E,Bussone G, Leone M, Savoiardo M. Headache and intracranialhypotension: neuroradiological findings. Neurol Sci 2004; 25Suppl 3: S138-S141.

36 May A. The contribution of functional neuroimaging to primaryheadaches. Neurol Sci 2004; 25 Suppl 3: S85-S88.

37 Davis KD. Neuroimaging of pain. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol 2004;57: 72-77.

38 Kakigi R, Inui K, Tamura Y. Electrophysiological studies onhuman pain perception. Clin Neurophysiol 2005; 116 (4): 743-763.

39 Lorenz J, Casey KL. Imaging of acute versus pathological pain inhumans. Eur J Pain 2005; 9 (2): 163-165.

40 Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK. Humanbrain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health anddisease. Eur J Pain 2005; 9 (4): 463-484.

41 Cohen AS, Goadsby PJ. Functional neuroimaging of primaryheadache disorders. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2005; 9 (2): 141-146.

42 May A. The role of imaging in the pathophysiology and diagno-sis of headache. Curr Opin Neurol 2005; 18 (3): 293-297.

43 Tracey I. Nociceptive processing in the human brain. Curr OpinNeurobiol 2005; 15 (4): 478-487.

44 de Leeuw R, Albuquerque R, Okeson J, Carlson C. The contri-bution of neuroimaging techniques to the understanding of su-praspinal pain circuits: implications for orofacial pain. Oral Surg

Oral Med Oral Pathol Oral Radiol Endod 2005; 100 (3): 308-314.45 Favier I, Haan J, Ferrari MD. . Chronic cluster headache: a review.

J Headache Pain 2005; 6 (1): 3-9.46 Rossi P, Serrao M, Perrotta A, Pierelli F, Sandrini G, Nappi G.

Neurophysiological approach to central pain modulation in pri-mary headaches. J Headache Pain 2005; 6 (4): 191-194.

47 Treede RD, Lenz FA. Passing lanes and slow lanes into thenociceptive network of the human brain. Pain 2006; 123 (3): 223-225.

48 Aurora SK, Bowyer SM. New insights into brain dysfunction inmigraine. Expert Rev Neurother 2006; 6 (3): 307-312.

49 May A. A review of diagnostic and functional imaging in headache.J Headache Pain 2006; 7 (4): 174-184.

50 Mee S, Bunney BG, Reist C, Potkin SG, Bunney WE. Psycho-logical pain: a review of evidence. J Psychiatr Res 2006; 40 (8):680-690.

51 Cohen AS, Goadsby PJ. Functional neuroimaging of primaryheadache disorders. Expert Rev Neurother 2006; 6 (8): 1159-1171.

52 Derbyshire SW. Burning questions about the brain in pain. Pain2006; 1226 (3): 217-218.

53 Anderson WS, O’Hara S, Lawson HC, Treede RD, Lenz FA.Plasticity of pain-related neuronal activity in the human thalamus.Prog Brain Res 2006; 157: 353-364.

54 Schweinhardt P, Lee M, Tracey I. Imaging pain in patients: is itmeaningful? Curr Opin Neurol 2006; 19 (4): 392-400.

55 Borsook D, Burstein R, Moulton E, Becerra L. Functional imag-ing of the trigeminal system: applications to migrainepathophysiology. Headache 2006; 46 Suppl 1: S32-S38.

56 Detsky ME, McDonald DR, Baerlocher MO, Tomlinson GA,McCrory DC, Booth CM. Does this patient with headache havea migraine or need neuroimaging? JAMA 2006; 296 (10): 1274-1283.

57 Williams DA, Gracely RH. Biology and therapy of fibromyalgia.Functional magnetic resonance imaging findings in fibromyalgia.Arthritis Res Ther 2006; 8 (6): 224.

58 Jackson PL, Rainville P, Decety J. To what extent do we sharethe pain of others? Insight from the neural bases of pain empathy.Pain 2006; 125 (1-2): 5-9.

59 Mayer EA, Naliboff BD, Craig AD. Neuroimaging of the brain-gut axis: from basic understanding to treatment of functional GIdisorders. Gastroenterology 2006; 131 (6): 1925-1942.

60 Duquette M, Roy M, Leporé F, Peretz I, Rainville P. Cerebralmechanisms involved in the interaction between pain and emotion.Rev Neurol (Paris) 2007; 163 (2): 169-179.

61 DaSilva AF, Goadsby PJ, Borsook D. Cluster headache: a re-view of neuroimaging findings. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2007; 11(2): 131-136.

62 May A, Matharu M. New insights into migraine: application offunctional and structural imaging. Curr Opin Neurol 2007; 20 (3):

Page 9: Pain perception and its genesis in the human brain · PDF file678 Acta Physiologica Sinica, October 25, 2008, 60 (5): 677-685 pain[9]. Treede (1999): Pain-related evoked potentials

685Andrew CN CHEN: Pain Perception and its Genesis in the Human Brain

306-309.63 Cook DB, Stegner AJ, McLoughlin MJ. Imaging pain of

fibromyalgia. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2007; 11 (3): 190-200.64 May A. Neuroimaging: visualising the brain in pain. Neurol Sci

2007; 28 Suppl 2: S101-107.65 Leone M, Proietti Cecchini A, Mea E, Curone M, Tullo V,

Casucci G, Bonavita V, Bussone G. Functional neuroimagingand headache pathophysiology: new findings and new prospects.Neurol Sci 2007; 28 Suppl 2: S108-S113.

66 Moisset X, Bouhassira D. Brain imaging of neuropathic pain.Neuroimage 2007; 37 Suppl 1: S80-S88.

67 Tracey I, Mantyh PW. The cerebral signature for pain percep-tion and its modulation. Neuron 2007; 55 (3): 377-391.

68 Linder SL. Post-traumatic headache. Curr Pain Headache Rep2007; 11 (5): 396-400.

69 Veldhuijzen DS, Greenspan JD, Kim JH, Coghill RC, TreedeRD, Ohara S, Lenz FA. Imaging central pain syndromes. CurrPain Headache Rep 2007; 11 (3): 183-189.

70 Derbyshire SW, Osborn J. Modeling pain circuits: how imagingmay modify perception. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 2007; 17 (4):485-493.

71 Derbyshire SW. Imaging visceral pain. Curr Pain Headache Rep2007; 11 (3): 178-182.

72 Wood PB. Role of central dopamine in pain and analgesia. ExpertRev Neurother 2008; 8 (5): 781-797.

73 Tracey I. Imaging pain. Br J Anaesth 2008; 101(1): 32-39.74 Peyron R, Laurent B, García-Larrea L. Functional imaging of

brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. NeurophysiolClin 2000; 30 (5): 263-288.

75 Price DD. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affectivedimension of pain. Science 2000; 288: 1769-1772.

76 Porro CA, Cettolo V, Francescato MP, Baraldi P. Temporal andintensity coding of pain in human cortex. J Neurophysiol 1998;80 (6): 3312-3320.

77 Porro CA, Baraldi P, Pagnoni G, Serafini M, Facchin P, MaieronM, Nichelli P. Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nocicep-tive systems? J Neurosci 2002; 22 (8): 3206-3214.

78 Derbyshire SW, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA. Cere-bral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain.Neuroimage 2004; 23 (1): 392-401.

79 Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When words arepainful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuro-science 2007; 147 (2): 260-271.

80 Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in noceboand placebo effects. Pain 2008; 136 (1-2): 211-218.

81 Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a briefhistory of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 2007; 37 (4): 1083-1090.

82 Baliki MN, Geha PY, Apkarian AV, Chialvo DR. Beyond feeling:chronic pain hurts the brain, disrupting the default-mode net-work dynamics. J Neurosci 2008; 28 (6): 1398-1403.