Top Banner
DOI 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0080 Rev. Neurosci. 2012; 23(5-6): 659–672 Simona Cabib*, Paolo Campus and Valentina Colelli Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience Abstract: Stress is the main non-genetic source of psy- chopathology. Therefore, the identification of neurobio- logical bases of resilience, the resistance to pathological outcomes of stress, is a most relevant topic of research. It is an accepted view that resilient individuals are those who do not develop helplessness, or other depression-like phenotypes, following a history of stress. In the present review, we discuss the phenotypic differences between mice of the inbred C57BL/6J and DBA/2J strains that could be associated with the strain-specific resistance to help- lessness observable in DBA/2J mice. The reviewed results support the hypothesis that resilience to stress-promoted helplessness develops through interactions between a specific genetic makeup and a history of stress, and is associated with an active coping style, a bias toward the use of stimulus-response learning, and specific adap- tive changes of mesoaccumbens dopamine transmission under stress. Finally, evidence that compulsivity rep- resents a side effect of the neuroadaptive processes fos- tering resistance to develop depressive-like phenotypes under stress is discussed. Keywords: adrenal hormones; D2 dopamine recep- tors; endophenotypes; fitness; habit-like responses; hippocampus. *Corresponding author: Simona Cabib, Department of Psychology and CRIN, Sapienza University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy, e-mail: [email protected] Simona Cabib: Centro Europeo di Ricerca sul Cervello (CERC), Fondazione Santa Lucia IRCCS, Via del Fosso di Fiorano, 64, I-00143 Rome, Italy Paolo Campus: Department of Psychology and CRIN, Sapienza University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy Valentina Colelli: Department of Psychology and CRIN, Sapienza University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy Introduction Stress is the main non-genetic source of psychopathol- ogy. Therefore, the identification of mechanisms capable of moderating the pathogenic effects of stress is a major goal of preclinical research. Recent studies focused on resilience – the resistance to environmental risk experi- ences shown by some individuals (Charney, 2004; Rutter, 2006; McEwen, 2007; Feder et al., 2009). Indeed, the path- ogenic potential of stress experiences does not depend solely on their severity: a large proportion of individuals exposed to traumatic experiences do not show patho- logical outcomes (Charney, 2004) and events appraised as positive by most people can be pathogenic stressors. Thus, either bereavement or marriage is a potentially pathogenic experience (Paykel, 1997). There is evidence from studies in humans and in experimental animals that resilience can result from a history of stress (the so-called steeling or immunizing effects) (Rutter, 2006). The brain undergoes major plasti- city events under stress challenge; these include genomic and non-genomic changes and reorganization of neural connectivity. Some of these events have been associated with an increased risk for pathological outcomes, whereas others seem involved in the development of resilience (Krishnan et al., 2007; Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009; Mitra et al., 2009; Shansky et al., 2009; Vialou et al., 2010). These observations suggest the hypothesis that stress resilience can emerge from neuroadaptive changes promoted by the interaction between a stress history and a specific genetic makeup; in other words, from specific gene-environment (G × E) interactions. Adult brain plasticity is associated with learning, and there is evidence of a bidirectional relationship between stress and learning in mature organisms. Stress can either increase or reduce the persistence of long-term memo- ries and can bias learning toward habitual responses (Schwabe and Wolf, 2012; Schwabe et al., 2012). More- over, the experience of being unable to remove, avoid, or control a stressor facilitates subsequent fear conditioning but renders organisms resistant to the extinction of an acquired fear, whereas the experience of stress control has opposite effects (Baratta et al., 2007; Amat et al., 2012). Finally, coping strategies acquired through previous stress experiences moderate the impact of a subsequent stress challenge (Maier and Seligman, 1976; Maier and Watkins, 2010). These findings support the view that brain learn- ing systems are a major target of stress neuroplasticity and suggest that learning-associated neurophenotypes could constrain neuroplastic outcomes of stress experiences. Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154 Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM
14

Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

Jan 29, 2023

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

DOI 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0080      Rev. Neurosci. 2012; 23(5-6): 659–672

Simona Cabib * , Paolo Campus and Valentina Colelli

Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience Abstract: Stress is the main non-genetic source of psy-

chopathology. Therefore, the identification of neurobio-

logical bases of resilience, the resistance to pathological

outcomes of stress, is a most relevant topic of research.

It is an accepted view that resilient individuals are those

who do not develop helplessness, or other depression-like

phenotypes, following a history of stress. In the present

review, we discuss the phenotypic differences between

mice of the inbred C57BL/6J and DBA/2J strains that could

be associated with the strain-specific resistance to help-

lessness observable in DBA/2J mice. The reviewed results

support the hypothesis that resilience to stress-promoted

helplessness develops through interactions between a

specific genetic makeup and a history of stress, and is

associated with an active coping style, a bias toward the

use of stimulus-response learning, and specific adap-

tive changes of mesoaccumbens dopamine transmission

under stress. Finally, evidence that compulsivity rep-

resents a side effect of the neuroadaptive processes fos-

tering resistance to develop depressive-like phenotypes

under stress is discussed.

Keywords: adrenal hormones; D2 dopamine recep-

tors; endophenotypes; fitness; habit-like responses;

hippocampus.

*Corresponding author: Simona Cabib, Department of Psychology

and CRIN, Sapienza University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185

Rome, Italy, e-mail: [email protected]

Simona Cabib: Centro Europeo di Ricerca sul Cervello (CERC),

Fondazione Santa Lucia IRCCS, Via del Fosso di Fiorano, 64, I-00143

Rome, Italy

Paolo Campus: Department of Psychology and CRIN, Sapienza

University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy

Valentina Colelli: Department of Psychology and CRIN, Sapienza

University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, I-00185 Rome, Italy

Introduction Stress is the main non-genetic source of psychopathol-

ogy. Therefore, the identification of mechanisms capable

of moderating the pathogenic effects of stress is a major

goal of preclinical research. Recent studies focused on

resilience – the resistance to environmental risk experi-

ences shown by some individuals (Charney, 2004; Rutter,

2006; McEwen, 2007; Feder et   al., 2009). Indeed, the path-

ogenic potential of stress experiences does not depend

solely on their severity: a large proportion of individuals

exposed to traumatic experiences do not show patho-

logical outcomes (Charney, 2004) and events appraised

as positive by most people can be pathogenic stressors.

Thus, either bereavement or marriage is a potentially

pathogenic experience (Paykel, 1997).

There is evidence from studies in humans and in

experimental animals that resilience can result from a

history of stress (the so-called steeling or immunizing

effects) (Rutter, 2006). The brain undergoes major plasti-

city events under stress challenge; these include genomic

and non-genomic changes and reorganization of neural

connectivity. Some of these events have been associated

with an increased risk for pathological outcomes, whereas

others seem involved in the development of resilience

(Krishnan et   al., 2007; Dias-Ferreira et   al., 2009; Mitra

et   al., 2009; Shansky et   al., 2009; Vialou et   al., 2010). These

observations suggest the hypothesis that stress resilience

can emerge from neuroadaptive changes promoted by the

interaction between a stress history and a specific genetic

makeup; in other words, from specific gene-environment

(G × E) interactions.

Adult brain plasticity is associated with learning, and

there is evidence of a bidirectional relationship between

stress and learning in mature organisms. Stress can either

increase or reduce the persistence of long-term memo-

ries and can bias learning toward habitual responses

(Schwabe and Wolf, 2012; Schwabe et   al., 2012). More-

over, the experience of being unable to remove, avoid, or

control a stressor facilitates subsequent fear conditioning

but renders organisms resistant to the extinction of an

acquired fear, whereas the experience of stress control has

opposite effects (Baratta et   al., 2007; Amat et   al., 2012).

Finally, coping strategies acquired through previous stress

experiences moderate the impact of a subsequent stress

challenge (Maier and Seligman, 1976; Maier and Watkins,

2010). These findings support the view that brain learn-

ing systems are a major target of stress neuroplasticity and

suggest that learning-associated neurophenotypes could

constrain neuroplastic outcomes of stress experiences.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 2: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

660      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

In the following pages, we will present evidences from a

mouse model of individual resistance to helplessness, a

known pathological outcome of stress experience, sup-

porting development of this phenotype through G × E

interactions promoting plasticity within specific learning

systems.

Stress, emotion, and coping Stress-related concepts suffer from their massive, and

usually inappropriate, utilization in common language.

Moreover, there is a diffuse, although repeatedly chal-

lenged, view that stress is anything increasing the phasic

levels of cortisol/corticosterone. Although a discussion of

these topics is beyond the goal of the present review (see

Koolhaas et   al., 2011; Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 2012, for

recent reviews), some definitions are required.

The present review deals with psychological stress-

ors, and the term stressor will indicate stimuli, experi-

ences, events, or problems appraised by the organism as

challenges demanding beyond its actual means (Folkman

et   al., 1986a,b; Lazarus, 1993; Koolhaas et   al., 2011). The

appraisal of a stressor is associated with very high levels of

emotional arousal and with a stereotypic pattern of physi-

ological responses such as increased release of cortico-

tropin-releasing hormone, adrenocorticotropic hormone,

and corticosterone/cortisol. These have profound effects

on peripheral systems and support the organisms in the

face of stressful challenges. However, they are dangerous

for the organisms ’ survival and well-being if sustained;

thus, once activated, they have to be terminated as soon

as possible (McEwen, 2007).

Coping responses develop from action-oriented and

intrapsychic efforts to manage the demands created by

stressors (Taylor and Stanton, 2007). They terminate or

moderate stress responses, preventing allostatic load

(McEwen, 2007), thus protecting the organisms against

most serious health threats. Therefore, coping responses

appraised as successful are acquired and consolidated to

be readily implemented in similar situations (Maier and

Seligman, 1976). There are two broad categories of coping

responses: those targeting the source of stress (problem-

focused) and those targeting the emotional arousal that

sustains stress responses (emotion-focused) (Folkman

et   al., 1986b; Lazarus, 1993). The success of one or the

other type of coping depends on the stressor. Thus, when

stressors are susceptible to action (avoidable/control-

lable), problem-focused coping strategies are most suc-

cessful. However, when stress is promoted by problems or

events devoid of solution, unavoidable, or insensitive to

the subject ’ s action (unavoidable/uncontrollable stress-

ors), the only effective strategies are those aimed at regu-

lating emotional arousal (Austenfeld and Stanton, 2004;

Maier and Watkins, 2010).

Studies on the effects of psychogenic stressors have

demonstrated that experimental animals use different

strategies when dealing with controllable or uncontrolla-

ble stressors. Exposure to controllable stressors promotes

development and consolidation of specific defensive

responses (freezing, escape, avoidance, and species-

typical social displays), whereas uncontrollable stressors

promote helplessness: a condition characterized by the

absence of attempts at removing/avoiding the source of

stress. The two patterns of responding to stressful events

are known as active and passive coping and are strongly

reminiscent of human problem-focused and emotion-

focused coping (Drugan et   al., 1985; Cabib and Puglisi-

Allegra, 1994; Anisman and Merali, 2001; Maier and

Watkins, 2005, 2010). Indeed, activation of brain opioid

receptors moderates emotional arousal and terminates

stress responses, and release of endogenous opioids is pro-

moted by uncontrollable but not by controllable stressors

(Maier et   al., 1982; Drugan et   al., 1985; Whitehouse et   al.,

1985; Drolet et   al., 2001; Ribeiro et   al., 2005). Thus, in both

humans and non-human animals, passive coping is asso-

ciated with blunting stress-induced emotional arousal.

In summary, stressful events promote necessary but

unsustainable stress responses supported by very high

levels of emotional arousal. These responses can be ter-

minated by eliminating the stressor through the expres-

sion of active coping or reducing the emotional arousal

through passive coping. Finally, either type of coping is

adaptive depending on the degree of controllability of the

stressful condition.

Coping styles Because active and passive coping responses are effec-

tive in different stressful situations, flexible coping is to

be considered the healthiest strategy at the individual

level (Austenfeld and Stanton, 2004; Nes and Seger-

strom, 2008). Nonetheless, some individuals use passive

or active coping strategies preferentially, and naturali-

stic and experimental studies in different animal species

support the existence of true coping styles, i.e., the con-

sistent choice of one of the two coping strategies (Koolhaas

et   al., 1999). There is evidence that coping styles depend

on genetic predisposition, and it has been proposed that

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 3: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      661

individual variance of this phenotype supports popula-

tion fitness in variable environments, a hypothesis tested

by selection studies in mice from wild populations (Kool-

haas et   al., 2007).

Moreover, individual coping styles can result from

acquisition and consolidation of coping responses

through a history of stress. In animal models, both avoid-

ance/escape responses (active coping) and helplessness

(passive coping) are acquired even in single trials and

have lasting influences. Moreover, repeated experience

of uncontrollable stress promotes a form of persistent

and generalized helplessness, the learned helplessness,

whereas experiences of controllability promote resistance

to shift into passive coping in the presence of novel uncon-

trollable/unavoidable stressors (Maier and Seligman,

1976; Maier and Watkins, 2010). Finally, as previously sug-

gested, genetic predisposition and stress history can inter-

act to develop a stable coping style (Moffitt et   al., 2006;

Rutter, 2006, 2008). Thus, low maternal care increases the

risk of developing helplessness in response to stress in

adult life, and the impact of the early environmental chal-

lenge is moderated by a polymorphism in the serotonin

transporter (5-HTT) (Carola et   al., 2007).

There is increasing hope to identify fragility factors

involved in G × E interactions responsible for psychopa-

thology by the use of genetically manipulated mice (Caspi

and Moffitt, 2006; Carola et   al., 2007). The observation that

a phenotype is expressed by mice with a targeted genetic

alteration only following alteration of the early environ-

ment supports an interaction between these two factors

and suggests that the product of the altered genes plays

a main role in the neurobiological mediation of environ-

mental effects. However, G × E interactions responsible for

behavioral disturbances involve a large number of genes

each contributing to phenotypic variability to a very small

extent (Plomin and Davis, 2009; Plomin et   al., 2009).

The possibility to measure or even detect the phenotypic

effects of individual manipulation of most of these genes

is virtually non-existent. Moreover, although early expe-

rience is especially formative for individuals, the brain

maintains a plasticity that can be the target of stressful

experiences through the lifetime (Szyf et   al., 2008).

Comparative studies in inbred strains of mice repre-

sent a useful tool to identify polygenic influences in G × E

interactions (Cabib et   al., 1997, 2000; Puglisi-Allegra and

Cabib, 1997; Alcaro et   al., 2002; van der Veen et   al., 2007,

2008a,b; Mozhui et   al., 2010). Inbred mouse strains do

not offer the empirical precision that is the hallmark of

directed mutagenesis. Nonetheless, studies of the physio-

logy and behavior of inbred mice can facilitate the unbi-

ased discovery of biological and genetic correlations that

may help identify the genes and molecular mechanisms

mediating stress effects. Indeed, inbred strains have been

used for the creation of chromosome substitution strains

and recombinant inbred strains of mice, which provide

a permanent resource for studying the genetic control of

phenotypic variation (Williams et   al., 2001; Singer et   al.,

2005).

When compared with mice of the standard genetic

background C57BL/6J (C57), mice of the genetically unre-

lated inbred DBA/2J (DBA) strain show resistance to help-

lessness. Indeed, C57 mice show a much faster develop-

ment of helplessness in the forced swimming test (FST)

than DBA mice (Ventura et   al., 2001). Moreover, C57 but

not DBA mice develop learned helplessness. Thus, expo-

sure to an inescapable shock reduces active swimming in

a Y maze up to 24 h after the experience in C57 mice but not

in DBA mice (Shanks and Anisman, 1988). Strain-specific

liability to development and consolidation of helpless

responses is also observable in social stress. Thus, in the

course of repeated confrontations with an aggressive resi-

dent, only C57 mice show progressive decrease of escape

attempts; moreover, when confronted with a non-aggres-

sive resident 24 h after being defeated, DBA mice show

defensive responses whereas mice of the C57 strain show

prevalent immobility/withdrawal (Siegfried et   al., 1984;

Kulling et   al., 1987). Finally, previous stress experiences

further reduce helplessness induced by a different uncon-

trollable/unavoidable stressor ( ‘ immunization ’ ) and

prevent retrieval of a consolidated helplessness in DBA

mice but not in mice of the inbred C57 strain (Alcaro et   al.,

2002; Cabib et   al., 2002; Mozhui et   al., 2010). The latter

observation supports the view that resistance to helpless-

ness develops in DBA mice through G × E interactions.

The strain-specific resistance to helplessness shown

by DBA mice when compared with C57 mice supports the

use of phenotypic differences between these mouse strains

to investigate the mechanisms responsible for the develop-

ment of stress resilience. Indeed, it has been proposed that

an active coping style supports stress resilience because

clinical results indicate an association between emotion-

oriented coping strategies and higher levels of depressive

symptoms in clinical populations (Southwick et   al., 2005).

Brain plasticity, learning systems, and stress hormones G × E interactions are generally described in terms of

genetic modulation of individual susceptibility/sensiti-

vity to environmental challenge. However, comparative

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 4: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

662      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

studies on anxiety-associated behavioral phenotypes and

hormonal stress responses in mice from the inbred DBA

and C57 strains have produced, at best, contrasting results

(Cabib et   al., 1990, 1996; Rogers et   al., 1999; Ryabinin et   al.,

1999; Griebel et   al., 2000; McNamara and Lenox, 2004;

Voikar et   al., 2005; Mozhui et   al., 2010). These findings

are in line with the observation that variations in coping

style do not correlate with variation in the amount of emo-

tional reactivity to stress in selection studies in mice from

wild populations (Koolhaas et   al., 2007). Moreover, DBA

are more susceptible than C57 mice to stress-induced defi-

cits of brain self-stimulation (Zacharko et   al., 1987) and to

stress-induced sensitization to the rewarding and psycho-

motor effects of addictive drugs (Cabib et   al., 2000; van

der Veen et   al., 2007).

The observations that strain-specific resistance to

helplessness in DBA mice is not associated with low

sensitivity to stress and that previous stress experience

increases resistance to helplessness in this strain of

mice support the hypothesis that resilience is the result

of a genotype-specific neuroadaptation to environmen-

tal challenges. Indeed, plastic events promoted by stress

experience show individual variability (Krishnan et   al.,

2007; Mitra et   al., 2009). In line with this hypothesis,

stress-induced expression changes in the brain of C57 and

DBA mice involve different genes (Mozhui et   al., 2010),

and stressed DBA and C57 mice show opposite changes

in brain dopamine (DA) functioning (Cabib and Puglisi-

Allegra, 1991; Cabib et   al., 1998; Ventura et   al., 2002).

Strain differences between DBA and C57 mice in

neuroplasticity were classically reported by studies on

hippocampus-dependent learning. Thus, impaired hip-

pocampal long-term potentiation, due to low protein

kinase C (PKC) levels, and reduced training-induced hip-

pocampal neurogenesis, CREB phosphorylation, and

72-kDa heat shock protein expression were reported in

DBA mice when compared with C57 (Matsuyama et   al.,

1997; Kempermann and Gage, 2002; Ambrosini et   al.,

2005; Sung et   al., 2008). Impaired hippocampal plasticity

is considered responsible for the bias toward the use of

hippocampus-independent learning strategies characteri-

stic of DBA mice, in sharp contrast with the bias toward

the use of hippocampus-dependent learning typical of

C57 mice (Gerlai, 1998; Passino et   al., 2002; Sung et   al.,

2008). In addition, mice of the DBA strain do not require

a functioning hippocampus in different types of learning

tasks (Arns et   al., 1999; Middei et   al., 2004; Baarendse

et   al., 2008).

However, the hypothesis of a strain-specific impair-

ment of hippocampal plasticity is challenged by some

observations. Thus, C57 rather than DBA mice show

stress-induced reduction of hippocampal PKC (McNamara

and Lenox, 2004); increased hippocampal PKC levels were

reported in DBA mice following 8 weeks of moderate-pace

treadmill running (5   days/week, 60   min/day) (Fordyce

and Wehner, 1993); and 43   days of free wheel running

increased adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice of the

DBA strain but not in C57 mice (Clark et   al., 2012). These

findings indicate a low learning-associated hippocampal

plasticity in DBA mice that could be the result rather than

the cause of a bias toward the use of hippocampus-inde-

pendent strategies.

The preferential use of hippocampus-dependent

or   -independent learning strategies has also been reported

by studies in human subjects. Indeed, engagement of hip-

pocampus-dependent circuits in learning is a dimension

along which people vary (Hartley et   al., 2003; Iaria et   al.,

2003; Bohbot et   al., 2007; Banner et   al., 2011; Marchette

et   al., 2011). Moreover, a recent study has associated this

variance to the Val66Met polymorphism of the brain-

derived neurotrophic factor ( BDNF ) gene (Banner et   al.,

2011), a finding that supports genetic determinants of this

phenotype. However, acute stress experiences have been

shown to shift humans and experimental animals from

spatial (hippocampus-dependent) to stimulus-response

(hippocampus-independent) learning (Schwabe et   al.,

2007; Dias-Ferreira et   al., 2009; Packard, 2009; Schwabe

and Wolf, 2009), supporting a role for stress experience

in the promotion of individual bias toward the use of a

hippocampus-independent learning strategy. Finally, it

has been proposed that the relationship (either healthy

or pathological) between brain systems supporting hip-

pocampus-dependent and   -independent learning is deter-

mined by interactions between an individual ’ s genetic

makeup, pre- and postnatal developmental events, and

accumulated experience through life (McDonald et   al.,

2004); in other words, by life-long G × E interactions.

Stress hormones are main mediators of stress-induced

neuroplasticity and are involved in stress-induced bias

toward the use of hippocampus-independent strategies

(Sousa et   al., 2008; Schwabe and Wolf, 2009; Schwabe

et   al., 2010). Indirect evidence supports a role for these

hormones in strain-specific neuroadaptation to stress.

Behavioral sensitization, the increase of the psychomo-

tor stimulant effects of addictive drugs, is promoted by

repeated exposure to the drug as well as by exposure

to stressors. Moreover, it is supported by neuroplastic

events, which are partially dependent on stress hormones

(Marinelli and Piazza, 2002; Marinelli, 2007; Robin-

son and Kolb, 2004; Renthal et   al., 2008; Thomas et   al.,

2008). Mice of the DBA strain are more susceptible than

C57 mice to stress-induced behavioral sensitization to

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 5: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      663

psychostimulants (Badiani et   al., 1992; Cabib et   al., 2000),

and psychosti mulant-induced behavioral sensitization

is prevented by adrenalectomy in mice of the DBA strain

but not in C57 mice (de Jong et   al., 2007). Moreover, both

repeated psychostimulant and repeated stress promote

strain-specific changes of DA receptors of the D2 type in

the meso-striatal system in these mouse strains (Puglisi-

Allegra and Cabib, 1997; Cabib et   al., 1998; de Jong et   al.,

2008). Finally, exogenous co-administration of corticos-

teroids and epinephrine, but not administration of each

one, reverses the strain-specific effects of adrenalectomy

on cocaine sensitization (de Jong et   al., 2009) and mimics

the effects of stress on learning strategies in humans

(Schwabe and Wolf, 2009; Schwabe et   al., 2010).

In summary, the reviewed evidences support the

involvement of stress-induced neuroadaptation in the

development of strain-specific resistance to helplessness

by DBA mice. Moreover, they suggest that these neuroad-

aptive processes could be involved in the development of the

stable bias toward the use of hippocampus-independent

learning strategies that characterizes this mouse strain.

The nucleus accumbens in coping and learning Strain-specific adaptation of meso-accumbens DA trans-

mission to stress could be a relevant mechanism of resili-

ence in mice of the DBA strain. Indeed, DA transmission

in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) plays a major role in the

shift between active and passive coping (see Cabib and

Puglisi-Allegra, 2012, for a recent review). High levels

of NAc DA characterize the response to a controllable/

avoidable stressors and support active coping responses

by activating local D2 receptors. Instead, inhibition of DA

in the NAc characterizes the response to uncontrollable

stressors and is associated with the expression of help-

lessness. Mice of the inbred C57 strain are susceptible to

rapid reduction of DA in the NAc when under stress, and

this effect is prevented by a chronic antidepressant treat-

ment capable of reducing helpless responses. DBA mice,

instead, require prolonged exposure to an uncontrollable

stressor to show reduced NAc DA levels, and this resist-

ance is enhanced by a previous experience with a dif-

ferent stressor (Ventura et   al., 2001, 2002; Alcaro et   al.,

2002; Cabib et   al., 2002). Moreover, the two strains differ

in meso-striatal D2 receptor functioning, depending on a

G × E interaction possibly modulating the alternative splic-

ing of the D2 gene (Puglisi-Allegra and Cabib, 1997; Cabib

et   al., 1998; Colelli et   al., 2010). Nevertheless, NAc DA is

required for different types of learning. In particular, acti-

vation of DA D2 receptors in the NAc is necessary for both

active and passive avoidance learning (Manag ò et   al.,

2009; Boschen et   al., 2012). As discussed, these types of

learning stabilize active coping strategies.

NAc integrates limbic and cortical inputs arising from

monosynaptic glutamatergic projections that originate in

the ventral subiculum of the hippocampus, basolateral

amygdala (BLA), and prefrontal cortex (Groenewegen

et   al., 1987; O ’ Donnell and Grace, 1995; French and Tot-

terdell, 2003). Each of these regions is believed to supply

a different mode of input to the NAc, thus the interplay

between these different inputs is suggested to determine

the relative weight of discrete or contextual information

in associative learning. There is convincing evidence that

the interplay between cortical and limbic information is

modu lated by DA transmission in the two compartments

of the NAc: the shell (NAcS) and the core (NAcC). In rats,

excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampus or of the NAcS

prevent acquisition of place-reward association, whereas

lesions of the BLA or of the NAcC impair cue-reward asso-

ciation (Ito et   al., 2006). In 6-hydroxydopamine-lesioned

rats, impairment in the discrimination of substituted

(non-spatial) or displaced (spatial) objects depends on

the extent of DA depletion in the NAcC and NAcS, respec-

tively (Nelson et   al., 2012). Moreover, repeated intra-NAcS

amphetamine infusions significantly and selectively

enhance conditioned preference for a place associated

with a sweet reward. Instead, repeated intra-NAcC amphet-

amine infusions attenuate conditioned place preference,

whereas blockade of DA receptors in the NAcC facilitate

context-reward association (Ito and Hayen, 2012; Nelson

et   al., 2012). These results support the view that two dis-

tinct and competing circuits involving the hippocampus

and the NAcS on one side and the BLA and the NAcC on

the other mediate the relative relevance of spatial/config-

ural and discrete/elemental information in learning pro-

cesses. It should be pointed out that DA outflow during

stressful events typically involves NAcS rather than NAcC;

D2 receptors mediating the expression of active coping are

located in the NAcS; and NAcS rather than NAcC is the site

of action of stress hormones activated by addictive drugs

(Wu et   al., 1999; Marinelli and Piazza, 2002; Scornaiencki

et   al., 2009). Therefore, learning circuits involving NAcS

could be central in stress-induced neuroplasticity.

NAc lesions have been reported to improve cue-fear

association and to impair context-fear association in C57

mice, thus mimicking the effects of hippocampal lesions.

In DBA mice, lesions of the hippocampus are ineffec-

tive on either context- or cue-elicited conditioned fear,

whereas NAc lesions impair both (Ammassari-Teule et   al.,

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 6: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

664      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

2000b). These observations suggest a strain-specific

organization of learning circuits involving the NAc. In

line with this hypothesis, lesions targeting the NAcS have

been reported to facilitate latent inhibition in C57 mice

and to prevent it in mice of the DBA strain (Restivo et   al.,

2002). Latent inhibition requires a preassociation of a ‘ to

be ’ conditioned stimulus (CS) with the absence of any

relevant experience. Lesions of the NAcS could facilitate

the preassociation between the CS and the absence of

consequences in C57 mice by removing the interference of

contextual information. Indeed, C57 mice acquire latent

inhibition only after prolonged preexposure to the CS

within the training context, in line with the hypothesis of

competition between context and discrete cues for associ-

ation (Ito and Hayen, 2012; Nelson et   al., 2012). The obser-

vations that lesions of the entire NAc prevent context-fear

association, facilitate cue-fear association, and mimic the

effects of hippocampal lesions in C57 mice suggest a func-

tional predominance of the hippocampus-NAcS system in

this strain of mice. Also in DBA mice, the effects of selec-

tive NAcS and entire NAc lesions were similar. However,

both types of lesions reduced the associative strength of

discrete cues, as demonstrated by impairment of either

latent inhibition or CS-fear association, supporting a less

specific functional compartmentalization of the NAc in

this mouse strain.

However, lesions of the entire NAc also prevented

context-fear association in DBA mice. Although problems

with lesion localization in the brain of mice from differ-

ent inbred strains cannot be ruled out, there is a differ-

ent and more interesting explanation for this set of data.

Indeed, it has been proposed that performance in the

‘ context ’ version of the fear-conditioning test may be

based on complex configural (hippocampal) learning, but

it can also be based on an elemental (non-hippocampal)

learning (Gerlai, 1998). Mice of the DBA strain could rely

on this elemental learning in contextual fear conditioning

explaining the impairing effects of NAc lesion in either the

‘ cue ’ or the ‘ context ’ version of the task. Indeed, C57 mice

perform better than DBA in tasks that require the forma-

tion of complex cognitive representations (configural),

whereas DBA mice have a strong predisposition to process

single elements (Ammassari-Teule et   al., 2000a,b; Restivo

et   al., 2002). Moreover, when compared with DBA mice,

C57 mice are relatively poor learners in tasks that require

discrimination of a discrete cue (Bovet et   al., 1969; Izqui-

erdo et   al., 2006), and DBA mice show better discrimina-

tion of a novel object than C57 mice although these are

more efficient in discriminating displaced objects (Cabib

et   al., 2003; Orsini et   al., 2004). Together, results of inac-

tivation studies indicate that a circuit involving both the

NAcS and the hippocampus mediates learning based on

complex configural information in C57 mice, as reported

in rats. Instead, the observation that inactivation of the

hippocampus spares all forms of learning whereas inacti-

vation of the NAcS or of the whole NAc impairs them sup-

ports the absence of this functional circuit in DBA mice.

In conclusion, the influences of G × E interactions on

NAc-centered circuits could be responsible for the coping

style, learning strategy, and stress-induced neuroplasti-

city that characterize DBA mice.

The cost of resilience As discussed, it has been proposed that an active coping

style supports stress resilience (Southwick et   al., 2005).

However, findings from G × E studies suggest that resil-

ience can be better understood as a resistance to specific

stress outcomes rather than a general insensitivity to

stress. Indeed, negative childhood experiences increase

resistance to the development of depression and anxiety

in adulthood and increase the risk of developing anti-

social behavior in adolescence. The 5-HTT-LPR L allele

exerts a protective effect against the effects of early experi-

ences on depressive and anxiety phenotypes, whereas it is

high MAO A activity that protects against the development

of antisocial behavior in adolescence (Rutter, 2006; Carola

et   al., 2007). Therefore, a bias toward active coping could

specifically increase resistance against pro-depressant

effects of adverse life events rather than promote general

stress resilience.

Moreover, in view of the main role of stress in the

development of a wide range of disturbances (Thompson

et   al., 2004; Jones and Fernyhough, 2007; Gramotnev and

Gramotnev, 2011), an interpretation of resilience solely in

terms of low depression-like phenotypes can dangerously

underestimate other pathological outcomes of adapta-

tion to stress. In line with this view, chronic social stress

has been shown to protect organisms against the deve-

lopment of depressive-like responses at the expense of

central leptin functioning (Chuang et   al., 2010). Similarly,

food restriction protects DBA mice against stabilization

of helpless responses (Alcaro et   al., 2002) and promotes

strain-specific changes in the dynamics of leptin levels

(Gelegen et   al., 2007). Moreover, helplessness-resistant,

food-restricted DBA mice express high levels of cage ste-

reotypies, sensitization to the psychomotor-stimulant

effects of amphetamine, and increased sensitivity to the

rewarding effects of the addictive drug – effects that are

either absent or reduced in food-restricted C57 mice (Cabib

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 7: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      665

and Bonaventura, 1997; Cabib et   al., 2000) (see Figure   1

for a summary of the strain-specific effects of restricted

feeding). It is worth pointing out that strain-specific sen-

sitization to the behavioral effects of addictive drugs has

been reported in DBA mice following different types of

chronic/repeated stressful experiences (Badiani et   al.,

1992; van der Veen et   al., 2007). Finally, food-restricted

DBA mice develop compulsive wheel running, leading to

a dramatic loss of body weight and to death by starvation.

Instead, food-restricted C57 mice reduce their wheel acti-

vity, thus containing weight loss and reducing the risk of

starvation (Gelegen et   al., 2007).

These results suggest that the strain-specific adapta-

tion to chronic stressful experiences that characterizes

mice of the inbred DBA strain increases their resistance

to helplessness but renders these mice liable to com-

pulsive behavior. In line with this hypothesis, DBA mice

present endophenotypes associated with liability to com-

pulsion-like responses. Endophenotypes are measurable,

heritable traits theoretically situated in an intermediate

position between the clinical phenotype and the disease-

susceptibility genotype, which are proposed to be more

directly related to genetic risk (Gottesman and Hanson,

2005; Ersche et   al., 2010; Fineberg et   al., 2010). In the case

of compulsion-associated disturbances, including addic-

tion, most likely endophenotypes are impulsivity and low

striatal DA D2 receptors (Denys et   al., 2004; Everitt et   al.,

2008; Volkow et   al., 2009; Ersche et   al., 2010; Fineberg

et   al., 2010). When compared with C57, DBA mice are char-

acterized by both phenotypes (Puglisi-Allegra and Cabib,

1997; Cabib et   al., 1998; Patel et   al., 2006; Colelli et   al.,

2010; Loos et   al., 2010; Pinkston and Lamb, 2011). There-

fore, development of compulsion-associated phenotypes

by stressed DBA mice could reproduce the effects of life

events on individuals characterized by a specific genetic

liability.

It has been proposed that compulsive responses

engage a dorsolateral striatum (putamen)-centered circuit

involved in the acquisition and expression of stimulus-

bound habit-like responses. Indeed, human and animal

studies support the view that compulsions are associ-

ated with pathological neuroplasticity within this circuit

(Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Fineberg et   al., 2011; Gillan

et   al., 2011). Habit learning is normally developed through

extensive instrumental training (overtraining), leading to

a progressive shift from goal-directed responses (flexible,

dependent on goal value and contingencies) to habitual

responses (rather inflexible, independent of goals and

contingencies). It has been proposed that the shift from

goal-directed to habit-like responses is dependent on a

progressive replacement of ventromedial striatal circuits

by dorsolateral striatal systems in the control of behavior

(Dickinson, 1985; Balleine and Dickinson, 1998; Robbins

et   al., 2008; Belin et   al., 2009). Finally, reciprocal connec-

tions between the NAc and mesencephalic DA cells consti-

tute a relevant initial station of this process (Belin et   al.,

2009).

There is consistent evidence that stress, stress hor-

mones, and sensitizing treatments with psychostimulant

drugs share the ability to prompt a rapid shift into habit

learning in moderately trained organisms by influencing

plasticity within corticostriatal circuits. Thus, in a dual-

solution water cross maze, rats show a progressive shift

from a place (go to a target location) to a response stra-

tegy (make a specific body turn) to reach the escape plat-

form with increased training. However, an acute stressful

experience prompts the use of the response strategy by

moderately trained rats (Packard, 2009). Similarly, in a

learning task that allowed differentiating spatial from

habit-like learning strategies, stressed participants used

the latter strategy significantly more often than controls

(Kim et   al., 2001; Schwabe et   al., 2007). The latter effect

was mimicked by pharmacological costimulation of glu-

cocorticoids and noradrenergic activity, but not selective

stimulation of each one (Schwabe et   al., 2010). Moreover,

a chronic stressful experience fosters the development of

a stable bias toward habit learning and opposite struc-

tural plasticity within the frontal cortex and the dorsal

striatum in the rats (Dias-Ferreira et   al., 2009), and similar

neuroplastic changes are promoted by a protracted expo-

sure to exogenous corticosterone (Sousa et   al., 2008).

Finally, it has been shown that chronic exposure to a

psychostimulant drug capable of promoting behavio-

ral sensitization fosters a rapid shift to habit learning in

moderately trained rats (Nelson and Killcross, 2006). As

previously discussed, stress hormones are involved in

psychostimulant-induced neuroplasticity (Marinelli and

Piazza, 2002); thus, they could be responsible, at least

in part, for the increased habitual responding following

sensitization.

On the basis of the previously discussed strain differ-

ences between DBA and C57 mice, one could expect differ-

ences in their liability to shift into habit learning following

stress. This hypothesis has never been tested; however,

there is some indirect support. It has been reported that

either C57 or DBA mice use a spatial strategy to a similar

extent in the appetitive version of a Barnes maze, but DBA

mice abandon the spatial strategy in a negative version of

the same task (Youn et   al., 2012). Moreover, a temporary

experience of restricted feeding improves the detection of

configural changes by C57 mice and of elemental changes

by DBA mice (Orsini et   al., 2004).

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 8: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

666      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

180A

B

C

D

C57BL/6DBA/2

160140120100

Imm

obili

ty (m

in)

80604020

0

0

Sample

Sample

Test

Test

0

05

1015202530

40 §

§

§

§

35

10

20

30

40

50

60

Dis

crim

inat

ion

inde

x(te

st-s

ampl

e ex

plor

atio

n)D

iscr

imin

atio

n in

dex

(nov

el-s

ampl

ed e

xplo

ratio

n)

1020304050

Per

cent

cha

ges

from

free

-feed

ing

60708090

100

Naive Experienced

Forced swimming test

Conditioned place preference

Sal 0.5Amphetamine (mg/kg)

Spatial novelty

Food

Novel object

Nfood

Food Nfood

1

Exp+Nfood

Figure   1  A summary of the strain-specific effects of a temporary reduction of food availability (12 – 14   days ending 48   h before behavioral

experiments). (A) Helplessness (mean immobility duration ± SEM) expressed in a 5-min FST by mice exposed for the first time to the stressful

experience (Na ï ve), mice with experiences of the stressful situation (10   min, 14   days earlier: Experienced), and by mice that were exposed to

FST for 10   min 24   h before the start of 12   days of food restriction (Exp+NFood). Data were published in Alcaro et   al. (2002). (B) Conditioned

preference for a context repeatedly associated with amphetamine (mean % changes ± SEM from preference expressed by free-fed mice)

shown by mice exposed to 14   days of food restriction 48   h before the drug-context pairing. Data were published in Cabib et   al. (2002).

(C) Discrimination of displaced objects (mean differences ± SEM between exploration of the displaced objects on the test session and on

the last sampling session) by mice exposed to 14   days of food restriction 48   h before the sampling session and free-fed mice. Data were

published in Orsini et   al. (2004). (D) Discrimination of a novel object (mean differences ± SEM) between exploration of the novel and the

sampled object by mice exposed to 14   days of food restriction 48   h before the sampling session and free-fed mice. Data were published in

Orsini et   al. (2004). * p < 0.05 vs. DBA. §p < 0.05 vs. free-feeding.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 9: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      667

Conclusions The evidences reviewed indicate that mice of the inbred

DBA strain are characterized by a bias toward the use of

active coping and a resistance to develop helplessness

under stress. These phenotypes are strain specific, indicat-

ing dependence on genetic makeup; however, resistance to

helplessness increases following chronic or repeated stress-

ful experiences in this strain of mice but not in mice of the

inbred C57 strain, supporting a G × E interaction. Because

resistance to helplessness is considered the marker of stress

resilience, the neurobiological processes involved in the

G × E interaction responsible for resistance to helplessness in

DBA mice could represent important moderators of patho-

logical outcomes of stress experience. The review of the

results obtained by comparative studies in mice of the two

strains points to mesoaccumbens DA functioning and stress

hormone-induced neuroplasticity within the meso-cortico-

limbic circuits as the neurobological bases of resilience.

Finally, the evidences reviewed suggest that geno-

type-specific neurodapative changes associated with

resilience to stress-induced helplessness in mice of the

DBA strain could render this mouse strain most suscepti-

ble to compulsive-like phenotypes. Indeed, mice from this

inbred strain show endophenotypes associated with com-

pulsion liability and express compulsion-like phenotypes

following stress experiences. This finding does not support

the view that resilience to a specific stress outcome indi-

cates resistance to stress nor the hypothesis that active

coping supports resilience whereas passive coping is a

source of pathological outcomes.

It has been proposed that individual variability

in coping strategies is maintained within populations

because it supports population fitness in variable environ-

mental conditions (Koolhaas et   al., 2007). The opposite

extreme phenotypes expressed by stressed DBA and C57

mice could play such a role. A life history of uncontrol-

lable stressful events can predict environmental changes

dangerous for population survival. In these situations,

allostasis, individual neuroadaptive changes regulated by

G × E interactions, and extreme phenotypes can increase

population fitness (Meylan et   al., 2012). Dispersion is a

highly adaptive way of dealing with dangerous ecological

alterations, and proneness to develop compulsive active

coping under persistent stressful conditions could foster

successful dispersion of part of the endangered popula-

tion. In contrast, a strong bias toward the use of passive

coping strategies would protect part of the population

against the risks of migration, leading to successful out-

comes if the dangerous ecological change is temporary.

Received May 30, 2012; accepted October 7, 2012

References Alcaro, A., Cabib, S., Ventura, R., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2002).

Genotype- and experience-dependent susceptibility to

depressive-like responses in the forced-swimming test.

Psychopharmacology (Berl) 164 , 138 – 143.

Amat, J., Aleksejev, R.M., Paul, E., Watkins, L.R., and Maier, S.F.

(2012). Behavioral control over shock blocks behavioral and

neurochemical effects of later social defeat. Neuroscience 165 ,

1031 – 1038.

Ambrosini, M.V., Mariucci, G., Tantucci, M., Van Hooijdonk, L.,

and Ammassari-Teule, M. (2005). Hippocampal 72-kDa heat

shock protein expression varies according to mice learning

performance independently from chronic exposure to stress.

Hippocampus 15 , 413 – 417.

Ammassari-Teule, M., Restivo, L., and Passino, E. (2000a).

Contextual-dependent effects of nucleus accumbens lesions

on spatial learning in mice. NeuroReport 11 , 2485 – 2490.

Ammassari-Teule, M., Passino, E., Restivo, L., and de Marsanich,

B. (2000b). Fear conditioning in C57/BL/6 and DBA/2 mice,

variability in nucleus accumbens function according to the

strain predisposition to show contextual- or cue-based

responding. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12 , 4467 – 4474.

Anisman, H. and Merali, Z. (2001). Rodent models of depression:

learned helplessness induced in mice. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci.

Chapter 8 , Unit8.10C.

Arns, M., Sauvage, M., and Steckler, T. (1999). Excitotoxic

hippocampal lesions disrupt allocentric spatial learning in

mice: effects of strain and task demands. Behav. Brain Res.

106 , 151 – 164.

Austenfeld, J.L. and Stanton, A.L. (2004). Coping through emotional

approach: a new look at emotion, coping, and health-related

outcomes. J. Person. 72 , 1335 – 1364.

Baarendse, P.J., van Grootheest, G., Jansen, R.F., Pieneman, A.W.,

Ogren, S.O., Verhage, M., and Stiedl, O. (2008). Differential

involvement of the dorsal hippocampus in passive avoidance

in C57bl/6J and DBA/2J mice. Hippocampus 18 , 11 – 19.

Badiani, A., Cabib, S., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1992). Chronic stress

induces strain-dependent sensitization to the behavioral

effects of amphetamine in the mouse. Pharmacol. Biochem.

Behav. 43 , 53 – 60.

Balleine, B.W. and Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental

action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical

substrates. Neuropharmacology 37 , 407 – 419.

Banner, H., Bhat, V., Etchamendy, N., Joober, R., and Bohbot,

V.D. (2011). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met

polymorphism is associated with reduced functional magnetic

resonance imaging activity in the hippocampus and increased

use of caudate nucleus-dependent strategies in a human

virtual navigation task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33 , 968 – 977.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 10: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

668      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

Baratta, M.V., Christianson, J.P., Gomez, D.M., Zarza, C.M., Amat, J.,

Masini, C.V., Watkins, L.R., and Maier, S.F. (2007). Controllable

versus uncontrollable stressors bi-directionally modulate

conditioned but not innate fear. Neuroscience 146 , 1495 – 1503.

Belin, D., Jonkman, S., Dickinson, A., Robbins, T.W., and Everitt, B.J.

(2009). Parallel and interactive learning processes within the

basal ganglia: relevance for the understanding of addiction.

Behav. Brain Res. 199 , 89 – 102.

Bohbot, Vr.D., Lerch, J., Thorndycraft, B., Iaria, G., and Zijdenbos,

A.P. (2007). Gray matter differences correlate with

spontaneous strategies in a human virtual navigation task.

J. Neurosci. 27 , 10078 – 10083.

Boschen, S.L., Wietzikoski, E.C., Winn, P., and Da Cunha, C. (2012).

The role of nucleus accumbens and dorsolateral striatal D2

receptors in active avoidance conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn.

Mem. 96 , 254 – 262.

Bovet, D., Bovet-Nitti, F., and Oliverio, A. (1969). Genetic aspects of

learning and memory in mice. Science 163 , 139 – 149.

Cabib, S. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1991). Genotype-dependent effects

of chronic stress on apomorphine-induced alterations of

striatal and mesolimbic dopamine metabolism. Brain Res. 542 ,

91 – 96.

Cabib, S. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1994). Opposite responses

of mesolimbic dopamine system to controllable and

uncontrollable aversive experiences. J. Neurosci. 14 ,

3333 – 3340.

Cabib, S. and Bonaventura, N. (1997). Parallel strain-dependent

susceptibility to environmentally-induced stereotypies and

stress-induced behavioral sensitization in mice. Physiol.

Behav. 61 , 499 – 506.

Cabib, S. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2012). The mesoaccumbens

dopamine in coping with stress. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36 ,

78 – 89.

Cabib, S., Algeri, S., Perego, C., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1990).

Behavioral and biochemical changes monitored in two

inbred strains of mice during exploration of an unfamiliar

environment. Physiol. Behav. 47 , 749 – 753.

Cabib, S., Castellano, C., Patacchioli, F.R., Cigliana, G., Angelucci, L.,

and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1996). Opposite strain-dependent

effects of post-training corticosterone in a passive avoidance

task in mice: role of dopamine. Brain Res. 729 , 110 – 118.

Cabib, S., Oliverio, A., Ventura, R., Lucchese, F., and Puglisi-Allegra,

S. (1997). Brain dopamine receptor plasticity, testing a

diathesis-stress hypothesis in an animal model. Psychophar-

macology (Berl) 132 , 153 – 160.

Cabib, S., Giardino, L., Calza, L., Zanni, M., Mele, A., and Puglisi-

Allegra, S. (1998). Stress promotes major changes in dopamine

receptor densities within the mesoaccumbens and

nigrostriatal systems. Neuroscience 84 , 193 – 200.

Cabib, S., Orsini, C., Le Moal, M., and Piazza, P.V. (2000). Abolition

and reversal of strain differences in behavioral responses to

drugs of abuse after a brief experience. Science 289 , 463 – 465.

Cabib, S., Ventura, R., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2002). Opposite

imbalances between mesocortical and mesoaccumbens

dopamine responses to stress by the same genotype depending

on living conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 129 , 179 – 185.

Cabib, S., Pascucci, T., Ventura, R., Romano, V., and Puglisi-Allegra,

S. (2003). The behavioral profile of severe mental retardation

in a genetic mouse model of phenylketonuria. Behav. Genet.

33 , 301 – 310.

Carola, V., Frazzetto, G., Pascucci, T., Audero, E., Puglisi-Allegra, S.,

Cabib, S., Lesch, K.P., and Gross, C. (2007). Identifying

molecular substrates in a mouse model of the serotonin

transporter × environment risk factor for anxiety and

depression. Biol. Psychiatry 63 , 840 – 846.

Caspi, A. and Moffitt, T.E. (2006). Gene-environment interactions

in psychiatry, joining forces with neuroscience. Nat. Rev.

Neurosci. 7 , 583 – 590.

Charney, D.S. (2004). Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience

and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to

extreme stress. Am. J. Psychiatry 161 , 195 – 216.

Chuang, J.-C., Krishnan, V., Yu, H.G., Mason, B., Cui, H., Wilkinson,

M.B., Zigman, J.M., Elmquist, J.K., Nestler, E.J., and Lutter, M.

(2010). A [ β ]3-adrenergic-leptin-melanocortin circuit regulates

behavioral and metabolic changes induced by chronic stress.

Biol. Psychiatry 67 , 1075 – 1082.

Clark, P.J., Kohman, R.A., Miller, D.S., Bhattacharya, T.K.,

Brzezinska, W.J., and Rhodes, J.S. (2012). Genetic influences

on exercise-induced adult hippocampal neurogenesis across

12 divergent mouse strains. Genes Brain Behav. 10 , 345 – 353.

Colelli, V., Fiorenza, M.T., Conversi, D., Orsini, C., and Cabib, S.

(2010). Strain-specific proportion of the two isoforms of the

dopamine D2 receptor in the mouse striatum: associated

neural and behavioral phenotypes. Genes Brain Behav. 9 ,

703 – 711.

de Jong, I.E., Oitzl, M.S., and de Kloet, E.R. (2007). Adrenalectomy

prevents behavioural sensitisation of mice to cocaine in a

genotype-dependent manner. Behav. Brain Res. 177 , 329 – 339.

de Jong, I.E., Steenbergen, P.J., and de Kloet, E.R. (2008). Strain

differences in the effects of adrenalectomy on the midbrain

dopamine system: implication for behavioral sensitization to

cocaine. Neuroscience 153 , 594 – 604.

de Jong, I.E., Steenbergen, P.J., and de Kloet, E.R. (2009). Behavioral

sensitization to cocaine, cooperation between glucocorticoids

and epinephrine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204 , 693 – 703.

Denys, D., van der Wee, N., Janssen, J., De Geus, F., and

Westenberg, H.G. (2004). Low level of dopaminergic D2

receptor binding in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol.

Psychiatry 55 , 1041 – 1045.

Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J.C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., Mesquita, A.R.,

Cerqueira, J.J., Costa, R.M., and Sousa, N. (2009). Chronic

stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects

decision-making. Science 325 , 621 – 625.

Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of

behavioural autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, Biol. Sci.

308 , 67 – 78.

Drolet, G., Dumont, E.C., Gosselin, I., Kinkead, R., Laforest, S.,

and Trottier, J.F. (2001). Role of endogenous opioid system

in the regulation of the stress response. Prog. Neuropsycho-

pharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 25 , 729 – 741.

Drugan, R.C., Ader, D.N., and Maier, S.F. (1985). Shock control-

lability and the nature of stress-induced analgesia. Behav.

Neurosci. 99 , 791 – 801.

Ersche, K.D., Turton, A.J., Pradhan, S., Bullmore, E.T., and Robbins,

T.W. (2010). Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus

sensation-seeking personality traits. Biol. Psychiatry 68 ,

770 – 773.

Everitt, B.J. and Robbins, T.W. (2005). Neural systems of

reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to

compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8 , 1481 – 1489.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 11: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      669

Everitt, B.J., Belin, D., Economidou, D., Pelloux, Y., Dalley, J.W., and

Robbins, T.W. (2008). Review. Neural mechanisms underlying

the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits

and addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363 ,

3125 – 3135.

Feder, A., Nestler, E.J., and Charney, D.S. (2009). Psychobiology

and molecular genetics of resilience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 ,

446 – 457.

Fineberg, N.A., Potenza, M.N., Chamberlain, S.R., Berlin, H.A.,

Menzies, L., Bechara, A., Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W.,

Bullmore, E.T., and Hollander, E. (2010). Probing compulsive

and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophe-

notypes: a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology 35 ,

591 – 604.

Fineberg, N., Chamberlain, S., Hollander, E., Boulougouris, V., and

Robbins, T. (2011). Translational approaches to obsessive-

compulsive disorder: from animal models to clinical treatment.

Br. J. Pharmacol. 164 , 1044 – 1061.

Folkman, S., Lazarus, R.S., Gruen, R.J., and DeLongis, A. (1986a).

Appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms.

J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50 , 571 – 579.

Folkman, S., Lazarus, R.S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A.,

and Gruen, R.J. (1986b). Dynamics of a stressful encounter:

cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. J. Pers.

Soc. Psychol. 50 , 992 – 1003.

Fordyce, D.E. and Wehner, J.M. (1993). Physical activity enhances

spatial learning performance with an associated alteration in

hippocampal protein kinase C activity in C57BL/6 and DBA/2

mice. Brain Res. 619 , 111 – 119.

French, S.J. and Totterdell, S. (2003). Individual nucleus

accumbens-projection neurons receive both basolateral

amygdala and ventral subicular afferents in rats. Neuroscience

119 , 19 – 31.

Gelegen, C., Collier, D.A., Campbell, I.C., Oppelaar, H., van den

Heuvel, J., Adan, R.A., and Kas, M.J. (2007). Difference in

susceptibility to activity-based anorexia in two inbred strains

of mice. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17 , 199 – 205.

Gerlai, R. (1998). Contextual learning and cue association in fear

conditioning in mice: a strain comparison and a lesion study.

Behav. Brain Res. 95 , 191 – 203.

Gillan, C.M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B.J.,

Fineberg, N.A., Robbins, T.W., and de Wit, S. (2011). Disruption

in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit

learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry

168 , 718 – 726.

Gottesman, I. and Hanson, D.R. (2005). Human development:

biological and genetic processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56 ,

263 – 286.

Gramotnev, D.K. and Gramotnev, G. (2011). Psychological stress

and psychosomatic treatment: major impact on serious blood

disorders? Neuroimmunomodulation 18 , 171 – 183.

Griebel, G., Belzung, C., Perrault, G., and Sanger, D.J. (2000).

Differences in anxiety-related behaviours and in sensitivity to

diazepam in inbred and outbred strains of mice. Psychophar-

macology (Berl) 148 , 164 – 170.

Groenewegen, H.J., Vermeulen-Van der Zee, E., te Kortschot, A.,

and Witter, M.P. (1987). Organization of the projections from

the subiculum to the ventral striatum in the rat. A study using

anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin.

Neuroscience 23 , 103 – 120.

Hartley, T., Maguire, E.A., Spiers, H.J., and Burgess, N. (2003). The

well-worn route and the path less traveled: distinct neural

bases of route following and wayfinding in humans. Neuron 37,

877 – 888.

Iaria, G., Petrides, M., Dagher, A., Pike, B., and Bohbot, Vr.D.

(2003). Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus

and caudate nucleus in human navigation: variability and

change with practice. J. Neurosci. 23 , 5945 – 5952.

Ito, R. and Hayen, A. (2012). Opposing roles of nucleus accumbens

core and shell dopamine in the modulation of limbic

information processing. J. Neurosci. 31 , 6001 – 6007.

Ito, R., Robbins, T.W., McNaughton, B.L., and Everitt, B.J. (2006).

Selective excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampus and

basolateral amygdala have dissociable effects on appetitive

cue and place conditioning based on path integration in a

novel Y-maze procedure. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23 , 3071 – 3080.

Izquierdo, A., Wiedholz, L.M., Millstein, R.A., Yang, R.J.,

Bussey, T.J., Saksida, L.M., and Holmes, A. (2006). Genetic

and dopaminergic modulation of reversal learning in a

touchscreen-based operant procedure for mice. Behav. Brain

Res. 171 , 181 – 188.

Jones, S.R. and Fernyhough, C. (2007). A new look at the neural

diathesis – stress model of schizophrenia: the primacy of

social-evaluative and uncontrollable situations. Schizophr.

Bull. 33 , 1171 – 1177.

Kempermann, G. and Gage, F.H. (2002). Genetic influence on

phenotypic differentiation in adult hippocampal neurogenesis.

Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 134 , 1 – 12.

Kim, J.J., Lee, H.J., Han, J.S., and Packard, M.G. (2001). Amygdala is

critical for stress-induced modulation of hippocampal long-term

potentiation and learning. J. Neurosci. 21 , 5222 – 5228.

Koolhaas, J.M., Korte, S.M., De Boer, S.F., Van Der Vegt, B.J., Van

Reenen, C.G., Hopster, H., De Jong, I.C., Ruis, M.A., and

Blokhuis, H.J. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status

in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

23 , 925 – 935.

Koolhaas, J.M., de Boer, S.F., Buwalda, B., and van Reenen, K.

(2007). Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidi-

mensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms.

Brain Behav. Evol. 70 , 218 – 226.

Koolhaas, J.M., Bartolomucci, A., Buwalda, B., de Boer, S.F., Flugge, G.,

Korte, S.M., Meerlo, P., Murison, R., Olivier, B., Palanza, P.,

et al. (2011). Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress

concept. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35 , 1291 – 1301.

Krishnan, V., Han, M.H., Graham, D.L., Berton, O., Renthal, W.,

Russo, S.J., Laplant, Q., Graham, A., Lutter, M., Lagace, D.C.,

et al. (2007). Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility

and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell

131 , 391 – 404.

Kulling, P., Frischknecht, H.R., Pasi, A., Waser, P.G., and Siegfried, B.

(1987). Effects of repeated as compared to single aggressive

confrontation on nociception and defense behavior in C57BL/6

and DBA/2 mice. Physiol Behav. 39 , 599 – 605.

Lazarus, R. (1993). Coping theory and research: past, present, and

future. Psychosom. Med. 55 , 234 – 247.

Loos, M., Staal, J., Schoffelmeer, A.N.M., Smit, A.B., Spijker, S.,

and Pattij, T. (2010). Inhibitory control and response latency

differences between C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice in a Go/No-Go

and 5-choice serial reaction time task and strain-specific

responsivity to amphetamine. Behav. Brain Res. 214 , 216 – 224.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 12: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

670      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

Maier, S.F. and Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Learned helplessness:

theory and evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. 105 , 3 – 46.

Maier, S.F. and Watkins, L.R. (2005). Stressor controllability and

learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus,

serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci.

Biobehav. Rev. 29 , 829 – 841.

Maier, S.F. and Watkins, L.R. (2010). Role of the medial prefrontal

cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Res. 1355 , 52 – 60.

Maier, S.F., Drugan, R.C., and Grau, J.W. (1982). Controllability,

coping behavior, and stress-induced analgesia in the rat. Pain

12 , 47 – 56.

Manag ò , F., Castellano, C., Oliverio, A., Mele, A., and De Leonibus,

E. (2009). Role of dopamine receptors subtypes, D1-like and

D2-like, within the nucleus accumbens subregions, core and

shell, on memory consolidation in the one-trial inhibitory

avoidance task. Learn. Mem. 16 , 46 – 52.

Marchette, S.A., Bakker, A., and Shelton, A.L. (2011). Cognitive

mappers to creatures of habit: differential engagement of

place and response learning mechanisms predicts human

navigational behavior. J. Neurosci. 31 , 15264 – 15268.

Marinelli, M. (2007). Dopaminergic reward pathways and effects of

stress. In: Stress and Addiction: Biological and Psychological

Mechanisms, M. al ’ Absi, ed. (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science),

pp.   41 – 83.

Marinelli, M. and Piazza, P.V. (2002). Interaction between

glucocorticoid hormones, stress and psychostimulant drugs.

Eur. J. Neurosci. 16 , 387 – 394.

Matsuyama, S., Namgung, U., and Routtenberg, A. (1997).

Long-term potentiation persistence greater in C57BL/6 than

DBA/2 mice: predicted on basis of protein kinase C levels and

learning performance. Brain Res. 763 , 127 – 130.

McDonald, R.J., Devan, B.D., and Hong, N.S. (2004). Multiple

memory systems: the power of interactions. Neurobiol. Learn.

Mem. 82 , 333 – 346.

McEwen, B.S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress

and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87 ,

873 – 904.

McNamara, R.K. and Lenox, R.H. (2004). Acute restraint stress

reduces protein kinase C gamma in the hippocampus of

C57BL/6 but not DBA/2 mice. Neurosci. Lett. 368 , 293 – 296.

Meylan, S., Miles, D.B., and Clobert, J. (2012). Hormonally mediated

maternal effects, individual strategy and global change. Philos.

Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367 , 1647 – 1664.

Middei, S., Restivo, L., Sgobio, C., Passino, E., and Ammassari-

Teule, M. (2004). Reversible inactivation of hippocampus

and dorsolateral striatum in C57BL/6 and DBA/2 inbred mice

failed to show interaction between memory systems in these

genotypes. Behav. Brain Res. 154 , 527 – 534.

Mitra, R., Adamec, R., and Sapolsky, R. (2009). Resilience against

predator stress and dendritic morphology of amygdala

neurons. Behav. Brain Res. 205 , 535 – 543.

Moffitt, T.E., Caspi, A., and Rutter, M. (2006). Measured

gene-environment interactions in psychopathology. Perspect.

Psychol. Sci. 1 , 1745 – 6924.

Mozhui, K., Karlsson, R.-M., Kash, T.L., Ihne, J., Norcross, M.,

Patel, S., Farrell, M.R., Hill, E.E., Graybeal, C., Martin, K.P.,

et al. (2010). Strain differences in stress responsivity are

associated with divergent amygdala gene expression and

glutamate-mediated neuronal excitability. J. Neurosci. 30 ,

5357 – 5367.

Nelson, A. and Killcross, S. (2006). Amphetamine exposure

enhances habit formation. J. Neurosci. 26 , 3805 – 3812.

Nelson, A.J., Thur, K.E., Marsden, C.A., and Cassaday, H.J. (2012).

Dissociable roles of dopamine within the core and medial shell

of the nucleus accumbens in memory for objects and place.

Behav. Neurosci. 124 , 789 – 799.

Nes, L.S. and Segerstrom, S.Z. (2008). Conceptualizing coping:

optimism as a case study. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2 ,

2125 – 2140.

O ’ Donnell, P. and Grace, A.A. (1995). Synaptic interactions

among excitatory afferents to nucleus accumbens neurons:

hippocampal gating of prefrontal cortical input. J. Neurosci. 15 ,

3622 – 3639.

Orsini, C., Buchini, F., Conversi, D., and Cabib, S. (2004). Selective

improvement of strain-dependent performances of cognitive

tasks by food restriction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 81 , 96 – 99.

Packard, M.G. (2009). Anxiety, cognition, and habit: a multiple

memory systems perspective. Brain Res. 1293 , 121 – 128.

Passino, E., Middei, S., Restivo, L., Bertaina-Anglade, V., and

Ammassari-Teule, M. (2002). Genetic approach to variability of

memory systems: analysis of place vs. response learning and

fos-related expression in hippocampal and striatal areas of

C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice. Hippocampus 12 , 63 – 75.

Patel, S., Stolerman, I.P., Asherson, P., and Sluyter, F. (2006).

Attentional performance of C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice in the

5-choice serial reaction time task. Behav. Brain Res. 170 , 197 – 203.

Paykel, E.S. (1997). The interview for recent life events. Psychol.

Med. 27 , 301 – 310.

Pinkston, J.W. and Lamb, R.J. (2011). Delay discounting in C57BL/6J

and DBA/2J mice: adolescent-limited and life-persistent

patterns of impulsivity. Behav. Neurosci. 125 , 194 – 201.

Plomin, R. and Davis, O.S. (2009). The future of genetics in psychology

and psychiatry: microarrays, genome-wide association, and

non-coding RNA. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50 , 63 – 71.

Plomin, R., Haworth, C.M., and Davis, O.S. (2009). Common

disorders are quantitative traits. Nat. Rev. Genet. 10 , 872 – 878.

Puglisi-Allegra, S. and Cabib, S. (1997). Psychopharmacology of

dopamine, the contribution of comparative studies in inbred

strains of mice. Prog. Neurobiol. 51 , 637 – 661.

Renthal, W., Carle, T.L., Maze, I., Covington, H.E., 3rd, Truong, H.T.,

Alibhai, I., Kumar, A., Montgomery, R.L., Olson, E.N., and

Nestler, E.J. (2008). Delta FosB mediates epigenetic desensi-

tization of the c-fos gene after chronic amphetamine exposure.

J. Neurosci. 28 , 7344 – 7349.

Restivo, L., Passino, E., Middei, S., and Ammassari-Teule, M.

(2002). The strain-specific involvement of nucleus accumbens

in latent inhibition might depend on differences in processing

configural- and cue-based information between C57BL/6 and

DBA mice. Brain Res. Bull. 57 , 35 – 39.

Ribeiro, S.C., Kennedy, S.E., Smith, Y.R., Stohler, C.S., and

Zubieta, J.K. (2005). Interface of physical and emotional

stress regulation through the endogenous opioid system and

mu-opioid receptors. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol.

Psychiatry 29 , 1264 – 1280.

Robbins, T.W., Ersche, K.D., and Everitt, B.J. (2008). Drug addiction

and the memory systems of the brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

1141 , 1 – 21.

Robinson, T.E. and Kolb, B. (2004). Structural plasticity associated

with exposure to drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology 47 Suppl

1, 33 – 46.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 13: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress      671

Rogers, D.C., Jones, D.N., Nelson, P.R., Jones, C.M., Quilter, C.A.,

Robinson, T.L., and Hagan, J.J. (1999). Use of SHIRPA and

discriminant analysis to characterise marked differences in

the behavioural phenotype of six inbred mouse strains. Behav.

Brain Res. 105 , 207 – 217.

Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific

understanding. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094 , 1 – 12.

Rutter, M. (2008). Biological implications of gene-environment

interaction. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 36 , 969 – 975.

Ryabinin, A.E., Wang, Y.M., and Finn, D.A. (1999). Different levels

of Fos immunoreactivity after repeated handling and injection

stress in two inbred strains of mice. Pharmacol. Biochem.

Behav. 63 , 143 – 151.

Schwabe, L. and Wolf, O.T. (2009). Stress prompts habit behavior in

humans. J. Neurosci. 29 , 7191 – 7198.

Schwabe, L. and Wolf, O.T. (2012). Stress increases behavioral

resistance to extinction. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36 ,

1287 – 1293.

Schwabe, L., Oitzl, M.S., Philippsen, C., Richter, S., Bohringer, A.,

Wippich, W., and Schachinger, H. (2007). Stress modulates the

use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in

humans. Learn. Mem. 14 , 109 – 116.

Schwabe, L., Tegenthoff, M., Hoffken, O., and Wolf, O.T. (2010).

Concurrent glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity shifts

instrumental behavior from goal-directed to habitual control.

J. Neurosci. 30 , 8190 – 8196.

Schwabe, L., Schachinger, H., de Kloet, E.R., and Oitzl, M.S. (2012).

Corticosteroids operate as a switch between memory systems.

J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22 , 1362 – 1372.

Scornaiencki, R., Cantrup, R., Rushlow, W.J., and Rajakumar, N.

(2009). Prefrontal cortical D1 dopamine receptors modulate

subcortical D2 dopamine receptor-mediated stress respon-

siveness. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12 , 1195 – 1208.

Shanks, N. and Anisman, H. (1988). Stressor-provoked behavioral

changes in six strains of mice. Behav. Neurosci. 102 , 894 – 905.

Shansky, R.M., Hamo, C., Hof, P.R., McEwen, B.S., and Morrison, J.H.

(2009). Stress-induced dendritic remodeling in the prefrontal

cortex is circuit specific. Cereb. Cortex 19 , 2479 – 2484.

Siegfried, B., Frischknecht, H.R., and Waser, P.G. (1984). Defeat,

learned submissiveness, and analgesia in mice: effect of

genotype. Behav. Neural Biol. 42 , 91 – 97.

Singer, J.B., Hill, A.E., Nadeau, J.H., and Lander, E.S. (2005).

Mapping quantitative trait loci for anxiety in chromosome

substitution strains of mice. Genetics 169 , 855 – 862.

Sousa, N., Cerqueira, J.J., and Almeida, O.F. (2008). Corticosteroid

receptors and neuroplasticity. Brain Res. Rev. 57 , 561 – 570.

Southwick, S.M., Vythilingam, M., and Charney, D.S. (2005).

The psychobiology of depressione and resilience to stress:

implications for prevention and treatment. Annu. Rev. Clin.

Psychol. 1 , 255 – 291.

Sung, J.-Y., Goo, J.-S., Lee, D.-E., Jin, D.-Q., Bizon, J.L., Gallagher, M.,

and Han, J.-S. (2008). Learning strategy selection in the water

maze and hippocampal CREB phosphorylation differ in two

inbred strains of mice Learn. Mem. 15 , 183 – 188.

Szyf, M., McGowan, P., and Meaney, M.J. (2008). The social

environment and the epigenome. Environ. Mol. Mutagen 49 ,

46 – 60.

Taylor, A.E. and Stanton, A.L. (2007). Coping resources, coping

processes, and mental health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3 ,

377 – 401.

Thomas, M.J., Kalivas, P.W., and Shaham, Y. (2008). Neuroplasticity

in the mesolimbic dopamine system and cocaine addiction.

Br. J. Pharmacol. 154 , 327 – 342.

Thompson, J.L., Pogue-Geile, M.F., and Grace, A.A. (2004).

Developmental pathology, dopamine, and stress: a model for

the age of onset of schizophrenia symptoms. Schizophr. Bull.

30 , 875 – 900.

van der Veen, R., Piazza, P.V., and Deroche-Gamonet, V. (2007).

Gene-environment interactions in vulnerability to cocaine

intravenous self-administration: a brief social experience

affects intake in DBA/2J but not in C57BL/6J mice. Psychophar-

macology (Berl) 193 , 179 – 186.

van der Veen, R., Abrous, D.N., de Kloet, E.R., Piazza, P.V., and

Koehl, M. (2008a). Impact of intra- and interstrain cross-

fostering on mouse maternal care. Genes Brain Behav. 7 ,

184 – 192.

van der Veen, R., Koehl, M., Abrous, D.N., de Kloet, E.R., Piazza, P.V.,

and Deroche-Gamonet, V. (2008b). Maternal environment

influences cocaine intake in adulthood in a genotype-

dependent manner. PLoS One 3 , e2245.

Ventura, R., Cabib, S., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2001). Opposite

genotype-dependent mesocorticolimbic dopamine response to

stress. Neuroscience 104 , 627 – 631.

Ventura, R., Cabib, S., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2002). Genetic

susceptibility of mesocortical dopamine to stress determines

liability to inhibition of mesoaccumbens dopamine and

to behavioral ‘ despair ’ in a mouse model of depression.

Neuroscience 115 , 999 – 1007.

Vialou, V., Robison, A.J., Laplant, Q.C., Covington, H.E. 3rd, Dietz,

D.M., Ohnishi, Y.N., Mouzon, E., Rush, A.J. 3rd, Watts, E.L.,

Wallace, D.L., et al. (2010). DeltaFosB in brain reward circuits

mediates resilience to stress and antidepressant responses.

Nat. Neurosci. 13 , 745 – 752.

Voikar, V., Polus, A., Vasar, E., and Rauvala, H. (2005). Long-term

individual housing in C57BL/6J and DBA/2 mice: assessment

of behavioral consequences. Genes Brain Behav. 4 , 240 – 252.

Volkow, N.D., Fowler, J.S., Wang, G.J., Baler, R., and Telang, F.

(2009). Imaging dopamine ’ s role in drug abuse and addiction.

Neuropharmacology 56 (Suppl 1), 3 – 8.

Whitehouse, W.G., Blustein, J.E., Walker, J., Bersh, P.J., and

Margules, D.L. (1985). Shock controllability and opioid

substrates of escape performance and nociception: differential

effects of peripherally and centrally acting naltrexone. Behav.

Neurosci. 99 , 717 – 733.

Williams, R.W., Gu, J., Qi, S., and Lu, L. (2001). The genetic structure

of recombinant inbred mice: high-resolution consensus maps

for complex trait analysis. Genome Biol. 2 , RESEARCH0046.

Wu, Y.L., Yoshida, M., Emoto, H., and Tanaka, M. (1999). Psycho-

logical stress selectively increases extracellular dopamine in

the ‘ shell ’ , but not in the ‘ core ’ of the rat nucleus accumbens:

a novel dual-needle probe simultaneous microdialysis study.

Neurosci. Lett. 275 , 69 – 72.

Youn, J., Ellenbroek, B.A., van Eck, I., Roubos, S., Verhage, M.,

and Stiedl, O. (2012). Finding the right motivation: genotype-

dependent differences in effective reinforcements for spatial

learning. Behav. Brain Res. 226 , 397 – 403.

Zacharko, R.M., Lalonde, G.T., Kasian, M., and Anisman, H. (1987).

Strain-specific effects of inescapable shock on intracranial

self-stimulation from the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res.

426 , 164 – 168.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM

Page 14: Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience

672      S. Cabib et al.: Learning to cope with stress

Simona Cabib is a full Professor of the Department of Psychology,

University of Rome “Sapienza”. She has an MA degree in

Philosophy from University of Naples Federico II°, Italy; an MA

degree in Education from Columbia University, NY USA; and a PhD in

Neurosciences from University L. Pasteur, Strasbourg, France. Her

fields of research are stress-promoted neuroplasticity, with special

emphasis on the brain dopamine systems, and neurobiological

mechanisms of gene-environment interactions in the mature brain.

Paolo Campus is a student of the PhD program Behavioral

Neuroscience of “Sapienza” University of Rome. He has an MA

in Cognitive Neurosciences and is preparing a dissertation on

strain-specific effects of restricted feeding on behavioral and neural

responses to stress challenge in mice.

Valentina Colelli is currently a Post-Doctoral Fellow of the

Department of Psychology of University of Rome “Sapienza”. She

has an MA in Cognitive Neuroscience and a PhD in Psychobiology

and Psychopharmacology from University of Rome “Sapienza”. She

investigates neural phenotypes promoted by gene-environment

interactions in mice including splicing activity of dopamine D2Rs

and system-specific expression of different transcription factors.

Unauthenticated | 151.100.156.154Download Date | 1/14/13 3:10 PM