Top Banner
Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet Department of Linguistics, University of Vienna June 14–15, 2019 Abstracts in alphabetical order by surname of first author. Balogn´ e B´ erces & Garami Balsemin Bedar, Quellec & Voeltzel Chabot Csides Enguehard & Lahrouchi Huszthy Luo Mutlu Nasukawa Passino ochtrager Polg´ ardi Rebrus & Szigetv´ ari Rennison Russo Savu Sch¨ okler Taylor-Raebel Ulfsbjorninn Yoshida
29

Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Aug 05, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Government Phonology Round Table 2019

Abstract booklet

Department of Linguistics, University of Vienna

June 14–15, 2019

Abstracts in alphabetical order by surname of first author.

• Balogne Berces & Garami

• Balsemin

• Bedar, Quellec & Voeltzel

• Chabot

• Csides

• Enguehard & Lahrouchi

• Huszthy

• Luo

• Mutlu

• Nasukawa

• Passino

• Pochtrager

• Polgardi

• Rebrus & Szigetvari

• Rennison

• Russo

• Savu

• Schokler

• Taylor-Raebel

• Ulfsbjorninn

• Yoshida

Page 2: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Sonorant patterns in consonant clusters of West Slavonic –

What’s wrong with the left edge of words?

Katalin Balogné Bérces & Anett Réka Garami* Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Budapest

[email protected] | [email protected]

This study is aimed to describe syllabic consonant forming patterns within a CVCV Phonology frame. We analyse West Slavonic languages and Slovene with some evidence from English. A spreading analysis is generally used to describe syllabic consonant formation (Balogné Bérces 2005, Blaho 2004, Caratini et al. 2011, Scheer 2004). Szigetvári 1999, Scheer 2004, Garami 2019, among others, argue for a left-branching structure, in which a consonantal head spreads onto a vocalic position on its left while Rowicka 1999 and Blaho 2004 assume a right-branching structure. Previous studies argue against a monopositional nuclear representation of syllabic consonants.

The left edge of words, that is, sonorants in word-initial position are examined in this paper since they seem to show asymmetry in their behaviour. As Caratini et al. say the direction of syllabic consonant formation depends on which empty V position is active. In addition, more recently, Savu 2019 proposes that branching direction is determined by which adjacent empty V needs licensing/silencing. Following their idea we argue that SCF depends on whether the sonorant’s own empty V or the empty V belonging to another CV slot is more attractive for the sonorant. This mechanism distinguishes types of word-initial SCF.

(1) Word-internal syllabic consonants in Slovak (2) Word-final syllabic consonants in Czech

(3) Word-initial trapped consonants in Polish (4) Word-initial trapped consonants in Czech

Sonorants in word-internal or word-final position in Czech, Slovak and Slovene are left-branchers. In the case of Polish, Czech and Slovak the word-initial sonorant spreads to its right-hand-side V, but does not adopt its vocalic features – that is, it becomes “trapped” (a non-syllabic sonority violation). Apparently, spreading to a V position of a “neighbouring” CV slot makes a stronger relation than attaching to an own V. However, if we take a look at word-initial sonorants in Slovene in words like rdeč ’red’ we face some difficulties (see Jones 2002). The trill at the beginning of the word is syllabic but has no possible V slot on its left-hand-side to spread to. Furthermore, in words like Slovak hrdlo

’throat’ not every empty nuclear position can be licensed by left-spreading. In these cases the sonorants may spread to their right-hand-side. Our idea is that syllabic consonants prefer left-branching unless they are forced to spread right.

* The authors are listed in alphabetical order.

Page 3: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

References

Balogné Bérces, K. 2005. Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Blaho, S. 2004. Syllabic consonants in Strict CV. MA thesis, Pázmány Péter Catholic University. Caratini, E., T. Scheer, O. Rizzolo & M. Ziková 2011. There are two kinds of syllabic consonants: (Germanic)

left-branchers and (Slavic) right-branchers. Poster presented at OCP8, Marrakech, 19–22 January 2011. Garami, A. R. 2019. #sC vs. #RT: Banned syllabic consonants in #_C position and Magic Licensing in West

Slavonic. Paper presented at ConSOLE XXVII, Berlin, 21–23 February 2019. Jones, J. M. 2002. The status of the “syllabic” trill in Slovene: a phonological and phonetic analysis. Slovene

Studies Journal 24.1–2: 27–45. Savu, C. 2019. Again on the representation of long and short syllabic consonants in Czech and Slovak.

Paper to be presented at GPRT 2019, 14–15 June 2019. Abstract. Rowicka, G. 1999. On ghost vowels: A Strict CV approach. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics. Scheer, T. 2004. A lateral theory of phonology: What is CVCV, and why should it be? Mouton de Gruyter,

Berlin/New York.

Page 4: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Final Obstruent Devoicing, Vowel Lengthening and L. Tommaso Balsemin - University of Padua - [email protected] This paper considers the relationship between vowel length and final obstruent devoicing (FOD) in Northern Italo-Romance varieties with the aim to investigate its phonological properties and formalisation, and to shed light on the interaction between segmental properties (voicing) and structural ones (length). Whereas some scholars have entertained the hypothesis that there is no interaction between vowel length and FOD (Loporcaro 2015 a.o.), others advocated a more direct interplay between the two (Vanelli 2005, Iosad 2012, Torres-Tamarit 2015). I propose that there is a direct and non-arbitrary connection between FOD and vowel lengthening and that a formalisation couched within Government Phonology 2.0 (GP 2.0, Pöchtrager 2006, Kaye / Pöchtrager 2013) is better suited to capture this. The testing ground for my proposal is specifically provided by Friulian. Vowel length in Northern Italo-Romance varieties has phonological status (Loporcaro 2015): [naːs] ‘nose’ / [nas] ‘she is born’ Milanese (Lombard); [goːt] ‘forest’ / [got] ‘drop’ Surmeiran (Romansh); [maːt] ‘alone’ / [mat] ‘crazy, m.’ Marebbano (Central Ladin); [liːs] ‘worn out, m.’ / [lis] ‘smooth, m.’ Friulian. In Friulian the presence of a long vowel is always predictable (with obstruents) based on the voicing of the following consonant: lengthening ensues when the following consonants gets devoiced word-finally: [ˈlade] ‘gone, f.’ / [laːt] ‘gone, m.’; [fuˈgut] ‘small fire’ / [fuːk] ‘fire’; [uˈlive] ‘olive, f.’ / [uˈliːf] ‘olive tree, m.’; [peˈza] ‘to weight’ / [ˈpeːs] ‘weight’. In a rule-based account (Vanelli 2005), the interaction between length and FOD is captured by a rule as: V → Vː | __ /C[+voi]/ #. As the author points out, such a rule is arbitrary, as there is no reason why a melodic property like voicing should have any effect on the length of the preceding vowel. One of the main principles of Government Phonology is the Non-Arbitrariness Principle, that requires every phonological process to have a direct connection with its environment. To establish a non-arbitrary connection between FOD and vowel lengthening, I propose the following formalisation within GP 2.0. In GP 2.0 the elements |ʔ| and |H| (responsible for stopness and voicelessness) are replaced with structural configurations (Pöchtrager 2006). Taking [laːt] as representative, we start with the structure for /lad/ in (1), where |L| (voicing) annotates x3. In GP 2.0 x-slots represent structural space, similar to skeletal points in the Autosegmental Model. xNs and xOs represent Nuclear and Onset projections (vowels and consonants).

In (2) FOD applies, deleting |L|. Since in GP 2.0 every element occupies a structural position, |L|-deletion results in an unannotated empty position (x3) that needs licensing (every empty x-slot must be licensed). In (3) the resulting empty x-slot gets licensed by the domain head xN1 via m-command (the structural relation shown by the arrows from xN1 to x3). In GP 2.0 length is encoded by how many x-slots are “taken up”. When x-slots (structural space) are m-commanded, length ensues. Here, xN1 ends up m-commanding x3, resulting in a long vowel. In conclusion, this account predicts a direct link between FOD and vowel lengthening, as the data show, while establishing a non-arbitrary connection between the two.

(1) (2) (3)

Page 5: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

The Glide Distribution in Kabyle

Berber Kabyle (North Algeria) has two glides [j, w]. Besides lexical glides (that appear in roots), [j, w] can

also surface in the course of morphological constructions. Namely in the three following cases : (i) in the

plural formation, (ii) in the construct state formation and (iii) at the boundaries between verbs/nouns and

verbal/nominal affixes. In this presentation, we investigate the distribution of both glides at the junction of

verbal/nominal bases and affixes. We will focus on three morphemes: -aki (‘this one’) and -ihin (that one

over there’) – which are used with nouns –, and ara (negation) – which is used with verbs.

An inflected noun can be followed by two suffixes: a demonstrative (-aki ‘this one’ or -ihin ‘that one

over there’) and/or a possessive pronoun (e.g. -jnu 1sg). If they appear simultaneously, the order is strict. The

examples in (1) illustrate the behaviour of both demonstratives.(1) (a) axxam-aki [axxamaki] ‘this house’ islan-aki [islanaki] ‘these husbands’ aɣjul-aki [aɣjulaki] ‘this donkey’

axxam-ihin [axxamihin] ‘that house’ islan-ihin [islanihin] ‘those husbands’ aɣjul-ihin [aɣjulihin] ‘that donkey’

(b) θaziba-aki [θazibajaki] ‘this necklace’ isli-aki [islijaki] ‘this husband’ asaru-aki [asarujaki] ‘this hook’

θaziba-ihin [θazibajihin] ˈthat necklaceˈ isli-ihin [islijihin] ‘that husband’ asaru-ihin [asarujihin] ‘that hook’

As the data in (1b) show, when the noun ends in a vowel, then [j] is inserted between the nominal base and

-aki or -ihin, regardless the quality of the preceding vowel. If the noun ends in a consonant, then the suffix

surfaces in its default form.

In verbal constructions including a negation, a glide appear in the same condition as previously

illustrated, however the inserted glide is [w].1

(2) (a) ixðəm ‘he did’ ur ixðim ara [urixðimara] ‘he did not do’

jərwəl ˈhe flew ur jərwil ara [urjərwilara] ‘he did not flew’

(b) izra ‘he did see’ ur izri ara [urizriwara] ‘he did not see’ (or, alternately, ur izr ara [urizrara])

iʃəffu ‘he remembered’ ur iʃiffu ara [uriʃiffuwara] ‘he did not say’ (or, alternately, ur iʃff ara [uriʃffara])

Once again, the quality of the glide is not related to the vocalic environment, as [w] is attested after all

vowels [a, i, u].

The phonetic surrounding may be identical in the cases of nominal and verbal construction (1b and

2b), yet the inserted glide varies. One could posit that the quality of the glide is constrained by the

grammatical nature of the base: [j] intervenes after a noun, [w] after a verb.

However, if we have a look at the extra data given in (3), we see that [j] can also be inserted after a

verbal base. A verb can have three complements, which function as suffixes: indirect object, direct object and

directional – in that specific order. The indirect object can start with a vowel, e.g. -as (3sg.m). In the same

manner as in (1b) and (2b), if the verb ends in a consonant, the indirect object surface directly adjacent to the

verb. However if the verb ends in a vowel, then [j] is inserted. Once again, the surrounding vocalic identity

does not seem to play any role.(3) jənna-as [jənnajas] ‘he said to him’ jəwwi-as [jəwwijas] ‘he brought to him’ ʃnu-as [ʃnujas] ‘sing to him!’

Both glides appear in the same vocalic environment and under the same government conditions. We

propose to analyse their distribution as follows: (i) the glides are indeed sensitive to morphosyntactical

boundaries (some triggers [j], some trigger [w]). However, the question is not about the nominal vs. verbal

nature of the boundary, but rather about how many boundaries separate the base from its suffixes. All

dependents are not at the same level in the word structure; (in)direct objects maintain a closer relationship

with the verbal base, than the negation. In other words, the boundary that separates the verb from the

negation morpheme is stronger than the boundary between the verb and the indirect object. We argue that

these specific relationships are made explicit by the distribution of glides.2

In (4a), we illustrate the inner organisation of the full verbal construction (when all complements and

the negation are present).3 In (4b), we illustrate the inner structure of the full the nominal construction. The

square brackets with numbers represent the different boundaries.(4) (a) 2[ 1[PERS . – V ROOT – PERS.]1 INDIRECT OBJ. – DIRECT OBJ. – DIRECTIONAL]2

jənna j -as -θ -id

he said 3sg.m 3sg.m

(4) (b) NEG. 2[INDIRECT OBJ. – DIRECT OBJ. – DIRECTIONAL 1[PERS . – V ROOT – PERS.]1 ]2 NEG

ur- s- θ- id- izri w -ara

neg. 3sg.m 3sg.m he saw neg.

(c) 2[ 1[GENDER/NUMBER – N ROOT – GENDER/NUMBER]1 DEMONSTRATIVE – POSSESSIVE]2

asaru j -aki -jnu

hook this my

When two vowels are separated by a boundary marked with 1, then the glide [j] is inserted; when two vowels

are separated by a boundary marked with 2, then the glide [w] is inserted.

1 In order to form negation, ur is added before the inflected verb and ara after the inflected verb. Note that a vowel [i] is also present in the root

as a third negation marker in some verb templates (see Bendjaballah 2001).

2 A similar analysis, based on phases and Distributed Morphology (Halle & Marantz 1993, 1995) has been proposed by Piggott & Newell (2006)

to account for the hiatus situation in Ojibwa.

3 Note that in this case, the complements clitisise and appear before the verb.

Amazigh Bedar, Lucie Quellec & Laurence Voeltzel
Page 6: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Conspiracies in Brazilian Portuguese: a CVCV analysis of epenthesis and codas

Alex Chabot – Université Côte d’Azur

Joaquim Maoso Câmara Jr. (1972:46) remarked that one of the main diachronic tendencies

that shaped the evolution from Latin to Brazilian Portuguese (BP) was “the tendency to

reduce, or in certain cases completely eliminate, closed syllables.” Indeed, a salient feature of

BP phonology is the preponderance of open syllables – the result of both diachronic

tendencies as well as active, productive synchronic processes.

In the variety of Portuguese spoken in the city of Rio de Janeiro, known as carioca

(Callou 2009), there is a synchronic process at work which reinforces the general tendency

towards open syllables: epenthesis. However, only obstruents trigger syllableopening

epenthesis; /r l N S/ are possible coda segments in BP. This means there are two kinds of

lexical codas – obstruents and sonorants – and two outcomes – epenthesis and realization as

surface codas. However, the segments which can be realized as codas pay a price: they are all

subject to allomorphy. In addition, although /S/ seems like an obvious candidate for

membership in a natural class with /R l N/, it exhibits certain behaviors which make such a

classi;cation untenable.

This paper outlines an analysis in Government Phonology (GP) (Charee 1990; Harris

1990; Kaye et al. 1990; Harris & Kaye 1990), using the melodic theory of Element Theory

(Kaye et al. 1985; Harris & Lindsey 1993, 1995; Backley 2011) and the prosodic theory of

CVCV phonology (Lowenstamm 1996; Scheer 2004, 2012) to give a uni;ed account of these

divergences in outcome and coda allomorphy. It will be argued that the reason obstruents

cannot be realized in coda position is due to two factors: their melodic complexity (Harris

1990; Cyran 2010), and the strong and weak prosodic positions described by the lateral forces

of Government and Licensing.

Obstruents are melodically complex segments which may not be realized in a

prosodically weak position – this position corresponds to the coda and is ill-formed in BP.

Obstruent epenthesis is a solution to potential ill-formedness; the epenthetic vowel secures

the obstruent a prosodically stronger position. On the other hand, /r l N/ are melodically less

complex, and have no such well-formedness requirements: they are free to be realized as

codas. They are however, subject to allophony, which is given a uni;ed treatment as an

identical loss of melodic material when in the prosodically-weak coda. Finally, this paper will

argue that what the sonorants and sibilants have in common in being possible codas is an

epiphenomenon of their melodic composition; they do not form a natural class based on their

exact melodic composition.

Page 7: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Metrical government in Strict CV-Phonology

Csaba Csides

Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church

Budapest, Hungary

The aim of the proposed presentation is to seek a principled account for the distribution of different types of vocalic position, perhaps by proposing a single algorithm that treats all vocalic positions deployed in the phonological skeleton. I will attempt to demonstrate that vowel-reduction and static/dynamic silence are but two different sides of the same coin, manifestations of relative and absolute silence in the phonological string. Ultimately all contentful vocalic positions are incorporated into the metrical hierarchy, and all empty vocalic positions are incorporated into the phonological hierarchy. The structural force that seems to control the distribution of different vocalic positions is government, which has two different manifestations: proper government and metrical government.

Contrary to mainstream assumptions, I wish to argue for bidirectional government in Phonology. Unidirectional theories have been promoted mainly in the phonological literature. For Scheer (2004) government is strictly right-to-left, while for Rowicka (1997) it is left-to-right. One of my goals is to demonstrate that government goes in both directions but in a principled manner, following a strict algorithm. Evidence in favour of the proposed framework comes from English stress-assignment, syncope, the distribution of different “consonant clusters”. The minimal word-constraint in English can be also be accounted for in a more principled way if the proposed algorithm is adopted.

References Rowicka, Grazyna (1999) On Ghost Vowels: A Strict CV Approach. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics. Tobias Scheer (2004)A Lateral Theory of Phonology, vol. 1: What is CVCV, and why should it be?: Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Page 8: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Towards a derived typology of branching onsets

The purpose of this work is to derive a typology of branching onsets based on the Obligatory ContourPrinciple (OCP), and the CVCV skeleton introduced by Lowenstamm (1996).

Branching onsets have phonotactic properties that distinguish them from simple onsets and otherconsonant clusters: i. they can be split into subparts when reduplicated (see Lowenstamm 2003), ii.they are not subject to Grammont's Three-Consonant Law (see Scheer 1996), and iii. they aretransparent to stress rules (see Scheer & Szigetvári 2005). In the CVCV framework, consonant clustersare represented with an intermediate empty nucleus. This empty nucleus conditions the Three-Consonant Law Law and the syllable weight. If branching onsets are not subject to either of these twophenomena, we should therefore admit that they form a consonant cluster without an intermediateempty nucleus (1).

(1) C

T

C

R

Assuming that skeletal positions are but the root nodes of melodic feature bundles (Jensen 1994,Szigetvári 2004, Nasukawa & Backley 2005, Pöchtrager 2006, Passino 2017), and that the strictperiodicity between C positions and V positions is the result of a dissimilation driven by the OCP(Carvalho 2002:22-23, Enguehard 2018), then an empty nucleus is optional only if two consonants areenough distinct to be adjacent without violating the OCP on each tier of the element geometryproposed by Harris (1994) (compare 2a with 2b).

(2) a.

ʔH

ʔH

*T T

b.

ʔH

T R

c.

ʔH

TR

V

Finally, following Pöchtrager (2001) and Enguehard (2019), we assume that sonorants (consonantswithout a noise element |H|) must be associated to the nucleus on their right in order to prevent it frombeing dropped (2c). Hence, the absence of branching onsets starting with a sonorant.

If we admit all these principles, we arrive at an interesting prediction as to the typology of branchonsets If two consonants are sufficiently distinct to form a branching onset, then any set of even moredistinct consonants can legitimately also form a branching onset. In other words, for any branchingonset with n violations of OCP, there is a branching onset with n-1 violations of OCP (3, followingpage).1

In conclusion, the typology of branching onsets can be partly derived from a concept as simple andfundamental as the OCP. Of course, the predictions are not in perfect adequacy with the available dataon branching onsets, but the result obtained (including /tl/ implies /tr/) must be assessed in the light ofits low theoretical cost.

1 OCP violations are indicated in boxes. The organization of the elements does not correspond to a particularhierarchy. The dash represents an absence of element. The place elements are represented side by side

regardless of their value. According to Harris (1990), we assume that the stps contain both |ʔ| and |H|.

Guillaume Enguehard & Mohamed Lahrouchi
Page 9: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

(3)unmarked

involves 3c

involves 3b

involves 3a

H -

ʔ -

H H

ʔ -

H H

- ʔ

pr

ps ft

H -

- ʔ

fl

U A U A

U A U A

H -

ʔ ʔ

H -

- -

pl fr

U A U A

H - H -

tr sl

A A A A

ʔ - - ʔ

H H

ʔ ʔ

H H

- -

pt fs

U A U A

H H

ʔ -

H H

- ʔ

ts st

A A A A

tl sr

A A A A

ʔ ʔ - -

H - H -

H H

ʔ ʔ

tt

A A

H H

- -

ss

A A

1 violation of OCP

2 violations of OCP

3 violations of OCP

0 violation of OCPa

b

c

d

Carvalho (Brandão de), J. (2002) De la syllabation en termes de contours CV. HDR dissertation [ms]. EHESS. Enguehard, G. (2018)‘Strict CV without Government’, Acta Linguistica Academica, 65(1), pp. 29–45. Harris, J. (1994) English Sound Structure. Oxford:Blackwell. Harris, J. (1990) ‘Segmental Complexity and Phonological Government’, Phonology, 7(2), pp. 255–300. Harris, J. and Lindsey,G. (1995) ‘The elements of phonological representation’, in Durand, J. and Katamba, F. (eds) Frontiers of phonology: atoms, structures,

derivations. Harlow: Longman, pp. 34–79. Jensen, S. (1994) ‘Is ʔ an element? Towards a non-segmental phonology’, SOAS Working Papers

in Linguistics & Phonetics, 4, pp. 71–78. Lowenstamm, J. (2003) ‘Remarks on Mutae cum Liquida and Branching Onsets’, in Ploch, S. (ed.)Living on the Edge, 28 Papers in Honor of Jonathan Kaye. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter (Studies in Generative Grammar, 62), pp. 339–363.Lowenstamm, J. (1996) ‘CV as the Only Syllable Type’, in Durand, J. and Laks, B. (eds) Current Trends in Phonology Models and

Methods. University of Salford: European Studies Research Institute, pp. 419–442. Nasukawa, K. and Backley, P. (2005) ‘DependencyRelations in Element Theory’, in Kula, N. and van de Weijer, J. (eds) Papers in Government Phonology, pp. 77–93. Passino, D. (2017) ‘Unalphabet commun pour la représentation de la mélodie et de la structure syllabique?’ Réseau Français de Phonologie, Grenoble. Pöchtrager,M. (2001) Finnish Consonant Gradation. MA dissertation [ms]. University of Vienna. Pöchtrager, M. A. (2006) The structure of length.PhD dissertation [ms]. University of Wien. Scheer, T. (1996) Une théorie de l’interaction directe entre consonnes. PhD dissertation [ms].University Paris 7. Scheer, T. and Szigetvári, P. (2005) ‘Unified Representations for Stress and the Syllable’, Phonology, 22(1), pp. 37–75.Szigetvári, P. (2004) ‘Manner as a skeletal relation’. the 9th International Phonology Meeting, Vienna.

Page 10: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Is Italian Swedish? An uncommon look at Italian laryngeal phonology Bálint Huszthy, PPCU, RIL-HAS, Budapest The laryngeal phonology of Italian is quite underrepresented in the phonological literature, although it presents a unique panorama. Italian has a prevoiced series of initial lenis stops and a mildly aspirated series of initial fortis stops. The two sets are in phonological opposition upon the [voice] feature, still, we cannot identify postlexical regressive voice assimilation (RVA) in obstruent clusters (Huszthy 2019).

In the native vocabulary of Italian /sC/ is the only kind of obstruent cluster, which undergoes an interesting voicing process, labelled “lexical voice assimilation” in the literature (Bertinetto 1999); however, it is not in compliance with RVA known from classical voice languages, it rather seems to be an optional lenition process which spreads by analogy in synchrony. On the other hand, RVA is not detectable in the loanwords of Italian, where normally entirely voiced and entirely voiceless obstruents stand next to each other, e.g. vo[dk]a, foo[tb]all, a[fɡ]ano ‘Afghan’, e[kdz]ema ‘eczema’, iceberg [sb], etc. (Huszthy 2019).

The characterisation of Italian laryngeal phonology appears to be very similar to that of Swedish (Ringen & Helgason 2004; Helgason & Ringen 2008). The main phonetic difference between the two languages is that the fortis series of stops is heavily aspirated in Swedish, while only mildly aspirated in Italian; however, the lenis set is equally prevoiced, and neither languages present RVA. According to Laryngeal Relativism (Cyran 2011), there are languages which simply “overshoot” the minimal phonetic distance which is required to obtain the laryngeal contrast among obstruents, apparently so do Swedish and Italian. Aspiration found in Italian can also be considered a phonetic side-effect which does not bring phonological consequences, as well as prevoicing found in Swedish lenis stops is also considered phonologically irrelevant.

However, in the combined frameworks of Laryngeal Relativism and Element Theory Swedish and Italian can also be approached as h-languages (Balogné Bérces & Huszthy to appear). The category of h-languages practically coincides with that of aspiration languages, like most varieties of English and German. In these languages the fortis and lenis sets differ in structural aspects, and there is no laryngeal spreading (that is, RVA). However, the h-systems can also include languages whose laryngeal phonology has always been a riddle for phonologists, like Swedish. In fact, if the sufficient discriminability in production and perception is a major driving force in the phonetic implementation of phonological contrasts, languages with voiced lenis series and aspirated fortis series may belong to the same h-system.

As far as Italian is concerned, from a phonetic point of view we find substantial voicing in lenis obstruents and voicelessness in the fortis set; phonologically, however, we fail to identify true laryngeal activity. In the present system, this suggests that Italian can be categorised as an h-language similar to Swedish, making phonetic use of the sufficient discriminability between fully voiced and voiceless mildly aspirated stops. References Balogné Bérces, K. & B. Huszthy (to appear). Laryngeal Relativism predicts Italian. In: Yearbook of Poznań

Linguistic Meeting 47. Bertinetto, P. M. (1999). Boundary strength and linguistic ecology (mostly exemplified on intervocalic /s/-voicing

in Italian). Folia Linguistica 33, pp. 267-286. Cyran, E. (2011). Laryngeal realism and laryngeal relativism: Two voicing systems in Polish. Studies in Polish

Linguistics 6, pp. 45˗80. Helgason, P. & C. Ringen (2008). Voicing and aspiration in Swedish stops. Journal of Phonetics 36, pp. 607˗628. Huszthy, B. (2019). How can Italian phonology lack voice assimilation? Phd Dissertation. Budapest: PPCU. Ringen, C. & P. Helgason (2004). Distinctive [voice] does not imply regressive assimilation: Evidence from

Swedish. International Journal of English Studies 4/2, pp. 53˗71.

Page 11: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

OCP, branching and consonant / zero alternation Xiaoliang Luo

Université de Tours / Ecole Polytechnique [email protected]

Compared to vowel / zero alternation, there are few discussions on consonant / zero alternation (Vaux 2002 for example). In CVCV framework (Lowenstamm 1996; Scheer 2004), Carvalho (2002) proposes that just as Government between nuclei, accounting for vowel / zero alternation, Government also exists between onsets and goes from left to right. He analyses German ['kaos] vs [ka'Ɂo:tiʃ], shown in (1):

(1) O N O N … O N O N O N … k a o … k a o … Ɂ

Following this idea, Luo (2013) has proposed an analysis of the glottal stop / zero alternation in Mandarin compounds.

However, recent claims in CVCV framework such as Enguehard (2018), Luo & Enguehard (to appear), Enguehard & Luo (2019) propose that lateral relations can be derived from branching and contour: consonants branch to each other as long as No-crossing-lines constraint is not violated, and OCP conditions vowel / zero alternation. Furthermore, Enguehard & Luo (to appear b) propose that symmetrically, vowels should behave as consonants: in a diphthong, vowels branch to each other.

One of the consequences of their claim on vowels is to simplify immediately the account of consonant / zero alternation. As examples, data in (1) can be reanalysed as follows:

(2) O N O N … O N O2 N O3 N … k a o … k a o … Ɂ

In ['kaos], no glottal insertion since this would violate No-crossing-line constraint; in [ka'o:tiʃ], glottal stop must be inserted since otherwise, O2 and O3 will be both empty, violating OCP.

As we can see, the major consequence is to forbid two consecutive onsets or more to be empty phonetically. Evidence from Mongolian, when a root ends by a vowel and the suffix begins with a long vowel, an epenthetic [g] must arise: dalai + aas ‘sea-ABLATIVE’ must give dalai[g]aas (Rialland & Djamouri 1984).

This prediction may be contested (by triphthong for instance), but 1) triphthong should imply glide; 2) we must take into account that epenthetic consonants are often not noted in language specific academic traditions. In Japanese phonology, glottal stops (and other epenthetic consonants) are never noted. Consider: anei ‘shadow’, interpreted as [aN.e:] (N as moraic uvular nasal). In hereby attached recordings from http://research.nii.ac.jp/src/en/ETL-WD.html database, both native speaker pronounce [ɁaN.je:], since [aN.e:] implies four consecutive empty onsets, thus ill-formed. Of course, further studies must be done to verify the range of the present framework.

Page 12: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

References Carvalho, J. B. de (2002). De la syllabation en termes de contours CV. Habilitation Thesis,

EHESS. Enguehard, G. (2018). ‘Strict CV without Government’, Acta Linguistica Academica, 65 (1),

29–45. Enguehard, G. & X. Luo (2019). ‘Length and Reduplication of Branching Onsets’, in Szpyra-

Kozłowska, Jolanta ed. Phonetics and Phonology in Action, 193-210, Peter Lang. Lowenstamm, J. (1996) Luo, X. & G. Enguehard (to appear). ‘Strength is length’, to appear in Acta Linguistica

Academia. Rialland, A. & R. Djamouri (1984). ‘Harmonie vocalique, consonantique et structures de

dépendance en mongol khalkha’, Bulletin de la Société de Linguistique de Paris, 79, 333-383.

Scheer, T. (2004). What is CVCV and why should it be. Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter. Vaux, B. (2002). ‘Consonant epenthesis and the problem of unnatural phonology’, Yale

University Linguistics Colloquium.

Page 13: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Unite and resist: How is [m] like [s], and why is [s] so odd?

In MORPHOLOGICALLY SIMPLEX words, there are two homorganicity constraints with an exception each (1). Nasal stop+plosive ( N T) is homorganic, with the possible exception of [ m], e.g.: Turkish zam k ‘glue’, German Am t‘office’. Likewise, plosive+fricative ( TF ) is homorganic, except with [ s ], e.g.: axe, lapse. Both these constraints, their direction and exceptions must ideally be derived from a single principle. Existing theories do not achieve this. I will offer a theory of phonology based on the asymmetry principle which addresses this issue, among others. 1) Homorganicity Chipewyan [tθɛθ] ‘hide’, Cockney [kxɛˑb] ‘cab’, German Zeit ‘time’, Bavarian hupfa ‘jump’

Filiz Mutlu
Page 14: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Phonetic linearisation of morpheme-internal phonological structure Kuniya Nasukawa (Tohoku Gakuin University)

How to derive precedence relations between units is one of the key topics in discussing the phonetic externalization of linguistic structure. This is seen as one of the most controversial issues in the field of (morpho)syntax, since it is assumed that precedence relations are not formally specified in syntactic structure. To shed light on this issue, many different approaches to the linearisation processes have been proposed (Kayne 1994; Kural 2005; Abels and Neeleman 2012, Tokizaki 2013, 2018; Toyoshima 2013). In the literature on morpheme-internal phonological structure, on the other hand, the issue has not been widely discussed: only a handful of papers have dealt with linearisation, even though the properties relating to linear ordering are believed to lie within the scope of phonology (Anderson 1986, Takahashi 2004, Nasukawa 2011). Given this situation, the present paper discusses linealisation processes at structural levels below that of morphology.

The discussion is framed within the approach known as Precedence-free Phonology (PfP), an offshoot of Government Phonology and Element Theory (Nasukawa 2011, 2014, 2015, 2016, 2017abc; Nasukawa and Backley 2015, Forthcoming). Following most syntactic theories, but departing from other theories of phonological representation, PfP holds that no precedence relations between phonological units are specified. Instead, the linear ordering of segments, morphemes and words is taken to be a by-product of the phonetic externalization of head-dependency relations between linguistic units (Nasukawa 2011; cf. Takahashi 2004).

PfP echoes the syntax literature in claiming that the phonetic manifestation of head-dependency relations is parametrically determined. That is, in most languages of the world, the structurally dependent part (which is relatively more salient in terms of the extent of carrier-signal modulation) is assumed to be phonetically realized before the head. By contrast, there are also languages such as Kaqchikel, Arrernte and Kunjen in which the linealisation of phonological structure proceeds in the opposite direction, i.e. phonetically, the dependent is preceded by the head (Nasukawa et al. 2018). This mechanism functions consistently at all levels of morpheme-internal phonological hierarchical structure.

Page 15: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

The phonetic interpretation of Elements: the case of the Italo-Romance dialect of San Valentino Diana Passino Unversité Côte d’Azur & CNRS BCL

1. Phonetic interpretation in Element Theory As pointed out by Harris & Lindsay (1995:47-49), differently from differently from standard generative phonological theory, where one of the main jobs of the phonological rule component is to transform abstract representational objects into ever more physical ones, in classic Element Theory phonological processes remain on the competence side and relate phonological forms. Phonological processes capture generalizations over alternations and distributional regularities and map phonological objects into other phonological objects. Initial representations and representations obtained from a phonological process are phonetically interpretable at every stage of the derivation Harris & Lindsey (1995:36). The phonetics/phonology interface in ET is then understood in terms of phonetic interpretation of Elements. However, different perspectives may be found in the literature concerning the relation between phonetics and phonology in the phonetic interpretation of element-based phonological representations. The traditional stance represented by Harris & Lindsey (1995) maintains that elements are universally interpreted according to their acoustic signature (A, mAss, lowness, I dIp, frontness, U rUmp, roundness) and combination thereof. Slight departures from these assumptions are supposed in Gussmann (2007) and Backley (2011), according to whom some deviation from the typical signature of the element are possible when acoustic space is available and no neutralisation of contrast derives from a slack phonetic interpretation. In the phonetics/phonology interface scenario proposed by Scheer (2014), on the other hand, the phonetic interpretation of an element may correspond to its physical pattern but it does not have to. According to Scheer (2014: 258ff), given two distinct modules, phonology and phonetics, communication can only take place through some kind of translation. In compliance to the workings of modularity, a spell-out operation that converts the output of phonology into units of the phonetic alphabet must be posited: a phonetic item α is assigned to a phonological item x in the lexicon. The two items of the correspondence are not related by a computation that is based on an independently stored list of instructions and modifies one in order to produce the other (phonological processes in standard generative theory). The match of the two items is in principle arbitrary and the exceptions to conversion are impossible (Scheer 2014: 259f.). The one-to-one relationship between phonological categories and their phonetic realization that one often witnesses, according to Scheer (2014:256) has a diachronic origin: freshly grammaticalized phonological processes are phonetically faithful. However, age and cramming of processes can change this situation, as is shown to be the case for Sanvalentinese.

2. The vowel system of Sanvalentinese (Upper-Southern Italo-Romance dialect) Based on fieldwork data carried out in collaboration with Diego Pescarini, the vowel system of Sanvalentinese, an Italo-Romance dialect spoken in the village of San Valentino in Abruzzo Citeriore, is described and analysed, pointing out to a number of peculiar outcomes with respect to the Proto-Romance system. The result is a typologically rare inventory of vowels. The surface inventory includes two schwas (rounded and unrounded) bearing word-stress, three mid back rounded vowels and a very limited distribution of the high-front vowel. To account for these evolutions, an outline of the present system and a reconstruction of previous stages of the dialect are proposed. A suggestion is also made as to the phonological processes and interactions that may have generated such an atypical system. The focus of this talk is on the phonological representation of the underlying vowel inventory, proposed on the basis of synchronic reduction process, contrast and phonetic form, according to the tenets of Element Theory. The phonological representation of the system that is suggested shows that, as opposed to the surface inventory, the underlying system is not so peculiar after all and in some cases the phonological expressions proposed underlyingly, do not correspond to phonetic form that instantiate the acoustic signatures of the element involved and no synchronic phonological processes

Page 16: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

can lead to the representation. This suggests that elements are not interpreted according to specifications corresponding to their physical patterns but that they may entertain an arbitrary relationship with their phonetic spell-out. In the case where elements are spelled-out inconsistently with respect to their physical signature (A spelled-out as schwa in tonic open syllable, among others), a suggestion is made concerning the diachronic path that may have brought to store the inconsistent phonetic item in the lexicon as corresponding to the element at hand. All the suggestions made are geolinguistically consistent with the phonological characteristics of the dialectological area investigated. References Backley, Phillip 2011. An introduction to Element Theory. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press Harris, J. & G. Lindsey (1995). ‘The elements of phonological representation’ in J. Durand & F.

Katamba (eds.), Frontiers of phonology: atoms, structures, derivations. Harlow, Essex: Longman. 34-79.

Gussmann, Edmund. 2007. The phonology of Polish. (The Phonology of the World's Languages.) Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Scheer, Tobias 2014. Spell-Out, Post-Phonological. Crossing Phonetics-Phonology Lines, edited by Eugeniusz Cyran & Jolanta Szpyra-Kozlowska, 255-275. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars

Page 17: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Towards a non-arbitrary account of affrication Markus A. Pöchtrager, University of Vienna, [email protected]

This paper presents a new analysis of affrication of d/t in Japanese, (various dialects of) Brazilian Portuguese, and Québec French, as well as a general theory of affrication. Problem. In Japanese (Labrune 2012, Yoshida 1996, 2001), t–tʃ–ts stand in a special relationship with each other, such that t occurs before [a, e, o], tʃ before [i] and ts before [ɯ]. (Analogously for the triplet d–dʒ–dz.) The distribution of tʃ–ts (and dʒ–dz) can easily be modelled as the spreading of the element I from a nucleus onto the onset with tʃ/dʒ. What is less clear is why an coronal stop is only allowed before a non-high vowel, but breaks down into an affricate before high vowels. The problem becomes even more vexing once other, slightly different affrication patterns are considered. Several dialects of Brazilian Portuguese (Cristófaro-Silva 2003) have tʃ/dʒ before [i] but t/d elsewhere. Again, palatalisation can be explained as I-spreading, but not affrication. Lastly, Québec French (Walker 1984) has ts/dz (not tʃ/dʒ) before [i/ɪ] and [y/ʏ] but t/d elsewhere. What is particularly instructive is that affrication occurs without palatalisation. This paper argues that those three cases can be unified and that the affrication/palatalisation patterns are derivable from general principles. The analysis is couched within Government Phonology (GP) 2.0 (Pöchtrager 2006, Kaye & Pöchtrager 2013). Proposal. From the above facts we can extract a descriptive generalisation: Affrication (though not necessarily palatalisation) occurs before a nucleus that contains I by itself, [i], or one that is empty, [ɯ]. This is true of all three languages. The following questions arise: (a) What do these two kinds of nuclei (or their complement set) have in common? (b) What singles out coronal stops for affrication, while labials or velars stay unaffected? (c) What is the link between (a) and (b) that explains affrication? P1, ad (a). Pöchtrager (2009, 2015) and Živanovič & Pöchtrager (2010) presented a theory of phonological binding that restricts the distribution of elements within phonological representations. Crucially, positions annotated with I and those without any annotation (empty positions) have the same binding requirements, i.e. they cannot be bound. This was argued for on the basis of an in-depth analysis of Putonghua, where the sequences *(d)ya/*(d)wa as well as *(d)way are out, while (d)yaw is grammatical. The argument runs as follows: Onglides sit in a higher position than offglides, i.e. onglides can bind offglides. In *(d)ya/*(d)wa, the offglide position is empty (no glide following a) and bound by the onglide (y/w), hence both forms are out. In *(d)way the onglide w binds the offglide y, ruling out *(d)way. Empty offglides and the offglide y (element I) function alike. In (d)yaw the onglide y binds the offglide w (element U), but since there no binding restrictions on U, the structure is licit. The same asymmetry holds in English (diphthong oy but *ew), cf. Pöchtrager (2009, 2015). This is exactly what we need for grouping [i] and (empty) [ɯ] together. P2, ad (b). In many earlier versions of GP, coronals were characterised by the element A. In GP 2.0, A is replaced by structure, based (amongst other things) on data like these: In English, long vowels before clusters only occur if both members of the cluster are coronals: haunt vs. *haump, *haunk. That is, longer structures are made possible by (the “coronal element”) A. Examples like those are also found in German, Finnish, Hungarian etc. (Pöchtrager 2012, 2013). Since A consistently interacts with structure, it must be structural itself. Under such a reinterpretation, objects that contained old A are now structurally bigger than those without. Thus, coronals are bigger than velars or labials, readily explaining why in English it is d/t that undergo lenition (tapping): They are the biggest objects and thus easy targets (Pöchtrager 2016). This also extends to vowel reduction (typically of non-high

Page 18: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

vowels) in unstressed position as e.g. in Portuguese or Catalan (Harris 1997): Unstressed o/e is reduced to u/i. Again, this is expressible as the loss of structure in the weak part of the foot (Pöchtrager 2018). Lastly, extra size is the key to affrication as well. P3, ad (c). Given that positions annotated with I and empty positions form a set for binding, cf. (P1), I will argue that in affrication we are dealing with a binding violation. Without going into the exact shape of the tree, it is clear that the additional structure characterising d/t must contain a position that can bind following [i]/[ɯ]. In order to remedy this violation, affrication occurs, which, I submit, consists in the removal of one layer of that extra structure in d/t. (Another layer remains to guarantee that ts/dz are coronal.) Following [u] will be unproblematic as U has no binding requirements, cf. (P1). Whether the resulting affricate ts/dz palatalises to tʃ/dʒ will depend on whether the following I spreads or not, but that is independent of affrication. Note also that mid-vowels like [e] do not trigger affrication, i.e. the I contained in [e] must be located in a position that does not violate binding. Further issues. The present proposal bears a certain similarity to Yoshida (2001), who arranges elements in a feature geometrical tree and links affrication to structural properties of that tree. There are at least three differences to the present proposal, however. 1. The constraints that apply in Yoshida’s tree seem tailormade for Japanese, while the current proposal attempts to integrate affrication patterns in several languages into a larger theory of melodic distribution originally conceived for distributional patterns in diphthongs. 2. Yoshida’s account fares well for tʃ/dʒ but remains unclear for ts/dz. 3. Yoshida’s account is able to express changes from k/g to tʃ/dʒ which the present account – correctly, I submit – excludes. In order to go from k/g to alveopalatal tʃ/dʒ, not only do we need to add an I (the source of which could be in the environment), but also extra structure for coronality, and this extra structure can neither simply come out of nowhere nor can it come from the environment (typically a following [i]). The prediction is then that such changes are not phonological, and thus we expect them to be highly idiosyncractic and exceptional. Italian and Polish have such alternations, and the prediction that they are subject to a host of exceptions is correct, cf. Italian di[k]o/di[tʃ]i ‘I/you say’ but evo[k]o/evo[k]i ‘I/you evoke’. This incorrect prediction is avoided by the present model, which also has a larger empirical fit than its predecessors. References. Cristófaro-Silva, Th. 2003. Palatalisation in Brazilian Portuguese. In: S. Ploch (ed.) Living on the Edge. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 243–257. ♦ Kaye, J & M. A. Pöchtrager. 2013. GP 2.0. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics. 16. 51–64. ♦ Harris, J. 1997. Licensing Inheritance: an integrated theory of neutralisation. Phonology. 14. 315–370. ♦ Labrune, L. 2012. The Phonology of Japanese. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2006. The Structure of Length. PhD dissertation, University of Vienna. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2009. Diphthongi, ei know thyselfi. Binding in Phonology. Paper at the “17th Manchester Phonology Meeting”, 28–30 May 2009, Manchester/UK. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2012. Deconstructing A. Paper at the “MFM Fringe Meeting on Segmental Architecture”, 23 May 2012, Manchester. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2013. Alveolars, size and lenition. Paper at the “21st Manchester Phonology Meeting”, 23–25 May 2013, Manchester. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2015. Binding in Phonology. In: H. van Riemsdijk & M. van Oostendorp (eds.): Representing Structure in Phonology and Syntax. Berlin: de Gruyter. 255–275. ♦ Yoshida, Sh. 1996. Phonological Government in Japanese. Canberra: The Australian National University. ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2016. It’s all about size. Péter Szigetvári (ed.): 70 snippets to mark Ádám Nádasdy’s 70th birthday. http://seas3.elte.hu/nadasdy70/pochtrager.html ♦ Pöchtrager, M. A. 2018. Sawing off the branch you are sitting on. Acta Linguistica Academica. 65:1. 47–68. ♦ Yoshida, Sh. 2001. An Element-Based Analysis of Affrication in Japanese. In: J. van de Weijer & N. Tetsuo (eds.) Issues in Japanese Phonology and Morphology. Berlin: de Gruyter. 193–214. ♦ Walker, D. C. 1984. The Pronunciation of Canadian French. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press. ♦ Živanovič, S. & M. A. Pöchtrager. 2010. GP 2, and Putonghua too. Acta Linguistica Hungarica. 57:4. 357–380.

Page 19: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Blackfoot [s] can be a consonant or a vowel, or both KRISZTINA POLGÁRDI

Research Institute for Linguistics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences [s] has always been noted for its special clustering abilities (Goad 2011), but in Blackfoot (Algonquian) its distribution is even freer than what we are familiar with from other languages: short [s] and long [sː] can occur in between consonants, and long [sː] and overlong [sːː] can appear between a consonant and a vowel (Frantz 2017, Frantz & Russell 2017). Goad & Shimada 2014, in a moraic approach, propose that [s] here can in fact occupy the nucleus (monomoraic or bimoraic), from where it can additionally link to a preceding and/or following onset. In this talk, I will re-analyse the pattern in strict CV (Lowenstamm 1996), utilising trochaic proper government (Rowicka 1999), and examine the questions it raises concerning Element Theory (Backley 2011) and the phonetic interpretation of phonological expressions. I will extend the analysis to additional data and to a possible origin of vocalic [s].

Backley, P. 2011. An Introduction to Element Theory. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press Frantz, D.G. 2017. Blackfoot grammar, 3rd edition. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (1st edition 1991.) Frantz, D.G. & N.J. Russell. 2017. Blackfoot dictionary of stems, roots and affixes, 3rd edition. Toronto:

University of Toronto Press. (1st edition 1989.) Goad, H. 2011. The representation of sC clusters. In The Blackwell companion to phonology. 898–923 Goad, H. & A. Shimada. 2014. /s/ can be a vocoid. NELS 44. 135–148 Lowenstamm, J. 1996. CV as the only syllable type. In Current Trends in Phonology. 419-441 Rowicka, G. 1999. On Ghost Vowels. A Strict CV Approach. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics

Page 20: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Gradual phonotactics: Hungarian diminutivesPéter Rebrus <[email protected]> & Péter Szigetvári <[email protected]>

The availability of consonant clusters, typologically, historically, synchronically, hasattracted much attention in phonological theory. Explanations for the absence of cer-tain clusters abound. Many involve representations, using principles and parametersthat make certain clusters illicit (eg GP and its forks), others discuss the difficulty ofperceiving or distinguishing some clusters (or single consonants) in some environ-ments (eg Steriade inter alii).

In this paper we show that consonant cluster types are arrangeable in availabilityhierarchies (which are strongly correlatedwithmarkedness). For example, the plosive-final cluster types can be arranged in the following implicational scale:

TT > NT < RT < ST < PT < MT

(where ‘>’ = implies, ‘<’ = is implied by, TT = geminate, NT = homorganicnasal+plosive cluster, R = liquid, S = fricative, PT = heterorganic plosive cluster,MT = heterorganic nasal+plosive cluster).

Languages select a contiguous interval of these scales. We predict that the furtherup a cluster type is on a given scale, the less available it is, ie the ratio of availableand existing clusters of the given types decreases. So we expect more or as many ofthe possible homorganic nasal+plosive clusters to be available than of the possibleliquid+plosive clusters, and more or as many of the latter than of the possible frica-tive+plosive clusters, etc. The state that some clusters are very common in a languageand the state that others are never found are the two extremes of a gradually changingscale. Grammars often establish arbitrary cutoff points announcing rare clusters tobe nonexistent.

We use data of the two-syllable, i-final diminutive template of Hungarian (eg sün‘hedgehog’ > süni, büntetés ‘punishment’ > bünti, Márta > Márti, történelem ‘history’ >töri, kolbász ‘sausage’ > kolbi, Zoltán > Zoli, izgalmas ‘exciting’ > izgi, Oszkár > Oszi,Magdolna > Magdi, sapka ‘cap’ > sapi) to show that the scale can be used to predictthe ratio of cluster simplification in diminutive formation.

1

Page 21: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

The phonology of noun-class markers in Niger-Congo John R. Rennison, Dept. of Linguistics, University of Vienna [email protected]

Abstract

The phonetic realization of most noun-class markers in Niger-Congo languages looks simple enough: a (C)V syllable with a mono-elemental vowel (/ɪ/, /ʊ/ or /a/). But appearances are deceptive, and in this paper I look in greater detail at the properties relevant to the realization of noun-class markers in Niger-Congo languages:

x� segment structure(s), x� syllable structure(s), x� phonological processes affecting the marker or its host.

We will see that the differences between prefixed and suffixed noun-class markers is far more complex than many researchers imagine – not least because the phonological processes have not previously been recognised or correctly analysed. The discussion will centre around the facts of (prefixing) Venda and Yukuben, and (suffixing) Koromfe and Mòoré. We will look not only at changes to the shape of the affixes (e.g. Koromfe /gʊ/ → [oŋ]), but also at mutations (2 types in Venda) and vowel assimilation/harmony (in the other 3 languages).

Page 22: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

1

Recursion and Phases in Phonology. Conceptual and empirical issues from the Italian ‘innamorare’ / to enamour

The “lexical” Syntactic Doubling (Raddoppiamento Sintattico = RS) typical of Italian

(and Central / Southern dialects) is characterized as in (1) by the gemination of the initial consonant/onset after certain words (triggers of RS) in a sequence W1W2:

(1) a tte → acv-te = at-te ‘to you’ w1 w2 [PP acv [DP tte]]

In this “lexical” Italo-Romance RS the interpretation of the non-specified consonantal position present in the structure of the atonic W1 depends on the syntactic relation assigned to the proclitic and to the category within a phrase: [Prep [Det [N…

If the trigger element is cliticised the RS applies by default. The W1 that trigger RS (/a/ Lat. AD, /in/ IN...) are (pro)clitics with a syntactically fixed place in an XP (here above a PP): [PP acv [DP tte]].

In this talk we aim to discuss a special type of Syntactic Doubling, which affects W1 (mostly prepositions) before a vocalic onset of W2.

The Modern Italian forms (/in/ + Lex) innamorare ‘to enamour’, innanzi ‘ahead’, or Medieval Florentine inn alto ‘Ita. in alto/up’ show a pre-vocalic Syntactic Doubling with the gemination of the final consonant associated to the W1 lexical entry (as [inn] /in/). See the following forms in (2):

(2) Prevocalic RS – Medieval Dialects and Modern Italian ‘innamorare’ ‘to enamour’

Florentine : /in/: inn alto - /non/: nonn à - /per/ : perr ispesa, perrò (Lat. PER HOC); Lucchese /in/: inn anni; Senese /in/: inn una, /per/: perrò; Perugino /con/: chonn uno, Marchigiano /in/: inn una, Aquilano /dis/ : dessonorare Romanesco /in/: inn erva, Napoletano /in/: inn auto /dis/ : dessarmare.

We assume that latent segments are lexically non linearized, and syllabification is established on hierarchical relationships in the sequence W1W2.

(3) Latent segment /incv/ i - n - P c

The prevocalic “liaison” and gemination are an effect of cliticization. We analyse prepositions or determinants W1 as prenominal heads. The inert or active role of the discontinuous segment is decided by syntax: the discontinuous segments are activated only if they are involved in a resulting process of an upper syntactic head. The gemination is obligatory and occurs when the clitic is placed to the left of a lexical category. In our model the syllabic structure has an X-bar structure (4):

(4) Syllables

In (4) Recursion potentially involves Z and W, since they can have the same structure of X: a dependent of a head (here W) is a maximal projection, and Z is an adjunction.

Michela Russo
Page 23: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

2

Thus, a proclitic preposition as /in/ has a hierarchical CV recursive small syllable after i° head, as a consonantal trace of /n/ which is the segmental coda syllabified as onset in : icv.nal.to = in.nal.to. We observe two Phases:

(5) [PP [P i° [AdjP [Adj nalto] Move > [PP [P in [AdjP [Adj .nalto] - Geminate = [innalto]

The first Phonological Phase gives the syllabification of /n/ as an onset (liaison: [i.nalto]) leaving a consonantal trace as a recursive syllable CV in the coda of the proclitic (icv = in).

In the second Syntactic Phase, the syntactic head P triggers gemination according to the rule that RS always requires a geminate triggered by a syntactic head and licences syntactically the discontinuous (latent) segment associated to the lexical entry of the proclitic.

(6) Phases - Unmarked Form [Pi-l [N l-O-K-o-N]] /in/ + /lok(o)/ = [illoko] RS _C

- Marked Form: [Pi-n [Adj n-a-l-t-o-Adj]] /in/ + /alto/ = [innalto] RS_V

The Cø syllables, when a language accepts it, is not constrained by lexical or morphological availability of the nucleus. Cø can only be licensed in so far as it is immediately adjacent to a syllabifying domain. The nasal liaison (to the vowel) indicates an autonomous (re)syllabification: [i.nal.to]. Successively, a linearization process starts in the syntactic head [in] and the dependents [n] and [alto] are treated successively as a single domain [nalto]: [in.nal.to].

In order to achieve a phase, the linearization must be able to start in both a head and a dependent head. The process that starts from the highest head does not integrate its dependent in the lexical status, however, it integrates it in a status resulted from the interpretation of the resyllabified structure: [in.nal.to]. The form [in.nalto] have “two” prefixes as a result of two different phases.

The syllabification immediately identifies a head in the XP, thus, the liaison selects and activates the phonic latent consonantal fragment as a final consonant. It is the phonological interpretation of the structure that produces a direct effect of cliticization.

Another important consequence follows from this. These forms with a prevocalic geminate ([Pi-n [Adj n-a-l-t-o-Adj]]) do not allow themselves to be treated in a phonological and morphological conception of a Phase centred on the Root. These forms exhibit a Phase and iterative treatment effect of syllabification that affects the prefix and not the Root. In the prefixed forms it. innanzi and innamorare, the morphology comes from the phonological interpretation of the lexical and syntactic structures. The Phase effect cannot be deduced from Roots.

References

Hulst van der, Harry (2010). Recursion and Human Language. Berlin: De Gruyter. Formentin, Vittorio (1997). Un fenomeno di giuntura italo-romanzo: il rafforzamento prevocalico della consonante finale dei monosillabi. Lingua nostra 58, 90-104.

Page 24: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Again on the representation of long and short syllabic consonants in Czech and Slovak

Carmen SavuCASTL, UiT The Arctic University of Norway

[email protected]

1. Introduction

This talk contributes to the debate on how to represent West Slavic (Czech and Slovak) syllabic consonantsin the CVCV sub-branch of Government Phonology (Scheer 2004). Long syllabic consonants in Slovak areproposed to not be linked to a skeletal C slot, which means that they have the same representation as Slovakvowels. As for short syllabic consonants, their branching direction is not fixed. They can be linked to the Veither on the left or the right of their home C slot, depending on which V needs melody for a structure to besaved.

2. Long syllabic consonants

Both Czech and Slovak have short vowels and long vowels. However, while Slovak has long and short syllabicconsonants, Czech only allows long vowels and bans their syllabic consonant counterparts. Long syllabicconsonants have been argued to be linked to two V slots, in addition to their home C slot, which leads toa ‘ternary branching’ structure. I propose a ban on ternary linking, and suggest capturing the di↵erencesbetween Czech and Slovak through the links that are present in the lexicon. In Slovak, the link to the C slothas been lost, which allows the segment to link to two V slots. By contrast, Czech maintains the link to theC slot. This leaves the segment with only one more link available. As such, Czech syllabic consonants canonly ever be short.

3. Short syllabic consonants: branching direction

Previous studies on the topic agree that a short syllabic consonant should be represented as linked to a Cslot and one of the neighboring V slots. The question is which of the slots it should be. Arguments havebeen brought forth for both left-branching (Scheer 2004) and right-branching (e.g. Blaho 2001, 2004). Ithas even been proposed that the branching direction is parametric, with Germanic being left-branching andSlavic being right-branching (Caratini et. al 2011). All of these studies have in common the assumption thatsyllabic consonants always have the same representation, at least within the same language. I explore thepossibility that both branching directions can co-exist in one language. This will be argued on the basis ofcases in which one structure needs left-branching (right-branching would not ensure that all empty nucleiare governed), while another structure in the same language needs right branching. Such cases exist in bothCzech and Slovak, which is an indication that the branching direction is not fixed.

(1) Czech syllabic consonant

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

m

�!

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

m

or

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

(2) Slovak syllabic consonant

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

m

�!

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

m

or

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

m

or

m

C

r

V

r

C

v

V

4. Conclusion

I argue that syllabic consonants do not have a uniform representation, even within the same language. Czechand Slovak provide an excellent opportunity to observe various possibilities. Two CV units are at the disposalof a syllabic consonant, with several options for linking. A link to only one V slot on either side producesa short syllabic consonant, while being linked to both V slots makes for a long counterpart. In addition,whether a syllabic consonant has all the powers of a vowel depends on whether the melody is anchored to aC slot.

Page 25: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

— Government Phonology Round Table 2019 —Accent Patterns of Japanese Verbs

Thomas Schökler

While nouns in Standard Japanese exhibit multiple pitch-accent patterns, verbs(as well as adjectives) seem form a two-pattern system, only distinguishing betweenaccented and unaccented stems. Within the inflectional paradigm, however, theaccent kernel (i.e. the position after which the pitch contour falls) is not linkedto a specific vowel in the stem, but seems to shift depending on the su�x(es):shiméru ‘close’, shimemásu, shímeta, shimeraréru, shímechau, etc. Compared to thenumerous elaborate explanations for nouns, previous accounts of this phenomenonhave been rather limited, for example focusing on specific su�xes (Y. Yoshida1995 for -ru and -ta), or simply stating that there have to be “specific rules”(Labrune 2012) for the various su�xes.

In this talk, I would like move away from the idea of assigning individual rulesto each su�x. Instead, I will propose a typology classifying the su�xes based ontheir inherent properties, where the di�erent accentual patterns are the directresult of those properties. Also I want to discuss cases of verbs not perfectly fittinginto the two-pattern system, like káeru ‘go back’ or kotáeru≥kotaéru ‘answer’.Finally, after presenting the problems and possible solutions, I would like to havean open discussion as to how to treat these patterns within a GP framework.

References

Haraguchi, Shosuke (1999). ‘Accent’. In: The handbook of Japanese linguistics.Ed. by Natsuko Tsujimura. Malden: Blackwell Publishers, pp. 1–30.

Kubozono, Haruo (2008). ‘Japanese Accent’. In: The Oxford Handbook of JapaneseLinguistics. Ed. by Shigeru Miyagawa. Oxford University Press, pp. 353–384.

Labrune, Laurence (2012). The Phonology of Japanese. Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.

Smith, Jennifer L. (2011). ‘Category-Specific E�ects’. In: The Blackwell companionto phonology. Vol. 4. John Wiley & Sons. Chap. 201, pp. 2439–2463.

Yoshida, Shohei (1996). Phonological Government in Japanese. Canberra: TheAustralian National University.

Yoshida, Yuko (1995). On pitch accent phenomena in Standard Japanese. TheHague: Thesus.

Page 26: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Cornish Consonant Mutation and Floating Operators

by

Gary Taylor-Raebel

Newcastle University

Cornish, like all Celtic languages, has a series of words, mainly prepositions and determiners which trigger a mutation of the onset consonant of the following word. This study aims to give a review of the types of mutation which take place and an analysis using element theory. A table showing the mutations along with their elemental analysis is given below:

Unmutated consonant

Soft mutation Aspirate mutation Hard mutation Mixed mutation

p ({H,ʔ},U)

b ({ʔ},U)

f ({H},U)

p ({H,ʔ},U)

p ({H,ʔ},U)

t ({H,ʔ},A)

d ({ʔ},A)

th ({H},A)

t ({H,ʔ},A)

t ({H,ʔ},A)

k ({H,ʔ},_)

g ({ʔ},_)

h ({},H)

k ({H,ʔ},_)

k ({H,ʔ},_)

b ({ʔ},U)

v ({L},U)

b ({ʔ},U)

p ({H,ʔ},U)

f ({H},U)

d ({ʔ},A)

dh ({L},A)

d ({ʔ},A)

t ({H,ʔ},A)

t ({H,ʔ},A)

g+unround ({ʔ},_)

Ø g ({ʔ},_)

k ({H,ʔ},_)

h ({},H)

g+round ({ʔ},_)

w ({U},_)

g ({ʔ},_)

k ({H,ʔ},_)

hw ({},H)({U},_)

gw ({ʔ},_)({U},_)

w ({U},_)

gw ({ʔ},_)({U},_)

kw ({H,ʔ},_)({U},_)

hw ({},H)({U},_)

m ({L,ʔ},U)

v ({L},U)

m ({L,ʔ},U)

m ({L,ʔ},U)

f ({H},U)

ch ({H,ʔ},I)

j ({ʔ},I)

ch ({H,ʔ},I)

ch ({H,ʔ},I)

ch ({H,ʔ},I)

Addition of L (cancels with H)

– loss of ʔ if there’s a complex

operator

Simplification of operator

Addition of H Complex

This study is in its initial stages and not all the changes have been completely examined, but as can be seen a preliminary analysis would suggest that the mutations usually involve a change to the operator of the onset head, either by adding an element or deleting one. The addition of an element would suggest an underlying ‘floating’ onset operator following the words which trigger the mutation.

The analysis will obviously need to be fleshed out more fully, but I hope to get some input as to the initial analysis.

Page 27: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Bare Element Geometry: beyond x-bar in phonology

Background

The question of how elements come together to form complex expressions dominates GP work on melody. Previous approaches tend to use diacritics, tiers, templates ({_}_), or an x-bar schema as the organising principle of element combinations. The problem shared by all of these approaches is the stipulation of the tier, template or schema. The x-bar approaches attempt to justify their phonological schema by homology to syntax (GP 2.0 (Pöchtrager 2006, Kaye & Pöchtrager 2013) (cf. Rennison & Neubarth 2003))), however, in the Minimalist Program, syntax itself no longer operates with x-bar phrase structure (Chomsky 1993, 1995; 2004). Proposal

In this talk, I will propose a novel framework of element combination called: Bare Element Geometry (BEG). This is based on the Bare Phrase Structure of the Minimalist Program. The advantage is that its foundations rest on general principles of combination and the first order geometric properties that result from this computation. BEG builds expressions from the simplest combinatory mechanism: Merge (ibid.). This is conceptually similar to Precedence Free Phonology (Nasukawa to appear), except that BEG’s use of Merge to build structure deliberately refers only to melody.1 Moreover, in BEG there is at most one application of external Merge and one application of internal Merge.

In these terms, I will formally define the two ways an expression can be complex and offer the asymmetric theory of headedness. This correctly predicts two basic types of vowel-reduction, one based on cardinality (number of elements in a set) and the other based purely on hierarchy. BEG crucially shares Breit (2013)’s observation that a headed-expression must contain the headed element as part of its overall set. Except that in Breit’s formulation, the possibility of a head (position) is still necessarily stipulated. In BEG headedness emerges entirely from asymmetries in hierarchy (asymmetric c-command), including the possibility of Self/I-Merge.

(1) X(X,Y) The head is grey-shaded X (X,Y) X Y Consequently, single element expressions can be unheaded: (X) or headed: (X(X,Ø)).2 While, two element expressions can come in three unique configurations, unheaded: (X,Y) & headed: (X(X,Y) or (Y(X,Y)). As for combinations of 3 or more elements, BEG seems to make the prediction that three-elements expressions can never be (truly) headless. 1 Melodic and skeletal structure are two distinct tiers related to each other by association lines (not projection). 2 (X,X) is a meaningless because (X,X) = (X,Ø) = (X)

Shanti Ulfsbjorninn
Page 28: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Advantages

This proposal has some further advantages. If headedness is fundamentally a relational property between elements (Harris 1994), there

is a paradox in having empty headed-expressions: ({X}_) (Kaye 2001 (see also Faust (2017))). In BEG this is easily avoided, as there can be no empty headed expressions. Similarly, if headedness is relational, there is no paradox created by simplex expressions that are headed: |I| ó [i] vs. |I| ó [ɪ]. Traditionally, this is seen as a problem: what does it mean for an element to be a head in isolation?

Another benefit of the asymmetric-head hypothesis, is that BEG avoids the problem of having empty expressions (Ø) (empty nuclei) in languages that also have the following Licensing Constraint (LC): all phonological expressions must be headed (Charette & Göksel 1998). This is paradoxical because satisfying the LC means that all phonological expressions must contain an element in the head position. But how then can the language have empty nuclei/expressions? In BEG this problem evaporates because the LC is automatically redefined as in (2).

(2) All Expressions Must be Headed

All nodes must be asymmetrically c-commanded.

Empty expressions, by definition, have no nodes, therefore, the condition in (2) does not apply to them.

Page 29: Government Phonology Round Table 2019 Abstract booklet · 2019-06-10 · Strict CV Phonology and the English cross-word puzzle. PhD dissertation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest.

Constraints on p-licensing in Cairene Arabic

Shohei Yoshida Yokohama National University [email protected]

This paper analyses the three phonetic realisations of the feminine suffix in Cairene Arabic in light of GP 2.0 (Pöchtrager 2006). This suffix is realized as [t], [it] or [a], depending on what precedes and follows it, and interacts with vowel shortening, vowel epenthesis and vowel deletion, as seen below. 1. Underived feminine nouns ending with the feminine suffix

|Sa//-a ‘apartment’ |s≥u˘r-a ‘photo’

2. Feminine noun derived by attaching the feminine suffix to a masculine noun

|s≥a˘ħib ‘friend (s.m.)’ � |s≥aħb-a ‘friend (s.f.)’ 3. The same three nouns followed by possessive pronoun suffixes ‘apartment’ ‘photo’ ‘friend’ Sa/|/-it-u |s≥ur-t-u s≥aħ|b-it-u his Sa/|/-it-ha s≥u|r-it-ha s≥aħ|b-it-ha her Sa/|/-it-hum s≥u|r-it-hum s≥aħ|b-it-hum their Sa/|/-it-ak |s≥ur-t-ak s≥aħ|b-it-ak your (s.m.) Sa/|/-it-ik |s≥ur-t-ik s≥aħ|b-it-ik your (s.f.) Sa/|/-it-ku s≥a|r-it-ku s≥aħ|b-it-ku your (pl) Sa/|/-it-i |s≥ur-t-i s≥aħ|b-it-i my Sa/|/-it-na s≥u|r-it-na s≥aħ|b-it-na our

This paper proposes that the feminine suffix is the sequence of two empty nuclear heads with an onset flanked between the nuclei as in 4, where, among other things, the onset has the a-coronal adjunct (Pöchtrager 2010). In an attempt to account for the forms above, a focus is placed on the phonetic realisation and non-realisation of xN1. xN1 is phonetically unrealised if p-licensed and realised if not p-licensed. This paper argues that xN1 is not p-licensed if (i) “xN*”, a nuclear head preceding xN1 is p-licensed, (ii) xN* m-commands its complement, or (iii) xN1 licenses its onset to m-command xN*’s complement. The possibility of the a-coronal adjunct being moved by Move a is also discussed.

4.