Top Banner
Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1) 171–197 © The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: http://www. sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0272431609350923 http://jea.sagepub.com Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups Eric S. Buhs, 1 Meredith McGinley, 2 and Michael D.Toland 3 Abstract This study examined the factorial invariance and construct validity equival- ence of a self-report of victimization and exclusion (SVEX) for Latino and European American early adolescent participants (fifth and sixth grades; mean age 11.3).The instrument included an expanded set of relational victimization items that more thoroughly tapped exclusion behaviors relevant to dev- elopmental and cross-cultural use. Confirmatory factor analyses techniques demonstrated acceptable (partial) factorial invariance across ethnic groups, fifth and sixth graders, and across gender. Linkages between the SVEX scores, peer nominations, internalizing indices, and three demographic variables also supported construct validity equivalence for the SVEX. Findings supported 1 University of Nebraska–Lincoln 2 University of Illinois at Chicago 3 University of Kentucky Corresponding Author: Eric S. Buhs, 226 Teachers College Hall, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, 68588 Email: [email protected] by guest on February 9, 2016 jea.sagepub.com Downloaded from
27

Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

May 16, 2023

Download

Documents

Ovamir Anjum
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Journal of Early Adolescence30(1) 171 –197

© The Author(s) 2010Reprints and permission: http://www. sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

DOI: 10.1177/0272431609350923http://jea.sagepub.com

Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Eric S. Buhs,1 Meredith McGinley,2 and Michael D. Toland3

Abstract

This study examined the factorial invariance and construct validity equival­ence of a self­report of victimization and exclusion (SVEX) for Latino and European American early adolescent participants (fifth and sixth grades; mean age 11.3). The instrument included an expanded set of relational victimization items that more thoroughly tapped exclusion behaviors relevant to dev­elopmental and cross­cultural use. Confirmatory factor analyses techniques demonstrated acceptable (partial) factorial invariance across ethnic groups, fifth and sixth graders, and across gender. Linkages between the SVEX scores, peer nominations, internalizing indices, and three demographic variables also supported construct validity equivalence for the SVEX. Findings supported

1University of Nebraska–Lincoln2University of Illinois at Chicago3University of Kentucky

Corresponding Author:Eric S. Buhs, 226 Teachers College Hall, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, 68588Email: [email protected]

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

172 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

a two­factor model similar to that of Crick and colleagues (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1995) and suggested that the instrument provided an acceptable level of equivalence for overt and relational victimization forms across these groups.

Keywords

Victimization, ethnicity, invariance, exclusion

Investigations of peer aggression and victimization suggest that a significant proportion of children and youth are regularly victimized in school (e.g., app­roximately 8%­10% of school age children, see Nansel et al., 2001; Olweus, 1994) and indicate that frequent victims of peer aggression are more likely than less victimized peers to experience short­ and long­term adjustment problems (Boivin & Hymel, 1997; Buhs, Ladd, & Herald, 2006; Hodges & Perry, 1999). Evidence also demonstrates that youth victimize peers using varied forms of aggression, including overt (e.g., physical abuse, verbal taunts) and relational (behaviors intended to damage others’ relationships) types. Crick and colleagues’ (Crick, 1995; Crick & Grotpeter, 1996), for example, developed overt (direct physical and verbal victimization) and rela­tional aggression (indirect attempts to damage relationships) constructs and operationalizations that are well established and widely used in research (see Crick et al., 1999, for a review). Despite this work and the numerous empirical studies of these forms of peer victimization, important aspects of victimiza­tion instruments used to measure these constructs have not been adequately examined.

While a number of published self­report instruments tap victimization constructs (e.g., Crick & Bigbee, 1998; Dill, Vernberg, Fonagy, Twemlow, & Gamm, 2004; Hawker & Boulton, 2000; Hodges & Perry, 1999; Mynard & Joseph, 2000; Nishina & Juvonen, 2005; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003; Schwartz, Gorman, Nakamoto, & Toblin, 2005), few studies have examined the equivalence of these measures across the various groups (i.e., examining properties beyond means, standard deviations, and internal consistency) that are an emerging focus of much victimization research. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) has helped create useful methods for evaluating the hypoth­esized factor structures and scale/item invariance across groups. Construct validity equivalence, in addition to factorial invariance, requires an examina­tion of the similarities of the intercepts and slopes of the latent constructs on related constructs across the groups of interest. Factorial invariance and con­struct validity equivalence are important to establishing the comparability of

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 173

observed score differences across groups, yet they have not been examined in aggression/victimization instruments (see Knight & Hill, 1998, for a discus­sion of measurement equivalence).

While there are studies available in the literature that have carefully exam­ined victimization across gender groups (Crick et al., 1999; Underwood, 2003) and within various ethnic contexts (see Graham, Taylor, & Ho, 2009), there has not been a parallel focus on measurement equivalence. Similarly, the empirical picture of potential victimization changes linked to school structure and development (e.g., from fifth to sixth grade) is also less clear and studies have presented conflicting findings (see Galen & Underwood, 1997; Underwood, 2003). The accuracy of the instruments used may be a factor in these disparate findings as well. Other examinations of victimiza­tion (Underwood, Scott, Galperin, Bjornstad, & Sexton, 2004) indicated that social exclusion may also be an important aspect of relational victimization. Exclusion behaviors have not been adequately tapped by most current instru­ments and this may also have contributed to poorer measurement of victimization.

Examinations of group differences in victimization and related phenomena have thus been hampered by two central problems addressed here: (a) the ins­truments used may not measure culturally or grade­level appropriate aspects of the constructs that would allow potential group differences to be accurately identified (i.e., non–European American groups, see Peña, 2007, for a review of concerns with cross­cultural research in developmental psychology) and (b) the cross­group equivalence of these instruments (i.e., factorial invariance and construct validity equivalence) have not been thoroughly examined across groups of interest. Both problems need to be addressed for empirical work to adequately establish that group differences/similarities uncovered accurately reflect differences and are not measurement artifacts.

Victimization Constructs and Hypothesized Factor StructureThe current study examined a two­factor model of victimization similar to that of Crick (1995). Relational victimization refers to behaviors intended to harm through “purposeful manipulation and damage of their peer relation­ships” (Crick, 1995, p. 711). Relational victimization is typically tapped by items such as “tells lies about you to make other kids not like you anymore” and “keeps others from liking you by saying mean things about you” (Crick & Grotpeter, 1996). Overt victimization factors typically include physical (e.g., hitting) and verbal behaviors (e.g., insults) intended to directly harm others. Relational and overt victimization tend to be moderately to highly

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

174 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

correlated, but evidence suggests that they are distinct aspects independently linked to adjustment (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Loeber & Hay, 1997; Vaillancourt, Brendgen, Boivin, & Tremblay, 2003). Crick and others hypoth­esized that overt victimization addresses goals or themes of physical dominance and instrumentality and differs from relational forms that address relational themes (i.e., “establishing close, intimate connections” Crick & Grotpeter, 1995, p. 710).

Augmenting the relational victimization construct: social exclusion. We propose that exclusion behaviors related to sets of themes or goals that are likely salient to cross­cultural (e.g., collectivist beliefs–see below), gender and develop­mental differences in victimization have not been adequately addressed in previous instruments. Findings suggest that exclusion may be central to more accurate conceptions of victimization (e.g., Buhs et al., 2006; Bukowski & Sippola, 2001; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003; Underwood et al., 2004). Exclusion may be especially important in school contexts because it has been linked to later academic and social–emotional adjustment (Buhs, 2005; Buhs et al., 2006). Though relational victimization variables may include items describing exclusion (e.g., Crick, 1995), they have typically included too few to adequately address these behaviors and may have displayed construct underrepresentation (Messick, 1995). Although direct examination of group differences was not a focus of the current study and the limited sample size made extended analyses addressing mean­level group differences untenable, establishing an acceptable level of equivalence for an instrument that repre­sents exclusion behaviors will be an important component of future studies of such differences. Sandstrom and Cillessen (2003) constructed a measure with an exclusion construct (e.g., being ignored, not being allowed to join a game), though they used a daily diary method that differed from traditional self­reports. They modeled exclusion as a distinct factor (i.e., exclusion, overt and relational factors). CFA analyses from this study indicated good fit and showed that the exclusion factor moderately correlated with relational aggression—because of this, a three­factor structure was also examined here (below).

Factorial Invariance, construct validity equivalence and potential gender, ethnic and grade-level differences. Researchers (see Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006; Underwood, 2003) continue to debate the impact of gender on victimization, though the general contention that gender is linked to group differences has received empirical support. The “two cultures theory” (Maccoby, 1998) suggests that boys and girls develop distinct cultu­res within the segregated gender groups typical in childhood and adolescence. These differences are construed as impacting the goals and themes related to

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 175

victimization. Accordingly, girls’ peer relations tend to display a greater focus on interrelatedness and group inclusion than boys. If this is so, then we sug­gest that a greater focus on exclusion behaviors (see Underwood et al., 2004) in measurements of victimization across gender groups is warranted. This also suggests it is possible that girls view different behaviors as evidence of overt and relational victimization forms—test of factorial invariance will examine the current instrument for potential differences in item function across groups. Construct validity equivalence analyses examined hypothe­sized links to peer nominations of similar victimization behaviors and to internalizing outcomes (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Hoglund, 2007).

Following parallel conceptual arguments to those presented for gender differences, we argue that the potential differences in victimization forms and behaviors need to be examined across ethnic groups. First, several aspects of culturally relevant theory suggest that appropriate victimization constructs need to adequately tap exclusion behaviors in Latino groups. Latino culture has frequently been described as more interdependent/collectivist than typi­cal European American groups (Leyendecker & Lamb, 1999). The values of Simpatia (conformity, empathy, and harmony in family/community relations; Marin & Marin, 1991) and Personalismo (social warmth and reciprocity with family/group; Cuellar, Arnold, & González, 1995) suggest that these themes and behaviors are more heavily emphasized by Latinos. Evidence that Latino youth tend to display higher levels of prosocial behavior than European Americans (e.g., Knight & Kagan, 1977; Schwartz, Zamboanga, & Jarvis, 2007) can also be construed as evidence of a collectivist orientation. Given these differences Latino youth may thus display different victimization behav­iors (i.e., more subtle, less severe). Graham (2006) has also suggested that when the aggression/victimization originates within one’s own, numerical­majority group (Latino students in this study comprised the majority proportion of their groups) these nonnormative aspects of victimization contribute to a greater significance for those behaviors in that context (i.e., the misfit hypothesis—see also Bellmore, Nishina, Witkow, Graham, & Juvonen, 2007). Relational victimization and exclusion, in particular, may thus com­municate stronger messages about group membership and adherence to group norms (Bukowski & Sippola, 2001).

This conceptual framework and related findings do not only present us with precise hypotheses about the frequency/rate of victimization in Latino groups but also about the potential form and relative effects of victimization. Given the emergent state of examinations of victimization in Latino contexts and the differences in prosocial behaviors, it is unclear whether rates among Latinos might be higher or lower than other groups (some findings suggest

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

176 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

lower relative rates of general victimization in early adolescence; see Graham et al., 2009)—the measure developed here may allow more accurate estimates of self­reported rates and links to adjustment. For the purposes of our con­struct equivalence analyses, we expected that the factor means would be similar across ethnic groups and that the relationships to internalizing out­comes would be slightly greater for Latinos relative to European Americans.

Hypotheses about victimization linked to grade level and school structure are also relevant to the current study. Early adolescence and the transition to middle school is the period where cliques and crowd structures emerge (Brown, Mory, & Kinney, 1994; Collins & Steinberg, 2006). These are con­texts where we expected relational victimization/exclusion to be salient as youth develop more complex groups and as adult sanctions for overt aggres­sion also tend to become more severe—evidence suggests that overt aggression thus becomes less frequent relative to relational forms. Findings also suggest that the reorganization of peer groups at the transition to middle school affects victimization in these grade­level groups (Long & Pelligrini, 2003) and that exclusion may play a central role in this context (Bukowski & Sip­pola, 2001; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003; Underwood et al., 2004). We argue that greater representation of exclusion behaviors in self­reports used with these grade­level groups is thus required. We also hypothesize that our con­struct validity equivalence analyses may display greater relational victimization means for sixth graders relative to fifth graders (because of potential gender difference and because we lack the sample size to examine gender groups within/across grade level, we were unable to examine hypotheses about overt vs. relational victimization means).

Self­reports that adequately represent overt and relational victimization thus need to be developed, evaluated, and will need to demonstrate equiva­lence across gender, ethnic, and grade­level contexts. The invariance analyses presented here were one important step in establishing some of the measure­ment components that will help ascertain whether group differences are measurement artifacts or accurately reflect observed differences (see Knight & Hill, 1998; Knight, Tein, Prost, & Gonzales, 2002 for a discussion of mea­surement equivalence).

The Current StudySelf-report of victimization. Numerous victimization instruments have been

introduced by scholars (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Mynard & Joseph, 2000; Hodges & Perry, 1999; Nishina & Juvonen, 2005; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003; Schwartz et al., 2005). Self­reports, in particular, are commonly used

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 177

with children and adolescents (Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Ladd & Kochenderfer­Ladd, 2002; Pellegrini & Bartini, 2000; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003) and we examined one here for several reasons. First, observers may be less accurate raters in early adolescence because of the intimate knowledge of peer relation­ships required to make accurate judgments about the peers’ intentions (e.g., differentiating between friendly and aggressive teasing). Second, relative to observations or teacher/peer reports, self­reports allow for a more parsimoni­ous application of invariance analyses (e.g., as compared to teacher ratings that may reflect greater group biases). Finally, as noted above, self­reports allow reporting of victimization conducted outside of observers’ view; a skill adoles­cents may develop to a greater degree than younger children. Because findings indicate that self­report data may include significant biases affecting validity (Ladd & Kochenderfer­Ladd, 2002; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003), our evalu­ation of validity evidence included data from other sources (i.e., peer reports).

The current instrument includes items common to other self­reports of victimization but differs from others primarily by including more items tap­ping exclusion. Prior self­reports included few such items that described behaviors that were clear, observable markers of exclusion (e.g., the SEQ­S included a single exclusion item; Crick & Grotpeter, 1996—see Storch, Crisp, Roberti, Bagner, & Masia­Warner, 2005, for a CFA examination of the Social Experience Questionaire ­ Self Report Form (SEQ­S). The current instrument included more items describing highly public displays of exclu­sion (see Table 1) so that this might be more accurately reflected in our operationalization of the construct.

Ethnicity. The current study drew Latino and European American partici­pants from small cities in predominantly rural, agricultural regions. Latino populations have increased rapidly in the regions that were the context of the current study (Gouveia, Carranza, & Cogua, 2005), and the proportion of Latino students in many schools in these areas is rapidly increasing. The poten­tial differences in victimization experiences for Latino groups discussed above suggest that instruments that allow for more accurate detection of differences will contribute to more culturally sensitive educational and social support and interventions.

Grade level. Measures of victimization need to be able to produce valid and reliable scores at the transition to middle school because victimization appears to be frequently used to establish and maintain group structures across this transition (Long & Pelligrini, 2003). Fifth and sixth graders were selected here because, in each of the participating school districts, this range included the transition from several elementary schools to a single middle school and allowed us to examine instrument properties for students on either side of this transition.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

178 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Table 1. Self­Reported Victimization Items, Alphas, and CFA Standardized Factor Loadings, Item Error Variances, and Item R2

Item error Item Items Loading variance R2

Item stem: How often do the kids at school do the following:

Overt victimization (a = .89) Threaten to hurt or beat you up? .63 .61 .39Tease you in a mean way or say .82 .34 .66 rude things to you?

Push you around at school? .80 .37 .64Hit, kick, or push you in a mean .65 .58 .42 way at school?

Insult you in order to hurt you? .75 .43 .57Hit you?a — Call you bad names? .79 .37 .63Say mean things to you?a —

Relational aggression (a = .91) Relational victimization items (subscale a = .84)say they won’t be your friend .61 .63 .38 in order to hurt you or get their way?

say bad things about you to other kids .76 .42 .58 at school?

stop talking to you or ignore you .64 .59 .41 when they are mad at you?

tell other kids not to be your friend? .76 .43 .57tell lies, gossips or spread bad rumors .76 .42 .58 about you?

Social Exclusion Items (subscale a = .86) leave you out of what they are doing? .72 .48 .52not pick you for a group activity .65 .58 .42 or game?

not invite you to a party or social .62 .62 .38 gathering?

leave you out of conversations, games .82 .33 .67 or activities?

choose not to sit near you .80 .36 .64 when you want to sit with them?

Note: All standardized factor pattern loadings were significant at the p < .05 significance level.a. Items dropped (see text).

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 179

Gender. Prior conceptual frameworks and empirical findings have sugges­ted differences in frequency and type of victimization linked to gender (see Crick at al., 1999; Underwood, 2003, for reviews)—the evidence is mixed, but some findings suggest that victimization may differ in female groups rela­tive to males. Our examination of factorial invariance and construct equivalence across gender groups was important because these conflicting findings from prior studies need to be further explored with instruments established as capa­ble of accurately detecting potential gender­linked differences.

MethodParticipants

Data used here were gathered from 270 Latino and European American chil­dren (160 girls, 144 Latinos) who were part of a larger investigation of children’s peer relations and school adjustment. They were assessed in public schools (fifth and sixth grades, M age = 11.3, SD = .69) located in two small Midwestern cities located in rural areas. Written, informed parental consent and youth assent was obtained from all participants. Sample ethnic composi­tion was 0.7% African American, 1.0% Asian American, 48% European American, 42.4% Hispanic, and 7.9% other (only European American and Latino participant data were used here). The proportion of students in the two districts who were eligible for government subsidized lunch programs were 75.1% and 33.6%, respectively. Students completed questionnaires adminis­tered by the first author and trained graduate assistants in classroom/grade level groups (10 fifth­grade classrooms, 5 sixth­grade groups). All fifth­ and sixth­grade students were invited to participate. Participation rates ranged from 64% to 92%. Teachers indicated that all participating Latino students were perform­ing at or near grade level in English language skills and students completed all measures in English. In classroom groups with Latino students, Latinos com­prised from 56.3% to 100% of the ethnic composition of those classrooms.

MeasuresStudents completed a 27­item Self­report of Victimization and Exclusion (SVEX) that included overt and relational victimization items (six items and five items, res pectively) and social exclusion (five items—included in the rela­tional factor). Items were drawn from established victimization self­reports (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Hawker & Boulton, 2000; Hodges & Perry, 1999;

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

180 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Nishina & Juvonen, 2005; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003; Schwartz et al., 2005) with additional social exclusion items added (see Table 1). Additional items were developed from youth responses to open­ended questions about victim­ization from previous pilot data with a subset of these participants. Students were instructed in the use of the 5­point frequency response scale (1 = almost never, 3 = sometimes, 5 = almost always) regarding their victimization at school (Overt: M = 1.73, SD = .85; Relational: M = 1.96, SD =.87)

Victimization: peer reports. Peer nominations of victimization were gathered from participants using peer rosters of classmates. Students made limited nom­inations (three per item) of peers on four items tapping overt (one verbal and one physical item, combined), relational, and social exclusion behaviors (one item each, combined). Peer nominations are based on information drawn from multiple raters—single­item scales may thus produce high interrater reliability (see Coie, Terry, Lenox, Lochman, & Hyman, 1995). Students nominated peers who “were often left out of conversations, games and activities” (social exclu­sion), “other kids gossiped about or said bad things about behind their backs” (relational victimization), and who “were hit, pushed and kicked” and “get called bad names” (overt victimization). Scores were summed and standard­ized within groups (Overt: M = –.03, SD = .89; Relational: M = –.03, SD =.85).

Internalizing symptoms. Subsets of the Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression measure (CES­D; Radloff, 1991) and the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC; March, 1997) indicated depressive and anxious symptoms. Both measures are established and have exhibited acceptable reli­ability and validity across the range of participants in the current study (Crockett, Randall, Shen, Russell, & Driscoll, 2005; March & Parker, 2004). Scores on the respective items were summed and the mean taken to create the depression and anxiety scores. The CES­D (five items) included items such as “I felt depressed” and exhibited an alpha of .72 (M = 1.98, SD = .83). The Social Anxiety (e.g., “I worry about other people laughing at me”) scale of the MASC was used to indi­cate anxiety symptoms (8 items; a= .83, M = 1.16, SD = .75).

AnalysesConfirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was used to examine the hypothesized two­factor model (overt and relational victimization factors) and competing one­ and three­factor models while taking measurement error into account. The three­factor model was drawn from Sandstrom and Cillessen (2003—see above) and the one­factor model was examined because of evidence that the overt and relational factors are typically moderately correlated. Prior to test­ing measurement invariance and construct validity equivalence, the fit of the

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 11: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 181

measurement model was assessed using the Satorra­Bentler Scaled c2 (S­Bc2; Satorra & Bentler, 1988), an appropriate test when data is multivariate non­normal (see descriptive statistics below). Because using the chi­square test alone is problematic (see Brown, 2006), fit was evaluated using a set of indi­ces: the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). When using the S­Bc2, a nonsignificant p value is desired. Because the other fit indices lack probability distributions, researchers have suggested cutoffs to determine model fit. Fit was labeled acceptable when the CFI was greater than .90 (Medsker, Williams, & Holahan, 1994), the RMSEA had a value less than .08, and the SRMR had a value less than .10 (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 1998; Weston & Gore, 2006).

Six increasingly restrictive models of measurement invariance (config­ural, metric, strong, strict, latent factor means, and latent factor correlations) were examined for each between­group comparison (see Knight & Hill, 1998; Millsap & Kwok, 2004; Widaman & Reise, 1997). Configural inv­ariance constrained items to associate with the same factors across groups, but factor loadings, unique error variances, and item intercepts were freely estimated. Metric invariance constrained the factor loadings to be equal across groups, but unique error variances and item intercepts were freely estimated. Assuming metric invariance, strong invariance was tested by con­straining item intercepts to be equal across groups, but unique error variances were freely estimated. Given evidence of strong invariance, strict invariance constrained unique item error variances to be equal across groups. Evidence of strong or strict measurement invariance is one component necessary for meaningful comparisons of scores across groups (Gregorich, 2006; Knight & Hill, 1998; Little, 1997; Millsap & Kwok, 2004; Widaman & Reise, 1997).

Construct validity equivalence tests assessed functional and scalar equiv­alence of the victimization scales across groups. First, group mean differences on the latent victimization factors and differences on the relations among the two factors were examined (Table 2). Next, separate structural equation models were used to regress each of the four construct validity variables (depression, anxiety, overt victimization peer nominations, relational victim­ization peer nominations) onto each of the latent self­report factors (see Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Hoglund, 2007 for findings linking internalizing prob­lems to victimization). These analyses compared an unconstrained model to one that constrained the slopes and scale intercepts between the victimization factors and the construct validity scores to be equal across groups (Table 3). Slope and intercept equivalence indicated that a latent factor was associated with the same score on the construct validity variable for each group.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 12: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

182 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Table 2. Chi­Square Difference Tests and Fit Indices for the Measurement Invariance Tests Across Ethnic (Latino and European­American), Gender, and Age Groups for the Two­Factor Model

Model c2 df Dc2a D df CFI RMSEA SRMR

Ethnic group Configural 392.65 206 — — .90 .08 .06 Metric 420.13 220 19.82 14 .89 .08 .08 Strong 457.70 234 38.81* 14 .88 .08 .08 Partial strong 436.45 230 16.40 10 .89 .08 .08 Partial strict 444.89 242 4.71 12 .89 .08 .08 Latent means 448.27 244 3.30 2 .89 .08 .08 Latent 448.67 245 0.24 1 .89 .08 .09

correlationGender group Configural 338.08 206 — — .92 .07 .06 Metric 372.57 220 24.46* 14 .91 .07 .08 Partial metric 363.06 218 19.31 12 .92 .07 .07 Partial strong 395.47 230 32.19* 12 .90 .07 .08 Partial strongb 381.37 229 18.50 11 .91 .07 .07 Partial strict 412.95 242 18.93 13 .90 .07 .08 Latent meansc 425.55 244 13.25* 2 .89 .07 .08 Latent 414.79 243 1.28 1 .90 .07 .10

correlationd

Grade group Configural 382.98 206 — — .90 .08 .06 Metric 406.54 220 18.46 14 .89 .08 .07 Strong 430.42 234 24.89* 14 .89 .08 .07 Partial strong 424.64 233 18.78 13 .89 .08 .07 Partial strict 451.20 248 16.09 15 .88 .08 .08 Latent means 458.44 250 7.15* 2 .88 .08 .08 Partial latent 452.37 249 1.03 1 .88 .08 .08

means Latent 394.38 229 1.45 1 .88 .08 .10

correlation

Note: CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.a. As the Satorra­Bentler scaled chi­square was used, a corrected chi­square difference test was implemented. Thus, the resulting values are not exactly equal to the chi­square of the nested model subtracted from chi­square of the nonnested model.b. The second partial strong model includes an intercept in addition to the initial partial strong model, see text.c. Both latent means were freed between gender groups.d. Model is compared to partial strict model because both means were freed on previous step.*p < .05.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 13: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Tabl

e 3.

Sum

mar

y of

the

Con

stru

ct V

alid

ity E

quiv

alen

ce T

ests

acr

oss

Ethn

ic, G

ende

r, an

d G

rade

Gro

ups

Bc2

df

D S­

Bc2

D df

C

FI

RM

SEA

SR

MR

Ethn

ic g

roup

Anx

iety

Mod

el fr

eely

est

imat

ing

the

inte

rcep

t an

d sl

opes

39

5.40

25

1 —

.9

2 .0

7 .0

8A

ll in

terc

epts

and

slo

pes

cons

trai

ned

403.

86

254

7.36

3

.92

.07

.08

Dep

ress

ion

Mod

el fr

eely

est

imat

ing

the

inte

rcep

t an

d sl

opes

39

4.23

25

1 —

.9

2 .0

7 .0

8A

ll in

terc

epts

and

slo

pes

cons

trai

ned

396.

86

254

2.28

3

.93

.06

.08

Ove

rt N

omin

atio

nM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

395.

92

251

.92

.07

.08

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 40

1.77

25

4 5.

04

3 .9

2 .0

7 .0

8R

elat

iona

l Nom

inat

ion

Mod

el fr

eely

est

imat

ing

the

inte

rcep

t an

d sl

opes

40

2.25

25

1 —

.9

2 .0

7 .0

8A

ll in

terc

epts

and

slo

pes

cons

trai

ned

409.

81

254

7.16

3

.92

.07

.08

Gen

der

Gro

upA

nxie

tyM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

386.

52

252

.93

.06

.08

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 39

1.08

25

5 2.

98

3 .9

2 .0

6 .0

9D

epre

ssio

nM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

374.

65

252

.93

.06

.08

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 37

7.30

25

5 2.

22

3 .9

3 .0

6 .0

8O

vert

Nom

inat

ion

Mod

el fr

eely

est

imat

ing

the

inte

rcep

t an

d sl

opes

38

2.67

25

2 —

.9

3 .0

6 .0

8A

ll in

terc

epts

and

slo

pes

cons

trai

ned

387.

87

255

4.33

3

.93

.06

.09

(con

tinue

d)

183

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 14: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

184

Tabl

e 3.

(co

ntin

ued)

Bc2

df

D S­

Bc2

D df

C

FI

RM

SEA

SR

MR

Rel

atio

nal N

omin

atio

nM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

382.

91

252

.93

.06

.08

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 38

6.02

25

5 3.

08

3 .9

3 .0

6 .0

9G

rade

Gro

upA

nxie

tyM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

437.

99

257

.90

.07

.09

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 44

0.57

26

0 2.

12

3 .9

0 .0

7 .0

9D

epre

ssio

nM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

431.

87

257

.91

.07

.09

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 43

7.43

26

0 4.

26

3 .9

0 .0

7 <.

10O

vert

Nom

inat

ion

Mod

el fr

eely

est

imat

ing

the

inte

rcep

t an

d sl

opes

43

2.19

25

7 —

.9

0 .0

7 .0

9A

ll in

terc

epts

and

slo

pes

cons

trai

ned

434.

97

260

2.33

3

.90

.07

.09

Rel

atio

nal N

omin

atio

nM

odel

free

ly e

stim

atin

g th

e in

terc

ept

and

slop

es

431.

32

257

.91

.07

.09

All

inte

rcep

ts a

nd s

lope

s co

nstr

aine

d 43

5.06

26

0 3.

34

3 .9

1 .0

7 .0

9

*p <

.05.

**p

< .0

.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 15: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 185

A c2 difference test was also used to compare a more restricted (nested) model with measurement invariance constraints (e.g., metric invariance) and a model without such constraints (e.g., configural invariance). When the c2 difference test demonstrated no significant differences, the more restrictive level of invariance was accepted. Conversely, if a significant dif­ference occurred, the less restricted model was retained. Because chi­square

criteria are sensitive to trivial modifications, we also examined whether the Dc2 was accompanied by a meaningful drop in the practical fit indices (see Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Kline, 1998). In the event of unacceptable fit, minor revisions in model structure could be carried out within constraints provided by conceptual frameworks (above) and relevant measurement practice. Modification indices (i.e., the Lagrange Multiplier) were used to identify which parameters (e.g., standardized factor loadings/slopes, item/factor intercepts, etc.) were significantly different across the groups and might be independently estimated. After these parameter estimates were freed, partial invariance was established if follow­up chi­square tests were not significant and fit was similar to the initial model (Byrne, Shavelson, & Muthén, 1989).

ResultsDescriptive Statistics

Item means for the overall sample ranged from 1.40 to 2.23 (SDs ranged from .89 to 1.30). Items were positively skewed (values ranging from .37 to 2.51) and negatively to positively kurtotic (values ranging from –.65 to 5.99). The normalized value of Mardia’s index of multivariate kurtosis was 71.92. This indicated that the distribution was not multivariate normal and the Satorra­Bentler maximum likelihood parameter estimates with standard errors and a mean­adjusted chi­square test statistic was used. In Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2004), the S­Bc2 is referred to as the MLM chi­square (Mplus 5.21 was used to conduct all analyses).

The Factor ModelsAn initial test of the two­factor model showed unsatisfactory fit for the overall sample, S­Bc2(134) = 353.92, p < .01, CFI = .88, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .06. We explored improving model fit by examining items for redundancy and examining the model estimation results. Our conceptual framework and sig­nificantly correlated error estimates for some items suggested modifications.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 16: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

186 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Error terms of the items “How often do the kids in your school hit, kick, or push you in a mean way at school?” and “How often do the kids in your school hit you?” were highly correlated. Because of the similarity/redundancy between them, the latter was dropped from the overt factor. A second overt victimiza­tion item, “Say mean things to you,” correlated with other items and also loaded on the relational factor; because it measured a less precise set of behaviors it was also removed. After removing the two items, the revised model displayed adequate fit for data from the total sample (see Table 4). Standardized factor loadings for the overall two­factor model were positive and significant (.61 to .82; see Table 1). This model was also tested separately for each group; indices indicated adequate fit for all groups (Table 4).

We also evaluated one­factor and three­factor models (above). The two­factor model fit the data better than the one­factor model (one­factor estimates: c2 (104) = 283.05, p < .05, CFI = .89, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .06). Fit for the three­factor model (overt, relational, and exclusion factors), was similar to that of the two­factor model (three­factor estimates: c2 (101) = 210.21, p < .01, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .06, SRMR = .05). To determine the best fitting model, we considered comparisons of the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987) and the BIC Bayesian information criterion (Schwarz, 1978), as well as the collective difference in fit indices. Chi­square difference tests were not implemented because the two­ and three­factor models are not true nested models. In the comparison of two­factor and three­factor models, the

Table 4. Results for the Two­Factor Model for the Overall Sample, European Americans, Latinos, Boys, Girls, Fifth Graders, and Sixth Graders

Model c2 (df = 103) CFI RMSEA SRMR

Overall 234.99* .92 .07 .05Ethnic group European Americans 187.99* .90 .08 .06 Latinos 204.79* .89 .09 .06Gender group Boys 137.58* .95 .06 .05 Girls 206.78* .89 .08 .06Grade group Fifth graders 206.92* .89 .09 .06 Sixth graders 175.97* .91 .07 .06

Note: CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.*p < .05.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 17: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 187

AIC and BIC suggested that the three­factor model fit was not appreciably better than the two­factor model (2 vs. 3; AIC: 10942.65 vs. 10978.547; BIC: 11154.87 vs. 11126.17). In addition, the collective difference in the model fit indices was not remarkable (2 vs. 3: CFI .92 vs. 93; RMSEA .07 vs. .06; SRMR .05 vs. .05). However, there was a greater improvement in fit when comparing the one­ and two­factor models according to the information cri­terion indices (1 vs. 2; AIC: 11052.37 vs. 10978.55; BIC: 11225.09 vs. 11154.87) as well as the collective fit indices (1 vs. 2: CFI .88 vs. 92; RMSEA .08 vs. .07; SRMR .06 vs. .05). Given the greater support and parsimony of the two­factor model, it was retained for all subsequent analyses.

Measurement InvarianceEthnic groups. Results of all measurement invariance analyses are summa­

rized in Table 2. Fit indices for configural invariance indicated adequate fit for the model. A subsequent c2 difference test indicated that metric invari­ance could be assumed across the ethnic groups. Next, a test for strong invariance (constraining item intercepts be equal) returned a significant c2 difference test. Partial strong invariance was then tested, allowing four intercepts to be freely estimated across the European American and Latino groups. Modifica­tion indices suggested that two item intercepts (“Insult you in order to hurt you?” and “Not invite you to a party or social gathering when they know you want to go?”) were higher for European Americans and two others (“Choose not to sit near you when you want to sit with them?” and “Hit, kick, or push you in a mean way at school?”) appeared higher for Latinos (ps < .05). When these intercepts were freely estimated, this partial strong invariance model demonstrated fit that was not significantly different from the metric invari­ance model. Finally, the last model tested partial strict invariance by imposing additional cross ethnic­group equality constraints on all corresponding item error variances (except those of the three items identified above). The overall partial strict invariance model indicated acceptable model fit, and the c2 dif­ference test between this model and the partial strong invariance model was not significant. The partial strict invariance model was retained as the best­fitting model.

Gender groups. A test for configural invariance across gender groups indi­cated adequate model fit (see Table 2). The next model (testing metric invariance) constrained all factor loadings to be equal across groups. The c2

difference test showed a significant loss in fit for this model relative to the configural invariance model. Modification indices suggested that two item loadings (“Threaten to hurt or beat you up?” and “Hit, kick, or push you in a

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 18: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

188 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

mean way at school?”) were higher for boys (p < .05). Freely estimating these loadings produced a revised model of partial metric invariance that showed no significant improvement in fit relative to the configural invari­ance model. The partial strong invariance model was then estimated (excluding the two items identified above) and showed a significant loss of fit in relation to the metric invariance model. Modification indices suggested that one item intercept (“Stop talking to you or ignore you when they are mad at you?”) was significantly higher for girls (p < .05). Freely estimating this intercept produced a revised model of partial strong invariance that showed no significant improvement in fit relative to the metric invariance model. The final model constrained item error variances across groups to test strict invariance (except for the three items noted above). Results from the c2 dif­ference test indicated that this model did not differ from the partial strong invariance model; that model was retained as the best­fitting model.

Grade-level groups. Fit indices for configural invariance indicated adequate fit for the two­factor model across the two grade groups (Table 2). Given con­figural invariance, metric invariance was tested and the results indicated no significant difference between the configural and metric invariance models (see Table 2). Next, for the strong invariance model, the c2 difference test showed a significant loss in fit relative to the metric invariance model. Modifi­cation indices suggested that one item intercept (“Push you around at school?”) was significantly higher for fifth graders (p < .05). Freely estimating this intercept produced a revised model of partial strong invariance that showed no significant improvement in fit relative to the metric invariance model. Finally, a c2 difference test compared the difference between the partial strong and partial strict (excluding the item above) invariance models and showed no significant difference in fit. The strict invariance model was thus retained as the best­fitting model for the grade­group comparison.

Construct Validity EquivalenceAll groups. The invariance test of the latent factor means and correlations

indicated that both parameters were invariant across ethnic groups and the correlation was invariant for all three sets of comparison groups. However, latent factor means were not invariant across gender and grade groups; a significant c2 difference test was returned for both tests. For gender groups, modification indices suggested that both means be freely estimated for boys and for girls. The overt factor mean was significantly higher for boys and the relational factor mean was significantly higher for girls (ps < .05). For grade groups, modification indices suggested that the relational mean be freely

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 19: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 189

estimated for fifth and sixth graders. The relational factor mean was signifi­cantly higher for fifth graders than sixth graders (p < .05).

Except for anxiety in the ethnic group construct equivalence analyses, all construct intercepts and slopes were invariant across all groups (European American/Latino, boys/girls, and fifth­grade/sixth­grade; see Table 3 for a summary). Modification indices suggested that the intercept for anxiety was significantly higher for European Americans (p < .05), and this intercept was allowed to vary between ethnic groups. For all groups in the ethnicity, gender and grade analyses, the following path coefficients were positive and signifi­cant: relational victimization and anxiety (bs ranged from .15 to .20); relational victimization and depression (bs ranged from .39 to .49); relational victimization and relational victimization, peer nominations (bs ranged from .17 to .25); overt victimization and depression (bs ranged from .32 to .41); and overt victimization and overt victimization peer nominations (bs ranged from .14 to .27). The path coefficient between overt victimization and anxi­ety was not significant in any of the groups.

DiscussionThe results from this study present several important contributions to the lit­erature on victimization by examining the cross­group equivalence of a self­report measure of victimization. The two­factor CFA model fit our data well and indicated that overt and relational factors similar to those estab­lished by Crick and colleagues were replicated for the entire sample and across gender, ethnic, and grade­level groups. This suggests that similar victimiza­tion constructs were tapped by this instrument for these groups. These findings extend previous research by examining this factor structure in Latino and European American groups, across the fifth and sixth graders, and with boys and girls. Our analyses provided further evidence that these constructs remain useful avenues for investigations of victimization for these groups. This examination of the SVEX instrument also demonstrated that the current set of items performed similarly across all of the groups examined (at the level of partial strong invariance or greater).

In addition, the social exclusion items added to the instrument also per­formed well across groups and these findings suggest that the addition of these items may allow researchers to accurately tap these important victim­ization behaviors. Exclusion may also prove to be viable as a distinct factor/construct (note that the three­factor model also fit the data well here) if future studies present additional evidence of distinct predictive relations to adjust­ment outcomes or other unique attributes relative to relational victimization.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 20: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

190 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Exclusion may, for example, be uniquely predictive of adjustment outcomes such as internalizing problems—prior findings have indicated that relational victimization, including limited indices of exclusion, independently predicted internalizing outcomes (relative to overt victimization). It is possible that, within studies that include larger subgroups of participants, more precise examinations of predictive relations would support a distinct role for exclu­sion, especially in more collectivist groups.

The construct validity equivalence information we gathered here indi­cated that the relational and overt factor scores predicted the internalizing outcomes and peer nominations of relational and overt victimization simi­larly across all of the groups (overt victimization, however, did not predict anxiety in any group) and there were no significant differences in slope or intercept across the models. The links to internalizing symptoms were, in general, consistent with prior findings (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1996; Hoglund, 2007) and indicated an acceptable level of invariance. The finding that overt victimization did not predict anxiety may indicate that, in these groups, feelings of social anxiety are more dependent on other aspects of peer relationships (e.g., relational victimization). Some evidence suggests (Hoglund, 2007) that relational victimization is a more consistent predictor of internalizing problems in the age groups examined here (see the limita­tions section, below, for a discussion of the mean­level differences in overt and relational victimization for girls and boys). In total, these findings sug­gest an acceptable level of equivalence across the groups examined here.

The study and our findings had several important limitations. It is important to note that, though more rural schools and students tend to be an understudied population (especially rural non–European American groups), the findings here may not generalize to youth from larger urban areas. The small cities in this study each supported a few elementary schools and a single middle school. Students in these communities were likely more familiar with each other (on average) than students from larger schools/districts and this may have affected the strength of linkages between relational victimization and the validity outcomes—in contrast to the findings of Pelligrini and colleagues (Pelligini & Bartini, 2000), greater familiarity among the youth in these schools could foster more stable peer networks that might ameliorate vic­timization and related effects relative to youth in larger schools (also consistent with Bukowski & Sippola, 2001).

More extensive investigations of victimization effects within/across these groups will also be necessary to more fully understand potential links to important adjustment outcomes such as academic achievement and external­izing disorders. If victimization is linked to disengagement and victimization

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 21: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 191

effects are stronger when it originates within one’s own group, then accurate assessments of these linkages across the groups examined here will be key to creating school­based, culturally sensitive interventions designed to improve school adjustment.

In addition, while the SVEX instrument and scales demonstrated an acc­eptable level of equivalence here, invariance/equivalence was not uniform in all comparisons or necessarily displayed at the highest levels (several items displayed different item intercepts across groups). It is not unusual to find this level of variance in relatively complex instruments, but this may have indicated items did not function identically across groups and further examinations should be conducted. At least 80% of the items demonstrated strict invariance (the most restrictive conditions) across the groups, how­ever, and this proportion will likely produce reliable results (Gregorich, 2006, p. 87). Finally, although the construct validity evidence generally dis­played an interpretable pattern here and suggested equivalence, a single mean­level difference emerged and indicated that boys reported greater levels of overt victimization and that girls displayed greater levels of rela­tional victimization. Although not invariant, this finding is consistent with other evidence of similar patterns (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1995) and is also consistent with theoretical frameworks (see Underwood, 2003, for a discus­sion). This instrument also likely displayed the limitations of self­reports relevant to measuring victimization, especially for studies relying on self­reported outcomes. The models predicting associations among victimization and the internalizing symptoms examined here were a good example of links where the magnitude of associations may be inflated due to shared­source variance/response biases.

Finally, and perhaps most important, though there is a significant body of research surrounding potential socialization differences linked to Latino youth, families, and communities, it is essential to design investigations that explic­itly tap relevant aspects of these constructs (e.g., individualist/collectivist socialization patterns, family behaviors, etc.) and directly evaluate links to peer processes and outcomes. This may be especially important in relatively more isolated rural areas where a smaller Latino community may be even more interdependent than is typical in larger urban areas. Latino youth in this sample reported feeling more isolated than they had in the western and south­western urban contexts many of them had migrated from—such feelings, if present, may affect the effects of own­ as well as other­group victimization.

Despite these limitations, this study demonstrated sophisticated CFA and structural equation modeling (SEM) techniques that can be used within devel­opmental research to more closely examine the equivalence of instruments

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 22: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

192 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

across groups. To our knowledge, this study represents the first empirical examination of factorial invariance and construct validity equivalence across ethnic and grade­level groups for a measure of aggression or victimization .The findings also demonstrated the utility of this particular instrument with the groups examined and indicated that it displayed acceptable levels of equiva­lence across gender, grade level, and Latino/European American youth. Conflicting findings in current literature regarding group differences in victimization may in part be due to shortcomings of the instruments used (see Graham et al., 2009; Underwood, 2003, for discussions of conflicting findings).

Finally, the availability of a self­report instrument that allows for better representation and reporting of exclusion behaviors (in groups where these behaviors may emerge as central aspects of victimization) will allow youth to more accurately represent their peer experiences. Exclusion is an important part of relational victimization for youth and it is likely to be highly salient to the peer group as well as the victim. Consistent relational victimization and exclusion by group behaviors as described by items in this measure would be a very visible marker of social status (especially if it originates from mem­bers of one’s own group). This, along with other types of victimization, appears likely to contribute to adjustment processes by directly limiting youths’ access to peer academic and social–emotional support. The relative role of exclusion in victimization and related adjustment thus becomes central to interventions tailored for greater effectiveness in specific peer contexts (e.g., gender and ethnic groups). Accurate instruments tapping relevant victimization behav­iors will be an important component for the development and evaluation of such interventions and related research.

Acknowledgment

The authors wish to thank the school districts and students and the University of Nebraska Latino Research Initiative for their support.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interests with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

The authors declared that data analyses and manuscript preparation was supported by Nebraska Tobacco Settlement Biomedical Research Enhancement Funds.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317­332.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 23: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 193

Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., Witkow, M., Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (2007). The influ­ence of classroom ethnic composition on same­ and other­ethnicity peer nomina­tions in middle school. Social Development, 16, 720­740

Boivin, M., & Hymel, S. (1997). Peer experiences and social self­perceptions: A sequential model. Developmental Psychology, 33, 135­145.

Brown, B., Mory, M., & Kinney, D. (1994). Casting adolescent crowds in a relational perspective: Caricature, channel, and context. In R. Montemayor, G. Adams, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during adolescence (pp. 123­167). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136­162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Buhs, E. (2005). Peer rejection, negative peer treatment, and school adjustment: Self­concept and classroom engagement as mediating processes. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 407­424.

Buhs, E., Ladd, G., & Herald, S. (2006). Peer exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children’s classroom engagement and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 1­13.

Bukowski, W., & Sippola, L. (2001). Groups, individuals, and victimization: A view of the peer system. J. Jovonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 355­377). New York: Guilford Press.

Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthén, B. (1989). Testing for equivalence of fac­tor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 456­466.

Coie, J., Terry, R., Lenox, K., Lochman, J., & Hyman, C. (1995). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 697­713.

Collins, W., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal context. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychol­ogy: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 1003­1067). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Crick, N. (1995). Relational aggression: The role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation type. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 313­322.

Crick, N., & Bigbee, M. A. (1998). Relational and overt forms of peer victimization: A multi informant approach. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 337­347.

Crick, N., & Grotpeter, J. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social­psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710­722.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 24: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

194 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Crick, N., & Grotpeter, J. (1996). Children’s treatment by peers: Victims of relational and overt aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 367­380.

Crick, N. R., Werner, N. E., Casas, J. F., O’Brien, K. M., Nelson, D. A., Grotpeter, J. K., et al. (1999). Childhood aggression and gender: A new look at an old problem. In D. Bernstein (Ed.), 45th Annual Symposium on Motivation: Gender and motiva­tion (pp. 75­141). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Crockett, L., Randall, B., Shen, Y., Russell, S., & Driscoll, A. (2005). Measurement equivalence of the center for epidemiological studies depression scale for latino and anglo adolescents: A national study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy­chology, 73, 47­58.

Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & González, G. (1995). Cognitive referents of acculturation: Assessment of cultural constructs in Mexican Americans. Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 339­356.

Dill, E., Vernberg, E., Fonagy, P., Twemlow, S., & Gamm, B. (2004). Negative affect in victimized children: The roles of social withdrawal, peer rejection, and attitudes toward bullying. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 159­173.

Galen, B. R., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). A developmental investigation of social aggression among children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 589­600.

Gouveia, L., Carranza, M. A., & Cogua, J. (2005). The Great Plains migration: Mexi­canos and Latinos in Nebraska. In V. Zuniga & R. Hernandez­Leon (Eds.), New destinations—Mexican immigration in the United States (pp. 23­49). New York: Sage Foundation.

Graham, S. (2006). Peer victimization in school: Exploring the ethnic context. Cur­rent Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 317­321.

Graham, S., Taylor, A. Z., & Ho, A. Y. (2009). Race and ethnicity in peer relations research. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 394­413). New York: Guilford Press.

Gregorich, S. E. (2006). Do self­report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the con­firmatory factor analyses framework. Medical Care, 44, 78­94.

Hawker, D., & Boulton, M. (2000). Twenty years’ research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta­analytic review of cross­sectional studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 441­455.

Hodges, E., & Perry, D. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and conse­quences of victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677­685.

Hoglund, W. (2007). School functioning in early adolescence: Gender­linked responses to peer victimization. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 683­699.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Mod­eling, 6, 1­55.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 25: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 195

Kline, R. B. (1998). Principals and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford.

Knight, G., & Kagan, S. (1977). Development of prosocial and competitive behav­iors in Anglo­American and Mexican­American children. Child Development, 48, 1385­1394

Knight, G. P., & Hill, N. E. (1998). Measurement equivalence in research involv­ing minority adolescents. In V. McLoyd & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Studying minority adolescents: Conceptual, methodological and theoretical issues (pp. 183­210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Knight, G. P., Tein, J.­Y., Prost, J. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (2002). Measurement equivalence and research on Latino children and families: The importance of culturally informed theory. In J. Contreras, K. Kears, & A. Neal­Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families in the United States: Current research and future directions (pp. 181­201). Westport, CN: Praeger.

Ladd, G., & Kochenderfer­Ladd, B. (2002). Identifying victims of peer aggression from early to middle childhood: Analysis of cross­informant data for concordance, estimation of relational adjustment, prevalence of victimization, and characteris­tics of identified victims. Psychological Assessment, 14, 74­96.

Leyendecker, B., & Lamb, M. (1999). Latino families. In M. Lamb (Ed.), Parenting and child development in “nontraditional” families (pp. 247­262). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Little, T. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross­cultural data: Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 53­76.

Loeber, R., & Hay, D. F. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and vio­lence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 371­410.

Long, J., & Pellegrini, A. (2003). Studying change in dominance and bullying with linear mixed models. School Psychology Review, 32, 401­417.

Maccoby, E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.

March, J. S. (1997). Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children: Technical manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi­Health Systems.

March, J., & Parker, J. (2004). The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC). In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment: Vol. 2: Instruments for children and adoles­cents (3rd ed., pp. 39­62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Marín, G., & Marín, B. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Medsker, G. J., Williams, L. J., & Holahan, P. J. (1994). A review of current practices for evaluating causal­models in organizational­behavior and human­resources management research. Journal of Management, 20, 439–464.

Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: Validation of inferences from persons’ responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score mean­ing. American Psychologist, 50, 741­749.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 26: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

196 Journal of Early Adolescence 30(1)

Millsap, R. E., & Kwok, O­M. (2004). Evaluating the impact of partial factorial invariance on selection in two populations. Psychological Methods, 9, 93­115.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2004). Mplus for Windows (Version 4.21) [Com­puter software]. Los Angeles: Authors.

Mynard, H., & Joseph, S. (2000). Development of the multidimensional peer­victimization scale. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 169­178.

Nansel, T., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R., Ruan, W., Simons­Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094­2100.

Nishina, A., & Juvonen, J. (2005). Daily reports of witnessing and experiencing peer harassment in middle school. Child Development, 76, 435­450.

Olweus, D. (1994). Annotation: Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1171­1190.

Pellegrini, A., & Bartini, M. (2000). An empirical comparison of methods of sampling aggression and victimization in school settings. Journal of Educational Psychol­ogy, 92, 360­366.

Peña, E. (2007). Lost in translation: Methodological considerations in cross­cultural research. Child Development, 78, 1255­1264.

Radloff, L. (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149­166.

Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relation­ships, and groups. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 571­645). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Sandstrom, M., & Cillessen, A. (2003). Sociometric status and children’s peer expe­riences: Use of the daily diary method. Merrill­Palmer Quarterly, 49, 427­452.

Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1988). Scaling corrections for chi­square statistics in covari­ance structure analysis. Proceedings of the American Statistical Association, 308­313.

Schwartz, D., Gorman, A., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. (2005). Victimization in the peer group and children’s academic functioning. Journal of Educational Psychol­ogy, 97, 425­435.

Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., & Jarvis, L. H. (2007). Ethnic identity and accul­turation in Hispanic early adolescents: Mediated relationships to academic grades, prosocial behaviors, and externalizing symptoms. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 364­373.

Schwarz, G. E. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461­464.

Storch, E., Crisp, H., Roberti, J., Bagner, D., & Masia­Warner, C. (2005). Psychomet­ric evaluation of the social experience questionnaire in adolescents: Descriptive

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 27: Overt and Relational Victimization in Latinos and European Americans: Measurement Equivalence Across Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade Level in Early Adolescent Groups

Buhs et al. 197

data, reliability, and factorial validity. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 36, 167­176.

Underwood, M. (2003). Social aggression among girls. New York: Guilford Press.Underwood, M., Scott, B., Galperin, M., Bjornstad, G., & Sexton, A. (2004). An

observational study of social exclusion under varied conditions: Gender and developmental differences. Child Development, 75, 1538­1555.

Vaillancourt, T., Brendgen, M., Boivin, M., & Tremblay, R. (2003). Longitudinal con­firmatory factor analysis of indirect and physical aggression: Evidence of two factors over time? Child Development, 74, 1628­1638.

Weston, R., & Gore, P. A. (2006). A brief guide to structural equation modeling. The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 719­751.

Widaman, K. F., & Reise, S. P. (1997). Exploring the measurement invariance of psy­chological instruments: Applicants in the substance use domain. In K. J. Bryant, M. Windle & S. G. West (Eds.), The science of prevention: Methodological advances from alcohol and substance abuse research (pp. 281­324). Washington, DC: APA.

Bios

Eric S. Buhs is an associate professor of Educational Psychology at the University of Nebraska–Lincoln and conducts research examining peer relations and links to aca­demic and psychological adjustment. His recent work has focused on cross­ethnic differences in peer relations and school adjustment for students in the Great Plains region.

Meredith McGinley is a recent graduate of the Psychology program at the University of Nebraska–Lincoln and is currently a visiting senior research specialist in health sciences in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Illinois at Chicago. She has focused her research on children’s social development and on the role of prosocial behavior.

Michael D. Toland is recent graduate of the quantitative, qualitative, and psychomet­ric methods program in educational psychology at the University of Nebraska–Lincoln and is currently an assistant professor at the University of Kentucky in the Depart­ment of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology. His work has applied structural equations modeling and measurement techniques to a broad range of devel­opmental and educational research.

by guest on February 9, 2016jea.sagepub.comDownloaded from