Top Banner
Familism Through a Developmental Lens Gabriela L. Stein, Alexandra M. Cupito, Julia L. Mendez, Juan Prandoni, Nadia Huq, and Diana Westerberg University of North Carolina at Greensboro This article reviews an emerging literature examining the effects of familism across childhood and adolescence. Familism has been described as a Latino cultural value that emphasizes obligation, filial piety, family support and obedience, and its effects have been documented as primarily protective across childhood and adolescence. This review seeks to organize and critique existing research using a developmental science framework. Key tenets of this perspective that are highlighted in the review are close consideration of how familism develops within an individual across time, manifests itself at different points in development, and impacts child, adolescent, and family functioning. Forty-four articles were examined and categorized with results showing that the protective influence of familism is most evident during the period of adoles- cence. Consideration of expressions of familism and the impact of familism on outcomes during earlier and later periods of development is offered as a recommen- dation for deriving a more complete understanding of the function of familism in Latino families. Keywords: familism, developmental science Research conducted over the past 40 years finds that familial cultural values, primarily termed familism, function as one of the core cultural values guiding Latino families in the United States (e.g., Knight et al., 2010). While the roles of family and familism have been firmly established as impacting the lives of La- tinos (Sabogal, Marín, Otero-Sabogal, VanOss Marín, & Perez-Stable, 1987), familism was primarily conceptualized and researched within adult populations. More recently, the construct has been extended downward and applied to research with younger populations as research- ers examine the role familism plays in predict- ing psychosocial and educational outcomes in Latino youth. However, despite this increased research activity, the majority of the current literature has overlooked the potentially dy- namic nature of familism and has not consid- ered the construct from a developmental science perspective (see Calzada, Tamis-LeMonda, & Yoshikowa, 2013 for an exception). To fill this critical gap in the literature, this article will apply a developmental science framework to the study of familism, with a focus on how familism develops, how it is per- petuated across development stages, and how it relates to outcomes within these stages. We begin by briefly summarizing the existing defi- nitions of familism, describing tenets of devel- opmental science, and proposing an organiza- tional framework for the study of familism. To better illustrate the relation between develop- mental principles and the emergence of familism, we review articles within develop- mental stage, and critique the findings accord- ing to an understanding of stage-salient issues differentially impacting children and adoles- cents. Historical and Definitional Issues Introduced in 1945 by Burgess and Locke (Burgess & Locke, 1945), the construct of familism was defined as a value that character- ized the social structure of traditional modern peasant-based societies as opposed to the indi- Gabriela L. Stein, Alexandra M. Cupito, Julia L. Mendez, Juan Prandoni, Nadia Huq, and Diana Westerberg, Depart- ment of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro. Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- dressed to Gabriela L. Stein, Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 296 Eberhart Building, P.O. Box 26170, Greensboro, NC 27402-6170. E-mail: [email protected] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Journal of Latina/o Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association 2014, Vol. 2, No. 4, 224 –250 2168-1678/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/lat0000025 224
27

Familism through a developmental lens

May 16, 2023

Download

Documents

Anne E. Parsons
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Familism through a developmental lens

Familism Through a Developmental Lens

Gabriela L. Stein, Alexandra M. Cupito, Julia L. Mendez, Juan Prandoni,Nadia Huq, and Diana Westerberg

University of North Carolina at Greensboro

This article reviews an emerging literature examining the effects of familism acrosschildhood and adolescence. Familism has been described as a Latino cultural value thatemphasizes obligation, filial piety, family support and obedience, and its effects havebeen documented as primarily protective across childhood and adolescence. Thisreview seeks to organize and critique existing research using a developmental scienceframework. Key tenets of this perspective that are highlighted in the review are closeconsideration of how familism develops within an individual across time, manifestsitself at different points in development, and impacts child, adolescent, and familyfunctioning. Forty-four articles were examined and categorized with results showingthat the protective influence of familism is most evident during the period of adoles-cence. Consideration of expressions of familism and the impact of familism onoutcomes during earlier and later periods of development is offered as a recommen-dation for deriving a more complete understanding of the function of familism in Latinofamilies.

Keywords: familism, developmental science

Research conducted over the past 40 yearsfinds that familial cultural values, primarilytermed familism, function as one of the corecultural values guiding Latino families in theUnited States (e.g., Knight et al., 2010). Whilethe roles of family and familism have beenfirmly established as impacting the lives of La-tinos (Sabogal, Marín, Otero-Sabogal, VanOssMarín, & Perez-Stable, 1987), familism wasprimarily conceptualized and researched withinadult populations. More recently, the constructhas been extended downward and applied toresearch with younger populations as research-ers examine the role familism plays in predict-ing psychosocial and educational outcomes inLatino youth. However, despite this increasedresearch activity, the majority of the currentliterature has overlooked the potentially dy-

namic nature of familism and has not consid-ered the construct from a developmental scienceperspective (see Calzada, Tamis-LeMonda, &Yoshikowa, 2013 for an exception).

To fill this critical gap in the literature, thisarticle will apply a developmental scienceframework to the study of familism, with afocus on how familism develops, how it is per-petuated across development stages, and how itrelates to outcomes within these stages. Webegin by briefly summarizing the existing defi-nitions of familism, describing tenets of devel-opmental science, and proposing an organiza-tional framework for the study of familism. Tobetter illustrate the relation between develop-mental principles and the emergence offamilism, we review articles within develop-mental stage, and critique the findings accord-ing to an understanding of stage-salient issuesdifferentially impacting children and adoles-cents.

Historical and Definitional Issues

Introduced in 1945 by Burgess and Locke(Burgess & Locke, 1945), the construct offamilism was defined as a value that character-ized the social structure of traditional modernpeasant-based societies as opposed to the indi-

Gabriela L. Stein, Alexandra M. Cupito, Julia L. Mendez,Juan Prandoni, Nadia Huq, and Diana Westerberg, Depart-ment of Psychology, University of North Carolina atGreensboro.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Gabriela L. Stein, Department of Psychology,University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 296 EberhartBuilding, P.O. Box 26170, Greensboro, NC 27402-6170.E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Journal of Latina/o Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 2, No. 4, 224–250 2168-1678/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/lat0000025

224

Page 2: Familism through a developmental lens

vidualism that was characteristic of modern ur-ban societies, and at this point, the value wasnot specific to Latinos. Although familism wasdiscussed as an important value specific to La-tinos in the 1970s, it was not until the late 1980sthat the first widely used familism scale wasdeveloped for Latinos (Sabogal et al., 1987).Familism was conceptualized as being com-prised of three factors: familial obligations (ob-ligation to provide material and emotional sup-port), perceived support from the family (theextent to which family members are reliablesources of support), and family as referents (theuse of relatives as behavioral and attitudinalreferents). In their revision of the familism con-struct, Lugo Steidel and Contreras (2003) ar-gued that past conceptualizations had failed tocapture key aspects of familism (e.g., protectingthe family name, family reciprocity and inter-connectedness, and the subjugation of self forthe family), and incorporated these aspects intheir new measure.

Although all of these conceptualizations wererooted in the experience of adult Latinos, newresearch has examined the definition offamilism in younger populations, thereby estab-lishing its role in earlier in development. Fuligniand colleagues (1999, Fuligni & Pedersen,2002) examined the salience of filial obligationsin Latino youth, taking account the develop-mental tasks of adolescence. Similarly, in focusgroups with Mexican American adolescents andtheir families, participants discussed three dis-tinct aspects of familism: the importance ofclose family relationships, obligations to thefamily, and the family serving as a referent(Knight et al., 2010). In a study with Dominicanand Mexican origin mothers, Calzada and col-leagues (2013) reported that parents discussedthe four factors of attitudinal familism sug-gested by Lugo Steidel and Contreras (2003). Insummary, familism appears to be salient toyounger populations and comprised of the samefactors, but research has yet to consider howfamilism develops within the individual andhow its development predicts psychosocialfunctioning.

Developmental Science

Developmental science offers a valuable per-spective to the study of familism and we pro-pose that using this perspective will further our

understanding of how familism develops over-time and also how it impacts developmentalprocesses and outcomes. Masten (2006) out-lined six principles that can be applied to thestudy of familism. They included the impor-tance of a developmental perspective when ex-amining psychopathology, normative develop-ment within a historical and cultural context,existence of individuals within complex sys-tems, individual functioning dependent upon in-tegrated, multilevel systems from genetics tobehavior to surrounding systems, individualswho are active agents in their own development,normal and abnormal outcomes or behaviorsthat are mutually informative and reveal howdifferent trajectories arise in development, andfinally, longitudinal research best illustrates theinterplay among aspects of development andcontext over time. In our conceptual analysis,we seek to show how individual trajectoriescould be impacted by the cultural familial valueof familism at different developmental stages.Moreover, we argue that examining familismwithout an appreciation of the context in whichit occurs may result in flawed conclusions aboutthe contributions that familism makes to even-tual adaptive or maladaptive outcomes for La-tino youth. Finally, this review highlights theclear need for specific longitudinal work thatcan capture how familism functions differentlyfor youth, depending upon their earlier devel-opment, current level of risk and/or protection,as well as differentially across key contexts.

Integrating across the past literature onfamilism and the developmental science per-spective, we propose an organizational frame-work to guide future research and our currentreview (see Table 1). We posit that obligations,respect, support or cohesion, and family as ref-erent are four central components of familismthat could be studied across each major stage ofdevelopment. Further, we argue that the accu-rate study of this construct requires a consider-ation of both parental and child perspectives.Although some work distinguishes respeto fromfamilism, we place respect along with obedi-ence in our framework for two reasons. Weargue that respeto is a developmentally appro-priate component of familism evident in earlychildhood. The parenting practices designed toinstill respeto serve as a foundation to the valueof familism, primarily because respeto provideschildren with a role within the family and an

225FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 3: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le1

Pro

pose

dO

rgan

izat

iona

lF

ram

ewor

kfo

rth

eIm

pact

ofF

amil

ism

onC

hild

ren

and

Par

ents

Thr

ough

out

Dev

elop

men

t

Ear

lych

ildho

od(2

–6)

Mid

dle

child

hood

(7–1

1)A

dole

scen

ce(1

2–18

)

Chi

ldPa

rent

Chi

ldPa

rent

Chi

ldPa

rent

Fam

ilism

Prim

arily

beha

vior

alm

anif

esta

tions

Pare

ntla

ysfo

unda

tion

for

cultu

ral

valu

esan

dex

pect

atio

ns;

prim

arily

focu

sed

onre

spet

oan

dbe

havi

oral

com

plia

nce

Chi

ldst

arts

inte

rnal

izin

gva

lues

that

unde

rgir

dbe

havi

ors

Pare

nts

cont

inue

emph

asiz

eob

edie

nce,

com

plia

nce,

resp

ect;

star

tin

crea

sing

oblig

atio

nsde

man

ds

Val

ues

inte

rnal

ized

and

impa

ctbe

havi

ors

Pare

ntex

pect

sin

tern

aliz

atio

nof

valu

esan

dco

ngru

ent

beha

vior

Obl

igat

ions

Atti

tudi

nal

Em

ergi

ngO

blig

atio

nsto

othe

rfa

mily

;ne

edto

help

othe

rs

Dev

elop

sun

ders

tand

ing

ofth

ings

shou

ldbe

doin

gin

the

hom

e

Con

tinua

tion

ofde

man

dsin

earl

ych

ildho

od;

grea

ter

expe

ctat

ions

ofob

ligat

ions

for

child

ren

Inte

rnal

ized

valu

eof

oblig

atio

ns;

soph

istic

ated

Obl

igat

ions

and

expe

ctat

ions

the

grea

test

Beh

avio

ral

Com

ply

with

pare

ntal

requ

ests

for

assi

stan

ce(e

.g.,

set

the

tabl

e)

Add

ition

alad

ults

inho

me;

time

spen

tw

ithot

her

fam

ilym

embe

rs;

soci

aliz

ing

oblig

atio

nsth

roug

hm

odel

ing

Tim

esp

ent

doin

gch

ores

,he

lpin

gfa

mily

mem

bers

,in

terp

retin

g

Con

tinua

tion

ofde

man

dsin

earl

ych

ildho

od;

requ

ests

ofch

ild’s

oblig

atio

nan

dso

cial

izat

ion

oblig

atio

nsve

rbal

ly

Incr

ease

dob

ligat

ions

inth

eho

me

due

toag

e(c

aret

akin

g,co

okin

g,an

dcl

eani

ng)

Obl

igat

ions

com

mun

icat

edto

child

Res

pect

Atti

tudi

nal

Em

ergi

ngun

ders

tand

ing

ofbe

havi

oral

expe

ctat

ions

inth

eho

me

Gui

deex

pect

atio

nsfo

rch

ildre

n’s

beha

vior

inho

me

Inte

rnal

ized

valu

esof

obed

ienc

eor

resp

ect

Exp

ect

child

ren

tobe

bien

educ

ados

and

have

inte

rnal

ized

resp

eto

Inte

rnal

ized

valu

esof

obed

ienc

eE

xpec

tch

ildre

nto

dem

onst

rate

resp

ect,

not

disa

gree

orar

gue

Beh

avio

ral

Prim

ary

form

fam

ilism

may

beex

pres

sed;

obed

ient

,qu

iet,

resp

ectf

ulto

adul

ts

Pare

ntso

cial

izat

ion

mes

sage

sdi

rect

and

indi

rect

;m

odel

resp

eto

toel

ders

Con

tinue

sto

dem

onst

rate

resp

eto;

beha

vior

alco

mpl

ianc

eex

tend

edto

othe

rco

ntex

ts;

few

erex

tern

aliz

ing

prob

lem

s

Con

tinua

tion

ofea

rly

child

hood

mes

sage

sC

ompl

ianc

ew

ithpa

rent

alru

les,

low

leve

lsof

exte

rnal

izin

gbe

havi

ors,

less

open

disa

gree

men

tw

ithpa

rent

s,lo

wle

vels

ofco

nflic

tw

ithpa

rent

sif

com

ply

but

high

leve

lsif

not

exhi

bite

d

Con

tinue

topr

ovid

eso

cial

izat

ion

onre

spet

oor

obed

ienc

e;lo

wco

nflic

tif

child

conf

orm

sbu

thi

ghco

nflic

tif

pare

ntal

expe

ctat

ions

for

resp

ect

not

bein

gm

etSu

ppor

tor

cohe

sion

Atti

tudi

nal

Em

ergi

ngFe

elsu

ppor

tby

othe

rsas

pare

nt;

need

topr

ovid

esu

ppor

tto

othe

rs;

need

tobe

ago

odpa

rent

Dev

elop

san

unde

rsta

ndin

gof

need

ing

topr

ovid

esu

ppor

tfo

rot

hers

Con

tinua

tion

ofea

rly

child

hood

belie

fsIn

tern

aliz

edva

lues

ofpr

ovis

ion

ofsu

ppor

tPr

ovid

esu

ppor

tto

child

astr

ansi

tion

toad

ulth

ood

226 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 4: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le1

(con

tinu

ed)

Ear

lych

ildho

od(2

–6)

Mid

dle

child

hood

(7–1

1)A

dole

scen

ce(1

2–18

)

Chi

ldPa

rent

Chi

ldPa

rent

Chi

ldPa

rent

Beh

avio

ral

Dev

elop

men

tof

atta

chm

ent;

deve

lopm

ent

ofsi

blin

gor

fam

ilycl

osen

ess

(e.g

.,tim

esp

ent

with

sibl

ings

)

Supp

ortin

gat

tach

men

t;se

nsiti

vepa

rent

ing;

prov

ided

with

soci

alsu

ppor

t;m

odel

soci

alsu

ppor

tor

war

mth

Rel

atio

nshi

pm

easu

res

ofpa

rent

-chi

ldre

latio

nshi

p;de

velo

psw

arm

,ca

ring

rela

tions

hip

with

pare

nt

Con

tinue

sto

build

ast

rong

emot

iona

lre

latio

nshi

pw

ithch

ild;

prov

ides

supp

ort

toot

hers

;so

cial

izat

ion

mes

sage

sof

unite

d,st

rong

fam

ily

War

mre

latio

nshi

pw

ithpa

rent

s,si

blin

gs,

fam

ily;

high

leve

lsof

fam

ilyco

hesi

on;

satis

fact

ion

with

pare

nts

Con

tinue

tosh

owpo

sitiv

ere

latio

nshi

pch

arac

teri

stic

s:w

arm

th,

supp

ort

cari

ng

Fam

ilyas

refe

rent

Atti

tudi

nal

Em

ergi

ngV

iew

ing

fam

ilyas

expe

rts

inpa

rent

ing;

view

sch

ildco

mpo

rtm

ent

asre

flect

ion

ofpa

rent

ing

self

Dev

elop

san

unde

rsta

ndin

gth

atpa

rent

sar

eul

timat

eau

thor

ity;

beha

vior

atsc

hool

refle

cts

onfa

mily

Con

tinua

tion

ofea

rly

child

hood

belie

fsIn

tern

aliz

edva

lues

;vi

ewpa

rent

sas

legi

timat

eau

thor

ity

Bel

ieve

ultim

ate

auth

ority

,ex

pect

posi

tive

beha

vior

asre

flect

ion

offa

mily

Beh

avio

ral

Beh

ave

inse

tting

sto

refle

ctw

ell

onth

efa

mily

Tak

ing

advi

cefr

omfa

mily

abou

tpa

rent

ing

deci

sion

s;gi

ves

mes

sage

sth

atbe

havi

orre

flect

sfa

mily

Com

mun

icat

ion

with

pare

nts;

seek

ing

advi

ce;

beha

vior

alco

mpl

ianc

eor

acad

emic

succ

ess

asre

flect

ion

offa

mily

Prov

ide

child

with

dire

ctm

essa

ges

rega

rdin

gbe

havi

orre

flect

ing

fam

ilyan

dpa

rent

alau

thor

ity

Les

sop

endi

sagr

eem

ent

with

pare

nts,

perf

orm

wel

lin

scho

ol,

less

invo

lvem

ent

inne

gativ

ebe

havi

ors

Con

tinue

dm

essa

ges

prov

ided

toch

ildre

n;co

nflic

tif

pare

ntal

expe

ctat

ions

for

beha

vior

not

met

Prim

ary

stag

e-sa

lient

issu

esth

atm

ayre

late

tofa

mili

sm

Atta

chm

ent

Dev

elop

men

tof

peer

rela

tions

Indi

vidu

atio

nan

did

entit

ySe

lf-r

egul

atio

nT

rans

ition

tosc

hool

Dat

ing

and

rela

tions

hips

Indi

vidu

atio

nfr

omca

regi

ver

Aca

dem

icsk

ills

Prep

arat

ion

for

high

ered

ucat

ion

Bur

geon

ing

inde

pend

ence

Val

ues

inte

rnal

izat

ion

Pote

ntia

lri

skPe

ople

inho

me

over

crow

ding

;Se

lect

ion

ofda

ycar

e

Car

ing

for

othe

rfa

mily

mem

bers

(fina

ncia

lly,

emot

iona

lly)

stra

inof

care

taki

ng

Obe

dien

t,re

spec

tful

beha

vior

lead

sto

less

asse

rtiv

enes

sin

setti

ngs;

rece

ive

Uni

ted

Stat

esm

ains

trea

mm

essa

ges

pote

ntia

lco

nflic

tw

ithpa

rent

s

Car

ing

for

othe

rfa

mily

mem

bers

(fina

ncia

lly,

emot

iona

lly);

stra

inof

care

taki

ng;

mes

sage

sdi

ffer

ent

from

Uni

ted

Stat

esm

ains

trea

mcu

lture

Inte

rnal

izin

gpa

thol

ogy;

high

leve

lsof

guilt

orsh

ame;

extr

eme

leve

lsof

oblig

atio

ns;

cultu

ral

valu

esga

p

Car

ing

for

othe

rfa

mily

mem

bers

(fina

ncia

lly,

emot

iona

lly);

stra

inof

care

taki

ng;

mes

sage

sdi

ffer

ent

from

Uni

ted

Stat

esm

ains

trea

mcu

lture

;cu

ltura

lva

lues

gap

Pote

ntia

lpr

otec

tion

Foun

datio

nof

pros

ocia

lbe

havi

or;

com

plia

nce;

foun

datio

nof

posi

tive

pare

nt–c

hild

rela

tions

hips

secu

reat

tach

men

tle

ads

toem

otio

nre

gula

tion

Soci

alsu

ppor

tpr

ovid

edto

pare

nts;

finan

cial

supp

ort

prov

ided

topa

rent

s

Obe

dien

tan

dpr

osoc

ial

skill

sfa

cilit

ate

inte

ract

ions

outs

ide

the

hom

e(e

.g.,

with

peer

san

dte

ache

rs)

Supp

ort

prov

ided

byot

hers

;ch

ildex

hibi

tsco

mpl

ianc

ean

dre

spet

oan

din

tern

aliz

esva

lues

Sens

eof

purp

ose,

posi

tive

ethn

icid

entit

y,m

otiv

ate

scho

olen

gage

men

t;an

dac

adem

icpe

rfor

man

ce

Hig

hle

vels

ofm

onito

ring

,w

arm

rela

tions

hips

with

child

,co

ngru

ent

valu

esat

neig

hbor

hood

orsc

hool

227FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 5: Familism through a developmental lens

expected behavior to which to conform, thatalso serves to promote family cohesion duringthe early childhood years (Calzada, Fernandez,& Cortes, 2010). Second, adolescent familismresearch suggests that there may be a theoreticaloverlap among the constructs as obedience, def-erence and respect for adults, and family asreferent are included in their definitions (e.g.,Fuligni et al., 1999; Lugo Steidel & Contreras,2003). In adolescent studies examiningfamilism and respeto separately, there are highcorrelations between the constructs further sug-gesting conceptual overlap (e.g., Esparza &Sanchez, 2008; Lorenzo-Blanco et al., 2012).By placing respeto and familism within a de-velopmental organizational framework, this re-view applies a developmental science model toguide longitudinal studies that are necessary tocharacterize how the emergence of these aspectsof familism relates to one across development,and to important stage salient outcomes. How-ever, we acknowledge that the relationship ofrespeto and familism across time needs to bestudied longitudinally to ascertain whether theyindeed operate as one construct across develop-ment.

Our framework also organizes existing re-search by a closer consideration of attitudinaland behavioral aspects of familism as theyemerge in development (e.g., Sabogal et al.,1987; Valenzuela & Dornbusch, 1994). Attitu-dinal familism refers to the actual beliefs andvalues, whereas behavioral familism refers tothe behavioral expression of those beliefs. Thisdistinction is particularly useful as many haveused behavioral and attitudinal measures inter-changeably leading to confusion in the litera-ture. We argue that it is important consider theinterplay of behavioral and attitudinal familismthroughout development. It is possible that be-havioral manifestations of familism would bemore predictive of functioning in a preschoolerbut that attitudinal familism becomes more rel-evant in adolescence as children become morecognitively advanced and develop greaterawareness of the values that undergird theirbehavior. The behavioral expression offamilism likely results from attitudinal beliefsinteracting with contextual factors (Calzada etal., 2013), and these need to be consideredcarefully. Within each stage, we considerwhether research has examined the impact offamilism on important stage salient issues as

well the contextual factors that may influence itseffects as outlined below.

Method

We identified qualitative and quantitative ar-ticles by using Google Scholar and PsycInfodatabases for all years up to 2013. We used thefollowing search terms: familism, familial cul-tural values, familismo, family, family values,affiliative obedience, respeto, filial obligation,and family obligation, and located 55 articleswithin our age range. Given that our focus wason the development of familism, we selected 44articles that fell into early childhood (birth to 7),middle childhood (8–12), and adolescence (12–18) and measured an aspect of familism with aLatino sample (the majority of studies had100% Latino participants; only four studies hadmultiethnic samples and examined Latino par-ticipants in separate analyses or was a signifi-cant portion of the participants). Table 2 pres-ents the salient demographic information, age ofchild population, familism measure, reporter,and main findings. The majority of studies wereconducted in adolescence (73%) and involvedattitudinal measures of familism (84%).

Studies examining familism have been con-ducted with Latinos from different countries oforigin demonstrating that this value cuts acrosssubethnicity. However, much of this work hasbeen conducted with Mexican origin samples asseen in Table 2 (43% of samples MexicanAmerican). Although not a focus of this review,future work should examine whether these val-ues operate differently across Latino subethnici-ties.

Results

Early Childhood (Birth to 7)

Literature review. The majority of re-search on familism at this stage focuses onunderstanding the parenting practices and goalsof Latino parents, and only two articles werelocated examining familism in particular, andthus, we include articles examining respeto.This reflects our conceptualization of the role ofrespeto, as we argue that it is an early manifes-tation of familism and that parenting aimed atinstilling respeto lays the foundation for theinternalization of familism later in develop-

228 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 6: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

Rev

iew

Art

icle

s

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Ear

lych

ildho

odC

alza

da,

Fern

ande

z,an

dC

orte

s(2

010)

Imm

igra

ntM

exic

an,

Imm

igra

ntD

omin

ican

and

U.S

.-bo

rnD

omin

ican

mot

hers

ofpr

esch

oole

rs(a

ges

3–6)

Chi

ldge

nera

tion

stat

usno

tpr

ovid

ed.

48Im

mig

.M

ex�

31.4

7ye

ars

(5.6

6)Im

mig

.D

om.

�35

.26

year

s(9

.47)

U.S

.-bo

rnD

om.

�28

.71

(4.8

2)

Mot

her

Focu

sgr

oups

with

open

ende

dqu

estio

nsab

out

cultu

ral

valu

es.

Acr

oss

grou

ps,

afo

cus

onfa

mily

desc

ribe

das

both

supp

ort

and

clos

enes

sto

fam

ilym

embe

rs(“

exte

nded

fam

ilyse

rvin

ga

prim

ary

role

inpr

ovid

ing

soci

alan

dem

otio

nal

supp

ort”

)an

din

prox

imity

(“B

eyon

dfa

mily

asa

supp

ort

syst

em,

mot

hers

talk

edab

out

fam

ilyliv

ing

and

spen

ding

time

toge

ther

”).

Cal

zada

,T

amis

-LeM

onda

,an

dY

oshi

kaw

a(2

013)

11M

exic

anan

d12

Dom

inic

anfa

mili

esC

hild

gene

rati

onst

atus

not

prov

ided

.

23dy

ads

Chi

ldag

era

nged

from

3–36

mon

ths

or10

–12

year

s

Obs

erva

tion

byfie

ldw

orke

rC

areg

iver

Beh

avio

ral

Res

ults

also

show

edth

atfr

eque

ntan

dre

gula

rin

terp

erso

nal

cont

act,

incl

udin

gliv

ing

with

exte

nded

kin,

isno

rmat

ive

inL

atin

ofa

mili

es.

Res

ults

iden

tified

five

area

sin

whi

chbe

havi

oral

fam

ilism

om

anif

ests

,in

clud

ing

finan

cial

supp

ort,

shar

edda

ilyac

tiviti

es,

shar

edliv

ing,

shar

edch

ildre

arin

g,an

dim

mig

ratio

n.

Gam

ble

and

Mod

rey-

Man

dell

(200

8)Fa

mili

esof

Mex

ican

desc

ent

(86%

ofm

othe

rsw

ere

first

gene

ratio

nM

exic

anA

mer

ican

)

55dy

ads

Mag

e�

57.5

mon

ths

(SD

�4.

94)

You

nger

sibl

ing

�36

mon

ths

(13.

09)

Old

ersi

blin

g�

72m

onth

s(3

8.04

)

Mot

her

12-i

tem

subs

cale

from

the

fam

ilyre

latio

nshi

psva

lues

Q-s

ort

mea

sure

ofcu

ltura

lco

nstr

ucts

amon

gM

exic

an-A

mer

ican

s(W

ozni

ak,

Sung

,C

rum

p,E

dgar

-Sm

ith,

&L

itzin

ger,

1996

).It

ems

wer

eco

nver

ted

toa

Lik

ert

scal

e.(a

ttitu

dina

l)

Fam

ilism

was

foun

dto

act

asa

mod

erat

or,

whe

rew

arm

than

dcl

osen

ess

infa

mily

rela

tions

hips

coup

led

with

the

endo

rsem

ent

offa

mili

smw

asas

soci

ated

with

mor

eop

timal

func

tioni

ngin

pres

choo

lcl

assr

oom

s(e

mot

iona

lad

just

men

t,pe

erac

cept

ance

,lo

wer

inte

rnal

izin

gpr

oble

ms)

.

(tab

leco

ntin

ues)

229FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 7: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Val

dés

(199

6)M

exic

anA

mer

ican

pare

nts

10N

otre

port

edM

othe

rQ

ualit

ativ

eda

taab

out

how

pare

nts

use

stra

tegi

esto

teac

hch

ildre

nab

out

appr

opri

ate

inte

ract

ions

with

adul

ts,

repr

esen

ting

the

valu

eof

resp

eto.

Mot

hers

repo

rted

that

they

pref

erre

dto

leav

ech

ildre

nw

ithre

lativ

esin

stea

dof

nonr

elat

ive

care

,w

hich

was

upse

tting

,th

ough

acce

ptab

le.

Chi

ldca

reus

eK

arol

yan

dG

onza

lez

(201

1)Fa

mili

esw

itha

child

oron

epa

rent

born

inan

yco

untr

you

tsid

eth

eU

.S.

(im

mig

rant

)Fa

mili

esw

ithch

ildre

nan

dpa

rent

sbo

rnin

the

U.S

.(n

ativ

e)

NA

Use

islo

oked

atfo

rch

ildre

nag

e0–

2ye

as,

3–ye

ars,

and

4–ye

ars

NA

The

surv

eys

exam

ined

child

care

usag

e(b

oth

nonp

aren

tal

hom

e-ba

sed

care

and

cent

er-

base

dch

ildca

re)

Imm

igra

ntch

ildre

nof

all

ages

wer

ele

sslik

ely

tobe

ince

nter

-bas

edca

reor

nonp

aren

tal

hom

e-ba

sed

care

(bot

hre

lativ

ean

dno

nrel

ativ

e).

Mul

ligan

,B

rim

hall,

and

Wes

t(2

005)

Chi

ldre

nun

der

6in

the

U.S

.(g

roup

edby

Whi

te(6

1%),

Bla

ck(1

5%),

His

pani

c(1

8%),

and

Oth

er(6

%))

Cou

ntry

ofor

igin

not

prov

ided

NA

Bir

th–6

NA

Chi

ldca

reus

age

His

pani

cch

ildre

nw

ere

less

likel

yto

part

icip

ate

inno

npar

enta

lca

reat

leas

t1

time

per

wee

k,co

mpa

red

with

Whi

tean

dB

lack

child

ren.

Of

thos

eH

ispa

nic

child

ren

who

did

part

icip

ate

inch

ildca

re,

rate

sfo

rre

lativ

eba

sed

care

and

cent

erba

sed

care

wer

eco

mpa

rabl

e.

Yes

il-D

agli

(201

1)H

ispa

nic

pres

choo

lag

ech

ildre

nC

ount

ryof

orig

inno

tpr

ovid

ed

657

36–5

9m

onth

sN

AD

emog

raph

icva

riab

les

and

child

care

usag

eT

heda

tasu

gges

ted

that

use

ofce

nter

-bas

edch

ildca

reis

mor

efr

eque

ntth

anus

eof

pare

ntal

care

,re

lativ

eca

re,

orno

nrel

ativ

eca

re.

Inge

nera

l,fa

mily

pove

rty

stat

us,

mot

her’

sed

ucat

ion,

hous

ehol

dco

mpo

sitio

n,m

othe

r’s

wor

kst

atus

,an

dac

cultu

ratio

nar

eal

lsi

gnifi

cant

pred

icto

rsof

cent

er-b

ased

child

care

use.

230 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 8: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Mid

dle

child

hood

Cal

deró

n-T

ena,

Kni

ght,

and

Car

lo(2

011)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

an;

29%

ofyo

uth

U.S

.bo

rn(5

4%of

thei

rpa

rent

sfo

reig

nbo

rn);

51%

fem

ale

yout

h;A

rizo

na

204

Mag

e�

10.9

(SD

�0.

84)

(9–1

3)C

hild

-rep

ort;

Pare

nt-r

epor

tFa

mili

smsu

bsca

le(M

AC

VS;

Kni

ght

etal

.,20

10;

attit

udin

al)

Mot

hers

’fa

mili

smva

lues

pred

icte

dpr

osco

cial

pare

ntin

gw

hich

intu

rnpr

edic

ted

pros

ocia

lbe

havi

oral

tend

enci

esin

adol

esce

nce,

Chi

ldfa

mili

smva

lues

part

ially

med

iate

dth

ere

latio

nbe

twee

nad

oles

cent

s’pe

rcep

tion

ofpr

osoc

ial

pare

ntin

gpr

actic

esan

dpr

osoc

ial

beha

vior

alte

nden

cies

.

Mor

cillo

,D

uart

e,Sh

en,

Bla

nco,

Can

ino,

and

Bir

d(2

011)

Puer

toR

ican

child

ren

age

5to

13liv

ing

inth

eB

ronx

,N

Yan

dSa

nJu

anan

dC

agua

s,Pu

erto

Ric

oan

dth

eir

care

give

r

NY

�1,

138

dyad

sPR

�1,

353

dyad

s

Mag

e�

9.2

(SD

�0.

1)Pa

rent

alA

bbre

viat

edad

apte

dve

rsio

nof

the

Sabo

gal

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(1

0ite

ms

ona

4-po

int

Lik

ert

scal

e;at

titud

inal

)ch

ildfa

mili

smdi

dno

tha

vego

odin

tern

alco

nsis

tenc

y(�

0.30

)

Pare

ntal

fam

ilism

was

prot

ectiv

eag

ains

tan

tisoc

ial

beha

vior

sin

girl

sat

each

stag

e.Fo

rbo

ys,

pare

ntal

fam

ilism

was

only

prot

ectiv

ein

5-to

9-ye

ar-o

lds.

The

prot

ectiv

eef

fect

ofpa

rent

alfa

mili

smon

antis

ocia

lbe

havi

ors

was

med

iate

dby

care

give

rst

ruct

urin

gan

dw

arm

th.

Rom

ero,

Rob

inso

n,H

ayde

l,M

endo

za,

and

Kill

en(2

004)

4th

grad

est

uden

tsan

dth

eir

mot

hers

who

iden

tified

asM

exic

an

219

dyad

sM

age

�9.

5(S

D�

0.37

)M

othe

rC

hild

Mot

her

fam

ilism

:A

scal

ede

velo

ped

for

colle

ctiv

istic

grou

ps(M

arku

s&

Kita

yam

a,19

91)

child

fam

ilism

:Fa

mily

Impa

ctSc

ale

(11

item

sas

sess

ing

valu

esan

dbe

havi

ors

(Col

on,

1998

;at

titud

inal

and

beha

vior

al)

Hig

her

pare

nted

ucat

ion

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithhi

gher

mat

erna

lfa

mili

sm.

Chi

ldpr

efer

ence

for

Eng

lish

orbi

lingu

alis

mw

asre

late

dto

high

erch

ildfa

mili

sm.

Tay

lor,

Lar

sen-

Rif

e,C

onge

r,an

dW

idam

in(2

012)

Mex

ican

orig

in;

30%

ofyo

uth

Mex

ico-

born

;51

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Cal

ifor

nia

549

tria

dsM

age

�10

.85;

age

rang

e10

–12

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

16-i

tem

fam

ilism

scal

e(M

AC

VS;

Kni

ght

etal

.,20

10;

attit

udin

al)

Pare

nts’

fam

ilist

icva

lues

wer

ene

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

edw

ithin

terp

aren

tal

confl

ict

for

both

mot

hers

and

fath

ers.

Pare

nts’

fam

ilist

icva

lues

wer

eal

soin

dire

ctly

asso

ciat

edw

ithpa

rent

ing

thro

ugh

the

mar

ital

rela

tions

hip. (t

able

cont

inue

s)

231FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 9: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Whi

te,

Zei

ders

,G

onza

les,

Tei

n,an

dR

oosa

(201

3)

Mex

ican

orig

infa

mili

es;

78.6

%m

othe

rsan

d79

.9%

fath

ers

born

inM

exic

o;48

.1%

fem

ale

yout

h;So

uthw

est

462

mot

her,

fath

er,

yout

htr

iads

Mag

e�

10.4

(SD

�.5

5)C

hild

-rep

ort;

Pare

nt-r

epor

t(b

oth

mot

her

and

fath

er)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)

Pare

nts’

cultu

ral

valu

esw

ere

asso

ciat

edw

ithth

elik

elih

ood

ofus

ing

are

spon

sive

and

dem

andi

ngpa

rent

ing

styl

eco

mpa

red

with

othe

rle

ssin

volv

edpa

rent

ing

styl

es.

Ado

lesc

ence

Ayó

n,M

arsi

glia

,an

dB

erm

udez

-Par

sai

(201

0)

Mex

ican

and

Cen

tral

Am

eric

ande

scen

tpa

rent

-chi

lddy

ads;

pare

nts:

94%

mot

hers

;87

.3%

imm

igra

ntpa

rent

s;ad

oles

cent

s:60

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

55%

U.S

.-bo

rn;

Sout

hwes

t

150

dyad

sM

age

�15

.50

(SD

�1.

25)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

6ite

ms

from

the

fam

ilism

scal

eus

edby

Gil,

Wag

ner,

and

Veg

a(2

000)

and

deve

lope

dby

Ols

onan

dco

lleag

ues

(198

3).

Item

sas

sess

attit

udes

ofre

spec

tan

dlo

yalty

tow

ards

one’

sfa

mily

(atti

tudi

nal)

Fam

ilism

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithde

crea

sed

men

tal

heal

thsy

mpt

omat

olog

yam

ong

fam

ilies

,an

dfa

mili

smdi

dno

tre

duce

the

nega

tive

effe

cts

ofdi

scri

min

atio

n.

Bám

aca-

Col

bert

,U

mañ

a-T

aylo

r,an

dG

ayle

s(2

012)

Mex

ican

orig

in;

7th

grad

ers:

62.1

%U

.S.-

born

;10

thgr

ader

s60

.6%

U.S

.-bo

rn;

100%

fem

ale

yout

h;So

uthw

est

271

dyad

s7t

hgr

ader

s:M

age

12.2

6(S

D�

0.46

);10

thgr

ader

s:M

age

15.2

0(S

D�

0.43

)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;M

othe

r-re

port

Beh

avio

ral

auto

nom

yex

pect

atio

nsT

een

Tim

etab

leQ

uest

ionn

aire

(Fel

dman

&Q

uatm

an,

1988

;at

titud

inal

)

Mot

her–

daug

hter

auto

nom

yex

pect

atio

ndi

scre

panc

ies

wer

epo

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

edw

ithm

othe

r–da

ught

erco

nflic

t,bu

tth

isas

soci

atio

nw

asfo

und

only

amon

gea

rly

adol

esce

nts.

Bau

man

n,K

uhlb

erg,

and

Zay

as(2

010)

Lat

ina

(73%

U.S

.bo

rn;

32%

Puer

toR

ican

,28

%D

omin

ican

,15

%M

exic

an,

11%

Col

ombi

an);

100%

fem

ale

yout

h;51

%ha

dat

tem

pted

suic

ide;

Nor

thea

st

169

dyad

sM

age

�15

.19

(SD

�1.

87)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;M

othe

r-re

port

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(L

ugo-

Stei

del

and

Con

trer

as,

2003

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilism

gaps

pred

icte

dle

ssm

othe

r–da

ught

erm

utua

lity

and

mor

eex

tern

aliz

ing

beha

vior

sin

the

adol

esce

nts.

Ber

kel

etal

.(2

010)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

an(7

4.3%

ofm

othe

rsan

d79

.9%

fath

ers

fore

ign

born

);49

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Ari

zona

711

Mag

e�

10.4

2(S

D�

0.55

)at

Tim

e1

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

The

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)

Dis

crim

inat

ion

pred

icte

dgr

eate

rM

exic

anA

mer

ican

valu

esw

hich

then

pred

icte

dle

ssin

tern

aliz

ing

sym

ptom

san

dbe

tter

acad

emic

outc

omes

.

232 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 10: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Bus

h,Su

pple

,an

dL

ash

(200

4)M

exic

anyo

uth

livin

gin

Mex

ico;

55%

fem

ale

yout

h

534

Mag

e�

13.4

3(S

D�

1.31

)C

hild

-rep

ort

The

Bar

dis

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(B

ardi

s,19

59;

attit

udin

al)

Age

and

pare

ntal

educ

atio

nne

gativ

ely

rela

ted

tofa

mili

sm.

Em

otio

nal

conn

ectio

nto

pare

nts

rela

ted

tofa

mili

smin

girl

sbu

tno

tbo

ys.

Pare

ntal

mon

itori

ngas

soci

ated

with

fam

ilism

but

not

afte

rta

king

into

acco

unt

pare

ntal

auth

ority

.Pa

rent

alle

gitim

ate

auth

ority

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithfa

mili

sm.

Del

gado

,U

pdeg

raff

,R

oosa

,an

dU

mañ

a-T

aylo

r(2

011)

Mex

ican

orig

in(6

6an

d67

%of

pare

nts

fore

ign

born

;62

%of

targ

etyo

uth

U.S

.bo

rn);

targ

etyo

uth

(7th

grad

ers)

51%

fem

ale;

olde

rsi

blin

gs:

50%

fem

ale;

Ari

zona

246

tria

dsT

arge

tch

ildre

n:M

age

�12

.8(S

D�

.57)

;ol

der

sibl

ings

Mag

e�

15.7

0(S

D�

1.50

)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

16-i

tem

fam

ilism

scal

e(M

AC

VS:

Kni

ght

etal

.,20

10;

attit

udin

al)

Pate

rnal

attit

udin

alfa

mili

smpr

edic

ted

few

erde

vian

tpe

eras

soci

atio

ns,

adol

esce

ntfa

mili

smas

soci

ated

biva

riat

ely

with

less

depr

essi

vesy

mpt

oms,

risk

ybe

havi

ors,

and

devi

ant

peer

s.

Eas

tan

dW

eisn

er(2

009)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

an;

85%

ofyo

uth

U.S

.bo

rn;

60%

fem

ale

yout

h;ol

der

sibl

ings

insa

mpl

eha

dte

enag

epr

egna

ncy;

sout

hern

Cal

ifor

nia

110

dyad

sM

age

�13

.9(S

D�

1.83

)C

hild

-rep

ort

5ite

ms

onfa

mili

alob

ligat

ions

scal

eby

Sabo

gal,

Mar

ín,

Ote

ro-S

abog

al,

Van

Oss

Mar

ín,

and

Pere

z-St

able

(198

7;at

titud

inal

)ca

regi

ving

hour

sto

baby

(beh

avio

ral)

Car

egiv

ing

pred

icte

dan

incr

ease

insc

hool

abse

nces

and

disc

iplin

ary

prob

lem

s.Fa

mily

oblig

atio

nsw

ere

not

prot

ectiv

eag

ains

tca

regi

ving

stre

ssbu

t,ra

ther

,fu

rthe

rco

mpr

omis

edyo

uths

’w

ell-

bein

gfo

rth

ose

who

wer

ehi

ghly

invo

lved

inth

eir

fam

ily’s

care

.

Esp

arza

and

Sánc

hez

(200

8)42

%;

Mex

ican

orig

in39

%Pu

erto

Ric

anor

igin

;16

%ot

her

Lat

ino;

3%bi

raci

al;

32%

1st

gene

ratio

n;51

%2n

dge

nera

tion;

52%

fem

ale

yout

h;ur

ban

area

s

143

17.8

7ye

ars

(SD

�0.

66)

Chi

ld-r

epor

tT

heFa

mili

smSc

ale

(Lug

o-St

eide

l&

Con

trer

as,

2003

;at

titud

inal

)

Hig

hat

titud

inal

fam

ilism

pred

icte

dgr

eate

rac

adem

icef

fort

.A

lso,

whe

nm

othe

rs’

educ

atio

nal

leve

lw

aslo

w,

attit

udin

alfa

mili

smw

aspo

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

edto

stud

ents

’G

PA.

(tab

leco

ntin

ues)

233FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 11: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Fulig

nian

dPe

ders

on(2

002)

34%

Filip

ino,

15%

Eas

tA

sian

,26

%L

atin

Am

eric

an,

25%

Eur

opea

nA

mer

ican

;53

%fe

mal

e

745

Mag

e�

20.1

Chi

ld-r

epor

tFa

mily

oblig

atio

nsc

ales

:fa

mily

resp

ect,

curr

ent

assi

stan

ce,

and

futu

resu

ppor

t(F

ulig

ni,

Tse

ng,

&L

am,

1999

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilyob

ligat

ions

incr

ease

din

late

adol

esce

nce

and

wer

ere

late

dto

bette

rem

otio

nal

wel

l-be

ing

and

educ

atio

nal

pers

iste

nce

for

adol

esce

nts

rece

ivin

glo

wto

mod

erat

egr

ades

in12

thgr

ade.

Fulig

ni,

Tse

ng,

and

Lam

(199

9)38

%Fi

lipin

oor

igin

,13

%C

hine

seor

igin

,15

%M

exic

anor

igin

,12

%C

entr

al/S

outh

Am

eric

anor

igin

,23

%E

urop

ean

orig

in;

29%

1st

gene

ratio

n;44

%se

cond

gene

ratio

n;27

%3�

gene

ratio

n;54

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

sout

hern

Cal

ifor

nia

820

10th

grad

ers

(Mag

e�

15.7

year

s);

12th

grad

ers

(M�

17.7

)

Chi

ld-r

epor

tFa

mily

oblig

atio

nsc

ales

:fa

mily

resp

ect,

curr

ent

assi

stan

ce,

and

futu

resu

ppor

t(F

ulig

ni,

Tse

ng,

&L

am,

1999

;at

titud

inal

)

All

thre

esc

ales

asso

ciat

edgr

eate

rpa

tern

alan

dm

ater

ial

cohe

sion

and

bette

rco

mm

unic

atio

nw

ithfa

mily

.A

llth

ree

scal

esas

soci

ated

with

grea

ter

stud

ytim

e,an

dre

spec

tan

dcu

rren

tas

sist

ance

asso

ciat

edw

ithed

ucat

iona

las

pira

tions

and

expe

ctat

ions

.C

urvi

linea

ras

soci

atio

nw

ithgr

ades

such

that

the

mod

erat

een

dors

emen

tof

curr

ent

assi

stan

cem

ost

prot

ectiv

e.

Ger

mán

,G

onza

lez,

and

Dum

ka(2

009)

Mex

ican

orig

infa

mili

es;

did

not

repo

rt%

fore

ign

born

;50

.6%

fem

ale

yout

h;79

.1%

U.S

.-B

orn

adol

esce

nts

Phoe

nix,

Ari

zona

598 ad

oles

cent

s,57

3m

othe

rs,

331

fath

ers

Mag

e�

12.3

;(a

gera

nge

�11

–14)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

16ite

ms

wer

eta

ken

from

thre

efa

mili

smsu

bsca

les

from

the

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)

Ado

lesc

ent,

mat

erna

l,an

dpa

tern

alfa

mili

smva

lues

inte

ract

edpr

otec

tivel

yw

ithde

vian

tpe

eraf

filia

tions

topr

edic

tlo

wer

leve

lsof

teac

her

repo

rted

exte

rnal

izin

gpr

oble

ms.

The

sere

latio

nsw

ere

not

foun

dw

ithpa

rent

repo

rts

ofad

oles

cent

exte

rnal

izin

gpr

oble

ms

alth

ough

thes

em

odel

ssh

owed

adi

rect

,pr

otec

tive

effe

ctof

mat

erna

lfa

mili

smva

lues

.

234 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 12: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Gil,

Wag

ner,

and

Veg

a(2

000)

40%

Cub

ans;

13%

Nic

arag

uans

;47

%ot

her

Lat

ino;

All

mal

esa

mpl

e;52

%fo

reig

nbo

rn;

Sout

hFl

orid

a

2,01

96t

han

d7t

hgr

ader

sfo

llow

ed3

year

sC

hild

-rep

ort

7-ite

mfa

mili

smm

easu

re(O

lson

and

colle

ague

s,19

83;

attit

udin

al)

Acc

ultu

ratio

nan

dac

cultu

rativ

est

ress

asso

ciat

edw

ithin

crea

sed

alco

hol

use

thro

ugh

the

dete

rior

atio

nof

Lat

ino

fam

ilyva

lues

,at

titud

es,

and

fam

ilist

icbe

havi

ors.

The

rela

tions

hip

betw

een

accu

ltura

tive

stre

ssan

dal

coho

lus

ew

asin

fluen

ced

byna

tivity

.

Gui

lam

o-R

amos

etal

.(2

007)

70%

Dom

inic

anan

d30

%Pu

erto

Ric

an;

80%

mot

hers

fore

ign

born

;50

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Bro

nx,

NY

63m

othe

r-ad

oles

cent

dyad

s

11–1

4ye

ars

old

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;M

othe

r-re

port

Focu

sgr

oups

(atti

tudi

nal

and

beha

vior

al)

Con

tent

anal

ysis

ofpa

rent

s’fo

cus

grou

psre

veal

edfiv

ees

sent

ial

Lat

ino

pare

ntin

gpr

actic

esde

scri

bed

bybo

thyo

uth

and

thei

rm

othe

rs.

Kia

ngan

dFu

ligni

(200

9)41

%L

atin

Am

eric

an38

%A

sian

,an

d21

%E

urop

ean;

50%

fem

ale

yout

h;L

osA

ngel

esar

ea

679

Mag

e�

14.8

7(S

D�

0.40

)C

hild

-rep

ort

Fam

ilyre

spec

t(F

ulig

niet

al.

1999

);fa

mily

oblig

atio

nssc

ale

(Ful

igni

etal

.19

99;

attit

udin

al)

Eth

nic

iden

tity

was

mor

est

rong

lyre

late

dto

fam

ilyre

spec

tan

dob

ligat

ions

than

cohe

sion

.A

dole

scen

tsfr

omL

atin

Am

eric

anan

dA

sian

back

grou

nds

repo

rted

sign

ifica

ntly

high

erle

vels

ofob

ligat

ion

and

assi

stan

ceas

com

pare

dw

ithad

oles

cent

sw

ithE

urop

ean

back

grou

nds,

and

thes

eet

hnic

diff

eren

ces

wer

em

edia

ted

byet

hnic

iden

tity.

Dai

lydi

ary

data

for

com

plet

ion

of8

filia

lob

ligat

ion

task

s(b

ehav

iora

l)

Kni

ght

etal

.(2

011)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

an(7

4.3%

ofm

othe

rsan

d79

.9%

fath

ers

fore

ign

born

);49

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Ari

zona

750 ad

oles

cent

san

dm

othe

rs,

467

fath

ers

Mag

e�

10.4

2qa

(SD

�0.

55)

atT

ime

1

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(M

AC

VS;

Kni

ght

etal

.,20

10;

attit

udin

al)

The

soci

aliz

atio

nof

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anva

lues

was

prim

arily

afu

nctio

nof

mot

hers

’M

exic

anA

mer

ican

valu

esan

det

hnic

soci

aliz

atio

n.Fa

ther

sva

lues

orso

cial

izat

ion

not

rela

ted

toyo

uth

endo

rsem

ent.

(tab

leco

ntin

ues)

235FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 13: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Kuh

lber

g,Pe

ña,

and

Zay

as(2

010)

Lat

ina

(72%

U.S

.bo

rn;

35%

Puer

toR

ican

,28

%D

omin

can,

12%

Mex

ican

,10

%C

olom

bian

;15

%ot

her)

100%

fem

ale

yout

h;53

.54%

suic

ide

atte

mpt

ers;

Nor

thea

st

226

Mag

e�

15.4

7(S

D�

2.01

)C

hild

-rep

ort

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(L

ugo

Stei

del

&C

ontr

eras

,20

03;

attit

udin

al)

Fam

ilism

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithlo

wer

leve

lsof

pare

nt–a

dole

scen

tco

nflic

t,bu

thi

gher

leve

lsof

inte

rnal

izin

gbe

havi

ors.

Not

asso

ciat

edw

ithsu

icid

eat

tem

pthi

stor

y.

Kup

erm

inc,

Jurk

ovic

,an

dC

asey

(200

9)L

atin

o(6

5.3%

Mex

ican

orig

in;

16.3

%C

entr

alA

mer

ican

12.2

%C

arib

bean

;6.

1%So

uth

Am

eric

an)

74%

yout

hfo

reig

nbo

rn;

64%

fem

ale

yout

h;So

uthe

ast

129

Mag

e�

16.8

(SD

�1.

15)

Chi

ld-r

epor

tFi

lial

Res

pons

ibili

tySc

ale-

You

th(F

RS-

Y;

Jurk

ovic

,K

uper

min

c,Sa

rac,

&W

eiss

haar

,20

05;

beha

vior

al)

Filia

lob

ligat

ions

rela

ted

tole

ssps

ycho

logi

cal

dist

ress

,m

ore

soci

alco

mpe

tenc

e,an

dgr

eate

rse

lf-e

ffica

cy;

Perc

eive

dfa

irne

ssof

oblig

atio

nsal

soa

pred

icto

rof

psyc

holo

gica

ldi

stre

ss.

Lor

enzo

-Bla

nco

etal

.(2

012)

His

pani

c(8

4%U

.S.

Bor

n;84

%ha

dM

exic

anpa

rent

s;9%

El

Salv

ador

ian

pare

nts,

6%G

uate

mal

anpa

rent

s);

53%

fem

ale

yout

h;So

uthe

rnC

alif

orni

a

1,92

29–

11th

grad

est

uden

ts;

86%

ofsa

mpl

ew

as14

Chi

ld-r

epor

tT

hree

ofth

eite

ms

from

the

fam

ilism

scal

eSa

boga

let

al.

(198

7),

and

one

item

cam

efr

omth

efa

mili

smsc

ale

desc

ribe

dby

Cue

llar,

Arn

old,

and

Gon

zale

z(1

995)

and

Cue

llar,

Arn

old,

and

Mal

dona

do(1

995)

.Fo

urite

ms

asse

ssed

the

cultu

ral

valu

eof

resp

eto

(Ung

eret

al.,

2002

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilism

and

resp

eto

wer

eas

soci

ated

with

high

erfa

mily

cohe

sion

and

low

erfa

mily

confl

ict,

and

this

effe

ctw

asst

rong

erfo

rgi

rls

than

boys

.B

oth

accu

ltura

tion

and

encu

ltura

tion

wer

ere

late

dto

grea

ter

fam

ilism

and

resp

eto.

Mar

sigl

ia,

Pars

ai,

and

Kul

is(2

009)

Mex

ican

desc

ent;

56%

born

inth

eU

.S.;

60%

fem

ale;

Ari

zona

and

Nor

thC

arol

ina

151

Mag

e�

15.5

3(S

D�

1.25

)C

hild

-rep

ort

The

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(G

il,W

agne

r,&

Veg

a,20

00;

attit

udin

al)

Fam

ilism

ispr

edic

ted

less

aggr

essi

vebe

havi

or,

cond

uct

prob

lem

s,an

dru

lebr

eaki

ng.

Fam

ilism

and

cohe

sion

did

not

inte

ract

topr

edic

tfu

nctio

ning

.

Nol

le,

Gul

bas,

Kuh

lber

g,an

dZ

ayas

(201

2)Su

b-sa

mpl

eof

Kuh

lber

get

al.

(201

0);

88%

bon

inth

eU

.S.

orPu

erto

Ric

o;50

%at

tem

pted

suic

ide;

Nor

thea

st

24tr

iads

(you

th,

mot

her,

fath

er)

Mag

e�

15C

hild

-rep

ort;

Pare

nt-r

epor

tQ

ualit

ativ

ein

terv

iew

s.(a

ttitu

dina

lan

dbe

havi

oral

)Fa

mili

smem

erge

das

ath

eme

for

both

atte

mpt

ers

and

non-

atte

mpt

ers.

For

atte

mpt

ers

who

expr

esse

da

desi

reto

kill

them

selv

esin

thei

rat

tem

ptre

port

edw

antin

gto

mak

eth

ings

bette

rfo

rth

eir

fam

ilies

.

236 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 14: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Peña

etal

.(2

011)

35.7

%Pu

erto

Ric

an,

29.6

%D

omin

ican

,10

.2%

Mex

ican

,10

.2%

Col

ombi

an,

14.4

%ot

her

His

pani

c;50

%su

icid

eat

tem

pter

s;10

0%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

New

Yor

kC

ity

216

Mag

e�

15.5

(SD

�2.

0)C

hild

-rep

ort

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(L

ugo

Stei

del

&C

ontr

eras

,20

03;

attit

udin

al)

Fam

ilism

posi

tivel

yas

soci

ated

with

adol

esce

nts

bein

gpa

rtof

tight

-kni

tfa

mili

es,

and

adol

esce

nts

inth

ese

fam

ilies

wer

esi

gnifi

cant

lyle

sslik

ely

toat

tem

ptsu

icid

eco

mpa

red

with

less

tight

lykn

itfa

mili

es.

Polo

and

Lop

ez(2

009)

Mex

ican

orig

in(5

2%of

yout

hU

.S.

born

);50

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Los

Ang

eles

area

159

dyad

sM

age

�13

.2C

hild

-rep

ort;

Pare

nt-r

epor

tT

heA

ffilia

tive

Obe

dien

ceve

rsus

Act

ive

Self

-Affi

rmat

ion

mea

sure

(Día

z-G

uerr

ero,

1994

;at

titud

inal

)

Gre

ater

child

-rep

orte

daf

filia

tive

obed

ienc

epr

edic

ted

few

erde

pres

sive

sym

ptom

san

din

tern

aliz

ing

prob

lem

sco

ntro

lling

for

dem

ogra

phic

char

acte

rist

ics.

Smok

owsk

ian

dB

acal

lao

(200

7)13

%M

exic

o,21

%C

entr

alA

mer

ica,

21%

Sout

hA

mer

ica;

97%

born

outs

ide

the

U.S

.;51

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Nor

thC

arol

ina

323

Mag

e�

15(S

D�

1.8)

Chi

ld-r

epor

tFa

mili

smM

easu

re(G

il,W

agne

r,&

Veg

a;20

00;

base

don

Ols

onet

al,

1983

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilism

asso

ciat

edw

ithfe

wer

inte

rnal

izin

gpr

oble

ms

and

high

erse

lf-e

stee

m.

The

prot

ectiv

eef

fect

offa

mili

smon

inte

rnal

izin

gpr

oble

ms

was

med

iate

dby

pare

nt-

adol

esce

ntco

nflic

t.

Smok

owsk

i,R

ose,

and

Bac

alla

o(2

010)

Lat

ino

(66%

ofad

oles

cent

sfo

reig

nbo

rn);

asu

bsam

ple

ofSm

okow

ski

etal

.(2

010)

349

dyad

sM

edia

ngr

ade:

10th

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;Pa

rent

-rep

ort

(90%

mot

hers

)

Fam

ilism

mea

sure

(Gil,

Wag

ner,

&V

ega,

2000

;ba

sed

onO

lson

etal

,19

83;

attit

udin

al)

Atti

tudi

nal

fam

ilism

asso

ciat

edw

ithfe

wer

inte

rnal

izin

gsy

mpt

oms

and

high

erse

lf-

este

emac

ross

time

and

effe

cts

med

iate

dby

pare

nt–

child

confl

ict.

Stei

n,G

onza

lez,

Cup

ito,

Kia

ng,

and

Supp

le(2

013)

Lat

ino

yout

h:78

%M

exic

an-o

rigi

n,2%

Nic

arag

uan,

2%D

omin

ican

,2%

Salv

ador

ian,

and

8%L

atin

om

ixed

;53

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Nor

thC

arol

ina

173

Mag

e�

14.0

8C

hild

-rep

ort

18-i

tem

Atti

tudi

nal

Fam

ilism

Scal

e(L

ugo

Stei

del

&C

ontr

eras

,20

03)

Fam

ilism

asso

ciat

edw

ithpo

sitiv

eps

ycho

soci

alan

ded

ucat

iona

lou

tcom

es,

but

itdi

dno

tm

oder

ate

the

nega

tive

effe

cts

ofpe

rcei

ved

peer

disc

rim

inat

ion

onth

ese

outc

omes

. (tab

leco

ntin

ues)

237FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 15: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Stei

nan

dPo

lo(2

013)

Mex

ican

orig

in(5

2%of

yout

hU

.S.

born

);50

%fe

mal

eyo

uth;

Los

Ang

eles

area

159

dyad

sM

age

�13

.1(S

D�

.73)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t;M

othe

r-re

port

The

Affi

liativ

eO

bedi

ence

vers

usA

ctiv

eSe

lf-A

ffirm

atio

nm

easu

re(D

íaz-

Gue

rrer

o,19

94;

attit

udin

al)

Cul

tura

lva

lue

gaps

onob

edie

nce

rela

ted

toad

oles

cent

depr

essi

vesy

mpt

oms,

and

this

rela

tions

hip

was

mos

tpr

onou

nced

for

olde

rad

oles

cent

s.

Tel

zer,

Fulig

ni,

Lie

berm

an,

and

Gal

van

(201

3)

Mex

ican

back

grou

nds;

56%

fem

ale

yout

h(n

oot

her

info

rmat

ion

prov

ided

);so

uthe

rnC

alif

orni

a

4814

to16

.5ye

ars

(Mag

e�

15.2

3)C

hild

-rep

ort

12-i

tem

Fam

ilyO

blig

atio

nsSc

ale

(cur

rent

assi

stan

ce;

Fulig

niet

al.,

1999

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilyob

ligat

ion

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithde

crea

ses

inne

urol

ogic

ally

evid

ence

dre

war

dse

nsiti

vity

and

enha

ncem

ents

inco

gniti

veco

ntro

l,th

ereb

yre

duci

ngri

sk-t

akin

gbe

havi

ors.

Um

aña-

Tay

lor,

Alf

aro,

Bam

aca,

and

Gui

mon

d(2

009)

Lat

ino

(77%

Mex

ican

orig

in,

15%

Lat

ino/

His

pani

c;6%

Puer

toR

ican

);49

.9%

fem

ale

yout

h;M

idw

est

323

Mag

e�

15.2

1(S

D�

0.73

)C

hild

-rep

ort

The

Cul

tura

lV

alue

sSc

ale

(Ung

eret

al.,

2002

;at

titud

inal

)

Gen

erat

iona

lst

atus

was

not

dire

ctly

asso

ciat

edw

ithad

oles

cent

s’re

port

sof

fam

ilist

icva

lues

,bu

tits

effe

ctw

asfu

llym

edia

ted

byfa

mili

es’

ethn

icso

cial

izat

ion

prac

tices

.A

rgue

dth

atfa

mili

smm

easu

red

supp

ort

not

oblig

atio

ns.

Upd

egra

ff,

McH

ale,

Whi

tem

an,

Tha

yer,

and

Del

gado

(200

5)

Mex

ican

Am

eric

an(7

0%of

pare

nts

born

outs

ide

the

U.S

.);

51%

fem

ale

yout

h;A

rizo

na

234

sibl

ing

dyad

sO

lder

sibl

ings

Mag

e�

15.7

(SD

�1.

6);

youn

ger

sibl

ings

Mag

e�

12.8

(SD

�0.

58)

Chi

ld-r

epor

t17

-ite

mfa

mili

smsc

ale

(MA

CV

S;K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)an

dtim

esp

ent

with

adul

t-ki

n,si

blin

gs(b

ehav

iora

l)

Atti

tudi

nal

fam

ilism

asso

ciat

edw

ithbe

tter

sibl

ing

rela

tions

hip

qual

ity(g

reat

erin

timac

yan

dle

ssne

gativ

ity)

but

not

with

time

spen

tto

geth

er.

Upd

egra

ff,

Um

aña-

Tay

lor,

Pere

z-B

reña

,an

dPfl

iege

r(2

012)

Dat

aus

edis

from

Upd

egra

ffet

al.

(200

5);

Mex

ican

orig

infa

mili

es;

62%

ofad

oles

cent

sU

.S.-

born

;51

%fe

mal

eyo

uth

Sout

hwes

t

240

fam

ilies

Mag

e�

12.8

(SD

�05

8)C

hild

-rep

ort;

Pare

nt-r

epor

tFa

mili

smva

lues

and

trad

ition

alpa

tria

rcha

lge

nder

role

attit

udes

mea

sure

dus

ing

subs

cale

sof

the

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)

Fam

ilies

endo

rsin

gtr

aditi

onal

gend

ered

pare

ntin

gro

leat

titud

esdi

spla

yed

the

high

est

leve

lsof

fam

ilism

.M

ore

accu

ltura

ted

fam

ilies

disp

layi

ngco

ngru

ent

pare

ntin

gro

leat

titud

esre

port

edsi

gnifi

cant

lylo

wer

leve

lsof

fam

ilism

.

238 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 16: Familism through a developmental lens

Tab

le2

(con

tinu

ed)

Cita

tion

Dem

ogra

phic

sSa

mpl

esi

zeC

hild

age

Rep

orte

rM

easu

reus

ed(a

ttitu

dina

lvs

.be

havi

oral

)M

ain

findi

ngs

Upd

egra

ff,

Um

aña-

Tay

lor,

McH

ale,

Whe

eler

,an

dPe

rez-

Bre

na(2

012)

Dat

aus

edis

from

Upd

egra

ffet

al.

(200

5);

Mex

ican

orig

infa

mili

es;

62%

ofad

oles

cent

sU

.S.-

born

;51

%fe

mal

eyo

uth

Sout

hwes

t

Phas

e1

�24

6fa

mili

esPh

ase

2�

184

fam

ilies

Phas

e1

(Mag

e�

12.8

)Ph

ase

2(M

age

�17

.75

Chi

ld-r

epor

tFa

mili

smva

lues

and

trad

ition

alpa

tria

rcha

lge

nder

role

attit

udes

mea

sure

dus

ing

subs

cale

sof

the

Mex

ican

Am

eric

anC

ultu

ral

Val

ues

Scal

e(K

nigh

tet

al.,

2010

;at

titud

inal

)

Fem

ales

show

edst

eepe

rde

clin

esin

trad

ition

alge

nder

role

attit

udes

than

did

mal

es.

Ove

rall,

all

adol

esce

nts

decl

ined

infa

mili

smva

lues

,tim

esp

ent

with

fam

ily,

and

invo

lvem

ent

inM

exic

ancu

lture

.Fo

und

bidi

rect

iona

lre

latio

nshi

psbe

twee

ncu

ltura

lor

ient

atio

nsan

dad

just

men

tso

me

ofw

hich

wer

em

oder

ated

byad

oles

cent

nativ

ityan

dge

nder

.

Val

enzu

ela

and

Dor

nbus

ch(1

994)

84%

Ang

loan

d16

%M

exic

anor

igin

;pr

imar

ilyad

oles

cent

sw

ithU

Sbo

rnpa

rent

s52

%M

exic

an-o

rigi

nfe

mal

eyo

uth;

San

Fran

cisc

oar

ea

3,15

8H

igh

scho

olst

uden

ts;

spec

ific

age

ofth

esa

mpl

eno

tre

port

ed

Chi

ld-r

epor

t1-

item

beha

vior

alfa

mili

sm(t

alk

tono

npar

enta

lki

n);

1-ite

mst

ruct

ural

fam

ilism

(rel

ativ

esin

prox

imity

);14

-ite

mat

titud

inal

fam

ilism

scal

e(4

item

sfr

omK

eefe

,19

84;

attit

udin

alan

dbe

havi

oral

)

Whe

nhi

ghle

vels

ofat

titud

inal

fam

ilism

wer

eco

uple

dw

ithhi

ghle

vels

ofpa

rent

aled

ucat

ion,

the

inte

ract

ion

was

asso

ciat

edw

ithse

lf-

repo

rted

high

ergr

ades

.T

his

resu

ltw

ason

lyfo

und

for

the

Mex

ican

-ori

gin

part

icip

ants

.

239FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 17: Familism through a developmental lens

ment. For example, in her study of respeto,Valdés (1996) found that parents used verbaland nonverbal strategies to teach children aboutappropriate interactions with adults, such asgreeting elders politely, not challenging elders’points of view, and not interrupting adults. Val-dés (1996) explained that these behaviors rep-resented the value of respeto that specificallyteaches children about how they should defer toadults and their role in the family as a daughter,son, sister, and so forth. Similarly, Latino moth-ers voiced the importance of teaching their chil-dren about Latino cultural values, including thecentrality of family, religious beliefs, and res-peto (Calzada et al., 2010). Finally, research onsocial behavior for Mexican American childrenat home and school points to the role of respetoin fostering “bien educado” in Latino children,which is defined as appropriate social compe-tencies such as comportment and obediencewithin the family and other settings. Indeed,researchers studying the transmission of cul-tural values during early childhood point torespeto as creating cooperation and cohesionamong members of the family, which is closelylinked to the development of attitudinalfamilism (Bridges et al., 2012).

While instilling cultural values are a centralparenting goal for Latino parents, familism mayalso serve to foster positive parent–child inter-action and promote adaptive social behavior.Because these values emphasize familial inter-connectedness, support, and cohesion, parentsmay demonstrate high levels of warmth, fosterpositive attachment, and spend time with theirchildren (see Table 1). One study directly ex-amined this question in early childhood andestablished that mothers who report high levelsof familism report greater warmth and closeness(Gamble & Modry-Mandell, 2008). Moreover,familism moderated the relation between mater-nal-child closeness and children’s emotional ad-justment as rated by teachers, such that chil-dren’s adjustment scores were significantlylower when mothers reported low levels offamilism, despite higher levels of mother—child closeness. Maternal familism also pre-dicted emotional and peer adjustment in thecontext of high levels of sibling warmth. Thus,maternal familism related to behavioral adjust-ment of children at school, thereby illustratingthe saliency of examining two important social-ization contexts of home and school.

Empirical literature examining familism inearly childhood across key contexts is limited(only two studies located). For example, studieshave examined how familism may relate to pa-rental decision-making about childcare usageand relative care. National studies have sug-gested that Latino parents utilize out of homechildcare, including center based care, at alower rate than non-Hispanic White and Blackfamilies (Karoly & Gonzalez, 2011), but otherstudies have found comparable rates of centerbased care among Latinos (Mulligan, Brimhall, &West, 2005; Yesil-Dagli, 2011). Limited dataare available to inform whether Valdés’ (1996)observation that mothers preferred to leave chil-dren in the care of relatives because of familismvalues remains accurate today, as other factorsare likely involved in childcare access, such asfinancial and language barriers and awarenessof resources (Karoly & Gonzalez, 2011; Mulli-gan et al., 2005). In the domain of neighborhoodand community factors and familism, a qualita-tive study by Calzada and colleagues (2013)observed that frequent and regular interpersonalcontact is normative for Latino families. Partic-ipants in her study spent extended periods ofshared living arrangements with extended kin.However, they also found examples of the be-havioral expression of familism that may eitherinfluence families positively (e.g., child rearingsupport, financial support) or negatively (e.g.,overcrowding, financial strain of other rela-tives). Taken together, these studies suggest thatparental familism attitudes may impact day-careselection, living arrangements, and contact withextended kin, which in turn likely predicts psy-chosocial outcomes. Greater exploration ofthese relations over time (during early child-hood) is needed.

Critical synthesis and future directions.Research conducted at this stage in develop-ment suggests that Latino families may be ex-pressing the importance of familism, primarilyvia the messages involving respect for adultswithin the family; however, we contend that theroots of familism as expressed within familieswith young children is less well understood.Studies have not precisely examined how pa-rental attitudinal familism influences parentalsocialization at this stage, and the only studythat examined familism impact on parentingbehaviors relied solely on self-report. More re-search is necessary to understand how familism

240 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 18: Familism through a developmental lens

values influences parental behaviors using ob-servational methodology to rule out single-method bias. The research on the selection ofchild-care is flawed in that the large-scale stud-ies assume that familism may influence selec-tion but no large-scale studies have specificallymeasured this question. More research needs toexamine how parental attitudinal familism pre-dicts behavioral manifestations (e.g., selectionof childcare, parenting practices) especiallywithin different contexts (e.g., urban vs. rural,economic stress). For example, familism influ-ences housing arrangements in urban environ-ments (Calzada et al., 2013), but understandinghow a range of community characteristics couldplay a role in the expression of behavioralfamilism among families with young children isnecessary especially as it relates to housing,childcare selection, and the transition to school.

Middle Childhood (Ages 7–11)

Literature review. During the period ofmiddle childhood, children may start internaliz-ing the values and beliefs that underpin theconstruct of familism and their behavior may beinfluenced by their own beliefs in addition toparental directives. However, only five studieswere located examining familism solely at thisstage. Four studies focused on parental attitudi-nal familism predicting parent behaviors, suchthat parental attitudinal familism was associatedwith less interparental conflict (Taylor, Larsen-Rife, Conger, & Widaman, 2012), more respon-sive, warm, and structured parenting (Morcilloet al., 2011; White et al., 2013), and parentingpractices aimed at promoting prosocial behav-iors (Calderón-Tena, Knight, & Carlo, 2011),confirming that these values shape the familialcontext at this stage in development. Two ofthese studies linked these values and practicesto outcomes. In a longitudinal examinationacross childhood, parental attitudinal familismwas associated with lower levels of parent re-ported antisocial behavior over the two yearlyfollow-ups, controlling for other environmentaland child risk factors, parental warmth andstructure were found to mediate these relation-ships (Morcillo et al., 2011). However, only oneof these studies examined the impact of theseparenting practices on child behaviors via childfamilism values such that maternal attitudinalfamilism was directly and indirectly associated

with child prosocial behavior partially throughchild familism values (Calderón-Tena et al.,2011). One other article at this stage examinedfamily contextual predictors finding that higherlevels of parental education were associatedwith higher maternal familism, and also surpris-ingly, preference for English or bilingualismwas also associated with higher child familism(Romero, Robinson, Haydel, Mendoza, & Kil-len, 2004).

Critical synthesis and future directions.Although studies increased in frequency, mid-dle childhood is an underdeveloped stage forfamilism research relative to adolescence. Ingeneral, the majority of studies focused on pa-rental familism values, despite the fact thatyouth at this stage of development (particularlythe latter period of middle childhood) have be-gun to internalize these values. Although it isclear that parental attitudinal familism impactsparental behavior, it is less clear how parentalfamilism impacts the internalization of childfamilism. The only study linking parental andchild attitudinal familism at this stage (Calde-rón-Tena et al., 2011) was limited in that theprosocial parenting scale appeared to includeitems directly associated with familism expec-tations (e.g., “My mother expects me to takecare of younger siblings”) and was cross-sectional. In a rare longitudinal investigation,consistent with a developmental science per-spective, Morcillo et al. (2011) was innovativein its design, but unfortunately, it did not mea-sure child attitudinal familism to link whetherthe internalization of these values also contrib-utes to its positive effects.

Moreover, the manifestation of attitudinalversus behavioral familism at this stage may beparticularly important to clarify. In addition topredicting parental behaviors, attitudinal paren-tal familism may also predict child manifesta-tions of behavioral familism (e.g., compliantbehavior), which has not been examined at thisstage in development. Additionally, studieshave intermixed both behavioral and attitudinalcomponents in its measurement making it dif-ficult to disentangle whether it was the internal-ization of these values or the behavioral enact-ments that lead to positive outcomes (e.g.,Romero et al., 2004). Therefore, a developmen-tally appropriate measure of attitudinal and be-havioral familism needs to be developed thatcan guide these questions at this stage, and to

241FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 19: Familism through a developmental lens

critically examine the interplay of these twoaspects of familism throughout development(e.g., their alignment vs. misalignment). Addi-tionally, contextual factors need to be consid-ered more fully. For example, the behavioralmanifestation of familism may also pose a riskin school contexts if the child shows overlydeferential behavior toward adults. Thus, re-search at this stage should be mindful of howattitudinal and behavioral child familism influ-ences the development of relationships outsideof the home context (e.g., peer, teachers).

Adolescence (12–18)

Literature review. We located 32 studiesexamining familism values in adolescence thatwill be discussed below.

Family functioning. Contrary to work ear-lier in development, research conducted in ad-olescence examines how familism manifests it-self from both a parent and adolescentperspective. Across studies, Latino mothersdemonstrate parenting strategies that are consis-tent with familism: closely monitoring theirchildren, controlling their activities, having ex-pectations of obedience, and maintaining warmand supportive relationships that foster inter-connectedness (Guilamo-Ramos et al., 2007;Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, Perez-Brena, &Pflieger, 2012). Adolescents often interpretthese parental behaviors as the manifestation offamilism; for example, they report feeling thatparents should closely monitor them and spendtime with them, viewing this behavior as beingdriven by parental love and concern (Guilamo-Ramos et al., 2007). Latino adolescents alsodemonstrate many behaviors consistent withfamilism as evident with studies documentingtime spent interpreting for parents (Sy, 2006),completing household chores (Raffaelli & On-tai, 2004), caring for siblings (Hafford, 2010),and time spent with siblings and other familymembers (Updegraff, McHale, Whiteman,Thayer, & Delgado, 2005).

Not surprisingly, familism has been associ-ated with a positive parent–child relationship(Delgado, Updegraff, Roosa, & Umaña-Taylor,2011; Lorenzo-Blanco et al., 2012; Taylor,Larsen-Rife, Conger, & Widaman, 2012). Ado-lescents who value familism reported greaterfeelings of connectedness and cohesion with thefamily and better parent–child communication

(Fuligni et al., 1999; Lorenzo-Blanco et al.,2012), and families high in familism were char-acterized as having high cohesion (Peña et al.,2011). Adolescent familism also predicted lowlevels of parent-adolescent conflict (e.g.,Smokowski, Rose, & Bacallao, 2010), and con-flict served to mediate the positive effects ofattitudinal familism. However, consistent withtenets of developmental science regarding theindividual’s transaction with the surroundingcontext, research has found that family conflictin the presence of high levels of attitudinalfamilism may be more detrimental as it violatesthe expectations of family harmony (Hernán-dez, Ramírez Garcia, & Flynn, 2010; Kuhlberg,Peña, & Zayas, 2010).

During midlate adolescence, the increaseddesire for autonomy and individuation from thefamily may impact how familism is expressedwithin the family context, particularly how ad-olescents perceive their parents’ behavior. Atti-tudinal adolescent familism has been associatedwith the perception of parents serving as legit-imate sources of guidance and authority (Bush,Supple, & Lash, 2004), such as for makingdecisions about dating (Guilamo-Ramos et al.,2007). This perception of parents’ legitimateauthority leads to improved family functioningas well as positive adolescent outcomes, includ-ing less distress and more prosocial behaviors(e.g., Kuperminc et al., 2009). However, whenLatino adolescents do not align with their par-ents on autonomy expectations, there is in-creased risk for parent– child conflict andgreater psychopathology (e.g., Bámaca-Colbert,Umaña-Taylor, & Gayles, 2012). One longitu-dinal study has examined the natural trajectoryof familism values across adolescence, and con-sistent with the notion that familism maychange in adolescence because of autonomy,attitudinal familism across 7th and 12th gradedecreases (Updegraff, Umaña-Taylor, McHale,Wheeler, & Perez-Brena, 2012), whereas an-other study documented increases in filial obli-gations in the transition out of 12th grade (Fu-ligni & Pedersen, 2002). Likely the distinctaspects of familism (i.e., respect vs. obligations)may demonstrate differential growth across ad-olescence, but more research is needed to clar-ify these trajectories.

Psychosocial and academic outcomes.The majority of research suggests that attitudi-nal adolescent familism serves a compensatory

242 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 20: Familism through a developmental lens

function and predicts better psychosocial func-tioning (i.e., fewer depressive symptoms, lesssubstance use, and less behavioral problems)(e.g., Ayón, Marsiglia, & Bermudez-Parsai,2010; Germán, Gonzales, & Dumka, 2009; Gil,Wagner, & Vega, 2000; Marsiglia, Parsai, &Kulis, 2009; Polo & Lopez, 2009). Addition-ally, behavioral familism, as conceptualized asfulfilling familial obligations, has also beenshown to predict the development of compe-tence and maturity in Latino adolescents (Ku-perminc, Jurkovic, & Casey, 2009). However,although attitudinal and behavioral familismcan often be protective, it can also result indetrimental outcomes in stressful contexts act-ing as a potentiating factor. East and Weisner(2009) found that extensive family responsibil-ities predicted adolescent stress, internalizingsymptoms, and worse school outcomes, andfamilism did not buffer against the detrimentaleffects of extensive caregiving in the context ofsibling teenage parenting. Similarly, in theirstudy of suicidal adolescents (Nolle, Gulbas,Kuhlberg, & Zayas, 2012), participants sacri-ficed their material needs or subjugated theiremotions to avoid unduly burdening their fam-ilies, and when they failed to fulfill their obli-gations, they felt that sacrificing themselvesthrough suicide would serve as an appropriatesolution.

Fewer studies have examined paternal andmaternal attitudinal familism predicting adoles-cent outcomes and produced mixed findings.Paternal attitudinal familism was negatively as-sociated with adolescent deviant peers associa-tion whereas adolescent attitudinal familismwas associated with fewer depressive symp-toms, risk engagement, and peer association,but maternal reports were only correlated withfewer depressive symptoms in older adolescents(Delgado et al., 2011). In another study, mater-nal, paternal, and adolescent attitudinalfamilism protected adolescents from deviantpeer association in the prediction of externaliz-ing symptoms, but only maternal familismshowed direct effects (Germán, Gonzales, &Dumka, 2009). Finally, other studies have con-sidered discrepancies in parent and child reportsof attitudinal familism. Parent-child alignmenton attitudinal familism was protective againstboth internalizing and externalizing symptoms(Bamaca-Colbert & Gayles, 2010; Baumann,

Kuhlberg, & Zayas, 2010; Kuhlberg, Peña, &Zayas, 2010; Stein & Polo, 2014).

Research examining familism values and ac-ademic outcomes has generally focused on at-titudes about family obligations. Attitudinal fa-milial obligations and respect contribute toLatino adolescents’ academic motivation be-cause of the fact that students desire to helptheir families in the future (Fuligni et al., 1999;Sánchez, Esparza, Colón, & Davis, 2010).However, other research documents potentialrisk as family obligation attitudes influencedstudents to forego attending college to supporttheir families (Sánchez et al., 2010). These con-tradictory findings suggest that contextualforces are likely leading to differential out-comes. In fact, parental education was a signif-icant moderator, but again findings were con-flicting with one study finding less risk at lowlevels of education and the other finding lessrisk at high levels of education (Esparza &Sánchez, 2008; Valenzuela & Dornbusch,1994). Furthermore, a curvilinear effect existsbetween attitudinal family obligations andgrades, such that students reporting the greatestobligations had school grades just as low oreven lower than those reporting the weakestfamily obligations (Fuligni et al., 1999). We canconclude from these studies that attitudinal andbehavioral familism may differentially impactacademic outcomes, and further, these relation-ships are likely impacted by contextual factorssuch as SES and generational status, but pres-ently mechanisms are less clear.

Attitudinal familism has also been associatedwith a greater sense of school belonging (Stein,Gonzalez, Cupito, Kiang, & Supple, 2013), anda strong sense of school belonging has beenpredictive of higher grade point average (GPA)among Latino students (Sánchez, Colón, & Es-parza, 2005). Attitudinal familism may help ad-olescents develop psychosocial competenciesallowing them to successfully create feelings ofconnectedness and solidarity in the school set-ting (Knight & Carlo, 2012). Consistent withthis idea, attitudinal and behavioral familismhave also been found to promote prosocial be-havior tendencies (i.e., actions that are intendedto benefit others) (Calderón-Tena et al., 2011)and social competence (Kuperminc et al.,2009). Therefore, familism may lead to adoles-cents being more cognizant of others before

243FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 21: Familism through a developmental lens

they act leading to positive outcomes in con-texts outside of the home.

New research has examined the neural mech-anisms underlying the relationship between at-titudinal filial obligations, cognitive control,and risk-taking behavior (Telzer, Fuligni,Lieberman, & Galván, 2013); adolescents re-porting high filial obligations were found toshow a neural pattern consistent with greaterrisk aversion, lower sensitivity to rewards, andmore mature cognitive control. Of interest to theauthors, family cohesion and support did notshow similar neural responses, indicating thatonly specific types of family relationships areassociated with these protective effects. Thiswork exemplifies a developmental science per-spective as it integrates across systems examin-ing the neurological mechanisms that may ex-plain in part the protective function of familism,and suggests that the internalization of thesevalues changes how adolescents may respond totheir environments and the neurological path-ways that may be implicated in their behavior.

Work on contextual influences has also ex-tended our current understanding of the role offamilism by examining the role of neighbor-hood level familism (Gonzales et al., 2010),calculated by averaging mothers’ and fathers’familism in a census block. Neighborhoodfamilism conferred the most robust protectiveeffects of all contextual predictors (e.g., familyincome, subjective economic hardship, andneighborhood disadvantage). Thus, having acommunity with shared values about the impor-tance of family may allow for collective super-vision of youth, more resources for youth topursue goals, positive opportunities, more safeplaces, and may validate and support parents’commitment to family (Gonzales et al., 2010).

Work has also examined how attitudinalfamilism operates in the context of experiencesof discrimination. Although adolescent attitudi-nal familism was protective against the negativeeffects of discrimination on risk-taking behav-iors at low levels of discrimination, it was notprotective at high levels of discrimination(Umaña-Taylor, Updegraff, & Gonzales-Backen, 2011). Similarly, attitudinal familismfailed to buffer against peer discrimination inpredicting depressive symptoms and psycholog-ical distress (Ayón et al., 2010; Delgado et al.,2011; Stein et al., 2013). Moreover, daughtersof mothers who report high levels of attitudinal

familism perceived greater discrimination (Del-gado et al., 2011), and yet, discrimination re-sults in an increase in Latino cultural values(Berkel et al., 2010). As suggested by Berkel etal. (2010), familism may not operate as a bufferbut instead a risk reducer in the context of stressand in conjunction with ethnic identity, butmore work is needed to elucidate these pro-cesses.

Critical Synthesis and Future Directions

Socialization. It is clear that familism im-pacts family functioning in adolescence, butstudies at this period suggest that parents andyouth do not universally align on attitudinalfamilism. Most studies have documented non-significant correlations between parent and ad-olescent reports of attitudinal familism (e.g.,Delgado et al., 2011; Germán et al., 2009;Knight et al., 2011). This suggests two possibleinterpretations. First, consistent with accultura-tion gap models, it is likely that there are fam-ilies that align on these values and those who donot align equally on these values. Second, andperhaps more importantly for the field, little isknown about how families come to align onthese values and, more specifically, how ado-lescents come to internalize these values(Knight et al., 2011). Recent research suggeststhat parental, especially maternal, ethnic social-ization during early adolescence leads to in-creases in adolescent attitudinal familistic val-ues (Knight et al., 2011; Umaña-Taylor, Alfaro,Bámaca, & Guimond, 2009). Thus, direct ethnicsocialization is likely to be one of the manypathways fostering the internalization of attitu-dinal familism, but socialization measures usedin the literature have not been specific tofamilism. This makes it unclear whether parentsexplicitly socialize around these values, or assuggested by Valdés (1996), this is done moreindirectly. Additionally, research should disen-tangle whether the messages parents provide aremore directly related to the behavioral manifes-tation of familism (i.e., completing chores) oralso include messages about the values them-selves (e.g., we should always support our fam-ily). Therefore, more work is needed to under-stand the ethnic socialization of familism inLatino families and how these values are in-stilled both in the family context and extrafa-

244 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 22: Familism through a developmental lens

milial contexts (e.g., school, neighborhoods) assuggested in Table 1.

Attitudinal versus behavioral familism.It is evident that attitudinal familism is associ-ated with family functioning, but how theseconstructs relate to one another in the predictionof psychological functioning is less clear inadolescence. Clarification is needed as towhether positive family functioning (e.g.,warmth, cohesion) constitutes a behavioralmanifestation of attitudinal familism, orwhether they are distinct constructs. Medita-tional models finding that attitudinal familisminfluences family functioning leading to posi-tive psychological outcomes support this no-tion. However, moderational models wouldsuggest that these are indeed separate constructsand that attitudinal familism functions as thecultural framework that influences how individ-uals interpret each other’s behavior. Althoughboth models can be true in that these values mayguide behavior but then also serve as cognitiveframes to understand that behavior, researchersshould be mindful as to what construct theirmeasure captures and which model is guidingtheir research questions as suggested by Table1. Again, longitudinal studies will be particu-larly useful in disentangling the familial andindividual mediating mechanisms.

Similarly, the literature continues to beplagued by a lack of clear theoretical and mea-surement clarity concerning adolescent attitudi-nal versus behavioral familism as predictingoutcomes. More work needs to examine thedifferential impact of both aspects of familism,with a specific focus on the intersection of thetwo as the review finds that both attitudinal andbehavioral familism can pose a threat to psy-chological and academic functioning (East &Weisner, 2009; Nolle et al., 2012; Sánchez etal., 2010). Because these studies all utilizeddifferent methodology, it is difficult to drawfirm conclusions, but it is likely that attitudinaland behavioral factors serve as both risk andprotective factors and this relationship dependsboth on the type of familism in question as wellas contextual factors (e.g., Calzada et al., 2013).

Attention to context. The contextual influ-ences that may impact the effects of familismneed to be elucidated more clearly as suggestedin Table 1. Studies demonstrating a detrimentaleffect of attitudinal familism have been con-ducted in at risk populations (high levels of

psychopathology, Bauman, Kuhlberg, & Zayas,2010; teenage pregnancy, East & Weisner,2009; low SES, Sánchez et al., 2010). Similarly,the role of familism, parental education, andbroader SES context in predicting academicoutcomes is not well understood; disparatefindings could be linked to different family orcultural contexts (immigrant vs. United Statesborn parents, Esparza & Sánchez, 2008; Va-lenzuela & Dornbusch, 1994). These findingsalign with a developmental science perspec-tive suggesting that contextual factors need tobe carefully considered to understand howfamilism operates in adolescence.

The relation between acculturation andfamilism is complex and studies in adolescencehave found no relation between generation sta-tus and familism (e.g., Delgado et al., 2011;Esparza & Sanchez, 2008; Umaña-Taylor et al.,2009). Similarly, some studies have found norelation between acculturation variables and en-dorsement of familism values (e.g., Updegraffet al., 2005), but other studies find that bothacculturation to the United States and culture oforigin are both related to the endorsement offamilism (e.g., Lorenzo-Blanco et al., 2012).These differences may be because of measure-ment and sample characteristics as some ofthese have included mostly youth living in im-migrant families, and generation or accultura-tion differences may be found in more diversesamples. However, the majority of research onthe relation of familism and acculturation failsto consider attitudinal versus behavioralfamilism, as there may be differences in theenactment of familism but not the values acrossgenerations or acculturation. Future researchshould continue to explore how attitudinal andbehavioral familism functions across contexts,with special attention paid to elucidating themechanisms that may underlie the protectiveand/or risk mechanisms.

Reporter. Differential findings across re-porter in adolescence are evident in our currentreview, and some of these differences may bebecause of age differences in the samples (e.g.,Delgado et al., 2011; Germán et al., 2009).There is some convergence of findings suggest-ing that maternal familism may be particularlysalient (e.g., Knight et al., 2011) because of theprimary role mothers play in structuring familyenvironments and maintaining family values inthe home. Given these findings, there is a need

245FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 23: Familism through a developmental lens

for closer consideration of how and why report-ers of familism are selected, and how theseperspectives can be best considered simultane-ously.

Conclusions

Taken together, we can conclude fromthese findings that parental and child attitudi-nal familism is associated with positive fam-ily functioning, which we argue can be con-strued as a behavioral manifestation offamilism. Moreover, attitudinal familism hasalso been associated with multiple positiveoutcomes in Latino youth, primarily in ado-lescence (e.g., fewer internalizing and exter-nalizing symptoms, greater social compe-tence). However, the literature is plaguedwith some significant methodological flaws.Without a gold standard measure of attitudi-nal or behavioral familism, our conclusionsacross studies are hampered. It is unclearwhat aspects of familism are particularly pro-tective and whether the aspect of familismmatters, and this is particularly salient forclarifying the differential role of attitudinalversus behavioral familism. As noted in Table1, researchers should specify the particularaspect of familism being assessed in theirstudy to reduce this confusion in the litera-ture.

Across developmental stages, researchshould more consistently examine how childgender may not only influence the internal-ization and enactment of familism, but alsoinfluence its protective or potentiating effects.Past research suggests that girls may be moreheavily burdened by obligations in adoles-cence compared to boys (Stein et al., 2013;Rafaelli & Ontai, 2004), but few studies havesystematically examined how familism maydifferentially impact psychosocial function-ing across gender. Some studies above sug-gest that familism may confer more protec-tion for girls (e.g., Lorenzo-Blanco et al.,2012; Morcillo et al., 2011), but gender hasnot been a consistent moderator (e.g., Stein &Polo, 2014). Likely, the effects of familismacross gender depends on the aspect offamilism under study as well as other contex-tual factors (e.g., poverty, birth order, or im-migrant status) that need to be better eluci-dated.

Our review highlights the need for furtherinquiry in the developmental processes asso-ciated with familism, especially longitudinalstudies that can clarify how familism mani-fests itself across development and how thismanifestation depends on transitions acrosschildhood. There is a dearth of research onfamilism in early childhood and middle child-hood to fully describe how familism unfoldsacross development, and how it may differ-entially relate to outcomes. From a parentalperspective, further work should examine thecontinuity in parental attitudinal familismacross childhood and adolescence, and how itis influenced by child directed effects or con-text directed effects. Greater attention to howthe behavioral expression of familism duringan earlier developmental stage (e.g., earlychildhood) may influence both attitudinal andbehavioral familism during later developmentmay help to differentially predict outcomes inadolescence. In the same vein, research needsto explore whether there is a developmentalshift in adolescence such that striving forautonomy leads to lessening of familism val-ues as suggested by Updegraff and colleagues(2012). Likely, there is variability in thesetrajectories during this time of identity forma-tion where some adolescents solidify andstrengthen their familistic orientation whileother adolescents move away from it; we needto understand the familial and contextual pre-dictors of these trajectories. Similarly, riskand protective mechanisms may also differacross development as familism may be pro-tective for a specific psychosocial outcome atone point in development but not at another,and this may also hold true across contextswhere expectations may be incongruent (e.g.,home and school, or home and peers).

Few studies have used longitudinal meth-odology to examine the developmental courseof familism and this work is necessary toidentify causal mediators as well track devel-opmental trajectories associated withfamilism. As we learn more about howfamilism intersects with important stage-salient issues, we will be able to clarify someof the mechanisms that underlie familism’seffects on functioning across contexts. Insummary, future research should consider theattitudinal and behavioral aspects of familismfrom both the parent and child perspective

246 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 24: Familism through a developmental lens

across development to understand the func-tion of familism for Latino youth.

Abstracto

Este ensayo revisa una literatura emergente que ex-amina los efectos del familismo a través de la niñezhasta la adolescencia. El familismo ha sido definidocomo un valor cultural Latino que enfatiza obli-gación, lealtad hacia la familia, y el apoyo y obedi-encia familiar, y sus efectos durante estas etapas dedesarrollo han sido documentados de manera positivaen general por la literatura. Esta revisión de la litera-tura intenta organizar y criticar lo que se ha investi-gado hasta hoy en día utilizando el esquema de laciencia del desarrollo. Los principios claves de estaperspectiva subrayados por esta revisión son la con-sideración detallada de como se desarrolla elfamilismo en un individuo a través del tiempo, comose manifiesta el familismo en diferentes puntos du-rante el desarrollo, y como impacta el funciona-miento del niño, del adolecente, y de la familia.Cuarenta y cuatro ensayos fueron examinados y cat-egorizados, y los resultados demuestran que la influ-encia protectora del familismo es mayormente evi-dente durante el periodo de la adolescencia.Consideraciones sobre los diferentes modos de ex-presar el familismo y el impacto que tiene sobre losresultados del desarrollo anterior y posterior se ofre-cen como recomendaciones para derivar un entendi-miento mas completo del funcionamiento delFamilismo en las familias Latinas.

References

Ayón, C., Marsiglia, F. F., & Bermudez-Parsai, M.(2010). Latino family mental health: Exploring therole of discrimination and familismo. Journal ofCommunity Psychology, 38, 742–756. doi:10.1002/jcop.20392

Bámaca-Colbert, M. Y., & Gayles, J. G. (2010).Variable-centered and person-centered approachesto studying Mexican-origin mother–daughter cul-tural orientation dissonance. Journal of Youth andAdolescence, 39, 1274–1292. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9447-3

Bámaca-Colbert, M. Y., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., &Gayles, J. G. (2012). A developmental-contextualmodel of depressive symptoms in Mexican-originfemale adolescents. Developmental Psychology,48, 406–421. doi:10.1037/a0025666

Bardis, P. D. (1959). A familism scale. Marriage &Family Living, 21, 340–341.

Baumann, A. A., Kuhlberg, J., & Zayas, L. (2010).Familism, mother-daughter mutuality and sui-cide attempts of adolescent Latinas. Journal of

Family Psychology, 24, 616 – 624. doi:10.1037/a0020584

Berkel, C., Knight, G. P., Zeiders, K. H., Tein, J. Y.,Roosa, M. W., Gonzales, N. A., & Saenz, D.(2010). Discrimination and adjustment for Mexi-can American adolescents: A prospective exami-nation of the benefits of culturally related values.Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 893–915.doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00668.x

Bridges, M., Cohen, S. R., McGuire, L. W., Yamada,H., Fuller, B., Mireles, L., & Scott, L. (2012). BienEducado: Measuring the social behaviors of Mex-ican American children. Early Childhood Re-search Quarterly, 27, 555–567.

Burgess, E. W., & Locke, H. S. (1945). The family:From institution to companionship. New York,NY: American.

Bush, K. R., Supple, A. J., & Lash, S. B. (2004).Mexican adolescents’ perceptions of parental be-haviors and authority as predictors of their self-esteem and sense of familism. Marriage & FamilyReview, 36, 35–65. doi:10.1300/J002v36n01_03

Calderón-Tena, C. O., Knight, G. P., & Carlo, G.(2011). The socialization of prosocial behavioraltendencies among Mexican American adolescents:The role of familism values. Cultural Diversityand Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 98–106. doi:10.1037/a0021825

Calzada, E. J., Fernandez, Y., & Cortes, D. E. (2010).Incorporating the cultural value of respeto into aframework of Latino parenting. Cultural Diversity& Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 77–86. doi:10.1037/a0016071

Calzada, E. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Yoshikawa,H. (2013). Familismo in Mexican and DominicanFamilies from low-income, urban communities.Journal of Family Issues, 33, 1–29. doi:10.1177/0192513X12460218

Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Gonzalez, G. (1995). Cog-nitive referents of acculturation: Assessment ofcultural constructs in Mexican Americans. Journalof Community Psychology, 23, 339–356.

Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995).Acculturation rating scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA scale. His-panic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275–304.

Colon, R. (1998). Causal relationships of familialinfluence and adolescents’ attitude toward sub-stance use in the theory of planned behavior: Asocial impact model. Dissertation Abstracts Inter-national, 59, 2482. (UMI No. AAM9835729).

Delgado, M. Y., Updegraff, K. A., Roosa, M. W., &Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2011). Discrimination andMexican-origin adolescents’ adjustment: Themoderating roles of adolescents’, mothers’, andfathers’ cultural orientations and values. Journal of

247FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 25: Familism through a developmental lens

Youth and Adolescence, 40, 125–139. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9467-z

Diaz-Guerrero, R. (1994). La psicología del Mexi-cano: Descubrimiento de la etnopsicología (6thed.) [The psychology of the Mexican: Discovery ofethnopsychology]. Mexico City: Trillas.

East, P. L., & Weisner, T. S. (2009). Mexican Amer-ican adolescents’ family caregiving: Selection ef-fects and longitudinal associations with adjust-ment. Family Relations, 58, 562–577. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2009.00575.x

Esparza, P., & Sánchez, B. (2008). The role of atti-tudinal familism in academic outcomes: A study ofurban, Latino high school seniors. Cultural Diver-sity & Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 193–200.doi:10.1037/1099-9809.14.3.193

Feldman, S. S., & Quatman, T. (1988). Factors in-fluencing age expectations for adolescent auton-omy: A study of early adolescents and parents. TheJournal of Early Adolescence, 8, 325–343.

Fuligni, A., & Pedersen, S. (2002). Family obligationand the transition to young adulthood. Develop-mental Psychology, 38, 856–868. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.856

Fuligni, A., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudestoward family obligations among American Ado-lescents with Asian, Latin American, and Euro-pean Backgrounds. Child Development, 70, 1030–1044. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00075

Gamble, W. C., & Modry-Mandell, K. (2008). Fam-ily relations and the adjustment of young childrenof Mexican descent: Do family cultural valuesmoderate these associations? Social Development,17, 358 –379. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00429.x

Germán, M., Gonzales, N., & Dumka, L. (2009).Familism values as a protective factor for Mexi-can-origin adolescents exposed to deviant peers.The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29, 16–42.doi:10.1177/0272431608324475

Gil, A. G., Wagner, E. F., & Vega, W. A. (2000).Acculturation, familism, and alcohol use amongLatino adolescent males: Longitudinal relations.Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 443–458.doi:10.1002/1520-6629(200007)28:4�443::AID-JCOP6�3.0.CO;2-A

Gonzales, N. A., Coxe, S., Roosa, M. W., White, R. M.B., Knight, G. P., Zeiders, K. H., & Saenz, D. (2010).Economic hardship, neighborhood context, and par-enting: Prospective effects on Mexican-American ad-olescents mental health. American Journal of Com-munity Psychology, 47, 98 –113. doi:10.1007/s10464-010-9366-1

Guilamo-Ramos, V., Dittus, P., Jaccard, J., Johans-son, M., Bouris, A., & Acosta, N. (2007). Parent-ing practices among Dominican and Puerto Ricanmothers. Social Work, 52, 17–30. doi:10.1093/sw/52.1.17

Hafford, C. (2010). Sibling caretaking in immigrantfamilies: Understanding cultural practices to in-form child welfare practice and evaluation. Eval-uation and Program Planning, 33, 294–302. doi:10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2009.05.003

Hernández, B., Ramírez García, J. I., & Flynn, M.(2010). The role of familism in the relation be-tween parent–child discord and psychological dis-tress among emerging adults of Mexican descent.Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 105–114. doi:10.1037/a0019140

Jurkovic, G. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Sarac, T., & Weis-shaar, D. (2005). Role of filial responsibility in thepost-war adjustment of Bosnian young adoles-cents. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 5, 219–235.

Keefe, S. E. (1984). Real and ideal extendedfamilism among Mexican Americans and AngloAmericans: On the meaning of close family ties.Human Organization, 43, 65–69.

Karoly, L. A., & Gonzalez, G. C. (2011). Early careand education for children in immigrant families.The Future of Children, 21, 71–101. doi:10.1353/foc.2011.0005

Kiang, L., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Ethnic identity andfamily processes among adolescents from LatinAmerican, Asian, and European backgrounds.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 228–241.doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9353-0

Knight, G. P., Berkel, C., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Gonza-les, N. A., Ettekal, I., Jaconis, M., & Boyd, B. M.(2011). The familial socialization of culturally relatedvalues in Mexican American families. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 73, 913–925. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00856.x

Knight, G. P., & Carlo, G. (2012). Prosocial devel-opment among Mexican American youth. ChildDevelopment Perspectives, 6, 258 –263. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00233.x

Knight, G. P., Gonzales, N. A., Saenz, D. S., Bonds,D. D., Germán, M., Deardorff, J., . . . Updegraff,K. A. (2010). The Mexican American cultural val-ues scale for adolescents and adults. The Journal ofEarly Adolescence, 30, 444–481. doi:10.1177/0272431609338178

Kuhlberg, J. A., Peña, J. B., & Zayas, L. H. (2010).Familism, parent-adolescent conflict, self-esteem,internalizing behaviors and suicide attemptsamong adolescent Latinas. Child Psychiatry andHuman Development, 41, 425–440. doi:10.1007/s10578-010-0179-0

Kuperminc, G. P., Jurkovic, C. J., & Casey, S.(2009). Relation of filial responsibility to the per-sonal and social adjustment of Latino adolescentsfrom immigrant families. Journal of Family Psy-chology, 23, 14–22. doi:10.1037/a0014064

Lorenzo-Blanco, E. I., Unger, J. B., Baezconde-Garbanati, L., Ritt-Olson, A., & Soto, D. (2012).Acculturation, enculturation, and symptoms of de-

248 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 26: Familism through a developmental lens

pression in Hispanic youth: The roles of gender,Hispanic cultural values, and family functioning.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 1350–1365. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9774-7

Lugo Steidel, A., & Contreras, J. M. (2003). A newfamilism scale for use with Latino populations.Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 25, 312–330. doi:10.1177/0739986303256912

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture andthe self: Implications for cognition, emotion, andmotivation. Psychological review, 98, 224–253.

Marsiglia, F. F., Parsai, M., & Kulis, S. (2009).Effects of familism and family cohesion on prob-lem behaviors among adolescents in Mexican im-migrant families in the southwest United States.Journal of Ethnic and Cultural Diversity in SocialWork, 18, 203–220. doi:10.1080/15313200903070965

Masten, A. S. (2006). Developmental psychopathol-ogy: Pathways to the future. International Journalof Behavioral Development, 30, 47–54. doi:10.1177/0165025406059974

Morcillo, C., Duarte, C. S., Shen, S., Blanco, C.,Canino, G., & Bird, H. R. (2011). Parentalfamilism and antisocial behaviors: Development,gender, and potential mechanisms. Journal of theAmerican Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychi-atry, 50, 471–479. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.014

Mulligan, G. M., Brimhall, D., & West, J. (2005).Child care and early education arrangements ofinfants, toddlers, and preschoolers: 2001. Statisti-cal Analysis Report. NCES 2006–039. NationalCenter for Education Statistics, Washington, DC.

Nolle, A. P., Gulbas, L., Kuhlberg, J. A., & Zayas,L. H. (2012). Sacrifice for the sake of the family:Expressions of familism by Latina teens in thecontext of suicide. American Journal of Orthopsy-chiatry 82, 3, 319–327. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01166.x

Olson, D. H., McCubbin, H., Barnes, H., Larsen,A. S., Muxen, M. J., & Wilson, M. A. (1983).Families: What makes them work. Beverly Hills,CA: Sage.

Peña, J. B., Kuhlberg, J. A., Zayas, L. H., Baumann,A. A., Gulbas, L., Hausmann-Stabile, C., & Nolle,A. P. (2011). Familism, family environment, andsuicide attempts among Latina youth. Suicide andLife-Threatening Behavior, 41, 330–341. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00032.x

Polo, A. J., & Lopez, S. R. (2009). Culture, context,and the internalizing distress of Mexican Americanyouth. Journal of Clinical Child & AdolescentPsychology, 38, 273–285. doi:10.1080/15374410802698370

Raffaelli, M., & Ontai, L. L. (2004). Gender social-ization in Latino/a families: Results from two ret-rospective studies. Sex Roles, 50, 287–299. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000018886.58945.06

Romero, A. J., Robinson, T. N., Haydel, K. F., Men-doza, F., & Killen, J. D. (2004). Associationsamong familism, language preference, and educa-tion in Mexican- American mothers and their chil-dren. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pe-diatrics, 25, 34–40.

Sabogal, F., Marín, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., VanOssMarín, B., & Perez-Stable, E. (1987). Hispanicfamilism and acculturation: What changes andwhat doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sci-ences, 9, 397–412.

Sánchez, B., Colón, Y., & Esparza, P. (2005). Therole of sense of school belonging and gender in theacademic adjustment of Latino adolescents. Jour-nal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 619–628. doi:10.1007/s10964-005-8950-4

Sánchez, B., Esparza, P., Colón, Y., & Davis, K. E.(2010). Tryin’ to make it during the transition fromhigh school: The role of family obligation attitudesand economic context for Latino emerging adults.Journal of Adolescent Research, 25, 858–884.

Smokowski, P. R., & Bacallao, M. (2007). Accultur-ation, internalizing mental health symptoms andself-esteem: Cultural experiences of Latino adoles-cents in North Carolina. Child Psychiatry and Hu-man Development, 37, 273–292. doi:10.1007/s10578-006-0035-4

Smokowski, P., Rose, R., & Bacallao, M. (2010).Influence of risk factors and cultural assets onLatino adolescents’ trajectories of self-esteem andinternalizing symptoms. Child Psychiatry and Hu-man Development, 41, 133–155. doi:10.1007/s10578-009-0157-6

Stein, G. L., Gonzalez, L. M., Cupito, A. M., Kiang,L., & Supple, A. J. (2013). The protective role offamilism in the lives of Latino adolescents. Jour-nal of Family Issues. Advance online publication.doi:10.1177/0192513X13502480

Stein, G. L., & Polo, A. J. (2014). Parent–childcultural value gaps and depressive symptomsamong Mexican American youth. Journal of Childand Family Studies, 23, 189–199. doi:10.1007/s10826-013-9724-3

Sy, S. R. (2006). Family and work influences on thetransition to college among Latina adolescents.Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 28, 368–386.

Taylor, Z. E., Larsen-Rife, D., Conger, R. D., &Widaman, K. F. (2012). Familism, interparentalconflict, and parenting in Mexican-origin Families:A cultural–contextual framework. Journal of Mar-riage and Family, 74, 312–327. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00958.x

Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., &Galván, A. (2013). Meaningful family relation-ships: Neurocognitive buffers of adolescent risktaking. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25,374–387. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00331

249FAMILISM

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 27: Familism through a developmental lens

Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Alfaro, E. C., Bámaca, M. Y.,& Guimond, A. B. (2009). The central role offamilial ethnic socialization in Latino adolescents’cultural orientation. Journal of Marriage andFamily, 71, 46–60. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00579.x

Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Updegraff, K., & Gonzales-Backen, M. (2011). Mexican-origin adolescentmothers’ stressors and psychosocial functioning:Examining ethnic identity affirmation andfamilism as moderators. Journal of Youth and Ad-olescence, 40, 140–157. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9511-z

Unger, J. B., Ritt-Olson, A., Teran, L., Huang, T.,Hoffman, B. R., & Palmer, P. (2002). Culturalvalues and substance use in a multiethnic sampleof California adolescents. Addiction Research The-ory, 10, 257–279.

Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., Whiteman, S. D.,Thayer, S. M., & Delgado, M. Y. (2005). Adoles-cent sibling relationships in Mexican Americanfamilies: Exploring the role of familism. Journalof Family Psychology, 19, 512–522. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.512

Updegraff, K. A., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., McHale,S. M., Wheeler, L. A., & Perez-Brena, N. J.(2012). Mexican-origin youth’s cultural orienta-tions and adjustment: Changes from early to lateadolescence. Child Development, 83, 1655–1671.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01800.x

Updegraff, K. A., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Perez-Brena,N. J., & Pflieger, J. (2012). Mother–daughter con-

flict and adjustment in Mexican-origin families:Exploring the role of family and sociocultural con-text. New Directions for Child and AdolescentDevelopment, 2012, 59–81. doi:10.1002/cd.20004

Valdés, G. (1996). Con respeto: Bridging the dis-tances between culturally diverse families andschools. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Valenzuela, A., & Dornbusch, S. (1994). Familismand social capital in the academic achievement ofMexican origin and Anglo adolescents. Social Sci-ence Quarterly, 75, 18–36.

White, R. M., Zeiders, K. H., Gonzales, N. A., Tein,J. Y., & Roosa, M. W. (2013). Cultural values, USneighborhood danger, and Mexican American par-ents’ parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 27,365–375. doi:10.1037/a0032888

Wozniak, R., Sung, S., Crump, T., Edgar-Smith, S.,& Litzinger, S. (1996). The relational family val-ues Q-sort. Unpublished manuscript, Bryn MawrCollege, Bryn Mawr, PA.

Yesil-Dagli, U. (2011). Predicting ELL students’ be-ginning first grade English oral reading fluencyfrom initial kindergarten vocabulary, letter nam-ing, and phonological awareness skills. EarlyChildhood Research Quarterly, 26, 15–29. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.06.001

Received September 5, 2013Revision received June 16, 2014

Accepted June 20, 2014 �

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!

Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will beavailable online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ andyou will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

250 STEIN ET AL.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.