Top Banner
Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society *Yoon-Hee Cha 1 , John Golding 2 , Behrang Keshavarz 3 , Joseph Furman 4 , Ji-Soo Kim 5 , Jose A. Lopez- Escamez 6 , Måns Magnusson 7 , Bill J. Yates 8 Advisers: Jeffrey Staab 9 and Alexandre Bisdorff 10 1 Department of Neurology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN., USA 2 Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, England 3 Toronto Rehabilitation Institute University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada; Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, ON, Canada 4 Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA., USA 5 Department of Neurology Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea 6 Otology and Neurotology Group CTS495, Department of Genomic Medicine, Centre for Genomics and Oncology Research Pfizer/Universidad de Granada/Junta de Andalucía (GENyO), PTS, Granada, Spain 7 Department of Otolaryngology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden 8 Department of Otolaryngology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA., USA 9 Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN., USA 10 Department of Neurology, Centre Hospitalier Emile Mayrisch, L-4005 Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg Abstract We present diagnostic criteria for motion sickness to be included in the International Classification of Vestibular Disorders. Motion sickness is a normal physiologic response that can be elicited in almost all individuals, but susceptibility and severity can be high enough to be considered a disorder in some cases. Motion sickness may be induced by motion of the head or by motion of the visual field. Sickness susceptibility to each motion stimulus type may vary within individuals. An episode of motion sickness may be diagnosed when all of the following criteria are met: A. One or more of the following symptoms triggered by passive or active head motion: nausea, stomach awareness, sweating, headache, or drowsiness, B. Symptom or symptoms develop during the motion exposure, C. Symptom(s) onset is gradual, D. Symptom(s) eventually stop after cessation of motion, E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder. Criteria for an episode of visually induced motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain or blurred vision may additionally qualify for Criterion A. A motion sickness disorder (MSD) may be diagnosed when all of the following criteria are met: A. Recurrent episodes of motion sickness triggered by the same class of motion stimuli (Definite MSD=five episodes, Probable MSD=two to four episodes), B. Symptoms are reliably triggered by the same class of motion stimuli, C. Symptom severity does not significantly decrease despite repeated exposure to the same class of motion stimuli, D. Symptoms lead to one or more of the following behavioral responses: a. Activity modification to abort sickness symptoms, b. Avoidance of motion stimuli that trigger sickness, c. Negative anticipatory emotions before exposure to motion stimuli, E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder. A diagnosis of Visually-Induced Motion Sickness
21

Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Oct 27, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria

Consensus document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society

*Yoon-Hee Cha1, John Golding2, Behrang Keshavarz3, Joseph Furman4, Ji-Soo Kim5, Jose A. Lopez-

Escamez6, Måns Magnusson7, Bill J. Yates8

Advisers: Jeffrey Staab9 and Alexandre Bisdorff10

1Department of Neurology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN., USA 2Department of Psychology, University of Westminster, London, England 3Toronto Rehabilitation Institute – University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada; Department of

Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, ON, Canada 4Department of Neurology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA., USA 5Department of Neurology Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea 6Otology and Neurotology Group CTS495, Department of Genomic Medicine, Centre for Genomics and

Oncology Research – Pfizer/Universidad de Granada/Junta de Andalucía (GENyO), PTS, Granada,

Spain 7Department of Otolaryngology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden

8Department of Otolaryngology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA., USA 9Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN., USA 10Department of Neurology, Centre Hospitalier Emile Mayrisch, L-4005 Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg

Abstract

We present diagnostic criteria for motion sickness to be included in the International Classification of

Vestibular Disorders. Motion sickness is a normal physiologic response that can be elicited in almost all

individuals, but susceptibility and severity can be high enough to be considered a disorder in some cases.

Motion sickness may be induced by motion of the head or by motion of the visual field. Sickness

susceptibility to each motion stimulus type may vary within individuals.

An episode of motion sickness may be diagnosed when all of the following criteria are met: A. One or

more of the following symptoms triggered by passive or active head motion: nausea, stomach

awareness, sweating, headache, or drowsiness, B. Symptom or symptoms develop during the motion

exposure, C. Symptom(s) onset is gradual, D. Symptom(s) eventually stop after cessation of motion, E.

Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder. Criteria for an episode of visually induced

motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion

and eye strain or blurred vision may additionally qualify for Criterion A.

A motion sickness disorder (MSD) may be diagnosed when all of the following criteria are met: A.

Recurrent episodes of motion sickness triggered by the same class of motion stimuli (Definite

MSD=five episodes, Probable MSD=two to four episodes), B. Symptoms are reliably triggered by the

same class of motion stimuli, C. Symptom severity does not significantly decrease despite repeated

exposure to the same class of motion stimuli, D. Symptoms lead to one or more of the following

behavioral responses: a. Activity modification to abort sickness symptoms, b. Avoidance of motion

stimuli that trigger sickness, c. Negative anticipatory emotions before exposure to motion stimuli, E. Not

better accounted for by another disease or disorder. A diagnosis of Visually-Induced Motion Sickness

Page 2: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Disorder (VIMSD) is made when the triggering stimulus is visual motion. An individual may be

diagnosed with MSD, VIMSD, or both.

1. Introduction

The Classification Committee of the Bárány Society was charged with developing diagnostic criteria for

motion sickness. These criteria were developed by an international group of vestibular specialists,

scientists, and therapists for the Bárány Society in order to promote a common reference frame across

disciplines involved in the care of the general public and in individuals with increased susceptibility to

motion sickness. Though a common phenomenon, sickness induced by head or visual motion can

adversely affect otherwise healthy individuals and present serious safety issues in situations that require

high levels of attention. The establishment of these criteria recognizes that motion sickness, while being

a normal physiologic response, can have profound negative effects when severe.

1.1 History

Descriptions of motion sickness date back to 400BCE and have trended with evolution in modes of

transportation [1]. Theories to explain this bothersome phenomenon evolved over the centuries,

including dysequilibrium of the four humors (yellow bile, black bile, phlegm, blood), abnormal flow of

qì (life force), coordination of sensory inputs through the eyes and ears, and the effects of motion on

inner ear function [1]-[3].

Interest in the mechanisms, treatment, and prevention of motion sickness had been historically driven by

the military as they faced practical aspects of long-distance transportation of personnel in caravans,

ships, and planes. The development of the space program in the 1960’s further spurred interest in motion

sickness research, creating a foundation of clinical and physiologic assessment tools on which modern

motion sickness research developed [4].

More recently, motion sickness that is mainly caused by stimulation of the visual system in the absence

of physical movement has received increased recognition [5][6]. Scientific reports of visually induced

motion sickness (VIMS) were first documented in the military domain when flight simulators were

found to cause motion sickness-like symptoms in some individuals [7]-[9]. Due to technological

advancements over the past decades, visual displays and applications have become pervasive (smart

phones, tablets, simulators, and virtual reality glasses), making VIMS a common phenomenon that

affects a broader population.

1.2 Theories

Different theories exist for the generation, consequence, and modulation of motion sickness and VIMS

(see Golding et al., 2016 and Keshavarz 2014, for discussions [5][10]) [4][11]-[13]. The sensory conflict

and mismatch theories of motion sickness are arguably the most common explanations; they pertain to

the conflict between expected versus actual visual, vestibular, and somatosensory inputs [4][11]. This

conflict can be due to dissonance among sensory systems [14], mismatch between canal and otolith

inputs (reviewed in [15]), or a discrepancy between perceived and expected verticality [16]. Motion

sickness may be a by-product of the activation of vestibulo-autonomic pathways by head motion or

visual stimuli to which the human nervous system is unable to or has not had sufficient time to adapt

[5][17][18]. These theories predict that motion sickness would diminish with repeated exposure as the

internal model of expected sensory input updates with new experiences.

Page 3: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

In contrast, the toxin theory of motion sickness postulates that motion induced nausea and emesis may

be evolutionarily protective responses to visual-vestibular mismatch that resembles the ingestion of

poisonous food [19]. Consequently, the organism responds with retching and vomiting to expel the

poison. A shared susceptibility to triggering the emetic pathway by different mechanisms is supported

by the observation that individuals with high motion sickness susceptibility are more likely to suffer

from nausea and vomiting from other causes, such as chemotherapy and actual toxins [20]. Motion

sickness may also be an unfortunate consequence of the proximity of anatomical pathways that mediate

vestibular signals with nausea and vomiting, without conferring any functional benefit to the individual.

[10][21].

2. Methods

Members of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society (CCBS) who met in Berlin, Germany in

March 2017 proposed the creation of a subcommittee to develop criteria for motion sickness for the

International Classification of Vestibular Disorders (ICVD). They selected a Chairperson (YHC) to

choose subcommittee members. Subcommittee members were chosen to represent the fields of

neurology, otolaryngology, psychology, rehabilitation, and vestibular physiology with committee

members representing at least three continents. Communication among subcommittee members and

between the Chairperson and individual committee members occurred through email and personal

communication at meetings. The CCBS met again before the 30th Bárány Society meeting in Uppsala,

Sweden in June 2018 and in Berlin, Germany in November 2019 to discuss progress. Diagnostic criteria

were developed through discussions among subcommittee members. Draft criteria were presented to the

CCBS in November 2019 and then modified based on comments. A revised draft was made available

for comment by the Bárány Society membership in January 2020. Further comments and concerns were

addressed before submission for publication.

3. Diagnostic Criteria for Motion Sickness

Motion sickness is a poly-symptomatic syndrome that is influenced by factors such as individual

susceptibility (age and sex), stimulus type (self or visual motion), and specific circumstances (biological

rhythms). The development of these criteria acknowledges the large body of observational,

interventional, epidemiologic, and physiological studies that have studied the contribution of these

factors to motion sickness severity and susceptibility [22]-[25].

3.1.1 Criteria for an Episode of Motion Sickness

An episode in which Criteria A through E are met:

A. One or more of the following symptoms triggered by passive or active head motion1:

1. Nausea2

2. Stomach awareness3

3. Sweating4

4. Headache5

5. Drowsiness6

B. Symptom or symptoms develop during the motion exposure7

C. Symptom(s) onset is gradual8

D. Symptom(s) eventually stop after cessation of motion9

E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder10

Page 4: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

NOTES:

1. A variety of motion stimuli can trigger motion sickness but susceptibility to one type of stimulus does

not necessarily correlate with susceptibility to others. Trigger subtypes may include but are not limited

to the following:

a. Water transportation, e.g., boats, rafts, snorkeling, scuba-diving, and docks

b. Air transportation, e.g., airplanes, helicopters, hang-gliding, parasailing, and parabolic flight

c. Land transportation, e.g., cars, trains, buses, other land-based vehicles, and animal-based

transportation

d. Other moving platform, e.g. amusement park rides, waterbeds, and treadmills.

e. Low frequency sway of the tops of very tall buildings

f. Vestibular stimulation in laboratory settings, e.g., off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR), horizontal

or vertical low frequency linear oscillation, and human centrifuges

g. Space travel, e.g., orbiting, launching, re-entry.

2. Nausea may be mild or severe. Though the probability of vomiting is generally related to the severity

of nausea, vomiting may also occur at lower levels of nausea in some individuals. These cases are

uncommon, however.

3. Stomach awareness is an early manifestation of increased interoceptive awareness in motion sickness.

Other less common manifestations include awareness of breathing and heartbeat; other visceral or

somatic perceptions may be present, however.

4. Sweating in motion sickness is generally a ‘cold’ sweat that can accompany other autonomic

symptoms and signs such as pallor, increased salivation, or decreased gastric motility.

5. Increased motion sickness susceptibility occurs in individuals with migraine or vestibular migraine

[26] (See section 4.5.1) but headaches triggered by motion are not necessarily migraine headaches.

6. Arousal changes may result in drowsiness, yawning, sleepiness, or lethargy. These symptoms have

historically been referred to as the ‘sopite’ syndrome [27].

7. Though some symptoms of motion sickness may persist after termination of the stimulus, the onset of

sickness symptoms must occur during the motion stimulus and not exclusively after the stimulus has

ended. This distinguishes motion sickness from mal de débarquement syndrome (MdDS), which only

begins once the motion has ended and lasts for at least 48-hours [28].

8. The onset of sickness symptoms occurs gradually and generally after some delay relative to the onset

of the sickness inducing motion. Symptoms that start immediately with head movement or are maximal

at onset should raise suspicion for a structural vestibular disorder. Though motion sickness gradually

increases with longer duration of exposure, severity starts to plateau with time. If vomiting is to occur, it

generally happens within 60-minutes of the onset of other symptoms, such as nausea [17][29].

Continued exposure may also lead to habituation and eventual reduction of sickness.

9. Recovery from motion sickness is usually rapid after the cessation of motion. Severity should be less

than 50% of the peak intensity within 1 hour of the cessation of motion. Some degree of residual

symptoms may persist for hours after the cessation of motion, but generally abate within the day. An

Page 5: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

exception is made for headache, which can last until specifically treated. In rare cases, an episode of

motion sickness can lead to persistent symptoms despite the cessation of motion. These cases should

prompt a search for secondary causes of symptoms that can overlap with motion sickness.

10. Motion sickness may coexist and can be impacted by the presence of vestibular disorders such as

vestibular migraine, vestibular neuritis, or persistent postural perceptual dizziness. In those situations,

both a diagnosis of motion sickness and the contributing disorder should be made. Motion sickness

should not be equated with motion-induced vertigo. Vertigo is specifically defined as a “sensation of

self-motion when no self-motion is occurring or the sensation of distorted self-motion during an

otherwise normal head movement,” [30]. Though clinical entities that cause vertigo can also cause

symptoms similar to motion sickness, motion sickness inducing stimuli do not cause an altered sensation

of self-motion. When an episode of vertigo leads to overlapping symptoms with motion sickness, these

symptoms are considered sequelae of the vertigo spell and not a separate motion sickness syndrome.

3.1.2 Criteria for an Episode of Visually Induced Motion Sickness (VIMS)

An episode in which Criteria A through E are met:

A. One or more of the following symptoms triggered by visual motion1:

1. Nausea2

2. Stomach awareness3

3. Sweating4

4. Headache5

5. Drowsiness6

6. Eye strain/blurred vision7

B. Symptom or symptoms develop during the visual motion exposure8

C. Symptom(s) onset is gradual9

D. Symptom(s) eventually stop after cessation of visual motion10

E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder11

NOTES:

1. Visual stimulation may be in either laboratory or real-life settings, e.g. virtual reality, simulators,

movies, tablets, smartphones, computers, video games, and optokinetic drums. When visually triggered

motion sickness also includes head motion, e.g., motion-based flight simulators, driving simulators, ship

simulators, the contributions of both stimuli should be noted.

2. Nausea may be mild or severe. Though the probability of vomiting is generally related to the severity

of nausea, vomiting may also occur at lower levels of nausea in some individuals. These cases are

uncommon, however.

3. Stomach awareness is an early manifestation of increased interoceptive attention in VIMS. Other less

common manifestations include awareness of breathing and heartbeat; other visceral or somatic

perceptions may be present, however.

4. Sweating in VIMS is generally a ‘cold’ sweat that can accompany other autonomic symptoms and

signs such as pallor, increased salivation, or decreased gastric motility.

Page 6: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

5. Headache is more common and may occur more frequently without nausea in VIMS than in motion

sickness due to movement of the head [5][6].

6. Arousal changes may result in drowsiness, yawning, sleepiness, or lethargy. These symptoms have

historically been referred to as the ‘sopite’ syndrome [27].

7. Eyestrain and/or visual blurring are particularly common symptoms of VIMS [5][6][24].

8. Though some symptoms of VIMS may persist after termination of the stimulus, the onset of sickness

symptoms must occur during the visual motion stimulus and not exclusively after the stimulus has

ended.

9. The onset of sickness symptoms occurs gradually and generally after some delay relative to the onset

of the sickness inducing motion. Symptoms that start immediately with visual motion should raise

suspicion for a primary ocular disorder. Though VIMS gradually increases with longer duration of

exposure, severity starts to plateau with time. If vomiting is to occur, it generally happens within 60-

minutes of the onset of other symptoms, such as nausea [17][29]. Continued exposure may also lead to

habituation and eventual reduction of VIMS.

10. Recovery from VIMS is usually rapid after the cessation of visual motion. Severity should be less

than 50% of the peak intensity within 1 hour of the cessation of visual motion. Some degree of residual

symptoms may persist for hours after the cessation of visual motion, but generally abate within the day.

An exception is made for headache, which can last until specifically treated. In rare cases, an episode of

VIMS can lead to persistent symptoms despite the cessation of visual motion. These cases should

prompt a search for secondary causes of symptoms that can overlap with VIMS.

11. VIMS should be distinguished from visually induced vertigo as defined in the ICVD [30]. Although

they can occur simultaneously, VIMS is primarily characterized by a visceral reaction (e.g., nausea,

stomach awareness) whereas visually induced vertigo relates to the perception of self-motion or vection.

Visually induced dizziness (VID) is recognized as a sense of spatial disorientation caused by moving or

visually complex stimuli but there are no formal criteria established for VID [30]. VIMS should be

diagnosed according to the criteria presented; when dizziness is the only symptom induced by visual

stimulation, the designation should remain as VID. It is remains to be determined, however, whether

VID and VIMS are biologically distinct disorders.

3.2.1 Criteria for Motion Sickness Disorder (MSD)

A syndrome in which Criteria A through E are met:

A. Recurrent episodes of motion sickness triggered by the same class of motion stimuli1

a. Probable MSD: two to four episodes

b. Definite MSD: five or more episodes

B. Symptoms are reliably triggered by the same class of motion stimuli2

C. Symptom severity does not significantly decrease after repeated exposure to the same class of motion

stimuli3

D. Symptoms lead to one or more of the following behavioral responses:

a. Activity modification to abort sickness symptoms

b. Avoidance of motion stimuli that trigger sickness

Page 7: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

c. Negative anticipatory emotions prior to exposure to motion stimuli4

E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder5

NOTES:

1. Susceptibility to motion sickness from one type of motion may not translate to other types of motion.

Therefore, motion sickness to each motion stimulus type, e.g. airplanes, automobiles, boats, should be

considered separately (See Note 1, Section 3.1.1)

2. Though vehicle types vary within each class, the induction of sickness should be reasonably

predictable for each stimulus class, e.g. automobiles.

3. Susceptibility to motion sickness normally habituates with repeated exposures. The repeated

triggering of sickness to the same stimulus signifies an inability to habituate and is a core feature of

MSD.

4. The usual behavioral response to motion sickness is to avoid sickness-inducing motion stimuli or to

shorten the duration of exposure, when possible. When neither is possible, negative anticipatory

emotions prior to exposure to motion stimuli may count toward a behavioral response to motion

sickness.

5. An individual may be diagnosed with MSD, Visually Induced Motion Sickness Disorder (VIMSD,

see Section 3.2.2), or both according to the stimulus class.

3.2.2 Criteria for Visually Induced Motion Sickness Disorder (VIMSD)

A syndrome in which Criteria A through E are met:

A. Recurrent episodes of motion sickness triggered by the same class of visual motion stimuli1

a. Probable VIMSD: two to four episodes

b. Definite VIMSD: five or more episodes

B. Symptoms are reliably triggered by the same class of visual motion stimuli2

C. Symptom severity does not significantly decrease after repeated exposure to the same class of visual

motion stimuli3

D. Symptoms lead to one or more of the following behavioral responses:

a. Activity modification to abort sickness symptoms

b. Avoidance of visual motion stimuli that trigger sickness

c. Negative anticipatory emotions prior to exposure to visual motion stimuli4

E. Not better accounted for by another disease or disorder5

NOTES:

1. Susceptibility to VIMS from one type of visual motion may not translate to other types of visual

motion. Therefore, VIMS to each visual motion stimulus type (See Note 1, Section 3.1.2) should be

considered separately.

2. Though stimulation types vary within each class, the induction of VIMS should be reasonably

predictable for each stimulus class, e.g. virtual reality goggles.

Page 8: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

3. Susceptibility to VIMS normally habituates with repeated exposures. The repeated triggering of

sickness to the same stimulus signifies an inability to habituate and is a core feature of VIMSD.

4. The usual behavioral response to VIMS is to avoid sickness-inducing motion stimuli or to shorten the

duration of exposure, when possible. When neither is possible, negative anticipatory emotions prior to

being exposed to visual motion may count toward a behavioral response to VIMS.

5. An individual may be diagnosed with VIMSD, MSD, or both according to the stimulus class.

3.3 Motion Sickness Susceptibility and Severity

The majority of existing motion sickness scales present composite scores made up of 1-5 categories of

symptoms with subscores for each category, when applicable. Higher scores represent greater severity or

susceptibility. The common elements of each scale are presented here to show the overlap in queried

symptoms (per Criteria 3.1 A) (Table 1 for Severity [10][22][24][25][31]-[35] and Table 2 for

Susceptibility [4][23][36]-[38].

The subcommittee recommends that each scale be used for its specific advantages to the research

question or clinical application, as needed, e.g. Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire (MSSQ-

Short form if several language translations are needed [23][38]; Simulator Susceptibility Questionnaire

(SSQ) if VIMS is the focus [24]. For brief assessments in clinical settings, patients may be asked how

frequently they feel sick during motion on a simple Likert scale, e.g. “Never,” “Sometimes,” “Often,” or

“Always.” Subjective susceptibility assessments on simple scales have strong correlation

with more detailed assessments that query sickness to specific triggers [23][38][39]. Similarly, activity

avoidance as a measure of susceptibility can be ascertained by querying how usual it is for the patient to

avoid certain motion stimuli in order to avoid becoming motion sick, e.g. “Never,” “Sometimes,”

“Often,” or “Always.”

While motion sickness can be induced in nearly all individuals with a sufficiently strong stimulus, there

are individuals at the extremes of the population distribution of susceptibility. These may occur in the

setting of normal or abnormal central or peripheral vestibular function. The distribution curve of

susceptibility is linear up to about the 75Th percentile of susceptibility and flattens out on the high end of

scores, e.g. on the MSSQ-Short form, out of a total possible score of 54, a score of 11=50th percentile,

19=75th percentile, 27=90th percentile, 31=95th percentile, indicating that only a small proportion of

individuals are severely susceptible to motion sickness, i.e. 90% of individuals score 0-27 while 10% of

individuals score 28-54 on the MSSQ scale [37].

Norms may be different depending on the age and sex distribution of the population sampled, which

should be considered when working with non-representative populations [39][40]. Motion sickness

susceptibility changes with age with a large transition occurring during adolescence. Therefore,

defining whether the symptoms refer to the ≤12 year range or the >12 year range will help in accurate

communication of current status and aid in determination of prognostic variables. Based on a simplified

scoring system of the MSSQ [23][38] adopted from Reason and Brand, 1975 [4], the Pearson correlation

between childhood (Part A) and adult motion sickness scores (Part B) is r = .65 [23][38].

Despite the similarity in etiology and symptomatology, susceptibility to motion sickness is not a reliable

predictor of susceptibility to VIMS. For instance, older adults are more susceptible to VIMS but not to

Page 9: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

motion sickness than younger adults [39]. Nausea and vomiting are typically less common in VIMS,

whereas ocular motor issues (e.g., eyestrain, blurred vision) and headache are more common in VIMS

than in motion sickness caused by self-motion [23][38][41].

Motion sickness severity and susceptibility can be modulated by reducing exposure to sickness-inducing

stimuli or, conversely, by engaging in measured amounts of motion exposure to induce habituation.

Habituation may be stimulus specific, however and may not generalize across motion types or situations.

Environmental adaptations such as roll-stabilizers on ships, active suspension in cars, or virtual reality

displays with short head motion response lags can reduce the chances of inducing motion sickness.

Interventions such as habituation exercises (eye-head motion, repeated exposure [17]), pharmacologic

pretreatment (anti-muscarinic, anti-histaminic, anti-cholinergic medications [42]); non-pharmacological

treatments (music, smells [43]), and behavioral techniques (breathing exercises, meditation [44]) can all

adjust severity.

3.4 MSD and VIMSD impact

Motion sickness that is mild, easily avoidable, or has no functional impact is common among

individuals with normal vestibular function and should not be considered a disorder. However, motion

sickness susceptibility that affects activities of daily living such as meeting basic transportation needs or

fulfilling obligations to family, employment, or social functions can have physical and mental health

consequences. Even in otherwise healthy individuals, motion sickness can reduce work or school

productivity through direct effects from being ill, avoidance of sickness-inducing activities, or sedation

induced by pharmacological motion sickness remedies [10][45][46]. Nausea, lethargy, drowsiness, and

increased interoceptive awareness can shift attention away from critical tasks while vomiting can lead to

dehydration or aspiration [27][47].

Formal disability scales may be paired with existing susceptibility scales to quantify disability due to

MSD or VIMSD. As an example, the Sheehan Disability Scale quantifies impact on work, family, or

social function on a 10-point scale along with a query of loss of work time or productivity due to

symptoms [48]. Alternatively, a basic impact assessment that queries whether work, social, family, or

travel difficulties are experienced (Yes or No) due to symptoms can be used as a quick assessment, e.g,

the Social Work Impact of Dizziness short form adapted for motion sickness [49][50].

Even if motion sickness does not create a disability, it should be recognized as a clinical entity that

deserves medical attention. In most cases, education, exposure minimization, short-term use of anti-

motion sickness medications, or simple habituation exercises may be employed to avoid motion sickness

contributing to morbidity and evolving into a disorder.

4. Motion Sickness Clinical Features

4.1 Prevalence: The prevalence of motion sickness in childhood has been estimated at 35-43% prior to

puberty and 25% in young adults; it is a frequent problem in 14% of adults younger than 30 years old

and 7% of adults 61 years old or older [51]-[53]. The prevalence of VIMS ranges widely depending on

the stimulus type and the visual content, with rates varying from 1% [54] to 60% [55] to 80-95%

[56][57].

4.2 Demographics: Susceptibility to motion sickness changes with age. Infants are fairly resistant to

motion sickness until about age two, at which point motion sickness susceptibility rises and peaks

Page 10: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

between the ages of 7-12 years and declines thereafter and throughout adulthood [15][51][58].

Susceptibility gradually declines with age but may increase in a small proportion of individuals [10].

Prevalence estimates should consider that people might self-restrict their behavior and avoid situations

that provoke motion sickness if they are aware of their elevated susceptibility.

In contrast, an increase in susceptibility to VIMS as a factor of age has been well-documented, with

older adults often reporting more VIMS than younger adults [39][59][60]. Sex differences in VIMS have

been observed in some studies [5][60]-[63], but it remains unclear whether women actually experience

more VIMS or are more open about reporting it than men [62]. A variety of other factors, including

technical manipulations such as the size of the field-of-view or individual habits such as video game

experience can affect the occurrence of VIMS [65][66].

Motion sickness prevalence and severity are higher in females than males by as much as a factor of two,

but estimates vary among studies depending on stimulus type, endpoint studied, age, ethnicity, and

hormonal status [23][53][67]-[71]. Specifically, even when the incidence of motion sickness is reported

to be similar between women and men, episodes of vomiting and duration of sickness days are higher in

women [71]. As a perspective, however, the effect of sex is about one-third that of age [72][73].

4.3 Genetics: Heritability for motion sickness in females show a 0.69 concordance in monozygotic twins

and 0.44 concordance in dizygotic twins for childhood motion sickness yielding a heritability estimate

of 70% [51]. A large genome-wide association study involving 80,494 individuals with carsickness

found 35 single nucleotide variants at genome-wide significant levels. The top ten genes involved in

these regions included: PVRL3, GPD2, ACO1, AUTS2, GPR26, UBE2E2, CBLN4, MUTED, LINGO2

and CPNE4 [53]. A genetic risk score using the number of risk alleles found on each individual for these

35 variants could be used to anticípate motion sickness susceptibility. These genes involve a wide

variety of functions such as brain, eye, and ear development and even insulin resistance. Some of these

loci overlap with genes in individuals who experience dizziness, post-operative nausea and vomiting,

altitude sickness, morning sickness, indigestion to dairy, and headache after red wine [53].

Studies that have assessed motion sickness susceptibility by questionnaire, rotation of the body with

head pitching, and exposure to optokinetic drums have reported heightened susceptibility in individuals

of Chinese compared to European descent with the majority of the differences occurring between

women in the two groups [66][74]. These differences persist in American-born children of the Chinese

parents, supporting at least a partial genetic component to motion sickness susceptibility [74].

4.4. Other Modifying Factors: Hormonal factors such as the use of oral contraceptives, menstruation,

pregnancy, and cortisol levels correlate with motion sickness susceptibility in women [72][75][76].

Other modifying factors may include baseline autonomic tone, glucose levels, and physical fitness [77]-

[79].

4.5 Associated Syndromes

4.5.1 Migraine: Symptoms of migraine and motion sickness overlap in many domains, e.g.,

nausea, stomach awareness, and headache (For review see [69] and [80]). Motion sickness is self-

reported in at least 50% of migraine headache sufferers with motion sickness in childhood correlating

with eventual development of migraine headaches in adolescence and adulthood [23][58][69][81]. These

associations suggest that neural pathways for nausea and emesis are particularly sensitive in individuals

Page 11: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

with migraine [23]. Scalp tenderness and nausea during optokinetic stimulation increase more for those

who experience migraine than for those who do not [82]. Motion sickness induced by OVAR is

especially intense in individuals with migraine [26]. This type of motion sickness may be blunted by

rizatriptan, a migraine abortive medication [83][84]. In contrast, VIMS does not appear to be mitigated

by pre-treatment with rizatriptan [85]. Individuals with migraine headaches experience both motion

sickness and VIMS to a higher degree than those without migraine, but the correlation between motion

sickness and VIMS in this group is weak [86].

4.5.2 Structural vestibular disorders: Loss of peripheral vestibular function significantly raises

the threshold for motion sickness and VIMS [26][87]-[89]. Congruently, patients with chronic vestibular

loss (unilateral and bilateral) report less motion sickness on clinical motion sickness ratings than healthy

controls. In comparison, patients with BPPV show a non-significant difference in motion sickness

susceptibility [58]. Patients with vestibular neuritis can experience either an increase or a decrease in

motion sickness susceptibility after the onset of their disorder, a difference attributable to whether the

loss has been compensated [26][41]. Two studies have shown that individuals with Meniere disease are

more susceptible to motion sickness than healthy controls but are not as susceptible as those with

migraine or vestibular migraine [90][91]. There is no correlation between the degree of caloric

asymmetry in vestibular patients and scores on a clinical motion sickness questionnaire. In contrast,

patients with vestibular disorders without vestibular loss report higher degrees of motion sickness than

controls [29][58]. About 10-minutes of exposure has been shown to distinguish between individuals of

different susceptibility to motion sickness in laboratory settings (vestibular neuritis, bilateral

vestibulopathy, vestibular migraine) [26].

5. Motion Sickness Laboratory Examinations

Motion sickness may be induced in laboratory settings in order to probe the components of

susceptibility, but these tests are not used for clinical diagnostic purposes. Laboratory studies may

elucidate why there may be differences in motion sickness sensitivity to different stimuli, however.

Correlations between the MSSQ with laboratory-induced nausea by cross-coupled stimulation has been

reported to be between 0.14 and 0.58 with generally higher correlations being seen for vertical

translational oscillations than horizontal oscillations [23]. Of the translational planes, motion through the

body-referenced X- or Y-axes (fore-aft or side-to-side motion when upright, respectively) are more

provocative than the body-referenced Z-axis, (up-down) but motion direction with respect to the gravity

vector (e.g. vertical versus horizontal) is a less important factor [92].

An area of controversy is whether a smaller phase lead of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), which

indicates enhanced velocity storage, is related pathophysiologically to motion sickness susceptibility as

found in some laboratories ([92]-[96]) but not replicated in others ([73][83]). It has thus been proposed

that the absolute value of the VOR may not be the relevant marker of motion sickness susceptibility but

rather the ability to modify the VOR in response to varying motion sickness-inducing stimuli that is key

to whether motion sickness develops to any given stimuli [10][73]. The relationship between motion

sickness susceptibility and adaptability of vestibular responses has also been supported by lower cervical

vestibular evoked myogenic potential (VEMPS) thresholds correlating with the ability to habituate to

seasickness; this may be attributable to the wider potential range of adaptive responses to motion stimuli

with lower VEMP thresholds [97].

6. Differential Diagnosis

Page 12: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Because motion sickness susceptibility declines with age, an adult patient presenting with increasing

susceptibility to motion should be evaluated for underlying causes of a lowered threshold for nausea.

This evaluation may include an assessment for migraine, endocrine abnormalities, ocular misalignment,

and other central nervous system disorders. Head motion-induced discomfort should be suspected as

being secondary to an uncompensated vestibular asymmetry. These cases should be evaluated with

vestibular laboratory testing.

7. Future Directions

The presentation of these criteria for motion sickness acknowledges that there are still critical

information gaps that need to be filled in order to determine a clear line between motion sickness as a

normal human response to movement and motion sickness as a disorder. The difficulty in determining

this line in no small part lies in the large number of variables that contribute to susceptibility and

morbidity, one of the most important being activity modification.

A challenge in motion sickness research has been the rapid technological advancements in transportation

and entertainment options that have inadvertently created an environment of increasing motion stimuli

in both novelty and number [98][99]. It remains to be determined whether human beings may be able to

push current boundaries of motion tolerance through enhanced habituation exercises, manipulation of

vestibular input, cognitive enhancement techniques, pharmacological therapies, or noninvasive brain

stimulation methods. Prediction of susceptibility based on demographics, hormonal rhythms, genetics,

and physiological status may lead to optimization of environments that reduce stimulus intensity and

morbidity.

Toward those ends, criteria for motion sickness have been proposed here to promote clearer

communication among clinicians and investigators. It is expected that criteria for motion sickness will

evolve as data on epidemiology, natural history, development of biomarkers, and assessments of future

health consequences are methodically acquired.

Acknowledgements:

The working meetings for the International Classification of Vestibular Disorders (ICVD) are financially

supported by the International Bárány Society and by Neuro+ e.V., Berlin, Germany, a non-profit

association for neurological research.

Page 13: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

References

[1] D. Huppert, J. Benson, T. Brandt, A historical view of motion sickness-a plague at sea and on land,

also with military impact, Front Neurol 8 (2017), 114.

[2] E. Darwin, Zoonomia, The Project Gutenberg Ebook ed. London; 1796.

[3] J.A. Irwin, The pathology of sea sickness, The Lancet 118 (1881), 907-9.

[4] J. Reason and J. Brand, Motion sickness, London, New York, San Francisco: Academic Press; 1975.

[5] B. Keshavarz, H. Hecht, B. Lawson, Visually induced motion sickness: Characteristics, causes, and

countermeasures. In: Hale K, Stanney K, editors. Handbook of Virtual Environments: Design,

Implementation, and Applications. Boca Raton, FL.: CRC Press; 2014. p. 648-97.

[6] L. Hettinger and G, Riccio G. Visually induced motion sickness in virtual environments, Presence:

Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 1 (1992), 306-10.

[7] R. Kennedy, G. Allgood, B. Van Hoy, M. Lilienthal, Motion sickness symptoms and postural

changes following flights in motion-based flight trainers, Journal of Low Frequency Noise, Vibration

and Active Control 6 (1987), 147-54.

[8] R.S. Kennedy, M.G. Lilienthal, K.S. Berbaum, D.R. Baltzley, M.E. McCauley, Simulator sickness in

U.S. Navy flight simulators, Aviat Space Environ Med 60 (1989), 10-6.

[9] D.R. Baltzley, R.S. Kennedy, K.S. Berbaum, M.G. Lilienthal, D.W. Gower, The time course of

postflight simulator sickness symptoms, Aviat Space Environ Med 60 (1989), 1043-8.

[10] J.F. Golding, Motion sickness. Handb Clin Neurol 137 (2016), 371-90.

[11] J.T. Reason, Motion sickness adaptation: A neural mismatch model, J R Soc Med 71 (1978), 819-

29.

[12] G. Riccio and T. Stoffregen, An ecological theory of motion sickness and postural instability,

Ecological Psychology 3 (1991), 195-240.

[13] S.M. Ebenholtz, M.M. Cohen, B.J. Linder, The possible role of nystagmus in motion sickness: A

hypothesis, Aviat Space Environ Med 65 (1994), 1032-5.

[14] C.M. Oman, Motion sickness: A synthesis and evaluation of the sensory conflict theory, Can J

Physiol Pharmacol 68 (1990), 294-303.

[15] J.F. Golding, Motion sickness susceptibility, Autonomic Neuroscience: Basic and Clinical 129

(2006), 67-76.

Page 14: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[16] J.E. Bos and W. Bles, Modelling motion sickness and subjective vertical mismatch detailed for

vertical motions, Brain Res Bull 47 (1998), 537-42.

[17] R. Kennedy, K. Stanney, W. Dunlap, Duration and exposure to virtual environments: Sickness

curves during and across sessions, Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 9 (2000), 463-72.

[18] B.J. Yates, A.D. Miller, J.B. Lucot, Physiological basis and pharmacology of motion sickness: An

update, Brain Res Bull 47 (1998), 395-406.

[19] M. Treisman, Motion sickness: An evolutionary hypothesis, Science 197 (1977), 493-5.

[20] G.R. Morrow, The effect of a susceptibility to motion sickness on the side effects of cancer

chemotherapy, Cancer 55 (1985), 2766-70.

[21] C.M. Oman, Are evolutionary hypotheses for motion sickness “just so” stories? J Vestib Res 22

(2012), 117-127.

[22] P.J. Gianaros, E.R. Muth, J.T. Mordkoff, M.E. Levine, R.M. Stern, A questionnaire for the

assessment of the multiple dimensions of motion sickness, Aviat Space Environ Med 72 (2001), 115-9.

[23] J.F. Golding, Motion sickness susceptibility questionnaire revised and its relationship to other

forms of sickness Brain Res Bull 47 (1998), 507-16.

[24] R. Kennedy, N. Lane, K. Berbaum, M. Lilienthal, Simulator sickness questionnaire: An enhanced

method for quantifying simulator sickness, The International Journal of Aviation Psychology 3 (1993),

203-20.

[25] A. Graybiel, C.D. Wood, E.F. Miller, D.B. Cramer, Diagnostic criteria for grading the severity of

acute motion sickness, Aerosp Med 39 (1968), 453-5.

[26] L. Murdin, F. Chamberlain, S. Cheema, Q. Arshad, M.A. Gresty, J.F. Golding, A. Bronstein,

Motion sickness in migraine and vestibular disorders, J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 86 (2015), 585-7.

[27] B.D. Lawson and A.M. Mead, The sopite syndrome revisited: Drowsiness and mood changes

during real or apparent motion, Acta Astronaut 43 (1998), 181-92.

[28] Y.H. Cha, Y.Y. Cui, R.W. Baloh, Comprehensive clinical profile of mal de débarquement

syndrome, Front Neurol 9 (2018), 261.

[29] J.F. Golding, A.C. Paillard, H. Normand, S. Besnard, P. Denise, Prevalence, predictors, and

prevention of motion sickness in zero-g parabolic flights, Aerosp Med Hum Perform 88 (2017), 3-9.

[30] A. Bisdorff, M. Von Brevern, T. Lempert, D.E. Newman-Toker, Classification of vestibular

symptoms: Towards an international classification of vestibular disorders, J Vestib Res 19 (2009), 1-13.

Page 15: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[31] R.S. Kellogg, R.S. Kennedy, A. Graybiel, Motion sickness symptomatology of labyrinthine

defective and normal subjects during zero gravity maneuvers, Aerosp Med 36 (1965), 315-8.

[32] J.F. Golding, H. M. Markey, J. R. Stott, The Effects of Motion Direction, Body Axis,

and Posture on Motion Sickness Induced by Low Frequency Linear Oscillation, Aviation,

Space and Environmental Medicine 66 (1995), 1046–1051.

[33] E.R. Muth, R.M. Stern, J.F. Thayer, K.L. Koch, Assessment of the multiple dimensions of nausea:

The nausea profile (NP), J Psychosom Res 40 (1996), 511-20.

[34] J.E. Bos, S.N. MacKinnon, A. Patterson, Motion sickness symptoms in a ship motion simulator:

Effects of inside, outside, and no view, Aviat Space Environ Med 76 (2005), 1111-8.

[35] B. Keshavarz and H. Hecht, Validating an efficient method to quantify motion sickness, Hum

Factors 53 (2011), 415-26.

[36] J.T. Reason, Relations between motion sickness susceptibility, the spiral after-effect and loudness

estimation, Br J Psychol 59 (1968), 385-93.

[37] R.S. Kennedy, W.P. Dunlap, J.E. Fowlkes JE, Prediction of Motion Sickness Susceptibility,

Chapter 11. in Motion and Space Sickness, ed. George H. Crampton. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL. 1990

[38] J.F. Golding, Motion Sickness. Predicting individual differences in motion sickness susceptibility

by questionnaire, Personality and Individual Differences, 41 (2006) 237–248

[39] S. Lamb and K.C. Kwok, MSSQ-Short norms may underestimate highly susceptible individuals:

Updating the MSSQ-short norms, Hum Factors 57 (2015), 622-33.

[40] B. Keshavarz, R. Ramkhalawansingh, B. Haycock, S. Shahab, J. Campos, Comparing simulator

sickness in younger and older adults during simulated driving under different multisensory conditions,

Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 54 (2018), 47-62.

[41] R.S. Kennedy and J.E. Fowlkes, Simulator sickness is polygenic and polysymptomatic:

Implications for research, International Journal of Aviation Psychology 2 (1992), 23-38.

[42] J.F. Golding and M.A. Gresty, Pathophysiology and treatment of motion sickness, Curr Opin

Neurol 28 (2015), 83-8.

[43] B. Keshavarz and H. Hecht, Pleasant music as a countermeasure against visually induced motion

sickness, Appl Ergon 45 (2014), 521-7.

[44] F.D. Yen Pik Sang, J.F. Golding, M.A. Gresty, Suppression of sickness by controlled breathing

during mildly nauseogenic motion, Aviat Space Environ Med 74 (2003), 998-1002.

[45] A. Brainard and C. Gresham, Prevention and treatment of motion sickness, Am Fam Physician 90

(2014), 41-6.

Page 16: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[46] A. Shupak and C.R. Gordon, Motion sickness: Advances in pathogenesis, prediction, prevention,

and treatment, Aviat Space Environ Med 77 (2006), 1213-23.

[47] O. Samuel and D. Tal, Airsickness: Etiology, treatment, and clinical importance-a review, Mil Med

180 (2015), 1135-9.

[48] D.V. Sheehan, K. Harnett-Sheehan, B.A. Raj, The measurement of disability, Int Clin

Psychopharmacol ;11 Suppl 3 (1996), 89-95.

[49] A.M. Bronstein, J.F. Golding, M.A. Gresty, M. Mandalà, D. Nuti, A. Shetye, Y. Silove. Validation

of the Social life & Work Impact of Dizziness questionnaire (SWID). 7th Meeting of the British Society

of neuro-Otology. Leicester Tigers Rugby Stadium, Friday 16 Oct 2009.

[50] A.M. Bronstein, J.F. Golding, M.A. Gresty, M. Mandalà, D. Nuti, A. Shetye, Y. Silove. The Social

Impact of Dizziness in London and Siena. J Neurol 257 (2010), 183-190.

[51] C.M. Reavley, J.F. Golding, L.F. Cherkas, T.D. Spector, A.J. MacGregor, Genetic influences on

motion sickness susceptibility in adult women: A classical twin study, Aviat Space Environ Med 77

(2006), 1148-52.

[52] I.F. Henriques, D.W. Douglas de Oliveira, F. Oliveira-Ferreira, P.M. Andrade, Motion sickness

prevalence in school children, Eur J Pediatr 173 (2014), 1473-82.

[53] B.S. Hromatka, J.Y. Tung, A.K. Kiefer, C.B. Do, D.A. Hinds, N. Eriksson, Genetic variants

associated with motion sickness point to roles for inner ear development, neurological processes and

glucose homeostasis, Hum Mol Genet 24 (2015), 2700-8.

[54] M. Kluver, C. Herrigel, C. Heinrich, H. Schoner, H. Hecht, The behavioral validity of dual-task

driving performance in fixed and moving base driving simulators, Transportation Research Part F:

Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 37 (2016), 78-96.

[55] K.M. Stanney, R.S. Kennedy, J.M. Drexler, D.L. Harm, Motion sickness and proprioceptive

aftereffects following virtual environment exposure, Appl Ergon 30 (1999), 27-38.

[56] S. Cobb, S. Nichols, A. Ramsey, J. Wilson, Virtual reality-induced symptoms and effects (VRISE).

Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 8 (1999), 169-86.

[57] K. Stanney, G. Salvendy, J. Deisinger, P. DiZio, S. Ellis, J. Ellison J, et al. Aftereffects and sense of

presence in virtual environments: Formulation of a research and development agenda, Int J Hum

Comput Interact 10 (1998), 135-87.

[58] A.C. Paillard, G. Quarck, F. Paolino, P. Denise, M. Paolino, J.F. Golding, V. Ghulyan-Bedikian,

Motion sickness susceptibility in healthy subjects and vestibular patients: Effects of gender, age and

trait-anxiety, J Vestib Res 23 (2013), 203-9.

Page 17: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[59] J.O. Brooks, R.R. Goodenough, M.C. Crisler, N.D. Klein, R.L. Alley, B.L. Koon, et al. Simulator

sickness during driving simulation studies, Accid Anal Prev 42 (2010), 788-96.

[60] N. Kawano K. Iwamoto, K. Ebe, B. Aleksic, A. Noda, H. Umegaki, et al. Slower adaptation to

driving simulator and simulator sickness in older adults, Aging Clin Exp Res 24 (2012), 285-9.

[61] M.B. Flanagan, J.G. May, T.G. Dobie, Sex differences in tolerance to visually-induced motion

sickness, Aviat Space Environ Med 76 (2005), 642-6.

[62] K.M. Stanney, K.S. Hale, I. Nahmens, R.S. Kennedy, What to expect from immersive virtual

environment exposure: Influences of gender, body mass index, and past experience, Hum Factors 45

(2003), 504-20.

[63] S. D'Amour, J.E. Bos, B. Keshavarz, The efficacy of airflow and seat vibration on reducing visually

induced motion sickness, Exp Brain Res 235 (2017), 2811-20.

[64] J. Bos, W. Bles, E. Groen, A theory on visually induced motion sickness, Displays 29 (2008), 47-

57.

[65] H. Ujike, K. Ukai, K. Nihei, Survey on motion sickness-like symptoms provoked by viewing a

video movie during junior high school class, Displays 29 (2008) 81-9.

[66] B. Keshavarz, A.C. Novak, L.J. Hettinger, T.A. Stoffregen, J.L. Campos, Passive restraint reduces

visually induced motion sickness in older adults, J Exp Psychol Appl 23 (2017), 85-99.

[67] S. Klosterhalfen, S. Kellermann, F. Pan, U. Stockhorst, G. Hall, P. Enck, Effects of ethnicity and

gender on motion sickness susceptibility, Aviat Space Environ Med 76 (2005), 1051-7.

[68] P.M. Gahlinger, Cabin location and the likelihood of motion sickness in cruise ship passengers, J

Travel Med 7 (2000), 120-4.

[69] D.A. Marcus, J.M. Furman, C.D. Balaban, Motion sickness in migraine sufferers, Expert Opin

Pharmacother 6 (2005), 2691-7.

[70] A. Lawther and M.J. Griffin, A survey of the occurrence of motion sickness amongst passengers at

sea, Aviat Space Environ Med 59 (1988), 399-406.

[71] M. Turner and M.J. Griffin, Motion sickness incidence during a round-the-world yacht race, Aviat

Space Environ Med 66 (1995), 849-56.

[72] J.F. Golding, P. Kadzere, M.A. Gresty, Motion sickness susceptibility fluctuates through the

menstrual cycle, Aviat Space Environ Med 76 (2005), 970-3.

[73] J.F. Golding and M.A. Gresty, Motion sickness, Curr Opin Neurol 18 (2005), 29-34.

Page 18: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[74] R.M. Stern, S. Hu, S.H. Uijtdehaage, E.R. Muth, L.H. Xu, K.L. Koch, Asian hypersusceptibility to

motion sickness, Hum Hered 46 (1996), 7-14.

[75] R.L. Matchock, M.E. Levine, P.J. Gianaros, R.M. Stern, Susceptibility to nausea and motion

sickness as a function of the menstrual cycle, Womens Health Issues 18 (2008), 328-35.

[76] K. Meissner, P. Enck, E.R. Muth, S. Kellermann, A. Klosterhalfen, Cortisol levels predict motion

sickness tolerance in women but not in men, Physiol Behav 97 (2009), 102-6.

[77] B.S. Cheung, K.E. Money, I. Jacobs, Motion sickness susceptibility and aerobic fitness: A

longitudinal study, Aviat Space Environ Med 61 (1990), 201-4.

[78] N. Rawat, C.W. Connor, J.A. Jones, I.B. Kozlovskaya, P. Sullivan, The correlation between aerobic

fitness and motion sickness susceptibility, Aviat Space Environ Med 73 (2002), 216-8.

[79] F.F. Mo, H.H. Qin, X.L. Wang, Z.L.Shen, Z. Xu, K.H. Wang, et al. Acute hyperglycemia is related

to gastrointestinal symptoms in motion sickness: An experimental study, Physiol Behav 105 (2012),

394-401.

[80] A. Cuomo-Granston and P.D. Drummond, Migraine and motion sickness: What is the link? Prog

Neurobiol, 91 (2010), 300-12.

[81] A. Kayan and J.D. Hood, Neuro-otological manifestations of migraine, Brain 107 (1984), 1123-42.

[82] P.D. Drummond, Motion sickness and migraine: Optokinetic stimulation increases scalp

tenderness, pain sensitivity in the fingers and photophobia, Cephalalgia 22 (2002), 117-24.

[83] J.M. Furman, D.A. Marcus, C.D. Balaban, Rizatriptan reduces vestibular-induced motion sickness

in migraineurs. J Headache Pain 12 (2011), 81-8.

[84] D.A. Marcus and J.M. Furman, Prevention of motion sickness with rizatriptan: A double-blind,

placebo-controlled pilot study, Med Sci Monit 12 (2006), PI1-7.

[85] J.M. Furman, D.A. Marcus, A pilot study of rizatriptan and visually-induced motion sickness in

migraineurs, Int J Med Sci 6 (2009), 212-7.

[86] P.D. Drummond, Triggers of motion sickness in migraine sufferers, Headache 45 (2005), 653-6.

[87] W.H. Johnson, F.A. Sunahara, J.P. Landolt, Importance of the vestibular system in visually induced

nausea and self-vection, J Vestib Res 9 (1999), 83-7.

[88] B.S. Cheung, I.P Howard, K.E. Money, Visually-induced sickness in normal and bilaterally

labyrinthine-defective subjects, Aviat Space Environ Med 62 (1991), 527-31.

Page 19: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

[89] R.S. Kennedy, A. Graybiel, R.C. McDonough, F.D. Beckwith, Symptomatology under storm

conditions in the north atlantic in control subjects and in persons with bilateral labyrinthine defects, Acta

Otolaryngol 66 (1968), 533-40.

[90] J.F. Golding and M. Patel, Meniere's, migraine, and motion sickness, Acta Otolaryngol 137 (2017),

495-502.

[91] J.D. Sharon and T.E. Hullar, Motion sensitivity and caloric responsiveness in vestibular migraine

and meniere's disease, Laryngoscope 124 (2014), 969-73.

[92] J.F. Golding, H.M. Markey, J.R. Stott, The effects of motion direction, body axis, and posture on

motion sickness induced by low frequency linear oscillation, Aviat Space Environ Med 66 (1995), 1046-

51.

[93] S.H. Jeong, S.Y. Oh, H.J. Kim, J.W. Koh, J.S. Kim, Vestibular dysfunction in migraine: Effects of

associated vertigo and motion sickness, J Neurol 257 (2010), 905-12.

[94] M. Dai, T. Raphan, B. Cohen, Prolonged reduction of motion sickness sensitivity by visual-

vestibular interaction, Exp Brain Res 210 (2011), 503-13.

[95] M.E. Hoffer, K. Gottshall, R.D. Kopke, P. Weisskopf, R. Moore, K.A. Allen, D. Wester, Vestibular

testing abnormalities in individuals with motion sickness, Otol Neurotol 24 (2003), 633-6.

[96] G. Clément, M.F. Reschke, Relationship between motion sickness susceptibility and vestibulo-

ocular reflex gain and phase, J Vestib Res 28 (2018), 295-304.

[97] D. Tal, D. Hershkovitz, G. Kaminski-Graif, G. Wiener, O Samuel, A. Shupak, Vestibular evoked

myogenic potentials and habituation to seasickness, Clin Neurophysiol 124 (2013), 2445-9.

[98] A. Sharp A, The rise of digital motion sickness: Video games, 3D films and ios7 set to make

condition the 21st century's biggest occupational disease. Daily Mail 30 September 2013.

[99] T.A. Stoffregen, Y.C. Chen, F.C. Koslucher, Motion control, motion sickness, and the postural

dynamics of mobile devices, Exp Brain Res 232 (2014), 1389-97.

Page 20: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Table 1: Motion Sickness Severity Scales

Knownas

Shorthand MSQ PDI SSQ SR NP MSAQ MISC FMS

Year 1965 1968 1993 1995* 1996 2001 2005 2011

Scoretype Compositescore Compositew/5parts Compositew/3parts Singleitem Compositew/3parts Compositew/4parts Singleitem Singleitem

Motiontypequeried Self Self Visual Self Visual Self Self visualValidation Parabolicflight Slowrotationroom,

lowfrequencyvibration,simulator

Flightsimulator Various(sea,zero-G,parabolicflight,etc.)

OKNdrum OKNdrum SSQ,visualstimulus

Measures Severity3-point Severity Severity Severity Severity Severity Severity Severity(realtime)

Susceptibility5-point

Components I=slightorminimal SlightMalaise Oculomotor(O) Singleitem,scored1-6 Gastrointestinal

distress(G)

Gastrointestinal(G) Singleitem,scored0-10 Singleitem,scored0-20

ModerateMalaiseA Disorientation(D) 1=nosymptoms Somaticdistress(S) Central(C) 0=noproblems 0=nosicknessModerateMalaiseB Nausea(N) 2=initialmotionsickness

symptoms,nonausea

Emotionaldistress(E) Peripheral(P) 1=Uneasiness

2=Vague*

20=franksickness

SevereMalaise 3=mildnausea 3=Slight*

FrankSickness 4=moderatenausea 4=Fairly*5=severenauseaand/or

retching5=Severe*

6=SlightNausea

6=vomiting 7=Fair(ly)nauseaSopite(S) 8=Severenausea

Scalefocusedonnauseabut

opentootherunspecifiedsymptomsofmotion

sickness.

9=Retching

10=Vomiting*Dizziness,warmth,

headache,stomachawareness,sweating,

othersymptoms

Scalefocusingonnauseaand

generaldiscomfortaspectofmotionsickness,notother

symptoms(e.g.,fatigue,dizziness,eyestrain)

Symptomsqueried

Nausearelated Nausea Nausea Nausea(N,D) Nausea Nausea(G) Nausea NauseaRetching Retching Retching Queasy(G) Queasy(G)

Vomiting Vomiting Vomiting Vomiting(G) Mayvomit(G) Vomiting

Sick(G) Sicktostomach(G)Gastrointestinalrelated Stomachawareness Epigastricawareness Stomachawareness

(N)

Stomach

awareness/discomfort(G)

Stomachawareness Stomachawareness

Epigastricdiscomfort Burping(N) Ill(G) Burping

Autonomicrelated Sweat (Cold)sweating Sweating(N) Sweaty(S) Sweaty(P) SweatingFlushing Warmth(S) Clammy/Coldsweat

(P)

Cold/warm

Hot/warm(P)

Pallor Pallor

Salivation Salivation Increasedsalivation(N)

Salivation

DrymouthDesirebowel

movement

Sopiterelated Drowsiness Drowsiness Drowsy(S)Fatigue(O) Tired/Fatigued(S) Tired/fatigued(S) Tiredness

Yawn YawningFrequentsighing

Centralrelated Headache Headache Headache(O) Headache

Dizziness Dizziness Dizziness(D) Dizzy(C)Vertigo(D) Spinning(C)

Blurredvision Blurredvision(O,D) BlurredvisionEystrain(O)

Difficultyfocusing(O)

Difficultyconcentrating(N,O)

Fullnessofhead(D)Other Generaldiscomfort Generaldiscomfort

(N,O)

Generaldiscomfort

Awarenessofbreathing

Weak(S) Faintlike(C)

Anorexia Lightheaded(S) Lightheaded(C)Upset(E) Annoyed/irritated(S)

Worried(E) Uneasy(S)Hopeless(E)Panicked(E)Nervous(E)

Scared/afraid(E)Shaky(S)

*NOTE:This6-pointscalewasusedasaconventionintheRoyalAirForceforsometimepriortoappearnceinpublications

SicknessRating[10][32]

Otherlevelsnotindicated

PensacolaMotionSickness

Questionnaire[31]

PensacolaDiagnosticIndex[25]

SimulatorSicknessQuestionnaire[24]

NauseaProfile[33] MotionSicknessAssessment

Questionnaire[22]

MiseryScale[34] FastMotionSicknessScale[35]

Page 21: Motion Sickness: Diagnostic Criteria Consensus document of ......motion sickness (VIMS) are the same as for motion sickness, but the triggering stimulus is visual motion and eye strain

Table 2: Motion Sickness Susceptibility Scales

Knownas

Shorthand MSQ MHQ MSSQ-Long(54-items)

MSSQ-Short(18-items)

Year 1968/1975 1990 1998 2006Scoretype Compositew/2parts Composite Compositew/2parts Compositew/2partsMotiontypequeried Self Self Self SelfComponents Child(<12yrs) PartAChild(<12yrs) PartAChild(<12yrs)

Adult(since12yrsorlast10yrs

PartBAdult(last10yrs)

PartBAdult(last10yrs)

Queriesamountofexposure,episodesof

feelingsickandnauseated,and

episodesofvomitingineachof9triggers

Queriesepisodesoffeelingsickor

nauseatedduringeachof9triggers

Queriesamountofexposure,episodesof

feelingsickandnauseated,and

episodesofvomitingineachof9triggers

MotionSicknessSusceptibility

Questionnaire[23]

MotionSicknessSusceptibility

Questionnaire[38]

MotionSicknessQuestionnaire[4][36]

PensacolaMotionHistory

Questionnaire[37]