Top Banner
How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)
59

How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Dec 19, 2015

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

How does the brain decide what to look at next?

John FindlayUniversity of Durham

(acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Page 2: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccades are quintessentially voluntary movements.The gaze selects informative detail but eye scans also

appear random and arbitrary

from Yarbus (1967)

Page 3: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Pathways involved in saccade generation

Page 4: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)
Page 5: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual pathways form a massively interconnected neural network

Page 6: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Retinotopic mapping is maintained through to the saccadic generator in the SC

Page 7: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Multiple interconnected maps offer the possibility of selection

by biased competition

“ Some kind of short-term description of the information currently needed must be used to control competitive bias in the visual system, such that inputs matching that description are favoured in the visual cortex.”

(Desimone and Duncan, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 1995)

Page 8: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Biased competition creates salience maps

• A salience map is a two-dimensional map in which a single scalar quantity (salience) is represented at each point.

• Biased competition results in similarity to the search target being represented as salience.

• Instantiated in various modelsItti L and Koch C (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for

overt and covert shifts of attention. Vision Research, 40, 1489-1506.

Hamker FH (2004). A dynamic model of how feature cues guide spatial attention. Vision Research, 44, 501-521.

Page 9: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus

The SC is the main final site for selection of saccade destinations

Page 10: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Activity in the superior colliculus related to saccades

Wurtz R H (1996). Vision for the control of movement. The Friedenwald Lecture Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 37, 2131-2145.

Page 11: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

How does the brain decide what to look at next?

• During visual search, biased competition creates a salience map and processes, probably in the SC, select the point of highest salience to convert to an orienting saccade.

• Supported by detailed studies of saccades during visual search (Findlay, Vision Research, 1997; Motter and Belky, Vision Research, 1998a,b)

Page 12: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Task:

Search for a

red cross

(Look at it)

Findlay J M (1997). Saccade target selection during visual search. Vision Research, 37, 617-631

Page 13: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Properties of first saccades(Findlay, 1997)

Short latency (~ 250 ms) – very similar for saccades to target and to distractor

Page 14: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Properties of first saccades(Findlay, 1997)

Short latency (~ 250 ms) – very similar for saccades to target and to distractor

Frequently (75%) on target when target is in inner ring, occasionally (26%) when target in outer ring.

.

Page 15: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Properties of first saccades(Findlay, 1997)

Short latency (~ 250 ms) – very similar for saccades to target and to distractor

Frequently (75%) on target when target is in inner ring, occasionally (26%) when target in outer ring.

Incorrect saccades go preferentially to distractor sharing a feature with the target.

Page 16: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Monkey visual search(Motter & Belky, Vision Research, 38, 1007-1022; 1885-1815, 1998)

• Monkeys trained to search for a conjunction target (colour and orientation)

Page 17: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade selection in visual search

• The conclusion in both the Findlay and the Motter & Belky studies was that the biased competition/ salience map approach provided the most satisfactory account of saccadic selection.

• In particular, no evidence for a rapid covert attentional scan (favoured by many psychologists).

• This conclusion was reached earlier in physiological studies of single cell responses in the visual system of primates carrying out search tasks

FEF Schall & Hanes (1993)IT Chelazzi, Miller, Duncan & Desimone (1993)

Page 18: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

How does the brain decide what to look at next during visual search ?

• During visual search, biased competition creates a salience map and processes, possibly in the SC, select the point of highest salience to convert to an orienting saccade.

Page 19: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

• Selection from a salience map is basic

• Supplementary processes

1. Inhibition of return

2. Saccade pipelining

3. Strategies

How does the brain decide where to look next?

4. Implicit learning

5. Contingent learning

6. Neuro-economics (Glimcher)

7. Task specific requirements for

information acquisition

(Land, Hayhoe, Ballard)

Page 20: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

• Selection from a salience map is basic

• Supplementary processes

1. Inhibition of return

2. Saccade pipelining

3. Strategies

How does the brain decide where to look next?

NO OTHER ATTENTIONAL

SELECTION

4. Implicit learning

5. Contingent learning

6. Neuro-economics (Glimcher)

7. Task specific requirements for

information acquisition

(Land, Hayhoe, Ballard)

Page 21: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

• Selection from a salience map is basic

How does the system avoid ‘salience loops’?

(B is the most salient location when A is fixated, then A becomes the most salient when B is fixated)

Salience map alone would give A > B > A > B . . . . .

Proposed answer - Inhibition of Return (IOR)

An attended location is subject to some form of inhibition

when attention is shifted elsewhere

Klein R M and MacInnes W J (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346-352

Page 22: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

The rings task

Scan order partly specified

Centre - red – free scan through blacks - blue

Count target letters and make Yes/No response

Page 23: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

The Rings Task

3 6 9 12

Scan through the rings, starting with the red and ending with the blue

(Scans from 6 individuals)

Page 24: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Rings task - typical eye scan

Page 25: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Deviations from sequential scan

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 2

BACKTRACK 1

OMISSION

Page 26: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Trials with deviations from sequential scan

Omissions Backtrack 1 Backtrack 2 Backtrack > 2

BR 15 15 1 15

JP 9 15 6 9

LS 6 12 3 4

LW 5 29 5 4

PB 5 39 5 10

SL 3 13 7 13

Proportion 0.09 0.26 0.06 0.11Error rate* 47 %

(target omissions)

6 % 15 % 23 %

* Error rate on trials with standard scan 4 %

Page 27: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Backtracking in visual search

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

Found by other workers

(Motter & Belky, 1998)(Peterson et al. 2001)

• IOR time course may relate to visual processingslower with increased processing demands, so not always immediate

• Backtracking sequences may be pre-planned (pipelined saccades)In A1, B, A2 fixation sequences, the B fixation was normal duration (228 ms) but A1 and A2 were both shorter than normal (~ 170 ms).The saccade following a backtracking sequence tended to follow the

direction of the last saccade in the sequence.

Page 28: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Backtracking in visual search

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

BACKTRACK 1

Found by other workers

(Motter & Belky, 1998)(Peterson et al. 2001)

• IOR time course may relate to visual processing

• Backtracking sequences may be pre-planned (pipelined saccades)In A1, B, A2 fixation sequences, the B fixation is normal duration but A1 and A2 are both shorter than normalThe saccade following a backtracking sequence tends to follow the

direction of the last saccade in the sequence

X

Page 29: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visually-guided and memory-guided saccades

How does the brain decide which?

Hikosaka et al (2000) argue for basal ganglia pathway (blue route)

Inhibitory effects on SC, others excitatory

Separate sets of cells in caudate and in SNr are active during visually guided and memory guided

movements.

Page 30: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Directional strategies

COUNTTHE

DOTS

(Convex Hull)

Page 31: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Directional strategies

COUNTTHE

DOTS

Directional strategies are one form of memory (Gilchrist & Harvey)

Page 32: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

NO OTHER ATTENTIONAL SELECTION

‘Visual attention selects the saccade target’

Statement supported by the finding that visual information at the destination point of a forthcoming saccade receives preferential pre-processing (Deubel and Schneider, 1996; Kowler et al. 1995)

Biased competition is a form of attentional selection but does not operate in a localised region of the visual field.

Visual attention is commonly thought of as selection of a localised region.

Page 33: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

‘Visual attention selects the saccade target’

Why I don’t like this statement

1. It’s getting close to a homunculus view

2. Localised visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

Page 34: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

nextsaccade ?

Page 35: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

Attentional spotlight selects next target

Page 36: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

Attentional spotlight selects next target

Page 37: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

Attentional spotlight selects next target

No effective spotlight

Page 38: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Visual attention should be able to eliminate distractor interference.

Attentional spotlight selects next target

No effective small spotlight

Global effect

Page 39: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

The Global Effect

Saccades to neighbouring target pairs tend to land towardsa centre-of-gravity position.

Findlay, 1981, 1982; Deubel, 1982; Ottes, Van Gisbergen and Eggermont, 1984

Reliably found with onset stimuli: does it occur in free scanning?

Page 40: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

How accurate are scanning saccades ?

SACCADETO CENTREOF GRAVITY ?

Page 41: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Is there a global effect in free scanning ?

• Are saccades less accurate when there is a distractor present in the critical sector (as defined by Walker et al. 1997)?

Subject Nodistractor

Withdistractor

JP 16.9 43.3

LW 14.9 36.7

PB 20.4 38.0

SL 22.8 55.1

Percentage of inaccurate saccades

Page 42: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Probability of inaccurate saccadefor distractors in differentlocations relative tothe target

Page 43: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Is accuracy higher follower longer fixations ?

accuracy coding

4 3 2

1

Accuracy is highest following short duration fixations (although distractors still decrease it).

This is the opposite to a speed-accuracytrade off.

Short(< 200 ms)

Medium(200 – 300 ms)

Long(> 300 ms)

Nodistractor

JP 2.00 2.17 1.86 2.73SL 2.60 2.47 2.19 2.95PB 2.66 2.42 2.33 3.01LW 2.55 2.26 2.49 2.84

mean 2.45 2.33 2.22 2.88

Page 44: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Attentional selection and saccades

• Saccades during a free scan of a set of identical elements show the global effect. Thus no evidence here for a spotlight-like attentional selection.

• In most practical situations, elements are not identical; hence biased competition will act to reduce the global effect

Page 45: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

How does the brain decide where to look next?

• Selection from a salience map is basic

• Supplementary processes influencing salience

1. Inhibition of return

2. Saccade pipelining

3. Strategies

4. Implicit learning

5. Etc. etc. etc.

Page 46: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

THE END

Thank you for your attention

Page 47: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)
Page 48: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Kowler et al (1995)

Page 49: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Do distractors that have been already scanned reduce accuracy ?

accuracy coding

4 3 2

1

nodistractor

scanneddistractor

new distractor

JP 2.73 2.06 2.13SL 2.95 2.41 2.30PB 3.01 2.48 2.63

LW 2.84 2.37 2.48Mean 2.88 2.33 2.38

Accuracy is reduced both by scanneddistractors and by new ones.

Page 50: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

REPLICA TRIALS NON REPLICA TRIALS

How replicable are scanning patterns ? (repeat run with one subject - different trial order)

NEAR REPLICA TRIALS

REPLICAS 26% overall, 68% ring count 3REPLICAS and NEAR REPLICAS 45% overall

Page 51: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Replicability of directional selection

Page 52: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade direction histogramsHeuristic scanners Strategic scanners

Page 53: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade direction change histograms

Page 54: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade Landing Points

Accuracy is largely independent of saccade size

SACCADES WITH NO DISTRACTOR IN SECTOR

Subject PB

Page 55: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade undershoot

1%

5%

10%

Page 56: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade variability : on-axis

10%

5%

1%

10%

5%

2%

Page 57: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Saccade variability : off-axis

5%

2%

1%

Page 58: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Corrective saccades in the multi-element scanning task

Page 59: How does the brain decide what to look at next? John Findlay University of Durham (acknowledgements to Val Brown)

Oculomotor capture in the multi-element scanning task