Top Banner
INFORMATION TO USERS This reproductionwas made from a copy of a manuscript sent to us forpublication and microfilming. While the most advanced technology has been used to pho- tograph and reproduce this manuscript, the qualityof the reproduction is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted. Pages in any manuscript mayhave indistinctprint. In all cases the best available copy has been filmed. The followingexplanationoftechniquesis provided to help clarifynotationswhich may appear on this reproduction. 1. Manuscripts may not always be complete. When it is not possible to obtain missing pages, a note appears to indicate this. 2. When copyrighted materials are removed from the manuscript, a note ap- pears to indicate this. 3. Oversize materials (maps, drawings, and charts) are photographedbysec- tioning the original, beginningat the upper left hand comer and continu- ing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each oversize page is also filmed as one exposure and is available, for an additional charge, as a standard 35mm slide or in black and white paper format. • 4. Most photographs reproduce acceptably on positive microfilm or micro- fiche but lack clarity on xerographic copies made from the microfilm. For an additional charge, all photographs are available in black and white standard 35mm slide format. * *For more information about black and white slides or enlarged paper reproductions, please contact the Dissertations Customer SelVices Department.
172

INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

Nov 24, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

INFORMATION TOUSERS

This reproduction was made from a copyofa manuscriptsent to us for publicationand microfilming. While the most advanced technology has been used to pho­tograph and reproduce this manuscript, the quality of the reproduction is heavilydependent upon the quality of the material submitted. Pages in any manuscriptmay have indistinct print. In all cases the best available copy has been filmed.

The followingexplanation of techniques is provided to help clarify notations whichmay appear on this reproduction.

1. Manuscripts may not always be complete. When it is not possible to obtainmissing pages, a note appears to indicate this.

2. When copyrighted materials are removed from the manuscript, a note ap­pears to indicate this.

3. Oversize materials (maps, drawings, and charts) are photographed by sec­tioning the original, beginning at the upper left hand comer and continu­ing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each oversizepage is also filmed as one exposure and is available, for an additionalcharge, as a standard 35mm slide or in black and white paper format. •

4. Most photographs reproduce acceptably on positive microfilm or micro­fiche but lack clarity on xerographic copies made from the microfilm. Foran additional charge, all photographs are available in black and whitestandard 35mm slide format. *

*For more information about black and white slides or enlarged paper reproductions,please contact the Dissertations CustomerSelVices Department.

Page 2: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),
Page 3: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

8603661

Wylie, Alexander Michael

RATIO STRAIN: CONDITIONS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENT OF HUMANOPERANT BEHAVIOR

University of Hawaii

UniversityMicrofilms

International 300N. ZeebRoad, AnnArbor,MI48106

PH.D. 1985

Copyright 1985

by

Wylie, Alexander Michael

All Rights Reserved

Page 4: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),
Page 5: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

PLEASE NOTE:

In all cases this material has been filmed in the best possible way fromthe available copy.Problemsencountered with this document have beenidentified herewith a check mark_",_.

1. GI"ssy photographs or pages__

2. Colored illustrations, paper or print _

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

Photographs with dark background~

Illustrations are poor copy _

Pageswith black marks, not original copy __

Print shows through as there is text on both sidesof page _

Indistinct, broken or small print on several pages /"

Print exceedsmargin requirements__

Tightly bound copy with print lost in spine _

Gomputer printout pageswith indistinct print _

11. Page(s) lacking when ml1terial received, and not available from school orauthor.

12. Page(s) seemto be missing in numbering only astext follows.

13. Two pages numbered . Text follows.

14. Curling and wrinkled pages__

15. Dissertation contains pages with print at a slant,filmed as received ",/

16. Other _

UniversityMicrofilms

International

Page 6: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),
Page 7: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

RATIO STRAIN:

CONDITIONS OFPERFORMANCE DECREMENT

OF HUMAN OPERANT BEHAVIOR

A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TOTHE GRADUATE DIVISION OFTHEUNIVERSITY OFHAWAII INPARTIAL FULFILLMENTOFTHEREQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

INPSYCHOLOGY

DECEMBER 1985

By

Alexander Michael Wylie

Dissertation Committee:

Richard A. Dubanoski, ChairmanJacCarison

Elaine M. HeibyKarl A. Minke, Jr.Arthur W.StaatsCharles Glisson

Page 8: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

© Copyright byAlexander Michael Wylie 1985

All Rights Reserved

iii

Page 9: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I wouldliketo briefly, yet sincerely, acknowledge a number of people who have, either

directly or indirectly, influenced this product. Firstandforemost I would liketo acknowledge the

membara of nitJ dissertation committee, andparticularly Richard Dubanoski, for whose

permission andguidance I amgrateful in thecurrent research. I would distinctly liketo

acknowledge the friendship and intellectual stimulation of Michael Layng who contributed

explic~ly to the ideas contained herein andwhose knowledge of behavior analysis andthings

mechanical continues to be invaluable. I amalsoindebted to a number of colleagues andpeers

including, but not limhed to, JoeGrossmann, KhJones, andFrankWojnicki. Iwouldliketo

acknowledge andthankEnabling Technologies of Chicago, IL for theextensive useof their

facilhies in the production of thisdocument. I wouldliketo particularly thankT.V.Joe Layng and

Israel Goldiamond who haveeachsubstantially influenced and shaped my professional

development.

Personally, I amgrateful to JoanTagashira for herhelpand support overa pretty demanding

(and lengthy) period of time. I would alsoliketo thankmyparents for their continued support in

mypursuit of knowledge. Last, but not least, I would liketo acknowledge andthankall of my

friends fromthe Behavior Analysis Research Laboratory of the University of Chicago, including

thosepreviously mentioned, who although are not getting any younger, arecertainly "fast and

scientific."

iv

Page 10: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

ABSTRACT

Ratio strain, or the disruption of established patterns of operant performance. was examined

in series of three single-subject design experiments specifically observing theconditions under

which performance decrement occurred. Leverpressing performance of six human responders

under the control of large fixed ratio schedules of monetary reinforcement wassubstantially

affected: by thedegree of increase in a regimen of increasing fixedratios. with performance

deterioration onlyoccurring with relatively large schedule increases (Experiment I); by the

presence or absence of alternative behavioral opportunities (Experiment III);and to a lesser

extent by the response effort requirements of the operant involved (Experiment II). Otherthan

underconditions of response decrement. pertormance wasmaintained atsubstantially higher

ratios than heretofore reported. In each of the threeexperiments. pattems of response

decrement werequalitatively andquantitatively different. suggesting thatstrained performance

canbe a function of multiple variables. Thetheoretical implications of these findings to the

concept of ratio strain arediscussed.

In addition to patterns of responding maintained by the referent fixed ratio schcdele

programmed to the main response lever, patterns of responding were observed in the presence

of a second time out lever and a thirdextra lever. A self-iliaiated andcontrolled timeout

contingency wasin effect on thetimeout leverwhile nocontingency arrangements wereplaced

onthe extra lever. It is suggested thatobserved patterns of extra leverand time out responding

wereactually induced patterns being brought about bydegrees of contingency change. Each

occurred predictably underconditions of schedule variability in the referent contingency

produced by a number of change conditions. Patterns of timeout reponding didnotsupport the

notion of such behaviors being maintained bythe removal of stimuli associated withaversive

schedule conditions (Azrin. 1961). TIme out responding was lessthanthat to the no contingency

extra leverand response rates to the time out keydecreased uponthe introduction of the time

outcontingency. Thissuggests that thetimeoutcontingency may beprocedurally classified as

punishing in thissituation in that it restricted overall responding to the timeoutkeyunderthe

reported conditions of contingency induced variablility. Thetheoretical andclinical implications of

these findings are discussed.

v

Page 11: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. . . . . .

ABSTRACT .

LIST OFTABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

LIST OFFIGURES ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

LIST OFABBREVIATIONS .

INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . .

iv·

. ...v

........ viii

. . . . . . . . ix

. xi

. 1

Physical Fatigue andRatio Strain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1Definitions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2The Post Reinforcement Pause 4The Ratio Strain Literature . . . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8Trne OutPerfonnance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11ConciJsions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16

EXPERIMENT I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .19

Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .19Subjecls . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 19Apparatus . . . . . . . . .. 21ProceclJre . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24

Results 29Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50

EXPERIMENT II . . .64

Method . . . . . . . . .65Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . 65Apparatus . . . . . . . . .. . .66ProcedJre . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. 66

Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. ..67Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82

EXPERIMENT III. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. 85

Method .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . .86Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. " 86Apparatus . . . . . . . . .. " 86Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. ..87

Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .89Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. " 95

vi

Page 12: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

GENERAL DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . " 99

APPENDIX I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108

APPENDIX II . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130

Method 130Subjects 130Apparatus . . . . . . . .. 130ProcedJre. . . . . . 131

Resulls. '.' 134Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145

REFERENCES 149

vii

Page 13: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE PAGE

1 Orderandincrementation sizeof theregimen of fixed ratio schedulesandthenumber of sessions spent responding to each FRby eachsubject in Experiment I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29

viii

Page 14: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

LISTOF FIGURES

FIGURE

1 The experimental response apparatus

PAGE

.. 22

2 Overview of the main keyexperimental result for each sUbject in Experiment I .... 30

3 sarJl)le curTlJlative records ofAR; Expo I

4 8arJlJIe CUrTlJlative records ofIN; Exp. I

5 8aJTl:>Ie curTlJlative records ofPW; Exp. I

6 5ar1lJIe curTlJlative records ofHN; Exp. I

.33

.35

.39

.41

7 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout,andextrakeysbyARin Experiment I and the clnation oftime out 44

8 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout,andextrakeysbyIN inExperiment I 45

9 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout, andextrakeysby PWinExperiment I . 0 • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • 47

10 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout, andextrakeysbyHNin Experiment I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 48

11 The effects 01 increasing the response forceof the mainkeyfrom 115gm.to 500 gm. on responding byall subjects in Experiment II

12 sarJllleCUrTlJlative records ofAR; Exp. II

13 saf11)1e curTlJlative records of PW; Exp. II

14 sarJlllecurTlJlative records of LD; Exp.1I

.68

.70

.72

075

15 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout, andextrakeysbyAR inExperiment II . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .78

16 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout, and extrakeysbyPW in Experiment II . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .79

17 Frequency of responding to the main, timeout, andextrakeysby LDandduration of timeout,andmain key response frequencydJring a time outinExperiment II ... .. . . . . . . . . . . . 0 • • • • • 0 80

ix

Page 15: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

18 Frequency of responding to themain, timeout,andextra/alternative keysbysubjects LO andAR, and total eamings persession in Experiment III . . . . . 88

19

20

salll>lecumulative records of LO; Exp. III

sarJl)le culTlJlative records ofAR; Exp. III

..........90

.93

21 Overview of main keyresponse frequency in each conditionofExperiment IIIbby LM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133

22 Frequency of responding to the main andextra keys by subject LMandduration of timespent engaging inthealternative in Experiment IJIbduring sessions when the altemative was present . . . . . . . . . . . . 135

23 Frequency of responding to themain, timeout,andextrakeysduring sessions when noalternative waspresent byLM in Experiment IIIb . . 136

24 sample cumulative records of LM w~h alternative; Exp. IJIb . . 139

25 Sarrple cumulative records of LM Mh noalternative; Exp. IIIb 141

x

Page 16: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

CER

DRL

Ext.

FI

FR

GR

PR

PRP

VR

ABBREVIATIONS

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conditioned Emotional Response

Differential Reinforcement of Low Rates

Extinction

Fixed imarval

Fixed Ratio

Geometric Ratio

Progressive Ratio

PostReinforcement Pause

..... Variable Ratio

xi

Page 17: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

INTRODUCTION

Response patterns engendered by ratio schedules of reinforcement aredistinct and

replicable. Both fixed and variable ratio schedules of reinforcement produce rapid and

consistent response rates when theoperant behavior selected for analysis is fairly simple.

However, under certain conditions these consistent response patterns become variable or

nonexistent. A general pause or abulia in response patterning under thecontrol of ratio

reinforcement schedules has been referred to asstrain. Catania (1968) defines ratio strain as

the appearance of pauses in fixed ratio or variable ratio responding at times other than after

reinforcement (ct. the post reinforcement pause, PRP) which is a consequence of large ratio

sizes and/or insufficient reinforcement. Variabilny in theconditions producing ratio strain is

suggested by the factthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules

requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965), while others

report strain under ratio schedules requiring asfewas4, 7, (Staats, Finley, Osborne, Quinn, &

Minke, 1963), or 20 responses (Thompson & Moershbaecher, 1980). Thisvariability andthe

presentation of a number of inconsistent and/or limiting definitions of ratio strain suggest that

the phenomenon is notwellunderstood, or at least hasnotbeen systematically analyzed. The

following review andexperiments attempt to explore issues important in theanalysis of strain

while clarifying the nature of theconcept.

Physical Fatigueand RatioStrain

Although fatigue is implied bytheterminology "ratio strain", authors arequick point outthat

strain does notreferto physical fatigue. This paradox canbeunderstood when examining the

introduction and laterevolution of theconcept. Inhis initial presentation ofoperant procedures,

Skinner (1938) discusses strain in thecontext of fatigue of a hypothesized response reflex

reserve. It should be noted that, by reflex, Skinner refers to anobserved relation between

stimulus and response asan analytical unit, descriptive rather than neurological. Purportedly,

theprocess of conditioning strengthens a reserve of responses. During extinction then, thIs

reserve becomes fatigued anddepleted. Conversly, responding will be maintained if

reinforcement is provided on a ratio schedule significantly smaller than the extinction reserve.

Hence, strain as initially formulated refers to conditions where the ratio schedule does not

provide reinforcement frequently enough to maintain or strengthen the reflex reserve.

Significantly, Skinner laterrefines many of his innial speculations on conditioning andthis

Page 18: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

2

hypothetical reflexreserve is abandoned. However, the termstrain haspersisted in the

nomenclature, generally referring to observed decreases in frequency of responding under

certain condhlons of ratio schedules. An important distinction is seenin manylaterreferences to

ratio strainwhenthe concept is actually applied to the environment, as in expanding, increasing,

stretching, or "straining a ratio", rather than in thecontext of exhausting a hypothesized

construct. Evidence as to whyphysical fatique is not implicated in strain is provided by Ferster

andSkinner(1957). Inone example of a badly strained ratio, pauses of 80,40,45,83, and35

minutes were recorded during one experimental session with a totalof one response occurring

to the variable ratioschedule in effect. All pauses wereterminated by the introduction of a

stimulus lightcorrelated with anintermediate interval schedule of reinforcement, underwhich

responding occurred as normal. In thiscase, responding occurred underthe discriminative

control of one reinforcement schedule andnotthe other. Physical fatigue wouldprohibit

responding equallyundereachschedule. If anything, one might referto stimulus, rather than

physical fatigue. A properacknowledgement might be thatprecise discriminative control by

each schedule andassociated stimuli wasoccurring in this s"uation, andthat responding reliably

occurred onlyunderone component of the multiple schedule.

Definitions

A number of definitions of strainhavebeenproposed. In general, these definitions

suggest that strain is a function of large ratio sizes (Catania, 1968; Ferster, Culbertson, & Boren,

1975) ancllor abrupt increases in ratio size (Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 19n; Whaley & Malott, 1971;

Reese, 1978). Thatthe step-size, or ratio increment regimen, maybe as important in the

development of strain as the overall raiiu~ize is suggested by a number of authors.

Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer (1977) define strain as:

a disruption inperformance when ratio requirements areveryhighor are raisedabruptly. An individual is saidto besuffering from"ratio strain" when previouslyhighratesof responding disintegrate.

Otherauthors simply referto "training theratio" asextinction resulting frommaking a ratio

too high, too quickly(Whaley & Malott, 1971). Reese (1978) suggests that:

a behavioral requirement for reinforcement canbe quitehighif progress alongthe wayis indicated or if increases aregradual. However, abrupt increases in ratiosizecan leadto ratiostrain, that is, extended pausing afterreinforcement andoftena breakdown of the performance.

Page 19: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

3

Reynolds (1975) similarly suggests that, although pigeons can bebrought under the

control of highratio schedules demanding 2000 responses when an initially small ratio is

gradually incremented, strain occurs when the value of theratio is increased too rapidly resulting

inabrupt pauses innormally smooth andrapid rates of responding.

If an optimal response level is desired, Sulzer-Azaroff and Mayer (1977) recommend using

small progressive andadjusting schedules to reach a response asymptote. Theyalsosuggest

thatratio strain can be avoided byincreasing ratio requirements verygradually andit canbe

treated by a temporary reduction in the ratio requirement. Ferster andSkinner (1957) provide

empirical evidence for these suggestions from the animal laboratory. They report sustained

performance without prolonged pausing by three pigeons on large fixed ratio schedules

through the useof an adjusting schedule. Anadjusting ratio is a schedule wherein the number

of responses required for reinforcement is changed progressively in terms of some

characteristic of thebehavior of theorganism (the PRP inthis case) during the preceding ratio. If

anystraining appears, as in thiscase evidenced by long PRPs, thesizeof the ratio is

automatically andtemporarily decreased. Thus, the Importance of theprocedures used when

increasing ratio schedule requirements hasbeen acknowledged boththeoretically and

empirically, yet thispoint Isoften notacknowledged during discussions of ratiostrain. For

example, the defin~ion of strain presented earlier (pg. 1)by Catania omits thispoint asdoes the

forthcoming definition presented by Ferster, CUlbertson, andBoren.

Definitions of strain may, then, differ in emphasis or emphasize contradictory points. In

otherexamples, Ferster, Culbertson andBoren (1975) limit the phenomenon to fixed ratio (FR)

schedules, while Catania (1968) suggests that strain canoccurundervariable ratio (VR)

schedules aswell. Catania indicates thatstrain is independent of the PRP whileReese (1977)

andThompson (1964) suggest thatstrain refers to extended pausing afterreinforcement. Strain

is defined by Ferster, Culbertson, andBoren (1975) as;

A termusedto describe thedecreased frequency of a performance that occurswhen the performance is reinforced ona large fixed ratio schedule. Under mostconditions strain occurs between long periods during which the performancedoes notoccur at all and periods when there arebursts of perfomance at highrates.

Although thisdefinition suggests thatstrain refers to perfomance ofbehavior at a

decreased rate rather than the absence of behavior, the authors suggest elsewhere thatstrain

refers to "thelongperiod of nobehavior thatoccurs when the number of (responses) required

Page 20: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

4

for reinforcement is increased". In thisformulation strain is referring to an absence of behavior,

thusevenw~hin definitions, contradiction occurs.

What isclearabout ratio strain is thatthe nature of theconcept is notveryclear. This is

perhaps notvery surprising given the factthat thehistorical referent of the concept haschanged

overthe last40years. Yet. ontheotherhand, ratio strain is acknowledged asa basic concept in

thefield of behavioral psychology. It Isperhaps thisprimacy thathasledto confusion. Strain is

readily acknowledged asa general termfor rate decreases underratio schedules, thespecifics of

which areassumed to be apparent or arenotquestioned. However, upon scrutiny thegenerality

of theconcept camouflages a number of intricate andpotentially confusing relations. These may

befairly trivial, for example as in the above discrepancy between the occurrence of strain and

Iim~ation to VRancllor FRschedules. Other questions arise aswell. Therelationship of the PRP

and strain is notwellunderstood, nor is thatof strain andextinction. Does strain result from too

large of ratio sizes or from some relationship of how the ratio sizeis increased? Doother

unspecified conditions influence strain? Some confusion exists as to whether strainrefers to

behavioral disruptions or to thecomplete cessation of behavior. Some of theseissues may be

answered through theoretical analysis andyetothers need to beprimarily addressed empirically.

ThePost ReinforcementPause

The relationship of strain andthe postreinforcement pause hasbeen seento be particularly

confusing. Catania (1968) hassuggested that strain is independent of the PRP whileother

authors equate postreinforcement pausing with strain (Reese, 1977) or usethe PRP as an

indicator of strain (Thompson, 1964). Historically, post reinforcement pausing wasposited to

occur by Skinner (1938) when the reflex reserve wasin a temporary stateof strain or exhaustion

from a preceding ratio run. This would suggest that indeed, at leastInearlytheoretical

formulations, strain andthePRP areessentially analogous. This issue of similarity is not

particularly addressed in laterformulations. Skinner (1953) does suggest however thatboth in

the laboratory study of ratio reinforcement and inthepractical applications of everyday life, the

firstsignsof strain areseen in,or indicated by,thepostreinforcement pause. This relationship

canbe further clarified by exploring the nature of the PRP andresearch examining thispausing.

Ferster andSkinner (1957) describe in detail the response patterns produced by a number

of schedules of reinforcement. Final performance on a small FR willshowa high andstable rate

of responding whereas intermediate andlarge FR's will likely produce post reinforcement

pauses, growing longer asthe number of responses in the FRis increased. The length of the

Page 21: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

5

PRP may varyalthough short pauses tendto group. After pausing, responding usually abruptly

shifts to a stable terminal rate until thenextreinforcer resulting in square break andrunpatterns

on a cumulative record. Similar response patterns aregenerated by variable ratio schedules,

however the overall rate is much more consistent withpostreinforcement pausing notreadily

apparent, occurring onlyunder very high variable ratios and then perhaps in smaller magnitude.

Theincreasing relationship between PRP length and FRsize has beendocumented by a

number or researchers (Thompson, 1964; Felton & Lyon, 1966; Powell, 1968). This relationship

haslead to confusion intheanalysis of ratio strain. It seems thatif strain is a function of large ratio

sizes, andif increases in ratio sizes produce increases in post reinforcement pausing, thenthey

might beone andthesame. Indeed, the two are treated asthesame bysome researchers

(Thompson, 1964). Ontheother hand, it hasbeen argued thatthe post reinforcement pause is a

basic and stable property of certain schedules of reinforcement (Nevin & Reynolds, 1973) and as

such is r.ot related to strain which is likely to occur under conditions of instability.

The perspective of thepostreinforcement pause being abasicproperty of certain

schedules arises from a fine grain analysis of response patterns produced byschedules of

reinforcement. Anybasic schedule of reinforcement generates a distinct andreplicable

response pattern (Ferster & Skinner, 1957). The particular pattern generated by anygiven

schedule is a function of theparticular schedule requirements andof subtle within-schedule

stimulus control relations dictated bythese requirements, aswellasbythe response repertoires

andschedule histories brought to thesituation bythe responder.

An example of how these subtle relations of stimulus control influence schedule

performance canbegleaned from a comparison of Interval and ratio performance. Typical ratio

performance has been described above andingeneral ratio schedules show consistent and

highrate patterns of responding, although differences exist between patterns produced under

fixed or variable ratio schedules. Interval schedules, ontheotherhand, produce lower rates of

responding again withdistinct patterns generated by fixed or variable intervals. A scalloped

pattern, asobserved on a cumulative record, of fewresponses following reinforcement followed

by rate increases throughout theinterval is likely to be seen undera fixed Interval, while steady

patterns aregenerated by variable interval schedules without scalloping.

The differences in rate observed underinterval and ratio schedules canbe analyzed as

being related to differences in discriminative schedule control, which actto optimize certain

patterns of performance under each schedule as a result of the different requirements imposed

byeach particular schedule. That is,given a particular reinforcement schedule, a number of

Page 22: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

6

behavioral regimens mayultimately fulfill reinforcement requirements. Some of these patterns

willbe efficient in terms of maximizing reinforcement overtimeor Interms of the amount of effort

involved, etc., while others will be inefficient. Discriminative schedule control refers to the

particular response patterns thatevolve or areselected bythe specific schedule requirements.

In terms of thethree termoperant contingency. thediscriminative stimulus in this case is notany

exteroceptive stimulus complex, but rather a complex of subtle relations and requirements

imposed bythe governing schedule.

Ratio schedules produce higher response rates thaninterval schedules. When looking at

thespecific schedule requirements it becomes apparent that ratio schedules differentially

reinforce higher response rates, as interval schedules differentially reinforce more moderate

rates. That is, the frequency of reinforcement is increased undera ratio schedule when higher

rates of responding occursince higher rates fulfill the schedule requirements sooner than lower

rates. Under an interval schedule onthe otherhand the rate of responding is for the roost part

inconsequential andthe actual pattern of responding that is reinforced iswaiting for the interval

to elapse, hence lower rates of responding areselected. Schedule requirements can thusbe

seen to selectively control response patterns generated by each schedule.

Even within a particular class of schedules, such asthe interval schedules. thespecific

schedule requirements will influence response patterns. The scalloped pattem of thefixed

interval arises as a result of local within-schedule contingency requirements. That is, post

reinforcement pausing is pronounced underfixed Interval schedules since responses arenever

reinforced inthe initial temporal area of the interrelnforcement Interval. Ontheotherhand,

variable interval schedules willoccasionally provide reinforcement within a short time of the

preceding reinforcer. hence consistent. steady lowrate pattems of responding areproduced

with lessapparent postreinforcement pausing.

These within-schedule contingency requirements areessential in finegrainanalysis oi

schedule performance andin thisparticular case in the analysis of thepostreinforcement pause

as it pertains to ratio strain. Nevin and Reynolds (1973) suggest that although fixed ratio

schedules do notspecify a timebetween reinforcement and in general reinforce highrates, in

actuality, a minimum temporal interval between reinforcement is imposed bythe time required to

complete theratio, so that. as in fixed interval schedules, stimuli associated withshort times after

reinforcement arealso associated withnonreinforcement. ThatIs. responses arenever

reinforced in the initial temporal area of the interreinforcement interval. Therefore. these

temporal stimuli control lowrates. as infixed interval schedules. hence postreinforcement

Page 23: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

7

pausing. Asthe sizeof thefixed ratio increases, theduration of the period of nonreinforcement

must necessarily increase, leading to increases in pausing afterreinforcement. This analysis of

post reinforcement pausing under fixed ratio schedules suggests that it is essentially

independent of strain and reconciles data suggesting an increasing relationship between FRsize

and postreinforcement pausing as being essentially similar to those patterns of pausing selected

byfixed interval schedules. Thedistribution of ratios is important aswell (Reynolds, 1975).

When, as in thevariable ratio schedule, short and medium ratios areincluded (hence variability in

thetemporal stimuli controlling reinforcement), postreinforcement pausing is notseen.

Responses, aswell astemporal relations, maybeadditional controlling dimensions in fixed

ratio schedule control which influence post reinforcement pausing. Ferster and Peele (1980)

suggest that postreinforcement pausing by pigeons under fixedratio schedules is influenced

bythe discriminative control of the number of pecks since reinforcement. Theycompared

alternating FRperformance withand without an added counter (counting pecks) to test this

notion. Reliable increases in postreinforcement pauses were witnessed with the addition of

thecounter, leading the authors to suggest thatthe added counter increases past

reinforcement pausing because of an increased discriminative effect. That is, PRPs may result

from thefact that reinforcement is never contingent on initial FR responding andthis relation

becomes more precise when anexteroceptive monitor ispresent.

A number of factors in addition to FR size have been shown to influence the PRP including

delayed reinforcement (Morgan, 1972); level of deprivation (Sidman & Stebbins, 1954); the

force requirement of the response lever (Azrin, 1959; Notterman & Mintz, 1965); theprobability

of reinforcement (McMillan, 1971); andreinforcement magnitude (Powell, 1969; Priddle-Higson,

Lowe & Harzem, 1976). Thepostreinforcement pause has, as is evident, been extensively

examined by a number of investigators largely instudies notexamining strain. Lawful relations

have beenshown by these investigators at ratio values notproducing strain. Capehart,

Eckerman, Guilkey andShull (1980), instudying thesimilarity anddissimilarity of PRPs infixed

interval andfixed ratio schedules, sU~-Jest thatthe PRP is unlikely to be dueto ratio strain or

some otherinstability in ratio performance at high values since a decreasing linear function exists

underFRschedules relating theproportion of pausing to timesince reinforcement andthis

function isat least asapparent atshorter FR values asat large FRvalues.

Thisresearch suggests then thatthepostreinforcement pause is a distinct and separate

phenomena from strain. ThePRP is infuenced by a number of varying conditions. It is possible

thatsome of the conditions influencing thepostreinforcement pause mayaswellbe influential

Page 24: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

8

in thedevelopment of ratio strain. If thisis thecase it may be appropriate to suggest, asSkinner

does, that the first signs of strain appear in the post reinforcement pause. That is, although the

PRP is a separate phenomena, the PRP may serve asan indicator of a class of controlling

conditions that may potentially lead to strain. However, this relationship cannotbe adequately

explained until theconditions defining ratio strain aredelineated.

The Ratio Strain Literature

Little research is available specifically exploring the nature of strain or theconditions

responsible for itsdevelopment. Ratio strain hasin various usages referred to the actual

conditions producing response decrement under thecontrol of ratio reinforcement schedules,

as in "straining a ratio" (Ferster &Skinner, 1957); to those behavioral changes resulting from

certain ratio schedule conditions (Catania, 1968); andhistorically at least to thetemporary

depletion of a response reserve (Skinner, 1938). Ratio strain, while notsystematically

examined, hasbeen reported in a number of research investigations examining a variety of

diverse phenomena andbehavioral patterns (Morse & Hermsteln, 1956; Ferster & Skinner,

1957; Dews, 1958; Green, Sanders & Squire, 1959; Ferster, 1960; Hutchinson & Azrin, 1961;

Ferster, Appel & Hiss, 1962; Thompson, 1964; Findley & Brady, 1965; Weissman &

Crossmann, 1966; McMillian, 1969; Laursen, 1972; Bryd, 1973; Dardano, 1973, 1974; Barnett,

1976; Boren, Moershbaecher & Whyte, 1978; Thompson & Moershbaecher, 1980; Wood,

Happ & Adams, 1983). The majority of these investigators report or inferstrain asoccurring

uponchanges in previously stable rates of responding underthe control of ratio schedules or

upona temporary or prolonged cessation of responding andmost experimental investigators

seem content using the term primarily asa descriptor of lowered response rate. Theincidental

nature of many of these reports makes it difficult to precisely assess specific variables

occasioning the occurrence of strain.

Strain, although again incidently, hasbeen perhaps more rigorously examined in a number

of studies exploring progr6ssive ratio performance (Dardano, 1968; Hodos & Trumble, 1967;

Hodos & Kallman, 1963; Thomas, 1974; Dardano & Sauerbrunn, 1966). Under a progressive

ratio (PR) schedule, the number of responses required in successive ratios increases bya fixed

increment. In general, andalthough notexplicitly addressed, these studies usually inferstrain

asoccurring at the "breaking point", or thatpoint in the increasing series of schedules where

responding "breaks" or stops. Thebreaking point is empirically defined by Individual

researchers as thefirstperiod of X-minutes (usually somewhere in the range of 3-15) where

Page 25: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

9

responding is absent. Thefocus of research utilizing PR ratio schedules has been directed at a

number of factors influencing progressive ratio performance or preference forprogressive ratio

versus concurrent alternatives undervarying conditions. Forexample, Thomas (1974) reports

that increased pressures of nitrogen, butnothelium, ina hyperbaric chamber lead to both

increases anddecreases interminal ratio size withthedifferential effects depending on both air

pressure andon the sizeof theprogressive ratio increment.

The sizeof the increasing series of ratios is important inprogressive ratio performance.

Hodos andKallman (1963) and Thomas (1974) each report the sizeof the PR increment to

affect the occurrence of the breaking point, with smaller increment sizes leading to greater

numbers of completed ratios priorto strain. Even though gleaned from the study of progressive

ratio performance, these results areconsistent withthose views of ratio strain positing the

importance of the regimen of schedule increases inthedevelopment or prevention of strain.

Hodos andTrumble (1967) report thatthe likelihood of engaging in a concurrent alternative

which resets the PR value increases astheprogressive ratio increases, yet isalso a function of

the number of responses required under thealternative priorto each reset. The likelihood of

responding to.thePRor engaging in thereset alternative Is alsoa function of shock intensity,

when responses to the alternative produce electric shock (Dardono & Sauerbrunn, 1966;

Dardono, 1968). Thus, Insummary, performance and the occurrence of strain under

progressive ratio schedules is influenced bythe increment sizeof the PR, whether an

opportunity exists to reset the PR, aswellas by theconsequences involved in responding to

thisalternative, andby theresponse costinvolved in responding to the ahemative in terms of

frequency requirements.

A few instances of ratio strain have been reported withhuman responders. Hutchinson

and Azrin (1961) conditioned mental patients on a FR 300knob-pulling schedule of

reinforcement without performance disruption. However, performance wasdisrupted in

instances where ratios were increased to values much greater than those previously

programmed. Contrary to theanimal literature, nosystematic increases in postreinforcement

pausing occurred asa function of increasing ratio sizepriorto disruption. This lack of post

reinforcement pausing in humans is a finding consistent with results reported by Holland (1958),

ina study of normal responders, andEllis, Barnett, and Pryer (1960), whoexamined patients

response patterns. Sanders (1969) investigated human schedule performance andfound no

evidence of PRP or strain, even at FR1000, leading the authorand others (Weiner, 1966) to

suggest thatcharacteristic animal response patterns may notoccurinhumans. Thisdiscrepancy

Page 26: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

10

mayhowever be a function of the procedures used, as Azrin (1958) produced PRPs under fixed

interval schedules by increasing theweight required to close a response keyfrom 15gm. to 300

gm.afterPRPs were notseen with the lighter response effort.

A study by Staats, Finley, Osborne, Quinn and Minke (1963) examining complex reading

behavior suggests thattheoperant selected for analysis caninfluence theschedule parameters

sustaining performance. Theauthors examined reading acquisiton under a fixed ratio schedule

of reinforcment andfound ratio strain of response acquisition by a 4-year-old child when the FR

schedule was raised from FR2to FR 4, leading theauthors to suggest thatthe response effort

involved in the complex verbal, motor and discriminative response prevented schedule

maintenance. Following thistheauthors implemented a procedural change and examined

acquisition undera chained FRCRF schedule, consisting of alternating FR and CRF

components. "In the CRF component fourone-response schedule completions were allowed

and reinforced before a return to the FR component. Reinforcement during the FRcomponent

was limited to access to theensuing CRF series. Under thisarrangement responding was

maintained undertheFR4, and schedule Increases thereafter occurred untilstrain wasseen at

FR 7, upon which time theexperiment was terminated.

Theauthors conclude thatthechained schedule produced general changes in rate

analogous to those observed with more simple behaviors of humans andInfrahumans, and that

theprocedure wasvaluable in thesystematic study of Intermittent reinforcement andthe

complex repertoire of reading. Additionally, theauthors suggest thatthe FR component could

likely be increased to even higher ratios through appropriate manipulation of the CRF

component. Thus procedural arrangements and details of response requirements have been

seen to influence theoccurrence of strain withhuman responders.

Ona theoretical basis littlework has been undertaken specifically developing or examining

the nature of ratio strain. Hursh (1980) ina theoretical article arguing therelevance of economic

principles to the experimental analysis of behavior, suggests thattheavailability of reinforcement

for otherthanthe targeted response will influence thepersistence of a targeted response under

increasing schedule requirements. Specifically, Hursh proposes thattheeconomic concept of

open vs.closed economy Is important in understanding ratioperformance and strain. A closed

economy, in research examining appetitive performance, results when daily consumption

occurs onlyas a resu~ of thesubjects interaction withthesupply (or reinforcement) schedules.

Bycontrast, anopen economy isanyof a variety of experimental arrangements providing a

measure of independence between daily consumption and the experimental contingencies, for

Page 27: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

11

example, when free feeding episodes follow experimental sessions. Hursh reviews and

compares dataof FRresponding under open (Felton & Lyon, 1966) andclosed (Collier, Hirsh, &

Hamlin, 1972) experimental economies, concluding that economic contingencies of

consumption strongly influence performance disruption and persistence with performance

being sustained underhigher ratios when the behavioral economy is closed. Goldiamond

(1975, 1984) similarly hasargued thatanythorough understanding of behavior must necessarily

examine notonly target behaviors and thecontingency relations influencing these but

alternative contingency relations and behaviors as theyrelate to the target or referent behavior.

In othertheoretical work, Rachlin (1978) andPear (1975) have independently proposed

theoretical quantitative formulae predicting points upon which stable schedule performance may

be disrupted, however, thiswork has notseen empirical extension. It is interesting to note that

Rachlin, in presenting his theory, makes the assumption thathighrates of responding under

ratio schedules are slightly punishing. Thisassumption is necessary inorderfor hypothesized

mathematical functions to match plotted datafrom existing experimental results. This view, that

high rates of responding are aversive andthatconditions producing ratio strain must be

aversive, is pervasive inthe experimental literature andin theoperant clinical nomenclature.

That is, ona theoretical level, strain asa behavioral phenomena is attributed to have anaversive

emotional component. In large, thisview results from a series of research investigations

examining ratio performance and time out responding. A detailed review of thisseries of articles

suggests thatperhaps thisassumption is unwarranted.

Time Out Performance

Azrin (1961) in naming the research area inanarticle entitled, Time OutfromPositive

Reinforcement suggests that "performance under a schedule of positive reinforcement may at

certain stages be aversive in spite of theapparent absence of aversive stimuli". This claim is

based on evidence of pigeons escaping fromconditions associated witha schedule of positive

reinforcement by initiating and spending increasing amounts of timein timeout as a function of

ratio sizewhen a fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement wassystematically increased andwhen

the opportunity to engage intime outwas available. Under thisexperimental arrangement, a

timeout produced a change in theambient chamber lightwhile interrupting the contingency

relanon to the main response lever such that noreinforcement wasavailable during thisperiod.

Indeed, oversessions, responding to the main keyduring a time outwas seento dropto zero.

Theduration of time spent engaging in timeoutwas reported to increase with increasing ratio

Page 28: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

12

sizes to the point that at FR200approximately 1/2of a 60 minute session might bespent in time

out,whereas onlya fewseconds were spent in timeout atFR 65.

Pigeons controlled the length of the timeout by initiating both the time out anda second

response to thesamekey returning the experimental conditions to "time in"andthere':>y

occasioning reinforcement under the referent fixedratio contingency. Azrln examined

performance of fourpigeons responding underan increasing series of fixed ratiovalues ranging

fromFR65to FR200andreported no procedural reversals. Thetimeouts wereseen to occur

eitherlate in the post reinforcement pause or early during the ratio run leading the author to

suggest that the subjects appeared to initiate timeouts justbefore engaging in the number of

responses required by the schedule.

A systematic replication of these results wasprovided by Thompson (1965) who, in a slight

procedural variation, examined theeffects of the imposition of a 30 sec. fixedduration timeout

onperformance by two pige~ns under ratio schedule requirements. A similar increasing

monotonic function between incidence of timeout andFR sizewas obtained. Thompson

reported frequency rather thanduration measures of timeout responding when examining

response patterns to the timeoutkeyunderan increasing series of main keyFRvalues. An

abrupt reversal of FRschedule value fromthe highest ratio examined to the smallest was

undertaken following the increasing series anda decrease in timeout responding wasseen,

returning to a levelcommensurate withthatof the initial lower presentation. Thompson also

observed timeoutsto occurduring the PRPandsuggested that the PRPwasthe mostaversive

partof the FRschedule as thatwaswhere time outstypically occurred.

Theoccurrence of timeoutswithin the PRP is well documented in a second studyby

Thompson (1964) examining performance of ratsunderincreasing FRrequirements giventhe

opportunity to engage in a 30 sec. fixed duration timeout. Thompson presents datafor four

animals suggesting thatthe frequency of timeout responding increases concomitantly as PRPs

increase underincreasing FRschedules. Time outsoccurred during the PRPs. Also whena

fifth animal wasplaced undera Mixed FR25 FR225schedule. timeouts were displaced at least

25 responses after reinforcement untilthat point where ratio discriminabi!ity produced pausing

(under such a schedule, noexteroceptive stimulus is present to signal theparticular schedule

component in effect andverybriefPRPs areseen, butextended pauses arelikelyto followan

initial burst of responses which meet the lower ratio value). The results of thefirst experiment

incorporating the foursubjects suggest thatwhen looking at timeout as a function of FRsize,

timeouts increased with increasing FRs. Twoascending and descending series of FR

Page 29: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

13

schedules were examined and reversal of effect wasobserved at each descending FRvalue

providing a comprehensive replication of the time outfromreinforcement phenomena.

Several othermanipulations, each wnh asingle animal, were additionally undertaken by

Thompson (1964). Extinction of a contingency of continuous reinforcement wasseento

produce an initial substantial increase Intimeout responding, even though timeout responding

wasalso represented underCRF. Another experiment examined the effects of increasing the

number of responses required to produce a timeouton the frequency of timeout responding

undera large FA. Time outswere observed to progressively decrease asthe number of

responses required to produce them were increased. At thehighest timeout response

requirement of FR27,notimeoutswere engaged in. Thompson reports thatoccasionally the

animals responded to the main response lever during a timeout. Thisandthe occurrence of

timeout during CRFseems to call intoquestion the j:iurported aversive nature of the ratio

response requirements. However, the three studies citedstrongly support the notion of

increasing response patterns to a timeoutkeyunderconditions of increasing response

requirements to a referent key.

Thetimeout from reinforcement relationship was further explored by Appel (1963) in a

similarexperiment withpigeons specifically examining the effects of stimulus change, alone and

in conjunction withthe timeoutcontingency, on responses to the second (time out)key.Azrin

(1961) reported that it wasthecontingency precluding reinforcement during timeout rather than

thesimultaneous stimulus change of the ambient chamber lightinvolved in the occurrence of

timeout thatwasthefactorresponsible for the observed patterns. Thisconclusion is based, in

part, on an explicit examination of thechanges inthe ambient chamber light. Azrin reported that

thechange in lightwas not maintaining the responses sincethetimeoutswereengaged in,

"regardless of whether an increase or decrease in illumination wasassociated withthe timeout".

However, hedid not examine the incidence of responses to thiskeywhen onlya change in the

ambient lightoccurred with nochange in thecontingency condition on themain response lever,

as examined by Appel.

Theworkof Appel replicates thefindings reported by Azrin wnh regard to the increasing

incidence of self-controlled timeoutsat higher ratio values, particularly when response

frequency rather thanduration measures were examined. However, a similar Increasing

response pattern wasalsoseen when onlythestimulus change component of the timeout

"package" was included. In fact, theduration of timespent engaging in thecondnion of only

stimulus change wasreported to increase as anexponential function of thefixed ratio size when

Page 30: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

14

reinforcement wasavailable und6:" each .>1imulus condition. that is.when noextinction

c.ontingency waspresent during ,flestimulus change. This resulted in a substantially greater

duration of timespent underthestimulus change condition when contrasted tc thetimeout

condition at the highest FRexamined. In addition to measures of duration. Appel reported

incidence of response frequency to the second keyandthis index showed similar increasing

patterns of response frequency under each of these twoconditions. asthe referent ratio

contingency was increased. The substantial difference in duration noted at the highest FRwas

not evident when patterns were examined in terms of frequency andin general. from a

frequency orientation, a slightly higher incidence of responding to thesecond keyoccurred

when thetimeoutcontingency was included with the stimulus change.

Incomparing the results of time outduration reported by Appel withthe earlier results

presented by Azrin, a decreased amount of timecanbeobserved to bespent in time outat the

higher schedule values. Forexample. whereas Azrin had reported a 50%incidence of time out

duration at FR200. Appel reports a 10%incidence at FR210. A number of interesting

similarities are reported by Appel. The time outs thatwere engaged inwere likely to occurduring

the PRP or early in theratio run. Also. the birds were notlikely to engage insustained main key

responding during a timeout. OCCasionally. particularly priorto stability of performance. thebirds

would. however. runoff ratios during thetime out. Inaddition to thetimeout and onlystimulus

change conditions examined by Appel. responses to thesecond keywere alsoobserved

during a thirdcondition underwhich responses onthe second key had noprogrammed

consequences andweresimply recorded. Substantially fewer responses were reported to the

keyunderthis arrangement when contrasted to the twopreviously reported conditions.

supporting thecontention of stimulus change as a condition potentiating response patterns.

however, a slightincreasing pattern underthis condition wasalsoobserved across the

increasirlQ FRs.

Theresults reported by Appel. then. both support and replicate the earlier research of Azrin

andyetsuggest thatstimulus change alone may besufficient to maintain the increasing

incidence of responses to a second keyunder increasing schedule parameters. Appel does

concur with the suggestion of Azrin andothers that it is theaversive nature of theoriginal

schedule that is responsible for the increased frequency of behaviors which function to change

the existing stimulus conditions to ones notassociated with that (aversive) schedule. This

hypothesis is called intoquestion by the large number of variables which seem to influence the

occurrence of timeout responding (Zimmerman & Ferster. 1964).

Page 31: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

15

Zimmerman and Ferster examined time out responding undera number of experimental

conditions and reported systematic relations between responding to a time outkey and the

controlling conditions of a referent schedule. However, the reported resuhs and relations were

substantially different from those previously reported. Theauthors suggest that some of these

differences maybe dueto a number of procedural variations in the respective studies. Notably,

Zimmerman andFerster report no monotonic relation between timeoutresponding and FRsize.

Instead theyreport timeout responding to bevariable andyet influenced bya number of

conditions. Theprocedures specific to thetime outwereseento be important. Results most

similar to those reported by Azrin (1961), Thompson (1964, 1965) andAppel (1963) above

were obtained when pigeons could initiate both thetimeoutandterminate the time outat any

timewitha second time in response. Very little timeout responding occurred whenthetimeout

wasof a fixed duration and which further required an absence of responding to anykeyduring

the time-down period inwhich thefixedtimeoutduration was elapsing. Thefollowing results

reported by Zimmerman andFerster weregenerally specific to only theself-controlled rather

thanfixed timeoutcondition.

As reported, Zimmerman andFerster didnotobserve an increasing overall incidence of time

outasthe main key FRwasincreased, rather theywitnessed an increased Incidence of timeout

during thefirstor second session following a schedule increase. This change wastransitory and

was reported to decrease by about thefourth session following a change. Furthermore, the

frequency of timeout responding wasseen to decrease markedly following a schedule change

to a smaller rather thanlarger FA. Time outs were observed to occurduring the PRP, however,

these were generally verybriefandresponses to thetimeout keytended to occurin even

numbered groups of 2 or 4 responses, etc. These response bursts onthetimeout keyresuhed

in time in conditions being reintroduced immediately or shortly afteranoddnumbered groupof

responses. Thus, the duration of timespent engaging in timeoutwasgenerally minimal. An

early and markedly increased incidence of time out responding wasobserved underconditions

of extinction, however a complete decline inthese responses occurred asthe main key

response patterns became weak. Theauthors pointout thata high preponderance of timeout

responding might be expected to occur during these later~Iriods of extinction if timeout

responding was indeed a function of aversive schedule properties, yet thiswasnotthe case.

Theincidence of timeout responding wasalso affected by the degree of stimulus change

occurring as result of the time out response. Zimmerman andFerster assessed the effects of

three increasing amounts of stimulus change following a time out response at a number of

Page 32: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

16

different referent FRvalues. Theyfound thatgreater stimulus change occasioned greater

incidence of timeout responding as measured by frequency, duration, andthe percentage of

sessions during which timeoutsoccurred. Stimulus change did not necessarily haveto be

linked to the timeout response to occasion an increase. Timeout responding wasseento

increase when a novel stimulus light wassimply introduced on themainresponse key.

The results reported by Zimmerman andFerster suggest thatthe phenomena of timeout

responding maybe considerably more complex than the escape froman aversive situation

implied by thetimeout from positive reinforcement literature. Theauthors propose that time cut

responding occurs as a relatively weakalternative behavior thatbecomes predominant under

certain conditions of instability in the referent main keycontingency. Theconditions

occasioning temporary lossof control by theschedule of foodreinforcement include thensuch

events asschedule changes resulting in reinforcement decrement, novel stimulus change, and

presumably others.

The aversive emotional component of large ratio schedules as incorporated in some

theoretical notions of ratio strain is thencalled intoquestion by results reported in several of the

articles reviewed in thisseries. The relationship of timeout responding, ratio schedules, and

strain is Iikowise notwellunderstood or documented.

Conclusions

Systematic attempts have notbeen undertaken to quantify thebehavioral patterns referred

to asstrained andas a result of this, a variety of measurement indices havebeen used. Strain in

the sense of performance cessation hasbeen examined by arbitrarily selected breaking points

in the study of progressive ratio performanca (Hodos & Kalman, 1963). Weissman and

Crossman (1966) have assumed straining to be indicated by decreases in response rate and

propose that a quantitative indexmightbederived by measuring changes in interreinforcement

response rate excluding reinforcement time, however nocriterion otherthan a decrease from a

previous level for selecting strained performance is suggested. Pedan andTimberlake (1984) in

suggesting that reinforcement magnitude will influence theoccurrence of strainreview

traditional measures of ratio strain as (a)themaximum number of keypecks or maximum rate of

keypecking on a givenschedule, (b) thecessation of keypecking, (c) the return to the operant

levelof keypecking, and(d)a disruption in the pattern of keypecking in which theduration of

pauses increase. Theauthors alsopropose several novel measures of strainspecific to their

economic analysis (ala, Hursh, 1980) ofclosed economy keypecking bypigeons including

Page 33: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

17

(a) changes inbody weight (b) changes in thesize andfrequency of individual meals, and (c) the

maximum price paid in the sense of a ratio relating pecks or cost to reinforcement or gain. It is not

surprising giventhisdiverse array of measurement indices andlack of consensus inthe

quantification of strain that behavioral andterminological inconsistencies abound asobserved

earlier when reviewing definitions and conceptualizations of strain.

In summary, it is suggested thatratio strain ascommonly used refers to thebehavioral

effects of a specific group of operant procedures, whereby ongoing performance decreases in

frequency underfixed or variable ratio schedules of reinforcement. Theparticulars of thisclass

of decremental operations have notbeen well idflntified, except perhaps through exclusion.

Forexample, rate decreases occurring under ratio schedules arenotlikely to becalled ratio

strain if those decreases areoccurring during extinction or following a pre-aversive stimulus in

research utilizing a condnioned emotional response (CER) paradigm (Brady & Hunt, 1955). It is

alsosuggested thatratio strain is exclusive of the PRP and does notrefer to decreases in

response probability which occur asthe PRP increases, nordoes strain referto physical fatigue.

Onthe otherhand, evidence from a number of areas certainly suggests thattheseries of

schedule increases used when increasing a ratio requirement iscritical asanoccasioning

condltlon of ratiostrain. That is, experimental investigators arelikely toclassify rate decreases as

strain following anabrupt increase ~n theschedule parameter.

Specific operations, then, which have been suggested to be influential in thedevelopment

of straininclude theabrupt increase of schedule requirements or, in general, too large of

schedule requirements. Ratio strain is often directly attributed or inferred to occurasa result of

too large of ratios, however these reports, asreviewed, are largely anecdotal by investigators not

specifically addressing performance disruption. Research reviewed also suggests thatthe

specifics involved in the constitution of "toolarge of schedule requirements" canvary

considerably, aswell. Theultimate value sustaining schedule performance is inpartdependent

onthe program of schedule increases, suggesting thatultimate ratio size is in some cases

redundant as a controlling condnion of ratio strain or is at least a function of theprocedures used

during the schedule increase regimen. Staats, et al. (1963) suggest thatthebehavior selected

for analysis is critical indefining schedule requirements thatare"too large". Other factors canbe

influential aswell. Hursh (1980) and Goldiamond (1975) suggest thatthe availability of

reinforcement for otherthanthetargeted behavior will influence thepersistance of that

behavior. It is suggested that thecontrolling conditions of ratlo strain or schedule disruption are

notadequately portrayed simply bytheacknowledgement of large ratio sizes, and infact, the

Page 34: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

18

assessment or attribution of ratio strain in these cases oftenprecludes a thorough functional

analysis examining and delineating specific causes of response breakdown.

Thissituation is, to a large extent, a result of thefact that for the most partsystematic

research examining the behavioral occurrences referred to as strain andtheconditions

producing these has notoccurred. Rather, ratio strainhas entered into thevocabulary of the

behavior analysis community without thebenefit of substantive experimental verification or

examination. The term hascome to beused bythiscommunity asa metaphorical descriptor of

response rate decreases (used onlyundercertain conditons), onewhose historical referent has

undergone considerable evolution andonewhose basic primacy has been reified through

convention. As a result, bothterminological andbehavioral confusion exists asto the

constitution of ratio strain.

Thefollowing research attempts to examine the phenomena of ratio strain and delineate a

number of procedural conditions underwhich performance disruption or breakdown occurs with

human responders. Although nota comprehensive buta preliminary analysis of performance

decrement underratio schedules, the three ensuing experiments attempt to explore issues

arising from the preceding review. Inparticular this research attempts toclarify and define the

nature of ratio strain by undertaking a series of systematic research investigations, as noted,

heretofore lacking. The specifics of each ofthese threeexperiments willbe described in detail,

butgenerally the purposes of thisprogram of inquiry are to; 1) notonlyelucidate the concept of

ratio strain by defining conditions underwhich it occurs, butto also; 2)further examine ratio

responding andtimeout initiation, andanyrelationship of strain andtime out responding, and;

3) usehuman responders. Theutilization ofhuman subjects in the examination of ratio

performance is enlightening notonly in terms of discussions of schedule performance and

across species generality of findings, but also in terms of thefactthat ratio strain ispostulated to

beof inf!uenc.e fin areas of human ciinical concern, notably depression (Ferster, 1973). This

research specifically examines a variety of environmental situations conducive to response

decrement. Although ratio strain, perse, is a thematic issue guiding the ensuing program of

investigation, it is notsuggested or intended for these threeexperiments to provide definitive

conceptual boundaries to ratio strain. Rather, conditions of response decrement might be

considered the topic of current attention. Ideally this broad based approach will shed lighton

those iII-defined behavior-environment relations traditionally referred to as ratio strain.

Page 35: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

19

EXPERIMENT I

Although it is often reported thatratio strain is a function ofsome terminal ratio size incapable

of sustaining performance (Catania, 1968; Ferster, Culbertson, & Boren, 1975), others suggest

that strain results fromthe methods inwhich the ratio schedule is increased (Reynolds, 1975;

Reese,1978). Thispoint is supported as well, by research exploring progressive ratio

performance where the sizeof the increasing series of ratios affects the occurrence of response

cessation (Hodos & Kallman, 1963; Thomas, 1974).

Thisexperiment explicitly addresses this issue by examining performance under large ratio

requirements following differential schedule increase regimens across foursubjects. A

hypothesis based on findings fromtheareaof progressive ratio performance predicts that the

critical feature of strained performance in thisexperiment will bethe step-size of the increasing

ratios rather thanthe ultimate ratio value attained. Thus, thefocus of thisexperiment is to

examine theeffects of schedule increases on deterioration in performance, particularly response

breakdown as a function of the different sizedvalues of s~hedule increment, or otherwise to

establish whether ratio strain is indifferent to theseries of schedule increases andmore likely to

be a function of some ultimate value. Ingeneral, this is effected by establishing a stable level of

performance on identical schedules of reinforcement for eachsublect andthen increasing the

requirements for reinforcement to high levels bydifferentially increasing regimens across

subjects andobserving anydifferential effects on schedule performance.

METHOD

SUbjects

Foursubjects were recruited from an advertisement appearing in the bi-weekly student

newspaper of a major midwest university. Theadvertisement read:

Paid subjects (min. wage) wanted for behavioral research. Must beavailable5-7daysperweek(5-6 weeks) for oneandone-half hours each day. Schedulingflexible.

Subjects were excluded from participation if initial questioning revealed thattheyhadmore

thanan introductory levelknowledge of behavioral or experimental psychology. Thefollowing

instructions were givento potential subjects upon their inquiry:

Page 36: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

20

We are interested in looking at howpeople perform in research taskssimilar to those studied in animal laboratories with rats andpigeons. Wearelooking to seewhatthesimilarities areand whatthedifferences are.Consequ6iiUy, the experimental taskthatyouwillbe doing is analagous, youwill sit inasmall room with a panel in front ofyouthathaslights and buttonsandpoint counters. You willtry andeampoints which canbe exchanged formoney. Unfortunately. I can't go into detail asto exactly what you willbedoing because it might influence the results. Thebestway to getan idea isto actually come in and dothetaskfor a 30-minute period, andafterthattimeyou can decide if you want to continue or not. There will be noelectricshocks or anything harmful andyou can leave theexperimental room atanytime so don't worry.

Although youwill be required to come in each scheduled dayfor oneandone-half hours theexperimental sessions areonly30 minutes in length.Each daywewill runtwosessions. thefirst andlast1/2hours thatyou arehere. Between thesessions youcan relax in a waiting room soyoumightbring a book or some work to doduring thistime. You will bepaid onlyforthetimeactually spent in the30 minute experimental sessions. You will earn a$1.00 base payfor each 1/2hour(or$2.00 an hour) simply for sitting in theexperimental room. You do not have to do anything to eamthis;however, ifyou leave the room priorto the endof thesession, you will forfeit this $1.00for that session. Additionally, undersome conditions of the experiment youmaybe able to eammore money by interacting with the equipment andearning points. Each point you eamis worth 1 centso If youeam100pointsyou have eamed $1.00. This is an experiment andthings mayvary, but ingeneral, yourbase payand bonus paymight average out to about minimumwage.

If aftersitting through the firstsession you elect to continue theexperiment. you will be expected to enter intoa contractual arrangement withusto come in and participate Ina minimum number of sessions overa 4-7weekperiod. You are free to dropoutof the experiment at anytime andyouwill be paid thebase rate of $2.00 an hour. However, because thedatawecollect willbe most valuable to us onlyat the endof the experiment, youmustattend theminimum agreed upon number of sessions in orderto collect theadditional bonus point money. You willbe paidat theendof the experiment.

Relevant characteristics of each of the foursubjects. herein identified by initials only. areas

follows:

AR Subject AR wasa 45-year-old black male currently working asan intake

coordinator for a sheltered workshop. AR hasa high school education and was

currently taking part-time undergraduate coursework. Subject AR participated in

47 sessions overa seven weekperiod collecting 4315 bonus points for a total

earning of $90.15, and was thuspaid$3.84 per hour.

Page 37: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

21

IN Subject IN wasa 21-year-old Caucasion female andcurrently a third year

undergraduate behavioral sciences student. Although IN hadtakenseveral

behavioral sciences courses. she hadlittleknowledge of behavioral psychology

andhadnocoursework inthis area or in experimental psychology. During the

course of the experiment IN worked consecutively part-time asa waitress andan

office assistant. Subject IN participated in 48 sessions overa sixweekperiod

collecting 1620 bonus points fora totalearning of $64.20 andwasactually paid

$80.40 orthe current minimum wage of $3.35 per hour.

PW Subject PWwasa 20-year-old Caucasion female andcurrently a second year

undergraduate chemistry student. During the course of the experiment PWwas

also working asa library assistant Subject PWparticipated in37 sessions overa

sixweekperiod collecting 1965 bonus points fora total earning of $56.65 and

wasactually paid$62.00 or thecurrent minimum wage of $3.35perhour.

HN Subject HNwasa 29-year-old black maleforeign graduate student fonn

Swaziland. Africa andhad livedin the United States for eightmonths priorto

being a subject. HNwasnearing completion of coursework for the M.A. degree at

a Theological Seminary atthe timeof participation. Subject HNparticipated in20

sessions overa twoweek period collecting 2395bonus points for a total earning

of $43.95. andwasthuspaid$4.40 per hour.

Apparatus

Experimental contingencies were controlled by a Rockwell International AIM-65

Microcomputer. A Gerbrands cumulative recorder was used to collectcumulative records of

schedule performance. Thisequipment was located adjacent to a small room 6 feet x 4 feetwide

x 8 feettall housing theexperimental apparatus. A one-way mirror allowed experimenter

observation andvideotaping of subjects. while white noise masked extraneous sounds. during

sessions. However, the cumulative recorder could be heard recording responses, albeit very

faintly. from inside the experimental room.

Page 38: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

22

I 0225 I

o I stop I

0)o I Go I

Figure 1. The experimental response apparatus. The consummatory/extra responselever is centered between the main response lever on the right and the time outlever on the left. Specific relationships between each of the response levers.stimulus lights. and the positive point counter are detailed in the text.

Page 39: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

23

During each session subjects saton awooden chair facing the experimental apparatus or

response panel, and adiagram of this apparatus canbeseen in Figure 1. Thisapparatus

consisted of awooden box 31 em. wide by36 em. deep wUh three response levers mounted 9

em. apart and centered across thefront. Thecenter lever was extended 2 em. from theoutside

two and was2 em. from thefront of theapparatus. Each lever was actually a standard adjustable

telegraph keyof thetype commercially available at Radio Shack.

Ontheapparatus a wooden housing covered all of theadjustable mechanisms andwiring

connections of these three keys so thatonlytheresponse buttons themselves were accessible

to thesubjects. Only the right-side keywasconnected to thecumulative recorder. The

response panel satona table 70 em. high such thatthelevers were easily accessible to the

subjects.

The experimental apparatus was additionally 31 em. inheight anda number ofcolored lights

andaccessories were mounted on a vertical plane located behind the response levers and set22

em. back fromthe apparatus front. Onthefar left-hand side a vertical string ofthree lights were

centered 4 em. from the panel's leftedge. These lights were colored red, yellow and green from

top to bottom, respectively. The upper redlightwas labeled "stop", while the lowergreen was

labeled "go". A horizontal string of three yellow lights were located 6 em. from thetopand16em.

fromthelefthand edge ofthe apparatus. All yellow lights were unlabeled. Each light in a string

was located 2.5em. from theonenext to U. These commercially available strings are made by

Coulboum Instruments and arecomponent modules designed foruse in animal experiment

chambers.

Two pointcounters, of which only onewas used during the experiment, were located 1 em.

below the horizontal string of yellow lights. The left-most counter was labeled "Pas. Points",

while the right-hand counter wasnot labeled, remained inoperable, andsetto zero during the

experiment. These counters were made by Sodeco Instruments. Centered below these two

counters wasa3 em. indiameter circular P.R. Mallory and Co.Sonalert. Mhoughthis was

capable of emitting a tone, Uwas notused during thisexperiment.

Theexperimental apparatus, supporting table, andchairwere freestanding and could be

moved andarranged within the experimental room bythe subject. Some IimUation was imposed

onthis movement bythree .6emelectrical cables running from the back of the apparatus to the

outside equipment. However, in general, veryIntle rearrangement of equipment occurred by any

of thesubjects.

Page 40: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

24

Procedure

All foursubjects went through an initial training procedure andwere then placed on a Fixed

Ratio 250baseline schedule of reinforcement. Aswill be described thisbaseline included a

response initiated option to engage in a timeoutat anytime and for anyduration of thesublect's

choosing. During a time out allexperimental apparatus lights wentout until a second timeout

response (designated the time in response) occurred. Time outandtimein responses were

arranged to occur on the left hand of the three response levers, although thiskeywas

inconsequential during the ·raining portion of the experiment. During andfollowing training all

responses toward schedule completion were required to occuronthe right hand response lever,

hereafter designated as the main response key. Thekeyforce required to close the contacts of

thiskey. located .15em. apart. wasadjusted to 115gms. during this experiment.

At thecompletion of a schedule requirement on themain response keythe fouryellow

apparatus lights were illuminated signalling theavailability of reinforcement, which (throughout

the entire series of experiments) consisted of a 5 point increment to thetotaldisplayed on the

"Pas. Point" counter. When theyellow lights were on. a separate "consummatory" response to

the centerof thethree keysactually collected thepoints by incrementing thecounter. afterwhich

the yellow lights went off leaving onlythe green "Go" light illuminated. Thegreen "Go" lightwas

illuminated constantly during each session inprogress, except during a time out. Aftercollection

of the points another schedule completion could be undertaken. Thus. thethreeresponse

levers included a main response keyto the right. a timeoutleverto the left. anda center

consumatory response key. Responses to thecenterkeyat times otherthanwhen theyellow

lights wereon were inconsequential. however. aswill beseen later. these responses were

recorded bythe control equipment.

Training. Prior to the first session of training each subject was escorted intothe

experimental room and wasseated in frontof the response panel while theexperimenter directly

repeated or paraphrased the information initially provided upon recruitment (reported above on

page 20). During thistimethe red light on theapparatus marked "Stop" was illuminated. The

subject was instructed thatthis light would beon before and aftereach session andthatwhen the

green "Go"lightcame on the session wasbeginning. Noother information wasprovided asto

whatthe experimental taskconsisted of. The subjects were toldthatafterthe session andwhen

the red lightcame backon theyshould wait in the experimental booth untilthe experimenter

Page 41: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

25

came to escort them backto thewaiting room. If anyquestions were asked theexperimenter

either repeated the relevant section of theabove recruitment information or stressed that the

question could notreally be answered asthismightinfluence the experimental results.

Three similar yetdistinct training programs were used to establish baseline responding.

Three of the foursubjects were initially exposed to an increasing Geometric Ratio 2 schedule of

reinforcement with a maximum value of FR 250 (GR 2wI FR250max). Thisschedule requires

initial completion of a Fixed Ratio 2 for reinforcement and then geometrically increases, requiring

an FR4 during thenextcompletion, followed by the geometric increases to 8, 16,32, 64,128

andfinally reaching a terminal FR 250component. This schedule wasselected because it

provides a high density of reinforcement during the initial training period.

Thefourth subject, HN, was exposed to an arithmetic Progressive Ratio 3 (PR 3)schedule of

reinforcement which provides aneven higher density of reinforcement with the increasing ratio

series being arithmetic: 3, 6, 9,12,15,18, ... etc. Afterfivesessions underthisschedule during

which the highest progressive ratio reached required 150responses for reinforcement, HNwas

switched to a FR250 schedule of reinforcement butwith notimeoutoption yet introduced, for

twosessions. Subject ARwasalsoswitched to ananalogous FR250for threesessions after

being subjected to threesessions underthe initial GR2 wI FR250max. At thispoint for each

subject the time outcontingency was introduced on the heretofore inconsequential lefthand

response lever. Subjects PW& IN were exposed to onesession onlyof the GR2 wI FR 250max

andthenswitched immediately to the FR 250 condition withsimu~aneous introduction of the

timeout contingency.

Verbal Introduction of Time Out. Pilot research intoperformance underincreasing

ratio schedules suggested thatperhaps an implicit instructional demand towards vigilance in

responding wasin part responsible forpervasive andcor.sistent high response rates, even under

thehighest values examined andextinction.. Several procedures have beenincorporated into

thecurrent study to circumvent or at least minimize anysuch implicit influences given the inherent

bias of sitting infront of a telegraph key. Forexample, thefinancial arrangements provide a base

rate of payfor simply sitting in theexperimental room regardless of relation to performance. This

may implicitly suggest that appropriate behavior might simply include sitting inthe experimental

room.

Theverbal introduction of thetimeoutcontingency wasexplicitly designed to minimize any

demand characteristics of theexperimental situation towards responding evenin those cases

Page 42: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

26

where minimal or no reinforcement is forthcoming. To thisend, subjects were instructed that, if

so desired, they could stopresponding andtakea break by engaging in a specific response.

Baseline sessions underthe Fixed Ratio 250 schedule of reinforcement began withthe

verbal introduction of thetime outcontingency onthe leftmost keypriorto experimental session

2 for subjects IN & PW, session 7 for AR andsession 8 forsubject HN. Thetimeout relationship

was introduced as follows:

Starting today, if youeverfeel likestopping for awhile youcan press thiskey(point to leftkey) andall of theequipment willshutoff andthe lights willgo out. When youpress it again everything will resume where you leftoff.Thesession length will remain at 30 minutes regardless ofwhether youtumoff the equipment or not.

Experimental Design. Theessential task in this experiment wasto establish a stable

basal levelof performance on thesame schedule of reinforcement for thefoursubjects andthen

to increase the requirements for reinforcement bydifferent values across SUbjects andobserve

anydifferential effects on schedule performance. In thisdesign the initial schedule change

following baseline is a critical component. In thisexperiment thebaseline levelwas incremented

by a factorof 2,4, 8, & 16forsubjects AR, IN, PW, & HN, respectively. If nostrain wasseen each

subject wasrununtilthestability criterion wasmetat the newschedule value which wasthen

increased again. Theoccurrence ofstrain occasioned a reversal to a value of the schedule

sustaining performance. Responding was reinstated at these !owe:- schedule values by the

verbal instruction, "Points will be easier to get, today" priorthe thefirstsession of reversal.

Following stability, a second different incremental regimen towards thedisruptive FRvalue was

undertaken, requiring stable patterns at each intermediate value prior to further increment.

Criterion for Stability and Strain. Sidman (1960) reports that a stable or steady state

of behavior maybe defined asoneinwhich thebehavior in question does notchange its

characteristics overa period of time. A steady state of behavior is particularly useful asa baseline

fromwhichto assess theeffects of anexperimental manipulation. In theprocess of changing

from one steady stateto another following sucha manipulation, behavior passes through a

transition phase or state. The two are notcompletely separate. A stability criterion may be useful

in experiments examining steady state performance in the provision of a method to distinguish

transition states fromsteady states.

Page 43: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

27

Theselection of a criterion forstabilny is by nature an ideographic process withthe utility of

thecrlterlon defined by the reliabilny andvalidity of thedataobtained by its use. Sidman argues

that if the steady state criterion yields orderly and replicable functional relations, it maybe

accepted as adequate. It need not, in fact, represent the ultimate stable stateof behavior in

question. If thecriterion of stability selected is one through which the behavior must inevitably

passon it'swayto an ultimate steady state, thenthe obtained datawill be orderly andmeaningful.

Thecriterion should select reproducible andgeneralizable states of behavior.

Optimally, a stability criterion might be developed from a longtermdescriptive analysis of

behavior undera steady state. However, a number of events maypreclude such an analysis

including limitations in resources or, as in thiscase. tnsparticular expeiimental question being

asked. Forexample, research tasks mayspecifically examine theeffects of a manipulation on

steady states during which a criterion of stability maybeparticularly useful, or may focus onlyon

transition states during which a stabilny criterion maybeof lessuse, or some intermediate aspect

thereof.

Experiment I posits ratio strain to be primarily a transition phenomenon related to theprogram

of schedule increases. Thisanalysis of ratio strain bynecessity dictates an examination of those

transition states where performance is deteriorating aswellas those states where performance is

being maintained, or is minimally ina transition statesuggesting anellor approaching steady state

performance maintenance. Therelatively laxstability criterion incorporated in thisexperiment

therefore is notselected asonewhich will portray an ultimate steady stateof perlormance but

rather onewhich suggests a likelihood of a similar ultimate steady state.

In examining response frequency, three pointsof reference are minimally necessary to

suggest stability. More arepreferable. Visually andstatistically these should portray an absence

of tendency in behavior to be increasing or decreasing in rate. That is, the third point should be

within the range established bythe first two,or the range of the previous points if morethanthree

reference points exist. Thedegree of variability within this range to betolerated in a definition of

stability will ultimately be determinecl by theconsistency of the functional relations soobtained

andby the degree of experimental control thatcan be obtained.

Optimally inthis experiment, the range of variability defining thesteady state would be

visually minimal. Statistically, in those cases defying visual concordance, the maximum acceptable

variability proposed consists of each of three minimum reference points falling within a range of

20%above or 20% below the mean response rate of those three points. Forexample, the mean

of the three consecutive datapoints, 4.5,5.5, and5.3 responses persecond is 5.1. A range of

Page 44: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

28

20%above and below would incorporate response rates from 4.1 to 6.1, therefore thevariability

represented bythese three points would be within the limns of acceptability. Additionally, the

thirdpointfalls within the range established by the firsttwomeeting the requirements suggested

earlier.

Thus, stability is defined in thisexperiment minimally bytheoccurrence of noupward or

downward trends in response rate over a minimum of three sessions, with variability of response

rate for anygiven point lessthan20% above or below the mean response rate of those three

sessions. Again, thiscriterion is notmeant to reflect anultimate steady state of performance but

rather to suggest the absence or occurrence of ratio strain. The crneria forassessing ratio strain,

then, are inextricably linked to this definition. Visual observation of performance may allow more

stringent analysis of stability overthese minimum statistical requirements, where feasible.

Ifwe assume this stability criterion, onesetof criteria for assessing strain arise logically.

Previous discussion has suggested thatthe termratio strain has been utilized asa rather

picturesque metaphor to suggest changes, notably decreases inperformance, undercertain

conditions of ratio reinforcement schedules. A criterion suggesting theoccurrence of strain

thuswould include a cleardownward trend in response rate during the transition phase following

anexperimental manipulation and/or steady state performance ata substantially lower response

rate than thatof thebaseline level. A quantification of this decrease in mean response rate as

greater than 20% from the mean baseline levelis commensurate withthepreviously established

stability criterion.

In evaluating the effects of different sized schedule or program increases, performance was

examined under the control of the baseline FR250schedule aswell asthe higher FRschedule

values of 500, 1000,2000,4000, & 8000. In addition, theperformance ofsubject IN was

examined during an extended period of extinction. Table1 shows the exact size of

incrementation of the fixed ratios for each subject insummarizing theorder ofschedule

presentation and the number of sessions spent responding to each value of theschedule. As

canbe seen, reading down, subject AR spent sixsessions at FR 500afterfourbaseline sessions

of FR250. Thiswasthenconsecutively doubled to FR 1000,2000, &4000 for3,5 &7 sessions,

respectively. These lastthree fixed ratio values comprised the initial values ofschedule increase

for the othersubjects. That is,subject IN initially wasexpoG~ (0 an FR1000 following baseline,

PWwasexposed to FR2000, while HN wasexposed to FR4000. Notice, forsubject PW, that

afterthis innial exposure to FR 2000 following baseline, a reversal to FR500 occurs for five

sessions atwhich time the incremental regimen is analogous to and replicates thatof AR. Onthe

Page 45: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

29

otherhand, the fixed ratio value is increased further forsubject IN from FR 1000 to FR4000, FR

8000and thenthesubject is observed under23 sessions of extinction.

Table 1

Order and Incrementation Sizeof the Regimen of Increasing Fixed Ratio Schedules andthe Number ofSessions (In Parentheses) SpentResponding to EachFR by EachSubjectin Experiment I.

AB ~ fW .I:Iti250 (4) 250(6) 250(7) 250 (5)

500 (6) 1000 (5) 2000 (3) 4000 (4)

1000(3) 4000 (5) 500(5) 250 (1)

2000 (5) 8000(7) 1000 (4) 500(3)

4000 (7) Ext (23) 2000 (4) 4000 (4)

Theapriori contractual arrangement with subject HNwasfor considerably shorter participation

thanthe othersubjects andconsequently afterthe foursessions of responding underFR4000,

a single sessions reversal to FR 250occurred afterwhich timetheschedule was increased to FR

500for 3 sessions, the minimum necessary to establish stability.

RESULTS

A number of performance measures wereemployed in the analysis of program increases and

strain. The various response patterns generated bythose fixedratio schedules programmed on

the mainresponse leverwere empirically analyzed intermsof responses persecond. Cumulative

records of these patterns were collected providing a visual representation, aswell. Although

these two measures weretheprimaiY toolsof analysis in thisexperiment, the frequency of

responses to both thecenterkeyandthetimeoutkey, aswell as theduration of timeengaged in

timeout,werealsoexamined.

Theeffects of varying the increment sizewhen programming schedule increases on stable

performance and strain canbeseenin Figure 2. This figure provides a general overview of

Experiment I in terms of themainkey responses persecond for each subject. Responses per

second are indexed along theordinate while experimental sessions are represented on the

abscissa with thechanging experimental conditions indicated by vertical bars.

Page 46: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

30

Subject AA~

~D- n .-a~~ ~g.g ..CI..,_

f-f-I- FA 250 FA SOD FA 1000 FA 2000 FA 4000~-.

76543Z1o 7 8 !) 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 252627 41 42 43 44

Subject IN6~

S C)o9 n .a..a432 FR 250 FA 100010

e 9 12

f-

~FA 2000

- a-" n a--" n n n n 0-9 9 • ~n .n...-d=I-- FR 250

~ FR 500 FR 1000 FR 2000 FR 4000I-

"'CCooQ)

CIJL.(1) Subject PW0.o 6CD 5o 4c: 3o 20.1o 0CD S 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 lS 16 11 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 2S 26 21 28

a:Subject HN

e54

321o

f-

f-~FR 4000 n a.-- c III

f-

f-

~T251°f- FR 250 FR 500-8 9 10 11 12 13 14 lS 115 17 18 19 20

Sessions

Figure 2. The effects of varying increases in the sr.!!el1l.lll! Clf rp.inforcement onfrequency of responding to the main key by all SUbjects during Experiment I.

Page 47: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

31

Theresponse rate index wascalculated by dividing the total number of responses to the

main response lever by the number of seconds in a 30minute session minus thetimeout

duration. Responses to the main response leverduring a timeoutwere calculated separately and

arenot included in this index. Response rate under ratio schedules of reinforcement mayrefer

to either the local rate, which is the rate of responding minus the postreinforcement pause and

usually reflects a higher rate of responding, or to anoverall rate which incorporates the PRP into

the calculation. In thepresent research theamount of timespentpausing afterreinforcement

was generally very brief (less than three seconds) by allsubjects. As a result these two indexes

were very similar andconsequently onlyoverall response rate will be reported.

Ascanbe seen in Figure 2, the baseline response rates of the foursubjects arequitehigh,

in the range of 4-6 responses persecond. These rates remain quite highandeven increase

through the experiment for subject AR, with the exception of the temporary decrease seenin

session 21. Note the interruption of consecutive sessions occurring with regard to ARbetween

the thirdandfourth session under the FR4000. Other experimental arrangements, these being

the subjects of experiments" & III,were introduced and withdrawn during this period andthese

wi;1 be detailed anddiscussed further during each ensuing experiment. Forthepresent

purposes, the reversal nature of the experimental design allows thecurrent comparison. Subject

AR was exposed to thesmallest series of schedule increases.

Thesame general pattern is seen forsubject IN in thata high consistent response rate is

generated during the more abrupt ascending schedule series of FR250,1000,4000, and8000.

In thiscase, response rate decreases slightly, yet consistently, witheach schedule change.

However, overall performance forIN and AR canbecharacterized similarly as represented bya

generally highand persistent rate of responding on themain leveracross the two regimens of

increasing schedules.

Thisis in contrast to thepatterns generated during extinction byIN andthose generated by

subjects PWand HNunderthe largest schedule increases of FR2000 and4000, respectively.

Two clearinstances of strain as identified both visually and empirically by the aforementioned

criteria occurinthe lattercases. Following thehighrate baseline performance ofsubject PW, ratio

strain occurs when the schedule is increased to FR2000. Following thisdisruption performance

wasreestablished at the lower schedule of FR500 and the high response rates were immediately

reinstated by theverbal instruction, "Points will be easier to get, today" priorto session 12. The

factthat these high rates were maintained during the following 16 sessions while the schedule

value was increased gradually to FR 4000 ina fashion analogous to thesmall increases reported

Page 48: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

32

for AR lends support to the suggested importance of program influences in the presence or

absence of strain.

A second instance of performance deterioration following these larger schedule increases is

seen in the case of HN. Strain canbeobserved to occurduring theschedule increase to FR4000

but notduring thebaseline or under thefollowing lowerratio schedule of FR500. Following the

fourth session of FR 4000 responding wasagain verbally reestablished, as reported for PW,

underonesession of FR 250witha subsequent increase to FR500. As reported in the

Procedures section forHNit wasnotpossible to further continue witha gradual program of

schedule increases to the FR 4000 value which produced the response ratebreakdown. It might

be noted, however, that each of thethree othersubjects showed sustained performance at that

value following a lesser series of increasing steps.

Response patterns during extinction were examined withsubject IN. During FR 8000 the

subject was onlyreceiving onereinforcer persession because of thetime commitment involved

incompleting the ratio requirement, yeta high sustained rate of performance wasobserved. IN

wastherefore run under anextended period of extinction to examine whether if indeed it wasthe

response-reinforcer relation thatwas maintaining thisperformance. During 24sessions of

extinction several severe instances of performance disruption areseen, increasing in frequency

of occurrence oversessions, asduring sessions 26,36, 46& 48. Response rateis lower during

the lasthalfof theextinction condition thanduring the initial sessions suggesting thatthe

process of extinction is Inprogress, yet incomplete. Theagreed uponcessation datewasmet

aftersession 48 andit wasnotpossible to continue running further extinction sessions withIN.

However, it does appear thatthe high rate patterns observed under the FR 8000 were indeed

maintained by the minimal reinforcement obtained.

CumulatIveRecords.

A more detailed analysis of response patterns may begleaned from cumulative records

representative of thevarious conditions for each subject. A cumulative record is a graphic record

of responding whereby a recording pen moves along theabscissa of therecord from leftto right

withpassage of timeand along theordinate each time a performance occurs (Catania. 1968). The

rate of performance is therefore demonstrated bythe slope of thecurve, withincreased rate

reflected by increased slope.

Page 49: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

33

Figure 3. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment I by Subject AR. A complete collection ofallcumulative recordsgenerated during this experiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 50: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

34

~-""========­C\I-=====-':==-g: -=========--- I

0--====:=====-;~

Page 51: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

35

Figure 4. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment I bySubject IN. The upperfour records represent responding underthe increasing series of fixedratio schedules while the lowersix records show the firstthreeandfinalthreesessions of extinction. A complete collection of all cumulative recordsgenerated during thisexperiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 52: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

PR 250 PH 1000

36

•••• » i ».»» i; » .; I I ••• i»'

PH 4000

Bxt1l1ctton 1 ot 24

Ext1no1:1on :3 ot 24

Extinotion 23 ot 24

Bxt1Dct1on 2 ot 24

Extinotion 22 ot 24

Bxt1nct1on 24 ot 24

Page 53: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

37

Figure 3 shows representative cumulative records of the experimental condltlons

experienced by subject AR. Two recording pens wereused in obtaining these records. One

pen. referred to astheresponse marker was linked to the main response lever of the

experimental panel andrecorded each response by moving successively along theordinate. The

response marker automatically reset when reaching thefurthest point from theabscissa asshown

onthese records bya line exactly perpendicular to the abscissa drawn asthepenreturned toward

the abscissa. Approximately 450 responses were required to increment the response marker to

thepoint of reset. Additionally. when the ratio requirements of the fixed ratios experimentally

programmed on themain keywere completed, the response marker engaged ina brief downward

deflection corresponding to the point of reinforcement availability. These reinforcement hatch

marks canbeseen periodically yet regularly along thecourse of the response marker.

The second penreferred to asthe reinforcement marker, simply moved along theabscissa

withthepassage of timemaking a downward deflection corresponding to theavailability and

collection of the points used asreinforcement. The linedrawn bythis pencanbe easily used to

observe reinforcement density under each condition of theexperiment. Each uninterrupted

cumulative record represents a 30 minute experimental session. Ascanbe seen in the

cumulative records shown forAR in Figure 3, a continuous highrate of responding reflected by

the steep slope with no interruptions occurs under all conditions. Decreases in reinforcement

density canbe observed overthecourse of theexperiment, to the point where onlytwo

reinforcers areobtained during thesession under FR4000. Nopostreinforcement pausing

occurs at anyschedule value.

Similar patterns canbe seen bysubject IN in the fourconditions priorto extinction

represented in Figure 4. Responses to thetimeoutkey are alsorepresented on thecumulative

record. These are indicated bya premature reset of the response marker and two such

occurrences areobservable during thefirstschedule completion of FR4000 byIN in thethird

record. The timeouts portrayed areeach less than onesecond induration and an immediate

resumption of main keyresponding canbeobserved. Thesecond schedule completion

continues withnofurther timeouts. A considerable PRP occurs following the completion of the

FR 8000 asseenin thefourth graph of Figure 4. This pause is approximately twominutes in

length, lasting untiltheendof thesession. Ingeneral, IN exhibited verylittle pausing after

reinforcement asshown in theother records andthisparticular example is likely a function of the

historical relation under FR8000 whereby IN could consistently onlycomplete oneratio during

the30 minute session.

Page 54: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

38

Figure 4 also shows thefirst three and final three sessions of extinction for IN. A variety of

response patterns canbeobserved. The fifth, seventh, and ninth cumulative records of Figure 4

all showthesame high rate response patterns exhibited earlier when reinforcement wasavailable.

The initial disruption of this steady pattern canbeseen during the second session of extinction,

shown by the sixth record. This session is characterized by a number of brief timeouts

interrupting high rate performances asat A, several substantial pauses occurring independent of

anytimeoutas atB anda second distinct lower rate of responding atC. Anequipment

malfunction occurs at 0 obscuring thebehavioral pattern portrayed. A time out is engaged in yet

the response marker does not reset completely untila laterpoint. A pause is reflected, however,

it is notcertain asto whether this is theactual behavior occurring here or not.

A distinctive pattern of responding is seen in theeighth graph of Figure 4, the 22nd session

of extinction. Hundreds of resets are shown representing consistent responding onthe timeout

keyaswellasthe main response key. The finalsession of extinction isshown in thefinal record

of Figure 4. A much lower rate of responding is observed without the repeated occurrence of

timeout responses. A good dealof variability occurs in this lower rate of responding withsome

shortbreaks of lessthan 1Q seconds occurring.

Cumulative records from each condition experienced bysubject PWcanbe seen in Figure 5.

Thefirst and lastfour cumulative records represent responding underthe FR250baseline and

the gradual program increase of FR 500,1000,2000& 4000, respectively. Theresponse

patterns reflected by these records represent a high andstable rate of responding withno PRPs

exhibited. This is in contrast to the second, third andfourth records which portray all three

sessions of the initial abrupt schedule change to FR2000. These threerecords arecharacterized

by a break andrunpattern withan increased amount of time spent pausing during the final two

sessions. A number of timeoutsoccur during thefirstsession butonlyone is observable during

the lasttwo. Only one reinforcement is obtained early Inthefirstsession, while the ratio

requirement is notcompleted during the finaltwosessions at FR2000, thusa functional

extinction of sorts Is observed.

In Figure 6,cumulative records forsubject HN canbe seen. The firstand fifthof these

records represent responding before andafterthe initial large schedule change andportray

response patterns generated underthe baseline ratio of FR250. Post reinforcement pausing is

clearly observable in these records. Notice that in each of these twosessions a timeoutoccurs

briefly andthatin each case thetimeoutoccurs during the PRP.

Page 55: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

39

Figure 5. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment I by SUbject PW. The second, thirdandfourth records present acomplete record of responding overthreesessions of the initial presentation of FR 2000. Acomplete collection of all cumulative records generated during this experiment is included inAppendix I.

Page 56: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

n 250 n 2000 1 ot J

40

n200020tJ

n 2000

.. 1000

n 4000

/

Page 57: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

41

Figure 6. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment I by Subject HN. Thesecond, third and fourth records showresponding during the first. second andfinalsessions of FR4000, respectively. A completecollection of allcumulative records generated during thisexperiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 58: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

FR 250 PR 4000 1 ot 4

r "'-T • , , , i » » i' » , , » » » , , i » » iii» » • i , , , »

PR 4000 2 at 4

FH 250

I

PR 4000 4 ot 4

PH 500

,. -.- , • » • » , • » » • , • » • , ,

..,.I\)

Page 59: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

43

Thesecond, third, andfourth record portray 3 of the 4 FR4000 sessions for HN(thethird

session of FR4000 is notshown). During the initial session of FR4000 a number of shortbreaks

areseenaswellas a good number of short timeouts, however, overall response rates remain

high although variable. Responding decreases in frequency during thesecond session

following an initial burstanda number of time outscanbeobserved, oftenof briefduration.

Towards the endof the session pausing isevident. Bythe lastsession of FR4000, mainkey

response frequency hasdecreased even more andwhen occurring the rate is lowerand more

variable. A highfrequency of timeout responding is also seen. Aswith PWunder the initial

exposure to FR 2000, HNonlyobtains onereinforcer during the first session of FR4000withno

further schedule completions during the following three sessions.

Thefinal record shown in Figure 6 portrays the final condition of FR500. Response patterns

aresimilar to the baseline patterns, with PRPs evident. Visual observation suggests a slight

increase in PRPs withthe schedule increase from FR 250to FR500, however anyincreases are

negligible.

Time Out Infonnation

A more detailed analysis of performance interms of the interaction of mainkeyresponding,

timeout responding and responding to the thirdcenter keyat times otherthanwhen

reinforcement wasavailable canbe seen foreachsubject in Figures 7, 8, 9, & 10. These

response patterns areshown for subject AR in Figure 7. Thetop graph of this figure reproduces

the main key response rate information presented earlier in Figure 2,while the middle two graphs

portray timeout responding. The lowergraph represents frequency of responding to the third

center key. Recall that responding to thiskeywas inconsequential at times otherthanwhen

reinforcement wasreadily available assignalled by theillumination of theyellow lights. Hereafter,

thiskeyshallbe referred to asthe extra key.

Since the response patterns to the main response keyhavealready been described earlier,

thecurrent discussion will focus on the ancillary timeoutandextrakeypatterns. Thefirstsix

sessions, andparticularly thefirst three, in Figure 7 show the evolution of response patterns

generated by the main keycontingency everpatterns to the otherlevers duringtraining. Notice

thedifferences of ordinate value for each graph. At session 7 the timeout contingency was

introduced to thesubject andthe formal baseline wasbegun.

Page 60: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

SubjectAR

f0o- I~

~~

~7~I~~

FR 250 I FR 250No r. o. I with FR 500 FR 1000 FR 2000 FR 4000.

I Tjme Out •123 456 7 e 910111213141516171819202122232425262741424344

44

9 1011 12131415161718192021222324252627 4142 43 44Sessions

Figure 7. Response patterns to the main key. time out key. and extra key by subject ARin all conditions of Experiment 1, and duration of time spent engaging in time out.

Page 61: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

SlJljecl IN

4DO (374):0-

~ (275)

I..II. 6

~ 4

.! 2I-

0

3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33 36 39 42 4S 48

Sessions

Figtre B. Frequency of responding to main key. time out key. and extra key by subject IN In Experiment I.

45

Page 62: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

46

If one examines the frequency of responses to the timeout key before(sessions 1-6)and

after (sessions 7-44) the introduction of the time outcontingency, it is possible to observe that

the contingency actually serves to restrict overall frequency of this response. Veryfew time outs

by AR occuroverthe course of this experiment. Thefew that do tendto occur, occurwithin the

first twosessions aftera schedule change, butareof relatively briefduration.

Notime outsare engaged in duringthe FR 250baseline andthe first occurs during the

second session of FR500. Withthe increase to FR 1000, two timeoutsoccur, whilethe greatest

increase in timeout duration occurs withthe schedule at FR2000. Thisparticular timeoutoccurs

during the session wherethe mainkey response patterns havedecreased in rateandthe

engagement in timeout maybe partially responsible for this drop. It is possible to suggest at this

pointthat the magnitude of time out responding in termsof frequency andduration is increasing

with increasing schedule change, however the magnitude of the time out measures at FR4000

andthe generally negligible aspects of all timeoutsalsosuggest that this conclusion maynotbe

warranted.

The response patterns to the extrakeyseenin the bottom graph of Figure 7 for ARarealso

fairlynegligible. In general, very few responses areseento this inconsequential key, however

periodically overthe sessions a burstof responding mayoccuronlyto subside in subsequent

sessions. A higher frequency of responding occurs to this key thanto the timeout key. It is not

applicable to graph frequency of responses to the extra key during sessions 41-44as a prior

experimental condition influencing the probability of theiroccurrence was undertaken.

The responses of IN to eachof the threekeyscan be seenin Figure 8. Again, the topgraph

reflects the previously discussed mainkey response pattern. Thecentergraphshows

responses to the time out key. Although notgraphed 1124responses occurto the timeout key

priorto the verbal introduction of the timeout contingency in session 1. Veryfewresponses to

this keyoccurafterthe introduction of the timeout contingency untilextinction when in particular

two substantial response increases occur in sessions 26 & 46. More responses to the timeout

keyoccur laterrather than earlier in the extinction condition. Of thetimeoutsthatoccurpriorto

extinction, they occurduring the first session of a schedule increase in two instances. The

amount of timespentengaging in each timeoutwasverybriefby IN, typically lessthan1 second

per engagement, andtime outduration is notgraphed.

Perhaps of moreinterest in the caseof IN is the distribution of responses across the

experimental sessions to the extra keyportrayed in the lowergraph of Figure 8. During the first

two experimental sessions a relatively highfrequency of responses occurto this key, however

Page 63: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

47

Subject PW

..5II•C

Q'O 4a,C.. QII 0 3a: II

(I)

"" 2II N~&C 1

..-4II

0~

I-

~ "- " .S' nn n n~I- -- ~D

I- FR 2000

~(FR 250 L FR 500 FR 1000 FR 2000 FR 4000

I

2 4 I) 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28

I) 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 282 4

4

2

O'-ioIIIIII'~_""""'''''-''''''''''''_.-...II...._w.......................ue;:

500

..QI..CClQ,litQI

It>. 30QI~

1lI 20.....)(w 10

0

-(457)

~ ,4OIl'\ 1-l-

eI-

I~ ~~ 0.... a..... ~2 4 Ii 8 10 12 14 1G 18 20 22 24 2G 28

Sessions

Figure9. Frequency of responding to main key. time out key.and extra key by subject PW in all conditions of Experiment I.

Page 64: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

48

Subject HN-0

>.cGI 0 FA 250 FA 4000 FA 250 FA 500:l'g 6om......~

• &='< t:~in Key~GI 4~Q, D.-C

05" Extn KeyGI2c .. --... c

0 0

z::~ 0GI

II 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20Ill: 8

>.u 400c FA 250GI;:,13 300GI

'"u, 200II

~ 1000Q,

• 0GI~ 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

~:lCl

200GIlIi""'-0 150~c

0~oo GIl 100erno e 50......~• 0...:lQ

I- FA 250 FA 4000 ~A 25b FA 500

l- N~

j-~ ~

8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

Sessions

Figure 10. Response patterns to the main, extra, and time out keys by

HN in Experiment I and the duration of time spent engaging in time out.

Page 65: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

49

these decrease overthe next fewsessions asthe main key response pattern becomes strong. A

temporary increase in responding to theextrakeyoccurs withthe schedule increase to FR 1000

andagain when increased to FR 4000. In both of these cases tnepattern returns to a lowlevelby

the thirdsession of each condition. A slight increase is witnessed withthechange to FR 1000.

Thispattern is notobserved withthe increase to FR8000, however, a higher frequency of

responding to this keyoverthe entire condition is observed.

During extinction an increase in extra keyresponding is observed aswell. Aswiththetime

out responding, the occurrence of extra key responding occurs withgreater frequency later

rather thanearlier in the extinction condition and an initial increase canbe observed during the

second session. When comparing main keyand extra key response patterns there is, in general,

a converse correspondence. That is,the lowest main key response rates areseenin sessions

26,36, 44,46, & 48,whereas thehighest extra key response patterns areobserved during the

same sessions.

Responses by subject PWto each of the three keysduring the course of thisexperiment

can be seen if Figure 9. Very fewresponses occur to eitherthe timeoutkeyor the extra key

except duringthe main keyperformance deterioration generated by the abrupt schedule

increase to FR2000. Notice thedifferences in theordinate value on each graph. Although both

timeout andextrakeyresponses occurduring the initialpresentation of the FR 2000, by far the

larger response increase is to theextralever. Incomparison timeout responding is negligible.

However, giventhe baseline andsucceeding frequencies of the timeout responses it is

suggested that those conditions producing strain are likewise generating the increase in timeout

responding. Theamount of timespent engaging in timeoutwasagain verybriefby subject PW.

A unique pattem of responding canbe seen by subject HNin the top graph of Figure 10.

Response patterns to both the main keyandextra keyareportrayed inthisgraph. Themainkey

pattern hasbeen previously discussed. Theresponse rateto theextrakey, alsocalculated in

responses persecond, hasbeen plotted on thesame graph andas can beseenis quite similar in

nature. Thissuggests andvisual observation confirms that thesubject was responding

simultaneously to bothresponse levers andthatthispattern wasadventitiously reinforced by the

fixed ratio contingency programmed onlyon themain lever. As might be recalled, subject HNwas

theonlysubject to consistently show PRPs andthe additional response effort of this two handed

pattern, might be onecontributing factor.

Th~ lower two graphs of Figure 10show time outdatafor HN. Ascan beseenin thecanter

graph the freqtJ/:mcy of responding to the timeoutkey increases withthe advent of the FR4000

Page 66: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

50

contingency and reverts to baseline levels upon withdrawal. The final session of FR4000 in

particular shows a drastic increase in response frequency. This pattern of time out increases

during strain and reversal upon reinstatement of main keyresponse patterns issimilar to that

displayed in thecase of PW in Figure 9,withthe exception of the disparity in overall frequency of

timeoutsas indicated byordinate values on each graph.

Themeasurement of time outby duration shows thissame general pattern, as shown inthe

lower graph of Figure 10.An increase in the amount of timespent engaging intimeoutoccurs

during the FR4000 overboth the FR250 baseline or schedule increase to FR 500. Theanalysis

of timeoutduration alsosuggests thatthetimeouts engaged inby HNwere very brief.

DISCUSSION

Human operant responding andconversely thedisruption of performance or ratio strain was

observed to be distinctly influenced bythe regimen of schedule increases selected for usewhen

increasing ratio size. Ratio strain occurred when large increases in response requirements were

programmed and notwhen more moderate increases were scheduled. That ratio strain is notthe

outcome of some tenninal ratio size incapable of sustaining performance is evidenced bythefact

thatsubjects were quiteable to sustain responding at ratio sizes considerably larger than those

inducing strain when moderate increases were programmed. When a relatively moderate

program of schedule increases wasfollowed, responding persisted bythe human subjects even

under the largest fixedratios examined and initially at least under extinction foronesubject,

providing strong evidence thatundertheconditions in thisexperiment thecritical feature of

strained performance was theschedule increase regimen.

In the present research responding waspersistent under FR4000 for subjects ARandPW

and FR 8000forIN following the moderate series of schedule increases of FR500, 1000, 2000,

4000 (AR andPW) and FR 1000,4000, and 8000 (IN) from thebaseline conditions of FR250.

Larger increases frombaseline were shown to produce performance disruption when increasing

theschedules to FR2000 for subject PW and to FR 4000 for HN. The performance patterns

generated in thecase of PW are particularly illustrative ofthe relation between ratio strain and the

procedural regimen incorporated when increasing response requirements. Immediate and

pervasive performance disruption wasobserved following the initial schedule increase from

baseline to FR2000, however following reinstatement of response patterns at a lower FRand an

ensuing moderate series of schedule increases, performance was strong andpersistent at the

same FR2000 andthen even at the larger ratio of FR 4000.

Page 67: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

51

These results suggest thatthe ratio size perse is notas important of variable in ratio strain as

is the environmental context within which thatcontingency requirement is placed. When, as in

thisparticular case, a substantial response requirement is placed within the context of a series of

increasing response requirements, the specifics areattainable whereas without a history of

gradual increases, performance breakdown occurs. To some extent this history influences

performance by providing variability in one aspect of the controlling contingency complex or

cthcrwcc ~!o~g oneparticular dimension of thecontrolling stimulus, lesstechnically along one

particular dimension of the rules for reinforcement, thatof frequency requirements. With a

gradual series of increases a history of change along thisdimension is established facilitating

transfer of schedule control overresponding underconditions of schedule change to new,

increased frequency requirements. Conversely, without such a history the behavior of subjects

may notfulfill requirements of this newrule for reinforcement frequently enough for this

contingency to establish formal control overresponding andparticularly in cases of extremely

large ratio increases, behavior maydecrease in frequency showing "strain" even though the

frequency requirements arephysically attainable asevidenced behaviorally following a gradual

series of increases.

This suggests that ratio strain in thiscontext may besimilar to theperformance disruption

which occurs during transition phases following a distinct change inschedules controlling

performance. Earlier it wassuggested that each schedule of reinforcement actsas a

discriminative stimulus in thateach schedule selectively reinforces certain response patterns.

When schedule requirements arechanged a disruption in previously stable pertormance OCCUiS

during a temporary stage of response instability referred to as thetransition phase. Stable

patterns reflecting control by the newschedule are likely to evolve following the transisiton

phase. Hence, during thetransition phase the discriminative properties of the newschedule do

notyetcome to control response patterns and the response patterns during thisphase may

initially reflect a predominance of thattopography which hasbeen historically reinforced. Later in

thetransition phase thepatterns may come to approximate more andmore those patterns

ultimately selected in the future steady state performance. During some intervening point,

however, thebehavioral patterns occuring willbeunderminimal schedule control, that is

performance breakdown or inconsistencies during the transition phase occuras a result of the

inconsistencies in stimulus control inherent in a schedule change. Ferster andSkinner (1957)

describe indetail anexperiment examining performance during thetransition phase when a

schedule change from a VR360to an FR360 is undertaken with pigeons. Prior to and for a

Page 68: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

52

period following thechange, typical stable high rate VR performance occurs. However, toward

the endof theexperimental session performance decreases to thepointof taking 45 minutes to

complete one360 ratio run. Several pauses of 5-15 minutes occur during this runanda long

pause occurs nearthestartof the next fixed ratio segment. Overthe nextfivesessions response

sequences acquire thetypical break and run FRpattern characteristic of animal performance.

It is suggested thatthe performance decreases andinconsistencies referred to in the

present experiment as ratio strain are essentially similar to those instabilities of performance

witnessed during tranistion phases. Ineach case performance previously understrong schedule

control is disrupted by a change in thecontrolling schedule. Ratio strain is invoked asoccurring

when performance does notcome under subsequent "strong" schedule control. When control

by a subsequent schedule is reestablished thedisruption is referred to as transitory. Ratio strain

is in this circumstance a schedule or stimulus control phenomena influenced by procedural

arrangements enhancing schedule control.

When schedule control deteriorates and particularly when extremely high ratio requirements

aredemanded, a functional extinction of sorts maybeobserved. Thisoccurred in the present

experiment iii insiances of ratio strain with subjects PWand HNfollowing the large schedule

increases from baseline to FR2000 & 4000, respectively. In each of thesecases substantial

decreases in responding occurred during the sessions Immediately following the large FR

increase. These decreases Inperformance were Incompatible withcompleting the new

significantly larger schedule requirements. Only oneschedule completion wasfulfilled by each

subject in the firstexperimental session following theschedule increase. Thereafter response

rates of each PWandHNwere so lowthatnofurther reinforcers were obtained overthenext

several sessions even though responding wasto some extent maintained overthose sessions.

It is unlikely that thispersistence would be maintained indefinitely, for without reinforcement

performance is likely to cease or decrease to minimal occurrence asunderextinction. Therefore a

functional extinction maybe observed with strained ratio performance as in the present case.

Thissituation is referred to asa type of functional extinction in that formal requirements of

extinction arenotmet. Extinction refers to a procedural arrangement whereby reinforcement of a

previously reinforced operant is discontinued. The usual effect of extinction is to decrease the

frequency of performance. A critical feature in theuseof thetermextinction andonenotmet in

theexamples of ratio strain cited is thatuse is specifically limited to theprocedure of discontinuing

reinforcement (Ferster, Culbertson & Boren, 1975). In the current casethecontingency

requirements remain intact, albeit increased, and reinforcement is available as evidenced byeach

Page 69: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

53

of thesubjects initial schedule completion, hence formally this is nota case of extinction.

However, the response patterns emitted by PWand HNwere behaviorally far removed from the

contingency requirements anda situation hadcome to exist where practically itwasas if the

contingency hadbeen broken.

It is important to useextinction as a description of the procedure of discontinuing

reinforcement ratherthanas a description of achange in frequency ofperformance since on

occasion frequency mayactually increase upon extinction. Forexample, behavior is frequently

observed to markedly increase upon the initial discontinuation of reinforcement (Reynolds.

1975). In another example behavior may be occurring at a lowrate when reinforced under a

schedule of Differential Reinforcement of Lowrates or DRL. In thissituation the behavior may

actually increase under extinction returning to a baseline rate actually higher than thatoccurring

when reinforced underthe DRL (Ferster & Skinner, 1957). Ferster, et al. (1975) point outthat if

wedefined extinction otherthanprocedurally in these cases wewould be, "inthe unfortunate

position of saying, 'he performance wasextinguished but it did notextinguish' ". Thus, froma

formal perspective extinction describes a procedural condition which is notcurrently met.

The formal conditions of extinction weremetandperformance patterns generated during

extinction were examined in the current axperiment in thecase of subject IN. Sustained high

rate performance by IN on FR8000wasobserved underconditions of verylittlereinforcement.

Oneobjective of examining performance underextinction wasto evaluate whether or notit was

the response-reinforcer relationship maintaining thishighrate behavior. IN wasexposed to 24

sessions of extinction priorto a mutually agreed upontermination point. In general responding

during extinction waspersistent although a cleardownward trend in response ratewasobserved

overthe course of the extinction period, suggesting an extinction curve in progress. Itmight be

noted that. in thecontext of thepreceding discussion. an initial increase in responding byIN

occurred during the initial session of extinction.

Although responding byIN was prolonged throughout the 24-session extinction period,

evidence suggests thatthis course of responding underextinction is commensurate with

extinction curves generated in operant research with infrahuman subjects following a similar

previous history of intermittent ratio reinforcement. Forexample, Hutchinson, Azrin and Hunt

(1968) report sustained responding bysquirrel monkeys overa 41-day period of extinction

during which 4·hourdailysessions were run. By Day 41 of extinction, the rate of barpressing was

significantly decreased. Notably, an approximate decrease of 50%wasobserved by Day 20. This

is somewhat similarto thepattern sean inthe case of IN in thepresent experiment and onemight

Page 70: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

54

speculate that a best fit line through theextinction datagenerated suggests an extinction curve

which might becomplete in ananalogous 40 or50 sessions. Lindsley (1960) likewise reports

thatextensive histories of intermittent reinforcement of human operant responding lead to

extensive patterns of responding during extinction. Inthecase reported anextinction period of

approximately 100sessions wasnecessary to effect a nearcomplete cessation of responding

following 260sessions of variable interval reinforcement.

Thepatterns observed during extinction bysubject IN suggest that indeed previous high

rate and high requirement patterns of responding under the FRswere maintained by

contingency requirements andthe reinforcement of suchpatterns. Anessential difference in the

patterns exhibited by IN during extinction andthose extinction-like patterns generated by PW

and HNfollowing large schedule increases liesin theabruptness of frequency decrement.

Responding substantially andimmediately decreased underconditions of strain whereas

frequency of responding decreased more gradually underconditions of extinction. However,

these differences maybe related to schedule history differences in thatIN had experienced 24

sessions of ratio reinforcement priorto extinction whereas subjects PW andHN experienced 8 &

12sessions, respectively, priorto the large schedule increases.

A number of interesting performance pattems and relations areobserved when examining

timeout responding in thecurrent research. Asmaybe recalled previous research suggests that

timeoutsare reinforcing under conditions of increasing FRrequirements in thatthey increase as

a function of these increasing requirements (Azrin, 1961; Appel, 1963; Thompson, 1964, 1965).

Thesame authors alsoreport thattime out responding is mostlikely to ooeur during the PRP or

early inthe response run. Forthe most part these results arenotreplicated in thecurrent

research. Timeout responding wasseen to occurpredominantly during conditions of schedule

instability following schedule increases andpartirolarly duringmain keyperformance deterioration

following large schedule increases and during extinction. These results agree in large with

pattems of timeout responding reported by Zimmerman and Ferster (1964).

As reported, Zimmerman and Ferster did notobserve an increasing overall incidence of time

outasthe main keyFRwasincreased, rather theywitnessed an increased incidence of time out

during thefirstorsecond session following a schedule increase. This change wastransitory and

wasreported to decrease by about thefourth session following a change. Furthermore, the

frequency of time out responding wasseen to decrease markedly following a schedule change to

a smaller rather thanlarger FA. TIme outs were observed to occur during thePRP, however,

these weregenerally verybriefandresponses to thetimeout keytended to occurin even

Page 71: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

55

numbered groups of 2 or4 responses, etc. These response bursts onthetime out key resulted

in time inconditions being reintroduced immediately or shortly afteranoddnumbered group of

responses. Thus, theduration of timespent engaging intimeoutwas generally minimal. These

results and others obtained lead the authors to propose that time out responding occurred as a

relatively weak alternative behavior becoming predominant undercertain conditions of instability

in the referent main keycontingency rather than as escape from reinforcement.

Similar patterns were observed tooccur inthecurrent research. For example, duration of

timeoutwasgenerally negligible with responses usually occurring in even numbered bursts, as

reported byZimmerman and Ferster, quickly returning conditions to time in. Additionally, time

outs were notobserved to increase systematically with increases in theratio requirements when

ratios were raised ina fashion notdisrupting main keyperformance, as inthecases of AR andIN

andduring the lattergradually increasing ..egimen of fixed ratios programmed for PW. However,

timeout responding did increase with increasing fixed ratios when those increases were large,

concommitantly producing strained main key performance, as in thecase of HNandduring the

initial schedule increase of PW. Thus timeouts seemed to be related more to conditions of

instability than increasing FRs, perse.

Time outs were often observed to occur during the post reinforcement pause as reported in

previous studies. Forexample uncler conditions of response maintenance, cumulative records of

subject HNshowtime outs occurring exclusively duiir.gbrief post reinforcement pauses.

However, timeoutsoccurred at times other than during the PRP aswell. Cumulative records from

subject IN showtime outs, albeit brief, occurring wellwithin ratio runs forexample. Whereas

subject HN tended to emit timeouts exclusively during the PRP, theothers showed no

consistent pattems of timeoutdistribution with time outsoccurring both Inandout of the PRP.

Generally, although instances of PRP related time out responding were observed, these results

were notconsistent enough to support theclaim of timeoutexclusivity inthePRP or early ratio

run. The reader is invited to examine location of timeout responding asportrayed in the

cumulative records for each subject located inAppendix I.

Time out responding wasseen to substantially increase during the initial stage of extinction

andthen latterduring extinction under conditions of variability in the main keyperformance

patterns in thecaseof subject IN. A large initial incidence of timeout responding during

extlnction has likewise been reported by Zimmerman and Ferster (1964), although these authors

report a decline in prevalence with thecessation of main keyperformance. Unfortunately, the

relationship of timeout responding during thefinal stages of extinction could notbe assessed in

Page 72: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

56

the current study. The results reported herein andthesimilarity of thecurrent results to those

previously reported by Zimmerman and Ferster argue strongly against the theory proposed by

Azrin (1961) andothers of timeout as reinforcing under increasing fixed ratios. Infactevidence

fromthe current research suggests that thetimeout contingency actually serves to restrict

responding acting as a punishment procedure.

Theevidence for this is twofold. It may be recalled thata verbal introduction of the timeout

contingency was introduced afterthe initial training session(s) for all subjects andduring the initial

training session(s) thetime outkeywasfreely availabla and responses to this keywere recorded

although no timeout contingency was in place. A substantial decrease in the incidence of

responding to the timeout key occurred for all subjects afterthis verbal introduction of the time

out contingency. Inthe case of subject AR, for example, asseenin Figure 7, thetimeout

contingency wasactually introduced priorto the seventh overall session afterwhich minimal time

out responding occurred, whilepriorto this introduction responding wasactive to that response

key. The timeoutcontingency was introduced priorto session two forsubjects PWandIN

duringwhich 0 & 2 timeoutsoccurred, respectively, while 33 & 1124 timeout responses were

recorded duringtheprevious session, respectively. The average number of responses to the

time out key engaged in by subject HNduring the five sessions priorto the introduction of the

time outwas 2550 whileand average of 29timeout responses occurred during the fivesessions

following the introduction of the timeoutcontingency. Therefore onelineof evidence

suggesting that thetimeoutcontingency actually restricts responding derives from analysis of

the pre-post contingency response rates.

During the initial training sessions response frequencies to the nocontingency timeout key

andto the extrakey were approximately similar. Thisis notparticularly surprising given thatthese

two keyswereessentially equivalent during thispartof theexperiment in having noprogrammed

consequences (except for when the extrakeyserved as the key for a consummatory response

following completion of the schedule requirements). In comparing the frequency of responding

to thesetwo keysafterthe introduction of the timeoutcontingency, it can be seen thata much

higher frequency of responding occurs to theextrakeyfor all subjects. It is proposed that this

difference is primarily dueto a decrease in response frequency to thetimeout keyresuUing from

the imposition of thetimeout contingency. Thus a second lineof evidence suggesting thatthe

timeoutservesasa punishment of responding to thatkeycomes fromcomparing theoverall

response frequencies of theextraandtimeoutkeys. When examining the response patterns to

these keysall foursubjects in this experiment showed a much higher frequency of responding to

Page 73: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

57

the extra key. Additionally, increases in timeout responding werevery likelyto be paralleled by a

symetrical increase in extrakey response patterns with substantially largerfrequency increases

occurringto the no contingency extrakey. Hence again evidence suggeststhat the timeout

contingency may actually serve to restrict responding.

A presumption is madein this analysis that the response rates to thesetwo keys mightbe

similarif no time out contingency waspresent. Caution in this premise is warranted in that an

increased response frequency to the extrakey mightariseas a spuriouscorrelation or response

generalization which results from the consummatory response requirement intermittently

programmed on that samekey. However, several factorsargueto the legitimacy of the premise.

First, a distinctarrayof stimulus lights served as theoccasion for theconsummatory response

potentially enhancing discrimination. Secondly, the behavioral patternsobserved to the

extra/consummatory keyduring the absence andpresence of the stimulus lightssuggestthat

two separateclassesof responding coexisted. In the presence of the lightsa single response

often occurred to this key whereas duringthe absence of the stimulus lightsresponse bursts and

extended runswere observed. Visual observation confirmed distinct response patterns. In the

case of a schedule completion subjects often relaxed, sittingback in the chair upon illumination of

the lights then reaching out and casually "slapping" the consummatory response key thereby

incrementing the pointscounter. In the absence of such lightsthe subjectsdisplayedintensity of

responding and behavioral patterns similarto thoseemittedduring main key response runs.

Giventhese indications of stimulus controlby the consummatory occasion and conversely by the

absence of such occasion, the premise of functional equivalence of the extrakey and the

pre-contingency time out key is strengthened. A secondpointof caution in accepting this

premise lies in the physical arrangement of the experimental apparatus. As can be seen in Figure

2 and as discussed in the apparatus section the centerextra/consummatory keywas physically

closerto the subject, perhapsfacilitating responding.

Someevidence existsin the timeout literature as well, suggesting that the actualtime out

contingency servesto limit responding to a second key. Appel(1963) in examining the

increasing incidence of time out responding under increasing fixed ratios reports that a similar

relationship is seen whenstimuluschange aloneresults as a consequence of the response to a

secondkey. In fact, Appel reports the duration of time spent engaging in theconditionof

stimuluschange but no time out to increase as an exponential function of the FR size whereas

the durationof time spentengaging in timeout was alsoseento increase with increasing FRsbut

at a less acclerated rate. This resulted in a substantially greaterdurationof timespentunderthe

Page 74: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

58

stimulus change condition relative to thetimeoutcondition at the highest FRtested. This

interpretation of the literature andevidence fromthe current research thensuggests that

although as initially formulated the mechanism of timeoutperformance wasoneof reinforcement,

in actuality it seems thatthe phenomena andrelatcns involved may be considerably different and

at least from oneperspective the effect of a timeoutcontingency maybe perhaps more akin to

punishment. This perspective of theeffects of timeout is more in linewiththetraditional position

(Ferster, 1957) on which clinical practice is based (SUlzer-Azaroff & Mayer. 1977).

Response patterns to boththe time out key and to theextra key seem to predictably occur

under conditions of schedule variability in the main keycontingency. This variability is most

obvious under those conditions producing strain andduring extinction. However. increased

responding is likely to be observed to the extra and timeoutkey following a schedule change as

well and. as mentioned. a higher frequency of responding is seen underthese conditions to the

extra rather than to thetime outkey. This is particularly apparent in thecases of subjects IN and

PWwhere response increases areseen in the firstsessions following a change, subsiding bythe

lattersessions. Increases in response patterns to these two keys areoften observed in the

second or thirdsession following a scheduIechange for subject ARaswell. Subject HN's

idiosyncratic pattern of responding simultaneously to the main keyandextra keyprevents a

similar analysis of responding.

In summary. it seems likely thattheresponse pattems to thetimeoutkeyin this experiment

occur as a result of variability in performance under thecontrol of the main keyschedule of

reinforcement and those conditions producing schedule variability. rather thanasan escape from

aversive conditions. and that the frequency of time out responding underthese conditons is less

thanto a second no contingency extra key, leading oneto suspect that thetimeout contingency

in thissituation mayitself be procedurally classified aspunishing Inthat it restricts response

patterns to the timeoutkeyunderthese conditions of contingency Induced variability.

Inan experimentally arranged situation. a behavior isdefined as operant when it is shown to

beunderthecontrol of consequences given a particular occasion. The relationship of the

consequences to the behavior. or theschedule of reinforcement, is critical in controlling and

predicting or otherwise "governing" theobserved patterns of behavior. Periodically, other

behaviors may beobserved to be systematically related to a referent contingency. yet notunder

operant control by thatcontingency in thesense of being defined by consequences. A behavior

is saidto be "adjunctive" (i.e., to the schedule) or "schedule-Induced" when it varies

systematically or it'spresence or absence is systematically related to a particular schedule of

Page 75: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

59

reinforcement and when it is notspecifically under theconsequential control of thatschedule.

Patterns of responding to the time outandextra keys in thecurrent experiment share certain

commonalities wnh such schedule-induced behavior. Adjunctive or induced behavior maynot

necessarily havedefined or apparent consequences andyetmaybe observed to occur

systematically or predictably asa result of contingency arrangements focussing on not

necessarily related referent pattems of behavior. Thus, "schedule-induced" behaviors can be

distinguished from"schedule-governed" or reinforced operant behaviors.

Thefieldof schedule-induced behavior wasbrought to attention by theworkof Falk (1961)

who reported polydipsia or increased waterconsumption as a systematic effect occurring under

specific conditions of traditional lever press-food reinforcement schedules. Under certain fixed

interval (FI) condnions ratswere observed to drink excessively, up to one-half of theirbody weight

inwater, overthecourse of an experimental session. Since this initial research, a variety of

schedule-induced behaviors, many of clinical relevance, have been reported in animals including,

but not limited to; alchohol consumption (Falk, Sampson, & Winger, 1972); defecation (Rayfield,

Segal &Goldiamond, 1982); wheel RJnning (King, 1974); air licking (Mendleson &Chilling,

1970); aggression (Yobum & Cohen, 1979); self-administration of various dRJgs (Oei, Singer,

Jefferys, Lang & Latiff, 1978); wood chewing (Roper, 1978); andpica(Villareal, Note 2). In

experimental snuations a variety of adjunctive or schedule-Induced behaviors have been

reported withhumans aswell,for example; pacing anddrinking (Kachanoff, Leveille, Mclelland &

Wayne, 1973); eating, drinking and grooming (Fallon, Allen &Butler, 1979); smoking (Wallace &

Singer, 1976), aggression (Frederickson & Peterson, 1974); locomotor activity (Muller, Crow&

Cheney, 1979); andvocalization (Porter, Brown & Goldsmnh, 1982).

In general theobserved condUions likely to induce adjunctive patterns areessentially similar

to those initially described by Falk (1961) andinclude exposure to temporal schedules of

reinforcement withgenerally constant interreinforcement intervals, hence, fixed interval or fixed

time (whereby reinforcement is delivered at a fixed timeregardless of anyrelationship to behavior)

schedules. Level of deprivation with regard to a referent contingency often influences the

degree of schedule induction asmaythe schedule parameter Ineffect. With regard to polydipsia

a bltonc function of schedule-induction hasbeen reported onsimple FIschedules wnh maximum

consumption occurring at FI values of 32, 64, 128seconds while lowerlevels of consumption

occurat larger and smaller schedule values (Falk, 1964). Behaviors have been reported to be

induced underotherconditions aswell. A number of articles reviewing thisarea andinherent

research intricacies areavailable (Falk, 1981; Roper, 1981).

Page 76: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

60

A second general class of contingency induced behaviors might be deemed "contingency

change" induced behaviors. Inthissituation behaviors adjunctive to a given referent

contingency have been reported underconditions of change in thatcontingency. The

contingencies involved in these relations arenotnecessarily temporal. Forexample, Hutchinson,

Azrin andHunt (1968) report increases in biting responses by monkeys upon the introduction of

high FRrequirements. Gentry (1968) likewise hasreported attack andaggression byonepigeon

upon another as an induced pattern uponintroduction of ratio schedules. Ingeneral, the

changes involved producing adjunctive pattems are in thedirection of increased response

requirement or decreased reinforcement density. At oneextreme, aggression andattack have

been reported by both humans (Kelly & Hake, 1980) andinfrahumans (Azrin, Hutchinson & Hake,

1966) under conditons of extinction in a referent contingency.

Thegenerating conditions of extra andtime out key response patterns andthese response

patterns in thepresent experiment arecommon withobserved contingency induced patterns

reported in the experimental literature. Hutchinson, Azrin andHunt(1968) examined the bar

pressing performance of squirrel monkeys under a variety of ratio schedules including CRF, both

gradually andabruptiy increasing fixed ratios, FR reversals to lowerrequirements, andunder

extinction. In addition, the authors examined the occurrence of a second concurrent response

which had noprogrammed consequence. Thissecond response consisted of bitinga rubber

hose. Thissecond biting response wasobserved to occurat an increased frequency undera

number of experimental conditions. When extinction wasprogrammed following barpress

responses under lowratios, biting increased substantially from a zerobaseline levelceasing

immediately uponthe reintroduction of reinforcement.

Biting was also seen to occurupon increases in thefixed ratio requirements, withbiting

initially occurring throughout the main response interreinforcement interval but latterstabilizing

andlocalizing in thepostreinforcement pause or early response run. Theoverall frequency of

biting wasobserved to increase with increases in the ratio values anddecrease upon

reintroduction of lower values, although these changes were notnecessarily spontaneous.

Finally afteran extended history of FRperformance extinction was undertaken withonemonkey

overa 41 dayperiod during which biting occurred at an initial high frequency decreasing gradually

overthecourse of the extinction period yet remaining pervasive throughout.

It is suggested thatpatterns of timeout responding asseen in thecurrent research andfor

that matter patterns of extra keyresponding areactually induced patterns brought about by

degrees of contingency change. These behaviors aresimilar to timeoutpatterns previously

Page 77: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

61

observed by Zimmerman and Ferster (1964) in thattheyappear to be relatively weak (in thesense

of frequency) alternative performances increasing in frequency underconditions of schedule

instability, which may be brought about by contingency change. To thedegree thatthe patterns

of responding currently observed aresimilar to, or arecommensurate with, those reported in the

contingency induced literature oneagain questions theoretical relevance of historical explanatory

mechanisms suggested by the timeout frompositive reinforcement literature.

Time out responding has, in fact, beenreported to occurexplicitly as an adjunctive activity

undertypical fixed interval inducing conditions by Brown andFlory (1972). They report that, as

withotherFI induced behaviors, timeout responding occurs as a bitonic function initially

increasing andthendecreasing under increasing fixed interval schedules andclearpatterns of

thiscan beseenin twoof five pigeons exposed to FIschedules ranging from FI30 to FI960sec.

A number of procedural differences prohibit a direct comparison of these pattems withthe

previous patterns of timeout responding seen underratio schedules or with thecurrent

research, however the authors conclude thatpreviously reported increasing functions of FR

response requirements andthe amount of escape (or timeout responding) was notobserved

andthattheir obtained results lend support to thenotions proposed by Falkof a class of

adjunctive or schedule-induced behaviors. One of the notable procedural differences in the

Brown and Flory analysis of timeout responding concerns the timeoutcontingency in effect

during a response to the escape or timeoutkey. Under theconditions reported a distinct

stimulus change waspresent yet thetimerof the referent temporal based contingency continued

to time. Hence, thisstudy might properly report schedule-induced stimulus change responding.

A particularly striking result arising fromthepresent research concerns the fixed ratio values

shown to sustain human schedule performance. To some extentit is notsurprising to see

response maintenance underlarge fixed ratios giventhe current experimental arrangements of

minimal response effort andthe implicit experimental demand characteristic of a small room anda

lonely telegraph key. What is surprising is thatFR values of the magnitude examined herein have

notbeenreported in previous studies of human FRperformance. Forexample, analogous

research utilizing a similar button pressing response with human responders typically has

examined performance underthecontrol of schedules withmaximum ratio values of FR 10

(Weiner, 1970a, 1979, 1981a), FR 20 (Azrin, 1959), FR 30 (Frazier & Biletto, 1969), FR40

(Weiner, 1964b, 1967, 19n, 1981b, 1982), FR50 (Long, 1963;Weiner, 1964a), FR 60 (Repp

& Dietz, 1975), FR 80 (Wasik, 1969; Weiner, 1972)•.FR200(Holland, 1958), FR250 (Weiner,

1966), FR 300 (Poppen, 1982), andFR 1000 (Sanders, 1969). This is in contrast to the current

Page 78: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

62

research utilizing an FR 250 baseline with increasing schedule regimens to FA4000 and FR

8000.

Human operant performance hasalso been examined under thecontrol ofplunger-pulling

schedules as large as FR20 (Lindlsey, 1S59), FR 120 (Duvinsky & Poppen, 1982), FR150

(Long, 1958), andFA300 (Hutchinson & Azrin, 1961). Additionally, Witters andBachrach (1965)

report aschedule of FR200 where a verbal response wasutilized astheoperant. As can be seen

typical FRresearch with humans heretofore hasbeen conducted atverylowratio values,

comparatively speaking. The large FRvalues studied in thecurrent research to some extent

parallel in magnitude those high ratios shown to sustain performance by pigeons (FR2000;

Reynolds, 1975) andmonkeys (FR 4000; Findley & Brady, 1965) and it is perhaps this relative

concurrence which hasproduced thedistinct humanlinfrahuman similarities noted in

performance with regard to time outand extra keypatterns.

Sanders (1969) in perhaps thestudy most closely related to thepresent interms of ratio

values examined, observed the button pressing performance of twohuman subjects over20

two-hour sessions. Theauthor specifically wasexamining concurrent fixed ratio andfixed interval

performance and reported sustained ratio responding at FRvalues of 50,100,150,300,500 and

1000. The largest ratio of FA 1000 wasreported to be in effect for 1.5 hours, and no strain or

postreinforcement pausing wasobserved at this value or overthecourse of the experiment.

Post reinforcement pausing is not reported to occurby human responders in those studies

examining performance at the large ratio values heretofore examined (Holland, 1958; Hutchinson

& Azrin, 1961; Poppen, 1982; Sanders, 1969; Weiner, 1966) norwas significant post

reinforcement pausing observed in the current study, although briefPRPs were observed in the

case of HN. This is incontrast to those aforementioned across-species similarities in thearea of

collateral response patterns. Analogous results lead Weiner (1966) to suggest that the

characteristic response patterns observed in infrahuman schedule research maynot necessarily

occurwithhumans.

Indeed, recent research andtheory in human operant behavior hassuggested thatmany of

the typical performance patterns observed in infrahuman (hereafter also called animal) research

maynotbe readily apparent when human responders areutilized (Matthews, Shimoff, Catania &

Sagvolden, 19n; Lowe, 1979; Harzem, 1984; Saunders, 1969). Some authors, notably Weiner

(1983), have argued thatthese results are likely a function of uncontrolled experimental variables.

histories andprocedures, while others have implicated theoccurrence of verbal behavior,

instructions, and self-instruction ascritical features of experimental differences (Lowe, 1979).

Page 79: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

63

Much of thiscontroversy hasevolved from a series of research articles studying human

perfonnance under the control of fixedinterval schedules of reinforcement where the PRP has

remained elusive aswell. Although thetypical scalloped pattern of FI responding inanimal

research hassometimes been observed by human responders (Buskit, Miller, & Bennet, 1980)

and hasbeenproduced when nonexistent through experimental arrangements (Azrin, 1959;

Weiner, 1972; Buskit, Miller, &Bennettt, 1981), otherpatterns including high undifferentiated

rates (Weiner, 1969, 1970), variable rates (Harzem, 1984), andlowrates of responding with only

oneor a fewresponses occurring close to reinforcement ( Matthews, Shimoff, Catania, &

Sagvolden, 1977) have been reported aswell, sometimes in different subjects of the same

experiment (Weiner, 1969; Buskit, at aI., 1980).

The importance of issues arising from thiscontroversy has, at extremes, called intoquestion

the verynature of the relevance of the experimental analysis of behavior to thehuman condition.

To the extent thatacross species replicability provides evidence forthe extrapolation of

information from animal to human behavior (Skinner, 1953), the issue seems gennane. Ona less

grandiose level, discrepancies areat least interesting, at times perplexing, and usually worthy of

further examination.

Thecurrent studyserves to emphasize that a good dealof basic research stillneeds to be

undertaken before strong opinions canbe voiced on some of these issues. Forexample, human

behavior had not really been examined underthe control of most of the schedules incorporated

inthisstudy. Itappears that more realistic appraisals ofperfonnance will result asa function of

further variation in experimental conditions. In general, the results of the present research show

more similarity than dissimilarity to traditional infrahuman behavioral patterns (Ferster & Skinner,

1957) and in thecurrent analysis of behavioral disruption relations arising from research in the

experimental analysis of animal behavior seem as helpful Wnot more helpful than those relations

derived fromtheexamination ofhuman operant behavior. Ideally thetwoareas arecompatible.

A clueto thediscrepancy in the case of the absent post reinforcement pause might be

gleaned fromthatsituation in thecurrent research where brief PRPs wereobserved, thatbeing in.pattems by subject HN. As may be recalled subject HNengaged in a superstitious two-handed

response pattem pressing the main response leverand the extra key in tandem withthe right and

left hands, respectively. It is speculated thatthe effort involved in maintaining this response style

isconsiderably greater thana one-handed pattern allowing a switching of the hand inoperation.

Hence it seems feasible that PRPs maybe indirectly related to response effort. The influences of

response effort on human operant perfonnance areaddressed in Experiment II.

Page 80: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

64

EXPERIMENT II

Responding wasobserved to besustained at high rates following a series of gradual

schedule increases in Experiment I, evert at fixed ratio values of 4000, 8000, andto an extent

during extinction, whereas strain wasobserved to occurfollowing more abrupt schedule

increases. At these higherratios responding wassustained at values much largerthan

previously reported in the human operant IUerature. It wassuggested that perhaps the nature

of the taskandinparticular the response effort requirements may have been partially

responsible for thisoccurrence.

Evidence suggests that indeed the behavior selected for analysis may becrltlcal in

schedule performance andsubsequently strain. Forexample, as reported previously Staats, et

at. (1963) suggest that the response effort involved Ina complex verbal, motorand

discriminative reading response waspartially responsible for the development of strain or

response breakdown on the lowFixed Ratios of 4 and7. Otherreports of response

deterioration at lowschedule parameters when complex response sequences are required

exist aswell. Ferster (1960) reports strained ratio performance wUh pigeons at the low ratio

values of FR95 & 47when theoperant selected wasa match to sample (Ferster, Culbertson, &

Boren, 1975) response. Likewise, Thompson andMoershboecher (1980) report ratio strain at

FR20when a chained sequential taskwasrequired of pigeons. Analysis of strain and schedule

performance in terms of response effortrequirements mayshed light onsome of the observed

patterns in thecurrent research andon discrepent results reported inother animal and human

schedule reports.

A variety of human behaviors have been selected asbehaviors of choice underoperant

schedule analysis inclUding button pressing (Sanders, 1969), plunger pUlling (Linsley, 1956;

Hutchinson & Azrin, 1961), andreading (Staats, et al. Note 1),among others. The response

efforts involved in eachof these behaviors may indeed substantially affect subsequent

schedule performance. Forexample, Hutchinson andAzrln report ratio strain at FR300with

pronounced post reinforcement pausing andwUhin runbreaks when plunger pulling requiring

a 300gm. force to pullwas utilized asa response, whereas Saunders (1969) reports sustained

responding at FR1000 (with no PRPs) when a 40gm.button pressing response wasutilized.

A similar button pressing response wasused in Experiment I andunder some conditions, at

least, similar response persistence wasobserved.

Page 81: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

65

The question arises asto the effect of response effort on performance andparticularly in

this case on response decrement or strain. Azrin (1958) reports thathumans engaging ina

button pressing response requiring approximately 15gms. of response effort showed

persistent high rate response patterns under fixed interval schedules of reinforcement. This

particular pattern isdiscrepant withthe typical scalloped FIpattern observed in most laboratory

animal research. The author reports a decrement in overall response rate andthe emergence

of scalloped FI responding when the response effort wasincreased to a several hundred gram

force requirement.

The evidence reviewed suggests that response effort requirements do indeed influence

performance patterns andmay be a precursor to strain, particularly at the large fixed ratio values

observed in Experiment I. This particular experiment examines performance disruption as a

function of increasing the response effort requirements involved in engaging the main

response keyandit is hypothesized that response effort increases will produce strained

performance. Specifically, performance isobserved under large FRs when, ina reversal

design, theresponse effort is increased from a 100gram effort requirement to a 500gram

requirement.

METHODS

SUbjects

Three subjects participated in this experiment. Two of these three subjects, ARand PW,

alsoparticipated in theprevious experiment and corresponding subject information can be

seenin thesubject identification section of Experiment I. A thirdsubject was recruited using

the recruitment procedures specified earlier.

LD Subject LDwasa 35-year-old Caucasion female currently working as a

secondary school teacher andtutor. Shehad completed the B.S. degree in

education andhad taken some graduate coursework in special education, as well.

LD participated in 58sessions overa sixweek period collecting 3020 bonus

points for a totalearning of $88.20 and wasactually paid$97.15 or thecurrent

minimum wage of $3.35 per hour.

Page 82: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

66

Apparatus

Theexperimental room, apparatus, andcontrol equipment usedin this experiment are

exactly thesame asdescribed in Experiment I. During some parts of this experiment, however,

the force required to close thecontacts of themain keyresponse leverwasincreased from the

heretofore used 115gm.,across .15em., to 500 gm.across 4.5em. These two response

forces arereferred to as minimum and maximum effort, respectively. In thisarrangement both

gram effort andthrow space Oi ~01 c~sure in centimeter wasvaried in orderto produce a

maximum change in response requirement given the limitations of the current experimental

apparatus.

Procedure

Unless otherwise specified the procedures used in this experiment canbe assumed

similar to those reported in Experiment I.

ExperimentalDesign. Thegeneral experimental design of thisstudywasthat of an

ABAreversal. A baseline level of responding wasestablished at a particular fixed ratio

schedule andwiththe minimum keyforce in effect, thenthe keyforcewas abruptly increased

to the maximum level and sessions were rununtil response patterns wereagain stable.

Following thismanipulation there wasa reversal in the keyforcereturning to the baseline level,

untilstability criterion weremet.

Fortwo subjects, ARandPW, thepartiQJlar schedule thatwasheld constant across these

three conditions wasan FR4000. ForSUbject LDtheschedule was FR500during thefirst two

components andthiswasincreased to FR1000during thethird, reversal, component.

Furthermore, in anexperimental design arrangement thatwill be discussed in detailduring

Experiment III, there wasa 9- and12-session interim between the maximum keyforceand

minimum reversal components for subjects AR and LD,respectively. During this time,other

experimental arrangements were introduced and withdrawn for each SUbject. Forthe sake of

clearpresentation, thesemanipulations will notbe presented here.1 Ontheotherhand, the

experimental arrangements andsequencing occurduring consecutive sessions for subject

PW, perhaps representing the mostrigorous experimental design.

1. As notedthese experimental arrangements willbe explicitly addressed as the subjectofdiscussion in Experiment III. Briefly, in the caseof subjectAR, a concurrent alternative reinforcementcontingency was introduced on the extrakeyand this alternative was available duringfour sessions(32-35) afterwhichtime the alternative waswithdrawn duringthe ensuing five sessions (36-40). Likewise

Page 83: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

67

History. Each of these three subjects entered thisparticular experiment withextensive

experimental history. The nature of thisexperiment does not require as meticulous

documentation of history as Experiment I, however for thetwo subjects, AR andPW, who

participated in thatexperiment, this history Iswelldocumented. Bycomparing the session

numbers located on the abscissa of the respective subject graphs in Figure 11 withthe

respective graphs andsession numbers of Figure 2 on page 30, the reader canspecifically

examine the experimental history leading upto this experiment. Training datawasreported for

these twosubjects in Experiment I, as well.

Subject LO participated in33 experimental sessions priorto thisstudy, of which 10of

these sessions were exclusively devoted to the training procedures culminating in the FR250

baseline. Eight sessions of FR1000were runfollowing four FR250baseline sessions.

Following this a reversal backto the FR250 occurred for foursessions andthenan extended

number of sessions (12) were runat FR500, the last5 ofwhichcompose the baseline for the

present study.

RESULTS

Theresults presented in thisexperiment will include those measures previously discussed

in Experiment I. These include response rate indices to each of the three response levers and

cumulative records. Please refer to Experiment I for specific information relating to calculation

of response rate or analysis of cumulative records.

Theeffects of increasing the main key response effort fromthe minimum to maximum

setting on the mainkey response patterns for thethreesubjects in this experiment canbe

observed in Figure 11. Thethree graphs fromtopto bottom referto datafor AR, PWand LO,

respectively. Theoverall responses persecond seen across conditions ranged from5-7 for

subject AR, 2-5for PW, and1-3for LOas noted on each respective ordinate.

for subject LD an alternative reinforcement contingency was introduced for threesessions (49-51) andthenwithdrawn for four sessions (52-55). Priorto the Introduction of the alternative for subject LD(sessions 43-48)the main keycontingency requirements were increased from FR500 to FR 1000although no effectof this increase wasobserved. In general for eachof thesesubjects, main keyresponse patterns decreased substantially withthe introduction of the alternative reinforcementcontingency to the extrakey and likewise extrakey responding increased substantially although theseeffects wereobserved to reverse withthe reversal of experimental arrangements. The nature of theexperimental design andthe reversal of experimental effectpermits the present analysis.

Page 84: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

68

SUbject AR

Subject lD

Subject PW

25 26 27 28 29 30 31 41 42 43 44

:~ .. n~

n n -l-

I-

- Min Max Min'-

- I FA 4000 JI • I

~

n/ g g .....g.

~ a ':I II .co.

I-n~

~Min Max Min

- I II FA 4000 I

1

6

5

4

3

2

1o

5

4

3

2

1

o

2.5I-~I

~2.0 '- I~~1.5 ~

Min I Max Min1.0 -.5 -I FA 500

1FA 1000

.0 I

34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 56 57 58

-0c:oo0)

enr-CDCo(I)

CD(J)

c:oCo(J)Q)a:

Sessions

Figure l l, The effects of increasing the response force on the main key from .115 gm. to 500 gm. on responding by all three subjects during Experiment II.

Page 85: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

69

Ascanbe seen in Figure 11 and for each subject. increasing the response effort on the

main lever results in a decrease in the referent response pattern witha corresponding increase

in rate upon reversal. This decrease. however, is notparticularly large, given thebaseline

values. Forexample. subject AR is responding at the high rate of 5 responses per second

even during the maximum effort condition. although this is a decrease of approximately 1

response persecond from the baseline levels. Forsubjects AR andPWthe response rates

decrease below the baseline levels across all4 sessions during the maximum effortcondition.

while thisoccurs on three outof foursessions for LD. The reversal of thiseffect suggests that

it is indeed the experimental manipuiation that is responsible for thedecrease in rate.

In Experiment I a criterion for assessing strained performance wasdeveloped targeting

either a cleardownward trend in response rate during thetransition phase following an

experimental manipulation anellor a steady state performance at a response ratedecreased by

greater than20%froma previously established baseline level. In thecurrent experiment only

one instance of strained performance canbe observed utilizing thiscriterion, that in thecase of

subject PW. Theaverage main keyresponse rate of subject PWduring thepremanipulation

baseline was3.65responses persecond. This figure was derived as the mean of the four

individual sessions comprising thebaseline. Theaverage response rate during the ensuing

fourexperimental sessions was2.65 or otherwise a decrease of 27%in mean responses per

second. Mean responses per second wereobserved to increase to a level above the initial

baseline during thepostexperimental baseline averaging 4.4responses persecond.

Subject AR responded to the main key at anaverage rate of 6.4 responses persecond

during thepremanipulation baseline period of thecurrent experiment. whereas responding

decreased approximately 16%to anaverage of 5.4 responses persecond during the

experimental manipulation. Baseline responding increased to an average level of 5.8

responses persecond following reversal of theexperimental procedure. Average responding

for subject LDoverthese threeexperimental periods was2.2. 1.8& 2.3responses per

second. respectively. with rates falling off approximately 18% during the experimental

manipulation. Neither of these performances would be deemed strained according to the

established criteria of a 20%decrement.

Page 86: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

70

Figure 12. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated duringeachcondition of Experiment II by Subject AR. The upper and lowerrecords represent the preandpost manipulation baseline sessions, respectively, while thecenterrecord shows respondingduring the maximum response effort experimental manipulation. A complete collection of allcumulative records generated during thisexperiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 87: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

PR 4000 Minimum response effortA

PR 4000 Maximum response effort

71

c

PR 4000 Minimum response effort

Page 88: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

72

Figure 13. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment II by Subject PW. Theupper and lower records represent the pre andpostmanipulation baseline sessions, respectively, while thecenter record shows respondingduring the maximum response effort experimental manipulation. A complete collection of allcumulative records generated during this experiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 89: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

PR 4000 Minimum reeponse effort

PR 4000 Maximum response effort

PR 4000 Minimum reepoDse effort

73

Page 90: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

74

Cumulative Records

Cumulative records of these main key response patterns can be seen inFigures 12, 13,&

14. A description of the operation and interpretation of cumulative records isavailable in

Experiment I. The records of three representative sessions, onefrom each condflon, for

subject AR areportrayed in Figure 12. Theupperandlower records represent the pre and

post (experimental manipulation) baseline sessions, respectively, with the minimum keyeffort.

These records aresimilar to those produced by AR in thefirstexperiment andreflect a

consistent andhighresponse rate. At FR 4000, AR iscollecting two reinforcers a session. A

brieftimeout as indicated by a premature reset of the response marker canbeobserved at A in

the upperrecord anda loweryetconsistent response rate canbe temporarily observed at D in

the lower record. Otherwise, these records portray a very steady rateof responding.

Thecenterrecord of Figure 12shows responding during the maximum effort condhlon.

Theoverall record is generally similar to thebaseline sessions. Forthe most parta highand

steady rate can again beobserved withonebrief timeoutoccurring during the initial ratio

completion as evidenced by the premature reset. However, a couple of distinctive features

suggest the influence of the experimental manipulation. Two instances of rough grain or

temporary andabrupt rate changes occur at B andthese represent more variable rate changes

thanthat at D. Also, it is possible to observe theoverall decrease in response frequency by

comparing the interreinforcement intervals andatwhatpoint in the session thefinal reinforcer is

delivered, at C,with the baseline sessions. During the response effort condUion thedecrease

in rate is reflected by the laterdelivery of reinforcement within the session.

Cumulative records showing typical response patterns bysubject PWarepresented in

Figure 13. Aswith the record of AR, the upperandlower cumulative records showpatterns

generated during the pre andpostbaseline periods, respectively, whilethe center record

portrays responding during the maximum effort experimental condition. The experimental

manipulation is seento decrease the overall response rate, as indexed by the placement of the

reinforcer on the record within the session, andalsoproduces within ratio pausing. These

pauses areof relatively briefduration (5-10 sec.) andarespaced regularly through the session,

suggesting perhaps a counting function. A slight decrease in response rateoccurs

immediately following reinforcement butthis increases shortly therafter. The baseline records

showsteady high rate performance withnosubstantial postreinforcement pausing by PW. In

the lowerrecord two ratios arecompleted in contrast to the single reinforcer obtained in each of

the uppertwo records.

Page 91: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

75

Figure 14. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment II by Subject LD. Theupper andlowerrecords represent the pre andpostmanipulation baseline sessions, respectively, while thecenter record shows respondingduring the maximum response effort experimental manipulation. A complete collection of allcumulative records generated during this experiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 92: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

PR 500 Minimum response effort

iR 500 Maximum response effort

PR 1000 Minimum response effort

76

Page 93: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

77

A lesssteadyrate of responding can be observed by subject LD in Figure 14. Theupper

record of thisfigureportrays pre manipulation baseline responding undera FR500. Ascan be

seen, a fairlysteadyrate of responding occurs, as notedearlierin the range of 2 to 2.5

responses per second, with a number of pauses duringthe ratios. For the mostpart these

pauses areshort, however, occasionally a longer pause occurs as at A. Several substantial

PRPs canbe observed andone brieftimeout occurs late in the session as indicated by the

premature resetof the response marker. A similar pattemoccursduringthe post manipulaton

baseline sessions underFR 1000shownin the lowerrecord. No time outsoccur during the

session shownandpausing is lesssubstantial.

Visual observation of the subject's behavior duringthesebaseline sessions provides

some information on the natureof the within ratio pausesby LD. Thissubjecttendedto

altemate pressing the mainresponse leverwith individual fingers of both hands. Forexample,

the subject might initially presswith the fifth digiton the righthandfor 75 responses andthen

switchto the fourthfor 100 responses, the third for 60 responses and so on, workingthrough

all fingers andthumbs on both hands. A briefpause wasthen likelyto occurwith eachswitch.

The centerrecord of Figure 14showsresponse patternsduringthe first sessionof the

experimental manipulation for LD. An increase in the number of time outsas evidenced by

cumulative record resets can be observed, however for the mostpart theseareof brief

duration. A timaout occurs following reinforcement at B, at whichtime mainkey responding

resumes during the timeout and continues untilC, when a second response to the timeout

lever returns the subject to time in. Main key responding thencontinues to the end of the

session. Response rate remains at a levelcommensurate with the baseline levelsduring this

session.

Time Out and Extra Key Infonnation

In addition to the mainkey response pattems portrayed earlierin Figure 11 and the

aforementioned cumulative records, Figures 15,16, & 17 showadditional response patterns

to the timeout and extra keyfor subjects AR,PWandLD, respectively. Subject AR, as shown

in the middle graphof Figure 15 responds minimally to the time out key overthe courseof this

experiment. The uppergraphof this Figure presents the previously discussed mainkey

response rate, whilethe lowershowsextra key responding. No effectsof the experimental

condition canbe observed with regard to the timeout or extrakey response patternsin this

experiment, although a higherfrequency of extra key rather thantime out responding by AR is

Page 94: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

78

SUbject AR

7

654

3

21o

:~ n n n~

n-

r- Min Max Minr-,.. I FA 40DO I

II

I

25 2G 27 28 29 30 31 41 42 43 44

:-.QcII

3:laII..

l&. 2..,:l0 1II•.... 0~

25 26 27 28 29 30 31 41 42 43 44

wCDwe 300Qa.wII 200II:

:-.CD

100:IeII not applicable..~

0I&l 25 2G 27 28 29 30 31 41 42 43 44

Sessions

Figure 15. The effects of increasing the main key response efforton frequency of responding to the main key. time out and extra key.

Page 95: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

SUbject PW

79

25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37

5

4

3

2

1

£I

~0c:GI:l 217•...

II...,:lQ

GIi

0....~

IIIII..e0 15Do..IIIII: 10~GI~

5•...~ 0lW

l-I'!- g g

D""'",g

I-~

I-~

~ Min Max Min1-' FR 4000 I

I I

I-

\I-

.. .. ..25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37

2S 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37

Sessions

Figure 16. The effects of Increasing the main key response efforton frequency of responding to the main key. time out and extra key.

Page 96: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

80

SUbject lD

5857

Min

FA 1000

4240 4139

o

36

Min

34 35

2.52.01.51.0

.5

.0 ~~~~~_~_~..:-~_~~~~~~_~_~.....

UI~III:lUICC:

ailia.QUI... 111~a:

34 3S 36 37 36 39 40 41 42I

56 57 58

5857

not applicable

563836 3734 3S

50

40

20

O~~~~~~~~~~~_~_~_~.......~_~_~ ..>'CIIII III~ ..

c:CIa100 a.~ ..x III1IJa:

ggI 0

III

I- _ I~I

..00 ­

300 ­

200 ­

100

o34 3S 36 31 38 39 41 42 56 51 58

34 3S 36 37 38

800 ~

800 ­

400 ­

200o

IIII

~I39 40 42

I 0

56

o

57 58

Sessions

Figure 11. The effects of increasing the main key response effort on frequencyof responding to the main key. time out and extra key, as well as duration oftime out and resecnses to the main key during the time out.

Page 97: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

81

observed. These results reflect a pattern of responding to each leversimilar to that portrayed

by AR in Experiment I andrepresents a rather high andsustained rate to the main response

leverwith little variation or systematic relation of performance to theothertwo response levers.

The response patterns to each of the three levers bysubject PW. on theotherhand, does

showan interrelationship to the experimental conditions as shown in Figure 16. Thedecrease

in response rate in the topgraph is conversely paralled by an increase in timeout responding

andextra keyresponding in the second andthird graphs, respectively, during themaximum

effort condition. albeit this increase represents an exceedingly minimal change in the case of

timeout responding. Thisoccurrence of twotimeout events insession 29 represents thefirst

occurrence of a timeoutby PWsince theoccasion of strain during the first experiment in

session 9. So even though thisdoes notreflect a substantial increase. it is interesting bysake

of the conditions underwhich it occurs. Thatis, subject PWwasonlyobserved to engage in 11

timeoutsoverthe entire course of Experiments I & II,or otherwise overa totalof 37 sessions,

the first of which occurred shortly afterthe introduction of the timeoutcontingency. Of the

remaining 10timeouts. eight were engaged in during theconditions of strain reported during

sessions 9 & 10of Experiment I with the remaining timeouts occurring presently in session 29.

In addition to thetimeouts, an increase in extra key responding overbaseline levels canbe

clearly seen in Figure 16 in two of thefoursessions undertheexperimental condition.

Responses by subject LDto each of thethree response levers during thecourse of this

experiment can beobserved in the topthree graphs of Figure 17. Nosystematic relation can

be seenwith regard to the frequency of timeout responding overthe experimental conditions,

although in looking at thefourth graph a substantial increase induration of timeout occurs

during the firstsession following the response effort manipulation. The lowergraph portrays

the frequency of main key responding during the initiated timeoutsandthesimilarity of this

graph to the graph of timeout duration suggests thatthe timeoutsengaged in were active

rather thanpassive. That is, the subject continued to respond even during the timeout(s). It

maybe recalled thatthispattem was observed inthecenter cumulative record of Figure 14. In

thiscase. although the experimental manipulation did notparticularly influence the frequency

of timeout responding or the general response rate to the main key, a temporary increase in

theduration of timeout andof otherpatterns engaged in during the timeoutcanbe observed

to occurduring thesession following the experimental change.

As seen in thecenter graph of Figure 17.a slight increase in the frequency of extrakey

responding overbaseline levels canbeobserved during twosessions, the first and fourth,

Page 98: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

82

following the increase inresponse effort aswellalthough this is notsubstantial by anymeans.

Thefinal baseline of extra key response frequency has notbeen graphed as a further

experimental condition influencing theprobability of occurrence wasundertaken during

sessions 43-55. notshown. andas mentioned thiswill be discussed further in Experiment III.

As with theothertwo subjects of this experiment a higher frequency of responding occurs to

the extrarather thantimeoutkeyover all experimental conditions.

DISCUSSION

Experiment II examined the influence of response effort onfixedratio schedule

performance andconversely on performance deterioration or strain. Human operant

responding underthe control of large fixedratio schedules wasexamined underconditions of

minimum andmaximum response effort given the response effort constraints of the current

experimental situation. Increased response effort systematically decreased response rate for

each of three subjects andthese rates were observed to increase uponreversal of the

response effort requirements. Thereversal and pervasiveness of this effect across three

subjects each withdistinct experimental arrangements suggests a strong systematic replication

of the effect (Sidman 1960) however these performance decrements were notsubstantial.

approximately one response persecond. During conditions of maximum response effort

responding was observed to be sustained, in the case of ARat the rateof5 responses per

second. and generally around two responses per second in theothercases. In onlyoneof

thesecases was response deterioration substantial enough to meet the somewhat arbitrarily

established criteria of strained performance proposed in Experiment I. Hence. response

deterioration undertheconditions of increased response effort in Experiment II wasnot

commensurate withthedegree of strained performance observed following the large schedule

increases of Experiment I. Ontheotherhand. increased response effort systematically

disrupted previously established levels of responding as revealed by response rate analysis

andexamination of cumulative records.

It maybe thatthe degree of performance deterioration resulting from response effort

manipulations is related to the amount of response effort invoked during an increase. The

current experimental apparatus provided a limitation inthis regard. It maybe possible thatlarger

disruptions anddecreases in performance would result from similar experiments examining

Page 99: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

83

largerchanges in effort. Forexample if a plunger pull(Lindsley, 1960) or, at an extreme,

swinging a baseball batwere utilized as anoperant it might beexpected thatstrainwould occur

earlier undersimilar ratio conditions particularly if increasing degrees of resistance orweights

were attached to the plunger or bat. However under the current conditions, this experiment

suggests that established human operant responding is quite persistent even under

conditions of increasing response effort, although response effort doesinfluence

performance.

Analysis of response patterns to the extra and time outkeys in Experiment IIsuggest a

continuity of effectandpatterns observed in the first experiment. Observation of the response

patterns to thesekeys in Experiment I suggested thatthe patterns were in largepart induced

underconditions of variability in the referent main keycontingency andthat responding was

more likely to be seen to the extra rather thanthetimeout key. The experimental manipulation

in the current experiment produced lessdisruption in mainkeyperformance than that reported

earlier and as a result onewould expect to see inthisexperiment a smaller degree of response

variability andcontingency induced patterns to theextra and timeout key, as is the case. By

andlarge noconsistent increases infrequency occur to either thetimeoutor extra key asa

result of the experimental manipulation for the twosubjects showing the leastdegreeof main

key experimental effect, thosebeing AR andLD. Ofthe responding observed to the ancillary

keys by these subjects, a greater frequency of responding was observed to the extra rather

thantime out key.

Thegreatest observed mainkeyexperimental effect andtheonlycase in the current

experiment of performance decrement fullfilling theutilized strain criterion occurred in thecase

of subject PW. Response patterns to theancillary keyswere conversely observed to Increase

overbaseline levels during the increased response effort experimental manipulation

suggesting a pattern of induced responding to thetimeout and extrakeys similar to that

reported in the first experiment. Again responding was more frequent to the extrakeyrather

thanthe timeout keyalthough frequency of performance to each was low, particularly when

compared to the persistent patterns generated to the mainkeyby thatcontingency.

Although response frequency patterns to the ancillary keys by subject LDshowno striking

experimental influence, a more detailed analysis of theseandotherperformance measures

suggest otherwise. As reported, nosystematic increases or decreases in response frequency

wereobserved to occurto the extra or timeoutkeyover thecourse of this experiment,

although on close examination it mightbe noted thatthe two highest frequencies of extra key

Page 100: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

84

responding do occurduring the increased effort manipulation. Thiseffect is not large.

however. A second measure of timeout responding. thatof duration, suggests a systematic

relation to the introduction of the experimental condition aswell. Time out duration as revealed

in Figure 17 increases substantially above baseline levels during the initial session of

experimental change returning the nextsession to baseline levels. Figure 17alsoreports the

response frequency of a heretofore unreported behavior, thatof responding to the mainkey

during a timeout. Although response frequency of thisbehavior wasmonitored andrecorded

throughout this entire series of experiments it'spresence hasnotbeen reported otherthan in

this instance, asoccurrence wasgenerally negligible andnonsystematic. However. in the

present casean increase and decrement parallel to that reported for timeoutduration occurs

during the initial session of the increased response effort. Thisparallel pattern is explained in

thatthe cumulative record of thissession confirms the subject engaging in a sustained period

of main key responding during a lengthy time out. The significance of this point is in

suggesting thata variety of otherbehaviors. not necessarily even monitored, maybe

systematically related to changes iiime referent contingency relation. Heretofore, this pattern

of sustained responding to the main keyduring a time outby subject LDwas notreadily

apparent suggesting on a theoretical level thatenvironmental changes in oneparticular

contingency relation mayproduce fairlynovel yet related behavioral changes.

A second point of theoretical significance brought to lightby this experiment relates to the

present inducing conditions. In Experiment I behavioral patterns to the ancillary keys were

brought about by schedule changes andparticularly large schedule increases resulting in a

decrement of reinforcement density. In thecurrent experiment theschedule, perse,wasnot

manipulated. The inducing conditions consisted of a change in the response effort toward a

greater behavioral requirement while reinforcement density wasgenerally maintained although

sometimes slightly decreased. Thus, procedural change notnecessarily related to a direct

schedule change maybesufficient to produce changes in alternative behavioral patterns. The

significance of alternative behavioral patterns andtheirrelationship to ratio strain is the topic of

Experiment III. Generally, induced patterns seem to be more likely when procedural or

schedule changes result in lessfavorable environmental conditions, meaning an increase in

the response requirements or a decrease in the reinforcement density.

Page 101: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

85

EXPERIMENT '"

Responding waspersistent in Experiment II at large ratio values and underconditions of

increased response effort. Fortwoexperimental subjects responding under FR 4000

contingencies onlyoneor two schedule completions occurred per30 minute session, hence

reinforcement density wasquitelean. It seems paradoxical that responding might be maintained

underthese conditions, in onecase at the rate of overfive responses persecond. It is possible

that response maintenance and conversely response cessation underthese conditions, given

theseresults, might bea function of the alternatives available. Thatis, responding may be more

likely to persist underunfavorable conditions, as in the current research, when the organism or

subject hasa restricted range of alternative behavioral opportunities inwhich to engage inand

which subsequently might be reinforced.

Thatthe availability of alternatives inwhich to engage inmaybe important in the analysis of

operant behavior andschedule performance has been suggested theoretically (Goldiarnond,

1975; Hursh, 1980) aswellasempirically (Catania, 1969; Holz, Azrin, & Ayllon, 1963). For

example, the breaking point of progressive ratio performance is influenced by the availability of an

alternative response. In thissituation responding will be maintained forgreater periods of time

when; a) there is no reset alternative available (Hodos & Trumble, 1967); b) when responding to

thealternative is punished (Dardano & Sauerbraun, 1966; Dardano, 1968), and;c) when the

response frequency requirements on thealternative are increased (Hodos & Trumble, 1967).

Thus clearly the availability of an alternative response andthecontingency requirements involved

when responding to thisalternative are important to the understanding of progressive ratio

schedule performance andsubsequent analysis of response cessation.

Similarconclusions aredrawn fromananalysis of punishment procedures on human

schedule performance. Holz, Azrin, andAyllon (1963) andHerman andAzrin (1964) examined

the effects of twodistinct punishment procedures on human responding maintained by interval

schedules of reinforcement. When an alternative operant response wasavailable which

procurred reiriforcement, mildpunishment of theinitial andprimary response completely

eliminated thatbehavior. However, responding waspersistent even under conditions of

punishment when no alternative response wasavailable, albeit at a lowered rate.

In thecurrent series of experiments SUbjects areseated in a small room fora fixed period of

timewith nofurniture otherthanthe experimental equipment, and little space. Under these

Page 102: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

86

conditions it seems likely that responding may be maintained at highrates through the restriction

of opportunities to engage in otherbehaviors inherent in thissituation andthis restriction may

influence the occurrence of ratio strain. Experiment III examines me influences oi an available

alternative behavior onschedule performance andschedule disruption. It is specnically

postulated that strain, asused in the context of decreased frequency of performance, is more

likely to occur withtheadvent of an available alternative response, particularly underthe

unfavorable reinforcement conditions observed in Experiment II. Inthecurrent experiment the

alternative consists of thespecific introduction andremoval of a concurrent (Catania, 1969)

reinforcement contingency on a second existing leverwhich hasa higher reinforcement density

thanthatcontingency progranlrTled io ihe maln lever,

METHODS

SUbjects

Subjects LD and AR served asthe participants in thisexperiment. Subject AR also

participated in Experiments I & " andidentifying information maybefound inthe Subjects section

of the firstexperiment. Subject LDparticipated in Experiment II andidentifying information may

be found there.

Apparatus

Theexperimental room, control equipment andexperimental apparatus remained during this

experiment asdescribed in Experiment I, withoneexception. During the experimental

manipulation of this stUdy the SUbjects could engage in an alternative behavior to effectively

access reinforcement contingencies otherthanthe referent fixed ratio programmed on themain

response lever. Response performance onthe mainkeywasthenexamined as a function of this

relation to anavailable alternative. Thisalternative consisted of a concurrent highdensity

reinforcement schedule brought into effect during theexperimental manipulation on the

heretofore inconsequential center response lever, previously referred to as theextrakey. This

centerlever, then,served twofunctions. A conditional relation wasin effectwhereby when the

yellow reinforcement lights wereilluminated signalling the availability of points (see the Apparatus

section of Experiment I for details), thecenter lever served asa "consummatory" occasion

whereby a response incremented the bonus point total. When theyellowlightswere not

illuminated, andonlyduring the manipulation phase of this experiment, the centerkey served as

the occasion for the effective alternative pattern. During the baseline conditions of this

Page 103: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

87

experiment, thiscenter key had nospecific programmed consequences andwashence, the

extrakey. The response effort in effect on the main response leverduringthis entire experiment

wasthatof the maximum setting or 500gm. The response effort onthe time out andcenterkeys

approximated the minimum effort or 115gm.

Procedure

Experimental Design and History. Theexperimental design usedin this experiment

wasthatof an ABA reversal, similar to thatused in Experiment II. The current design wasactually'

embedded in a larger ABCBA experimental design (Hersen & Bellack, 1981) andis represented

by the BCB components. It can be recalled that in Experiment II, the experimental sessions were

notconsecutive for AR andLDfollowing the response keyforce manipulation andpriorto the

final baseline. This interruption wasdueto thecurrent experimental investigation. Thatis, in the

larger context of the A1 B1C1 B2A2 design, Experiment II focussed on theA1B1A2 reversal while

the current investigation focusses on the embedded B1C1B2 reversal. As a result of this, andin

contrast to Experiment II, the current experiment consists of consecutive experimental sessions.

Additionally, the relevant experimental history of each LDandARcanbedetailed by examining

Experiment II andExperiments I & II, respectively.

Theexperimental manipulation in thisstudy involved introducing a second concurrent ratio

schedule on a second response leverwhich had a significantly higher density of reinforcement

than thefirst and examining the relation of thisavailable alternative to response patterns on the

main key. No instructions asto the presence or nature of this contingency weregiven. The fixed

ratio on the main response leverremained constant throughout thisexperiment at FR 1000for

subject LDand FR4000 for AR.

Following sixsessions of baseline responding withno alternative, an arithmetic Progressive

Ratio 3 (PR3) schedule of reinforcement (3,6,9,12, 15, ...etc.) was introduced on the center

response leverfor subject LD. The PR3 remained in effect for three sessions afterwhich the no

alternative baseline conditions were reestablished. The highest progressive ratio completed

during thesethree sessions byLDwas PR 177.

Subject AR responded underfoursessions of no alternative baseline, foursessions of

manipulation where an alternative waspresent andthen five sessions of baseline following the

withdrawal of the alternative. The alternative present during thefirstsession of the experimental

manipulation wasanarithmetic Progressive Ratio 3 which held constant oncethe increment

Page 104: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

88

nun lfieySubject LO C-<J

Extra/Alt. Key>. -......Q1

~i::Jr--FA 1000Q1 c: 3.5 G:> 0 NO

"P4 (J 3.0 AItern8tive+J Q1 5Ill(/)

2.5c: \.0 4100 Ql 2.0Q1 C.+J 3~

'" 1.5-e Ql 2

'" 1.01:1 ce 0 .5 1

ID C.

c: '" .0 4 4 4 4 4 4 0"P4 III 4344 45 4647 48 495051525354 55ilia:1:

SUbject AR

FA 4000I I

~Witll .r-AlterMt1ve

I110 cl~ 110

Altt!rMtivlI

Alt1!rnlltive

.. I ,I~I4 4 4 .... II I

2829 30 31323334 353637 383Q 40

>. 20 80CIII

\:::lcr 15 6QI~

II.... 10 4:::l0QI 5 2EI- 0 0

I

I

1III

III~:v

d II I I2829 3031 323334 35 36 37 38:m 40

28 29 3031323334 35363738:m 40

I-01

.-~IIIIQ.-.Q....gI

D n g g I CI n g g g

II• 00

2.00

1.00

3.00

$11. 00

43« 454647 4849505152535455

~

~ a- Cl g g

.00

2.00

1.00

3.00

$ 4.00

CIt01C

C~

IIIWCo..CIt1IIen

Sessions Sessions

Figure 18. Frequency of responding to the main key. the extra/alternative key. andthe time out key by subjects LD & AA in all conditions of Experiment III. and totalearnings per session. The left hand column of graphs portrays data for subject LD.while data for subject AR can be seen in the graphs of right hand column.

Page 105: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

89

reached 12(3, 6, 9, 12,12,12, etc.). For allpractical purposes thiswasan FR 12. Thealternative

available during the finalthree sessions of the experimental manipulation consisted of a

Geometric Ratio 2 witha terminal FR500 component (2,4, 8, 16,32,64,128,256,500,500,

500, etc.). Criteria for stability andstrain remained during thisexperiment asdefined previously

during the first experiment. Otherprocedures were asspecified earlier.

RESULTS

Theperformance moasures presented in discussing the results of this experiment include

cumulative records and graphs of response frequency to each of the threeresponse panel

levers. Additionally, graphed dataon monetary earnings persession will be presented asa

measure of reinforcement density in the baseline and experimental conditions.

Figure 18presents these graphed datafor bothsubjects, LDandAR. Data for subject LD

aregraphed inthe lefthand column, while dataforARareto the right. Thetopgraph in each

column represents mainkeyresponse rate for each subject, while also reflecting responses to

the alternative/extra key. Response rate to bothof these keysis graphed in responses per

second. The middle graphin each column portrays response frequency to the timeoutkey

during each condition while, finally, thelowergraph presents monetary earnings per session over

thecourse of this experiment for eachsubject.

The results of this experiment aresimilar for each subject. When presented with the

alternative, the heretofore persistent high rate main keyperformance quickly decreases, while

conversely, the previously lowrate and intermittent response patterns to theextrakey increase

substantially as a result of the attached accessible highdensity reinforcement contingency.

When thissecond contingency is removed a gradual yetsubstantial reversal of effect is observed

with an increase in the frequency of time outsalsoaccompanying thisdecrease in reinforcement

density. Thedecrease in main key response pattern as reflected in the top leftgraph of Figure 18

is immediate and, for all practical purposes, totalforsubject LD. Thisrepresents a complete

switch fromresponding on the main lever to responding on the alternative lever. This is in

contrast to the pattern evident in the top right graph. Subject ARcontinued to respond, although

at a lowerrate, on the main lever while substantially increasing response rates to the alternative

keyas well. During the experimental manipulation, in both of these cases, main key response

rate decreases infrequency to a pointsubstantially below the proposed 20%strain criterion.

Page 106: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

90

Figure 19. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment III by Subject LD. Thecenter record shows the initial session ofresponding during which a concurrent alternative schedule of reinforcement was available,while upperandlower records represent thepreandpostmanipulation baseline sessions,respectively. A complete collection of all cumulative records generated during this experimentis included in Appendix I.

Page 107: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

l!'R 1000 110 Alternative

PR 1000 with .u ternative

)~---------i-IB --.I.

91

1)),n,,11 "1,,",, n t ) , ) ) , ;

PR 1000 110 llternative

Page 108: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

92

Visual observation reveals thatthispattem of reponding by ARwasessentially a procedural

artifact in thata superstitious pattern of responding was reinforced wherein the subject would

alternately press the mainkey and thenthe alternative key in high rate succession

(main-alt.-main-alt., etc.), andthispattem was reinforced witheach completion of the alternative

keyschedule requirements. If thetwo keyshadbeenarranged to be independent, either

physically in space or behaviorally through programming, it is suspected that the realized

response patterns would parallel those of subject LD. Theresults of this inefficient response

pattern is reflected in a loweroverall reinforcement density for ARwhen contrasted to LD,during

thefinal threesessions of the experimental manipulation in the lowergraph of Figure 18.

Cumulative Records

Sample cumulative records fromeach of the three conditions of this experiment for SUbject

LDcanbe seenin Figure 19. The pre and post(experimental manipulation) baseline patterns are

shown in the upperand lower records, respectively, andto a large extent theserecords

represent schedule performance by LDsimilar to that portrayed and discussed inExperiment II. A

number of briefpauses occurduring the ratio runs andbriefPRPs occuras well. An extended

pause occurs during the initial portion of the lowerrecord andan estimate quiteconsistent with

thegraphed datawouldbethatduring this pause, thesubject wasresponding to the extrakeyas

a result of the historical andfavorable reinforcement conditions. Although speculative it is likely

thatduring this pause the subject wasresponding to theextrakey(some post-manipulation

responding is evident in Figure 18), sortof "feeling out"potential environmental conditions and

returning finally to the triedandtruemain keyupon environmental acknowledgement of a lackof

contingency.

Thecentercumulative record of Figure 19shows the first session of the experimental

manipulation andthe immediate decrease in response rate to the main key is evident. Initially, at

A, a number of responses can beseen, however responding to this leverquickly stops. At B a

timeout is engaged in asshown by the reset. During thiscondition, only, the reinforcement

marker along thebottom of the record wasattached to thealternative keyandmarked everytime a

reinforcer waspresented in thecontext of the alternative contingency. As can be seen

reinforcement is presented consistently throughout the session, representing sustained

responding to the alternative key. The interreinforcement interval canbe observed to increase

steadily as would be expected under the progressive ratio schedule.

Page 109: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

93

Figure 20. Sample cumulative records portraying response patterns generated during eachcondition of Experiment IIIby Subject AR. Theupperleft and lower right records represent thepre and postmanipulation baseline records, respectively. The upperright record shows thefirst of four sessions during which a concurrent alternative schedule of reinforcement wasavailable, whilethe lowerleft record shows thefinal session of when the alternative wasavailable. A complete collection of all cumulative records generated during this experiment isincluded in Appendix I.

Page 110: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

..

..

94

....0

•. ~..~ .... •..

E•§ .... .... ;0 ..• •~ ~.. .... ..

Page 111: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

95

Cumulative records from representative sessions for subject ARcan be seen in Figure 20.

Thefirst andfourth records represent the preandpostbaseline patterns of responding while the

second andthirdportray the firstandlastsessions of the experimental manipulation, respectively.

Responding underbothbaseline sessions canbeobserved to be of a high rateandconsistent

nature. Thispattern characterizes thefirsthalfof the initial session where the alternative was

present, as well.

Thedevelopment of response patterns to the alternative key in thesecond graph can be

observed by examining the reinforcement of responses to thealternative keyduring this

experimental condition. Following the initial schedule completion of the FR4000contingency

programmed on the main response lever, an initial alternative schedule iscompleted atA. Main

key responding remains in effect at the previous rate, however, untilC,when it deteriorates to a

much lower frequency, breakand run pattern. At B, andeven while the mainkeypattern is

strong, responding begins to occur more consistently to the the alternative key as reflected by

the reinforcement marks on the bottom of thecumulative record. This marks the beginning of the

aforementioned two-handed alternating pattern whichbecomes fully developed by D. The

period of C through D reflects a transition phase priorto the development of this two-handed

response pattern andthe period of the greatest strain with regard to themain key response

pattern.

The cumuiative record from the final session of the experimental manipulation forARshown

inthe thirdgraph of Figure 20shows a more or lessstable pattern of responding, at the rate

evident at D in theprevious record. A fewbrief timeoutsearly inthe session andseveral brief

pauses and/or intermittent rate changes areobservable during thissession, but for themostpart

the rate is consistent. The reinforcement marks on the bottom of the record suggest consistent

response patterns to the alternative key aswell. In addition to reinforcement available fromthe

alternative contingency. an instance of reinforcement fromfulfillment of the referent FR 4000

contingency can beobserved at A.

DISCUSSION

Experiment IIIexamined thepersistence of human operant performance as a function of

alternatives. Under conditions of high response requirement. lowreinforcement density,

increased response effort, andtimeout arrangements explicitly designed to minimize excess

Page 112: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

96

responding, performance wassustained for two subjects when accessibility to a reinforced

alternative wasnotpresent. Mainkeyperformance wasobserved to immediately and pervasively

decline to levels defined asstrained upon the introduction of an accessible alternative

reinforcement contingency andthis effect wasshown to be reversible upon removal of the

alternative. Hence, response decrement of human subjects wassubstantially influenced by the

availability of a response alternative.

The leveland immediacy of performance decrement in this situation suggests that the

. provision of an alternative is an effective method in reduction of behavior. Main key ref,pondir.g

wasnearly completely eliminated in thecase ofonesubject andsubstantially reduced I.:'y th::l

second subject. Thatthissecond pattern wasnotcompletely reduced was attributable to a

procedural artifact. Thebehavioral changes observed anddefined asstrained in thisexperiment

areremarkable when compared to performance decreases in Experiments I & II in themagnitude

andimmediacy of effect andthe ensuing stability of decreased performance levels. Hence, ratio

strain when examined from these perspectives wasnotcommensurate withstrain observed in

earlier experiments, although aspects of commonality areapparent.

It wassuggested in Experiment I that ratio strain was if notprimarily a transition phenomenon

thena stimulus control phenomenon closely resembling patterns observed during transition

phases. Thegreatest degree of performance disruption observed in the current experiment

appeared likewise as a transition phenomena, thatbeing in the case of subject ARasrevealed

briefly in the first session following the altemative introduction aswitnessed by cumulative record

in Figure 20. However, bythe endof thissession performance hadstabalized at the rate and

pattern observed throughout the remaining manipulation period. Inothercommonalities the

magnitude of observed effect was most closely related to that seen in Experiment I while later

stability wasparallel to patterns of Experiment 11.

Response patterns to the time outkeywere clearly observed to be related to contingency

change conditions influencing reinforcement density andsimilarpatterns wereobserved for both

subjects in thecurrent experiment. Time out responding wasseen to increase when overall

experimental reinforcement density decreased. Thiseffectappeared transitory in nature

supporting previous suggestions of timeout response patterns under ratio schedules as being

induced in nature. In particular timeout responding increased substantially during thefirst two

sessions following the experimental reversal and thenreturned to baseline levels. Thisincrease

paralleled a substantial decrease in overall reinforcement density which hadbeen elevated bythe

experimental manipulation. TIme out response frequency waselevated above baseline levels

Page 113: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

97

during the final three of four experimental sessions forsubject ARaswell, however this elevation

wasconsistent withthe current analysis in thata substantial decrease in reinforcement density

occurred fromthe initial experimental session wherein the subject earned approximately $11.00.

Increases in timeout responding did notoccurfollowing contingency change conditions

towards favorable experimental arrangements but rather onlytowards decreases inoverall

reinforcement density, in this particular experiment. When examining the present conditions

inducing response patterns to the ancillary timeoutkey, it is interesting to note that nochanges

occurred duringtheentire course of this experiment in the defined referent main key

contingency, although behavior patterns underthe control of thiscontingency were seento be

substantially effected. In this situation an analysis of the inducing context must necessarily

examine inter-contingency relations, for contingency induction waseffected by the introduction

andremoval of a concurrently arranged experimental contingency. Analysis of behavior in terms

of intersecting sets of contingencies (Goldiamond, 1975; 1984) certainly complicates traditional

behavioral formulations suchasthose in the case of ratio strain or schedule-induction, however

such analysis is apparently necessary to a thorough functional analysis.

Goldiamond (1975) presents a discussion andframework for thedescription andanalysis of

interacting alternative setsof contingencies. Current methods of analysis examining behavior

onlyas a directfunction of an influencing referent contingency arepresented as linearsystems of

analysis and contrasted withnonlinear analyses. Under nonlinear analyis, observed behavior

patterns are examined in the context of a jointfunction of the referent contingency andoneor

more alternative and influencing contingencies. Rationality of seemingly irrational behavior is

defined in thecontext of available alternative behaviors, contingencies, and jointeffects.

Forexample, as reported by Goldiamond, nondiscriminated avoidance responding is

paradoxical when examined linearly in the traditional methods of operant analysis. Behavior

underthe control of nondiscriminated avoidance tends to occurwith noobservable

consequence in that responding serves to postpone anotherwise imminent shock. Hence

linearly in the context of the three-term operant contingency, given theoccasion of the

experimental situation a response is followed by no discernible event. Onthe surface this

appears to be "irrational" behavior from aconsequential perspective andin appearance this

imitates extinction, yetavoidance responding is quite likely to persist for longperiods of time,

evenafterformal extinction procedures areundertaken (if, for example, the machine dispensing

the aversive consequences of not responding wereunplugged). When examined froma

nonlinear perspective this behavior is understood or "makes sense" in that responding is a

Page 114: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

98

function of alternatives. Onehasto lookat theconsequences of not responding as wellas of

responding to seethis rationality. That is,altemative contingency arrangements, inthiscase

shock, become probable whenno responding isobserved andthisstateof affairs more or less

forces the occurrence of an "irrational" behavior (that is, responding for noobservable

consequences), asdefined linearly.

A procedural limitation exists inthecurrent experimental analysis of ratio strain. Results of

this experiment clearly suggest the importance of available response opportunities andthe

contingency relations thereof in theanalysis of behavioral persistence anddecrement. However

the procedures undertaken to make thispoint areperhaps notthose most relevant to the

examination of ratio strain. Ratio strain when examined inthecontext of a myriad ofavailable

alternative behavioral opportunities refers to conditions which depotentiate or make less

probable a referent contingency relation andpresumably make more probable an alternative or

number of alternative existing contingencies. These depotentiating conditions would include

the current topicsof investigation among others.

Ratio strainwith regard to a referent contingency would refer to theconditions andpointat

which a freely available alternative was undertaken, andthis is the basis of the present procedural

limitation. In thecurrent experiment thealternative wasconcurrently introduced and later

withdrawn to affect response disruption. Ideally, onewould hope to examine ratio strain as a

function of existent alternatives andthose conditions influencing referent performanca versus

the engagement of an alternative. Forexample, it might be interesting to examine at which point

of increasing effort or requirement a particular alternative contingency relation becomes

prepotent when a number of topographically andfunctionally distinct altematives arefreely

available to a subject performing under schedule control. Ratio strain with regard to the referent

contingency in thissituation would referto the switching pointwhere analternative was

undertaken rather thanto the complete cessation of performance. Although analysis of

performance decrement in the context of freely available alternatives wasnotundertaken in the

current study, preliminary examination of thisarea suggests the present findings hold in that

responding is more likely to deteriorate or show strain in thepresence of a freely available

alternative than in theabsence of such alternative givenan increase in schedule requirements.

That is, performance is morelikely to besustained when noaltemative is present. The interested

reader is referred to Appendix II fora preliminary single-case inquiry into thisarea.

Page 115: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

99

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Ratio strain, or thedisruption of established patterns of operant perfonnance, wasexamined

in a series of three experiments specifically observing theconditions under which performance

decrement occurred. Performance of human responders wassubstantially affected bythe

degree of increase in a regimen of increasing fixed ratio schedules withperformance

deterioration onlyoccurring with relatively large schedule increases (Exp. I),by thepresence or

absence of alternative behavioral opportunities (Exp. III), andto a lesser extent bythe response

effort requirements of the operant involved (Exp. II).

In each of these three experiments patterns of response decrement deemed to be strained

were qualitatively andquantitatively different. Performance decrement in Exp. I was immediate

andprogressive inthatpatterns of responding became variable withthe advent of larger

schedule increases. Thisvariability, specifically response frequency decrement, occurred

increasingly overensuing sessions. In Exp. III perfonnance deterioration, although immediate

andsubstantial, reflected a stable level ofdecrement. Deterioration in Exp. IIwaslesssubstantial,

although immediate, andagain represented a stable levelof decrement.

Thediversity in experimental conditions producing behavioral decrement and thediversity in

the behavioral topographies produced under these conditions suggests thatstrained

performance canbe a function of multiple variables. These experiments have attempted to more

closely examine conditions producing ratio strain and these results leadto conclusions similar in

nature to those reported in a recent study by Peden andTimberlake (1984) whoconcur that ratio

strain "is nota unitary phenomena".

Often times researchers andclinicians implicitly imply thatratio strain isa unitary

phenomenon. It canbe seen upon closer scrutiny of theuseof ratio strain as a behavioral

descriptor, that theterm is readily accepted andused bythis population, and further thatthisuse

is specific to thedescription of performance decrement. However, thecurrent research and

literature reviewed suggests thatthissingularity of useat bestcamouflages a number of

experimental andbehavioral relationships andthrough precedent of terminological acceptance

precludes ongoing analysis of performance decrement relationships.

Thistraditional perspective of ratio strain is based ontheperspective of strain asa

construct-like phenomenon. That is, ratio strain is seen assomething that occurs. Anorganism

stops responding because it has "ratio strain". In actuality perfonnance either isor is notoccurring

Page 116: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

100

andthis responding or lackof responding canbe analyzed as a function of controlling variables. It

is thisdetailed analysis that is precluded by theevocation of strain.

The importance of controlling variables andenvironmental control in performance and

conversely, in response cessation is implicitly yetdistinctly acknowledged in thecurrent usage of

thedescriptor ratio strain bytheconditions underwhich it is used. Forexample, ratio strain

universally refers to performance decrement underthecontrol of ratio reinforcement schedules.

Often times, but notexclusively, strain is saidto be occurafterresponse decrement when the

ratio requirement is high, or afteran increase in the schedule requirements or perhaps aftera

decrease in reinforcement density. It is through these commonalities of performance decrement

conditions that ratio strain is reified to the extent that it is accepted asa construct influencing

behavior.

The purpose of thecurrent research hasnotbeento suggest that ratio strain is anantiquated

or useless term, for thecurrent widespread usage canbe considered onebarometer of value, but

rather to directattention in this usage to those specific conditions influencing the performance

decrement or disruption indicated by thedescriptor. Inthis sense ratio strainmight be thought of

more as a concept thana construct. That is,asa concept, ratio strain canbe defined bya

description of effect under the conditions of usage (Markle andTiemann, 1969). Inborrowing an

example from Markle andTiemann, a number of objects varying in topography might be

conceptualized as, or called, a chair. In actuallity, a chairis distinguished fromother sitting-objects

(stools, loveseats, beach lounge chairs, etc.) by onlythree defining attributes. That is, a chairhas

a backsupport, seats a single person, and sitting occurs at an appropriate (knees bentat a 90

degree angle) level. In defining these critical characteristics present when people label "chair",

we are not onlyableto identify irrelevant attributes thatmaycloud perception, but can promote

generalization and discrimination in useof thedescriptor "chair". Hence, we areapproaching the

essence of theconcept chairthrough definition of environmental conditions present upon

attribution of the concept. When analyzed in this regard ratio strain canbe observed to be a

performance decrement metaphor defined by theconditions present uponattribution. This

analysis directs theconfirmation of ratio strain towards the identification of generating or in this

case, de-generating conditions.

Ratio strain inthe context of the present analysis andexperiments refers to thebehavioral

effects of a specific class of operant decremental operations. Onlyasubset of alloperant

decremental operations are relevant to ratio strain. Forexample operant decrement brought

about through theuseof punishment, extinction, conditioned ernosonat reaction procedures,

Page 117: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

101

andphysical restraint areusually conceptualized as independent of ratio strain. Additionally, it was

earlier suggested that response decrement brought about by fatigue was not relevant to ratio

strain, norwas decrement resulting froma lengthening of the PRP.

Onthe otherhand the present experiments clearly identify several conditions of operant

performance decrement which maybe relevant to ratio strain. Certainly the present experiments

examine performance decrement underratio schedules, a precursor to thedevelopment of

strain, andspecifically underhigh ratio schedules, which have beenimplicated as being related to

ratio strain by a number of authors. As may begathered operant decrement, perse, maynotbe

directly relevant to thatwhich hastraditionally been conceptualized as ratio strain. However, an

understanding of what is mostrelevant to ratio strain mayperhaps bebestdelineated through

comprehensive examiiiaiioiiof decremental operations andthrough an ensuing process of

procedural classification.

The present experiments thenclearly identify several conditions of operant pertormance

decrement. Undoubtably, a number of otherconditions leadto performance disruption aswell

andfurther research in these areas will provide a comprehensive analysis of ratio decrement and

hence strain. In Exp. I the influence of program increases in performance decrement was

observed. Large increases wereshown to produce strain whilesmaller step increments

produced undisrupted response continuity at highratios. Significantly, strained performance was

notobserved at very high ratio values when response alternatives werenot present tc the

responders. Thisexperiment suggests thatthe program usedto establish responding is a critical

element in the generation of strained performance. This experiment alsosuggests that the

ultimate ratio values incorporated maynotbe important undercertain program conditions, that is

in response situations where no response alternatives exist. Exp. IIIshowed performance

decrement at these same highratio values when alternative response opportunities were

available to the responders. Thissuggests thatanother critical element in examining

performance decrement lies in the available response alternatives.

Although not explicitly examined in thisseries of experiments. reinforcement magnitude also

appears to be a variable influencing performance disruption underratio schedules (Peden and

Timberlake, 1984). Forexample the authors report that pigeons' responding wasmaintained

underconsiderably higherratios when theywere working for unlimited access to grainunder a

closed experimental economy, whereby all obtained food wasprocured duringexperimental

sessions as reinforcement. Under a similar experimental arrangement Findley reports that

pigeons will maintain responding at FR20,000 (asreported in Whaley and Malott, 1971)1 In a

Page 118: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

102

second experiment Peden andTimberlake report that strain was more Iikey to occurwhenthe

control of reinforcement access by a timerproduced smaller rather than larger reinforcement

magnitudes underincreasing FRs. Thus. strain has been postulated to occurdifferentially under

increasing FRswhen reinforcement density isdirectly manioulated,

A final response decrement operation examined in the current research pertains to the

response effortrequirements. Exp. II examined performance decrease occurring as a function of

increased response effort requirements. Under theconditons examined in the current research

response effortmanipulations didnotconsistently produce strained performance. While

response effortmanipulations consistently produced performance decrement. onlyoneof three

subjects showed a decrement substantial enough to meet the designated 20%response

decrement level selected for identifying strained performance.

Thedecreased response levels observed underthese procedural conditions were notthen

commensurate with those seen inthe otherexperiments of thisseries. These results suggest

that although response effort variables are important in perfonnance decrement and in selecting

operant levels of performance. theymay notbe specifically implicated in the occurrence of strain

astraditionally formulated. at least under theconditions observed in thisexperiment.

Performance decrement occasioned by response effort increases showed a stabilization of

effect. Thisstabilization leads one to suspect thateffortmanipulations perhaps produce

decreases in performance through theprovision of physical constraints on the frequency of

behavior ratherthanthrough other means. when a relatively high rate operant selected for

analysis is examined as inthe present case andwhen relatively low response forcerequirements

are incorporated. again as in thepresent case.

Although ratio strain hasbeen defined rather precisely in theory. by a number of authors. the

preceding review suggests thatthese definitions areinconsistent. In practice. strainis implicitly

defined asanyof a number of performance decrement situations traditionally summarized as

those situations in which the ratio is "too large" to sustain performance. It is currently argued that

a greater specification of behavior-environment relations during perfonnance decrement will lead

to a more complete understanding anddefinition of the concept of rntlo strain.

Page 119: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

103

Given this, a definition consistent withthisthesis might be that ratio strain refers to the

behavioral effects of a speciflc class of operant decremental operations whereby ongoing

performance decreases in frequency under fixed or variable ratio schedules of reinforcement asa

function of:

a) the increment program utilized in increasing ratio requirements;b) alternative contingency arrangements;c) arrangements of reinforcement magnitude anddensity;d) response effort requirements (although not explicitly suggested by the

present results); ande) presumably otherevents currently unspecified.

Ontheotherhand, ratio strain is exclusive of the PRP, that is, it does notreferto decrements in

response probability occurring as thePRP increases, nordoes it referto physical fatigue or other

currently defined performance decrement operations (stimulus change, extinction, restraint, CER

procedures, etc.).

When examined and defined in thismanner, andalthough a defining characteristic of strained

performance isoccurrence underratio schedules, it is suggested that similar procedures will affect

interval schedule performance aswell. However, to date theconcept of "interval strain" is

nonexistent. Thisis notanaltogether undesirable stateof affairs inthat investigators examining

interval schedules areforced to more closely define the procedures affecting performance rather

than envoking strain. It is suggested that a similar courtesy bepaidto those conditions of operant

decrement underratio schedules of reinforcement.

As it isunlikely thatthiscourtesy will be respected, ratio strain as a descriptor is perhaps most

appropriately reserved for description of the typeof performance decrement observed in the first

experiment herein, thatoccurring as a result of program increases. Thereasons for thissuggestion

aretwofold. First this is in linewithcurrent usage whereby ratio strain is posited to occur "when ratio

requirements areveryhigh or are raised abnJptly" (Sulzer-Azaroff andMayer, 1977). Secondly, this

targets aneasily identifiable procedural condition, whereas for example, "too large of ratio" is as

discussed influenced by a host of procedural arrangements. In thiscontext strain describes a

transition/stimulus control phenomenon whereby performance decreases occuras a function of

changes in stimulus control withperformance maintenance affected byprocedures enhancing the

transfer of stimulus control during schedule changes.

The nature of response patterns occurring to thetimeoutkey in thecurrent research suggest

thatthe aversive emotional components oftenattributed to strained performance mayneed to be

further researched and documented. Ratio strain, perhaps more thananyothercurrent concept in

Page 120: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

104

behavior analysis, is heavily anthropomorphic in use. Thatis, ratio strain suggests in usethe

uniquely human attributes of burnout, frustration, depression, etc. Thistendency results from

traditional formulations attributing aversive emotional components to highratio schedules (Azrin,

1961). Currently observed patterns indicate thattimeout responding is more of a

contingency-induced phenomenon and lesslikethe traditional escape from positive reinforcement

formulation. It is suggested that thisanthropomorphism is unwarranted.

Inthe current research timeout responding wassystematically observed to occurunder

conditions of increased variability in performance underthe control of the referent main key

contingency. To the extent thatperformance variability wasgenerated following a substantial

increase in ratio requirements, thecurrent results werein agreement withtraditional timeout

analyses. That is, increases in timeoutswereobserved withincreasing schedule requirements.

However, when theprogram of schedule increases wasgradual showing sustained main key

performance theseresults were notseen. In fact. otherobserved conditions of timeout

responding in the current experiments suggest that this behavior is undercomplex control andthat

the mechanisms governing timeout responding aresubstantially different fromthe postulated

escape fromincreasingly aversive conditions associated with increasing ratio requirements. The

introduction of thetimeout contingency in the current research was consistently observed to

decrease responding to the keyonwhich thatcontingency was imposed suggesting thatthe time

outwas in fact a punishment procedure, a point supported bycomparative examination of response

patterns to each thetimeout andextra keys.

It is suggested that response patterns observed in the current research to boththe timeout

key andthe ancillary extra key were undercontingeny-induced governance. That is, responding to

these keyswas induced by a variety of change-conditions in the referent main keycontingency. A

variety of generating conditions were observed in the present experiments including

contingency-change in the form of increasing schedule requirements andparticularly those large

increases producing strain (Exp. I),extinction (Exp. I), increases in response effort (Exp. II),and

nuances in inter-contingency relationships, specifically decreases in overall experimental

reinforcement density produced by extinction of a concurrent contingency (Exp. III). Ingeneral this

analysis of contingency-induced responding is consistent with Zimmerman and Ferster's (1964)

analysis of timeout responding. They postulated thattimeout responding occurred asa relatively

weakalternative behavior increasing in frequency underconditions of schedule variability.

Page 121: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

105

Patterns of timeout performance by human responders weretherefore consistent with those

reported by Zimmerman andFerster whoexamined infrahuman response patterns. Ingeneral the

human operant response patterns currently observed weresimilarto patterns evolving out of the

animal laboratory, although performance wasobserved to be sustained at quite highvalues with

littlepost-reinforcement pausing, a finding previously acknowledged by invesitigators examining

human operant performance (Sanders, 1969). An interesting avenue of research with regard to

post reinforcement pausing issuggested by Experiment IIIbpresented in Appendix II. Substantial

PRPs wereobserved when an alternative wasavailable to the subject. Theexperimental analysis of

alternative response opportunities is particularly germane to understanding human performance in

thateveryday environmental conditions typically provide a widerange of alternatives. The narrow

range of alternatives inherent in the laboratory studyof human operant performance is undoubtably

onecontributing factorto the large ratio values shown to sustain responding in thecurrent

experiments. These large FRs areparticularly notable in lightof thefactthattypical studies of

human operant responding examine performance at values considerably lessthanexamined

herein. Forexample. studies of human FRperformance haveexamined behavior underthe control

of ratio schedules ranging anywhere from 1-1000 responses whereas FR values of 1000, 2000,

4000. & 8000were currently programmed. Further examination of theeffects of alternatives on

behavior andof human performance underlarge schedules is warranted.

There area number of clinical implications of the present experimental analysis of ratio strain.

Forexample, Ferster (1973) hassuggested thatstrainandschedules of reinforcement are

important in the analysis of clinical depression. That is.depressed individuals may be suffering from

"ratio strain". Thecurrent research suggests several pertinent points. Thefirstof which is the

recognition thatstrain is a multicomponent phenomena andthat depression astheoretically tiedto

ratio strain is undoubtably likewise, in thatdifferent "types" of depression are likely to share

commonalities withdifferent "types" of strain. In otherwords, it is likely thatdistinctions in cases of

clinical depression will arise asa result of distinct behavior-environment relations. Thepresent

research thensuggests that individual cases of depression should be individually examined to

ascertain the specific environmental conditions influencing the depressed behavior. Tothe extent

that ratio strain is but onecontributing component to clinical depression as suggested by Ferster, it

is likewise important thateach case of clinical depression be approached asa particular ideographic

set of contingencies to be addressed rather thanthe currently acceptable division of depressed

disorders intomanic, reactive, bipolar, cyclothymic, dysthymic, unipolar, or various permutations

thereof.

Page 122: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

106

Given this ideographic approach, the current research suggests at least two procedural

conditions producing depressed behavior or affect andasa result suggests assessment and

treatment approaches. As mentioned earlier andasexamined in Experiment I, ratio strain can occur

as a result of changing environmental conditions producing a lossof stimulus or schedule control. It

might be expected that the verbal behavior of patients in thissituation may reflect this

environmental condition in that theymay"notknow what is expected of them" or otherwise may not

be ableto describe controlling schedules of reinforcement. In this regard a confusion over

appropriate behavior mayabound as in the case of thedepressed individual exclaiming things like,

"I don'tknowwhatyou expect from me," or "things usedto besosimple," or "justtellmewhatthe

(expletive) you wantmeto do". Thisview of depression to some extent approximates the learned

helplessness theoryin thata historical program of development leads to behavior notunder

defined stimulus control.

Depression mayalso be linked or arise as anoffshoot of the alternatives available to an

individual. Thecurrent research suggests thatanexamination of alternatives is critical in

understanding behavioral persistence anddecrement. Although topographically onemight argue

that depression or depressed behavior is relevant to the reduced frequency of behavior observed

upon the introduction of an alternative in actuality it seems more likely thatdepression may arise in

the converse situation where noalternatives arepresent, or depression itselfmaybethe only

available alternative. Inthissituation thecultivation of altematives andconstruction of behavioral

repertoires through skill-building interventions seem warranted.

Several points of clinical relevance to theelimination andconstruction of behavior evolve from

the current research. The persistance of behavior observed underconditions of extinction and

response effort suggest that these arenotveryviable procedures to eliminate behavior when that

behavior iscritical in a setof altematives, that iswhen noalternatives areavailable. Ontheother

hand the introduction of an accessible alternative wasseen to substantially eliminate behavior and

this suggests a valuable clinical procedure. The construction of alternatives as a specific

intervention in theelimination of disturbing behavior hasbeen termed theConstructional Approach

by Goldiamond (1974). Finally, theprogramming process utilized when developing behavioral

repertoires wasseento becritical in thecurrent research. Significant repertoires were developed

whenprogram increases weresystematic andgradual andnototherwise.

Several research areas aresuggested when looking toward potential future extensions of this

program of inquiry. A number of these have been suggested overthiscourse of discussion.

Perhaps first and foremost, further quantification anddelineation of operant decremental

Page 123: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

107

operations iswarranted in a continuing intensive analysis of thatwhich hastradltlonally been

summarized as strained performance. Secondly, a number of interesting contingency change

relations andcontingency-induced patterns havecome to lightin thecurrent study. It would be

interesting to pursue these issues andexamine theenvironmental conditions generating and

maintaining historical and novel induction. It has traditionally been assumed thatstimulus change is

onecondition likely to produce performance disruption (Azrin, 1959) andhence set the occasion

for behavioral variability. Thecurrent study suggests thatperhaps such change-induction is a more

pervasive phenomena, withstimulus change procedures being onesubset of a larger

contingency-change phenomenon. Forexample, schedule changes have been shown to induce

ancillary keyresponding in thecurrent research ashave response effort changes. It is possible that

each individual element of a contingency relationship issusceptible to generating induced

behavioral patterns upon change. Such elements might include deprivationallevels, schedule

relationships, stimulus control relationships, consequences, behavioral requirements, ambient

stimuli, instructional andabstractional requirements, etc., for example. In addition thecurrent

research suggests thatdifferential patterns of induced responding are related to the alternatives

present andthis areaof research maybeenlightning.

It hasalsobeen suggested thatan analysis of alternatives andhuman operant performance

mayprovide information on post reinforcement pausing by humans. Itwould be worthwhile to

systematically replicate the single case preliminary inquiry presented inAppendix II examining

human performance, ratio strain, andthe influence of alternatives. The lackof research with human

responders at large ratio values suggests a further area of stUdy.

Finally, an area of research notaddressed bythecurrent study concerns theanalysis and

assessment of verbal behavior, private events, andsubjective states by human subjects under

similar experimental control. Current experimental procedures andcontrols prohibited intensive

examination of these phenomena yet informal observation suggests that relationships between

public andprivate events would be aninteresting area of analysis. Numerous investigations in this

areaare possible however with regard to the current focus it would be interesting to examine

subjects verbal report andsubjective impressions of contingency control underconditions of

response maintenance andstrain. Thistype of study may provide valuable information asto, for

example, the purported aversive nature of highratio schedules or feelings of helplessness, etc.,

underconditions of lossof schedule control and may bea particularly valuable adjunct in the

extension of laboratory studies to the clinic.

Page 124: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

108

APPENDIX I

Cumulative records of sessions 1-44for subject ARcanbeseen onpages 109-112. Subject ARparticipated in Experiments I. II,& III.

Cumulative records of sessions 1-48 for subject IN canbeseen onpages 113-116. Subject IN participated in Experiment I.

Cumulative records ofsessions 1-37forsubject PWcanbeseenon pages 117-120. Subject PWparticipated in Experiments I &II.

Cumulative records of sessions 1-20 forsubject HNcanbeseen onpages 121-122. Subject HN participated in Experiment I.

Cumulative records of sessions 34-58 for subject LDcan beseenon pages 123-125. Subject LDparticipated in Experiments II & III.

Cumulative records of sessions 1-38 for subject LMcan beseenon pages 126-129. Subject LMparticipated in Experiment IIIb.

On the following pages. cumulative records andresponse frequency information to each of

the three response levers exists aswellas timeoutduration information foreach session of

participation by thesix experimental subjects observed overthecourse of these experiments.

When examining the lower portion of individual cumulative records analphabetical and

numerical legend exists describing relevant subject, condition, and response information.

Information is presented in the following manor:

Subject Initials; Session Number; Experimental Condition;Response Frequency to the Time OutLever/Duration of Time Out;Response Frequency to the Extra Key; andResponse Frequencyto the MainResponse Lever.

Forexample a legend: AR 1 GR2 FR 250 233 484 885. would read: Thefirst

session of theexperiment for subject AR responding undera Geometric Ratio 2 schedule witha

terminal component of FR 250(see text for specific details of experimental conditions). Subject

AR responded to thetimeout key233 times, to the extra key484times and to the main key885

times. Since noduration of timeout is reported. wecanassume thatthe time outcontingency

hadnotyetbeenintroduced to thatkey. If a duration of timeoutwere present the legend might

looklike this:PW3 FR250 1/2.6 4 7261, meaning thatsubject PWengaged in 1 time

out of a 2.6second duration.

Page 125: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

All 3 CIl Z~ Z50 0 17 lCJ721 All. ra Z50 no tID Cl& " • II • Hn

, ,

AIlS Fa:ZS1 no tID Cl& 0 11 10101 .• All. ra Z50 no tID Cl& • 20 .11 ,"1

~ .All 11 ft 500 Q • 7 "rno All 12 PIl 50Q ' . 0 Q 10M2

Page 126: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),
Page 127: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

All Z7 nl ~OOO Q . .:lS1 . 1l)(QJ All ZI ra - _.. Q . Q ~4

• rI .

All U III 4000 _ Q JI",. .Ajl JQ 1Il_ _ Q I f1~

r

. -All JS III 4000 _ Ql Z 1Il5GO ZI'.5 5190 S6IZ All JI III _ _ VI. I J4~ 7151

Page 128: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

112

AP. U f'Il 4O')Q Q ItT 10100 AP.4t n 4QQQ Q:I4' • lQJ4lI

Page 129: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

· JlI 11 1111000 0 0 M75 ~ IZ 1111000 0 J MZO

Page 130: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

'"J11 17 Do 4000 . • 0 2 1_' JII II ftl 1000 0 0 . 103

.. JII,' ralooo • 0 • 4G2 1371 JII20 ft 1000 1/.' JS4 1199

--J11 D III 8000 1/.7 _ . 1m -.. ZI III 1000 0 1 1001

Page 131: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

JII Z5 _

t.

-3J1 _

0" JJ _

o

o

o

·0

o . I44C

o lUG

.0Jll21 111&.

JII ZI E4, .

JII 10 111& o o 1,"

115

V.7 ".llIJi 111& 1017

Page 132: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

r

JJlf:ll 1:01: o .0 . no.

116

J

. Jlf J' .1:01: 1/1.7"Jlf 40 1:01:

,.., ..041

·JlI4\ 1:01: 51'11.a 454 '!170

1I

JlI4J - • 1/.3 500 ..II JlI44 - ..".' 111:1 44Z'

~I

L.1.oM1. ,~u.~1•. JlI 45 1:01: 1/1.1 . . lIS POZ •• JlI .- - :I14/2Ot.7 11Ol1 1I2t

Jlf 41 _ o 0 7171

Page 133: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

Jlf 1 c:a Z .lll Z:lO ID~ _ U 13 Jlf Z III Z50 O.

Jlf] III ZSO

Jlf 5 III Z50

1/2.' .

o .

Jlf 4 III Z50

1..

III t III ZOOO ~.J 113 mt

Jlf 11 III ZOOO ·0 ZI 1]50 Jlf IZ III 5GO o 1 1113

Page 134: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

IIf 13 n.soo. 7m.

1

IIf 15 n 50Q a a

a a 71"

aaIIIZ4 nzaaa

Page 135: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

I

IW ZS ,.4000

IWZS ,.4__

1W11 ,.4__

Q

7

.".

431Q

5411

IW a ft 4000

III J2 ,. 4000 ... Q

Q 2

119

1431

47111

z,

Page 136: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

rJ

o o --

120

Page 137: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

•,;

•!1

121

., .. 3 m "' m .Z "3. - 1701 1m

r~,

Jz

,,1..3 .. 3 1AO 1703 "'4 " •• I'lL 3 3511 2112 zt07

.S .. 3 30'

., 111250 "·.,0 111250 .7130

Page 138: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

122

.. 11 .. %SO 1/.' 1021 II" .. IZ. lIl.Z50 ~07., 7451 1571

. .

ICIZIU.' SUO .IZlI .. 14 III _.. . 1~5t•• %120 ~

.. IS III _ 3Q14'.' ,UI JUS .. ,. III - 43~1J.7 475 '154

. . SIt.' 1m "11 .. ,. "'00 0 9011 ~

.. ,t lit 500 0.,30 tolI1 • 20 .. SOO 0 I'" toG, .

Page 139: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

UI JI 1Il 500UI Jt 1Il 500 _

UI 40 1Il 500 """

U1U nt 500 """

UI 44 1Il laoo """ 11'.1 I

4011

Jln

UI II 1Il 500 """

UI U 1Il 100II"""

UI 4S 1Il laoo """

111.5

SIZ.t 1

JCIIlO

Page 140: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

al ~ nt 10Cl0 _

al ~ nt lOOl1 _

al 50 nt 10Cl0 _ .. 3 11.5

15

5179

30Cl0

...

al.7 ,. 1ClOO ....

al.' ,. lClOO .... fa 3

•I

al 51 ra lClOO.... fa 3

10 3141

124

al 51 nt 10Cl0 ....

al 54 ft lClOO _

17flU.. 131'

m

11M

1Z407

al 53 ft 1000 ....

XI) 55 n IClOO ....

Page 141: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

125

til,. 11l 1000 13/1'.Z Zit 3105 ID 57 1Il 1000 Vl7.' 123 U4t

til ,. 11l 1000 112

Page 142: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

= 126

~. l

--til I CIl2 nt 15O m 104 1173 tIl2 ca 2 llt 15O. no 1m 1454

til 11 nt 250 lllI Ale 11S'.. 3' 1424 tit 12 n. 250 Ale 1/212 2lI 10000·

Page 143: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

tI' ZJ nt 1000 ... Al.e 7"'" W 5000 tI' 2.' nt IllOQ ue 21'105 0 . 2llOQ

Page 144: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

til ~ rlt'ooo .u~ 1/15~ 0 1000 til 30 tIl'ooo ... .ut tI/12.' 34 4281

~ ;

f~ ,I

til JS III 250 ... A1~ 0 to 716' til JI til ~O .ut 111240 4 ~OO

Page 145: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

129

til 31 m 250 "" Ale WI'I1.9 14 3501

Page 146: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

130

APPENDIX"

Theinfluences of an available alternative behavior on schedule performance and schedule

disruption were examined in Experiment III. Inthat experiment the alternative consisted of the

specific introduction and removal of a concurrent reinforcement contingency on a second

response leverwhich wasof a higher reinforcement density thanthatcontingency programmed

to the main lever. This wasseen to have an immediate andsubstantial effect on the referent main

key response pattern suggesting that indeed alternative response opportunities andthe

contingency relations thereof areimportant in theanalysis of strain.

Although Experiment III examined the influence of alternatives in thecontext of the

introduction andremoval of a concurrent contingency, ideally onewould wantto examine ratio

strain asa function of existing topographically and functionally distinct alternatives. Thecurrent

experiment (1IIb) explores performance relations withsuch analternative. Specifically, referent FR

performance is observed underbothbaseline andincreased schedule conditions withand

without the presence of a topographically, in terms of response requirement, andfunctionally, in

termsof outcome, distinct alternative.

METHODS

SUbject

Onlyonesubject. LM, parlicipated in this experiment. Therecruitment procedures utilized in

contacting the subject were the same as specified in Experiment I. The relevant subject

information is asfollows:

LM Subject LM was an 18-year-{)ld Caucasian male high school student, living at

home. At the timeof participation thesubject wasunemployed although hehad

previously worked part-time asa computer programmer. Subject LMparticipated

in 38 sessions overa fourweekperiod collecting 3600 bonus points tor a total

earning of $74.00 andwasthuspaid$3.89 per hour.

Apparatus

Theexperimental apparatus, control equipment, andexparimental room were thesame as

described inthe firstexperiment. A commercially available hand heldMatteiElectronics Footbal/2

Page 147: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

131

electronic game, model number 1050-0320, wasadditionally incorporated intothis experiment as

an alternativo Independent of theexperimentally programmed reinforcement schedules.

Procedure

Unless otherwise specified theprocedures used inthls expsrmentwere similarto those

reported in Experiment J.

Experimental Design and Training. Thisexperiment examined response patterns

withandwithout an available alternative underbaseline conditions, an increase in the schedule to

FR 1000, anda return to the FR250baseline. Within this ABA design the effects of the

alternative were examined by randomly, on a .5probability basis, introducing a handheld

electronic game in anexperimental design similar to that of themultiple schedule (Hersen &

Barlow, 1976).

Undera multiple schedule. twoor more schedules of reinforcement arealternated either

consecutively, randomly. or onsome otherbasis wnheachschedule containing an explicit

exteroceptive stimulus. The reliable occurrence of differential response patterns underthe

occasion of each stimulus suggests differential experimental control by each component. In the

present experiment the presence or absence of the electronic gal!1e wasthe exteroceptive

stimulus. If indeed anavailable alternative is an antecedent to performance decrement,

differential responding should develop during these alternating conditions. Specifically, one

would expect the presence of such an alternative to make the referent contingency les-s potent

and influence response rates underconditions likely to produce strain, and lessso underother

conddons.Ina training procedure similar to thatreported earlier with subjects AR, PW, andIN, subject

LMinitially responded undera Geometric Ratio 2 schedule of reinforcement witha maximum

schedule value of 250for a totalof three sessions. At that timethe Time Outcontingency was

verbally introduced andthe following 15sessions werespent underthe FR250baseline. During

the final 14sessions of baseline there wasa .5probability thatthealternative would be

introduced during anygiven session. Thealternative wasactually introduced on 6 of these14

occasions.

Theensuing 14sessions were runwith the Fixed Ratio increased to 1000. The alternative

was introduced on 4 of these occasions, again on a .5probability basis. Finally, a return to the FR

250baseline occurred. This final baseline wasonlysixsessions in length asthe previously

Page 148: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

132

specified termination period wasmet. Thealternative was randomly introduced on three of these

sixoccasions.

Verbal Introduction of the ARernatlve. The alternative was initially provided during

the fourth baseline session. Prior to thistimethe electronic gamehadbeen available for the

subject to play in thewaiting room between sessions andit continued to be ~vailable at this time

during the entire course of the experiment. Thesubject engaged in thisactivity on a regular basis

during the between session break.

The hand held electronic game was chosen as a convenient alternative foruseas it generally

requires two hands to effectively play. Itwas incorporated aspartof a mutually exclusive

alternative contingency relation. However, pilot research suggested that everingeneous

subjects wereableto engage in both schedule andelectronic game activities to varying degrees

of efficiency by, for example, hitting the response leverwithan elbow whileplaying the game

with the hands. Forthis reason it was necessary to establish mutual exclusiveness through

instruction andthis procedure will bedescribed.

A second procedural issue in thedesign of thisexperiment involved obtaining anobjective

andempirical index of theamount of timespent engaging in the alternative behavior. Since the

existing control equipment andelectronic game couldnotbe easily adapted for thispurpose, a

decision was made to verbally linkthe instruction (onthe mutually exclusive nature of the

relationship between the game andthe main response lever), to the TIme Out leverwhich was, in

turn, linked to the existing control equipment. Under thisarrangement thesubject was required

to engage in a TIme OUt when playing thegame andto TIme Inwhen notplaying the game. This

allowed precise quantification of the timespent engaging in the alternative pattern. This

procedural deviation, however, necessitates caution when interpreting andcomparing the TIme

Outdata underthose conditions when the electronic game is present to when thegame is not

present or to Time Out relations of otherexperiments.

Thefollowing instruction wasused upon the initial introduction of thealternative:

During some sessions youwill havetheopportunity to playthiselectronic game. If youwantto playsimply hitthiskeyto shutoff theequipment andhit it again when you arethrough (point to left key) to tum theequipment backon.You arenotallowed to interact withthe experimentalpanel inanyotherwaywhile you areplaying. You wiii risk losing points jj youviolate these rules. However, feel freeto playor notplaythe game at yourdesire.

Page 149: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

133

SUbject lMNo Alt.

o1/ Alt..

9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33 36 39

FA 250

..

FA 1000FA 250",lolI~

GA 2QlG!o 8~ "0cQI

4cOrn.la.., ....J:iIIQ1 2llI:a,

03 6

Sessions

Figure 21. Overview of responses per second to the main responslJlever during all conditions of Experiment Jllb by subject LM.

Page 150: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

134

Although subject LMcould freely engage in a time outat any time during thosesessions

whenthe alternative waspresent yet notactually engage in playing the electronic game, visual

monitoring revealed thatthis never occurred. Conversly, LMcouldengage in the alternative

without initiating a timecut. This didoccuronce duringthesession when the alternative was

initially introduced but did notoccuragain afterthesubject was reminded that he risked losing

points by the violation, Hence thisprocedure effectively allowed quantification of response

patterns while establishing themutually exclusive nature of the experimental tasks.

RESULTS

As in priorexperiments, response rates to each of thethreeresponse levers wereexamined in

this experiment andcumulative records of pertormance to the mainkeywerecollected. Specific

procedural information regarding these measures may befound in earlier experiments. The

duration of timespent engaging in the available alternative wasalsoexamined.

Forthe sake of clarity, results undereach component of the multiple schedule design shallbe

discussed separately. Under the necessities of this arrangement, experimental sessions maynot

be in consecutive orderduring presentation of the respective results withandwithout the

alternative present. However, anoverview of the experimental results showing specific sequencing

arrangements canbe seen in Figure 2~. Responses persecond to the main leverare examined

underthe differing conditions of the experiment. The general ABAdesign of FR250, followed by

FR 1000with a return to FR250 canbe observed, along withthe first threesessions showing the

GR2training period. Sessions, plotted along the abscissa, during which the alternative waspresent

aredemarcated onthe graph bya triangle rather thana square. The squares represent sessions

during which the alternative was absent. Ingeneral, it canbe observed thatduring the initial FR250

baseline similarrates of responding canbe observed in each alternative-present/absent design

component whilerates separate when the schedule is increased to FR 1000. Lower response

rates areseen inthosesessions when the alternative is present. Aswill be discussed, upon

reversal to FR 250 a reversal of effect canbeobserved overthe initial threesessions but notover

the final three. A detailed discussion of this figure will notbe undertaken, in lieuof which Figures 22

& 23 are introduced. Response patterns under eachdesign component of the multiple schedule

design maybe observed more clearly from these.

Page 151: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

Subject LM

135

76

::.,100" 5IIII11C~~g 4c 5!.! 3...t _v.G~... 23:Q:CII

Q. 1o

I- FR 250 FR 1000 FR 250

-~--

\l-

I-

I-~ g g

7 8 12 13 14 18 24 29 31 32 33 36 37

7 8 12 13 14 18 24 29 31 32 33 36 37

::.,

g-:: 2000 ...---------..,.-----..,.-----~'"~0 ....3~ 100 1500c< ."lI.I II C~ ~ 0 1000C .... 0GI~ GIQ. G mmE: c 500lD III '"e~~:( 0 ... ... .... ...

r:...t

>.oCGI::Ir:t•...II.>.II~

•...~

XlI.I

60

SO40

30

20

10O... ... .....iOIiI.,_..

7 8 12 13 14 18 24 29 31 32 33 36 37

Sessions

Figure 22. Frequency of responding to the main key and the extra key duringthose sessions when an alternative was available. as well as the duration oftime spent engaging in the alternative by SUbject LM in Experiment IIlb.

Page 152: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

136

Subject LM

FA 250GA 2 FA 250

J~1 2 3 456 9 1011151617 19202122232526272830343538

7554321o--~ -- ~ ~~!'"'""'~ - ......

>. 1000

~0e

~OOQI::lr:rCD '°1..LI.

~60 No

::l40 iime0

CIl Out::I 20....I- 0

1 2

>.Cl

~ 2000::lgo 1600

~ 1200

: BOO~

• 400.. a)(w

1 2 3 4 5 6 s 10 11 15 1617 19 20 21 22 23 25 2527 28 30 34 35 38

Sessions

Figure 23. Frequency of responding to the main key. the time outkey, and the extra key by subject LM during those conditions whenno alternative was present in Experiment IIIb.

Page 153: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

137

Figure 22 shows the frequency of responding to the main keyandthe extrakey during those

sessions when thealternative wasavailable, aswellas the duration of tirnsspent engaging in the

alternative by LM. Thetop graph of Figure 22shows responding to the mainkeyduring the

presence of the alternative underthe three experimental conditions. During the FR 250 baseline

the response rate is in the range of 4-6persecond. A large decrease in rateoccurs withthe advent

of theschedule increase andthis is maintained through the condition. The rate increases to

baseline levels duringthe first session of the reversal, however, this rate is not maintained during

the final two sessions.

The middle graph of Figure 22 shows the duration of time in seconds spentengaging inthe

alternative activity undereachexperimental condition, and an inverse relationship of high

conformity can be observed between thisduration andthe mainkeyresponse rate. This

suggests that the subject was likelyto beengaging in thealtemative behavior when not

responding to the mainkey. So,for example, thisconformity suggests that during the final two

sessions of the FR 250 reversal, subject LM wasengaging in thealternative when not

responding to the mainkey. The lowergraph of thisfigure shows response frequency to the

extrakeyduringthis experiment. Although theoverallfrequency of responding is low, it is

interesting to notethat when an altemative is available, as in this experiment, frequency of

responding to this key is seento decrease withthe schedule increase ratherthanto increase as

seen in previous experiments undersimilar conditions of a schedule change to lessfavorable

conditions. However, this lackof increase maybedueto the priorsubsidence of extrakey

iesponding oversessions following the schedule increase, as the initial session with the

alternative present at FR1000 was, in fact, thesixth session of FR1000 overall. Extra key

responding increases with the return to the FR 250 condition, although decreases overthe next

twosessions.

Figure 23 portrays responding by LM to each of the three response levers during those

sessions of the experiment when no altemative was available. Themain key response rate

portrayed in the topgraphremains fairlyconstant across theexperimental conditions, alfl10ugh a

gooddealof variability in rate can be observed across sessions, particularly during the latter

portion of the FR 1000 condition. Thisgeneral continuity of a high rate is in contrast to the

pattern seenwhen the alternative waspresent as reported earlier. A slightdecrease in main key

response ratecan be observed with the increase in schedule requirement to FR1000, however

thisdecrease isnotnearly asdramatic asthat portrayed in Figure 22when thealternative was

available. A reversal of effect canbeseen during thefinalFR 250 condition in that main key

Page 154: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

138

patterns tendto stabalize, although a lower rate canbe seen during the finalexperimental

session.

When the alternative wasavailable in thisexperiment the timeoutkeyactually served to time

theduration of the alternative behavior rather thanfunctioning as a timeout in the manner

previously used(see the discussion of this under Procedure). However, the experimental

conditions prevalent during the noavailable alternative component of the multiple schedule

design, asseenin those sessions portrayed in Figure 23,are indeed similar to the experimental

conditions of the earller experiments, with regards to the functions of the three response levers,

permitting dlscusslon of common relations, particularly with regard to thetimeout lever.

Withthis in mind, several findings observed earlier are replicated when examining the lower

twographs of Figure 23. Theintroduction of the timeoutcontingency at session 4 can be

observed to severely restrict or decrease theamount of timeout responding, whereas the

frequency of responding to the extra keygradually decreases asthe main key response patterns

become predominant. Given this lowerlevel of timeoutengagement, a temporary increase in

frequency canbe seen to occur whenthe schedule is increased. A parallel pattern of increased

responding with the schedule increase canalsobe observed to the extrakey in the lowergraph.

In both of these cases a decrease to baseline levels occurs overensuing sessions. In comparing .

theordinates of the twographs, it can be seen thateven though theoverall performance patterns

appear similar for timeout and extra keyresponding, thegreater frequency of occurrence is to the

extra key.

Each of the respective conditions portrayed in Figures 22 & 23maynowbe moreeasily

recoginzed when placed in theoverall experimental context of Figure 21. As noted with regard to

those conditions where the alternative waspresent, temporary andincomplete reversal of effect

wasseenwith theschedule return to FR250. In Figure 21,a decreasing trend ingeneral across

both conditions canbeobserved during this reversal. In an ABAdesign a reversal of effect

permits the mostpowerful conclusions. Hence, the interpretation of these experimental results

arecomplicated bythis incomplete reversal. Theimplications of thisandthe conclusions drawn

from these results will be presented further during thediscussion 01 thisexperiment.

Cumulative Records

Cumulative records from representative sessions of those conditions whenthe alternative

wasavailabe to the subject are shown in Figure 24. Records fromsessions when the alternative

wasnot present areshown in Figure 25. In each of these figures, theuppertwo and lowertwo

Page 155: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

139

Figure 24. Sample cumulative records portraying representative response patterns generatedunderbothbaseline and increased fixed ratio schedule conditions when analternative wasavailable to subject LM in Experiment IIlb. Theupper two records portray responding duringthe initial FR250baseline andthe lower tworecords portray responding under similarconditions following a reversal. Thecenter twocumulative records portray responding underthe increased ratio condition of FR1000. A complete collection of all cumulative recordsgenerated during this experiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 156: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

1

140

·• =•

= •=• e •

j : e.. :~

:; :5..~

:5.. :5•.. ..

•~

•0

0'"

'" .. '".. .. .... ..

• • ·~ :: ~..• • •f C C

· : :... ... :;•

~

:5 :5 :5~

..• •0 8

0.... --.. .. '"

===.. .. ..eo eo

eo ::

Page 157: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

141

Figure 25. Sample cumulative records portraying representative response patterns generatedunderbothbaseline and increased fixed ratio schedule conditions whenno alternative wasavailable to subject LM in Experiment IIIb. Theupper two records portray respondinQ duringthe initial FR250baseline andthe lower two records portray responding undersimilarconditions following a reversal. Thecenter twocumulative records portray responding underthe increased ratio condition of FR 1000. A complete collection of all cumulative recordsgenerated duringthis experiment is included in Appendix I.

Page 158: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

142

...­.-•e:....o•oN

••

"e:..a•

-= 3 -~•e•.-....a•

••=

•.. •=

.- e •• ": ..: .. :.. ..- • ....

I0 • 0•• §0 0

In'" .. ...• • •• • •

Page 159: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

143

records portray response patterns generated during the preandpost manipulation FR 250

baseline conditions, respectively. Thecenter cumulative records are representative of those

generated underthe FR 1000 condition.

When comparing Figures 24 & 25 a striking difference canbe observed in response patterns

particularly during sessions when the schedule requirement is increased to FR 1000. When the

alternative is present fewerFixed Ratios are completed and longer post reinforcement pauses

occur, during which timethe altemative is engaged in asshown by thetimeout resets. Time outs

are more frequent during thesessions withthe altemative present giventheverbal linkage of the

timeoutsandthealtemative explained earlier. Ingeneral, the sessions where the alternative is

available seem to present less variability in response rate andthe response patterns givethe

impression of being more "organized". That is, responding to themain leveris eitherstrong, or

responding is to the alternative. More variability is seen in those records presented in Figure 25

in the sense thata variety ofshortpauses arepresent in thedifferent records particularly the third

andthese pauses don'tconform to anapproximately standard length. Responding, when

occurring does appear quite consistent however. Responding generally occurs throughout the

FR 1000sessions in contrast to those records portrayed in Figure 24.

Response patterns to themainleveroccurat a high and consistent rate in Figure 24. Time

outsoccurduring periods of no responding and visual observation confirms that at these times

thesubject wasengaging inthe altemative. The first andfifthgraphs showsomewhat similar

patterns of responding, l.e.,a highandconsistent rateoccursforthe majority of the session.

Towards the endof the session the subject stopsresponding and switches to the alternative

following a reinforcer. Theresponse rate of the first record is much higher than thatof the fifth

with40 reinforcers obtained during the former session, compared to 30 in the latter.

Thesecond cumulative record shows a similar yetdistinct pattern. During thissession 30

reinforcers areobtained with the alternative undertaken afterevery tenthreinforcer. Ingeneral

during this experiment, thesubject wasmore likely to pause following a "grouped" n!.!mber of

reinforcers such as 10,or 30, or 40,for example, than at othertimes.

Thethird,fourth, andfinal records of Figure 24showcomparable overall response patterns.

During each, thepredominant behavior portrayed is the ahemative. In each record an initial

schedule completion, or two in thecaseof thesixthrecord, is undertaken witha timeoutoccuring

afterreinforcement. It is during this timethatthe alternative is predominant. A second schedule

completion occurs about half-way throug.... the session of the third record, afterwhichthe

alternative is again undertaken.

Page 160: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

144

Sample cumulative records of response patterns generated under each schedule conditon

when thealternative wasnotavailable arepresented in Figure 25. The first tworecords of

responding underthe FA 250show a highrate pattern for most of each session whh a substantial

pause occuring toward the end of the session. Thispause is particularly evident inthe second

record, occuring after the thirtieth reinforcer rather thanthefortieth of the first record. An

additional schedule completion occurs in the first record following the 2-3 min. pause whereas in

the second a timeoutof approximately 2 min. in length occurs afterabout 10 min. of no

responding. Some variability in response ratecanbeobserved, even during the period of these

two records when responding is strong, however for the most part, response patterns portrayed

areconsistent andof highrate, with littlepostreinforcement pausing seen.

Thethird and fourth records of Figure 25 show sample patterns during the FA 1000

condition. A break andrunpattern canbeseenduring the third record, particularly early in the

session. During thefirst two schedule completions a number of breaks canbe observed,

however the final three schedule completions showa steady response pattern of lower overall

rate thandlJring the FA 250baseline sessions. severalsubstantial PAPs canbe observed anda

timeoutoccurs at theendof thesession. A higher response rate commensurate withthe

baseline sessions canbe seen inthe fourth cumulative record. Thissession is characterized bya

highandconsistent response rate whh littlepostreinforcement pausing, although brief PAPs can

be observed following the finaltwo reinforcers of the session.

Thefifth andsixthrecords of Figure 25show responding following theschedule reversal to

FA 250. Thirtyreinforcers areobtained during thesession portrayed bythefifth record. In

general, a steady high rate response pattern with no PAPs canbeobserved. A lowering and

some variability in response rate occurapproximately one-third of theway intothe session anda

single within ratio pause occurs two-thirds of thewaythrough the session. A timeoutoccurs

following the final reinforcer.

Thefinalsession portrayed in Figure 25shows an anomolous record of responding. The

initial FA 250schedule completion does notoccuruntilover10 min. into the session. During this

initial ratio a number of timeouts andbreaks canbeobserved. A long PAP then follows.

Aesponding resumes latein thesession andfor the most part is then characterized as steady,

showing a highrate, andwith little post reinforcement pausing. However, some variability in rate

canbe observed during the finalfewschedule completions anda brieftime out is also seen to

occurduring the initial stages of oneof the latter ratio runs. An interruption of the drawn line

during the reset of the response marker occurs intwo instances onthisrecord. This is a

Page 161: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

145

mechanical artifact produced by the reinforcement slash being drawn whilethepen is resetting,

and should bedisregarded.

DISCUSSION

Inthe current experiment ratio strain was examined in thecontext of an available alternative.

Heretofore, responding by human subjects wasobserved to be persistent at large fixed ratios

although instances of perfcrmance decrement were seen under specific conditions in

Experiments I & II. Experiment III suggested thatalternative contingency arrangements were

influential in understanding response persistence under thecontrol of a referent contingency,

although the specifics of that experiment were notnecessarily those mostappropriate to the

anlaysis of ratio strain. In thecurrent experiment procedural arrangements were modified to

better assess the effects of alternatives andstrain. and indeed performance disruption was

observed to occurto agreater degree under those circumstances where the subject wasgiven

the opportunity to engage inan alternative.

Specifically. performance was examined withand without theavailability of a hand-held

electronic gameunder a relatively lowfixed ratio of 250 and the higher FR 1000 with an ensuing

FR250 reversal. The electronic game was selected forusein thisexperiment because of its

topographical andfunctional independence relative to the established button pressing

response. Under the initial FR 250performance was seen to be persistent regardless of the

arrangements dictating the presence or absence of thealternative whereas preference for the

altemative wasclearly observed at thehigher fixed ratio. When thealternative was notpresent at

the higher FR responding was persistent suggesting that indeed the alternatives available to a

subject will influence theoccurrence of strain in thesense of performance decrement.

Thestrained performance examined herein is hence related to a largercontingency

ecosystem. In thiswell-defined experimental situation it is possible to clearly seethe influence of

alternative contingency arrangements onthe referent response pattern. In a less well controlled

environment. perhaps more akin to the world outside the laboratory, Itmaynotbe possible to

assess the presence or influence of alternative contingencies. Inthis situation performance

would be examined linearly rather than non-linearly in thatbehavior would be examined solely as

an effect of a specific occasion-behavior-consequence relationship rather thanin the context of a

host of suchcontingencies. Under linear analysis it is necessary to evoke theconstruct of ratio

Page 162: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

146

strain to explain classes of performance decrement analogous to that presently seen which wasa

direct function of the introduced occasion to engage in an alternative behavior. Conclusions as

to specific ratio sizes capable of sustaining human operant performance would varyconsiderably

under linear andnon-linear assessment giventhepresence and/or absence of alternatives.

An incomplete reversal of experimental effect wasobserved in thisexperiment. Following

theexperimental series of FR1000 aschedule reversal to FR250wasundertaken forsix

sessions afterwhich time the experiment wasterminated under a previous temporal agreement

between subject andinvestigator. A reversal of effect was observed during the initial three of

these sessions in thatperformance was persistent at a rate consistent with the initial FR250

baseline regardless of thepresence or absence of the alternative. Ideally, the strongest

experimental conclusions would have derived outof a continuation of thispattern overthe

remainder of the experiment. However, thiswasnotthecase. In the final three sessions a

distinct decrease in response rate occurred undereach of the twoalternative present andthe

single alternative absent experimental sessions.

It appears, while an initial reversal was present upon the reduction of the ratioto FR250, that

anoverall decreasing trend occurred during thefinal threesessions as presented in Figure 21. In

actuality it is hard to definitively assess thisdecreasing trend inthe no alternative session since

the lowered response rate observed during the final ;.0 aftemath:e session wasin line withsome

of theprevious FR1000 noalternative overall response rates. However, a distinct decreasing

trend wasobserved in those sessions where thealternative wasavailable, and indeed, the

subject wasseen to be engaging in thealternative during these sessions. Thiswas a definite

change in performance from the initial FR250 baseline.

It is difficult to interpret these results, in particular the lackof reversal in those sessions when

thealternative waspresent. A number of procedural limitations complicate the understanding of

these patterns. First and foremost is thatgiven thefactthatthisexperiment wasa preliminary

single-case inquiry, the response patterns of only onesubject were observed. This makes it

difficult to tease out anarnolous experimental results fromenduring experimental effects.

Secondly, thefinalFR250reversal wasonlysixsessions long whereas the otherconditions of

this experiment were 14sessions in length. Thisfinalbaseline maynotthenbe a representative

sample of thefinalcondition producing or notproducing a reversal.

Given these procedural limitations we areleftwith two potential states. Either the lackof

reversal is procedural in nature, in that a reversal would be observed undersustained

observation, or this is anactual sequential effect ofsome sort. Ifwe assume a trueeffect is

Page 163: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

147

represented andthatan incomplete reversal is an enduring quality of this experimental procedure

then an interesting state of affairs exists. A particular contingency whose behavioral fulfillment is

of lowprobability (that being engaging in thealternative) becomes probable or potent through a

distinct and independent contingency manipulation (increasing the referent FRfrom 250to

1000) andthispotency isenduring even afterthe inducing conditions are removed (theFRis

returned to the lower value). Thiswouldbe interesting in-and-of itselfandworthy of further study

inthat it provides anexample of generation of novel patterns of behavior andof howcertain

schedule-induced patterns maybecome locked intoenvironmental contingencies andthus

sustatined. This might be referred to as the "How canyoukeepthemdownonthe farmonce

they've seengay Paree?" effect.

Onthe otherhand these results maybe related to conditions making the referent

contingencies lesspotent or probable. Evidence e)(ists as to thedepotentiation of the

experimental conditions and arrangements for subject LM overthefinal fewdaysof this

experiment, cognitively causing himto "lose interest" in the experimental monetary

contingencies andthiswill bediscussed. Under these conditions depotentiation of the referent

contingency would leadto an increase in the behavioral alternatives present. Theevidence for

thissortof explanation of the observed patterns is twofold. Objectively, the subject reported to

the investigator during the lastweekof the experiment that he hadrecently accepted andstarted

working at a part-time jobwhere hewas reportedly drawing a substantial paycheck. Hence, the

monetary contingencies which initially hadserved to sustain performance were perhaps

influenced by access to the monetary reinforcer outside of the sessions. In otherwords the

behavioral ecOnomy "opened up" (Hursh, 1980) or inthecurrent framework a behavioral

alternative presented itselfpotentially influencing thecurrent behaviors of investigation.

Onmore of a subjective level the subject presented a picture towards theendof the

experiment of someone whowasbored withthe experiment and. in fact.verbalized this on

several occasions. Thesubject was anxiously counting thedaysuntilparticipation in the

experiment was complete andthiswasonereason thatan extension of participation wasnot

negotiated so thata 14rather than6 session finalbaseline could be run. Hence, it seems

possible that the incomplete reversal observed in thisexperiment wasrelated to motivational

issues.

A replication of thisexperiment is warranted andhopefully would clarifythe nature of the

current results. Even though the incomplete reversal observed in this experiment complicates

the interpretation of these results, the current experiment none-the-Iess provides continued

Page 164: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

148

support for the influence of alternative contingency arrangements in the analysis of strain and

suggests the need for an awarness andanalysis of alternatives in anythorough description of

behavioral changes.

It is interesting to note thatpost-reinforcement pausing is substantial by subject LM during

thealternative present experimental conditions, particularly during the higher FA. It is possible

that theoccasion foralternatives is an important element intre occurrence of post-reinforcement

pausing by humans and otherorganisms, for thatmatter. It is alsointeresting to note that

responding to the extrakeydecreased during the schedule increase to FR1000 during those

conditions when the alternative waspresent, while increasing when noalternative was present as

during previous experiments. Hence theoccurrence of alternatives may haveimplications for

bothanalysis of postreinforcement pause patterns andfor the selection andfrequency of

patterns induced under contingency-change conditions. Further research would bebeneficial in

these areas, aswell.

Ingeneral, with regards to patterns of ancillary key responding andgiven the aforementioned

frequency difference iii extrakey responding during sessions when the alternative waspresent,

thecurrent results support conclusions drawn earlier in Experiments I, II,& III. This isparticularly

apparent when frequency of responding is examined during those sessions when noalternative

is present. As revealed in Figure 23,timeout responding temporarily increases when the

schedule is increased fromFR250to FR 1000 yet notduring the schedule reversal to the lower

value. The same pattern canbeseen to the extra keyalthough on a comparable basis, extra key

response frequency is substantially higher leading oneto suspect that the timeout contingency

actually restricts response rate. Additional evidence for this exists when examining thepreand

posttimeoutcontingency introduction data. During sessions 1-3timeout responding occurs in

the600-1000 response frequency range while theposttimeout contingency range (sessions

4-38) is 0-80. Hence, patterns identical to those reported earlier areseen.

Page 165: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

149

REFERENCES

Appel, J.B. (1963). Aversive aspects of a schedule of positive reinforcement. Journalof theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 423-428.

Azrin, N.H. (1958). Some effects of noise on human behavior. Journal of the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 1, 183-200.

Azrin, N.H. (1961). Time-out from positive reinforcement. SCience, 133,382-383.

Azrin, N.H., Hutchinson, R.R., & Hake, D.F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. JournaloftheExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 191-204.

Barrett, J.E. (1976). Conjunctive schedules of reinforcement. III:A fixed-interval adjustingfixed-ratio schedule. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 157·164.

Boren, J.J., Moershbaecher, J.M., & Whyte, AA. Variability of response location on fixed-ratioandfixed~interval schedules of reinforcement. Journalof theExperimental Analysis ofBehavior, 30, 63-67.

Brady, J.V.,& Hunt, H.F. (1955). An experimental approach to theanaysis of emotionalbehavior. Journal of Psychology, 40, 313-324.

Brown, T.G., &Flory, R.K. (1972). Schedule-induced escape fromfixed-interval reinforcement.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 395-403.

Buskit, W.F., Miller, Jr.,H.L., & Bennett, R.H. (1980). Fixed-interval performance in humans:Sensitivity to temporal parameters when food is the reinforcer. The Psychological Record,3D, 111-121.

Buskit, W.F., Miller, Jr., H.L., & Bennett, R.H. (1981). Effects of instructional constraints onhuman fixed-interval performance. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35,217-225.

Byrd, L.D. (1973). Effects of d-amphetamine onschedule-eontrolled keypressing anddrinkingin thechimpanzee. Journal uf Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 185,633·641.

Capehart, G.W., Eckerman, D.A., Guilkey, M.& StlJlI, R.L. (1980). A comparison of ratio andinterval reinforcement schedules withcomparable inter-reinforcement times. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 61-76.

Catania, AC. (Ed.). (1968). Contemporary research Inoperant behavior. Glenview, IL:Scott,Forseman andCo.

Catania, AC. (1969). Concurrent performances: Inhibition of one response by reinforcement ofanother. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 12, 731-744.

Page 166: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

150

Collier, G.H., Hirsh, E., & Hamlin, P.H. (1972). Theecological determinants of reinforcement inthe rat. Physiology andBehavior, 9, 705-716.

Dardano, J.F. (1968). Reversal of preference underprogressive-ratio schedules of punishment.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 133-146.

Dardano, J.F. (1973). Self-imposedtimeouts under increasing response requirements. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19,269-287.

Dardano, J.F. (1974). Response-produced timeouts undera progressive-ratio schedule withareset option. Joumalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 103-113.

Dardano, J.F., & Sauerbrunn, D. (1964). Selective punishment of concurrent progressive-ratioperformance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 51-65.

Dews, P.B. (1958). Studies onbehavior. III:Stimulant actions of methamphetamine. JournalofPhamacologyand Experimental Therapeutics, 122, 137-147.

Duvinsky, J.D.,& Poppen R. (1982). Human performance on conjunctive fixed-intervalfixed-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 243-250.

Ellis, N.R., Barnett, C.D., & Pryer, M.W. (1960). Operant behavior in mental defectives:Exploratory studies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 63-69.

Falk, J.L. (1961). Production of polydipsia in normal ratsby an intermittent foodschedule.Science, 133,195-196.

Falk, J.L. (1964). Studies on schedule-induced polydipsia. InM.J. Wayner (Ed.), Thirst: Firstintemational symposium on thirstin the regulation of bodywater. NewYork: PergamonPress.

Falk, J.L. (1981). Theenvironmental generation of excessive bebavlor. In S.J. Mule, (ed.),Behavior in excess. New York: Free Press.

Falk, J.L., Sampson, H.H., & Winger, G. (1972). Behavioral maintenance of highconcentrationsc)fblood ethanol andphysical dependence in the rat. Science, 177, 811-813.

Fallon, J.H., Jr., Allen, J.D., &Butler, J.A. (1979). Assessment of adjunctive behavior in humansusing a stngent control procedure. Physiology andBehavior, 22, 1089-1092.

Felton, M.,& Lyon, D.O. (1966). Thepost-reinforcement pause. Journalof the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 3,131-134.

Ferster, C.B. (1957). Withdrawal of positive reinforcement as punishment. Science, 126,509.

Ferster, C.B. (1960). Intermittent reinforcement of matching to sample inthepigeon. Journal oftheExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 9. 259-272.

Ferster, C.B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. American Psychologisf, 28, 15-45.

Page 167: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

151

Fersler, C.B.,Appel, J.B.,& Hiss, R.A. (1962). Theeffects of drugs on a fixed-ratio performancesuppressed by a pre-timeout stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5,73-88.

Ferster, C.B., Culbertson, S., & Boren, M.C.P. (1975). Behavior principles, (2nd Ed.).Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.

Ferster, C.B., & Peele, D.B. (1980). The contribution of an added counter to a fixed-ratioschedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34,93-105.

Ferster, C.B.,& Skinner, B.F. (1957). SChedules of reinforcement. NewYork:Appleton-Century-Crotts.

Findley, J.D.,& Brady, J.V. (1965). Facilitation of large ratio performance by useofconditionedreinforcement. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 125-129.

Frazier, T.W., & Bitetto, V.E. (1969). The control of human vigilance byconcurrent schedules.Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 12,591-600.

Frederiksen, L.W., & Peterson, G.L. (1974). Schedule-induced aggression in nursery schoolchildren. The Psychological Record, 24,343-351.

Gentry, W.O. (1968). Fixed-ratio schedule-induced aggression. Joumal of the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 11, 813-817.

Goldiamond, I. (1974;. Toward a constructional approach to social problems: Ethical andconstitutional issues raised by applied behavior analysis. Behaviorism, 2, 1-85.

Goldiamond, I. (1975) Alternative setsas a framework forbehavioral formulations andresearch.Behavion'sm, 3, 49-86.

Goldiamond,l. (1984). Training parent trainers andethicists in nonlinear analysis of behavior. InR.F. Dangle & R.A. Polster, (Eds.), Parent training: Foundations of research andpractice.New York: Guilford Press.

Green, R.J., Sanders, R.M., & Squire, R.W. (1959). SChedules of reinforcement anddiscrimination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 293-299.

Harzem, P. (1984). Experimental analysis of individual differences andpersonality. JoumaloftheExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 42,385-395.

Herman, R.L., & Azrin, N.H. (1964). Punishment by noise inan alternative response situation.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 185-188.

Hersen, M.,&Barlow, D.H. (1976). Single case experimental designs: Strategies forstudyingbehaviorchange. NewYork: Pergamon Press.

Page 168: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

152

Hodos, W.•&Kallman. G. (1963). Effects of increment size andreinforcer volume onprogressive-ratio performance. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6,387-392.

Hodes, W.•& Trumble. G.H. (1967). Strategies of schecute preference in chimpanzees. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10.503-514.

Holland. J.G. (1958). Human vigilance. SCience. 128, 61-67.

Holz, W.C.•Azrin, N.H.• & Allyon. T. (1963). Elimination of behavior of mental patients byresponse-produced extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6,407-412.

Hursh. S.R. (1980). Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219·238.

Hutchinson, R.R.. & Azrin, N.H. (1961). Conditioning of mental hospital patients to fixed-ratioschedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 87-95.

Hutchinson. R.R., Azrin. N.H.•& Hunt, G.M. (1968). Attack produced by inttermittentreinforcement of a concurrent operant response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 11,489-495.

Kachanoff, R.. Leveille. R., McLelland. J.P.•& Wayner, M.J. (1973). SChedule-induced behaviorin humans. Physiology andBehavior, 11.395·398.

Kelly. J.F.• & Hake. D.F. (1980). Anextinction-induced increase in an aggressive response withhumans. Journalof the Experimental Analysis ofBaha'ofioi, 14, 153-164.

King, G. (1974). Wheel running intherat induced by afixed timepresentation of water. AnimalLearning and Behavior. 2, 325-328.

Laursen. A.M. (1972). Post-reinforcement pauses and response rate of monkeys ona two-handFRschedule. Journalof the Experimental Analysis cfBehavior, 17,85·94.

Lindsley,O.R. (1956). Operant conditioning methods applied to research in chronicschizophrenia. Psychiatric Research Reports of theAmerican Psychiatric Association. 5.118·139. 140-153.

Lindsley. O.R. (1959). Reduction in rate of vocal psychotic symptoms by differential positivereinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 269.

Lindsley,O.R. (1960). Characteristics of the behavior of chronic psychotics as revealed byfree-operant conditioning methods. Diseases of the Nervous System Monograph, '-1.61-78.

Long, E.R. (1963). Chained andtandem scheduling withchildren. Journal of the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 6, 459-472.

Page 169: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

153

Long, E.R., Hammack, J.T., May. F., & Campbell, B.J. (1958). Intermittent reinforcement ofoperant behavior in children. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1. 315-339.

Lowe. C.F. (1979). Determinants of human operant behavior. InM.D. Zeiler & P.Harzem, (Eds.),Advances inanalysis of behavior (vol. 1):Reinforcement andtheorganization of behavior.Chichester, U.K.: Wiley. 159-192.

Markle. S.M., & Tiemann. P.W. (1969). Really understanding concepts: Orin frumious pursuitofthejabberwock. Champaign, IL: Stipes Publishing Co.

Matthews, B.A., Shimoff. E., Catania. A.C., & Sagvolden, T. (19n). Uninstru{,~edhumanresponding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of theExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 27.453-467.

McMillan. D.E. (1969). Effects of d-amphetamine on performance under several parameters ofmultiple FR FIschedules. Journal of Pharmacology andExperimental Therapeutics, 167,26-33.

McMillan, J.C. (1971). Percentage reinforcement of fixed-ratio andvariable-interval performance.Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 297-302.

Medelson. J.• & Chillag, D. (1970). Schedule-induced air licking in rats. Physiology andBehavior, 5, 535-537.

Morgan, M.J. (1972). FRperformance underconditions of delayed reinforcement. Journal oftheExperimental Analysis of Behavior. 17, 95-98.

Morse. W.H.• & Herrnstein, R.J. (1956). Effects of drugs oncharacteristics of behaviormaintained by complex schedules of intermittent positive reinforcement. Annals of theNewYork Academy of Sciences, 65. 303-317.

Mull9r, P.C., Crow. R.E.•& Cheney, C.D. (1979). Schedule-induced locomotor activity inhumans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 83·90.

Nevin, J.A., & Reynolds, G.S., (Eds.), (1973). The studyof behavior. Glenview. II:Scott.Forseman and Co.

Notterman, J.M.•& Mintz, D.E. (1965). Dynamics of response. New York: John Wiley.

oet, T.P.S., Singer. G.•Jeffreys, D.,Lang, W., & Latiff. A. (1978). SChedule induced selfinjection of nicotine. heroin andmethadone by naive animals. In EC. Colpeart & J.A.Rosencrans (Eds.), Stimulus properties of drugs: Ten years ofprogress. Amsterdam:Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press.

Paer, J.J. (1975). Implications of the matching lawfor ratio respor.ding. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior. 23. 139-140.

Peden, B.F.•& Timberlake, W. (1984). Effects of reward magnitude onkeypecking andeatingby pigeons in a closed economy. The Psychological Record, 34,397-415.

Page 170: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

154

Poppen, R. (1982). Human fixed-interval performance with concurrently programmedschedules: A parametric analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37,251-266.

Porter, J.H., Brown, RT., & Goldsmith, P.A. (1982). Adjunctive behavior in children on fixedinterval food reinforcement schedules. Physiology andBehavior, 28, 609-612.

Powell, RW. (1968). Theeffect of small sequential changes in FRsize upon the PRP. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 589-593.

Powell, RW. (1969). The effect of reinforcement magnitude uponresponding underFRschedules. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 605-608.

Priddle-Higson, P.J., Lowe, C.F., & Harzem, P. (1976). Aftereffects of reinforcement onvariable-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 347-354.

Rachlin, H. (1978). A molartheory of reinforcement schedules. Journal of the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 30, 345-360.

Rayfield, F.,Segal, M.,& Goldiamond, I. (1982). SChedule-induced defecation. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 38,19-34.

Reese, E.P. (1978). Human operant behavior: Ana/ysis andapplication. Debuque, Iowa: Wm. C.Brown Co.

Repp, A.C., & Deitz, S.M. (1975). A comparison of fixed-ratio andvariable-ratio token-productionschedules with human subjects. ThePsychological Record, 25, 13i-137.

Reynolds, G.S. (1975). A primerof operant conditioning. Glenview, IL:Scott, Forseman andCo.

Roper, T.J. (1978). Diversity and substitutibility of adjunctive activities underfixed-intervalschedules of food reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30,83-96.

Roper, T.J. (1981). What is meant by the term"schedule-induced," andhowgeneral is scheduleinduction? AnimalLearning andBehavior, 9, 433-440.

Sanders, A.M. (1969). Concurrent fixed-ratio fixed-interval performances in adulthumansubjects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 601-604.

Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research. New York: Basic Books, Inc.

Sidman, M.,& Stebbins, W.C. (1954). Satiation effects underFRschedules of reinforcement.Journalof Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 114-116.

Skinner, B.F. (1938). The behaviorof organisms. NewYork: Appleton-Centurey-Crofts.

Skinner, B.F. (1953). Science andhuman behavior. NewYork: McMillan.

Page 171: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

155

Staats, A.W.,. _!'j, J.R., Osborne, J.G, Quinn,W.D., & Minke, K.A. (1963). The useof chainschedules in thestudyof reinforcement variables in a reading task. Unpublished manuscript(Technical Report No.25 (1963). Office of Naval Research. Contract Nonr-2794(02)).

Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G.R. (1977). Applying behavior-analysis procedures withchildrenandyouth. NewYork: Holt, Reinhart andWinston.

Thomas, J.R. (1974). Changes in progressive-ratio performance underincreased pressures ofair. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 553-562.

Thompson, D.M. (1964). Escape fromdiscriminative stimulus associated with fixed-ratioreinforcement. Journalof theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 1-8.

Thompson, D.M. (1965). Timeout from FR reinforcement: A systematic replication.Psychonomic Science, 2, 109-110.

Thompson, D.M., & Moershbaecher, J.M. (1980). Effects of d-amphetamine andcocaine onstrained ratio behavior in a repeated-acquisition task. Journalof theExperimental Analysis ofBehavior, 33,141-148.

Villareal, J. (1967, April). Schedule-inducedpica. Paperpresented at meeting of the EasternPsychological Association, Boston, MA.

Wallace, M., &Singer, C. Adjunctive behavior and smoking induced by a maze solving schedulein humans. Physiology andBehavior, 17, 849-852.

Wasik, B.H. (1969). The effects of fixed ratio andcontingent time on human lever-pressingbehavior. The Psychological Record, 19, 95-104.

Weiner, H. (1964a). Response costeffects during extinction following fixed-intervalreinforcement in humans. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 333-335.

Weiner, H. (1964b). Conditioning history andhuman fixed-interval performance. Journalof theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 383-385.

Weiner, H. (1966). Preference andswitching underratio contingencies with humans.Psychological Reports, 18,239-246.

Weiner, H. (1967). Costto payoff contributions to preference underfixed-ratio contingencieswith humans. Psychological Reports, 20,31-35.

Weiner, H. (1970). Instructional control of human operant responding during extinction followingfixed-ratio conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13,391-394.

Weiner, H. (1972). Controlling human fixed·interval responding with fixed ratio responding ordifferential reinforcement of low rate responding in mixed schedules. Psychonomic Science,26,191-192.

Page 172: INFORMATION TOUSERS€¦ · suggested bythefactthatsome authors report sustained responding under schedules requiring 2000 (Reynolds, 1975) and4000 responses (Findley & Brady, 1965),

156

Weiner, H. (19n). Anoperant analysis of human ahruistic responding. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Benavioi, 27.515-528.

Weiner. H. (1979). Some observations on apathetic operant responding 01 chronicschizophrenics. Perceptual andMotorSkills, 48. 1083-1090.

Weiner, H. (1981a). Base-line muscle tensions andmotoric responding of schizophrenics.British Journalof Clinical Psychology, 20. 25-33.

Weiner, H. (1981b). Contributions of reinforcement schedule histories to our understanding ofdrugeffects in human subjects. InT. Thompson & C.E. Johanson (Eds.). Behavioralpharmacology of human drugdependence. National Institute on Drug Abuse ResearchMonograph 37. Washington. D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Weiner, H. (1982). Histories of response omission and human operant behavior underafixed-ratio schedule of reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 32, 409-434.

Weiner, H. (1983). Some thoughts on discrepant human-animal performances underschedulesof reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 33, 521-532.

Weissman. N.W.•& Crossman. E.K. (1966). A comparison of two types of extinction followingfixed-ratio training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9. 41-46.

Whaley. D.L.. & Malott, R.W. (1971). Elementary principles of behavior. Englewood Cliffs. NJ:Prentice-Hall.

Witters. D.R., & Bachrach. A.J. (1965). Theeffects of competing contingencies uponfixed ratiobaselines in a smallgroup situation. The Psychological Record, 15, 103-110.

Woods. K.A.. Happ, A.J.•& Adams. C.K. (1983). Ratio responding as a function of concurrentavoidance schedules, yoked shocks andratio value. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 39. 449-456.

Yobum, B.C.•& Cohen, P.C. (1979). Schedule-induced attack on a pictorial target in feralpigeons (Columbia livia). Bulletin of thePsychonomic Society, 13. 7-8.

Zimmerman, J.•& Ferster. C.B. (1964). Some notes on timeout from reinforcement. JoumaloftheExperimental Analysis of Behavior. 7,13-19.