Top Banner
Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex Adam R. Aron 1,2 , Trevor W. Robbins 3 and Russell A. Poldrack 2 1 Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK 2 Department of Psychology, Franz Hall, Box 951563, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA 3 Department of Experimental Psychology, Downing Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK It is controversial whether different cognitive functions can be mapped to discrete regions of the prefrontal cor- tex (PFC). The localisationist tradition has associated one cognitive function – inhibition – by turns with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal cortex (IFC), or orbital frontal cortex (OFC). Inhibition is postulated to be a mechanism by which PFC exerts its effects on subcortical and posterior-cortical regions to implement executive control. We review evidence con- cerning inhibition of responses and task-sets. Whereas neuroimaging implicates diverse PFC foci, advances in human lesion-mapping support the functional localiz- ation of such inhibition to right IFC alone. Future research should investigate the generality of this pro- posed inhibitory function to other task domains, and its interaction within a wider network. Many researchers agree that the function of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is broadly one of ‘executive control’ (i.e. the scheduling and optimizing of subsidiary processes imple- mented by posterior cortical and subcortical regions; see [1] for a review). There is, however, theoretical controversy over whether subregions of PFC are functionally differen- tiated. One influential view is that different areas within PFC perform the same operation (i.e. ‘working memory’) but for different sensory inputs [2] (but see [3]). A variant of the ‘working memory’ hypothesis is one which regards the PFC as providing top-down bias of posterior cortical and subcortical ‘modules’ [4]. Accordingly, the PFC acts like the signalman at a railway junction; depending on the context, different incoming traffic gets directed towards different outcomes [1]. Another, complementary, view of PFC func- tion is that it integrates events across time [5]. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging results suggests a localization of function to a network of PFC regions. It appears that, regardless of the particular contrast of tasks, there is regularity of (bilateral) activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal cortex (IFC), and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), but not other frontal regions [6]. This indicates a surprising sort of specialization of the PFC: a specific frontal network con- sistently recruited for solution of diverse cognitive problems. Although it is not disputed that memory is a funda- mental function of the PFC, nor that most neuroimaging task comparisons activate the same set of PFC regions (often including bilateral DLPFC, IFC and ACC), recent advances suggest that the IFC, right-lateralized (Figure 1a), can be identified with a particular function. We review recent evidence from behavioural studies of patients with unilateral PFC lesions. Lesion studies, unlike neuroimag- ing, can establish which brain regions are necessary for cognition, and advances in lesion-mapping technology, using structural MRI, allow better lesion resolution. The evidence supplements classic monkey-lesion work [7,8], by showing that damage to the right IFC impairs indepen- dent measures of executive control by disrupting inhi- bition (specifically of responses and task-sets). This poses a challenge to alternative views concerning the localization of such inhibitory functions to DLPFC [9] or orbital frontal cortex (OFC) [10] (see [11] for a review). The right IFC and inhibitory control Historically, an important paradigm for studying execu- tive control has been the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). The subject sorts a series of cards on different dimensions such as colour, number and shape. Once the subject has established the currently appropriate rule (e.g. ‘sort successive cards by color’), the experimenter gives negative feedback, and the subject is required to change classification to another dimension. Patients with frontal cortical damage are notoriously bad at the change stage (see [12] for a review) often explained by ‘perseveration’ of the previously appropriate rule. How- ever, because the WCST is complex, requiring not just shifting – but hypothesis generation, memory, and so on – any component could be affected by lesion damage. Hence, researchers have used executive control paradigms that more effectively decompose cognitive components. Two such influential paradigms are response inhibition (see [13] for a review), and task-set switching (see [14] for a review). Damage to right IFC crucially affects performance in these paradigms, apparently by disrupting inhibition. Addition- ally, we review studies showing that wider areas of the right PFC are required for the suppression of memories and responses to visual or auditory distractors. Response inhibition Response inhibition is the cognitive process required to cancel an intended movement. It is tested using Go/No-Go and stop-signal tasks [13]. The subject is required to Corresponding author: Adam R. Aron ([email protected]). Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.4 April 2004 www.sciencedirect.com 1364-6613/$ - see front matter q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
8

Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex

Feb 09, 2023

Download

Documents

Sophie Gallet
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex Adam R. Aron1,2, Trevor W. Robbins3 and Russell A. Poldrack2
1Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK 2Department of Psychology, Franz Hall, Box 951563, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA 3Department of Experimental Psychology, Downing Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK
It is controversial whether different cognitive functions
can be mapped to discrete regions of the prefrontal cor-
tex (PFC). The localisationist tradition has associated
one cognitive function – inhibition – by turns with
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal
cortex (IFC), or orbital frontal cortex (OFC). Inhibition is
postulated to be a mechanism by which PFC exerts its
effects on subcortical and posterior-cortical regions to
implement executive control. We review evidence con-
cerning inhibition of responses and task-sets. Whereas
neuroimaging implicates diverse PFC foci, advances in
human lesion-mapping support the functional localiz-
ation of such inhibition to right IFC alone. Future
research should investigate the generality of this pro-
posed inhibitory function to other task domains, and its
interaction within a wider network.
Many researchers agree that the function of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is broadly one of ‘executive control’ (i.e. the scheduling and optimizing of subsidiary processes imple- mented by posterior cortical and subcortical regions; see [1] for a review). There is, however, theoretical controversy over whether subregions of PFC are functionally differen- tiated. One influential view is that different areas within PFC perform the same operation (i.e. ‘working memory’) but for different sensory inputs [2] (but see [3]). Avariant of the ‘working memory’ hypothesis is one which regards the PFC as providing top-down bias of posterior cortical and subcortical ‘modules’ [4]. Accordingly, the PFC acts like the signalman at a railway junction; depending on the context, different incoming traffic gets directed towards different outcomes [1]. Another, complementary, view of PFC func- tion is that it integrates events across time [5].
Meta-analysis of neuroimaging results suggests a localization of function to a network of PFC regions. It appears that, regardless of the particular contrast of tasks, there is regularity of (bilateral) activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), inferior frontal cortex (IFC), and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), but not other frontal regions [6]. This indicates a surprising sort of specialization of the PFC: a specific frontal network con- sistently recruited for solution of diverse cognitive problems.
Although it is not disputed that memory is a funda- mental function of the PFC, nor that most neuroimaging
task comparisons activate the same set of PFC regions (often including bilateral DLPFC, IFC and ACC), recent advances suggest that the IFC, right-lateralized (Figure 1a), can be identified with a particular function. We review recent evidence from behavioural studies of patients with unilateral PFC lesions. Lesion studies, unlike neuroimag- ing, can establish which brain regions are necessary for cognition, and advances in lesion-mapping technology, using structural MRI, allow better lesion resolution. The evidence supplements classic monkey-lesion work [7,8], by showing that damage to the right IFC impairs indepen- dent measures of executive control by disrupting inhi- bition (specifically of responses and task-sets). This poses a challenge to alternative views concerning the localization of such inhibitory functions to DLPFC [9] or orbital frontal cortex (OFC) [10] (see [11] for a review).
The right IFC and inhibitory control
Historically, an important paradigm for studying execu- tive control has been the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). The subject sorts a series of cards on different dimensions such as colour, number and shape. Once the subject has established the currently appropriate rule (e.g. ‘sort successive cards by color’), the experimenter gives negative feedback, and the subject is required to change classification to another dimension. Patients with frontal cortical damage are notoriously bad at the change stage (see [12] for a review) – often explained by ‘perseveration’ of the previously appropriate rule. How- ever, because the WCST is complex, requiring not just shifting – but hypothesis generation, memory, and so on – any component could be affected by lesion damage. Hence, researchers have used executive control paradigms that more effectively decompose cognitive components. Two such influential paradigms are response inhibition (see [13] for a review), and task-set switching (see [14] for a review). Damage to right IFC crucially affects performance in these paradigms, apparently by disrupting inhibition. Addition- ally, we review studies showing that wider areas of the right PFC are required for the suppression of memories and responses to visual or auditory distractors.
Response inhibition
Response inhibition is the cognitive process required to cancel an intended movement. It is tested using Go/No-Go and stop-signal tasks [13]. The subject is required toCorresponding author: Adam R. Aron ([email protected]).
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.4 April 2004
www.sciencedirect.com 1364-6613/$ - see front matter q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
Task-set switching
Changing from performing one task to another exercises executive control. A precise measure is given by the task- set switching paradigm (for a review see [14]), which measures switching in terms of the time taken to switch compared with repeating a task (the ‘switch cost’). In brief, subjects perform a series of trials of task A and then switch to performing a series of task B. For each subject, the switch cost is computed by subtracting the average reaction time (RT) of non-switch trials from the average RT of switch trials. Intuitively, it is clear that having to
switch task requires configuring a new attentional and response set (e.g. getting ready to take up your cup once you have finished pouring the coffee). Apart from taking time to load new stimulus–response (S–R) mappings and choosing which attributes to attend to, changing tasks might require the inhibition of competing S–R links specified by the now inappropriate task, or even the inhibition of the entire task [31].
Converging evidence suggests the right frontal cortex might subserve inhibitory processes underlying switching. Neuroimaging studies of the WCST [32–34], reversal learning (e.g. [32,35]) and task-set switching (e.g. [36,37–39]) have especially reported activation of DLPFC and right IFC (although sometimes there is co-activation of left frontal cortex). A direct neuroimaging comparison of a form of switching (the WCST) and response inhibition demonstrated a common locus in the right IFC [18]. A combined EEG/fMRI study investigating Go/No-Go and Switch/Repeat factors suggested that the right IFC was responsible for ‘switching into a suppression mode’ [40]. Most persuasively of all, a study of patients with unilateral PFC damage demonstrated that the greater the damage to the right IFC, the greater the switch cost [41] (Figure 2a,b). This was not true for damage to any other region of right or left PFC. The switch deficit of these patients with right frontal damage appeared most consis- tent with impaired ability to suppress irrelevant responses or irrelevant task-sets on the switch trial relative to non- switch trials. In addition to being reliably correlated with the amount of damage to the right IFC, the switch cost was also reliably correlated with the SSRT measure of response inhibition (Figure 2c). This suggests disruption to a common mechanism underlying performance of the two independent tasks.
Inhibition during memory retrieval
In the course of daily life we often try to ‘push out of mind’ unpleasant events or memories. Such blocking of memory retrieval could be like overriding a pre-potent motor
Figure 1. Disruption of response inhibition by right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) damage. (a) A single coronal slice through a structural template of the human brain. The
thick white line demarcates the IFC in the right hemisphere. For each patient, the volume of lesion damage to this region was computed from a structural MRI scan (see [22]
for methods). (b) Extent of damage to right IFC, but not other regions, correlated with a response-inhibition measure (indexed by stop-signal reaction time, SSRT): greater
damage leads to slower inhibition ðr ¼ 0:83; P , 0:0001Þ [22]. (c) There was also a reliable correlation between SSRT and damage to a more specific region of IFC, the pars
opercularis (a posterior-ventral region; see Box 1).
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
re gi
on : p
ar s
op er
cu la
ris d
am ag
e (c
To ta
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.4 April 2004 171
response (for a review see [42]). Using a related paradigm, a recent neuroimaging study identified the neural systems involved in keeping unwanted memories out of mind [43]. Subjects were first trained on cue-target word pairs
(e.g. ‘ordeal–roach’). Later they were shown only a cue word on each trial (e.g. ordeal) and depending on the color in which it was written, they were required to either respond (subvocally) with the target word or else to try to
Figure 2. Disruption of task-switching by right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) damage. (a) Extent of damage to right IFC, but not other regions, correlated significantly with a
reaction-time measure of the switch cost for 18 patients with right-frontal damage ðr ¼ 0:82; P , 0:0001Þ [41]. (b) An even stronger correlation was apparent between extent
of damage to pars opercularis of IFC and switch cost ðr ¼ 0:84; P , 0:0001Þ: (c) A statistically reliable correlation ðr ¼ 0:59; P , 0:005Þ between the same measure of switch
cost and the response inhibition measure (SSRT), was reported for the same right frontal patients [22].
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
To ta
re gi
on : p
ar s
op er
cu la
ris d
am ag
e (c
S S
R T
( m
s)
Box 1. Comparative anatomy and function of IFC in man and monkey
The IFC (otherwise known as ‘ventrolateral’ PFC) in humans comprises
Brodmann Areas 44 (pars opercularis), 45 (pars triangularis) and 47/12
(pars orbitalis) (Figure Ia) [81]. However, relating lesion damage and
functional activation to any subregion of IFC must be performed
with caution as the correspondence between sulcul landmarks and
the underlying cytoarchitectonic areas (i.e. Brodmann Areas) is only
approximate [82,83].
In the monkey brain, the ventralmost part of the anterior bank of
the lower limb of the arcuate sulcus (extending onto the adjacent
ventrolateral prefrontal convexity, areas 45A and 45B in Figure Ib)
has similar architectonic characteristics to human area 45 [81]. An
area with architectonic features corresponding to area 44 in the
human brain is found in the lower limb of the arcuate sulcus in
monkeys (Figure Ib) [81].
Adequate comparison between species requires functional studies,
for example, with fMRI. One such study used a modified version of the
WCST to investigate cognitive set-shifting [33]. The most prominent
shift-related activation of the PFC was found in the bilateral IFC across
both species (Figure Ic,Id).
The IFC is one of the most heavily connected regions of the PFC,
receiving polymodal input from posterior cortical areas, and commu-
nicating heavily with other PFC regions [1]. It is one of the last brain
regions to develop in both ontogeny and phylogeny [84]. Immature
development of the IFC in children versus adults could explain signi-
ficantly different functional activity for response inhibition [15]. Detailed
neuroanatomical knowledge of this region, in tandem with better
understanding of innervating catecholamine neurotransmitter sys-
tems, is likely to complement future functional studies.
Figure I. Cytoarchitectonic maps of the lateral surface of the human (a) and
macaque monkey (b) prefrontal cortex. (Adapted from [81] with permission; only
left hemisphere available.) In (b), the principal sulcus is demarcated by the
mainly horizontal bold line. The arcuate sulcus is shown magnified in the inset.
(c) Bilateral inferior frontal functional MRI activations associated with Wisconsin
Card Sort shifting for monkey (top row) and humans (bottom row). (d) Same
activations displayed on inflated surface reconstructions of human (left) and
monkey (right) brains. (Adapted from [33] with permission; only left hemisphere
available.) Yellow arrow (left): inferior frontal sulcus; green arrow (right):
principal sulscus; blue arrow: arcuate sulcus.
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.4 April 2004172
suppress the target word. Activation was compared during suppression and respond trials. Although the authors emphasized a DLPFC focus associated with inhibition of unwanted memories, activation foci were also found in bilateral IFC. Investigating the DLPFC focus alone, the authors found evidence that it interacts with the medial temporal lobe (MTL), a region crucial for memory, during attempts to suppress recollection.
Future lesion studies are required to establish which PFC regions are necessary for the inhibition of unwanted memories, and whether these might in fact overlap with the right IFC region. A recent lesion study [44] provided some support for this hypothesis using a ‘directed forgetting’ procedure. On each trial, the subject was given a single word, followed by an instruction to remember or forget. This led to a high level of recall for to-be-remembered items and a low level of recall for to-be-forgotten items. Patients with right (as opposed to left) frontal-lobe damage showed impairments for directed forgetting [44]; however the locus of lesion damage was not specified at greater resolution.
The above studies concern inhibitory mechanisms in long-term memory. Evidence also exists for right IFC involvement in working memory retrieval [45,46], perhaps also related to inhibition. There is also evidence for left IFC recruitment related to inhibitory mechanisms in working memory, specifically for resolving interference from pre- vious trials [47,48]. In a common manipulation, subjects perform a test of item recognition: target letters are presented for storage followed, after a brief interval, by a probe letter that could match a target letter or not. On some trials, when the probe did not match a target letter, and required a ‘no’ response, the probe had matched a target letter of the previous trial, so on these trials a ‘yes’ response was prepotent and supposedly had to be inhi- bited. A patient with damage restricted to the left inferior and middle frontal gyrus showed a particularly large effect, reflecting increased interference in the prepotent response condition [49].
Interference tasks and negative priming
There exists other evidence for the role of right frontal cortex in cognitive inhibition. In a selective attention task requiring subjects to reach and touch targets but not distractors, an index of distractor suppression correlated reliably with lateral PFC damage in both hemispheres (albeit a region more diffuse than IFC alone) [50]. Increased distractibility as a consequence of lateral PFC damage has been demonstrated in auditory and antisaccade tasks in monkeys and humans [51–53]. Other neuropsychological studies have explored the phenomenon of negative priming – ostensibly reflecting the after-effects of inhibition: if a subject suppresses a response to a location or object on trial t, then responding to that object or location on trial tþ1 is slower relative to responding to a novel object or location (for a review see [54]). Negative priming is reduced in patients with RF damage [55,56]. The require- ment to overcome distraction or interference is also necessary for the Eriksen Flanker paradigm: comparison of incongruent trials (affording two potential responses) with congruent trials (affording one potential response)
produces right IFC activation [15,57,58]. Finally, right- IFC activation is also reliably greater as a consequence of dual-task interference; that is, when the interval between one task and the next is short relative to when it is long [59]. One interpretation of this latter finding is that the IFC is recruited to suppress the second task until pro- cessing resources are liberated.
Neurophysiological evidence
Although we have argued from the above evidence that functional activations in the right IFC reflect a cognitive inhibitory mechanism and that lesions to this region in non-human primates and humans alike disrupt this mechanism, it is unclear how cognitive inhibition relates to inhibition in a neural sense (by ‘neural’ we mean the systems level rather than that of single neurons).
Evidence for systems-level inhibition underlying cog- nitive inhibition comes from monkey neurophysiology [60]. Electrical stimulation of PFC No-Go foci produced reduced electrical activity in motor cortex, concomitant with a cancelled manual response (Figure 3). One No-Go focus was within the principal sulcus, whereas another was in the ‘rostroventral corner’ of the PFC (in both hemi- spheres). Although this latter region is anterior to the prefrontal convexity (i.e. the homologous region of human IFC; see Box 1), there was considerable variability between monkeys, and resolution was limited.
Other research has shown that electrical stimulation of frontal eye field neurons causes inhibition of saccade production, possibly through suppression of brainstem eye movement generators [61]. Most of the suppression sites were located deep within the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus (i.e. partly overlapping with IFC; see Box 1, and figure 1 in [62]). Saccade inhibition has also been explored with the stop-signal paradigm (for a review see [62]). When a stop-signal is given, activity in frontal eye field saccade- generating neurons rapidly decays, within the SSRT, whereas that for fixation neurons rapidly increases. It is possible that some foci within monkey frontal eye field affect the balance between fixation and saccade production neurons in such a way as to cancel movement [61].
In humans, as noted above, PFC apparently interacts with posterior-cortical regions such as MTL during cog- nitive inhibition of unwanted memories [43]. It is moreover possible in the case of response inhibition, that the right PFC suppresses basal-ganglia output, perhaps via the subthalamic nucleus (STN). Recent research has shown that patients with deep-brain stimulation of the STN had significantly improved response inhibition relative to a group with stimulation of the thalamus (W. van den Wildenberg, PhD thesis, University of Amsterdam, 2003). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation increases firing of STN output neurons, which increases inhibition of thalamo- cortical projections [63].
The emerging picture suggests an interaction between right PFC and (i) basal-ganglia, (ii) primary motor regions and (iii) memory-related MTL, in implementing cognitive inhibition. It remains to be elaborated whether the PFC source of such cognitive inhibition is specifically the right IFC, and whether neural activity in this region is itself
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.4 April 2004 173
excitatory or inhibitory with respect to neural activation (see also [64]).
Defining inhibition in neural-systems terms
A component of executive control, cognitive inhibition, can be localized to a specific subregion of the PFC, the right IFC (in particular, the pars opercularis1). The voluntary
blocking of memory retrieval might also depend on this same region, and a wider prediction is that any task requiring cognitive suppression of responses, task-sets or memories will be affected by damage or momentary deactivation of this region. ‘Inhibition’, as we therefore define it means the ‘suppression of inappropriate responses, S-R mappings or task-sets when the context changes, and suppression of interfering memories during retrieval’. Future research could establish to what extent this usage overlaps with other mentions of frontal ‘inhibition’ such as the inhibition of psychomotor representations in the parietal lobe [65], inputs to the sensory cortices [66], motor channels of the basal-ganglia [67], reflexes [68], orienting of attention (inhibition of return) [69], perse- veration in WCST [70] (and see [11,66,71] for a review).
Inhibition might interact with other PFC-implemented
cognitive functions
Cognitive inhibition could be one of a set of functions (including working-memory maintenance of task sets and items, selection and manipulation of information in working memory, and conflict detection) implemented by different, possibly overlapping, PFC regions. Which func- tions of the set get expressed for a particular task, and when they get expressed, might change. This motivates a different interpretation of neuroimaging meta-analysis: the reason why the same set of PFC regions is almost always activated (i.e. DLPFC, ACC and IFC; see [6]) might be because those regions separately implement different cognitive functions, and these interact to facilitate task performance. It is plausible that left-lateral PFC main- tains goals/sets [17,41,72], the ACC detects conflict when the stimulus does not match those goals [73], and right IFC…