Top Banner
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/307639438 MODIFIED REY-OSTERRIETH COMPLEX FIGURE COPYING TASK DIFFERENTIATES FRONTAL AND POSTERIOR LESIONS OF BRAIN Working Paper · October 2007 DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.2.12885.22242 CITATIONS 0 READS 1,211 1 author: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Modified R-OCFT copy differentiates frontal and posterior lesions of brain View project Paweł Przybylski SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities 2 PUBLICATIONS 1 CITATION SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Paweł Przybylski on 06 September 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
15

MODIFIED REY-OSTERRIETH COMPLEX FIGURE COPYING TASK … · Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure copying is a method of assessment of visuoconstructional skills and is sensitive to pathology

Jan 25, 2021

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/307639438

    MODIFIED REY-OSTERRIETH COMPLEX FIGURE COPYING TASK

    DIFFERENTIATES FRONTAL AND POSTERIOR LESIONS OF BRAIN

    Working Paper · October 2007

    DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.2.12885.22242

    CITATIONS

    0READS

    1,211

    1 author:

    Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

    Modified R-OCFT copy differentiates frontal and posterior lesions of brain View project

    Paweł Przybylski

    SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities

    2 PUBLICATIONS   1 CITATION   

    SEE PROFILE

    All content following this page was uploaded by Paweł Przybylski on 06 September 2016.

    The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/307639438_MODIFIED_REY-OSTERRIETH_COMPLEX_FIGURE_COPYING_TASK_DIFFERENTIATES_FRONTAL_AND_POSTERIOR_LESIONS_OF_BRAIN?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/publication/307639438_MODIFIED_REY-OSTERRIETH_COMPLEX_FIGURE_COPYING_TASK_DIFFERENTIATES_FRONTAL_AND_POSTERIOR_LESIONS_OF_BRAIN?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/project/Modified-R-OCFT-copy-differentiates-frontal-and-posterior-lesions-of-brain?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Pawel-Przybylski?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Pawel-Przybylski?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/institution/SWPS-University-of-Social-Sciences-and-Humanities?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Pawel-Przybylski?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdfhttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Pawel-Przybylski?enrichId=rgreq-81af5fbe53b199d84d092549fb5593e2-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNzYzOTQzODtBUzo0MDMyMzgyNDg1NjY3ODVAMTQ3MzE1MDg5ODI5MA%3D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

  • MODIFIED REY-OSTERRIETH COMPLEX

    FIGURE COPYING TASK DIFFERENTIATES

    FRONTAL AND POSTERIOR LESIONS OF BRAIN

    ABSTRACT:

    Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure copying is a method of assessment of visuoconstructional

    skills and is sensitive to pathology of posterior cortices. However, poor copy score might also

    reflect executive dysfunctions following frontal lobe lesion. Thus far there has been no meth-

    od allowing accurate differentiation between these two difficulties. In current study a proce-

    dure is introduced based on providing an organizational aid and observation of changes in

    copy. Two groups were examined: 1) people with posterior lesions of brain; 2) people with

    prefrontal lesions. Results showed that the method differentiate accurately between these

    groups, and as a result between the mechanisms of discussed disorders.

    KEYWORDS: visual perception; visuospatial; visuoconstructional; planning impairment; ex-

    ecutive functions; neuropsychological diagnosis

    INTRODUCTION

    Copying of the the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (R-OCF) is a widely known method of

    neuropsychological assessment of visual perception and organization as well as visuospatial

    and visuoconstructional skills (Spreen & Strauss, 1998; Fisher & Loring, 2004; Strauss et al.,

    2006). As the standardized tool of examining the latest it has remarkable, empirically proven,

    diagnostic accuracy. Klitzke (1997) examined a group of posteriorly lesioned subjects suffer-

    ing from visuospatial dysfunctions with R-OCF and the subtest “Block Designs” from WAIS

    battery. In the first task all subjects scored below fifth cumulative percent according to norms

    for healthy people while in the second task such a low result was achieved only by 5 out of 20

    people.

    On the other hand, Luria and Tswietkowa (after: Pillon, 1981) noticed that there are

    two kinds of problems appearing in complex visuoconstructional tasks. They called them two

    kinds of visuoconstructional difficulties, although this term does not seem to be adequate with

    reference to both of the deficits in question. The first kind encompasses troubles resulting

    from inability to perceive correctly the stimulus set or to utilize created percept for the pur-

    pose of constructional behavior. These arise due to the disintegration of visual and

    visuospatial processing and result from the damage to the posterior parts of brain, mainly oc-

  • cipital lobe and parieto-occipital junction (Luria, 1973; Lindsay & Norman, 1977; Walsh,

    1991; Girkin & Miller, 2000).

    Second kind of problems arises due to inability to develop and implement an appropriate plan

    of action. Inertness and impulsiveness, lack of capacity or motivation to predict the outcomes

    of successively taken steps or inability to take advantage of feedback are the basis of troubles

    of this kind (Lhermitte et al., 1972; Pillon, 1981: Shorr et al., 1992; Walsh, 1994; 1991). Such

    dysfunctions are usually an effect of prefrontal lesion and they can be seen not only in con-

    structional tasks. They can manifest in virtually any kind of cognitive activity (Owen, 1997;

    Della Sala et al., 1998; Miyake et al., 2000; Goldberg, 2001; Stuss & Knight, 2002; Hommel,

    2003; Wood & Grafman, 2003).

    Differentiating the reasons of patient’s difficulties in the test is necessary in the pro-

    cess of neuropsychological diagnosis from the prognostic point of view or for the sake of con-

    structing adequate compensation.

    There are two ways of examining reasons for diminished quality of the copy of R-

    OCF described in the literature. The first way is based on description of planned approach to

    copying process regardless of copy quality. A classic example is the categorization of copying

    strategies proposed by Osterrieth (Fisher & Loring, 2004). Other methods of this kind include

    subjective assessment of observed copying strategy adequacy (Hamby et al.; Anderson et al.;

    Waber & Holmes; Boston Qualitative Scoring System after: Fisher & Loring, 2004), or ob-

    jective measurement of systematic approach to copying based on continuity and order of pro-

    duction of specific figure’s elements (Binder after: Fisher & Loring, 2004; Bennett-Levy,

    1984; Shorr et al., 1992).

    Unfortunately, methods of this kind are not suitable for differentiation between

    mechanisms of difficulties in copying. Planned approach to copying are reflected in the copy

    quality and perceptual difficulties always lead to chaotic copying. In a study by Przybylski,

    (2006) a group of people with perceptual or visuoconstructional deficits after a damage to

    posterior regions of brain achieved mean perceptual clustering ratio – an index created by

    Shorr et al., (1992) – as low as the frontally lesioned group did, and this measure is hard to

    interpret in terms of quality of the plan. Frequencies of copying strategies also did not differ-

    entiate significantly between compared groups (Przybylski, 2006).

    Second group of procedures is based on providing an external structure of the copy-

    ing process. Examiner imposes a framework that diminishes or eliminates a chaos in copying.

    The extent to which such help is efficient can be a subject of qualitative or quantitative as-

    sessment. Two such methods have been described in literature and in both cases organization-

  • al help consisted in splitting a process of copying into some stages. For the first time

    Lhermitte et al. (1972) used it on patient with extensive frontal damage whose planning im-

    pairments caused substantial distortion of the copy. There were six stages of copying and their

    employment resulted in equally substantial improvement in the quality of the copy. However,

    this study has a casuistic character therefore the range of generalization is limited. Besides,

    this result was not compared to a result of patient with posterior lesion, so it is impossible to

    tell whether this kind of help would not have been efficient as well.

    Pillon (1981) used similar idea and employed similar organizational help on a group

    of patients with frontal lesions and a group of patients with lesions of posterior regions (oc-

    cipital lobes and their junctions). The procedure he applied was efficient, but unfortunately in

    both groups. This means that it was nonspecific, so it cannot be used in differentiating the

    reasons of diminished copy quality. Thereby the problem of differentiating between mecha-

    nisms of difficulties in copying remained unresolved.

    Basing on the results of studies by Lhermitte et al. (1972) and Pillon (1981) an as-

    sumption was made that copying of R-OCF may be employed for differentiating between

    frontal and posterior pathology, but it requires introducing such a modification of the proce-

    dure that would minimize the engagement of executive functions and, at the same time, would

    not facilitate the task perceptually. In other words the help has to be specific towards plan-

    ning impairment. This hypothesis has been verified with the help of the procedure described

    below in details.

    Analyzing the method used by K.W. Walsh (1991) in the “Block Designs” task from

    WAIS battery, conclusion was formulated that organizational help in R-OCF copying has to

    be based on inserting additional visual material without splitting copying into stages. We con-

    sider that such splitting of copying process would simplify the stimulus set at initial stages of

    performance, and consequently makes it less demanding to visual perception. This, in turn,

    causes undesirable improvement within a group of patients with posterior lesions.

    In this study a method has been introduced consisting in drawing lacking elements

    of the figure onto already provided frame and observing to what degree the copy improved

    after introducing such an aid. Frame provides defined and outlined edges and geometrical

    middle point of the figure. Thus participants are given spatial points of reference in copying.

    Defining these points is thought to be the greatest organizational problem in the process of

    copying. At the same time, such an aid not only does not cause a simplification of stimulus set

    at any stage, but actually can make the task more demanding to visual perception. This should

    guarantee specificity of the aid towards planning impairment.

  • It was expected that standard copy score would not differentiate sufficiently between

    these clinical groups. Introducing the aid should cause significant increase in copy quality

    within the frontal group, but not within the posterior group and finally, level of copy im-

    provement should accurately differentiate between the groups.

    METHODS

    SUBJECTS

    Adult patients took part in current research. Selection of subjects for the research was carried

    out according to the localization of the lesion of central nervous system. Patients with lesions

    limited to either frontal lobe (frontal group – FG) or occipital lobe, parieto-occipital junction

    and temporo-occipital junction (posterior group – PG) were included in the sample. Subjects

    were recruited from among patients of neurological, neurosurgical and rehabilitation wards of

    public hospitals from Lublin, Sosnowiec and Katowice, Poland.

    Aetiology of lesions was varying and encompassed: cerebral ischaemiae (6 sub-

    jects), cerebral haemorrhages (8 subjects), intracranial tumors (7 subjects), neurosurgical re-

    sections of tumors (7 subjects), closed head injuries (6 subjects), frontotemporal dementia (2

    subjects), 1 subject with brain abscess and 1 subject with lesion of mixed aetiology. 38 sub-

    jects were examined, among them 21 with frontal lesions and 17 with posterior brain patholo-

    gy. Distribution of lateralization of damage is presented in table 1. As one can see, there is

    more subjects with bilateral damage in the frontal group. Significance of the difference as

    measured with Pearson χ2 test is p = 0,153. It will be controlled in further analyzes.

    Right-sided Left-sided Bilateral

    FG 7 (33,3%) 4 (19%) 10 (47,7%)

    PG 9 (52,9%) 5 (29,45%) 3 (17,65%) Table 1 Distribution of lesion lateralization.

    Percent of males is higher in the frontal group (66,7%) than in the posterior group

    (47,1%), but the difference as measured with Pearson χ2 test is statistically insignificant (p =

    0,224). Means and standard deviations of age and years of formal education in both groups

    are presented in table 2.

    Age M(SD) Years of education M(SD)

    FG 48,24(15,937) 12,43(3,25)

    PG 51,88(17,776) 10,71(3,6)

    Sign. level p = 0,515 p = 0,135 Table 2 Means and standard deviations of side variables in each of compared group with the statistical

    significance of differences as measured with t test.

  • Because compared groups differed significantly in education, this variable will be

    included in statistical analyzes.

    PROCEDURES

    Subjects were asked to make a copy of the figure with a pencil on an A5 sheet of paper as

    carefully as possible. Subjects were allowed to rotate their drawings, which was aimed at

    maximizing the quality of their copies, but they were not allowed to rotate the figure. Ferraro

    et al. (2002) showed that the position of the figure and the paper had no influence on the qual-

    ity of the produced copies. Erasing was allowed – pencils were fit with an eraser. So far only

    Meyers & Meyers after: Spreen & Strauss (1998) have expressed their opinion about this is-

    sue and they allowed it. We assume that this helps to differentiate better between groups.

    Next, subjects produced another (assisted) copy, this time with an external aid. They

    were asked to draw lacking details of the figure into the frame consisting of: a large main rec-

    tangle (element 2 according to Osterrieth's numeration), the diagonals of the large main rec-

    tangle (element 3) and a large triangle on the right (element 13) as carefully as possible – to

    produce possibly accurate copy. Other conditions remained unchanged. Stimulus set – R-OCF

    as well as the help – the frame is presented in the figure 1.

    Rysunek Fig. 1. Stimulus set – Rey-Osterrieth Coplex Figure (left) and the aid – frame (right)

    Both copies were scored according to L. Taylor’s criteria (Spreen & Strauss, 1998).

    The use of these strict criteria is crucial for the described method because using more lenient

    ones could have resulted in overlooking some of the subtle errors. The score in the first copy

    was converted into proportion of maximum score (36 points) according to the formula: %C =

  • C / 36; where: C – raw standard copy score; %C – percent of maximum score in standard

    copy. An analogical method was employed in computing a proportion of maximum score in

    the assisted copy. Maximum score this time is 30 (3 elements worth 2 points each were al-

    ready provided as a frame), so the formula looks as follows: %CH = CH / 30; where: CH –

    raw score in copy with help; %CH – percent of maximum score in copy with help. Next, the

    first proportion has to be subtracted from the second, which gives Copy Change Ratio (CCR):

    %CH – % C = CCR. This index theoretically ranges from -1 (negative values mean that the

    aid actually increased the distortion of the copy) through 0 (the help had no influence on copy

    quality) to 1 (positive values mean that the help improved copy quality), but practically ex-

    treme values (especially negative) are highly unlikely.

    RESULTS

    RESULTS OF COPYING

    Table 3 shows means and standard deviations of “Raw Copy Score” in frontal and posterior

    group, together with the level of statistical significance of differences between groups as well

    as between-subjects effects in ANOVA analysis. The analysis was conducted with lesion site,

    lesion hemisphere and their interaction as factors and years of education as covariate. As one

    can see intergroup differences did not reach the level of significance and after controlling for

    influence of education this effect becomes even weaker. Simple and interactive influence of

    lesion hemisphere on copy quality was insignificant.

    FG

    M

    (SD)

    PG

    M

    (SD)

    Significance

    of differences in

    means: t test

    Significance of Between-Subject Effects: ANOVA

    A(ANOVA) Lesion

    Site

    Lesion

    Hemisphere

    Interaction:

    S*H

    Years of

    education

    Raw

    copy

    26,238

    (5,137)

    21,588

    (9,707)

    p = 0,088 p = 0,121 p = 0,508 p = 0,637 p = 0,088

    Table 3. Means and standard deviations of standard copy raw score with statistical

    analysis of the results.

    COPY CHANGE WITH EXTERNAL AID

    Mean and standard deviations of CCR in each group together with the results of statistical an-

    alyzes are presented in table 4. For this variable an analysis of variance was conducted with

    the same factorial model as previously. As one can see in table 4 intergroup differences in

  • CCR are much more pronounced than for raw copy score. Moreover, controlling for the side

    effects of education did not decrease significantly the strength of the relationship.

    In figure 2 and in table 5 means and standard deviations of percent of maximum

    score in standard and helped copy in each group are presented. Table 5 presents also the re-

    sults of the analyzes concerning significance of change in means and variances in copying

    after applying the help within each of the lesion site groups. To estimate significance of dif-

    ferences in variances the formula for dependent groups based on t-distribution was used (Fer-

    guson & Takane, 1989, formula 12.6).

    FG

    M

    (SD)

    PG

    M

    (SD)

    Significance

    of differences in

    means: t test

    Significance of Between-Subject Effects (ANOVA)

    Lesion

    Site

    Lesion

    Hemisphere

    Interaction

    S*H

    Years of

    education

    CCR 0,164

    (0,102)

    0,016

    (0,113)

    p = 0,00015** p = 0,001** p = 0,867 p = 0,997 p = 0,946

    Table 4. Copy change ratios in both groups together with results of statistical analyses.

    ** - sinificant at a level p = 0,01

    Fig. 2. Results in copying of R-OCF in each group before and after providing the aid. The graph shows

    means (bars) and standard deviations (whiskers) of scores expressed in the percent of maximum score.

  • %C

    M(SD)

    %CH

    M(SD)

    Significance of

    differences in means:

    t test for dependent groups

    Significance of

    differences in variances:

    t test for dependent groups

    FG 0,7288(0,1427) 0,892(0,0844) p < 0,001** p = 0,0015**

    PG 0,599(0,269) 0,615(0,271) p = 0,567 p = 0,4696

    Table 5. Means and standard deviations of results in standard copy and assisted copy

    expressed as a proportion of maximum score, together with levels of significance of

    changes. ** - significant at p = 0,01.

    Data presented in table 5 show two important results: First, the mean result in copy-

    ing rose significantly after applying the aid for frontal lesions, but did not rise for posterior

    lesions. Second, in FG applying the help caused significant decrease in variance of results –

    so to speak, leveling it – which did not happen within PG. It is worth noticing that in terms of

    either means or variances the results within FG nearly equaled the results of the group of neu-

    rologically healthy controls (Przybylski, 2006).

    In the frontal group 14 subjects achieved CCR ≥ 0,1277 (the same proportion was

    with cut-point at 0,10). In the posterior group CCR ≥ 0,1277 was achieved only by 2 subjects,

    3 subjects had CCR ≥ 0,11 and 4 of them had CCR ≥ 0,10.

    However, in patients from posterior group with highest CCR's some specific mecha-

    nism occurred. All 3 subjects from PG with highest CCRs suffered from either unilateral

    hemianopsia or hemispherical neglect, which resulted in dramatic decrease in copy score.

    Subsequent providing an organizational aid had significant positive influence on copy quality

    although mechanisms were clearly distinct (it helped them focus on previously neglected parts

    of the figure) and easily distinguishable from that observed in subjects with frontal damage.

    Moreover, in the frontal group among 7 subjects with the CCR < 0,1277 as many as

    5 of them achieved raw copy score ≥ 30. Such copy is considered correct, and in such case

    there is little room for improvement an no need for differentiation.

    Considering these two facts we can cautiously assume that high CCR can be regard-

    ed as an indicator of distortion resulting from impaired planning / dysexecutive symptoms,

    although improvement of copy quality should always be assessed individually.

    DISCUSSION

    An analysis of results of the current research gave a strong support for hypotheses stated.

    Standard copy score poorly differentiated between the lesion sites. Although generally pa-

  • tients with frontal damage achieved slightly higher scores than those with posterior pathology,

    the difference did not reach the level of statistical significance. In other words data has con-

    firmed the hypothesis that low score in copy of the R-OCF can be the result of disorders of

    perceptual functions specific for this task, which are common consequence of posterior le-

    sions, as well as the result of disorders of non-specific executive functions due to damage to

    frontal lobes. It was also demonstrated that copy score alone is insufficient to distinguish be-

    tween these mechanisms. It is worth noticing that both groups have significantly lower stand-

    ard copy scores than neurologically healthy controls (Przybylski, 2006).

    Much more useful differentiating measure was the level of disintegration or serious

    structural distortions of the copy. In the current study, patients with frontal damage rarely

    produced copies so distorted that some structural elements were unrecognizable. Mechanisms

    to a greatest extent responsible for lower copy scores within the frontal group were major

    sloppiness, askewness and lack of symmetry – mainly due to badly defined midpoint. In the

    posterior group poor quality of the copy was related to its disintegration and incompleteness.

    Errors occurring in such cases were glaring.

    Figures 3 and 4 show copies drawn by two female patients with frontal lobe pathol-

    ogy. The first patient – E.G. was 56 years old and sustained right frontal ischemia as a result

    of ruptured aneurysm 1,5 year before the examination. The second – A.K. was 28 years old at

    the moment of examination and sustained bilateral anterior and orbital frontal damage due to

    TBI. In case of A.K. it is clearly visible that serious decrease in copy quality may take place

    even though the copy is complete.

    Fig. 3. Copies of R-OCF made by patient E.G. Standard copy (left) was scored 15 points while assisted

    copy (right) 25 points. Copy Change Ratio = 0,41666.

  • Fig. 4. Copies of R-OCF made by patient A.K. Standard copy (left) was scored 19,5 points while assisted

    copy (right) 25 points. Copy Change Ratio = 0,29166.

    Figures 5 and 6 present copies drawn by two male patients (S.Z. and M.B.) with oc-

    cipital damage. They were 69 and 62 years old. In both cases there had taken place surgical

    resection of tumors in occipital lobes, however, in case of S.Z. it was right-sided and in case

    of M.B. it was bilateral. In both copies one can see serious distortions of form, missing of

    some details, and in case of M.B. severe disintegration of the drawing.

    Fig. 5. Copies of R-OCF made by patient S.Z. Standard copy (left) was scored 12,5 points while c assisted

    copy (right) 11 points. Copy Change Ratio = 0,01944.

  • Fig. 6. Copies of R-OCF made by patient M.B. Standard copy (left) was scored 9 points, while assisted

    copy (right) 6,5 points. Copy Change Ratio = -0,03333.

    Most promising measure enabling to differentiate between dysfunctions that cause

    poor copy quality is CCR. In cases of distorted standard copies (scored

  • As one can see, providing an aid allowed to compensate for at least some of the difficulties.

    PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS

    The procedure described in this paper may find application in neuropsychological diagnosis

    as an improvement of methods of assessment of visual perception and visuoconstructional

    functions as well as features of disorganization of behaviour as an element of dysexecutive

    syndrome. The results of this study indicate that employment of copy with help as a supple-

    ment of the R-OCF test may be very advantageous in clinical practice due to the significant

    increase of informative value carried by the small modification of the current procedure.

    REFERENCES

    Ardila, A., Rosselli, M. (2002). Acalculia and Dyscalculia. Neuropsychology Review, 12(4),

    179–231.

    Bennett-Levy, J. (1984). Determinants of performance on the the Rey-Osterrieth Complex

    Figure Test: An analysis, and a new technique for single-case assessment. British Journal of

    Clinical Psychology, 23(2), 109–119.

    Coombs, C. H., Dawes, R. M., Tversky, A. (1970). Mathematical psychology: An elementary

    introduction. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Spinnler, H., Trivelli, C. (1998). Frontal Lobe Functioning in Man:

    The Riddle Revisited. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(8), 663–682.

    Diamond, A. (2000). Close Interrelations of Motor Development and Cognitive Development

    and of the Cerebellum and Prefrontal Cortex. Child Development, 71(1), 44–56.

    Diamond, A. (2002). Normal Development of Prefrontal Cortex from Birth to Young Adult-

    hood: Cognitive Functions, Anatomy, and Biochemistry. In: D. T. Stuss, R. T. Knight (edit.)

    Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, chap. 29, 466–503. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Ferguson, G. A., Takane, Y. (1989). Statistical Analysis in Psychology and Education. New

    York: McGraw-Hill.

    Ferraro, F. R., Grossman, J., Bren, A., Hoverson, A. (2002). Effects of orientation on Rey

    complex figure performance. Brain and Cognition, 50, 139–144.

  • Fisher, J. S., Loring, D. W. (2004). Construction. In: M. D. Lezak, D. B. Howieson, D. W.

    Loring (edit.) Neuropsychological Assessment, chap. 14. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Girkin, C. A., Miller, N. R. (2001). Central Disorders of Vision in Humans. Survey of

    Ophtalmology, 45(5), 379–405.

    Goldberg, E. (2001). The Executive Brain: Frontal Lobes and a Civilized Mind. New York:

    Oxford University Press.

    Grossi, D., Fragassi, N. A., Chiacchio, L., Valoroso, L., Tuccillo, R., Perrotta, C., Rapone, P.,

    Conchiglia, G., Trojano, L. (2002). Do visuospatial and constructional disturbances differen-

    tiate frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease? An experimental

    study of a clinical belief. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 641–648.

    Harciarek, M., Jodzio, K. (2005). Neuropsychological differences between frontotemporal

    dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Neuropsychological Review, 15(3), 131–45.

    Hommel, B. (2003). Planning and Representing Intentional Action. The Scientific World

    Journal, (3), 593–608.

    Klitzke, M. J. (1997). The Differential Sensitivity of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test.

    Report presented on 16th

    annual meeting of International Neuropsychological Society.

    Lhermitte, F., Derouesne, J., Signoret, J.-L. (1972). Analyse neuropsychologique du syn-

    drome frontal. Revue Neurologique, 127(4), 415–440.

    Lindsay, P. H., Norman, D. A. (1977). Human information processing. NY: Academic Press.

    Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P., Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J.,

    Keshavan, M. S., Genovese, C. R., Eddy, W. F., Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Maturation of Widely

    Distributed Brain Function Subserves Cognitive Development. NeuroImage, 13(5), 786–793.

    Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: an introduction to neuropsychology. NY: Basic.

    Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., Wager, T. D.

    (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex

    „Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Owen, A. M. (1997). Cognitive Planning in Humans: Neuropsychological, Neuroanatomical

    and Neuropharmacological Perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology, 53, 431–450.

  • Parrila, R. K., Das, J. P., Dash, U. N. (1996). Development of Planning and Its Relation to

    Other Cognitive Processes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 597–624.

    Pillon, B. (1981). Troubles visuo-constructifs et metodes de compensation: R´esultants de 85

    patients atteints de l´esions c´er´ebrales. Neuropsychologia, 19(3), 375–383.

    Przybylski, P. (2006). Zastosowanie testu Figury Złożonej Reya do różnicowania uszkodzeń

    płatów czołowych i tylnych obszarów mózgu. MA Thesis, University of Silesia, Katowice.

    Shalev, R. S., Gross-Tsur, V. (2001). Developmental Dyscalculia. Pediatric neurology, 24,

    337–342.

    Shorr, J. S., Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J. (1992). Memory for the the Rey-Osterrieth Figure:

    Perceptual Clustering, Encoding, Storage. Neuropsychology, 6(1), 43–50.

    Siri, S., Benaglio, I., Frigerio, A., Binetti, G., Cappa, S. F. (2001). A brief neuropsychological

    assessment for the differential diagnosis between frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s

    disease. European Journal of Neurology, 8, 125–132.

    Spreen, O., Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration,

    Norms and Commenetary. New York: Oxford University Press. 2nd

    edition

    Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests.

    Administration, Norms and Commenetary. New York: Oxford University Press. 3rd

    edition

    Stuss, D. T., Knight, R. T. (ed.) (2002). Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. New York: Ox-

    ford University Press.

    Tunstall, J. R. (1999). Improving the Utility of the Tower of London, a Neuropsychological

    Test of Planning. MS thesis, Griffith University, Brisbane.

    Walsh, K. W. (1991). Understanding brain damage. A primer of neuropsychological evalua-

    tion. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.

    Walsh, K. W. (1994). Neuropsychology. A Clinical Approach. Edinburgh: Churchill Living-

    stone.

    Wood, J. N., Grafman, J. (2003). Human Prefrontal Cortex: Processing and Representational

    Perspectives. Neuroscience: Nature Reviews, 4, 139–147.

    View publication statsView publication stats

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/307639438