Top Banner
Image: © flickr/srqpix CC BY 2.0 GENDER/GENRE: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PROFESSIONAL WRITING(?) Brian N. Larson January 26, 2015
49

Gender Genre and CPR Theory

Jul 17, 2016

Download

Documents

Brian Larson

Job talk I'm giving today.
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

Imag

e: ©

flic

kr/s

rqpi

x C

C B

Y 2.

0

GENDER/GENRE: GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PROFESSIONAL WRITING(?)

Brian N. Larson January 26, 2015

Page 2: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Housekeeping

•  Communications: See slide footer . . . – www.Rhetoricked.com (these slides + some

additional) – Twitter: @Rhetoricked –  [email protected]

•  Research supported by: –  Graduate Research Partnership Program fellowship (U

of M CLA), 2012 –  James I. Brown Summer Research Fellowship, 2014

Page 3: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Your visitor •  Brian N. Larson •  Ph.D. candidate:

Rhetoric and S&TC •  University of Minnesota •  Practicing attorney

–  14 years –  Focus on Internet, including

copyrights, trademarks, privacy, and media law

Page 4: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

My disciplinary profile exemplifies “mixed methods”

Teaching experience •  Technical and

professional comm’n •  Science, technology,

and law •  Argumentative writing •  First-year comp •  Legal writing (law

students)

Research •  Inquiry focused on

production of texts in professional and technical contexts

•  Mixed methods, including quantitative, qualitative, hermeneutic

Page 5: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Today’s topic: Do women and men write differently?

•  The answer: It depends. •  Before: Stylistic differences common in

studies – Often not clear how gender was ascribed – Texts in mixed genres (or no genres) – No common sense of goals or stakes among

writers •  Now: Some differences, but not the old

patterns. Why?

Page 6: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

I chose texts written late in the first year of law training

•  Law students at most law schools must write a brief or memorandum as their final writing project in legal writing at end of “1L” year

•  Usually in the form of a motion to dismiss or motion for summary judgment

•  In response to a hypothetical case set by the teacher or legal writing program

Page 7: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Gendered authors did not write genred texts differently

•  How should we assess difference? – Stylistic characteristics (though other

possibilities exist) •  What is a genred text? Writer

perceives . . . – Conventional forms and shared social

objectives – Social stakes associated with conformity

•  What is a gendered author?

Page 8: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

This study used an ad hoc gender research construct

•  When I talk about my own data, I’ll refer to – Gender F authors/writers: “Female,”

“Feminine”, “Fem,” “F” – Gender M authors/writers: “Male,” “Masculine,”

“M” •  These categories may or may not

correspond to other researchers’ –  {woman, female, feminine} –  {man, male, masculine}

Page 9: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

This study examined stylistic features (variables)

•  For now, those of Argamon et al. 2003 •  Relative frequencies of

–  429 “function words” (Argamon used 405) –  45 parts of speech from the Penn Treebank

tagset (Argamon used 76 BNC POS tags) –  100 common part-of-speech bigrams –  500 common POS trigrams – Other features, including varieties of pronouns

and contractions

Page 10: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

I calculated mean relative frequencies

•  For each feature – Mean relative frequency (SD) for Gender F

authors – Mean relative frequency (SD) for Gender M

authors – Statistical significance assessed with Mann-

Whitney U test (expressed as p-value) •  A priori threshold for significance: 0.05

Page 11: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

I compared results to Argamon et al. 2003

•  Used 500 published texts from BNC •  Mean 34,000 words (‘tokens’) per text •  Statistical analysis showed

correspondence to Biber’s (1995) “informational/involved” dimension

Page 12: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Biber’s informational/involved dimension figured in earlier studies

•  Biber (1995) labeled this a dimension of register variation after doing cluster analyses on frequencies to identify co-varying features as “dimensions”

•  Consistent with popular conceptions and works such as Tannen (1990 [2001]) that characterize women as “affiliative” and men as “informative”

Page 13: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Argamon: Features males used more vs. present study

Page 14: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Argamon: Features females used more v. present study

Page 15: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

And the pronouns

Page 16: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

The take-away?

•  Statistics: The non-significant differences should probably be regarded as non-significant –  In that case, M-informational/F-involved is not

confirmed in this study •  If the non-significant differences are taken

as suggestive – Evidence for M-informational/F-involved is still

mixed, especially in pronouns and present-tense verbs

Page 17: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Cognitive pragmatic rhetorical (CPR) theory

•  Grounded in relevance theory, a cognitive theory of linguistic pragmatics (Sperber & Wilson, 1995)

•  Enhanced with relevance philosophy of Alfred Schutz (1973, 1964, 1966)

•  HT to Straßheim (2010), who bridged them •  My own additions from rhetoric, cognitive

science, cognitive linguistics, and psycholinguistics

Page 18: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

CPR: The stage is set for production

•  Writer and reader have “cognitive environments,” presently accessible –  Assumptions (“representations” or beliefs about

the world) –  Emotions –  Goals

•  In RT terms, the writer’s cognitive environment is “manifest” to her

•  Her meta-representations about reader’s CE are “mutually manifest”

Page 19: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

CPR: Writer’s process

•  Writer seeks to modify reader’s CE – Making some assumptions, emotions, or goals

manifest, or more manifest than others •  Relevance: Writer balances

– Effort: Attention, invention, conscious stylistic choices (heuresis and lexis).

– Effect: Writer’s goals, modifications she seeks in reader’s CE

– Default: Habitual choice will always be easiest to “find”

Page 20: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

CPR: Reader’s process

•  Reader seeks to achieve his goals – To be educated, delighted, or moved

•  Relevance: Reader balances – Effort: Attention, heuristic comprehension

where stimuli are expected, “search costs” where stimuli are unexpected

– Effect: Advancing reader’s goals •  The most accessible interpretation is the

best one (though not always correct)

Page 21: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Explaining my findings with CPR theory

•  If children are acculturated to writing in certain genres and on certain topics in their youths depending on gender . . .

•  . . . they may unconsciously habituate to certain stylistic choices

•  . . . and may vary their habitual stylistic choices later with great effort –  Diminished relevance in most studies makes this

unlikely –  Communicative production will likely be habitual

Page 22: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Confronted with high-stakes social situation and conventional genre . . .

•  The effect side of the relevance ratio gets much more weight –  Increase in relevance makes variation from

habit likely – Students here exhibited that

Page 23: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

CPR explains genre stability and dynamism generally

•  Varying from genre conventions imposes processing costs on the reader

•  Sometimes, writer can seek to achieve her own goals only by breaking conventions

•  But she has to “sell” it to the reader – Alter the reader’s CE to perceive the

communication as having greater potential effects and therefore . . .

–  . . . being worthy of greater processing effort

Page 24: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Question and answer and Thank You!

•  Communications: – www.Rhetoricked.com (these slides + many

bonus slides) – Twitter: @Rhetoricked –  [email protected]

•  Research supported by: –  Graduate Research Partnership Program fellowship (U

of M CLA), 2012 –  James I. Brown Summer Research Fellowship, 2014

Page 25: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

BONUS SLIDES

•  These slides contain additional information that may be valuable for context for this talk

•  At the end are my works cited

Page 26: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Many researchers have asked

•  Do men and women communicate differently?

•  Much work inspired by Robin Lakoff (1975) •  Scholarly and popular works by Deborah

Tannen (e.g. 1990[2001]) and others •  Much of this research in oral/face-to-face

communication

Page 27: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Writing: Process and product

•  In writing studies, we can (roughly) divide process and product – Do men and women produce writing using

different processes? –  Is the writing they produce distinguishable

based on author gender?

Page 28: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Previous studies: Process research

•  Focus on interpersonal communications in mixed-gender contexts – Lay, 1989 (Schuster); Rehling, 1996; Raign

& Sims, 1993; Ton & Klecun, 2004; Wolfe & Alexander, 2005; Brown & Burnett, 2006; Wolfe & Powell, 2006, 2009.

Page 29: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Previous studies: Product research

•  In technical and professional communication – Sterkel, 1988 (20 stylistic chars) – Smeltzer & Werbel, 1986 (16 stylistic and

evaluative measures) – Tebeaux, 1990 (quality of responses) – Allen, 1994 (markers of authoritativeness)

•  Manual methods, small samples

Page 30: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Gender in computer-mediated communication (CMC)

•  CMC popular for NLP studies –  Data are readily available –  Data are voluminous

•  Examples –  Herring & Paolillo, 2006 (blog posts, stat analysis) –  Yan & Yan, 2006 (blog posts, MLA analysis) –  Argamon et al., 2007 (blog posts, MLA analysis) –  Rao et al., 2010 (Twitter, MLA analysis) –  Burger et al., 2011 (Twitter, MLA analysis)

Page 31: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Rationale: Why is the question important?

•  Lend support to one or more theories of gender –  ‘Two cultures’ (Maltz & Borker, 1982) –  ‘Standpoint’ (Barker & Zifcak, 1999) –  ‘Performative’ (Butler 1993, 1999, 2004) – Others

•  Sorting out methodological problems, particularly use of gender as a variable

Page 32: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Study design goals

•  Research questions –  Did Gender F and Gender M writers in a disciplinary

genre in which they are being trained use lexical and quasi-syntactic stylistic features with relative frequencies that varied with their genders?

–  If so, did the differences appear in interpretable patterns?

•  Examine a corpus of texts –  All of the same genre –  Where we can be confident of single authorship –  Where author gender is self-identified

Page 33: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Data collection

•  Major writing project at end of first year of law school – Students address hypothetical problem

(writing in same ‘genre’) – Students not allowed to collaborate – Plagiarism difficult (but still possible)

•  Students self-identified gender* •  193 texts (mean word tokens = 3764) *This study IRB-approved (UMN Study #1202E10685)

Page 34: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Gender construct in my study

•  Gender construct: “A loosely and culturally defined set of social behaviors that are expected to make it possible to distinguish persons of the two most common sexes from each other.”

•  Susceptible to application of gender label – Gender F (corresponding to Sex F): “female,”

“feminine,” “woman” – Gender M (corresponding to Sex M): “male,”

“masculine,” “man”

Page 35: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Gender ascription in my study

•  Series of “demographic” questions in survey

•  “Gender:” followed by open box allowing free-form response

•  Problem?

Page 36: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Proliferation of labels Response  

Number of participants  

Not answered   4  Cis Male  1  

F   5  Fem   1  

Female   95  female  3  

M   3  Male   84  

Masculine   1  Grand Total   197  

Page 37: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

My job: Make an argument

•  That these responses can be classed together as Gender M –  Cis Male –  M –  Male –  Masculine

•  And these as Gender F –  F –  Fem –  F/female

Page 38: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Text genre: Memorandum regarding motion to dismiss

•  Written to hypothetical court •  Supporting or opposing a motion before

the court •  High-level organization is formulaic

Page 39: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

r

•  t

Page 40: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Memorandum Sections

•  Caption** •  Introduction/summary* •  Facts •  Legal standard of review* •  Argument •  Conclusion •  Signature block**

* Not always present. **I did not analyze (content is highly formulaic)

Page 41: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

NLP methods allow larger corpora to be analyzed

•  Natural language processing (NLP) •  Allows processing of large quantities of

text data •  Study that attracted my attention

– Koppel, Argamon & Shimoni, 2002 (machine-learning algorithms)

– Argamon et al., 2003 (statistical analysis) –  I’ll focus on Argamon et al. in this talk

Page 42: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

What are ‘Part-of-speech’ tags? ‘Bigrams & trigrams’?

•  First, ‘tokenize’ each sentence (automated): –  ‘My aunt’s pen is on the table.’

Page 43: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

POS tags were assigned and tallied

•  Purple words are function words

•  Tag the parts of speech (automated) •  Then calculate relative frequency of

function words and POS tags (automated)

Page 44: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

POS bigrams and trigrams were assigned and tallied

•  A bigram or trigram is a 2- or 3-token ‘window’ on the sentence. –  Automated calculation

Page 45: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Other stylistic features were tallied

•  First-person pronouns (total) –  Singular: I, me, my, mine, myself. –  Plural: We, us, our, ours, ourselves.

•  Second-person pronouns: You, your, yours, yourself. •  Third-person pronouns (total)

–  Singular (total) •  Feminine: She, her, hers, herself. •  Masculine: He, him, his, himself.

–  Plural: They, them, their, theirs, themselves. •  Contractions: Including all instances of n’t, ’ld, ’ve, etc. •  All relative frequencies calculated (automated)

Page 46: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Situating the findings within gender & language theories

•  Findings weakly support or contradict – Two sociolinguistic cultures view (Maltz &

Borker 1982; Tannen 1990 [2001]) –  Intersectionality/performativity views (Barker &

Zifcak 1999; Butler; many others) •  Some gendered linguistic habits appeared

to resist retraining and conscious efforts to conform to register conventions . . .

•  . . . others were apparently overcome.

Page 47: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Are “gender” and “genre” the same?

•  Gender –  < Old French gen(d)re (French genre) = Spanish

and Portuguese genero, Italian genere, < Latin gener- stem form of genus race, kind

•  Genre –  < French genre kind: see gender n. –  ”a. Kind, sort, class; also, genus as opposed to

species”

¡  Oxford English Dictionary (n.d.). "gender, n.". Retrieved from http://www.oed.com/viewdictionaryentry/Entry/77468

Page 48: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Works cited Allen, J. (1994). Women and authority in business/technical communication scholarship: An analysis of writing... Technical Communication Quarterly, 3(3), 271. Argamon, S., Koppel, M., Fine, J., & Shimoni, A. R. (2003). Gender, genre, and writing style in formal written texts. Text, 23(3), 321–346. Argamon, S., Koppel, M., Pennebaker, J. W., & Schler, J. (2007). Mining the Blogosphere: Age, gender and the varieties of self-expression. First Monday, 12(9). Retrieved from http://firstmonday.org/issues/issue12_9/argamon/index.html Armstrong, C. L., & McAdams, M. J. (2009). Blogs of information: How gender cues and individual motivations influence perceptions of credibility. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(3), 435–456. Barker, R. T., & Zifcak, L. (1999). Communication and gender in workplace 2000: creating a contextually-based integrated paradigm. Journal of Technical Writing & Communication, 29(4), 335. Biber, D. (1995). Dimensions of register variation  : a cross-linguistic comparison. Cambridge  ;;New York: Cambridge University Press. Bird, S., Klein, E., & Loper, E. (2009). Natural Language Processing with Python (1st ed.). O’Reilly Media. Brown, S. M., & Burnett, R. E. (2006). Women hardly talk. Really! Communication practices of women in undergraduate engineering classes (pp. T3F1–T3F9). Presented at the 9th International Conference on Engineering Education, San Juan, Puerto Rico: International Network for Engineering Education & Research. Retrieved from http://ineer.org/Events/ICEE2006/papers/3219.pdf Burger, J., Henderson, J., Kim, G., & Zarrella, G. (2011). Discriminating gender on Twitter. Bedford, MA: MITRE Corporation. Retrieved from http://www.mitre.org/work/tech_papers/2011/11_0170/

Butler, J. (1993). Bodies that matter: on the discursive limits of“ sex.” New York: Routledge. Butler, J. (1999). Gender trouble. New York: Routledge. Butler, J. (2004). Undoing gender. New York: Routledge. Cunningham, H., Maynard, Diana, Bontcheva, K., Tablan, V., Aswani, N., Roberts, I., … Peters, W. (2012, December 28). Developing Language Processing Components with GATE Version 7 (a User Guide). GATE: General Architecture for Text Engineering. Retrieved January 1, 2013, from http://gate.ac.uk/sale/tao/split.html Cunningham, H., Tablan, V., Roberts, A., & Bontcheva, K. (2013). Getting More Out of Biomedical Documents with GATE’s Full Lifecycle Open Source Text Analytics. PLoS Computational Biology, 9(2), e1002854. Hall, M., Frank, E., Holmes, G., Pfahringer, B., Reutemann, P., & Witten, I. H. (2009). The WEKA Data Mining Software: An Update. SIGKDD Explorations, 11(1), 10–18. Herring, S. C., & Paolillo, J. C. (2006). Gender and genre variation in weblogs. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 10(4), 439–459. Koppel, M., Argamon, S., & Shimoni, A. R. (2002). Automatically categorizing written texts by author gender. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 17(4), 401 –412. Lakoff, R. T. (1975/2004). Language and Woman’s Place: Text and Commentaries. (M. Bucholtz, Ed.) (Revised and expanded ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Page 49: Gender Genre and CPR Theory

www.Rhetoricked.com @Rhetoricked

Works cited Lay, M. M. (1989). Interpersonal conflict in collaborative writing: What we can learn from gender studies. Journal of Business and Technical Communication, 3(2), 5–28. Maltz, D. N., & Borker, R. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J. J. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. 196–216). Cambridge U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Pakhomov, S. V., Hanson, P. L., Bjornsen, S. S., & Smith, S. A. (2008). Automatic classification of foot examination findings using clinical notes and machine learning. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 15, 198–202. Raign, K. R., & Sims, B. R. (1993). Gender, persuasion techniques, and collaboration. Technical Communication Quarterly, 2(1), 89–104. Rao, D., Yarowsky, D., Shreevats, A., & Gupta, M. (2010). Classifying latent user attributes in Twitter. In Proceedings of the 2nd international workshop on Search and mining user-generated contents (pp. 37–44). Toronto, ON, Canada: ACM. Rehling, L. (1996). Writing together: Gender’s effect on collaboration. Journal of Technical Writing and Communication, 26(2), 163–176. Smeltzer, L. R., & Werbel, J. D. (1986). Gender differences in managerial communication: Fact or folk-linguistics? Journal of Business Communication, 23(2), 41–50. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition (2nd ed.). Wiley-Blackwell. Sterkel, K. S. (1988). The relationship between gender and writing style in business communications. Journal of Business Communication, 25(4), 17–38. Tannen, D. (2001). You Just Don’t Understand: Women and Men in Conversation. William Morrow Paperbacks. Tebeaux, E. (1990). Toward an understanding of gender differences in written business communications: A suggested perspective for future research. Journal of Business and Technical Communication, 4(1), 25–43.

Tong, A., & Klecun, E. (2004). Toward accommodating gender differences in multimedia communication. Professional Communication, IEEE Transactions on, 47(2), 118–129. Wolfe, J., & Alexander, K. P. (2005). The computer expert in mixed-gendered collaborative writing groups. Journal of Business and Technical Communication, 19(2), 135–170. Wolfe, J., & Powell, B. (2006). Gender and expressions of dissatisfaction: A study of complaining in mixed-gendered student work groups. Women & Language, 29(2), 13–20. Wolfe, J., & Powell, E. (2009). Biases in interpersonal communication: How engineering students perceive gender typical speech acts in teamwork. Journal of Engineering Education, 98(1), 5–16. Yan, X., & Yan, L. (2006). Gender classification of weblog authors. In AAAI Spring Symposium: Computational Approaches to Analyzing Weblogs (pp. 228–230).