Top Banner
Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories Alexandra Karle ´n a , Tobias E. Karlsson a,1 , Anna Mattsson a,1 , Karin Lundstro ¨ mer a , Simone Codeluppi a , Therese M. Pham b , Cristina M. Ba ¨ ckman c , Sven Ove O ¨ gren a , Elin Åberg a , Alexander F. Hoffman c , Michael A. Sherling c , Carl R. Lupica c , Barry J. Hoffer c , Christian Spenger d , Anna Josephson a , Stefan Brene ´ b , and Lars Olson a,2 a Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Retzius va ¨ g 8, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden; b Department of Neurobiology, Caring Sciences and Society, and d Department of Clinical Science, Intervention and Technology, Karolinska Institutet, 141 86 Stockholm, Sweden; and c National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, 251 Bayview Dr., Baltimore, MD 21224 Edited by Floyd E. Bloom, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, and approved September 30, 2009 (received for review May 15, 2009) Formation of lasting memories is believed to rely on structural alterations at the synaptic level. We had found that increased neu- ronal activity down-regulates Nogo receptor-1 (NgR1) in brain regions linked to memory formation and storage, and postulated this to be required for formation of lasting memories. We now show that mice with inducible overexpression of NgR1 in forebrain neurons have normal long-term potentiation and normal 24-h memory, but se- verely impaired month-long memory in both passive avoidance and swim maze tests. Blocking transgene expression normalizes these memory impairments. Nogo, Lingo-1, Troy, endogenous NgR1, and BDNF mRNA expression levels were not altered by transgene expres- sion, suggesting that the impaired ability to form lasting memories is directly coupled to inability to down-regulate NgR1. Regulation of NgR1 may therefore serve as a key regulator of memory consolida- tion. Understanding the molecular underpinnings of synaptic rear- rangements that carry lasting memories may facilitate development of treatments for memory dysfunction. behavior hippocampus long-term potentiation myelin inhibitors synaptic plasticity E vents underlying formation of memories that last hours to days are partially understood. Less is known about mechanisms that allow such memories to become transformed into very long-term (months) memories. First demonstrated as reactive sprouting in response to injury (1), structural synaptic plasticity in the adult brain is known to be a normal feature of gray matter (2, 3) and may be how lasting memories are formed and maintained. Thus perturba- tions of sensory input, such as monocular deprivation, leave lasting traces in the cerebral cortex in the form of changes of synaptic contacts (4) and involves dynamic changes of the actin cytoskeleton (5). The lack of regenerative capacity in the mammalian CNS is partly due to the growth-inhibitory proteins Nogo (6–8), MAG (9, 10), and OMgp (11). These ligands can all bind to Nogo receptor 1 (NgR1) (12, 13). Since NgR1 lacks a cytoplasmic domain, addi- tional transmembrane molecules (14–17) are needed to mediate intracellular signaling, leading to growth cone collapse (18). We have previously demonstrated robust transcription of NgR1 in brain neurons, rather than glial cells, particularly in cortex cerebri and hippocampus (19), regions endowed with marked synaptic plasticity (20). Because Nogo is not only expressed in myelin, but also by many neurons (21), we hypothesized that NgR signaling might regulate activity-dependent synaptic reorganization under- lying long-term memory (22). We found that neuronal NgR1 mRNA levels were efficiently and transiently down-regulated in the hippocampal formation and cerebral cortex of rats by kainic acid (22). Such temporary down-regulation of NgR1 transcription also occurred during the learning phase of a running behavior (22). Using fMRI, we recently showed in rats subjected to thoracic spinal cord injury, that when forelimb sensory representation in cortex expands into neighboring areas, NgR1 becomes specifically down- regulated in those sensorimotor cortical areas undergoing the plastic changes (23). Moreover, mice lacking NgR1 maintain the ocular dominance shift response to monocular deprivation into adulthood (24), suggesting supranormal CNS plasticity in the absence of NgR1. A similar improvement of ocular dominance shift plasticity has also been found in mice lacking functional PirB (25), a recently identified additional receptor for Nogo, MAG, and OMgp (26). Here we test the hypothesis that NgR1 regulation plays an important role in long-term memory formation. Results Generation of Mice Overexpressing NgR1 in Forebrain Neurons. We first tested whether the prompt down-regulation of NgR mRNA expression in response to kainic acid seen in rats (22) also occurs in mice and found a similar temporally and spatially coupled reciprocal regulation of the NgR1 and BDNF genes in response to kainic acid (Fig. S1). We thus generated mice in which the normal, neural activity-driven down-regulation of endogenous NgR1 would be counteracted by constitutive expression of a NgR1 transgene. CamKII becomes increasingly active after birth in rat (27) and mouse (Fig. S2) forebrain neurons and exerts a key role in LTP and synaptic plasticity (28–30). We therefore used the CamKII pro- moter to limit transgene expression to forebrain neurons. We reasoned that the rapid temporary down-regulation of NgR1 normally seen during plastic events might be without effect if a NgR1 transgene was expressed in those same forebrain neurons, and hypothesized that this should render mice less able to undergo activity-dependent synaptic remodeling. Transgene induction was obtained using the tet-off system (Fig. S3). Four independent tet-off inducible NgR1 overexpressing mouse lines (L1–L4) were established to minimize risk of transgene genome integration errors. Transgenic mice were healthy with no obvious phenotype, although adult L1 and L2 mice, selected for further testing, weighed approximately 10% less than controls (P 0.047 and 0.057, respectively, two-tailed t-test). We found no, or only modest, changes in levels of noradrenaline, dopamine, DOPAC, HVA, serotonin, 5HIAA, or the striatal HVA/DA ratio, suggesting that monoaminergic neurotransmission was essentially intact in transgenic mice (Fig. S4). Downstream events of NgR activation includes RhoA activation. However, there was no dif- Author contributions: A.K., T.E.K., A.M., C.R.L., B.J.H., C.S., A.J., S.B., and L.O. designed research; A.K., T.E.K., A.M., K.L., S.C., T.M.P., C.M.B., E.Å., A.F.H., M.A.S., C.R.L., and L.O. performed research; S.C., S.O.O ¨ ., B.J.H., and L.O. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; A.K., T.E.K., A.M., K.L., S.C., T.M.P., E.Å., A.F.H., M.A.S., C.R.L., B.J.H., A.J., S.B., and L.O. analyzed data; and A.K., T.E.K., A.M., C.R.L., B.J.H., and L.O. wrote the paper. The authors declare no conflict of interest. This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. Freely available online through the PNAS open access option. 1 T.E.K. and A.M. contributed equally to this work. 2 To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/ 0905390106/DCSupplemental. 20476 –20481 PNAS December 1, 2009 vol. 106 no. 48 www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0905390106
6

Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

May 08, 2023

Download

Documents

Joseph Masdeu
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

Nogo receptor 1 regulates formationof lasting memoriesAlexandra Karlena, Tobias E. Karlssona,1, Anna Mattssona,1, Karin Lundstromera, Simone Codeluppia, Therese M. Phamb,Cristina M. Backmanc, Sven Ove Ogrena, Elin Åberga, Alexander F. Hoffmanc, Michael A. Sherlingc, Carl R. Lupicac,Barry J. Hofferc, Christian Spengerd, Anna Josephsona, Stefan Breneb, and Lars Olsona,2

aDepartment of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Retzius vag 8, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden; bDepartment of Neurobiology, Caring Sciences and Society,and dDepartment of Clinical Science, Intervention and Technology, Karolinska Institutet, 141 86 Stockholm, Sweden; and cNational Institute on DrugAbuse, National Institutes of Health, 251 Bayview Dr., Baltimore, MD 21224

Edited by Floyd E. Bloom, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, and approved September 30, 2009 (received for review May 15, 2009)

Formation of lasting memories is believed to rely on structuralalterations at the synaptic level. We had found that increased neu-ronal activity down-regulates Nogo receptor-1 (NgR1) in brain regionslinked to memory formation and storage, and postulated this to berequired for formation of lasting memories. We now show that micewith inducible overexpression of NgR1 in forebrain neurons havenormal long-term potentiation and normal 24-h memory, but se-verely impaired month-long memory in both passive avoidance andswim maze tests. Blocking transgene expression normalizes thesememory impairments. Nogo, Lingo-1, Troy, endogenous NgR1, andBDNF mRNA expression levels were not altered by transgene expres-sion, suggesting that the impaired ability to form lasting memories isdirectly coupled to inability to down-regulate NgR1. Regulation ofNgR1 may therefore serve as a key regulator of memory consolida-tion. Understanding the molecular underpinnings of synaptic rear-rangements that carry lasting memories may facilitate developmentof treatments for memory dysfunction.

behavior � hippocampus � long-term potentiation � myelin inhibitors �synaptic plasticity

Events underlying formation of memories that last hours to daysare partially understood. Less is known about mechanisms that

allow such memories to become transformed into very long-term(months) memories. First demonstrated as reactive sprouting inresponse to injury (1), structural synaptic plasticity in the adult brainis known to be a normal feature of gray matter (2, 3) and may behow lasting memories are formed and maintained. Thus perturba-tions of sensory input, such as monocular deprivation, leave lastingtraces in the cerebral cortex in the form of changes of synapticcontacts (4) and involves dynamic changes of the actin cytoskeleton(5).

The lack of regenerative capacity in the mammalian CNS is partlydue to the growth-inhibitory proteins Nogo (6–8), MAG (9, 10),and OMgp (11). These ligands can all bind to Nogo receptor 1(NgR1) (12, 13). Since NgR1 lacks a cytoplasmic domain, addi-tional transmembrane molecules (14–17) are needed to mediateintracellular signaling, leading to growth cone collapse (18).

We have previously demonstrated robust transcription of NgR1in brain neurons, rather than glial cells, particularly in cortex cerebriand hippocampus (19), regions endowed with marked synapticplasticity (20). Because Nogo is not only expressed in myelin, butalso by many neurons (21), we hypothesized that NgR signalingmight regulate activity-dependent synaptic reorganization under-lying long-term memory (22). We found that neuronal NgR1mRNA levels were efficiently and transiently down-regulated in thehippocampal formation and cerebral cortex of rats by kainic acid(22). Such temporary down-regulation of NgR1 transcription alsooccurred during the learning phase of a running behavior (22).Using fMRI, we recently showed in rats subjected to thoracic spinalcord injury, that when forelimb sensory representation in cortexexpands into neighboring areas, NgR1 becomes specifically down-regulated in those sensorimotor cortical areas undergoing the

plastic changes (23). Moreover, mice lacking NgR1 maintain theocular dominance shift response to monocular deprivation intoadulthood (24), suggesting supranormal CNS plasticity in theabsence of NgR1. A similar improvement of ocular dominance shiftplasticity has also been found in mice lacking functional PirB (25),a recently identified additional receptor for Nogo, MAG, andOMgp (26). Here we test the hypothesis that NgR1 regulation playsan important role in long-term memory formation.

ResultsGeneration of Mice Overexpressing NgR1 in Forebrain Neurons. Wefirst tested whether the prompt down-regulation of NgR mRNAexpression in response to kainic acid seen in rats (22) also occursin mice and found a similar temporally and spatially coupledreciprocal regulation of the NgR1 and BDNF genes in response tokainic acid (Fig. S1). We thus generated mice in which the normal,neural activity-driven down-regulation of endogenous NgR1 wouldbe counteracted by constitutive expression of a NgR1 transgene.CamKII becomes increasingly active after birth in rat (27) andmouse (Fig. S2) forebrain neurons and exerts a key role in LTP andsynaptic plasticity (28–30). We therefore used the CamKII pro-moter to limit transgene expression to forebrain neurons. Wereasoned that the rapid temporary down-regulation of NgR1normally seen during plastic events might be without effect if aNgR1 transgene was expressed in those same forebrain neurons,and hypothesized that this should render mice less able to undergoactivity-dependent synaptic remodeling. Transgene induction wasobtained using the tet-off system (Fig. S3).

Four independent tet-off inducible NgR1 overexpressing mouselines (L1–L4) were established to minimize risk of transgenegenome integration errors. Transgenic mice were healthy with noobvious phenotype, although adult L1 and L2 mice, selected forfurther testing, weighed approximately 10% less than controls (P �0.047 and 0.057, respectively, two-tailed t-test). We found no, oronly modest, changes in levels of noradrenaline, dopamine,DOPAC, HVA, serotonin, 5HIAA, or the striatal HVA/DA ratio,suggesting that monoaminergic neurotransmission was essentiallyintact in transgenic mice (Fig. S4). Downstream events of NgRactivation includes RhoA activation. However, there was no dif-

Author contributions: A.K., T.E.K., A.M., C.R.L., B.J.H., C.S., A.J., S.B., and L.O. designedresearch; A.K., T.E.K., A.M., K.L., S.C., T.M.P., C.M.B., E.Å., A.F.H., M.A.S., C.R.L., and L.O.performed research; S.C., S.O.O., B.J.H., and L.O. contributed new reagents/analytic tools;A.K., T.E.K., A.M., K.L., S.C., T.M.P., E.Å., A.F.H., M.A.S., C.R.L., B.J.H., A.J., S.B., and L.O.analyzed data; and A.K., T.E.K., A.M., C.R.L., B.J.H., and L.O. wrote the paper.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

1T.E.K. and A.M. contributed equally to this work.

2To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected].

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/0905390106/DCSupplemental.

20476–20481 � PNAS � December 1, 2009 � vol. 106 � no. 48 www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.0905390106

Page 2: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

ference in degree of hippocampal RhoA activation between L1 andcontrol mice (Fig. S4).

Robust Tet-Off Inducible Expression of Transgenic NgR1 mRNA andProtein. There were certain differences in the precise patternsand intensities of overexpression between lines (Fig. 1). NgR1overexpressing (double transgenic: CamKIIa and pTRE/NgR1)mice and heterozygous (single transgenic: CamKIIa or pTRE/NgR1) littermates from the L1 and L2 lines were chosen andproduced similar results. Consistent with choice of promoter,transgenic NgR (NgR1T) transcription was robust in striatum,hippocampus and cortex cerebri (Fig. 1 A and B), as well as inthe olfactory bulb and amygdala (Fig. 1B). While transgenicmRNA levels were higher than endogenous levels in all areas ofexpression in both L1 and L2 mice, L2 NgR1 levels weregenerally considerably lower than L1 levels (Fig. 1B). Doxycy-cline counteracted the increased NgR1 mRNA levels in L1 mice(hippocampal CA3 levels in nCi/g: control 154, control � dox 1month 145, L1 302, L1 � dox 1 month 149). We found strongincreases of NgR1 protein levels in hippocampus, cortex, stria-tum, and olfactory bulb of L1 mice (Fig. S5). After delivery ofdoxycycline for 1 month, NgR protein levels were no longer

increased in any of these areas (Fig. S5). NgR1 protein levelswere below the detection level in cerebellum and spinal cord(Fig. S5). In hippocampus, cortex, and striatum, protein levelswere markedly decreased 3 days after initiating doxycyclinetreatment, and protein overexpression could no longer be seenafter 9 days (Fig. 1C). As expected, subcellular fractionationshowed high amounts of NgR1 protein in the plasma membranefraction of hippocampal, cortical and striatal tissue (Fig. 1C). Weobserved that neither endogenous NgR1 mRNA levels (Fig. 1 Aand B), nor levels of Nogo, Lingo-1, Troy, or BDNF mRNA weresignificantly affected by transgene expression (Fig. S6). Usingimmunohistochemistry, we found supranormal levels of NgR-like immunoreactivity in cortical, hippocampal and striatal areasof L1 mice (Fig. 1D). Of note, NgR-like immunoreactivity wasfound not only in the striatal neuropil, presumably partly ema-nating from the cortico-striatal pathways, but also in substantianigra pars reticulata, presumably ref lecting NgR protein inprojections from NgR expressing striatal output neurons, asindicated by the marked CamKII-driven NgR mRNA expres-sion observed in striatal neurons (Fig. 1 A). Importantly, whentransgenic mice were challenged with kainic acid, NgR1transgene transcription was maintained (Fig. 1E).

Fig. 1. Characterization of inducibleNgR1 overexpression. (A) In situ hybrid-ization showing endogenous (NgRE) andtransgenic (NgRT) mRNA in two trans-genic mouse lines (L1 and L2) and con-trols (CON), quantified in (B) (OB: olfac-tory bulb, DG: dentate gyrus). Theexpressionof transgene is specific in fore-brain neurons and stronger in L1 than L2(n � 3). Means � SEM. (C) Upper threelanes: NgR1 protein in three brain areasof control and L1 mice before and after 1,3, and 9 days of treatment with doxycy-cline (dox). Loading controls: GAPDH. Forcortex, additional lanes between day 3and 9 were digitally removed from themembrane picture. Lower lane: NgR1protein in plasma membrane fractionsfrom three brain areas of control and L1mice. Loading control: N-Cadherin. (D)Strong NgR-like immunoreactivity inthree brain areas of L1 compared to con-trol mice. (Scale bar, 200 �m and 20 �mfor DG enlargement.) (E) Robust trans-gene overexpression in L1 mice is main-tained after kainic acid (KA).

Karlen et al. PNAS � December 1, 2009 � vol. 106 � no. 48 � 20477

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE

Page 3: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

No Impairment of LTP, L-LTP, De-Potentiation, or Cortical SpineNumbers. We did not expect NgR1 to affect the relatively rapidevents that initiate and maintain LTP. To investigate this, werecorded extracellular field EPSPs from CA1 in hippocampal slicesfrom control and L1 and L2 mice in response to 0.033-Hz electricalstimulation. After 10 min of stable baseline recordings, either highfrequency stimulation or theta-burst stimulation induced robustLTP (Fig. 2 A and B), lasting at least 70 min, with no markeddifferences between overexpressing and control mice suggestingthat NgR down-regulation is not necessary for LTP to becomeestablished. Furthermore, electrical stimulation of brain slices fromcontrol and NgR overexpressing mice produced fEPSPs withsimilar time courses and shapes (Fig. 2 A and B), and similarresponses across a range of stimulus intensities (Fig. S7). Thisindicates that baseline synaptic processes were also not altered bythe transgene. We also considered the possibility that the ability tomaintain LTP for longer periods (L-LTP) might be compromised.However, as shown in Fig. 2C, stable LTP was maintained for atleast 2 h following a single theta burst stimulation, and this was notsignificantly different between control and L2 mice (Fig. 2C) (P �0.54, two-way RM-ANOVA, genotype � time). While loss of NgRprevents the expression of hippocampal LTD (31), LTD is typicallyonly observed in slices obtained from juvenile animals (31, 32), andso would be unlikely to underlie the long-term behavioral changes

observed in our adult mice. Since the reversibility (‘de-potentiation’) of LTP by low frequency stimulation (LFS) is wellestablished in adult animals (33), and also reverses changes in spinemorphology that may be regulated by NgR (31, 34), we nextexamined this phenomenon. As shown in Fig. 2 D and E, LFS (1 Hz)started 5 min after theta burst stimulation, resulted in de-potentiation of the previously potentiated response. The extent ofde-potentiation was dependent on the number of stimuli (300 vs.900 pulses); however, no significant differences were observedbetween L2 and control mice using either paradigm (300 pulse, P �0.55; 900 pulse, P � 0.82; two-way RM-ANOVA, genotype � time).Together, these data suggest that the long-term memory deficitsobserved in L1 and L2 mice do not reflect intrinsic deficits inelectrophysiological hippocampal plasticity or metaplasticity. Wealso determined spine density on apical dendrites of corticalpyramids. There was no significant difference in the total amountof spines (Fig. 2 F and G, P � 0.82, two-tailed t-test) or the amountof mushroom shaped spines (Fig. 2G, P � 0.81, two-tailed t-test).

Disturbed Running Behavior in NgR1 Overexpressing Mice. To testwhether NgR1 overexpression would result in motor deficits weused the rotarod test (Fig. 3A) and found no significant difference,indicating that overexpressing mice have normal motor control.Similarly, there were no marked differences in spontaneous loco-motion between L1 or L2 mice and controls (Fig. 3 B and C). Thissuggests that NgR1 overexpression does not disturb innate loco-motor functions. Since we previously reported that NgR1 mRNAlevels are down-regulated in hippocampus and cerebral cortex ofrats by wheel running (22), we next exposed mice to running wheelsfor 5 weeks (Fig. S8). Both control and L1 mice significantlyincreased their running from week 1 to week 2. However, controlsincreased running significantly more (P � 0.05) and also plateauedat a higher level. Thus, while NgR1 overexpression does not appearto disturb innate locomotor activities, it appears to impair locomo-tor learning and/or the plasticity needed to develop a preference forrunning.

NgR Overexpression Impairs Long-Term Spatial Memory. To deter-mine whether presence of a NgR transgene compromises long-termlearning and spatial information, we used the Morris water maze,a hippocampus-dependent reference memory task (Fig. 3D). L1and L2 mice and controls all improved their day-to-day ability tofind a hidden platform in a fixed location (Fig. 3 E and F). Therewere no marked group differences. Similarly, a probe trial (withoutplatform) 24 h after the last training session, showed that all groupsspent an equal proportion of their time in the target quadrantsearching for the platform (Fig. 3G), indicating that 24-h memoryof the task was not impaired. However, when tested in the watermaze again, at day 60, with the platform in its original position, bothL1 and L2 mice spent a lower proportion of their swim time in thetarget quadrant before finding the platform (Fig. 3H). They alsoneeded a significantly longer time to find the platform (escapelatency) compared to controls (Fig. 3I). Swim speed did not differ(P � 0.59). Retested at day 61, both L1 and L2 mice had relearnedthe task and performed as well as controls. In a separate experi-ment, other L1 mice were trained to find the platform andthereafter subjected to reversal learning, where the platform loca-tion had been moved 180°. When retested 40 days later with theplatform back in its original location, there was no longer adifference in swim time between NgR1 overexpressing mice andcontrols [days � group, F (1, 6) � 0.8, P � 0.56], suggesting that itwas the advantage of remembering platform position by controlsthat caused the differences in Fig. 3I. Finally, we repeated thelong-term memory test with L1 mice, only this time a probe trial(platform removed) was carried out at day 39. Again, NgR1overexpressing mice were significantly impaired compared to lit-termate controls (Fig. 3J) (P � 0.037). These mice were 9 monthsold at testing, and showed a tendency of impaired learning already

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 800

50

100

150

200

250

CON (16)NgR (15)

HFS

Time (min)

fEPS

P Sl

ope

(% C

ontro

l)

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 800

50

100

150

200

250CON (24)NgR (18)

TBS

fEPS

P Sl

ope

(% C

ontro

l)

Time (min)

0.5 mV5 ms

CON NgR

5 ms0.5 mV

CON NgR

60 min 120 min0

50

100

150

CON (4) NgR (5)

Time after TBS

fEPS

P Sl

ope

(% C

ontro

l)

5 ms0.5 mV

CON NgR

0 10 20 30 40 500

50

100

150

200

250

0.5 mV5 ms

1 Hz

TBS

CON (12)NgR (12)

Time (min)

fEPS

P Sl

ope

(% C

ontro

l)

0 10 20 30 40 500

50

100

150

200

250

TBS

1 Hz CON (10)NgR (8)

Time (min)

fEPS

P Sl

ope

(% C

ontro

l)0.5 mV5 ms

Control5 minpost-TBS

20 minpost-1Hz

Control5 minpost-TBS

20 minpost-1Hz

2468

1012

CON (8)L1 (9)

Mushroom Total

14161820

CON L1

No

of s

pine

s/10

µm

F G

A

C

B

D

E

Fig. 2. LTP, L-LTP, de-potentiation, and spine counts. (A and B) No difference inLTP inducedbyeitherhighfrequency stimulation (HFS)or theta-burst stimulation(TBS). Representative fEPSPs are shown before (blue) or 60 min following (red)LTP-induction. (C) Similarly, there was no difference in L-LTP 60 or 120 min afterTBS. fEPSPs are shown before (blue) and 2 h post induction (red). (D and E) Nodifference in de-potentiation is seen using 5 min (D) or 15 min (E) 1-Hz stimula-tions, delivered 5 min following TBS. Representative fEPSPs are shown from twodifferent control mice. (F) Representative images of Golgi-stained apical den-drites from cortex cerebri (Scale bar, 50 �m.) (G) No difference was seen in thedensity of mushroom shaped spines or in total spine density.

20478 � www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.0905390106 Karlen et al.

Page 4: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

during the first 7 training days. We therefore also calculated theratio of time in the correct quadrant at day 39 compared to theirfinal performance at day 7 (at completion of training); this ratio wasalso significantly lower in the NgR overexpressing group.

We retested swim maze performance of L1 NgR1 overexpressingand control mice (those depicted in Fig. 3 E, H, and I) when theseanimals became 18 months old. During the learning phase, therewas a significant interaction between time and group (P � 0.05),such that the old controls performed better than the old L1 mice.By day 5, the escape latency time was identical for old controls andold L1 mice (Fig. S9). Retested 1 month later, old controlsperformed at the same level as day 5, suggesting that they remem-bered platform location well. In contrast, old L1 mice needed amuch longer time to find the platform, indicating that their abilityto form lasting memories was impaired (Fig. S9). This demonstratesthat 18-month-old mice are able to form spatial memories lasting amonth to the same extent as younger adults, and suggests that NgR1signaling remains important for memory formation also in old mice.

Immediate, But Not 1-Week-Delayed, Transgene Inactivation RescuesLong-Term Memory in a Passive Avoidance Setting. To examine theprocess of memory consolidation more closely, we used passiveavoidance (35, 36), a behavioral paradigm in which a robustlong-term memory for an unpleasant event is established andmeasured following a single training session (Fig. 4A). NgR1overexpressing mice (L1 and L2) avoided re-entering the dark

compartment 1 day after pairing it with a negative conditioningevent (foot shock), similar to controls (Fig. 4B). Seven days later,there was a tendency for overexpressing mice to reenter the darkcompartment faster than controls (L1: P � 0.13; L2 P � 0.24 versuscontrols). However, both L1 and L2 NgR1 overexpressing micewere clearly impaired 1 month later, entering the dark compart-ment significantly sooner than controls (Fig. 4B). Only 33% of theL1 NgR1 overexpressing mice, compared to 75% of the littermatecontrols, refrained from entering the dark compartment during theallotted trial time of 300 s. Similarly, only 25% of the L2 NgR1overexpressing mice compared to 60% of the littermate controlsremained in the light compartment. As mice did not get a newelectric shock if they re-entered the dark compartment at interimtests carried out on day 1 or 7, it could be argued that they mighthave relearned that the dark compartment was no longer danger-ous. To control for this, we performed an additional experimentwhere NgR1 overexpressing mice (L2 strain) and littermate con-trols received the initial conditioning shock but were not subjectedto any interim passive avoidance tests before a memory test after 28days. In this test, four of nine controls vs. seven of eight NgR1overexpressing mice re-entered the dark compartment, suggestingthat relearning was not an important factor in the previous tests.When transgenic NgR1 expression was turned off in adult mice bydoxycycline, L1 and L2 mice no longer differed significantly fromcontrols (Fig. 4C). These observations strongly suggest that pres-ence of the NgR1 transgene per se, rather than for example

A B C

Dis

tan

ce (

cm) Dis

tan

ce (

cm)

CON (7)L1 (8)

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

05 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60

Minutes

600

0

1200

1800

Dis

tan

ce (

cm)

CON (10)L2 (11)

Minutes

Dis

tan

ce (

cm)

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

05 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60

600

0

1200

1800

Late

ncy

to fa

ll do

wn

(s)

0

100

200

300CON (9)L1 (7)

D E F

G H I JE

scap

e la

tenc

y (s

)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7D ays

CON (9)L2 (11)

0

20

40

60

80

1 2 3 4 5 6 7D ays

Esc

ape

late

ncy

(s) CON (8)

L1 (7)

0

20

40

60

80

CO

N (8

) L1

(7)

CO

N (9

) L2

(11)

Qua

dran

t tim

e (%

)

Qua

dran

t tim

e (%

of e

scap

e la

tenc

y)

Probe Trial day 8

Probe Trial day 39

CO

N (

7)L1

(7)

L2 (

11)

CO

N (

9)

CO

N (

6)L1

(5)

Qua

dran

t tim

e (%

)

External cues

0

20

40

60

30

10

50

****

Esc

ape

late

ncy

(s)

CO

N (

7)L1

(7)

L2 (

11)

CO

N (

9)

***

*

0

20

40

60

30

10

50

0

20

40

60

30

10

50

0

20

40

60

30

10

50

Fig. 3. Rotarod, locomotion and swim maze;NgR1transgeneimpairs long-termspatialmemory.(A) L1 mice do not differ significantly from controlsin latency before falling off a rotarod. (B and C)Spontaneous locomotorbehaviorofL1andL2micedoes not differ markedly from that of controls.Insets show first 10 min. (D) Schematic illustrationof Morris swim maze. (E and F) L1 and L2 mice donotdiffer fromcontrols inMorriswatermazetrain-ing. (G) When exposed to a probe trial (platformremoved) 1 day after the learning period there wasno significant difference between the groups withregard to time spent in the target quadrant. (H andI) L1 and L2 mice retested at day 60, with theplatform in its original position, spent less time inthe platform-containing quadrant (H; quadranttime in % of total escape latency) and had longerescape latencies than controls (I). (J) Probe trial(platform removed) performed on day 39 on anadditional group of mice showed that L1 micespent less time in the target quadrant than con-trols. Means � SEM *, P � 0.05, **, P � 0.01.

Karlen et al. PNAS � December 1, 2009 � vol. 106 � no. 48 � 20479

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE

Page 5: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

transgene integration errors, impaired the establishment of verylong-term memory. We next tested if doxycycline treatment startingdirectly after, or 7 days after, the conditioning event could rescuelong-term memory formation and chose a passive avoidance pro-tocol in which the door between the two compartments is kept openduring test sessions. The total amount of time spent in the lightcompartment at day 30 was compared to that at day 1. Transgenicmice that received doxycycline immediately after the conditioningevent performed similar to controls (P � 0.56; Fig. 4D) whiletransgenic mice that did not receive doxycycline until day 7 per-formed significantly worse (P � 0.049). One possible reason forthese differences might be a difference in anxiety levels. Wetherefore examined mice in the elevated plus-maze (37), and foundno difference between L1 and control mice (Fig. 4 E and F).

DiscussionUnderstanding the mechanisms that underlie formation and long-term maintenance of learned skills and other forms of memoriesmay aid in understanding and in the development of treatments formemory impairments in aging and disease, including stroke (38).We hypothesized that down-regulation of NgR1, the key receptorcomponent of the Nogo nerve growth inhibitory signaling systemdiscovered by Schwab and colleagues (see ref. 39), may be aprerequisite for establishment of enduring memories (22). Here weprovide genetic evidence for the hypothesis; presence of a NgR1transgene that cannot be down-regulated by neuronal activity inforebrain neurons, neutralizes the effects of down-regulation ofendogenous NgR1, and severely impairs the transition of newlyobtained memories and skills into permanently stored engrams.NgR1-transgenic mice remember an aversive event or a spatial taskfor 24 h like normal mice, but fail to remember normally after amonth. This suggests that activity-driven down-regulation of NgR1constitutes a key permissive event allowing experience-dependentneuronal plasticity to lead to lasting memories.

Using the tet-off controlled fore-brain specific NgR overexpres-sion, we were able to conclude that ongoing NgR1 overexpressionin cortex and hippocampus does not impair the ability to formlasting memories, provided that transgene transcription is turnedoff by doxycycline directly after a conditioning event. However, iftransgene transcription is not turned off in forebrain cortical areasuntil a week after the conditioning event, leading to loss oftransgenic NgR protein a few days later, long-term memory for-mation is impaired, restricting the window of time during whichNgR1 down-regulation is important for memory consolidation todays 3–10 after a memory-forming event.

Since LTP, a model for the acute plastic changes in synapticstrength that are thought to underlie memory formation in mam-malian CNS (40, 41), occurs on a time scale of minutes, and ourbehavioral data suggested that 24 h memories were not affected byNgR1 overexpression, we hypothesized and demonstrated that LTPwas unaffected by NgR1 overexpression. Likewise, we found L-LTPnot to be affected. While LTD is difficult to observe in adult mice(32), a related phenomenon, de-potentiation, was also found not tobe affected. These results support previous work showing that lackof NgR1 also does not impair acute electrophysiological character-istics in adult hippocampal slices (31). Together this suggests thatNgR1 is not primarily involved in the initiation of memory.

An interesting mechanism for NgR1-mediated plasticity,whereby NgR1, but not NgR2, acts as a negative regulator ofFGF2-induced neuritic growth was recently demonstrated (31).FGF2 and FGF1 were shown to have high affinity for NgR1 and toexert NgR1-regulated effects. Our findings are compatible withsuch a modus operandi for NgR1 in the formation of lastingmemories but do not exclude a role for other NgR1 ligands, suchas Nogo itself.

The establishment of very long-term memories involves severalphases from immediate electrical and chemical bidirectional trans-synaptic events, via intermediate phases, which may involve synapticrearrangements, to a final stage in which memories presumably

A B

C

D E F

% d

urat

ion

in o

pen

arm

s

CON (7)L1 (7)

% o

pen

arm

s en

trie

s

CON (7)L1 (7)

***

24 h 1 month

Ret

entio

n la

tenc

y 120%

100%

80%

60%

40%

20%

0

140%

160%

180%

CO

N (1

6)L1

(15)

L2 (1

0)

CO

N (1

0)

CO

N (1

6)L1

(15)

L2 (1

0)

CO

N (1

0)

1 month + DOX

CO

N (2

4)

L1 (5

)L2

(12)

Ret

entio

n la

tenc

y 120%

100%

80%

60%

40%

20%

0

140%

160%

180%

10

20

30

40

50

60

+DO

X +a

cute

DO

X +d

elay

ed D

OX

70

80

90

100

Ligh

t are

a pr

efer

ence

(day

30

/day

1)

*

CON (17) L1 (10+9)

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

Fig. 4. Long-term passive avoidance impairment is NgR1transgene dependent, elevated plus maze behavior is notdisturbed. (A) Passive avoidance set up. (B) There is nodifference between L1 or L2 mice and controls 24 h aftertraining but at 30 days transgenic mice enter the darkcompartment significantly sooner than controls (datashown as % of control mean values � SEM; *, P � 0.05 **,P�0.01;Student’s two-tailed t-test). (C)Thisdifferencewasnot seen in doxycycline treated animals. (D) L1 mice ex-posed to doxycycline beginning immediately (acute) afterthe shock performed as controls, while L1 mice that re-ceived doxycycline from day 7 (delayed) performed worsethan controls. (E and F) Elevated plus maze behavior. Nei-ther time spent in the open arms (E), nor entries into theopen arms (F), differed between NgR overexpressing andcontrol mice (ANOVA). Means � SEM.

20480 � www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.0905390106 Karlen et al.

Page 6: Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories

become represented by more or less permanent synaptic rearrange-ments. Our results using passive avoidance and the Morris swimmaze suggest that these later stages, in which memories finallybecome stable constituents of CNS circuitry, are critically depen-dent on NgR1 signaling regulation. Because we find large amountsof transgenic NgR1 in the cell membrane, we hypothesize that lossof NgR1 in the pre- and/or postsynaptic membrane, in parallel withregulation of NCAM and other cell adhesion systems (42), areneeded for boutons and dendritic spines to become temporarilyinsensitive to Nogo and/or other NgR1 ligands in neurite mem-branes, detach, and become capable of rearrangements in responseto locally increased levels of BDNF (43), FGF (31), and otherpositive and negative neurotropic signals. Once new synaptic ar-rangements have been established, reversal of the activity-inducedmolecular changes would stabilize the connections.

The fact that there is a window in time during which retrogradeamnesia can be induced (44), suggests that there are checkpointsalong the road to lasting memories, such that newly developedstructural changes need time to become stable. Studies of LTP/LTD show that chemical changes underlie the changes of synapticstrength recorded by electrophysiological techniques (34). Struc-tural changes presumably constitute the intermediate step andbecome the long-term memory storage substratum. The timewindow for structural spine changes recently demonstrated in vivofor experience (monocular deprivation) to leave lasting changes incortical neurons (4), approximately 4–16 days, is fully compatiblewith both the temporal characteristics of retrograde amnesia, theshort memory span of our NgR1 overexpressing mice and the factthat immediate, but not 1-week-delayed silencing of the transgenenormalizes long-term memory in these mice. The fact that targeting

CamKII will cause mice to have impaired very long-term (10–50days), but not 1- to 3-day memories (29) is evidence of a crucialinvolvement of CamKII in the process of forming very long-termmemories. Our data show that NgR1 constitutes another keyregulator of this process. PirB, a second receptor for Nogo, MAGand OMgp (25), is another putative long-term memory regulator inneurons that express this gene. Finally, our results show that NgR1signaling continues to be important for the formation of lastingmemories also in aged mice.

Better insight into mechanisms underlying structural plasticityand its consolidation at the synaptic level will help explain howlifelong memories are formed and maintained. Understanding thefull repertoire of synaptic plasticity control exercised by NgR1regulation may aid the development of methods to improve plas-ticity and long-term memory.

MethodsThe two key long term memory tests used were passive avoidance and Morriswater maze (see SI Text for details). For generation of transgenic mice, in situhybridization, immunohistochemistry, immunoblotting, electrophysiology,RhoA assay, spine counts, HPLC, membrane fractionation, running wheel,rotarod, elevated plus-maze, locomotion tests, and statistical analysis, details,and descriptions are given in SI Text. Experiments were approved by theStockholm Animal Ethics committee.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank Eva Lindqvist and Karin Pernold for excellenttechnicalassistance,andMattiasKarlen for theartwork.Thisworkwassupportedby The Swedish Medical Research Council, Torsten and Ragnar Soderberg’s Foun-dation, The Swedish Brain Foundation, the Hallsten Foundation, Swedish BrainPower, AFA (Arbetsmarknadens forsakringsaktiebolag) Sweden, U.S. PublicHealth Service grants, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes ofHealth, and the Karolinska Institute.

1. Raisman G (1969) Neuronal plasticity in the septal nuclei of the adult rat. Brain Res14:25–48.

2. Engert F, Bonhoeffer T (1999) Dendritic spine changes associated with hippocampallong-term synaptic plasticity. Nature 399:66–70.

3. Holtmaat A, Wilbrecht L, Knott GW, Welker E, Svoboda K (2006) Experience-dependentand cell-type-specific spine growth in the neocortex. Nature 441:979–983.

4. Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Bonhoeffer T, Hubener M (2009) Experience leaves a lastingstructural trace in cortical circuits. Nature 457:313–317.

5. Matus A, Brinkhaus H, Wagner U (2000) Actin dynamics in dendritic spines: A form ofregulated plasticity at excitatory synapses. Hippocampus 10:555–560.

6. Prinjha R, et al. (2000) Inhibitor of neurite outgrowth in humans. Nature 403:383–384.7. Chen MS, et al. (2000) Nogo-A is a myelin-associated neurite outgrowth inhibitor and an

antigen for monoclonal antibody IN-1. Nature 403:434–439.8. GrandPre T, Nakamura F, Vartanian T, Strittmatter SM (2000) Identification of the Nogo

inhibitor of axon regeneration as a Reticulon protein. Nature 403:439–444.9. McKerracher L, et al. (1994) Identification of myelin-associated glycoprotein as a major

myelin-derived inhibitor of neurite growth. Neuron 13:805–811.10. Mukhopadhyay G, Doherty P, Walsh FS, Crocker PR, Filbin MT (1994) A novel role for

myelin-associated glycoprotein as an inhibitor of axonal regeneration. Neuron 13:757–767.

11. WangKC,etal. (2002)Oligodendrocyte-myelinglycoprotein isaNogoreceptor ligandthatinhibits neurite outgrowth. Nature 417:941–944.

12. Liu BP, Fournier A, GrandPre T, Strittmatter SM (2002) Myelin-associated glycoprotein asa functional ligand for the Nogo-66 receptor. Science 297:1190–1193.

13. Fournier AE, GrandPre T, Strittmatter SM (2001) Identification of a receptor mediatingNogo-66 inhibition of axonal regeneration. Nature 409:341–346.

14. Mi S, et al. (2004) LINGO-1 is a component of the Nogo-66 receptor/p75 signaling complex.Nat Neurosci 7:221–228.

15. Park JB, et al. (2005) A TNF receptor family member, TROY, is a coreceptor with Nogoreceptor in mediating the inhibitory activity of myelin inhibitors. Neuron 45:345–351.

16. Shao Z, et al. (2005) TAJ/TROY, an orphan TNF receptor family member, binds Nogo-66receptor 1 and regulates axonal regeneration. Neuron 45:353–359.

17. Wang KC, Kim JA, Sivasankaran R, Segal R, He Z (2002) p75 interacts with the Nogoreceptor as a co-receptor for Nogo, MAG and Omgp. Nature 420:74–78.

18. Chivatakarn O, Kaneko S, He Z, Tessier-Lavigne M, Giger RJ (2007) The Nogo-66 receptorNgR1 is required only for the acute growth cone-collapsing but not the chronic growth-inhibitory actions of myelin inhibitors. J Neurosci 27:7117–7124.

19. Josephson A, et al. (2002) Nogo-receptor gene activity: Cellular localization and develop-mental regulation of mRNA in mice and humans. J Comp Neurol 453:292–304.

20. Martin KC, Kandel ER (1996) Cell adhesion molecules, CREB, and the formation of newsynaptic connections. Neuron 17:567–570.

21. JosephsonA,Widenfalk J,WidmerHW,OlsonL,SpengerC (2001)NOGOmRNAexpressionin adult and fetal human and rat nervous tissue and in weight drop injury. Exp Neurol169:319–328.

22. Josephson A, et al. (2003) Activity-induced and developmental down-regulation of theNogo receptor. Cell Tissue Res 311:333–342.

23. Endo T, Spenger C, Tominaga T, Brene S, Olson L (2007) Cortical sensory map rearrange-ment after spinal cord injury: fMRI responses linked to Nogo signaling. Brain 130:2951–2961.

24. McGee AW, Yang Y, Fischer QS, Daw NW, Strittmatter SM (2005) Experience-drivenplasticity of visual cortex limited by myelin and Nogo receptor. Science 309:2222–2226.

25. Syken J, Grandpre T, Kanold PO, Shatz CJ (2006) PirB restricts ocular-dominance plasticityin visual cortex. Science 313:1795–1800.

26. Atwal JK, et al. (2008) PirB is a functional receptor for myelin inhibitors of axonalregeneration. Science 322:967–970.

27. Burgin KE, et al. (1990) In situ hybridization histochemistry of Ca2�/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase in developing rat brain. J Neurosci 10:1788–1798.

28. Bayer KU, De Koninck P, Leonard AS, Hell JW, Schulman H (2001) Interaction with theNMDA receptor locks CaMKII in an active conformation. Nature 411:801–805.

29. Frankland PW, O’Brien C, Ohno M, Kirkwood A, Silva AJ (2001) Alpha-CaMKII-dependentplasticity in the cortex is required for permanent memory. Nature 411:309–313.

30. Miller S, et al. (2002) Disruption of dendritic translation of CaMKIIalpha impairs stabiliza-tion of synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation. Neuron 36:507–519.

31. Lee H, et al. (2008) Synaptic function for the Nogo-66 receptor NgR1: Regulation ofdendritic spinemorphologyandactivity-dependent synaptic strength. JNeurosci28:2753–2765.

32. Wagner JJ, Alger BE (1996) Homosynaptic LTD and depotentiation: Do they differ in nameonly? Hippocampus 6:24–29.

33. Staubli U, Scafidi J (1999) Time-dependent reversal of long-term potentiation in areaCA1 of the freely moving rat induced by theta pulse stimulation. J Neurosci 19:8712–8719.

34. Yang Y, Wang XB, Frerking M, Zhou Q (2008) Spine expansion and stabilization associatedwith long-term potentiation. J Neurosci 28:5740–5751.

35. Madjid N, et al. (2006) 5-Hydroxytryptamine 1A receptor blockade facilitates aversivelearning in mice: Interactions with cholinergic and glutamatergic mechanisms. J Pharma-col Exp Ther 316:581–591.

36. StiedlO,MisaneI,Spiess J,OgrenSO(2000) Involvementofthe5-HT1Areceptors inclassicalfear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice. J Neurosci 20:8515–8527.

37. File SE (2001) Factors controlling measures of anxiety and responses to novelty in themouse. Behav Brain Res 125:151–157.

38. Lee JK, Kim JE, Sivula M, Strittmatter SM (2004) Nogo receptor antagonism promotesstroke recovery by enhancing axonal plasticity. J Neurosci 24:6209–6217.

39. Gonzenbach RR, Schwab ME (2008) Disinhibition of neurite growth to repair the injuredadult CNS: Focusing on Nogo. Cell Mol Life Sci 65:161–176.

40. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL (1993) A synaptic model of memory: Long-term potentiation inthe hippocampus. Nature 361:31–39.

41. Bliss TV, Lomo T (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentatearea of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol232:331–356.

42. Kleene R, Schachner M (2004) Glycans and neural cell interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci5:195–208.

43. Lu B (2003) BDNF and activity-dependent synaptic modulation. Learn Mem 10:86–98.44. McGaugh JL (2002) Memory consolidation and the amygdala: A systems perspective.

Trends Neurosci 25:456.

Karlen et al. PNAS � December 1, 2009 � vol. 106 � no. 48 � 20481

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE