Top Banner
Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and Social Competence in Autism Spectrum Disorder A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Clinical Psychology University of Regina By Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard Regina, Saskatchewan May, 2014 Copyright 2014: N.Berard
236

Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

Jul 12, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and

Social Competence in Autism Spectrum Disorder

A Thesis

Submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

in

Clinical Psychology

University of Regina

By

Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard

Regina, Saskatchewan

May, 2014

Copyright 2014: N.Berard

Page 2: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

UNIVERSITY OF REGINA

FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES AND RESEARCH

SUPERVISORY AND EXAMINING COMMITTEE

Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Clinical Psychology, has presented a thesis titled, Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and Social Competence in Autism Spectrum Disorder, in an oral examination held on April 14, 2014. The following committee members have found the thesis acceptable in form and content, and that the candidate demonstrated satisfactory knowledge of the subject material. External Examiner: *Dr. J. Montgomery, University of Manitoba

Co-Supervisor: Dr. Lynn Loutzenhiser, Department of Psychology

Co-Supervisor: Dr. Dennis Alfano, Department of Psychology

Committee Member: Dr. Chris Oriet, Department of Psychology

Committee Member: *Dr. Kristi Wright, Department of Psychology

Committee Member: Dr. Kerri Staples, Faculty of Kinesiology and Health Studies

Chair of Defense: Dr. Ken Montgomery, Faculty of Education *via teleconference

Page 3: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

i

ABSTRACT

The main objective of this study was to investigate the concurrent role of

multiple antecedents of social competence in a group of children with Autism

Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Existing models of social competence were adapted to

include three domains of executive function (EF: Cognitive, Behavioural, and

Emotional Regulation), and two domains of Social Emotional Learning (SEL:

Nonverbal Awareness, Social Understanding). The EF domains were related to

sustained attention, working memory, planning, behavioural inhibition, and affective

decision making; SEL domains included social comprehension, and identification and

interpretation of social cues. Social competence was defined in terms of social skills

and adaptive social functioning. The relationships amongst the EF and SEL domains,

and social competence were examined in a sample of 49 boys with ASD and 48

neurotypical boys, aged 8 to 13 years. Results showed that the ASD group performed

significantly below the control group on most SEL and EF domains. Children with

ASD were also rated significantly lower on social competence measures and parental

ratings of EF. Importantly, the EF domain of Cognitive Regulation predicted social

competence in boys with ASD whereas the SEL domain of Social Understanding

predicted social competence in neurotypical boys. These findings contribute

significantly to our understanding of social competence and quality of life in boys

with ASD. The observation that Cognitive Regulation predicts social competence in

boys with ASD has important clinical implications for specifically targeting EF in

both assessment and treatment.

Page 4: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

ii

Acknowledgements

The completion of this dissertation was made possible with the incredible

support and guidance of my supervisors, Dr. Lynn Loutzenhiser and Dr. Dennis

Alfano. They helped instill confidence in my work and taught me the importance of

perseverance. Thank you both for pushing me past what I thought I was able to

accomplish. I am so grateful for your constant availability and your valuable

expertise, feedback, and mentorship throughout all stages of this research. My

appreciation also goes out to the members of my advisory committee: Dr. Kerri

Staples, Dr. Chris Oriet, and Dr. Kristi Wright and also to Dr. Janine Montgomery,

the external examiner. Thank you all for your thoughtful consideration of this work.

I am grateful for the encouragement and moral support of my friends and

colleagues. To my colleagues at the Autism Centre, thank you for supporting me

throughout this research. A special thanks to Dr. Della Hunter, who instilled in me a

thirst to learn more about executive functioning and its importance in the lives of

children.

Lastly, I want to thank the children and families with whom I have worked

over the years, who taught me so much about the importance of vulnerability, hope

and gratitude. I am particularly grateful to all the children and families who took the

time to participate in this study.

Page 5: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

iii

Dedication

The support of my family has been invaluable to the completion of this

dissertation. I want to express my deep appreciation to my parents, Norbert and

Claudette, for the endless support they have given me throughout my life and for

always believing in me. Merci de m'avoir appuyé toute ma vie et lors de se long trajet

difficile et parfois frustrant. Cet accomplissement est pour vous, car malgré tous les

détours de la vie, vous m'avez appris qu'il faut garder le but en tête et persévéré.

To my husband, Colin, this has certainly been a shared journey. Thank you for

supporting my decision to attend graduate school. It has been a long and sometimes

difficult and frustrating road, and I could not have completed this dissertation without

your support, perspective and sense of humour along the way. I appreciate the

sacrifices you made so that I could achieve this goal. To my children, Cadell and

Milène, you are too young to understand the sacrifices you made that allowed me to

complete this research and fulfill my dream. I hope you always have the support I was

provided throughout this research so that you too may accomplish what you want in

life. Please remember that with confidence - belief in yourself, hard work and

determination you can fulfill your goals.

Page 6: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

iv

Table of Contents

Abstract .......................................................................................................................... i

Acknowledgements ....................................................................................................... ii

Dedication .................................................................................................................... iii

Table of Contents ......................................................................................................... iv

List of Tables .............................................................................................................. vii

List of Figures ............................................................................................................ viii

List of Appendices ....................................................................................................... ix

1.0 Overview ................................................................................................................. 1

2.0 Introduction ............................................................................................................. 4

2.1 Social Competence ...................................................................................... 4

2.2 Autism Spectrum Disorder .......................................................................... 5

2.3 Models of Social Competence .................................................................... 9

2.4 Executive Functions .................................................................................. 15

2.5 Executive Function in Relation to ASD and Social Competence ............. 18

2.6 Social Emotional Learning ........................................................................ 28

2.7 Heterogeneity in ASD ............................................................................... 37

2.8 Relationships among SEL, EF and Social Competence............................ 38

2.9 Limitations of Current Research ............................................................... 39

2.10 Integration and Aims of the Current Research ........................................... 41

3.0 Research Hypotheses ............................................................................................ 44

4.0 Methods................................................................................................................. 47

4.1 Participant Recruitment ............................................................................. 47

Page 7: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

v

4.2 Data Collection.......................................................................................... 48

4.3 Clinical File Review .................................................................................. 49

4.4 Participants ................................................................................................ 49

4.5 Child Measures of SEL and EF ................................................................. 50

4.6 Parent Measures of EF and Social Competence ....................................... 63

5.0 Results ................................................................................................................... 67

5.1 Data Analysis Plan ................................................................................... 67

5.2 Descriptive Information ............................................................................ 68

5.3 Hypothesis 1 .............................................................................................. 76

5.4 Hypothesis 2 .............................................................................................. 78

5.5 Hypothesis 3 .............................................................................................. 83

5.6 Hypothesis 4 .............................................................................................. 92

5.7 Hypothesis 5 ............................................................................................ 102

6.0 Discussion ........................................................................................................... 104

6.1 Social Competence ................................................................................. 104

6.2 Demographic Factors Associated with Social Competence .................. 106

6.3 Social Emotional Learning ..................................................................... 111

6.4 Executive Functions ............................................................................... 117

6.5 Model of Social Competence ................................................................. 121

6.6 Relationship between SEL and EF domains .......................................... 132

6.7 SEL and EF in Relation to Social Competence ..................................... 134

6.8 Predictors of Social Competence ........................................................... 139

6.9 Study Limitations ................................................................................... 149

Page 8: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

vi

6.10 Conclusion and Future Directions ............................................................... 151

7.0 References ........................................................................................................... 156

Page 9: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

vii

List of Tables

Table 1. Demographic variables for ASD and control group ..................................... 51

Table 2. Age, IQ, SEL & social competence correlations: control group .................. 71

Table 3. Age, IQ, SEL & social competence correlations: ASD group ...................... 72

Table 4. Age, IQ, EF & social competence correlations: control group ..................... 73

Table 5. Age, IQ, EF & social competence correlations: ASD group .......................... 74

Table 6. Descriptive statistics of SEL, EF and social competence measures ........... 79

Table 7. Correlations of SEL, EF & SC domains: control group ................................. 94

Table 8. Correlations of SEL, EF & SC domains: ASD group ..................................... 95

Table 9. Regression analyses predicting social competence: control group ............... 98

Table 10. Regression coefficients of the SEL and EF: control group ........................... 99

Table 11. Regression analyses predicting social competence: ASD group ............... 100

Table 12. Regression coefficients of the SEL and EF: ASD group............................. 101

Page 10: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

viii

List of Figures

Figure 1a. McKown three-factor SEL model ............................................................ 13

Figure 1b. McKown two-factor SEL model .............................................................. 13

Figure 3. The proposed tripartite EF and SEL domains .......................................................19

Figure 4. Proposed social competence model ............................................................ 43

Figure 5a. SEL domains for the control group .......................................................... 84

Figure 5b. SEL domains for the ASD group .............................................................. 84

Figure 6a. EF domains for the control group ............................................................. 90

Figure 6b. EF domains for the ASD group ................................................................ 90

Figure 7. SEL, EF and social competence: ASD group ........................................... 141

Figure 8. SEL, parent-rated EF and social competence: ASD group ...................... 142

Figure 9. SEL, EF and social competence: control group ....................................... 143

Figure 10. SEL, parent-rated EF and social competence: control group ................. 144

Page 11: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

ix

List of Appendices

Appendix A: Ethics approval ................................................................................... 197

Appendix B: Recruitment letter ............................................................................... 199

Appendix C: Consent and assent forms ................................................................... 200

Appendix D: Visual schedule .................................................................................. 209

Appendix E: List of all measures used in study ....................................................... 210

Appendix E: List of all measures used in study ....................................................... 211

Appendix G: Record forms for SEL & EF tasks ..................................................... 213

Page 12: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

1

1.0 Overview

Social competence, broadly defined as the ability to effectively engage in social

interactions (Rose-Krasnor, 1997), is a robust and critical predictor of positive outcomes

throughout the lifespan in both neurotypical and clinical populations. (Burt et al., 2008;

Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, Badura & Zimbardo, 2000; Crick & Dodge, 1994;

Eberly & Montemayor, 1998; Kumpfer, 1999; Rys & Bear, 1997; Mangham, McGrath,

Reid, & Stewart, 1995; McKown, Gumbiner, Russo & Lipton 2009; Smart & Sanson,

2003).

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder whose

cardinal feature is social impairment (Volkmar & Klin, 2005). Children with ASD are

thus at higher risk for poorer overall quality of life. Despite this increased risk, there is

currently limited understanding of social competence in ASD, factors that contribute to

the heterogeneity of social abilities in ASD, or effective clinical management strategies.

Developmental theories of social competence have tended to focus on the effects

of either socio-cognitive or socio-affective processes on social competence (Lipton &

Nowicki, 2009), and research on ASD has similarly tended to focus on socio-cognitive

and/or socio-emotional processes as a means of explaining deficits associated with the

diagnosis. More recent models have identified executive function (EF) as an additional

important potential contributor to social competence in children (Beauchamp &

Anderson, 2010). To date, however, the role of EF in the social competence of children

with ASD is unclear.

Typical interventions to assist children in learning behaviours to improve social

Page 13: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

2

interactions generally fall under the rubric of social emotional learning (SEL) skills,

which encompass socio-cognitive skills and socio-emotional skills, such as emotion

recognition, social problem solving, and taking the perspective of others. SEL skill

interventions, however, have not proven as effective in clinical populations such as

children with ASD, particularly those with high functioning ASD (Rao, Beidel &

Murray, 2008; White, Keonig & Scahill, 2007). An important limitation to such

conventional interventions is the assumption SEL skills are the primary antecedents to

social competence. However, the recent identification of EF as an important antecedent to

social competence warrants a more comprehensive approach to understanding and

improving social competence in ASD. There are thus clear theoretical grounds to revisit

current models of social competence, particularly with respect to the important role that

EF, either alone or in conjunction with SEL skills, play in the development of social

competence in both neurotypical children and those with ASD. The ultimate aim is to

inform the literature and to improve upon current interventions to increase social

competence in ASD.

In the following sections, a brief overview of ASD is provided, highlighting the

heterogeneity in this population. Existing theories of social competence are reviewed

with a focus on identifying a more comprehensive set of antecedents to social

competence that can be applied to children with ASD. Next, recent conceptualisations of

EF as a form of executive control of cognitive, behavioural, and emotional functioning

are explored (Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013). Particularly, the literature highlighting

the potential role of executive functions (EF) in the social competence of children is

examined. Thereafter, limitations and controversies in the ASD and EF literature are

Page 14: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

3

identified. Finally, the implications of a comprehensive model of social competence that

includes different domains of EF and SEL in ASD is addressed.

Page 15: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

4

2.0 Introduction

2.1. Social Competence

Social competence is commonly conceptualized as a multifaceted construct that

occurs along a continuum and which predicts the effectiveness of interactions with others

(Bohlin & Hagekull, 2009; Odom, McConnell & Brown, 2008; Burt, Obradovic, Long &

Masten, 2008; Rose-Krasnor, 1997; Segrin, 2000; Trentacosta & Fine, 2010). Social

competence includes behaviours such as sharing, helping, cooperating, initiating and

maintaining relationships, sensitively interacting with others, negotiating needs and

effectively handling conflict situations. From a developmental perspective, social

competence involves “the active and skilful coordination of multiple processes and

resources available to the child to meet social demands and achieve social goals in a

particular type of social interaction (e.g., parent-child, peer relations) and within a

specific context (e.g., home, school)” (Iarocci, Yager & Elfers, 2007, p. 113).

While social competence develops throughout childhood, there is a correlation

between social competence in early childhood and later adolescence, suggesting that it is

typically established early and is stable across time (Eisenberg et al., 1997; Masten &

Coatsworth, 1998). Social competence has been found to predict quality of peer

relationships, academic success in late childhood, and work competence in adolescence

(Burt et al., 2008; Caprara et al., 2000; Eisenberg et al., 1997; Masten et al., 1995a,

1995b). Social competence is also one of the most frequently identified attributes of

resiliency and overall mental health in children (Parrila, Ma, Fleming & Rinaldi, 2002).

When a mental disorder is present, poor social competence has been correlated with

Page 16: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

5

greater severity of illness, a higher risk of co-morbid disorders, and a poorer prognosis

(Jewel, Jordan, Hupp, & Everett, 2009). Given the importance of social competence to

quality of life, understanding the variables associated with social competence in child

mental health populations with social impairments is an important area of study. Autism

Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder whose prime feature is social

impairment (Volkmar & Klin, 2005). This research will focus on increasing our

understanding of social competence in ASD, factors that contribute to the heterogeneity

of social abilities in ASD, and implications for effective clinical management strategies.

2.2 Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASD is an aetiologically complex neurodevelopment disorder characterized by

core deficits in social functioning. It has been well-established that children with ASD

exhibit multiple pervasive deficits in social competence, including initiating interactions,

joint attention, play skills, reciprocal interactions, nonverbal communication, and peer

relations (Carter, Davis, Klin & Volkmar, 2005).

ASD subsumes three disorders that fall under the umbrella of ‘Pervasive

Developmental Disorders’ (PDD) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) (APA, 2000): Autistic Disorder,

Asperger's Disorder, Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD

NOS). All three disorders are defined by a core ‘triad’ of qualitative impairments in

social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication deficits, and restricted, repetitive,

and stereotyped interest. The term autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is frequently used in

the literature to encapsulate these three disorders due to evidence that has suggested a

substantial overlap among them (Szatmari, 1992; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz & Klin,

Page 17: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

6

2004; Volkmar & Klin, 2005; Wing, 2005). This approach is also taken in the recent

DSM-5, which has identified ASD as a single diagnostic category encompassing Autistic

Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder, and PDD NOS based on the idea that a single spectrum

better reflects the behavioural pathology (Swedo, 2009, p.1). In keeping with this, the

term ASD will be utilised for the remainder of this study.

The most recent prevalence rates for ASD range from 15 to 23 per 1,000 (Centers

for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012); the prevalence of ASD in Canada has been

estimated to be 11 per 1,000, with the majority (82%) being male (NHSR, 2013).

2.2.1 Social competence in ASD. The pathognomonic feature of ASD is social

impairment (Mundy, 2003). Children with ASD have multiple social deficits, including

impairment in the use of non-verbal behaviours to regulate social interaction, difficulty

establishing and maintaining peer relationships, a lack of shared enjoyment in the

interests and accomplishments of others, and a general paucity of social or emotional

reciprocity (APA, 2000). Nevertheless, ASD is also a disorder marked by variability that

is evident in symptom manifestation, cognitive abilities, and developmental course and

outcomes. For example, children with ASD and normal intellectual functioning are

referred to as having high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD) , and different

social trajectories have been demonstrated for children with and without co-morbid

intellectual disabilities (Szatmari, Bryson, Boyle, Streiner & Duku, 2003; Klin & Jones,

2009). Predictors of social competence have also been found to be different for children

with HFASD and those with ASD and intellectual disabilities, who comprise

approximately 30% of the population (CDC, 2012). Specifically, IQ and language

abilities were found to predict social competence in children with ASD and low

Page 18: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

7

intellectual function but found to have only weak associations in those with HFASD (Lis

et al., 2001; Sigman & Ruskin, 1999; Szatmari et al., 2003). Since language and IQ do

not necessarily predict social competence in children with HFASD, examining other

factors, such as social emotional comprehension and executive functions (EF), may

provide valuable information regarding social competence in this particular population.

Given the well-documented differences between children with ASD with and without an

intellectual disability, all information subsequently presented will specifically relate to

children with ASD without intellectual disabilities, unless otherwise mentioned.

It is also well demonstrated in the literature that children with ASD display a wide

range of social competence, with some doing better or worse than others. For example,

some children with ASD have reciprocal friendships whereas others do not (Orsmond et

al., 2004). In fact, within the triad of impairments associated with ASD, children with

ASD show significantly more variability in the social domains as compared to the

domains of communication and repetitive behaviours (Hazlett et al., 2009). Given the

variability in behavioural and clinical manifestations of ASD, examining the

heterogeneity in the social competence of children with average intelligence has

important clinical implications, particularly since the majority are in mainstream

classrooms and have to negotiate daily social demands with peers and teachers.

Significant variability in the social competence of children with ASD has been

identified (Szatmari et al., 2003; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz & Klin, 2004), but the

reason for the variability remains unclear and has not been a focus of research. Why are

some children with ASD able to maintain peer relationships? Why are some children with

ASD able to respond appropriately to certain social demands? Research in this area has

Page 19: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

8

likely been hindered by a focus on social deficits and maladaptive behaviours rather than

intact abilities. For example, social competence is primarily measured using the Autism

Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) [Rutter, Le Couteur, & Lord, 2003] or Autism

Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [Lord et al., 1989], which are diagnostic tools

designed to identify presence or absence of ASD symptoms and deficits. Equally

problematic is the measurement of social competence using brief screening instruments,

such as the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS: Constantino & Gruber, 2005), which

assesses non-social aspects of impairment, such as reactions to sensory stimuli and

repetitive behaviours. Although these measures evaluate difficulties salient to ASD and

provide meaningful information regarding the triad of impairments associated with the

disorder, they do not evaluate many other important aspects of social functioning, such as

sharing, helping, and cooperating. The singular focus of deficit or disability models thus

has inherent limitations in terms of accounting for the heterogeneity in social functioning

in ASD. Furthermore, recent reviews have documented that most individual and group-

based social skills training programs designed for children with ASD fail to demonstrate

significant improvements in social competence (Rao et al., 2008; White, Keonig, &

Scahill, 2007), perhaps due to a restricted view of factors relevant to social competence.

It is now well-established in the developmental literature that social competence

encompasses multiple variables. In this light, researchers have recently begun to explore

the heterogeneity of ASD using multidimensional conceptualisations of social

competence. The following section addresses these new conceptualizations. This

population bears particular importance to the study of social competence. Children with

ASD are at high risk for co-morbid mental health disorders that do not form part of

Page 20: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

9

diagnostic criteria for ASD (Mattila et al., 2010; Midouhas, Yogaratnam, Flouri and

Charman, 2013), as well as poor quality of life by virtue of the significant social

impairments associated with the disorder (Howlin et al., 2013; Baird, 2014). Moreover,

current social interventions have been fairly ineffective in improving social competence.

The importance of identifying better mechanisms for improving their quality of life via

increased social competence is therefore quite valuable.

2.3 Models of Social Competence

Early models of social competence focused predominantly on either social-

cognitive or social-affective processes or skills, as the key antecedents to social

competence. Socio-cognitive models emphasized the thought processes involved in social

competence and the importance of perception and interpretation of social information

(Crick & Dodge, 1994). Socio-affective models, on the other hand, emphasized the

importance of the perception of emotions in the development of social competence

(Halberstadt et al., 2001; Saarni, 1990).

One drawback of the above models is that they tend to focus on the contributions of

either emotion or cognition alone in social functioning rather than both. It is now

generally agreed that multiple processes must operate concurrently for the development

of effective social competence (Bierman, 2004; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Lipton &

Nowicki, 2009; McKown, 2007; McKown et al., 2009; McKown, Allen, Russo-Ponsaran

& Johnson, 2013). Another drawback is that while both models emphasize the

importance of self-regulation to social competence (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Eisenberg &

Fabes 1992; Eisenberg et al., 1997; Fabes et al., 1999; Halberstadt et al., 2001; Masten &

Coasworth, 1998; Smart & Sanson, 2003), self-regulation is not well-defined.

Page 21: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

10

Self-regulation is an umbrella term referring to the capacity to manage one’s

thoughts, feelings and actions in adaptive and flexible ways across a range of contexts

(Saarni, 1997; Vohs & Baumeister, 2004; 2011), and the importance of self-regulation in

social functioning is well-documented in the literature (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005;

Heatherton & Wagner, 2001; Tangney et al., 2004; Vohs & Baumeister, 2004; 2011).

Recent conceptualizations of social competence that incorporate self-regulation have

been significantly advanced by the notion of Executive Function (EF) (Beauchamp

&Anderson, 2010; McKown et al., 2009; Yeates et al., 2004). EF refers to a multiple

higher order process system that serves as an integrated supervisory control mechanism

for thought and action that is mediated by prefrontal areas of the brain (Golstein,

Naglieri, Princiotta & Otero, 2014). Self-regulation is generally conceived of as the

ability to guide, monitor, and direct one’s performance to successfully manage behaviour

and achieve goals (Singer & Bashiri, 1999, Hofmann, Schmeichel & Baddely, 2012). EF

and self-regulation are thus inextricably linked (Hofmann et al., 2012; Rueda, Possner &

Rothburt, 2005), but have largely been examined separately in relation to social

competence. This is likely because the concept of self regulation has been studied

predominantly in social and personality research, whereas EF has been examined

predominantly in cognitive psychology and the neurosciences. In the last few years,

however, there has been some attempt to link these concepts, and there is accumulating

support for the idea that EF skills in fact promote self-regulation in terms of direction and

control across the domains of cognition, action, and emotion (Hofmann et al., 2012;

McCloskey, 2009, 2011).

Conceptually, it makes sense that EF contributes to self-regulation. For example,

Page 22: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

11

when a child wants to hit a sibling but refrains from doing so, he/she is self-regulating an

impulse. When a child follows-through with a promise to help a friend with homework

instead of engaging in a fun activity, he/she is resisting temptation. Self-regulation also

includes the ability to delay gratification, such as when a child overrides the desire to buy

candy in order to save money to purchase a future item. Attention helps a child to resist

distraction, working memory helps to keep a goal in mind, inhibition helps withhold

predominant responses, and planning helps a child think ahead. Thus, in order to

successfully self-regulate behaviour, a child must successfully use various EF skills.

EF abilities are, therefore, central to self-regulatory processes and contribute

significantly to the development of socially acceptable behaviour. Recent

conceptualisations have emphasized the importance of EF skills in self-regulation

(Barkley, 2012; McCloskey, 2009, 2011), and have asserted that self-regulation should

indeed be subsumed under the overarching umbrella of EF (Berkman, Graham & Fisher,

2012; McCloskey, 2011).

2.3.1 The McKown model of social competence. This model represents a recent

integration of SEL and self-regulation (McKown et al., 2007). SEL skills comprise

various social-emotional comprehension skills, which constitute an integrated

representation of socio-cognitive and socio-affective factors. An important contribution

of this model is the robust empirical analysis of SEL antecedents to social competence.

McKown et al. (2013) proposed a two-factor and a three-factor model of SEL that

includes nonverbal awareness (affect recognition), social meaning (interpretation of

social cues) and social reasoning (social problem solving); the two-factor model included

nonverbal awareness in one domain and an amalgamation of social meaning and social

Page 23: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

12

problem solving in the other domain. McKown et al. decided on the three factor model

due to its better fit with the empirical data and closer correspondence to their

conceptualisation of SEL (see Figures 1a & 1b). McKown et al. (2009; 2013)

subsequently demonstrated that SEL skills are independent predictors of social

competence for both typically developing children and children with various mental

health disorders. Each domain in the two and three factor models of SEL was positively

correlated with social competence. Overall, the better developed a child’s SEL, the more

positive their social interactions and peer relationships. The McKown model thus has

important theoretical and clinical implications because it demonstrates that SEL skills

have concurrent, incremental and discriminative validity for social competence (Lipton &

Nowicki, 2009; McKown, 2007; McKown et al., 2009; McKown et al., 2013). Moreover,

McKown et al. (2013) demonstrated that the construct of social emotional comprehension

can be reliably assessed with psychometric measures that directly reflect the critical

dimensions of SEL. Also, the McKown model includes the concept of self regulation,

defined as "the ability to modulate attention and behaviour in response to a situation"

(McKown et al., 2009; p.860) and provides data demonstrating that both SEL and self-

regulation are significant independent predictors of social competence; indeed, self-

regulation was shown to be more highly correlated to ratings of social competence than

SEL.

One drawback of the McKown model, however, is the narrow measurement of

social competence using only three subscales of the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS:

Gresham & Elliot, 1990) in one study and two subscales of the Behavior Assessment

System for Children (BASC: Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) in another. Together, these

Figure 1: McKown et al. (2009) three-factor

model of the relationship between social-

emotional learning (SEL) skill, self-regulation,

and social competence

Page 24: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

13

Figure 1b: McKown et al. (2009) two-factor

model of the relationship between social-

emotional learning (SEL) skill, self-regulation,

and social competence

Figure 1a: McKown et al. (2009) three-factor

model of the relationship between social-

emotional learning (SEL) skill, self-regulation,

and social competence

Page 25: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

14

subscales reflect important aspects of social competence for children related to

cooperation, assertiveness and self-control, but do not tap the full range of indicators of

social competence. Another limitation with the McKown model is the narrow

measurement of self-regulation that focused only on attention and inhibition, and that was

assessed only through parental ratings. Further, the measurement of self-regulation and

social competence similarly used the same parental report, potentially influencing the

findings due to shared rater and method bias. The McKown Model could also be

expanded to include consider a wider range of self-regulatory behaviours that might be

re-conceptualised as the broader range of EF skills. Specifically, while the McKown

Model includes inattention, which is one component of cognitive regulation, there are

other aspects of cognitive regulation, such as planning and working memory, which may

also play a role in social competence. Similarly, while the McKown Model includes

inhibition, which is one component of behavioural regulation, it neglects to consider

other aspects of behavioural regulation, such as self-monitoring and suppression of

automatic impulses that may also be important. Moreover, despite being identified as a

basic element in social interactions and a critical component of social competence

(Eisenberg et al., 2000b; Halberstadt et al.2001; Saarni, 1999; Semrud-Clikeman, 2007;

Sigman & Ruskin, 1999), emotion regulation is not addressed in the McKown Model.

The McKown Model, therefore, might be improved upon by expanding the range

of indicators of social competence and by including a more comprehensive

conceptualization of cognitive, behavioural and emotional regulation consistent with

recent notions of EF. Specifically, direct evaluation of a broad range of EF skills that

subserve self-regulatory processes may provide a more comprehensive examination of

Page 26: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

15

antecedents of social competence and provide valuable information for targeted

intervention (Hofmann et al., 2012). To address these limitations, the present study

extends the McKown Model of social competence to include a comprehensive evaluation

of EF within the three domains of regulation – cognitive, behavioural and emotion

regulation – in order to determine the specific antecedents to social competence in ASD.

Such a comprehensive approach to studying factors that directly and indirectly interact in

the development and maintenance of social competence in both neurotypical and clinical

populations has previously been proposed (Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010; Yeates et al.,

2007). For example, in a study of 228 adolescents, Rinsky and Hinshaw (2012)

demonstrated that direct measures of planning, inhibition and working memory predicted

social competence in teens with and without ADHD. Interventions targeting specific EF

skills have demonstrated improvements in social behaviour in typical young children

(Diamond, Barnett, Thomas & Munro, 2007), and reduction in behaviour problems in

school-aged children (Riggs, Greenberg, Kusche & Pentz, 2006), and those with ADHD

(Berkman, Graham & Fisher, 2012).

2.4 Executive Functions

EF is generally defined as a multiple process system that serves to control and

integrate higher order cognitive processes that are involved in thought, action, and goal-

directed behaviour (Anderson, 2002; Kerr & Zelazo, 2004; Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2009).

Self-regulatory functions are considered a hallmark feature of EF (Giurak et al., 2009;

McCloskey, 2009, 2011; Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013; Ylisaker & Feeney, 2002;

Zhou, Chen & Main, 2012) and may be best understood as a core element of EF that is

Page 27: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

16

involved in the conscious regulation of thought, emotion, and behavior (McCloskey,

2006; Zelazo, 2010).

Recent conceptualizations of EF propose a multifaceted construct based on three

distinct but interrelated dimensions of self-regulatory processes: cognitive, behavioural

and emotion regulation (Ganesingham et al., 2006; 2007a; 2007b; Gioia et al., 2002;

Jahromi & Stifter, 2008; McCloskey, 2011; Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013; Zhou et al.,

2012). Cognitive regulation is typically operationalised as attention, planning, working

memory, flexibility and processing speed (Anderson, 2002; Hill, 2004; Roberts &

Pennington, 1996; Russo et al., 2007). Behavioural regulation refers to various aspects of

inhibitory control, including the ability to regulate activity level, self-monitor and

suppress automatic impulses (Hinnant & Obrien, 2007). Emotion regulation is

distinguished from Behavioural and Cognitive Regulation by the extent to which it

involves affect and regulation. Emotion regulation has been defined as the ability to

modulate emotional expressions to meet situational demands and achieve personal goals

(Blandon, Calkins & Keane, 2010; Dennis, 2010; Lewis, Lamm, Segalowitz, Stieben &

Zelazo, 2006). While some have conceptualised EF dimensions as interrelated and

interdependent (Anderson, 2002), others have conceptualised them as interrelated but

distinct (Korkman, Kirk & Kemp, 2007; Lehto et al. 2003; Miyake et al., 2000). EF

regulatory processes, however, are fundamentally and conceptually linked at the

behavioural and neural levels (Calkins, 2010; Ganesalingam et al., 2007).

Developmentally, EF skills emerge at different times during early childhood and

continue to improve throughout adolescence and early adulthood (Best & Miller, 2010).

An integrated model of EF thus considers developmental aspects of EF and the important

Page 28: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

17

contributions of cognitive, inhibitory and emotional aspects of EF to a child's developing

social competence (Barkley, 1997; Best & Miller, 2010; Blair, Zelazo & Greenberg,

2005; Hongwanishkul et al., 2005; Jahromi et al, 2008; McCloskey, 2009, 2011;

Ylvisaker & Sweeney, 2002).

While this three-dimensional conceptualisation of EF is supported in recent

literature (Egeland & Fallmyr, 2010; Gioia et al., 2002; Gioia et al., 2010; Jahromi &

Sifter, 2008), it has not been investigated in children with ASD. However, correlations

have been found among the cognitive, behavioural and emotion regulation domains in

young neurotypical children (Jahromi & Stifter, 2008) and children with acquired brain

injuries (Ganesingham et al., 2006, 2007). A three-part approach also accords with

present knowledge of multifaceted brain functioning (Egeland & Fallmyr, 2010;

Ganesingham et al., 2007; Gioia et al., 2002; Zelazo & Muller, 2002).

In sum, the three domains of EF – namely, cognitive, behavioural and emotional

regulatory processes – provide a more current and broader conceptualisation of EF to

explore in relation to social competence. While the McKown model provides an

empirically sound avenue to evaluate the role of SEL in the social competence of children

with ASD, adapting it in accordance with the tri-partite conceptualization of EF allows

for the direct and independent evaluation of two potential important antecedents of social

competence, namely SEL and EF. The current study extends McKown's SEL model to

further explore the relationship between the three domains of SEL, the three domains of

performance-based and parent-rated EF and social competence in children with high

functioning ASD (see Figure 3). Next, the three domains of EF – cognitive, behavioural

and emotional regulation – will be further explored in relation to children with ASD.

Page 29: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

18

2.5 EF in Relation to ASD and Social Competence

Cognitive, behavioural and emotion regulation domains of EF have been linked to

social competence in typically developing children and certain clinical groups (e.g.

ADHD; TBI), and the conceptualisation of EF as comprising cognitive, behavioural and

emotion regulatory processes has been supported in the recent literature (Ganesingham et

al., 2006; 2007a; 2007b; Gioia et al., 2002). Deficits in some executive functions are also

well-documented in school-age children with ASD (see reviews: Hill, 2004; Pennington

& Ozonoff, 1996; Russo et al., 2007; Sergeant, Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002). For

example, it has been demonstrated that, as a group, children with ASD perform

significantly worse than neurotypical children on both performance-based and parent-

rated measures of EF, and that their performance is not correlated with IQ (Landa &

Goldberg, 2005; Liss et al., 2001). However, there is still no consensus on which specific

aspects of EF are most impaired in school-aged children with ASD (Barron- Linnankoski

et al., 2014; Corbett et al., 2009; Narcizi et al., 2013; Rinehart, Bradshaw, Tonge,

Brereton & Bellgrove, 2002; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2010; van Rijn et al., 2013) and no

study has yet examined cognitive, behavioral, and emotional regulatory aspects of EF in

relation to social competence in ASD.

2.5.1 Cognitive regulation and social competence. Three aspects of cognitive

regulation - attention, planning and working memory – have been well-studied in

children with ASD. Attention processes include the capacity to selectively attend to

specific stimuli (selective attention) and to focus on a specific task and suppress

Page 30: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

19

Figure 3. The proposed tripartite EF and SEL domains using

McKown and colleagues' 2009 three-factor model of SEL as a

framework

Page 31: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

20

irrelevant stimuli (sustained attention) (Anderson, 2002). Planning is a "complex,

dynamic operation in which a sequence of planned actions must be constantly monitored,

re-evaluated, and updated" (Hill, 2004, p.192). Working memory entails simultaneously

remembering and manipulating incoming information (Russo et al., 2007).

A child's ability to attend to relevant social cues or social information is thought to

impact his/her behaviour in social situations. Furthermore, effective social interactions

rely on the ability to hold context- and content-specific information in mind regarding the

verbal and nonverbal messages being provided by another person, and then planning how

to respond appropriately in order to achieve a goal, be it social interaction, making a

request, or negotiating (Bennetto, Pennington & Rogers, 1996). In a series of studies with

typically developing children, sustained attention was significantly associated with

positive peer relationships in school-aged children (Hughes, Dunn, & White, 1998;

NICHD 2003; NICHD, 2009; Murphy, Brinkman & McNamara, 2007). In a longitudinal

study, attention and planning in childhood were significantly related to peer competence

in adolescence (Landry & Smith, 2010). Working memory has also been correlated with

factors related to social competence, such as facial affect recognition (Mathersul et al.,

2009), and with Theory of Mind (ToM) in children (Hughes, 1998). The importance of

working memory in social competence is often cited (e.g. Beauchamp & Anderson,

2010), but has rarely been directly examined in typically developing children.

Although there is some evidence that children with ASD perform significantly

worse than neurotypical peers on three aspects of cognitive regulation: sustained

attention, planning and working memory (Hill, 2004; Russo et al., 2007), findings are

mixed. For example, while some studies have also found that children with ASD perform

Page 32: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

21

significantly worse than neurotypical controls and children with ADHD on measures of

attention (Corbett et al., 2009; Corbett & Constantine, 2006; Kenworthy et al., 2009; van

Rijn et al., 2013), others have not (Goldberg et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2007; Sanders et

al., 2008). The inconsistent findings may be a result of the differences in the populations

sampled. For example, Goldberg and colleagues (2005) excluded children who were

rated above the cut-off scores on parent-rated measures of inattention, hyperactivity or

impulsivity. It is also unclear if both visual and auditory modes of sustained attention are

equally impacted in ASD (Corbett & Constantine, 2006; Corbett, Constantine, Hendren,

Rocke & Ozonoff, 2009).

There is substantial evidence that children with ASD have significant impairments

in planning tasks compared to neurotypical children (Hill, 2004; Liss et al., 2001;

Ozonoff & McEvoy, 1994; Robinson, Goddard, Dritschel, Wisley & Howlin, 2009) and

children with ADHD (Sergeant, Geurts & Oosterlaan, 2004).There is some evidence that

children with ASD have deficits in working memory, although the results are mixed.

Variable results appear to be a function of the task, with children doing better when

required to respond to verbal rather than visual stimuli. Therefore, while children with

ASD performed the same as neurotypical peers on measures of verbal working memory

(Geurts et al., 2004; Ozonoff & Strayer, 2001), they performed significantly worse on

measures of visual working memory (Landa & Goldberg, 2005; Goldberg et al., 2005). In

one well-designed study, the Goldberg research team (2005) demonstrated that children

with ASD performed significantly worse than neurotypical peers on measures of planning

and visual working memory despite being matched on age, verbal and performance IQ

and social economic status (SES). These findings emphasize that the nature of the task

Page 33: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

22

needs to be considered in any study design and analysis, and that visual working memory

should be specifically targeted in studies with the ASD population.

Few studies have examined the relationship between components of cognitive

regulation and social competence of children with ASD. Both spatial working memory

(Gilotty et al., 2002; Landa & Godlberg, 2005) and planning (Happe, Booth, Charlton &

Hughes, 2006) have been significantly associated with social deficits in children with

ASD. Happe and colleagues (2006) found that planning and working memory were

related to social competence for children with ASD but not for those with ADHD or

neurotypical children. In contrast, Joseph and Tager-Flusberg (2004) found no

relationship between cognitive indices of EF and social deficits in ASD. Thus, while

there is some evidence to support the hypothesis that the cognitive domains of EF are

associated with social competence in ASD, most studies focused on social impairment

and thus further study is clearly required (Riggs et al., 2006b).

2.5.2 Behavioural regulation and social competence. Behavioural regulation

refers to various aspects of inhibitory control, including the ability to regulate activity

level, self-monitor and suppress automatic impulses (Hinnant & Obrien, 2007). Effective

social interactions often require the withholding of dominant responses (Rueda, Posner &

Rothbart, 2005), and inhibitory skills are central in suppressing salient thoughts and

behaviours in favour of those that are more socially appropriate (Channon & Watts, 2003;

Ciairano et al., 2007). Many studies have evaluated the association between inhibition

and social outcome in school-aged children, but most have focused on problematic

behaviours rather than social competence. Decreased inhibition has been associated with

problematic behaviours, such as aggression, non-cooperative behaviour, and

Page 34: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

23

noncompliance (Huyder et al., 2012; Thorell, Bohlin & Rydell, 2004; Vuontele et al.,

2012). The association between inhibition and aspects of social competence, such as

cooperative behaviour, has been documented in neurotypical children (Ciairano, Visu-

Petra & Settanni, 2007). Inhibition has also been found to predict concurrent and future

levels of social competence in neurotypical children (NICHD 2003; NICHD, 2009; Nigg

et al., 1999; Smith, 2010).

The evidence regarding inhibitory control in children with ASD remains

inconclusive, with some studies reporting deficits relative to control groups (Kenworthy

et al., 2009; Narzisi, Muratori, Calderoni, Fabbro, & Urgesi, 2013; Robinson et al., 2009;

van Rijn et al., 2013) and others reporting intact inhibition (Barron et al., 2014; Christ et

al., 2007; Hill & Bird, 2006 ). The inconsistency between studies is likely related to the

type of inhibition evaluated. Specifically, children with ASD typically perform as well as

neurotypical peers during tasks of neutral inhibition (e.g., push button for X and withhold

for Y), but perform significantly worse when required to inhibit an over-learned or

dominant response in favour of an unusual one (Hill, 2004; Homack & Riccio, 2004;

Russo et al. 2007; Sanders et al., 2008). Only a few studies have directly compared the

effect of modality on performance, and again, results are inconclusive. Sanderson &

Allen (2013) administered multiple inhibitory tasks to 31 six to eleven year old children

with ASD and a mental-age matched control group. They found intact performance on

tasks requiring the suppression of a habitual response, but impaired performance on tasks

that required both suppression of dominant response and the replacement with an

opposing response. In contrast, another series of studies found that children and

adolescents with ASD performed as well as a control group on both types of inhibition

Page 35: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

24

(Christ, Holt, White & Green, 2007; Christ, Kester, Bodner &Miles, 2011). It is possible

that the wider age range in these studies had an impact on performance. Thus while it is

clear that children with ASD do not demonstrate a global inhibitory impairment, there is

evidence that performance differs across tasks and thus using multiple inhibitory tasks

when examining inhibition in children with ASD is warranted. Furthermore, although

task complexity likely impacts performance, it is unknown whether using stimuli that

require visual or auditory responses makes a difference.

Several studies have examined the relationship between behavioural regulation

and social functioning of children with ASD. Inhibition was significantly associated with

adaptive socialisation (Happe et al., 2006), symptom severity (Lieb, 2011; Scheeren,

Koot & Begeer, 2012) and behaviour problems (Lieb, 2011). Happe and colleagues

(2006) found that inhibition was related to social competence for children with ASD but

not for those with ADHD or the neurotypical children. In one small study of 3 to 10 year

old children with ASD, parental ratings of inhibition were significantly associated with

affect regulation (Konstantareas & Stewart, 2006). That is, children with lower inhibitory

control had less adaptive affect regulation strategies. In contrast, one study found no

significant association between inhibition and social deficits as measured by the ADOS

(Joseph & Tager-Flusberg, 2004).

2.5.3 Emotion regulation and social competence. Emotion regulation is

considered a'hot' executive function (Kerr &Zelazo, 2004) in the neuropsychological

literature, where it is more specifically defined as affective or value-based decision-

making and measured through various gambling tasks or simulated high-risk situations

(Ardila, 2013). The whole premise of these tasks is that those who have better affective

Page 36: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

25

decision making are able to strategically adjust their decisions based on previous wins

and losses in highly motivating situations where there is some sort of personal gain to be

made (Blair, Zelazo & Greenberg, 2005; Faja, 2013; Hooper, 2004). It is the experience

of emotion that is tied to, or guides the decision making process (Buelow & Suhr, 2009).

Developmentally, successful performance thus requires that children forgo short-term

gains for long-term benefits and also involves the ability to adjust behaviour on the basis

of feedback cues (Cassotti et al., 2011), which helps children operate in emotionally and

motivationally charged situations (Carlson, Zayas & Guthormsen, 2009; Zelazo &

Carlson, 2012).

Emotion regulation was previously viewed as a separate functional dimension but

has recently been incorporated as part of the tripartite EF construct. Neuro-imaging

studies and neuropsychological research support the role of emotion regulation as a

domain of EF. Neuro-imaging and lesion studies have demonstrated that affective

decision making is associated with a distinct functional-anatomical network within the

ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Glascher et al., 2012; Mitchell, Rhodes, Pine & Blair,

2008), sharing neural underpinnings with both behavioural and cognitive regulation

(Dennis, 2010; Dennis, Malone & Chen, 2009; Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee &

Zelazo, 2005; Lewis et al., 2006). Behaviourally, several recent confirmatory factor

analyses of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF) (Gioia,

Isquith, Guy & Kenworthy, 2000) also demonstrated a distinction between cognitive,

behavioural and emotion regulation (Egeland & Fallmyr; 2010Gioia et al., 2002).

The ability to regulate affect has been demonstrated to play an important role in

the development of social competence, particularly peer relationships (Aldao, Nolen-

Page 37: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

26

Hoeksema & Schweizer Lewis, 2010;Blandon et al., 2010; Carthy et al., 2010;Eisenberg

et al., 1995, 1997; Gross & Thompson, 2007; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan 2000;

Lewis, Zinbarg & Durbin, 2010; Thompson, Lewis & Calkins, 2008;), but the

relationship between affective decision making and social competence has not been

explored. Affective decision making has been studied in relationship to some EF tasks

(da Mata et al., 2011; Toplak, Sorge, Benoit, West & Stanovich, 2010), but not in

relationship to social competence. A few studies have found a significant association

between affective decision making and symptom severity of ADHD (Geurts, van der

Oord & Crone, 2006; Groen, Gaastra, Evans & Tuche, 2013; Toplak, Sorge, Benoit, West

& Stanovich, 2010). The few studies that investigated affective decision making within

the ASD population have all reported no differences compared to a control group

(deMartino et al., 2008; Faja, 2013; Johnson & Yechiam, 2006; Russo, 2002; Sawa et al.,

2013; South, 2011; South et al., 2008; Yechiam et al., 2010). However, several studies

reported that those with ASD respond differently to the emotionally relevant stimuli

(deMartino et al., 2008; Johnson et al., 2006; South et al., 2011; Yechian et al., 2010). In

a study of 8 to 18 year old children with ASD, South and colleagues (2011) demonstrated

that those with ASD adjusted their behaviour based on losses (punishment) whereas the

control group adjusted their behaviour based on gains (rewards). In addition, performance

on affective decision making was significantly associated with parent-rating of

behavioural inhibition. No studies to date, however, have examined the association

between emotion regulation and social competence in children with ASD.

2.5.4 Parent-ratings of EF. In addition to performance-based measures, numerous

studies incorporate parent-ratings when evaluating EF in children. Parent-ratings of EF

Page 38: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

27

provide an alternate measure of EF as applied to everyday life, and are assumed to

provide 'real-world context' to EF measurement. In the most commonly-used measure –

the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function ((BRIEF: Gioia et al., 2002) –

parent ratings provide information on a range of skills related to metacognitive,

behavioural and emotion regulation skills, such as working memory, inhibition and

emotional control. All published studies using the BRIEF with the ASD population have

demonstrated significant EF deficits compared to neurotypical samples (Kalbfleisch

&Loughan, 2012; Kenworthy et al., 2008; 2009; 2010; Rosenthal et al., 2013; Troyb et

al., 2013), even for children and adolescents diagnosed with ASD before age five but

who no longer met DSM-IV criteria (see Troyb et al., 2013).

Parent-ratings of EF have, however, also been criticized for their lack of

correlations with performance-based measures. McAuley and colleagues (2010) reviewed

12 studies involving neurotypical children between ages 6 and 18 that compared

performance-based EF scores with the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function

(BRIEF), and found eight studies that reported some significant correlations between

isolated EF tasks and parent ratings of EF. Given the wide range of performance-based

EF measures used across these studies, the association between performance-based EF

measures and parent ratings of their child's EF remains inconclusive. More recently,

Toplak and colleagues (2013) reviewed 13 studies comparing performance-based and

parent-rated EF and reported that 35 out of a total of 182 correlations (19%) were

statistically significant. Despite these criticisms, researchers continue to use EF ratings in

research with children (Denckla, 2002; Isquith, Roth & Gioia, 2013; Rosenthal et al.,

2013; Toplak, West & Stanovich, 2013). It is important to recognize that performance-

Page 39: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

28

based and parent-rated measures of EF are not equivalent measures per se, but that they

are likely measuring parallel and distinct EF processes. Incorporating a parent measure of

EF provides relevant information regarding a broad range of EF in everyday context and

the application of EF in less structured and more complex contexts (Denckla, 2002;

Donders& Larsen, 2012; Isquith, Roth & Gioia, 2013).

2.5.5 Summary. There is substantial evidence that children with ASD have

deficits in EF related to cognitive regulation and behavioural regulation. Although

emotion regulation has been largely ignored in the ASD research, there is some reason to

believe that affective decision making may be impaired in children with ASD. While

there is some evidence that the three domains of EF are associated with aspects of social

competence in both ASD and neurotypical children, further study is clearly required

(Happe et al., 2006; Riggs et al., 2006b). Studying the antecedents of social competence

in ASD in the context of a comprehensive model that incorporates the tripartite model of

EF and SEL may help to further explain the nature of social dysfunction in ASD and lead

to more effective intervention strategies. The relationship between SEL and social

competence of children with ASD is explored in the next section.

2.6 Social Emotional Learning

Social emotional learning skills are generally accepted as key antecedents of social

competence and are often an important focus of intervention in ASD (Rao et al., 2008).

The McKown (2013) model identifies three domains (Nonverbal Awareness, Social

Meaning and Social Reasoning) relevant to SEL.

2.6.1 Nonverbal awareness. Nonverbal awareness generally refers to affect

recognition, namely the ability to interpret nonverbal cues that signal others’ emotions.

Page 40: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

29

Affect can be inferred via facial expressions, tone of voice and/or posture. Affect

recognition is considered a core skill that promotes the development of more complex

aspects of SEL (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000; Izard, 2001; Singh et al., 1998). In other

words, a child must be able to first accurately recognise facial affect in order to interpret

the mental state of others, to understand others’ behaviour and to appropriately respond.

Izard (1971) first highlighted the role of affect recognition in the development of

social competence in children, and that since has been demonstrated to be a significant

predictor of longer-term social competence (Frith & Baron-Cohen, 1987; Izard et al.,

2001,; Joseph & Tager-Flusberg, 2004; Semrud-Clikeman, 2007). In a recent meta-

analysis, Trentacosta and Fine (2010) found nonverbal awareness to be a significant

correlate of social competence in a wide range of neurotypical samples. Furthermore,

there is evidence to suggest that inferring affect through facial expressions, voices and

posture is significantly correlated (McKown et al., 2009; Trentacosta & Fine, 2010).

More importantly, an association between the ability to recognize complex emotions in

faces and voices separately, as well as to recognize complex emotions when facial, vocal

and contextual information is integrated, has been demonstrated. These findings suggest

that isolated affect recognition tasks offer an ecologically valid assessment of nonverbal

awareness.

Studies examining facial affect recognition abilities in children with ASD have

found that they perform more poorly than neurotypical children when tasks involve the

identification of more complex and intense emotions, such as pride and jealousy (Begeer

et al., 2008). There is less agreement regarding the ability of children with ASD to

recognize facial affect in regard to more simple and basic emotions, such as happiness,

Page 41: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

30

anger, sadness and fear. Some studies have found significant deficits when compared to

neurotyical peers (Bolte & Poustka, 2003; Mazefsky and Oswald, 2007; Rump,

Giovannelli, Minshew & Strauss, 2009; Semrud-Clikeman, Walkowiak, Wilkinson &

Butcher, 2010), whereas others have not (Adolf, Sears & Piven, 2001; Castelli, 2005;

Gross, 2004). This disparity may be due to the different levels of emotion intensity used

across studies, which is often not identified or examined. For example, it is possible that

children with ASD are able to identify more obvious (i.e., high intensity) but not more

subtle (i.e., low intensity) demonstrations of affect. One study that directly examined the

role of intensity found that children with ASD performed more poorly than neurotypical

children when the tasks involved identifying more subtle facial expressions (i.e., low

intensity) and tone of voice (Mazefsky & Oswald, 2007). Therefore, studies examining

affect recognition with the ASD population need to consider both the intensity and

complexity of the emotions being assessed.

Data on voice affect recognition of children with ASD is sparse and the findings are

mixed. Some report that children with ASD show deficits compared to typically

developing peers (Lindner &Rosen, 2006; Oerlemans et al., 2013; Peppe, McCann,

Gibbon, O'Hare & Rutherford, 2007), while others do not (Brooks & Ploog, 2013;

Mazefsky & Oswald, 2007; Paul, Augustyn, Klin & Volkmar, 2005). For example,

Mazefsky and Oswald (2007) found that children with HFASD performed significantly

worse than the standardization sample whereas children with Asperger's performed

within expected range. Paul et al. (2005) found that qualitatively adolescents with ASD

relied solely on rate of speech to match voice to affect whereas typically developing peers

Page 42: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

31

used both rate of speech and intonation to recognize emotion. These results were recently

replicated by Brooks and Ploog (2013) using a novel video game format.

Affect recognition has also been significantly associated with social competence

and social impairments in some studies (Braverman, Fein, Lucci, and Waterhouse, 1989;

Joseph and Tager-Flusberg, 2004) but not others (Wong, Beidel, Sarver & Sims, 2012).

For example, Wong and colleagues (2012) found that although 9 to 13 year old children

with ASD performed significantly worse than neurotypical children in terms of affect

recognition, their performance was not related to social competence.

2.6.2 Social meaning. Social meaning involves the understanding and

interpretation of others’ communication. It encompasses the ability to interpret others’

intentions (ToM), interpret the social meaning of language (pragmatic language) and

appropriately respond to others’ emotions (empathy) (McKown et al., 2009). Two aspects

of social meaning – ToM and pragmatic language – have been thoroughly studied in

children with ASD whereas empathy has been less examined with this population.

Theory of mind (ToM) is the ability to perceive others’ mental states, such as their

beliefs and intentions (Baron-Cohen, 2009; Mckown et al., 2009). The ability to

understand that other’s thoughts and beliefs guide their behaviour allows a person to

understand, predict or explain the behaviour of others (Baron-Cohen, 2009; Peterson,

Garnett, Kelly & Attwood, 2009, p. 106). These 'mindreading' skills are considered a

critical element for social competence because they allow a person to consider the

perspective of others, which is critical to social competence.

Numerous studies have demonstrated a developmental progression of these skills.

Mastery of less complex, or first order ToM tasks (Sally thinks friend A believes X)

Page 43: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

32

typically occurs around age five, and mastery of more complex, or second order ToM

tasks (Sally thinks friend A believes friend B believes X) by age eight (Miller, 2009).

Simple and complex ToM skills have been demonstrated to be significantly correlated

with social competence (Astington, 2003; Hughes & Leekam, 2004; Miller, 2009;

Peterson et al., 2009) and to be predictive of future social competence (Razza & Blair,

2009). There is consensus that the majority of children with ASD are able to pass simpler

ToM tasks (e.g., A believes X), but results are mixed regarding more advanced ToM

tasks (e.g., A believes B believes X). While most studies found a deficit compared to

neurotypical peers (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Kaland et al., 2008; Mortimore & Robertson,

1997; Nilsen & Fecica, 2011; Peterson et al., 2009; Wellman & Liu, 2004;), others did

not (Begeer, Malle, Nieuwland & Keysar, 2010; Peterson et al., 2009). It has been

demonstrated that when mastery does occur, it is usually at a much older age than

typically developing peers (Miller, 2009; Peterson, Garnett, Kelly & Attwood, 2009).

ToM has been the most studied SEL in relation to social competence for children

with ASD. For example, Peterson et al. (2009) demonstrated that children with ASD who

passed ToM tasks were more socially competent than those who failed them. Tager-

Flusberg (2003) found that ToM was significantly associated with socialization skills in 4

to14 year old children with ASD, findings consistent with other studies (Frith et al., 1994;

2003; 2009; Peterson et al., 2009; Razza et al., 2009; Tager-Flusberg, 2003). However,

several studies have failed to find a significant relationship between ToM and social

competence (Begeer et al., 2011; Bennett et al., 2013) in similar-aged children with ASD.

Empathy is the ability to respond to someone else’s emotions in an appropriate

manner (McKown et al., 2009; Baron-Cohen, 2009). It involves the ability to affectively

Page 44: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

33

take the perspective of someone else and to demonstrate an emotional response congruent

to their emotional state (Eisenberg, 2003). Two meta-analyses found a significant

correlation between empathy and social competence (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987;

Underwood & Moore, 1982) in neurotypical children and a recent longitudinal study of

social competence (Smart & Sanson, 2003) reported that empathy was the most stable

skill from early childhood to early adulthood. The significant association between social

competence and empathy thus has important treatment implications for high-risk

populations.

Poor empathy is a hallmark of ASD, but there are relatively few studies examining

empathy in children with ASD. The majority of studies have demonstrated that children

with ASD perform significantly worse than neurotypical peers across the age span

(Auyeung et al., 2009; Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004; Charman et al., 1997;

Wakabayashi et al., 2007; Yirmiya, Sigman, Kasari & Mundy, 1992), but there are some

exceptions (Charman, Sweetenham, Baron-Cohen, Cox, Baird & Drew, 1997; Mcdonald

& Messinger, 2012). For example, in one recent study where adolescents were asked to

look at vignettes of emotionally charged situations and rate how they would feel if they

were the actors, no difference between teens with ASD and neurotypical peers was found

(Jones et al., 2010). The inconsistent findings may be explained by several factors. First,

the modality used may impact results. In the Jones and colleagues (2010) study, the

adolescents were not required to identify the emotion but rather rate the intensity of a

designated emotion (e.g., "How guilty would you feel?"). Thus, while children with ASD

may be able to rate emotions in a similar way to neurotypical peers, they may have more

difficulty when asked to identify or respond to the observed emotions (e.g. Yirmiya et al.,

Page 45: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

34

1992). Second, the inconsistent findings might be related to the inclusion of children of

all developmental levels, as studies that include children with lower cognitive levels tend

to demonstrate impairment in empathy (Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse & Allen, 1998;

Scambler et al., 2007). Third, most ASD research focuses on cognitive empathy (the

ability to infer someone else's feelings), not affective empathy (having a congruent

response to others' emotions). Dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy has

been demonstrated in several studies. For example, impaired cognitive empathy but intact

affective empathy was demonstrated in children and adults with ASD using self-rated

questionnaires (Jones et al., 2010; Rogers et al., 2007), as well as a direct measure of

empathy (Dziodek et al., 2008; Schwenck et al., 2012). Given that cognitive empathy

likely parallels ToM, more information on the affective empathy of children with ASD is

needed. There has been some study on the relationship between affective empathy and

social competence in ASD, with mixed results; positive correlations between empathy

and quality of social interaction have been found in young children with ASD (Scambler,

Hepburn, Rutherford, Wehner & Rogers, 2007; Travis, Sigman & Ruskin, 2001) but not

in older school-aged children (Scheeren, Koot, Mundy, Mous & Begeer, 2013).

Pragmatic language refers to the use and interpretation of language for social

meaning (Adams, 2002). Pragmatic skills involve “being able to use social contextual

cues in order to understand a speaker’s meaning” (Coplan & Weekes, 2009 p. 240). They

include rules for social communication, such as turn taking, responding to statements

made by others, and maintaining a topic of conversation, as well as understanding

subtleties of language, such as sarcasm and idioms. Pragmatic skills are significantly

associated with social competence in preschool (Gertner, Rice, & Hadley, 1994; Mccabe

Page 46: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

35

& Meller, 2004) and school-aged children (Adams, Lloyd, Aldred & Baxendale, 2006;

Coplan & Weeks, 2009; McKown, 2007; Place & Becker, 1991; Russel & Grizzle, 2008).

While pragmatic language impairments are frequently cited in the ASD literature,

they have not been extensively examined in empirical research. For example, in a recent

review Loukusa and Miliken (2009) found only 20 studies on pragmatic language in the

past two decades. In these studies, there was agreement across age ranges that children

with ASD performed significantly worse than neurotypical peers on pragmatic language

tasks. Few studies have directly examined the relationship between the pragmatics of

language and social competence in the ASD population, and the studies that have been

done produced mixed findings (Landa & Goldberg, 2005; Leonard, Milich & Losch,

2011).

2.6.3 Social reasoning. Social reasoning refers to the ability to problem solve when

faced with social dilemmas or social provocations. It involves detecting a problem, and

generating appropriate and effective solutions. Social reasoning is based on Crick and

Dodge's (1994) Social Information Processing (SIP) model. SIP is a robust theory of

social competence that has had a tremendous influence on our understanding of the

manner in which social information is processed.

A plethora of research confirms that social problem-solving skills account for

significant variance in social functioning throughout the life span (e.g. Bauminger,

Edelsztein, & Morash, 2005; Ganesingham, Yeates, Sanson & Anderson, 2007b; Mayeux

& Cillessen, 2003; Sibley, Evans & Serpell, 2010; Tur-Kaspa, 2004). Social problem-

solving skills also account for significant variance in social functioning in different

populations, such as children with ADHD (Sibley et al., 2010), children with internalising

Page 47: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

36

disorders (Luebbe, Bell, Allwood, Swenson & Early 2010; Siu & Shek, 2010) and

typically developing children (McKown, 2007; McKown et al., 2009).

There is general consensus that children with ASD are significantly poorer at

social problem solving compared to neurotypical peers. A number of studies have

consistently demonstrated that children (Meyer et al., 2006; Ziv, Hadad & Khateeb,

2013), adolescents (Flood, Hare & Wallis, 2011; Channon, Sharman, Heap, Crawford &

Rios, 2001) and adults (Goddard, Howlin, Dritschel & Patel, 2007) with ASD perform

significantly worse than neurotypical peers. Two studies specifically examining social

problem solving skills in 7 to 13 year old children with ASD are particularly noteworthy.

Demopoulos et al. (2013) studied 436 children with ADHD and 137 children with ASD

with varying levels of intelligence, and found significant differences in social problem

solving between the children and adolescents with ASD compared to the children with

ADHD, and in comparison to normative sample data. In a series of studies by Channon et

al., (2001), children were presented with short videos of situations involving some type of

social problem, such as a neighbour's dog constantly barking and interfering with the

actor's ability to do homework and sleep. The children were then asked a series of

questions regarding what the actor should do. The researchers found that while children

with ASD could identify the problem and come up with as many solutions as their

neurotypical peers, they needed more prompts to do so, and the social appropriateness

and effectiveness of their responses was significantly poorer. These impairments were

found during both videotaped and story format vignettes.

Significant relationships have also been found between social problem solving

and prosocial behaviours (Flood, Hare & Wallis, 2011), social difficulties and anxiety

Page 48: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

37

(Meyer et al., 2006), as well as problem behaviours (Ziv, Hadad & Khatee, 2013) in

children with ASD.

Overall, there is certainly some clear evidence that children with ASD do more

poorly across the three domains of SEL (Nonverbal Awareness, Social Meaning, Social

Reasoning) compared to neurotypical peers. Furthermore, although less well studied,

there is reason to assert that the three domains of SEL contribute to social competence in

neurotypical children, and several studies have demonstrated that SEL skills are

significantly associated with some aspects of social functioning in children with ASD.

Most of these studies, however, examined SEL skills in relation to social impairment or

problematic behaviour, rather than social competence, the focus of the present study.

2.7 Heterogeneity in ASD

The clinical heterogeneity of children with ASD has been demonstrated

throughout the SEL and EF literature. Statistically, a high degree of variances of scores in

ASD have been reported on SEL skills (Wright et al., 2008; Yirmiya, Sigman, Kasari &

Mundy, 1992), EF measures (Happe et al., 2006; Kenworthy et al., 2009) and parent

ratings of EF (Boyd et al., 2009). Thus, in addition to overall deficient performance in EF

and SEL areas of functioning, there is a greater range of skills and deficits in children

with ASD than in their neurotypical counterparts. While comparisons with neurotypical

peers have provided robust and valuable information regarding specific areas of SEL and

EF in children with ASD, few studies have directly examined the wide variance in

performance or explored the relationship between these skills and social competence. In

short, even though a diagnosis of ASD denotes significant social impairments, there is

still a wide range of social competence amongst this population (Bruining et al., 2010;

Page 49: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

38

Happe et al., 2006; Hazlett et al., 2009; Pelphrey et al., 2005; Pelphrey et al., 2007).

Exploring SEL and EF in the same study allows the thorough examination of factors that

may help explain the heterogeneous nature of social competence in ASD.

2.8 Relationships Among SEL, EF and Social Competence

The relationship between EF and SEL skills in children with ASD is unclear. The

EF components of working memory and sustained attention have been significantly

associated with the SEL skill of affect recognition (Mathersul et al., 2009). The most

robust findings come from the ToM literature and demonstrate that the SEL ability to

understand others' beliefs is significantly associated with planning and inhibition

(Carlson & Moses, 2001; Hill, 2004; Hughes, 2002; Joseph & Tager-Flusberg, 2004;

Pellicano, 2010). It is also possible that the relationship between EF and ToM may be

stronger for the ASD population than for neurotypical children. For example, Ozonoff et

al., (1991) found significant positive correlations between EF and the SEL skill of ToM

for children with ASD, but notfor neurotypical children. While such findings offer

important information regarding the association between EF and SEL, they do not allow

for a comprehensive understanding of the relationship.

At present, there is limited knowledge of the mechanism by which EF and SEL

may relate to social competence. One idea on how EF could affect social competence

suggests that cognitive, behavioural, and emotion regulation could impact the detection,

discrimination and processing of the subtle, dynamic, and complex cues in the social

environment. Impaired EF could thus disrupt accurate processing of nonverbal cues,

social meaning, and social reasoning, and thereby compromise a child's ability to

appropriately navigate the social world. The argument could then be made that EF

Page 50: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

39

impacts social competence via its relationship with nonverbal awareness, social meaning,

and social reasoning. Support for this view comes from the ToM literature, which has

demonstrated that EF predicts ToM, but not vice versa (Channon et al., 2001; Hughes et

al., 1998; Hughes et al., 2004; Ozonoff et al., 1991). Longitudinal studies also provide

compelling evidence that the EF components of working memory, planning, and

inhibition play a critical role in the emergence of ToM in both the neurotypical and ASD

population (Flynn, 2007; Hughes et al., 2000; Pellicano, 2010; Pellicano, 2007; Tager-

Flusberg & Joseph, 2005).

Mediating factors in the relationship between EF and social competence have not

yet been examined in ASD, but have been studied in other pediatric populations. For

example, in the area of traumatic brain injury, studies by Yeates and colleagues found

that SEL skills and EF independently predict social competence (Yeates et al., 2004;

Yeates et al., 2007), and social problem solving and pragmatic language were found to

partially mediate the effects of EF on social competence in children with frontal lobe

injuries (Channon & Watts, 2003; Muscara et al., 2008). Thus, despite the complexities

of the relationship between EF, SEL and social competence, the premise that SEL skills

may mediate the influence of EF on social competence is worthy of further evaluation in

children with ASD. As reviewed earlier, tripartite models for EF and SEL represent the

best statistical and theoretical conceptualisations for such study (Ganesalingham et al.,

2006; Gioia et al., 2010; McKown et al., 2009).

2.9 Limitations of the current literature

Most previous studies focused on: (1) differences between children with ASD and

other diagnostic groups or neurotypical children; or (2) trying to explain social deficits in

Page 51: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

40

ASD. Very few studies have explored the relationship between SEL or EF and social

competence in children with ASD. Those that did typically targeted particular SEL or EF

skills rather than explore the impact of the range of skills on social competence. While

some significant associations have been demonstrated, the relationship between EF and

SEL in relation to social competence for the ASD population is mainly speculative at this

time. Further, the measurement of social competence is problematic in many previous

studies. First, many studies that claim to examine social competence are in fact really

looking at the association between SEL skills or amongst isolated components of an SEL

skill and EF. For example, McCabe & Meller (2004) used measures of SEL skills of

affect recognition and social problem solving to assess social competence. Second, the

majority of studies in this area use the social interaction domain score from either the

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) or Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale

(ADOS) to measure social functioning. This is problematic because the associations

between social symptoms or deficits and social ability or adaptive functioning in daily

life are weak (Klin et al., 2007). Furthermore, the ADI-R and ADOS focus on core DSM-

IV symptoms rather than on a range of everyday social behaviours in natural settings, and

therefore, information regarding a wide range of social behaviours and interactions is not

considered. For example, the ADOS relies on an in-clinic administered assessment rather

than on information obtained from more natural settings, and from those that involve peer

interactions. There is also reason to believe that the ADOS social domain scores

overestimate everyday social functioning in children with ASD. For example, in one

study, 17% of the ASD sample did not reach criteria for an ASD diagnosis on the ADOS

Page 52: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

41

but were included because of confirmation of social impairment through comprehensive

interview and expert opinion (Landa & Goldberg, 2005).

2.10 Integration and Aims of the Current Study

Social competence clearly plays an important role in overall quality of life in

ASD. While the previous literature has demonstrated that children with ASD have

specific deficits in SEL and EF compared to neurotypical children, few studies have

examined the relationship between these variables and social competence in ASD. There

is also a wide range of social competence in children with ASD that has not yet been

fully studied.

The recent tri-partite model of EF allows a much broader conceptualization of

cognitive, behavioural and emotion regulation than has previously been used in the ASD

literature. Expanding on the previous narrow approaches to EF in the ASD literature

allows for the study of multiple possible antecedents to social competence and provides a

potential practical basis for targeted interventions. A comprehensive model of SEL has

also yet to be used in the study of children with ASD. Incorporating the empirically

validated and robust domains of SEL from the McKown model with the tripartite model

of EF provides a more comprehensive approach to studying the social competence of

children with ASD. Concurrently examining multiple domains of SEL and EF will also

allow the assessment of important relationships between the constructs. A mediation

model whereby SEL and EF impact social competence in children with ASD can also be

taken into consideration. Further, prior studies have tended to rely on social impairment

or behaviour problems rather than social ability or competence. More comprehensive

measures of social competence, such as parent observations of peer relations, social skills

Page 53: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

42

and adaptive social behaviour are needed to better examine the relationships among SEL,

EF, and social competence (Ladd, 2005; Brown et al., 2008).

The overall aim of the current study is to thoroughly examine the relationship

between SEL and EF abilities of children with ASD in relation to social competence.

This study addresses many of the limitations of previous research in this area by

comprehensively examining SEL and EF using recent tripartite models, and by

conceptualizing and assessing social competence using a broad multidimensional

approach (see Figure 4). The goal is to contribute valuable information regarding

theoretical relationships among SEL, EF, and social competence in ASD, as well as

implications for targeted clinical intervention to enhance social competence in children

with ASD.

Page 54: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

43

Figure 4. The proposed relationship between social

competence, SEL and EF: a partial mediation pathway using

McKown and colleagues' 2009 three-factor model of SEL as a

framework

Page 55: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

44

3.0 Research Hypotheses

Hypothesis 1: Children with ASD will perform significantly different than neurotypical

children in specific domains of EF, SEL and social competence.

a. It is hypothesized that, overall, children with ASD will score significantly

lower than neurotypical children on all measures.

b. It is hypothesized that a greater percentage of children with ASD than

neurotypical children will score within the clinical impairment ranges on

measures of SEL, EF and social competence.

Hypothesis 2: Measures of Nonverbal Awareness, Social Meaning and Social

Understanding will represent latent SEL domains for the ASD and control

groups.

a. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Nonverbal Awareness domain

will comprise scores from tests of facial affect and voice affect recognition

b. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Social Meaning domain will

comprise scores from tests of ToM, pragmatic language, and empathy

c. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Social Reasoning domain will

comprise scores from tests of social problem solving.

Page 56: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

45

Hypothesis 3: The measures of Cognitive, Behavioural and Emotional regulation will

represent three latent EF domains for the ASD and control groups.

a. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Cognitive Regulation domain will

comprise scores from visual attention, visual working memory, visual

attention, auditory attention, and planning.

b. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Behavioural Regulation domain

will comprise scores from auditory inhibition reaction time, auditory

inhibition accuracy, and visual inhibition

c. It is hypothesized that the factor-derived Emotional Regulation domain

will comprise scores from affective problem solving.

Hypothesis 4: The proposed model of SEL and EF will account for significant variance in

social competence among neurotypical children and children with ASD.

a. It is hypothesized that SEL will be significantly correlated with social

competence, such that children scoring higher on measures of SEL will be

rated higher in social competence by parents.

b. It is hypothesized that EF will be significantly correlated with social

competence, such that children scoring higher on measures of EF will be

rated higher in social competence by parents.

Page 57: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

46

c. It is hypothesized that SEL and EF will independently predict significant

variance in social competence.

d. It is hypothesized that EF will be more strongly associated with social

competence for the children with ASD than neurotypical children.

Hypothesis 5: SEL will partially mediate the effect of EF on social competence.

a. It is hypothesized that SEL will be significantly correlated with EF.

b. It is hypothesized that the EF will have a significantly smaller effect on

social competence after controlling for SEL.

Page 58: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

47

4.0 Methods

4.1 Participant Recruitment

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Regina, the Regina

Qu'Appelle Health Region and Five Hills Research Ethics Boards (see Appendix A).

Children with ASD were recruited through three Saskatchewan-based Autism Programs:

The Autism Centre in Regina (Regina Qu’Appelle Health Region), Autism Spectrum

Disorders Program in Moose Jaw (Five Hills Health Region) and Autism Services of

Saskatoon. The coordinator at each program emailed the recruitment letter (see Appendix

B) to the parents of children with ASD registered in their program. The researcher also

attended ASD parent support meetings and parent psycho-educational groups at the

Autism Centre to give a brief presentation about the study. Potential participants were

provided the recruitment letter and information on how to contact the researcher. Posters

about the study were put up at the Autism Centre and contact information was available

at the reception desk. Finally, social media was used to recruit children. The moderator of

the Saskatchewan Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Facebook group

posted the recruitment flyer for the study on the group webpage.

Children for the control group were recruited through a variety of means in the

Regina area. These included posts on Facebook and emails to friends requesting that they

forward to anyone who may be interested; emails to University of Regina and Regina

Qu'Appelle Health Region employees; posters at pediatrician's offices, in community

centers and grocery stores. The researcher also handed out recruitment flyers to parents at

community soccer, baseball, and football games. The families who expressed interest in

Page 59: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

48

the study were provided written information about the study and the researcher's contact

information.

4.2 Data Collection

Prior to participating in the study, parents were provided an electronic copy of the

consent and assent forms to read (see Appendix C). Data collection occurred at the

University of Regina and University of Saskatchewan. Upon arrival, parents were

provided a written copy of the consent form and signatures were obtained. Parents of

children with ASD provided diagnostic reports or their consent to access health records to

confirm the diagnosis of ASD, which had previously been made by either psychiatrists or

psychologists based on DSM-IV-TR criteria (American Psychiatric Association 2000) as

part of the clinical care of the child. Parents of children in the control group provided

verbal confirmation that their son did not have any mental health diagnoses or learning

disabilities. Before beginning, the researcher reviewed the assent form with the child and

provided a Cineplex Cinema gift certificate worth $10.00. Children were then provided a

visual schedule of all tasks they would be doing and instructed that they could ask for a

break at any time. Individual testing sessions took approximately 100 to 130 minutes.

Participants chose from a basket of small rewards (e.g. Mario cards; Star Wars stickers)

at the end of the session.

Parents completed the demographic form and questionnaires while the researcher

was administering the measures to the child in an adjoining room. All testing was

completed individually in a room free from distractions. The administration of the tasks,

which was the same for all participants, was in counterbalanced order, alternating

between SEL and EF measures. While the children were doing a task on the computer

Page 60: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

49

and again at the end of the session, the researcher reviewed the parental forms to ensure

full completion.

4.3 Clinical File Review

As per RQHR ethics approval, the researcher reviewed 31 health records from the

RQHR to confirm diagnoses. Eighteen parents of children with ASD provided

psychological or psychiatric assessment reports as confirmation of an ASD diagnosis.

Intellectual assessment scores for 11 children with ASD were obtained from the RQHR

file review. These 11 children had completed the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for

Children (WISC-IV) within the past 3 years and their full scale IQ scores were used for

the analyses.

4.4 Participants

A sample of 99 boys, aged 8-13 years and their parent(s) participated in this

study. The clinical group consisted of 51 boys with ASD and the control group consisted

of 48 boys without mental health diagnoses or learning disabilities. Data from two

participants with ASD were not used in the analyses because to the researcher they

seemed to demonstrate inadequate comprehension of the instructions for the tasks. In

terms of ASD diagnoses, seven children had a diagnosis of autistic disorder, 22 had a

diagnosis of Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified and 20 had a

diagnosis of Asperger’s Disorder. The children with ASD had received their diagnosis

between 2.5 and 10 years of age (M = 6.12, SD = 1.97).

Table 1 provides demographic information on the children and parents in this study. As

intended, the two groups did not significantly differ in terms of age, F(1, 96) = 1.10, p =

.30, family income, F(1, 96) = 3.21, p = .076, or parent education level, F(1, 96) = 3.73, p

Page 61: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

50

= .06. Parents in both groups reported high family incomes (77% percent of participants

had annual family incomes over $75 000) and high levels of education (86% of parents

had completed post-secondary education). Children in the control group had slightly

higher full scale IQ scores (m= 110.81, SD=10.00) than the children in the ASD group

(m= 105.69, SD=13.77), F(1, 96) = 4.37, p = .04. None of the children in the control

group were taking any stimulant medications or other psychopharmacological

interventions and 18 boys (36.7%) from the ASD group were on stimulant medication

specifically targeting attention problems, including Methylphenidate (Concerta),

Lisdexamfetamine (Biphentin), Dimesylate (Vyvanse) and Atomoxetine (Strattera).

4.5 Child Measures

Children were administered a measure of intellectual functioning to estimate IQ, 6

SEL tasks, and 8 EF tasks. A complete list of measures and can be found in Appendix E.

4.5.1 Intellectual ability. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

(WASI) (Wechsler, 1999) was used as a brief estimate of intellectual ability. The WASI

is part of a wide series of individually-administered Wechsler standardized instruments

designed to assess general intelligence, IQ. The WASI has a wide age range (6-89 years)

and is commonly used to estimate general intelligence in studies involving child clinical

populations (Blunden, Lushington, Lorenzen, Martin & Kennedy, 2005; McClure et al.,

2005; Raggio, Scattone & May, 2010), including ASD (Barnea-Goraly et al., 2004;

Fuentes, Mostofsky & Bastian, 2010; Garcia-Nonell et al., 2008). The WASI uses a

measure of Verbal Comprehension (Vocabulary) and Perceptual Reasoning (Matrix

Reasoning) to provide an estimate of full scale IQ (FSIQ). The construct validity of the

WASI has been demonstrated through various convergent validity coefficients (r= 0.85 to

Page 62: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

51

Table 1.

Demographic Variables for ASD and Control Group

ASD

Mean (SD)

CONTROL

Mean(SD)

F

p

N

49

48

Age (yrs.)

Range

10.0 (1.60)

8-13

10.31(1.69)

8-12

.36 .73

IQ

Range

105.69(13.77)

83-140

110.81(10.00)

90-131

4.37* .04

Education(yrs.)

Range

15.27(2.19)

8-16

16.13(2.20)

8-22

3.73 .06

Income

Range

105 549(43 068)

<25 000-

>150 000

120 000(36 495)

67 500-

>150 000

3.21 0.08

*indicates significance at p<.05

Page 63: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

52

0.89) (Canivez & Konold, 2009; Hays, Reas & Shaw, 2002) and exploratory and

confirmatory factor analyses (Canivez & Konold, 2009). Correlations between the WASI

estimated IQ score and the WISC-IV FS IQ is .83 (Saklofske, Prifitera, Rolphus, Zhu &.

Weiss, 2005).

WASI subtests are scaled in T score units (M =50, SD = 10), and the IQ scores are

scaled in traditional IQ - standard score units (M = 100, SD =15). The WASI Vocabulary

and Matrix Reasoning subtests were administered to all children in the control group and

to those children in the ASD group without a prior IQ score. Standard administration and

scoring were conducted according to instructions in the manual.

4.5.2 Social Emotional Learning. SEL was assessed using both structured and

semi-structured tasks. SEL domains that were assessed included Nonverbal Awareness,

Social Meaning, and Social Reasoning.

Tasks of facial and voice affect recognition were used to evaluate Non-Verbal

Awareness. The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2 (DANVA – 2; Nowicki &

Duke, 1994) was used to measure nonverbal awareness. The DANVA-2 measures the

ability to identify nonverbal cues regarding the emotional states of others. Reliability and

validity data for these measures suggests it is an appropriate measure of facial affect and

voice affect recognition for 8 to 13 year old children (Nowicki 1997; Nowicki & Carlton,

1993; Nowicki & Duke, 1994; Nowicki & Mitchell, 1998). In a trend analysis of 8

studies, accuracy scores were shown to increase with age (Nowicki and Rowe, 1997) and

social competence (McKown et al., 2009).

In this study, children were asked to identify whether faces or voices were happy,

angry, sad or worried. In the Child Faces condition, participants viewed 24 photographs

Page 64: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

53

of children between the ages of 7 and 12 on the computer. The pictures included 12

female and 12 male expressions showing an equal number of happy, sad, angry and

fearful faces of high and low intensity. In the Child Voices condition, the participants

made judgments about the emotions of others from audio recordings of children repeating

the same sentence ("I’m going out of the room now but I’ll be back later"). The

recordings included 12 male and 12 female voices with equal amounts of happy, sad,

angry and fearful tone of voice of high and low intensity. The four emotions were printed

on a piece of paper to help participants remember the choices. The number of correct

choices and errors was tallied for each test. Raw scores were used for most analyses but

standard scores were also derived from a table of age norms.

Tasks of ToM, empathy, and pragmatic language were used to evaluate Social

Meaning. The Strange Stories task (Happe, 1994; White, Hill, Happé, & Frith, 2009) was

used to assess ToM. The Strange Stories were originally designed for children and

adolescents with ASD and has become one of the most widely used ToM tasks in

published research (Miller, 2009). The Strange Stories task is considered a second-order

reasoning task designed to assess the ability to understand mental states such as belief,

intention and deception, as well as the ability to understand that others have their own

thoughts and ideas which may be different from their own. It consists of a series of brief

vignettes in which participants have to accurately identify the underlying intention behind

a character's statement. Normative data for the Strange Stories is based on typically

developing 5-12 year old children (O'Hare et al., 2009). Strong validity, inter-rater and

test-retest reliability have consistently been reported across studies (Hughes et al., 2000;

Kaland et al., 2002; Kaland et al., 2005; Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007; O'Hare et al.,

Page 65: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

54

2009). Performance on Strange Stories has been significantly correlated with age

(McKown et al., 2009) but not IQ (Happe, 1994; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999; Kaland

et al., 2008). Five vignettes from Happe’s (1994) set were used in this study. All

participants were provided a written copy and also read the scenario and then asked two

questions. The first question checked for comprehension ("was it true, what X said?") and

the second question asked for justification ("Why did X say/do that"?), which requires

knowledge of another individual’s point of view to answer correctly. Scoring is based on

the participants' response to the mental state question, which can be scored as 0

(incorrect), 1 (partial) or 2 (full and accurate, which involves thoughts, feelings, desires).

Total number of correct items was used as the raw score.

The Bryant Empathy Index (BEI) was used to assess affective empathy. The BEI is

a widely used seven-item self-report questionnaire that measures a child's empathy-

related responses. Consistent with previous studies (Krevans and Gibbs, 1996; Michalska

et al., 2013; Robinson & Strayer 2007; and Zoll & Enns, 2005), questions tapping attitude

were eliminated and the seven questions measuring affective empathy from the BEI were

used in the current study. Participants rated to what extent they would feel a given

emotion based on various situations, such as "It makes me sad to see a boy who can’t find

anyone to play with" and "I really like to watch people open presents, even when I don’t

get a present". The affective portion of the BEI has good internal consistency (Aristu et

al., 2008, del Barrio et al., 2004 & de Wied et al., 2007) and demonstrates strong test–

retest reliability for 8 -15 year old children (Bryant,1982; de Wied et al., 2007; Krevans

& Gibbs, 1996). Concurrent validity has been demonstrated through significant

correlations with the Empathy Continuum (deWied et al, 2005; Cohen & Strayer, 1996;

Page 66: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

55

Fraser, 1996; Krevans & Gibbs, 1996; Strayer, 1993) and with the Eisenberg self-report

questionnaire (Krevans & Gibbs, 1996). In this study, the seven questions were read to

the children and they were also provided a written copy. All children understood the

procedure, as indicated by their responses to trial items such as "I like ice

cream/chocolate" and "I like soap in my eyes" (taken from Strayer & Roberts, 1989). A

4-point visual rating system was used in which participants chose whether the statement

was 'not at all true' (0), 'a little true' (1), 'pretty much true' (2) or 'very much true' (3). The

parents were also asked to complete this questionnaire based on their observations of

their sons' behaviours in order to examine whether participants' self-ratings of empathy

corresponded to parental ratings. The sum of points was used as the total raw score.

Pragmatic language was assessed using the Test of Pragmatic Language – Second

Edition (TOPL-2; Phelps-Terasaki and Phelps-Gunn, 2007). The TOPL-2 is a 43-item

norm-referenced oral language assessment for children aged 6 to 18. It is a well-designed

assessment tool that provides a comprehensive evaluation of pragmatic language and

allows the evaluation of an individual's ability to view a social situation and make

judgments from the vantage point of an objective bystander (Hoffman et al., 2013). It

assesses seven underlying areas of pragmatic language, including Physical Context,

Audience, Topic, Purpose, Visual-Gestural Cues, Abstractions, and Pragmatic

Evaluation. The TOPL-2 used a standardization sample of 1136 children between the age

of 6 and 18. Good content, criterion and construct validity were demonstrated in the

development of the TOPL-2. Concurrent and discriminant validity was also established in

recent independent studies (Hoffman & Martens, 2013; Volden & Philips, 2010). The

TOPL-2 was also demonstrated to predict outcomes beyond the contribution of general

Page 67: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

56

language skills and nonverbal IQ (Volden & Philips, 2010). In this study, the 18

questions from the Pragmatic Evaluation component were administered to all participants

in order to reduce redundancy and overlap with other measures of SEL, such as facial-

affect recognition (visual-gestural cues), and ToM (abstractions). The Pragmatic

Evaluation questions measure the awareness of rules of language in relation to specific

social situations, while considering the situational context and intent of the

communication (Phelps-Terasaki & Phelps-Gunn, 2007). Participants were shown

pictures of common social situations, read a short vignette, and then asked to generate a

response for one of the characters in the picture. One example depicts a boy holding

broken roller blades whose parents are telling him they do not want to buy him new ones

because he didn't take care of his old ones. The child is first asked, “What can Chad say

to talk his parents into getting him new rollerblades?" and subsequently "how do you

know what he says will talk them into it?". The child’s response was scored as correct (1)

or incorrect (0) according to criteria provided in the TOPL-2 manual. The sum of the

points for the 18 questions was used as raw score.

The Test of Social Problem Solving (TOPS-3E: Bowers, Huisingh & LoGiudice,

2005) was used to evaluate Social Reasoning.The TOPS-3E is a standardized measure of

social reasoning composed of 18 photographs that depict different social situations.

Children are asked to interpret information about social situations by responding to

questions based on six areas of social reasoning. These include defining problems,

identifying the causes of social situations, predicting outcomes, understanding social

conventions, and generating solutions to social problems. For example, for one picture of

a group of children sitting on the lawn, participants are told "this group was coming home

Page 68: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

57

from their fieldtrip", then asked a series of questions, such as "how can you tell

something went wrong on their way home" and also "they've been waiting a long time,

why hasn't the school sent another bus to pick them up".

The TOPS-3E has a normative sample of over 1000 six to twelve year-old

children. Strong validity, test–retest reliability and inter-rater reliability for the

elementary edition have been demonstrated (Bowers et al. 2005; Zachman, Huisingh,

Barrett, Orman & LoGiudice, 1994). Strong correlations with a social information

processing interview were also reported (McGee et al., 2009). The TOPS-3E has been

used in several studies with children with ASD (Baghdadli, Binot-Dubois, Pinot &

Michelon, 2010; Stichter, O’Connor, Herzog, Lierheimer & McGhee, 2012; Stichter,

O'Connor, Hertzog, Lierheimer & McGhee, 2012) as well as children with FASD

(McGee, Bjorkquist, Price, Mattson & Riley, 2009), with both clinical groups performing

significantly worse on the TOPS-3E than neurotypical children (Baghdadli et al., 2010;

McGhee et al., 2009). Participants in this study were administered an abbreviated version

of the TOPS-3E which included answering 50 questions based on 9 pictures targeting all

6 areas of social reasoning. Responses were scored on a 3-point scale (0-2) according to

criteria provided in the TOPS-3E manual. Total points were tallied and used as raw the

score.

4.5.3 Executive function. EF was assessed using structured performance-based

tasks and parent ratings. The performance-based EF tasks were chosen on the basis of

their reported sensitivity to EF in children within the 7-12 year old range, which

corresponded well with the age range of the study sample. The measures that were used

Page 69: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

58

assessed a theoretically meaningful range of executive functions. The EF domains that

were assessed included measures of Cognitive, Behavioural and Emotion Regulation.

Measures of attention, planning and working memory were used to evaluate

Cognitive Regulation. The Auditory Attention subtest of the NEPSY-II (Korkman, Kirk

& Kemp, 2007) was used to assess auditory selective and sustained attention. The

standardization sample of the NEPSY-II included 1200 children between the ages of 3

and 16. Strong psychometric properties for reliability and validity were reported for the 8

to 13 year-old range. Construct validity was obtained by various means, including expert

review, extensive literature reviews, concurrent validity and empirical clinical

evaluations with clinical populations. For this task, children are required to point to a

colored circle each time they hear a target word in a series of random words on an audio-

recording. Accuracy scores were calculated by summing total items correct on the target

words, with a maximum score of 30.

The Tasks of Executive Control (TEC: Isquith, Roth, & Gioia, 2010) is computer-

administered measure that was used to assess visual attention and visual working

memory. The visual attention task is a simple choice response task. Participants are asked

to sort all zebras appearing in the center of the screen into a red box, while non-zebra

objects are to be sorted into the blue box. Sorting is done by pressing the relevant shift

key, identified with a red or blue dot, with the corresponding right or left hand.

Participants were not allowed to proceed with the task until they demonstrated adequate

understanding of the task (defined as correctly completing the 10 practice items for each

condition). Participants performed 100 trials per each task. The working memory task

uses an n-back paradigm – a well-established measure of working memory (Owen,

Page 70: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

59

McMillan, Laird, & Bullmore, 2005). Participants are required to monitor a series of

visual stimuli and to respond whenever a stimulus is presented that is the same as the one

presented in a previous trial from n-steps back in the sequence. Two levels of working

memory demand were presented (1, 2-back). In the first working memory task,

participants were asked to place an object in the red box if it appeared twice in a row (1-

back). In the second working memory task, participants were asked to place objects in the

red box if it appeared 2 objects back in the sequence (2-back).

Psychometric evaluation of the TEC has been primarily conducted through the

standardization sample of 1138 children aged 5 to 18 years old, although some recent

studies have demonstrated convergent validity with other measures of EF and in clinical

populations, including ADHD (Gomez-Guerrero, 2011; Mairena et al., 2012) and mild

TBI in children (Isquith, Roth & Gioia, 2010; Krivizty et al., 201; Roebuck-Spencer,

Roth, Blackstone, Johnson & Gioia, 2011; Wolfe et al., 2013). The computerized scoring

produces accuracy scores for the two tasks. Visual attention has a maximum score of 100

and working memory tasks have a combined maximum score of 40.

The Tower of London Drexel University – Second Edition (TOLDX

-2: Culbertson

& Zimmer) was used as a measure of planning ability. This task is modeled after

Shallice’s (1982) Tower of London. In this modified version, children are asked to

rearrange three different colored beads situated on three vertical pegs of descending

height, in order to replicate a pattern on the examiner's peg board. Participants were

instructed to replicate the examiner's bead configuration in as few moves as possible

without violating two rules (moving only one ball at a time directly from one peg to

another; and not putting more beads on a peg than it will accommodate). There are 10

Page 71: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

60

trials, which increase in difficulty from a minimum of three to seven moves. The validity

of the Tower task has been demonstrated in numerous neuropsychological studies

conducted during the past few decades (Kaller, Unterrainer & Stahl, 2012; Swanson,

2005). The ToLDX

-2 standardization sample consisted of 990 7 to 80 year olds, of which

370 were between the ages of 8 and 12. Criterion, convergent and divergent validity have

been established, in addition to moderate to high test-retest reliability. Numerous

published studies have demonstrated that the ToLDX

-2 is sensitive to executive-function

deficits in clinical populations, including children with ADHD (Culbertson & Zillmer,

1998, 2005), pediatric traumatic brain injury (Donders & Larsen, 2012) and ASD

(Wallace, Silvers, Martin & Kenworthy, 2009). The children's version (7-15 years old) of

the ToLDX

-2 was administered to all participants. The raw 'total moves' score was used

for analysis..

Behavioural regulation was measured using two inhibition measures, one from the

TEC and one from the NEPSY-II. The TEC measure is a visual inhibition task that uses

a go/no-go paradigm, in which a child must execute a response to visual stimulus 'A' but

withhold a response to visual stimulus 'B'. It involves a continuously presented series of

visual stimuli composed of frequent “go” cues to which participants respond as rapidly

as possible and infrequent “no-go” cues to which participants are not to respond. The

higher frequency of go cues creates a dominant tendency to respond that must then be

inhibited for no-go cues (Schulz et al., 2007). The computerized scoring is based on

accuracy of targets correct, with a maximum of 20. The NEPSY-II measure is an

auditory inhibition task that assesses the ability to inhibit automatic responses in favour

of novel ones. The task is a variation of the Stroop Color and Word Test (Homack &

Page 72: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

61

Riccio 2004; Stroop, 1935). There are two components to this task. For the naming task,

participants are asked to name a series of shapes and direction of arrows (up and down).

In the inhibition component, participants are asked to name the opposite shape (e.g. when

see circle say square) or direction (when the arrow is up say down). The reliability and

validity of this task is well-established for the 7-12 year old range (Korkman et al., 2007).

Scores are based on completion time and errors (a combination of self-corrected and

uncorrected errors).

The Hungry Donkey Task (HDT: Crone & van der Molen, 2004) was used to

assess Emotional Regulation; it is an affective decision making measure based on the

Iowa Gambling Task (IGT: Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994), a well-

established and widely-used adult measure. In the HDT, participants are asked to ‘‘feed’’

as many apples as possible to the hungry donkey by selecting from either two or four

doors, which have different proportions of gains and losses of apples. The premise is that

the tasks resemble real-life decisions in terms of reward, punishment, and uncertainty of

outcomes (Crone, Bunge, Latenstein, & van der Molen, 2005). There are three versions

of the Hungry Donkey task. Task complexity and punishment frequency are manipulated

in each version by using two or four doors and a 10% or 50% punishment schedule

(Crone et al., 2005). For this study, the two choice - 10% condition was chosen because

the two-choice version reduces demand on working memory (Kerr and Zelazo, 2004) and

developmental changes in decision making are only apparent when the loss is infrequent

(Crone et al., 2005).

Discriminant validity for the HDT has been demonstrated in several studies

(Crone, Vendel & van der Molen, 2003; Geurts et al., 2006; Hooper, Luciana, Conklin &

Page 73: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

62

Yarger, 2004; van Duijvenvoorde, Jansen, Visser & Huizenga, 2010). Age was positively

associated with making more advantageous choices (Crone et al., 2004, 2005, 2007;

Huizenga, Crone & Jansen, 2007), indicating HDT performance is sensitive to

developmental changes in childhood and adolescence. Furthermore, individual

differences in 8-12 year old children’s affective decision making could be detected at the

neural level (P300) (Carlson, Zayas & Guthormsen, 2009) and physiological level (Crone

et al., 2005) when losses incurred.

For this study, participants had to choose between two identical decks of cards,

behind which unpredictable losses of 10 or 50 apples were presented on 10% of the trials.

All pictures presented for this task were obtained from Inquisit Millisecond software. On

the side facing down, door A has either a picture of 2 apples or a picture of 10 apples

with an X through them while door B has either a picture of 4 apples or a picture of 50

apples with an X though them. Immediately after indicating their response and turning

over a card, the examiner either placed beads in or took them out of a glass jar in

correspondence to the card selection. This provided a visual stimulus for the gains and

losses of apples. In addition, prior to beginning the task, participants were shown a bin of

high-desire prizes and told ‘‘if at the very end of the game, you have won more apples

than you have lost, then you can chose a prize from this toy bin" in an effort to increase

their motivation and personal desire to win (as per Crone & van der Molen, 2004). In

reality, all participants were invited to select a prize at the end.

The HDT consisted of 100 trials. Door A is advantageous in the long run because

it results in smaller immediate gain and smaller unpredictable losses whereas Door B

results in high immediate rewards but also much higher unpredictable losses. Net

Page 74: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

63

difference scores were calculated by subtracting the number of disadvantageous choices

(Door B) from number of advantageous choices (Door A) (Bechara et al., 1994; Crone &

van der Molen, 2004; Skogli, Egeland, Anderson, Hovik & Oie, 2013).

4.6 Parent Measures of EF and social competence

Parents completed a questionnaire package that included ratings of their child’s

EF and social competence; demographic data was also obtained from the parents at this

time (demographic questionnaire is in Appendix F).

4.6.1 Ratings of EF. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions

(BRIEF: Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000) was used to obtain parent ratings of

children's EF. The BRIEF is a psychometrically sound and well-established standardized

parent report inventory of executive functioning for 5 to 18 year olds (Baron, 2000;

Strauss, Sherman & Spreen, 2006). The BRIEF is often used as a complement to

traditional performance-based measures of executive function to provide information

regarding everyday application of executive functions. It is sensitive to a broad range of

neurologic and developmental conditions such as ASD (Gilotty et al., 2002; Kenworthy

et al., 2008), ADHD (Reddy, Hale, & Brodzinsky, 2011; Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, &

Tannock, 2009) and TBI (Mangeot, Armstrong, Colvin, Yeates & Taylor, 2002). The

standardized Parent Report version comprises 86 items that tap a wide range of executive

functions involved in the regulation of attention, behavior and emotion.

The BRIEF was normed on a sample of 2139 children between the ages of 5-18,

of which 1191 were 8 to 12 year olds (Gioia et al., 2000). Reliability was demonstrated

through internal consistency, interrater reliability and test-retest reliability. Validity for

the BRIEF was demonstrated though content and construct validity. The BRIEF was

Page 75: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

64

originally produced with eight subscales and two indices (Metacognition and Behavioral

Regulation). However, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses based on both parent

and teacher report subsequently identified nine subscales and three indices: Cognitive,

Behavioural and Emotional Regulation (Egeland & Fallmyr, 2010; Gioia, Isquith,

Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002).

The BRIEF is the most widely-used parent EF rating for clinical use and research

(Donders, DenBraber & Vos, 2010; Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2013) and it has

demonstrated its reliability and validity as a measure of everyday executive function

(Huizinga and Smidts, 2013; Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony, & Wallace, 2008; Mahone et

al., 2002; Mangeot et al., 2002; Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2009). Of note,

significant age effects have been reported, indicating that parental ratings of executive

functions improved with age throughout childhood on all scales (Huizinga and Smidts,

2013). The BRIEF has also been significantly correlated to measures of adaptive

functioning in a small group of children (n=53) with ASD (Gilotty et al., 2002) and

predicted severity level of injury and adaptive and problem behaviours in a group of 189

children with TBI (Mangeot et al., 2002).

Parents rated the frequency of their child's behaviour on 3-point scale (never;

sometimes; often), with higher scores indicating more difficulties with executive

functioning. Questions include "underestimates time needed to finish tasks", "has trouble

thinking of different way to solve a problem when stuck", "needs to be told to begin a

task even when willing" and "has explosive angry outbursts". The nine subscales of the

BRIEF were calculated in this study (Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize,

Organization of Materials, Task Monitor, Self Monitor, Inhibit, Shift and Emotional

Page 76: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

65

Control) according to procedures described in the literature (Egeland & Fallmyr, 2010;

Gioia et al., 2002). Scores for the total Global Executive Composite (GEC) were

calculated by summing all points according to procedures in the manual.

4.6.2 Ratings of social competence. Social competence was measured using the

Social Skills Improvement Rating System (SSIS 2008: Elliott & Gresham) and the

Socialisation Domain of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (VABS-

2: Sparrow, Cicchetti & Balla, 2005). The SSIS is the most widely-used measure of

social skills in children (White et al., 2007) and the VABS-2 is the most widely-used

adaptive measure in ASD research (Lopata et al., 2013).

Parent ratings of the SSIS were used to measure functional pro-social behaviours.

The SSIS parent questionnaire includes 46-items divided into seven subscales targeting

communication, cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy, engagement and self-

control. Examples of questions include "invites others to join in activities", "makes eye

contact when talking " and "stays calm when teased". The US national standardization

sample of the SSIS was large (n = 4,550) across 3 broad age groupings (3-5 years, 5-12

years, and 13-18 years). Validity has been demonstrated by moderate to high correlations

with other widely used instruments such as the Behavioral Assessment System (BASC–2;

Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) and the SSRS (Gresham & Elliott, 1990). Internal

consistency and test–retest reliability for parent ratings was strong (Elliot and Gresham,

2008; Gresham et al., 2010). Parents indicated the frequency with which their child

exhibits each social skill on a 4-point scale of never, seldom, often, and almost always. A

total score was derived from summing points from 7 subscales according to standardized

procedures.

Page 77: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

66

Parent ratings on the Socialization domain of the VABS-2 was used as the second

measure of social competence. The VABS-2 Socialisation Domain is a commonly used

measure of social competence in clinical populations, particularly ASD (Gillespie-Lynch

et al., 2012). It measures a range of personal and interaction skills needed for everyday

adaptive social behaviour and independence in three areas: interpersonal relationships

(how child interacts with others), play (how child uses toys and leisure time) and coping

skills (how child demonstrates sensitivity to others and manages social challenges).

Examples of questions include "invites friends over", "takes turns without being asked"

and "acts when another person needs a helping hand (for example, holds door open, picks

up dropped items". The VABS-II was standardized using a representative American

sample of 3,695 children (Sparrow, Cicchetti & Balla, 2005). Concurrent and divergent

validity and test-retest and inter-rater reliability of the Socialization domain are well-

documented in the manual and independent studies (Anderson, Oti, Lord & Welch, 2009;

Perry et al., 2009). Parents indicated the frequency with which their child performs social

behaviours on a 3-point scale of never, sometimes or partially, and usually. Scores were

derived from summing points on the three subscales according to procedures in the

manual.

Page 78: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

67

5.0 Results

5.1 Data Analysis Plan

All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS 21.0. Preliminary data screening

was performed to examine basic assumptions of parametric data. Outliers were examined

using Boxplots and assumptions of normal distribution were assessed via group-based

histograms and Q-Q Plots. Homogeneity of variance was assessed via Levene’s statistic.

There was no missing data and for all analyses p < .05 was considered statistically

significant.

One outlier was identified from the parent ratings of the ASD group and

subsequently removed from all analyses involving social competence scores or parent EF

ratings. This outlier was discarded because the SSIS and Vineland scores were over three

standard deviations higher than the mean of the ASD group and it was having significant

impact on the results of the correlation and regression analyses. However, since the

adjoining child-based SEL and EF scores for this participant were not outliers and all

analyses subsequently performed with and without this participant's EF and SEL scores

found no significant differences in outcome, the child data was retained for group

comparisons.

In order to compare performances across scales and age ranges, two scores were

calculated. First, scores were transformed to an age-corrected z-score because

standardized scores were not available for all tasks. Since children's performance on

many skill-based measures of EF and SEL normatively improves with age

(Ganesalingham et al., 2006; McKown et al, 2009; McKown et al., 2013; White et al.,

Page 79: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

68

2010), the same raw score has a different meaning for children at different ages. Thus, to

minimize the impact of age, the scores on the SEL and EF tasks were converted to an

age-corrected standard score. To do so, each score was regressed separately on age and

then the residual score, which indicates the individual variance not explained by age, was

used to calculate z-scores based on the control group's mean and standard deviation.

These z-scores were then used to examine group differences. Second, in order to

investigate levels of clinical impairment, the percentage of children who scored one

standard deviation or more below the normative mean was calculated (McKown, 2007;

Rasmussen et al., 2013) for parent and child measures when standardized norms were

available.

The results are divided into six sections. In the first section, descriptive analyses

are provided. The five sections that follow are divided according to the main research

hypotheses. These include group comparisons on all measures; creation of SEL and EF

domains and calculation of composite scores; correlations and regressions of SEL and EF

domains with Social Competence to determine the extent to which SEL or EF predicts

social competence; and finally, further correlational analyses between SEL and EF to

evaluate the hypothesized partial mediation relationship.

5.2 Descriptive Information

Since 37% of children from the ASD group were taking stimulant medications,

analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine whether there were any

significant within-group differences on predictor and outcome measures between those

children taking medication and those who were not. There were no significant within-

group differences between the children taking medication and those not taking

Page 80: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

69

medications on any child measures, but there were significant differences on the SSIS,

F(1, 47) = 11.40, p < .01 but not on the Vineland, F(1, 47) = 3.12, p = .08, or the BRIEF,

F(1, 47) = 3.84, p = .06. On further analysis, within-group differences on the SSIS were

only significant for 8 to 10 year old children, F(1, 23) = 10.37, p < .01, but not the 11 to

13 year olds, F(1, 22) = 4.14, p = .06.

5.2.1 Correlation analysis. Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients

were calculated to examine the relationship between age, IQ and all predictor and

outcome measures. The analyses were conducted separately for the control and ASD

groups (see Tables 2-5).

Age. Preliminary analysis examined the relationship between age and raw scores

on each measure. For the control group, age was positively correlated with 4 of 6 SEL

and 7 of 8 EF measures but not with parental EF ratings. In terms of SEL, performance

increased with age on facial affect recognition, r(48) = .37 , p =.01, voice affect

recognition, r(48) = .37, p <.01, pragmatic language, r(48) = .59, p < .01 and problem

solving, r(48) = .396, p < .01. In terms of EF, performance improved with age on all EF

measures except for affective decision making. Specifically, there were significant

correlations between age and visual attention, r(48) = .40 , p < .01, auditory attention,

r(48) = .31 , p < .01, working memory, r(48) = .383, p < .01, planning, r(48) = -.30, p =

.04, visual inhibition r(48) = .41, p < .01, auditory inhibition time, r(48) = -.32, p = .029

and auditory inhibition accuracy r(48) = -.33, p = .02. As expected, age was positively

correlated with raw scores on both the Vineland, r(48) = .40 , p < .01 and SSIS, r(48) =

.39, p < .01, indicating that social competence increased with age. When using standard

scores, there was a significant correlation between age and the SSIS, r(48) = 0.35 p = .01,

Page 81: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

70

but not the Vineland, r(48) = -.11, p = .45 or the BRIEF, r(48) = 0.10, p = .49. For

children in the control group, older children performed better on most measures of SEL

and EF and obtained higher parent ratings of social competence and EF.

Age was not significantly correlated with any SEL task for children with ASD.

Age was, however, significantly correlated with four EF tasks. As age increased,

performance improved on visual attention, r(49) = .40 , p < .01, working memory, r(49)

= .36, p = .01, auditory inhibition (time), r(49) = -.34, p = .02 and affective decision

making, r(49) = .28, p = .05. There were no significant correlations between age and raw

scores on the Vineland, r(48) = .03, p = .86, the SSIS, r(48) = -0.05, p = .71, or the

BRIEF, r(48) = .04, p = .80. When using standard scores, there was a negative

association between age and the Vineland, r(48) = -0.53, p < .01 but not on the SSIS,

r(48) < 0.01, p = .94 or the BRIEF, r(48) = 0.07, p = .64. These results indicate that

performance on SEL, social competence and parent-rated EF did not improve with age

for the children with ASD. In fact, the standard scores suggest that there is an inverse

relationship between age and social adaptation. The significant correlations between age

and measures of SEL and EF are problematic because it indicates that the same raw score

has a different meaning for children at different ages. As such, age-corrected z scores

were calculated as described previously and used for all subsequent analyses.

IQ. For the control group, IQ was not significantly associated with any

performance-based EF measures or parent ratings of EF, but was positively associated

with 2 SEL tasks. Higher IQ scores were associated with higher scores on the Voice

Page 82: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

71

Table 2 Correlations among measures of Age, IQ, measures of SEL and social competence for the CONTROL group

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Age .

2. IQ -.03

3. face affect .34* .02

4. voice affect .37* .31* .32*

5. pragmatic 1

.59* .19 .34* .46*

6. empathy .-.05 -.30* .07 .08 .23

7. ToM .08 -.10 .07 .04 .09 .03

8. prob solving2

.40* .20 .22 .29* .55* .21 -.06

9. Vineland -.11 -.21 .36* .09 .28 .17 .36* .45*

10.SSIS .35* -.34* .27 -.13 .20 .29* .11 .40* .61*

Note: Age-corrected z scores used for SEL and standard scores used for Vineland and SSIS 1

Pragmatic Language; 2Social Problem Solving

P<.05

Page 83: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

72

Table 3.

Correlations among measures of Age, IQ, measures of SEL and social competence for

the ASD group

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Age .

2. IQ -.05

3. facial

affect .06 .31*

4. voice

affect .20 .33* .47*

5. prag1

.17 .36* .27 .36*

6. empathy -.23 -.08 .04 -.08 .02

7. ToM .27 .37* .34* .27 .56* .10

8. prob solving

2

.20 .46* .25 .46* .61* -.06 .50*

9.Vineland -.53* .05 .31 .08 .30* .09 .19 .26

10.SSIS .01 .14 .08 .10 .16 .18 .01 .18 .62*

Note: Age-corrected z scores used for SEL and standard scores used for Vineland and SSIS

1Pragmatic Language;

2Social Problem Solving

P<.05

Page 84: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

73

Table 4

Correlations between Age, IQ, measures of EF and social competence for the

CONTROL group

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Age . .

2. IQ -.03

3.aud

attn1

.31* -.06 .

4.vis

attn2

.40* .19 .06

5.WM3

.38* .11 .36* .54*

6.plan -.30* .13 .13 -.04 .22 .

7. inhib

time4

-.32* -.14 -.13 -.25 -.28 .20

8.inhib

errors5 -.33* -.25 -.15 -.22 -.23 .06 .32*

9. visual

inhib6

.41* .10 .39* .66* .61* -.01 -.23 -.34*

10.dec

making7

.06 -.25 .13 -.05 -.16 -.22 .21 .13 .01

11. BRIEF .10 -.00 .14 .15 .02 .16 -.13 .22 -.06 .06

12.

Vineland .-.11 -.21 .09 -.00 .10 -.34* -.09 -.15 .15 .18 -.39*

13. SSIS .35* -.34* .09 .02 .08 -.38* -.13 -.36* .16 .17 -.36*.61*

Note: Age-corrected z scores used for EF and standard scores used for Vineland and SSIS 1Auditory attention; 2Visual Attention;

3 Working memory;

4 Auditory inhibition time;

5Auditory inhibition

errors; 6Visual inhibition;

7Affective decision making

*p< .05

Page 85: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

74

Table 5

Correlations between Age, IQ, measures of EF and social competence for the

ASD group

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

10

11

1. Age .

2. IQ .05

3.aud

attn1

.24 .18

4.vis

attn2

.40* .30* .61*

5.WM3

.36* .32* .53* .70*

6.plan .17 -.39* -.21 -.44* -.40*

7. inhib

time4

-.34* -.41* -.24 -.32* -.32* .45*

8. inhib

errors5

-.16 -.39* -.26 -.50* -.35* .58* .38*

9. vis

inhib6

.03 -.02 .33* .51* .47 -.11 .01 -.17

10.dec

making7

.28* .00 .36* .35* .23 -.17 -.31* -.15 .29

11.

Vineland -.53* .05 .49* .31* .24 -.04 -.43* -.13 .36*

.30

12.SSIS .01 .145 .51* .20 .15 -.02 -.32* -.19 .33*

.31*

.62*

13. BRIEF .07 -.04 .37* -.16 -.22 -.16 .15 .08 -.44*

-.14

.67* 59*

Note: Age-corrected z scores used for EF and standard scores used for Vineland and SSIS 1Auditory attention; 2Visual Attention;

3 Working memory;

4 Auditory inhibition time;

5Auditory inhibition

errors; 6Visual inhibition;

7Affective decision making

*p< .05

Page 86: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

75

Affect Recognition r(48) = .31, p =.03 and with lower scores on Empathy r(48) = -.30, p

= .04. IQ scores were also negatively correlated with SSIS, r(48) = -.34 , p = .02. As

such, children in the control group with higher intelligence performed better on voice

affect recognition but rated themselves lower on empathy and received lower scores on

the social skills measure.

For the ASD group, IQ was significantly correlated with 5 of 6 SEL tasks and 5 of

8 EF tasks. Higher IQ scores were associated with better performance on the facial affect

recognition, r(49) = .31, p = .03, voice affect recognition, r(49) = .33, p = .02, ToM, r(49)

= .37, p = .01, pragmatic language, r(49) = .36, p = .01, and problem solving, r(49) = .46,

p < 0.01. Performance also improved as IQ increased on the visual attention, r(49) = .30,

p = .04, planning, r(49) = -.39, p =.01, working memory, (49) = .32, p = .02, auditory

inhibition time, r(49) = -.41, p < .01 and accuracy, r(49) = -.391, p = .005. There were no

significant correlations between IQ scores and the Vineland, r(48) = .05, p = .76, the

SSIS, r(48) = .15, p = .33, or the BRIEF, r(48) = -.04, p = .80. As such, children with

ASD and higher intelligence performed better on most SEL and EF tasks but were not

rated higher in social competence or parent-measured EF.

Age of diagnosis. For children with ASD, the age at which they were diagnosed

was significantly correlated with one SEL and two EF tasks. The older the children were

when they received their ASD diagnosis, the better their performance on ToM, r(49) =

.32, p = .037, ,visual attention, r(49) = .390, p< .01, and planning, r(49) = -.34 , p= .02.

Age of diagnosis was not significantly correlated with the Vineland, r(48) = -.03 , p= .83,

the SSIS, r(48) = -.03 , p= .84, or the BRIEF, r(48) = .10, p= .50. As such, although

children who received an ASD diagnosis at a later age performed better on ToM, visual

Page 87: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

76

attention and planning, they were not rated as having higher social competence or parent-

measured EF.

5.3 Hypothesis 1: Children with ASD will perform significantly different than

neurotypical children in specific domains of EF, SEL and social

competence.

In order to test the hypothesis that children with ASD would perform more poorly

on measures of SEL, EF and social competence, a series of ANOVAs were performed to

examine group differences between the tasks. The Brown-Forsythe statistic is reported

where scores violated assumptions of normality (Fagerland, 2012; Field, 2005). Since the

correlation analyses demonstrated that the relationship between age and measures of

SEL, EF and social competence differed across groups, using age as a covariate violated

the assumptions of homogeneity of regression slopes and thus the age-corrected scores

were used. See Table 6 for means and standard deviations of the SEL, EF and social

competence measures for the ASD and control groups.

5.3.1 SEL measures. There were significant differences on 4 of 6 measures.

Children with ASD performed significantly lower on voice affect recognition, F(1, 94) =

10.52, p < 0.01, ToM, F(1, 54) = 31.79, p < 0.01, pragmatic language, F(1, 75) = 50.97,

p < 0.01, and social problem solving, F(1, 66) = 41.84, p < 0.01. There were no

significant differences between the groups on facial affect recognition, F(1,94 ).=2.18, p

=.12, and empathy, F(1,94 ).=2.47, p =.20. In terms of clinical impairment, more children

with ASD performed at least one standard deviation below normative mean on all SEL

Page 88: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

77

measures that had standardized scores. These included the DANVA facial affect

recognition (14% versus 4%), DANVA voice affect recognition (28% versus 8%), Happe

Strange Stories -ToM (22% versus 0%), TOPL pragmatic language (15% versus 2 %) and

the TOPS problem solving (37% versus 2%). Interestingly, 41% of children with ASD

and 88% of neurotypical children passed all ToM questions.

5.3.2 EF measures. In terms of performance-based EF, there were significant

differences between the groups on 6 out of 8 tasks. Children with ASD performed

significantly lower than controls on auditory attention, F(1, 82) = 4.84, p = 0.03, visual

attention, F(1, 94) = 9.39, p < 0.01, working memory, F(1, 94) =4.23, p = 0.04, auditory

inhibition time, F(1, 85) = 5.63, p = 0.02, auditory inhibition accuracy, F(1,78) = 7.08, p

= 0.01, and planning, F(1, 94) =3.83, p = 0.05. In terms of clinical impairment, more

children with ASD performed at least one standard deviation below normative mean on

all these measures. These included Planning (27% versus 15%), Auditory Attention (47%

versus 23%), Visual Attention (12% versus 4%), Visual Working Memory (25% versus

8%) and Auditory Inhibition (40% versus 20%). Although there were no significant

differences between the groups on Visual Inhibition, more children in the ASD group

scored within the clinical impairment range (20% versus 12%).

5.3.3 Parent rated EF. With respect to parental ratings of EF, scores on the

BRIEF were significantly higher for children with ASD compared to the control group,

indicating greater overall executive dysfunction, F(1, 95), = 137.08, p < .01. When

looking at the two domains of the BRIEF, children with ASD were rated as having more

difficulties with Metacognition, F(1, 95) = 100.43, p < 0.01, and Behavioural Regulation,

F(1,95) = 150.23, p < 0.01. In terms of clinical impairment, 90% of children with ASD

Page 89: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

78

performed at least one standard deviation below normative mean compared to 8 % of

children from the control group on the General Executive Composite overall score.

5.3.4 Social competence. There were significant differences between groups on

both measures of social competence. As expected, children in the control group were

rated as significantly more socially competent than children in the ASD group on both the

Vineland F(1, 72) = 221.03, p < 0.01, and the SSIS F(1, 81) = 146.94, p < 0.01. In terms

of clinical impairment, 90% of children with ASD performed at least one and a half

standard deviations below normative mean on the Vineland compared to 8 % of children

from the control group. On the SSIS, 65% of children with ASD one and a half standard

deviations below the normative mean compared to no children from the control group.

5.4 Hypothesis 2: Measures of Nonverbal Awareness, Social Meaning and Social

Understanding will represent latent SEL domains for the ASD and

control groups.

In order to test the hypothesis that the SEL measures are associated, Pearson

product-moment correlation coefficients between SEL measures were calculated for the

ASD and control groups separately (see Tables 2 and 3). Exploratory factor analysis was

then performed to examine whether the SEL measures represent three distinct domains:

Nonverbal Awareness (facial and voice affect recognition); Social Meaning (ToM,

empathy and pragmatic language); and Social Reasoning (social problem solving). For

the control group, empathy and ToM were not significantly correlated with any SEL task.

Page 90: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

79

Table 6. Descriptive Statistics of Measures of SEL, EF and Social Competence

Variable

Control M (SD) N = 48

ASD M (SD) N = 49

F-value

SOCIAL EMOTIONAL LEARNING

Facial Affect Recognition1

Range 20.81(2.22) 15 – 24

20.16(2.13) 15 – 23

2.18

Voice Affect Recognition2

Range 18.53(2.88) 13 – 23

16.33(4.16) 2 – 23

10.52**

ToM3

Range 9.77(0.66) 7 – 10

7.61(2.68) 0 – 10

31.79**

Empathy (child ratings)4

Range Empathy (parent ratings) Range

11.37(3.49) 5 – 20 12. 53(3.54) 3 – 20

10.29(3.84) 4 – 21 9.68(4.00) 2 – 21

2.47

Pragmatic Language5

Range 12.10(2.28) 6 – 16

8.27(3.33) 2 – 14

50.97**

Social Problem Solving6

Range 86.00(7.80) 65 – 100

70.90(15.40) 14 – 92

41.84**

EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS

Auditory Attention7

Range 28.44(1.89) 22 – 30

27.61(2.76) 21 – 30

4.84*

Visual Attention8

Range 84.88(8.62) 66 – 100

79.84(11.55)

41 – 100

9.39**

Visual Working Memory9

Range 15.50(6.60) 6 – 34

13. 31(7.94)

0 -33

4.23*

Planning10

Range 26.33(13.32) 2 – 53

33.04(21.07) 4 – 115

3.83*

Visual Inhibition11

Range 10.90(9.56) 2 – 20

10.43(4.19) 3 – 20

.800

Auditory Inhibition Speed12

Range 25.23(8.18) 4 – 37

18.98(11.52) 2 – 53

5.63*

Auditory Inhibition Accuracy

13

Range

5.46(3.55) 0 – 15

7.55(5.57) 0 – 22

7.08**

Affective Decision Making14

Range 54.08(14.62) 13 – 84

51.94(14.76) 21 – 100

.821

EF PARENT RATINGS: BRIEF15

Range 48.15(7.78) 34 – 66

70.64(10.08) 40 – 86

137.08**

SOCIAL COMPETENCE

Vineland Socialisation16

Range 103.54(13.44) 78 – 138

70.64(10.88) 47 – 98

221.03**

SSIS

Range 110.81(9.98) 89 – 132

78.56(15.53) 40 – 107

146.94**

Note: Raw scores used for SEL and EF; standard scores used for Vineland, SSIS and BRIEF

Page 91: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

80

As expected, there were positive correlations between the facial and linguistic affect

recognition tasks, r(46) =.32, p=.03. Pragmatic language was positively correlated with

facial affect recognition, r(46) =.34, p=.02, voice affect recognition, r(46) =.46, p <.01

and social problem solving, r(46) = 0.55, p < .01. Social problem solving was also

positively correlated with voice affect recognition, r(46) = .29, p = .05. For the ASD

group, empathy was also not significantly correlated with any other SEL measures.

Similar to the control group, there were positive correlations between the facial and

linguistic affect recognition tasks, r(47) = .47, p < .01. In contrast to the control group,

ToM was positively correlated with facial affect recognition, r(47) = .34, p = .02,

pragmatic language, r(47) = .56, p < .01 and social problem solving, r(47) = .50, p < .01.

Pragmatic language was positively correlated with voice affect recognition, r(47) = .36, p

= .01 and social problem solving, r(47) = .61, p < .01. Lastly, social problem solving was

also positively correlated with voice affect recognition, r(47) = .46, p < .01.

5.4.1 Principal components analysis of SEL. A principal components analysis

(PCA) with varimax rotation was conducted to identify SEL domains and to create

composite scores. Creating composite scores allowed for data reduction for subsequent

analyses. All analyses were performed separately for the control and ASD groups.

Initially, data screening was performed on the SEL variables to determine the suitability

of the data for factor detection. Several well-recognized criteria were examined,

including inspecting inter-correlations between variables and ensuring some correlations

above 0.30. Two measures of sample adequacy were consulted – the Kaiser-Meyer Olkin

(KMO) index and the anti-image diagonal correlations of sampling adequacy, which

above 0.50 is considered suitable for factor analysis and are particularly important when

Page 92: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

81

the sample sizes are small (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2009). Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was

consulted to determine whether the data forms an identity matrix and to further determine

suitability for structure detection. The determinant of the correlation matrix detects

multicollinearity, which should be greater than 0.00001, was also checked. Multiple

extraction techniques were then utilised to ensure the best fit of the data, as recommended

in the literature (Field, 2009). These included Kaiser’s criteria of eigenvalues > 1 and

examination of the scree plot, communalities and cumulative percent of variance

extracted. Absolute values below 0.50 were suppressed to account for the small sample

sizes (Field, 2009). Finally, the variables that did not correlate with any other measures

were excluded from further analysis (Field, 2009). As a result of the differing correlations

between variables in each group, which is common in research with clinical groups

(Dowling, Hermann, LaRue & Sager, 2010; Meredith & Teresi, 2006; Raykov,

Marcoulides & Cheng-Hsien, 2012), different variables were entered for the control and

ASD groups, as described below.

Five variables were entered into the PCA analysis for the control group. These

included facial and voice affect recognition, empathy, pragmatic language and social

problem solving. ToM was excluded from analyses because it had very low correlations

with all other variables. Although there were no significant correlations between empathy

and the other SEL variables for the control group, it was retained because it was

approaching significance with pragmatic language, r(46) = 0.23, p= 0.06 and social

problem solving, r(46) = 0.22, p= 0.07. Empathy had a diagonal anti-image correlation of

0.77 and communalities above 0.80 for all analyses, which further supports its retention.

The KMO Index was 0.699 and the Bartlett’s test of Sphericity was significant, x2 (48) =

Page 93: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

82

36.67, p < 0.01. The diagonals of the anti-image correlations ranged from 0.65 to 0.79.

Finally, the communalities ranged from 0.580 to 0.970. Initial eigenvalues indicated a 2-

factor solution that accounted for 63.57% of the variance, but the communalities were

low, with three of the five being below 0.55. A three-factor solution was preferred based

on previous theoretical support and because the eigenvalues leveled off after three

factors, which accounted for 78.70% of the variance. Extracted components ranged from

0.51 to 0.98. The first factor included pragmatic language and social problem solving and

accounted for 34.31% of the variance. The second factor included voice and facial affect

recognition, accounted for 24.11% of the variance and the third factor of empathy

accounted for 20.28% of the variance. Voice affect recognition loaded equally on the first

and second factors, but was retained for the second factor based on several robust

theoretical grounds, including that these two variables were designed to conceptually

measure different aspects of the same underlying construct and also confirmation that

these same two variables loaded on the same factor in previous research with larger

samples (Lipton & Nowicki, 2009; McKown et al., 2009; McKown et al., 2013). The

factor labels of ‘nonverbal awareness’, ‘social meaning’ and social reasoning’ proposed

by Lipton & Nowicki (2009) and McKown and colleagues (2009; 2013) were modified to

suit the current data. The three distinct underlying SEL factors for the control group used

in subsequent analyses are Social Understanding (pragmatic language and social problem

solving), Nonverbal Awareness (facial affect and voice affect recognition), and Empathy

(empathy) (see Figure 5a).

Five variables were also entered into the PCA analysis for the ASD group. These

included facial and voice affect recognition, ToM, pragmatic language and social Facial & Voice

Affect Recognition

Page 94: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

83

problem solving. Empathy was excluded from analyses because it had very low

correlations with all other variables. The KMO Index was 0.72 and the Bartlett’s test of

Sphericity was significant, x2 (49) = 67.28, p < 0.01. The diagonals of the anti-image

correlations ranged from 0.65 to 0.79. Finally, the communalities ranged from 0.64 to

0.78. Initial eigenvalues indicated a 2-factor solution that accounted for 71.90% of the

variance. The first factor included ToM, pragmatic language, and social problem solving,

and accounted for 41.95% of the variance. The second factor included voice and facial

affect recognition and accounted for 29.95% of the variance. There were no cross

loadings and extracted components ranged from 0.78 to 0.87. The factor labels proposed

by Lipton & Nowicki (2009) and McKown and colleagues (2009; 2013) were again

modified to suit the current data. The two distinct underlying SEL factors for the ASD

group used in subsequent analyses are Social Understanding (ToM, pragmatic language

and Social Problem Solving) and Nonverbal Awareness (facial affect and voice affect

recognition) (see Figure 5b).

5.5 Hypothesis 3: The measures of Cognitive, Behavioural and Emotional regulation will

represent three latent EF domains for the ASD and control groups.

In order to test the hypothesis the EF measures are associated, Pearson product-

moment correlation coefficients were first calculated for the ASD and control groups

separately using eight EF measures: auditory attention, visual attention, planning, visual

working memory, visual inhibition, auditory inhibition reaction time, auditory inhibition

Page 95: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

84

Figure 5a. The SEL domains for the control group

Figure 5b. The SEL domains for the ASD group

Page 96: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

85

accuracy and affective decision making (see Tables 4 and 5). Exploratory factor analysis

was then performed to examine whether the EF measures represented three distinct

domains: Cognitive Regulation (planning, auditory attention, visual attention, visual

working memory); Behavioural Regulation (auditory inhibition accuracy, auditory

inhibition time, and visual inhibition); and Emotional Regulation (affective decision

making).

For the control group, planning and affective decision making were not

significantly correlated with any other EF measures. Although there were no significant

correlations between visual attention and auditory attention, they were both positively

correlated with the same measures. Visual working memory was positively correlated

with auditory attention, r(46) = .36, p = .01 and visual attention, r(46) = .54, p < .01.

Visual inhibition was also positively correlated with auditory attention, r(46) = .39, p =

.01 and visual attention, r(46) = .66, p < .01. Visual working memory was positively

correlated with visual inhibition, r(46) = .61, p < .01. Visual inhibition was negatively

correlated with auditory inhibition accuracy, r(46) = -34, p = .02 indicating better

performance on visual inhibition was associated with fewer errors on auditory inhibition.

Lastly, auditory inhibition errors and reaction time were positively correlated, r(48) = .32,

p = .03. For the ASD group, visual attention was significantly correlated with all EF

tasks and visual working memory was significantly correlated with all EF tasks except

affective decision making. Auditory attention was positively correlated with visual

attention, r(47) = .61, p < .01, working memory, r(47) = .53, p < .01, visual inhibition,

r(47) = .33, p = .02, and affective decision making, r(47) = .36, p = .01. Unlike the

control group, planning and affective decision making were each correlated with four EF

Page 97: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

86

tasks. Better performance on planning was correlated with better performance on visual

attention, r(47) = -.44, p < .01, visual working memory, r(47) = -.40, p = .01, auditory

inhibition reaction time, r(47) = .45, p < .01, and auditory inhibition accuracy, r(49) =

.58, p < .01. Better performance on affective decision making was correlated with better

performance on visual attention, r(49) = .35, p = .01, auditory attention, r(47) = .36, p =

.01, visual inhibition, r(47) = .29, p = .04 and auditory inhibition reaction time, r(47) = -

.31, p = .03. Lastly, the two auditory inhibition tasks were positively correlated, r(47) =

.38, p < .01.

There were no significant correlations between the parent ratings and any

performance-based EF tasks for the control group. For the ASD group, parent EF ratings

were negatively correlated with two performance-based EF measures for the ASD group.

Higher overall ratings of executive dysfunction were associated with lower scores on

auditory attention, r(46) = -.37, p = .01 and visual inhibition, r(46) = -.44, p < .01. This

confirms the importance of conducting separate analyses for performance-based and

parent-rated EF.

5.5.1 Principal components analysis of EF. A principal components analysis

(PCA) with varimax rotation was conducted to identify EF domains and to create

composite scores. All analyses were performed separately for the control and ASD

groups. Data screening was initially performed on the EF variables to determine the

suitability of the data for factor detection using the well-recognized criteria previously

described.

For the control group, six variables were used for PCA analysis: auditory

attention, visual attention, visual working memory, visual inhibition, auditory inhibition

Page 98: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

87

time and accuracy. Planning and affective decision making were excluded from analyses

because they had very low correlations with all other variables. The KMO Index was 0.68

and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, x2 (48) = 76.31, p < 0.001. The

diagonals of the anti-image correlations ranged from 0.50 to 0.81. Finally, the

communalities ranged from 0.65 to 0.95. Initial eigenvalues indicated a 3-factor solution

that accounted for 77.55% of the variance, Extracted components ranged from 0.76 to

0.96. The first factor included visual attention, visual inhibition and visual working

memory and accounted for 35.63% of the variance. The second factor included auditory

inhibition reaction time and accuracy and accounted for 22.65% of the variance. The

third factor included auditory attention and accounted for 19.28% of the variance. The

three EF labels originally proposed - cognitive, behavioral and affective self regulation

were not suitable for naming these factors. The three distinct underlying EF factors for

the control group included Visual Executive Control (visual working memory, attention

and inhibition), Inhibition (auditory inhibition reaction time and auditory inhibition

accuracy) and Auditory Attention (auditory attention) (see Figure 6a).

For the ASD group, the eight measures of EF were also entered into a PCA

analysis. The visual inhibition variable had a low diagonal correlation on the anti-image

matrix and thus was not retained for the analyses. With the remaining seven variables, the

KMO Index was 0.78 and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, x2 (49) =

110.68, p < 0.01. The diagonals of the anti-image correlations ranged from 0.75 to 0.82.

Finally, the communalities ranged from 0.70 to 0.86. Initial eigenvalues indicated a 3-

factor solution that accounted for 76.03% of the variance. Extracted components ranged

from 0.64 to 0.89. The first factor included visual attention, auditory attention and visual

Page 99: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

88

working memory and accounted for 31.03% of the variance. The second factor included

planning, auditory inhibition reaction time and accuracy and accounted for 28.03% of the

variance. The third factor included affective decision making and accounted for 16.98%

of the variance. There was some cross loading with auditory inhibition reaction time as it

loaded on the second (0.64) and third factors (-0.54). It was retained for the second factor

based on higher loading score and the theoretical basis that is conceptually related to

auditory inhibition accuracy.

The three EF labels originally proposed of cognitive, behavioral and emotional

regulation were suitable for these factors. The three distinct underlying EF factors for the

ASD group included Cognitive Regulation (visual working memory, visual attention and

auditory attention), Behavioural Regulation (planning, auditory inhibition reaction time

and auditory inhibition accuracy) and Emotion Regulation (affective decision making)

(see Figure 6b).

5.5.2 Principal components analysis of parent-rated EF. Exploratory factor

analysis for both groups was performed using all 9 subscales of the BRIEF to determine

whether the three domains recently identified in the literature (Egeland & Fallmyr, 2010;

Gioia, Isquith, Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002) were also appropriate in this sample.

For the control group, the KMO Index was 0.76 and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was

significant, x2 (48) = 199.80, p < 0.01. The diagonals of the anti-image correlations

ranged from 0.69 to 0.83. Finally, the communalities ranged from 0.45 to 0.83. Initial

eigenvalues indicated a 2-factor solution that accounted for 63.85% of the variance,

Extracted components ranged from 0.66 to 0.91. The first factor included plan/organize,

task monitor, initiate, working memory and organisation of material and accounted for

Page 100: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

89

34.83% of the variance. The second factor included self monitor, shift, inhibit and

emotional control and accounted for 28.02% of the variance.

For the ASD group, the KMO Index was 0.81 and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was

significant, x2 (49) = 181.54, p < 0.01. The diagonals of the anti-image correlations

ranged from 0.63 to 0.89. Finally, the communalities ranged from 0.46 to 0.78. Initial

eigenvalues indicated a 2-factor solution that accounted for 62.05% of the variance,

Extracted components ranged from 0.57 to 0.88. The first factor included self monitor,

shift, inhibit and emotional control and accounted for 34.61% of the variance. The second

factor included plan/organize, task monitor, initiate, working memory and organisation of

material and accounted for 27.44% of the variance. The three labels originally proposed

for the parent EF ratings, which included cognitive, behavioral and emotional self

regulation, were not suitable for these factors for either the control or ASD group. Rather,

the two domains of Metacognition and Behavioural Regulation provided in the BRIEF

manual were maintained for both groups. As such, the two distinct underlying parent-

rated EF factors for both groups included Metacognition (plan/organize, task monitor,

initiate, working memory and organisation of material), and Behavioural Regulation (self

monitor, shift, inhibit and emotional control) (see figure 6b). p = .01) whereas the BRIEF

Metacognition index did not (b = -.26, β =-.12 p = .46). For the ASD group, parent EF

ratings predicted 54.2% of the variance in social competence (r2 = .54). Coefficients

showed age (b = -1.52, β =-.22, p = .05) accounted for significance variance in Social

Competence but not IQ (b = .07, β =.08, p = .44). Behavioural Regulation (b = -1.6, β =-

.58, p < .01) predicted significant variance in social competence but Metacognition (b = -

.49, β =-.17, p = .21) did not for the ASD group.

Page 101: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

90

Figure 6a. The EF domains for the control group

Figure 6b. The EF domains for the ASD group

Auditory Attention

Affective Decision

Making

Page 102: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

91

5.5.3 Creation of composite scores. To reduce the number of variables and limit

the number of interactions tested, the SEL and EF factors identified in the previous

sections for the control and ASD groups were used to create composite scores. Multiple

indicators of a latent factor remove biasing effects of measurement error (Kenny, 2003)

and reduce probability of a Type I error (Field, 2009). The PCA of the SEL measures had

indicated that five measures were indicators of three latent SEL domains for the control

group: Social Understanding (pragmatic language and social problem solving);

Nonverbal Awareness (facial affect recognition and voice affect recognition); and

Empathy (empathy). For the ASD group, five SEL measures reflected two latent SEL

domains: Social Understanding (pragmatic language, social problem solving and ToM);

and Nonverbal Awareness (facial affect recognition and voice affect recognition). The

PCA of the EF measures had indicated that five measures were indicators of three latent

EF domains for the control group: Visual Executive Control (visual attention, visual

working memory and visual inhibition): Inhibition (auditory inhibition time and auditory

inhibition errors); and Auditory Attention (auditory attention). For the ASD group, seven

measures were indicators of three EF domains: Cognitive Regulation (visual attention,

visual working memory and auditory attention); Behavioural Regulation (auditory

inhibition time , auditory inhibition errors and planning); and Emotion Regulation

(affective decision making). The PCA analysis for the parent ratings of EF had indicated

that the 9 scales were indicators of two latent domains: Metacognition (plan/organize,

task monitor, initiate, working memory and organisation of material) and Behavioural

Regulation (self monitor, shift, inhibit and emotional control). Z-scores for SEL and EF

variables were summed according to each latent factor identified for the control and ASD

Page 103: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

92

groups separately. The z-scores of the SSIS and Vineland were combined to provide one

social competence score.

5.6 Hypothesis 4: The proposed model of SEL and EF will account for significant

variance in social competence among neurotypical children and

children with ASD.

To test the hypothesis that better scores on the SEL and EF domains would be

related to higher ratings of social competence, Pearson product-moment correlation

coefficients between the SEL and EF domains, and Social Competence were calculated

for each group separately (see Tables 7 and 8). Preliminary analyses were conducted by

examining histograms, Q-Q plots and Boxplots to ensure that composite score data met

assumption criteria for use in correlation analyses. As mentioned earlier, one case in the

ASD group was identified as an outlier because of very high social competence ratings,

and it was removed from the ASD group.

For the control group, Social Competence was positively correlated with two SEL

domains - Social Understanding, r(46) = .44, p < .01, and Empathy, r(46) = .29, p = .05,

but not Nonverbal Awareness, r(46) = .21, p = .16. Thus, neurotypical children who

performed better on two SEL domains - Social Understanding and Empathy - were also

rated higher in Social Competence. There were no significant correlations between Social

Competence and the three EF domains - Visual Executive Control r(46) = .14, p = .36,

Inhibition, r(46) = -.16, p = .27 or Auditory Attention, r(46) = .10, p = .51. Therefore,

Page 104: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

93

neurotypical children with better performance on any EF domains were not rated higher

in Social Competence.

For the ASD group, there were no significant correlations between Social

Competence and the two SEL domains - Nonverbal Awareness, r(46) = .20, p = .17 or

Social Understanding, r(46) = .17, p = .24. Thus, children with ASD who performed

better on either SEL domain were not rated higher in Social Competence. Social

Competence was positively correlated with two EF domains - Cognitive Regulation,

r(46) = .41, p < .01, and Emotional Regulation, r(46) = .32, p = .02, but not with

Behavioural Regulation, r(46) = -.23, p = .12. Thus, children with ASD who performed

better on Cognitive Regulation and Emotion Regulation were rated higher in Social

Competence. In terms of parent ratings, Social Competence was negatively correlated

with the BRIEF Behavioural Regulation Index, r(46) = -.50, p < .01 but not with the

BRIEF Metacognition Index, r(46) = -.18, p = .21 for the control group. Thus,

neurotypical children who were rated as having better Behavioural Regulation were also

rated as having higher Social Competence. For the ASD group, Social Competence was

negatively correlated with the BRIEF Behavioural Regulation, r(46) = -.73, p < .01 and

BRIEF Metacognition, r(46) = -.57 p < .01. Thus, children with ASD who were rated as

having better Behavioural Regulation and Metacognition were also rated as having higher

Social Competence.

5.6.1. Regression analyses. To test the hypothesis that SEL and EF

independently predict significant variance in social competence over and above the

influence of age and IQ, a series of multiple hierarchical regressions were performed for

each group separately. Parent-rated EF ratings were analysed separately based on the

Page 105: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

94

Table 7 Correlations between SEL and EF domains for the CONTROL group

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

11

1 SEL1

2. SEL2 .46*

3. SEL3 .25 .09

4. EF1 .31* .31* .01

5. EF2

-.23 .06 .15 -.37*

6. EF3 -.10 .24 -.06 .32* .17

7. BRIEF

Meta1 -.04 -.18 .03 .11 .01 .08

8. BRIEF Beh2

-.49* -.34* -.25 -.08 .12 .09 .50*

9.BRIEF

total -.17 -.20 -.10 .13 .-02 .16 .91* .77*

10Vineland .46* .11 .36* -.17 .07 -.06 .29* -.39* -.39*

11.SSIS .34* .09 .30* .10 -.12 .09 -.21 -.46* -.36* 61*

12. SC3 .44* -.21 .29* .14 -.16 .10 -.30* -.49* -.41* .90*

.81*

Note: SEL1= Social Understanding; SEL 2= Nonverbal Awareness; SEL 3 = Empathy EF 1 = Visual Executive Control. EF 2 = Inhibition EF 3 = Auditory Attention 1BRIEF Metacognition Index; 2BRIEF Behavioural Regulation Index; 3SC=sum of Vineland and SSIS *p< .05

Page 106: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

95

Table 8 Correlations between SEL and EF domains and social competence for the ASD group

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1SEL1

2. SEL2 .44*

3. EF 1 .49* .41

4. EF2 -.56* .40 -.49*

5. EF3

.16 .25 .37* -.26

6. BRIEF

Meta1

-.04 .15 -.11 -.07 -.14

7. BRIEF

BehInh2

-.08 .15 -.35* -.08 -.11 .61*

8. BRIEF total

-.08 -.03 -.26 .01 -.11 .87* .91*

9.Vinela

nd .11 .13 .10 -.06 -.09 -.47 -.64* -.67*

10.SSIS .14 .14 .33* -.26 .31* -.45* -.64* -.59* .62*

11. SC3

.20 .17 .41* -.23 .32* -.57* -.73* -.57* .82* .84*

SEL= Social Understanding; SEL 2= Nonverbal Awareness. EF 1 = Cognitive Regulation. EF 2 = Behavioural Regulation. EF 3 = Emotional Regulation 1BRIEF Metacognition Index; 2BRIEF Behavioural Regulation Index; 3SC=sum of Vineland and SSIS *p< .05

Page 107: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

96

results of the correlation analyses. Age and IQ were entered in as predictors in the first

block of each regression and social competence was entered as the criterion variable for

all analyses. Data screening was performed according to Field (2005).

For the control group, Social Understanding, Nonverbal Awareness and Empathy

were entered as predictor variables in the SEL analysis. Results indicated that the overall

SEL model was significant, F(5, 47) = 3.30, p = .01 and that two predictors accounted for

28.2% of the variance in Social Competence ratings (r2 = .28). Coefficients showed that

IQ (b = -.34 β = -.34, p = .03) significantly predicted Social Competence whereas age (b

= -.95, β = -.15, p = .41) was not significant in predicting Social Competence. Social

Understanding significantly predicted Social Competence beyond the effect of age and

IQ (b = .23, β = .40, p = .04) for neurotypical children whereas Nonverbal Awareness (b

= .37, β = .06, p = .70) and Empathy (b = 1.72, β = .17, p = .28) did not predict

significant variance in Social Competence for this control group. For the EF analysis,

Visual Executive Control, Inhibition and Auditory Attention were entered as predictor

variables. Results indicate that the overall EF model was not significant, F(5, 47) = .96, p

= .46, and explained 10.2% of the variance in Social Competence ratings (r2 = .10). In

terms of the control variables, coefficients show that age (b = .68, β = .11, p = .54) and IQ

(b = -.29, β = -.29, p = .06) were not significant in predicting Social Competence. Visual

Executive Control (b = -.32, β = -.08, p = .64), Inhibition (b = -.48, β = -.08, p = .65) and

Auditory Attention (b = -.29, β = -.03, p = 86) did not predict significant variance in

Social Competence for neurotypical children.

For the ASD group, Social Understanding and Nonverbal Awareness were entered

as predictor variables in the SEL analysis. The results indicated that the overall SEL

Page 108: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

97

model was not significant, F(4, 47) = 1.5, p = .23, and accounted for 12.2% of variance in

Social Competence (r2 = .12). Coefficients show that age (b = -2.21, β = -.31, p = .05)

significantly accounted for variance, but IQ (b = -.09, β = -.11, p = .56), Social

Understanding (b = .38, β = .21, p = .26) and Nonverbal Awareness (b = .1.02, β =-.18, p

= .30) were not significant in predicting Social Competence for children with ASD.

Cognitive Regulation, Behaviour Regulation and Emotion Regulation were entered as

predictor variables in the EF analysis. The results indicated that the overall EF model was

significant, F(5, 47) =3.19, p = .02 and accounted for 28% of the variance in Social

Competence (r2 = .28). In terms of the control variables, the coefficients showed that age

(b = -.35, β = -.50, p < .01) significantly predicted Social Competence whereas IQ (b = -

.14, β =-.16, p = .33) did not. Cognitive Regulation (b = 1.2, β =.44, p = .01) predicted

significant variance in Social Competence whereas Behavioural Regulation (b = -.56, β

=-.18, p = .31) and Emotional Regulation (b = 2.76, β =.24, p = .11) did not account for

significant variance.

In terms of parent ratings, the BRIEF Behavioural Regulation and Metacognition

indices were entered into separate regression analyses for each group. Overall, parent

ratings of EF significantly predicted Social Competence for both the control group, F(4,

48) = 4.82, p < .01 and the ASD group, F(4, 48) =13.34, p < .01. For the control group,

parent EF ratings predicted 31% of the variance in Social Competence (r2 = .31).

Coefficients showed that IQ (b = -.27, β =-.27, p = .04) accounted for significant variance

in Social Competence but age (b = .02, β =<.01, p = .98) did not; the Behavioural

Regulation index predicted significant variance in Social Competence (b = -1.31, β =-.41,

Page 109: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

98

Table 9 Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Social Competence for the CONTROL Group

Domain

R

R2

R2Change

F

F change

P

SEL

Age & IQ .30 .09 .09 2.29 2.30 .11

Model 1 .53 .28 .19 3.30 3.69 .013*

EF

Age & IQ .30 .09 .09 2.30 2.30 .11

Model 2 .32 .10 .01 .96 .147 .46

Parent Rated EF

Age & IQ

Model 3

.30

.56

.09

.31

.09

.21

2.30

4.82

2.30

6.77

.11

.00*

Note. SEL Model 1 age, IQ, Social Understanding, Nonverbal Awareness, Empathy EF Model 2 = age, IQ, Visual Executive Control, Inhibition, Auditory Attention. Parent-rated EF Model 3 = age, IQ, Metacognition, Behavioural Regulation. * p<.05

Page 110: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

99

Table 10 Regression Coefficients of the SEL and EF Composite Scores for the CONTROL Group

Domain

Beta

B

T

P

SEL

AGE

IQ

-.95

-.34

-.15

-.34

-.84

-2.2*

.41

.03

Social Understanding

Nonverbal Awareness

Empathy

2.23

.37

1.72

.40

.06

.17

2.12*

.38

1.1

.04

.70

.28

EF

Age

IQ

.68

-.29

.11

-.29

.61

-1.91

.54

.06

Visual Executive Control

Auditory Inhibition

Auditory Attention

-.32

-.48

-.29

-.08

-.08

-.03

-.48

-.46

-.18

.64

.65

.86

Parent Rated EF

Age

IQ

.02

-.27

.00

-.27

.02

-2.07

.98

.04

Metacognition -.30 -.12 -.74 .46

Behavioural Regulation -1.31 -.41 -2.60 .01

Page 111: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

100

Table 11 Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Social Competence for the ASD Group

Domain

R

R2

R2Change

F

F change

P

SEL

Age & IQ .24 .06 .06 2.80 1.31 .28

Model 1 .35 .12 .07 1.46 1.6 .23

EF

Age & IQ .23 .06 .06 2.80 1.31 .28

Model 2 .53 .28 .22 3.19 4.25 .02

Parent Rated EF

Age & IQ

Model 3

.24

.74

.06

.54

.06

.49

1.31

12.44

1.31

22.30

.28

.00

Note. SEL Model 1 = age, IQ, Social Understanding, Nonverbal Awareness. EF Model 2 = age, IQ, Cognitive Regulation, Behavioural Regulation, Emotional Regulation Parent-rated EF Model 3 = age, IQ, Metacognition, Behavioural Regulation. * p<.05

Page 112: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

101

Table 12 Regression Coefficients of the SEL and EF Composite Scores for the ASD Group

Domain

Beta

B

t

P

SEL

AGE

IQ

-2.21

-.09

-.31

-.11

-2.01

-.59

.05

.56

Social Understanding

Nonverbal Awareness

.3

1.02

.21

.18

1.15

1.06

.26

.30

EF

Age

IQ

-3.51

-.14

-.50

-.16

-2.01*

-.72

.00

.33

Cognitive Regulation

Behaviour Regulation

Emotional Regulation

1.21

-.56

2.76

.44

-.18

.24

2.62*

-1.04

1.62

.01

.31

.11

Parent Rated EF

Age

IQ

-1.52

.07

-.22

.08

-2.03*

.78

.05

.44

Metacognition -.49 -.17 -1.28 .21

Behavioural Inhibition -1.62 -.58 -4.34* .00

Page 113: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

102

1.31, β =-.41, p = .01) whereas the BRIEF Metacognition index did not (b = -.30, β =-.12,

p = .46). For the ASD group, parent EF ratings predicted 54.2% of the variance in social

competence (r2 = .54). Coefficients showed age (b = -1.52, β =-.22, p = .05) accounted

for significance variance in Social Competence but not IQ (b = .07, β =.08, p = .44).

Behavioural Regulation (b = -1.62, β =-.58, p < .01) predicted significant variance in

social competence but Metacognition (b = -.49, β =-.17, p = .21) did not for the ASD

group.

5.7 Hypothesis 5: SEL will partially mediate the effect of EF on social competence.

Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were calculated to test for

relationships among SEL and EF domains (see Table 7 and 8).

For the control group, two SEL domains were significantly correlated with one EF

domain. The EF domain of Visual Executive Control was significantly correlated with

SEL domains Social Understanding, r(48) = .31, p = .03 and Nonverbal Awareness r(48)

= .31, p = .03. In terms of parent ratings, Behavioural Regulation was negatively

correlated with both Social Understanding r(48) = -.49, p < .01 and Nonverbal

Awareness, r(48) = -.34, p = .02. As such, children in the control group who performed

well on Visual Executive Control also performed well on Social Understanding and

Nonverbal Awareness. Similarly, children in the control group who were rated as having

less executive dysfunction on the BRIEF Behavioural Regulation domain performed

better on both SEL domains of Social Understanding and Nonverbal Awareness.

Affective decision making was significantly correlated with one SEL domain - Empathy

Page 114: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

103

(r(48) = .31, p = .04) and affective decision making had even higher correlation with

parent ratings of empathy (r(48) = .61, p < .01).

For children with ASD, both SEL domains were significantly correlated with two

EF domains. Social Understanding was significantly correlated with Cognitive

Regulation, r(48) = .49, p < .01 and Behavioural Regulation, r(48) = -.56, p < .01.

Nonverbal Awareness was also significantly correlated with Cognitive Regulation, r(48)

= .39, p < .01 and Behavioural Regulation, r(48) = -.41, p < .01. As such, children with

ASD who performed well on Cognitive Regulation and Behavioural Regulation also

performed well on Social Understanding and Nonverbal Awareness. However, children's

performance on either SEL domain was not associated with parent ratings of EF.

The partial mediation hypothesis was not tested because the basic conditions

required to determine the presence of a partial mediation relationship were not met for

either group. For a partial mediation relationship to exist, EF and SEL must be

significantly correlated with each other and with Social Competence (Baron & Kenny,

1986; Holmbeck, 1997). Although there are significant correlations between SEL and EF

domains in both groups, SEL and EF were not significantly correlated with Social

Competence. In the control group, SEL was significantly correlated with Social

Competence but not EF. In the ASD group, EF was significantly correlated with Social

Competence whereas SEL was not.

Page 115: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

104

6.0 Discussion

The development of social competence is critical to overall mental health and is

associated with a range of positive outcomes throughout the lifespan. The results of this

study add significantly to our understanding of social competence in children with high

functioning ASD in a number of important ways. First, the results highlight the

variability in social competence, SEL, and EF in children with ASD. Second, the results

emphasize the relationships between multiple antecedents of social competence in terms

of SEL and EF domains in both children with ASD and neurotypical children. Third,

the results of this study demonstrate the unique contributions of performance-based and

parent-rated measures of EF in understanding social competence. Finally, this study

identifies EF as an important predictor of social competence for children with ASD.

6.1 Social Competence

Based on the previous literature, it was hypothesized that children with ASD

would score significantly lower than neurotypical controls on measures of social

competence. The results indicated that children with ASD were rated significantly lower

on both the Vineland-II Socialization domain and the SSIS, with their mean scores on

these measures consistent with other studies (Hus et al., 2013; Klin et al.2007; Lee &

Park, 2007; Lerner et al., 2011; Liss et al., 2001; Lopata et al., 2013; Matchullis, 2012;

Schohl et al., 2013; Weiss et al., 2013). The ASD group, on average, were rated within

the Low to Moderately Low range on the Vineland and within the Below Average range

on the SSIS. Moreover, one-half of the children with ASD scored at least two standard

deviations below the normative mean on the Vineland Socialisation domain and one third

Page 116: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

105

scored at least two standard deviations below the normative mean on SSIS. These

findings are consistent with significant clinical impairment in these social skills areas in

the ASD group. It also highlights the magnitude of the deficits in daily adaptive

socialisation and social skills despite intact IQ for many children with ASD. In contrast,

the control group, on average, scored in the Adequate range on the Vineland and in the

Average range on the SSIS; no children in the control group scored two standard

deviations below the normative mean on either measure. These findings are consistent

with no significant clinical impairment being identified in the control group.

The children with ASD also had a wide range of scores on the social competence

measures. Individual scores on the Vineland ranged from the Low to the Adequate range

on the Vineland and from the Well Below Average to the Average range on the SSIS.

While this range of social competence in children with ASD has not received much

attention in the literature, it is noteworthy. This finding suggests that although a diagnosis

of ASD entails shared core diagnostic features of social dysfunction, it merely

characterizes the diagnostic parameters of the disorder, and does not convey an individual

child's range of social functioning. This is important clinically, because many

intervention studies target aspects of social deficits or problem behaviours within this

population, without first assessing children's’ social functioning ability. It may thus be

more helpful to assess children's’ specific social functioning as part of intervention

planning, as opposed to targeting interventions generally based on the diagnosis of ASD

alone. Moreover, as demonstrated in this study, assessing the child’s social adaptation

(e.g., using the Vineland) and social skills (e.g., using the SSIS) allows for a more

comprehensive examination of social competence in children with ASD. This

Page 117: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

106

information can then be used to inform individual interventions or measure changes in

social functioning throughout childhood. This is particularly important given the

evidence that the gap in social functioning between children with ASD and their peers

typically widens with age.

6.2 Demographic factors and social competence

This study explored whether other child-demographic factors - age, IQ,

medication use, and age at diagnosis - were associated with social competence. Standard

scores and raw scores were used to explore the relationship between age and social

competence. Raw scores tell whether children perform more skills with age whereas

standard scores provide scores in relation to other same-aged children from the

standardization sample, thus accounting for typical increases in skills. Consistent with the

literature, the results of this study demonstrated that social competence decreased with

age for children with ASD, (Klin et al., 2007; Lee & Park, 2007; Perry et al., 2009;

Szatmari et al., 2003). For this group, decreasing standard scores on the Vineland

Socialisation Domain with age likely reflected the widening gap throughout elementary

years between children with ASD and neurotypical peers (Klin et al., 2007), rather than a

loss of skill with age, as indicated by the positive, albeit non-statistically significant,

correlation between age and raw scores. For the control group, the significant positive

correlation between age and raw scores on the Vineland suggests that neurotypical

children gain considerable social abilities as they age. However, there was no significant

relationship between age and standard scores on the Vineland, indicating that age did not

account for significant variance in social adaptive functioning for the control group and

Page 118: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

107

that increasing raw scores likely reflected typical developmental gains in social

behaviours.

In terms of the SSIS, there was no association between age and social skills using

either raw or standard scores for children with ASD. For the control group, both raw and

standard scores on the SSIS demonstrated that social skills increased significantly with

age. The SSIS does not provides standard scores for each year of age, as does the

Vineland, and the same standard scores are provided for all children across the age range

in this study. Given this, the Vineland may be a better measure of social competence than

the SSIS for researchers and clinicians specifically interested in developmental changes

in social skills across this age group.

Despite a wide range of IQ scores from Low Average to Superior, there was no

significant correlation found between IQ and social competence for children with ASD in

this study. At first glance these results may seem contradictory to the previous robust

finding that intelligence is associated with social outcomes in this population (Baird,

2014; Howlin, Goode, Hutton & Rutter, 2004; Howlin & Moss, 2012; Volkmar et al.,

2005). However, this association may only hold true when researchers include children

with a range of intellectual ability. Studies that restricted their participants to children

with IQs over 85 have not found intelligence to be associated with social functioning on

the Vineland Socialisation domain (Anderson et al., 2009; Howlin et al., 2004;

Kenworthy et al., 2010; Klin et al., 2007; Liss et al., 2001; Lopata et al., 2013; Szatmari

et al., 2000) or parent rated social skills (Vickerstaff et al., 2007). Furthermore,

longitudinal studies have also failed to find a relationship between intelligence in

childhood and social functioning in adulthood for those ASD and average or above

Page 119: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

108

average IQ (Gillepsie-Lynch, 2012; Howlin et al., 2004; 2014). The results of this study

thus support this notion that social competence is independent of intelligence for children

with ASD and intact IQ. This suggests that IQ may not have any added benefit to social

competence once a certain level is reached and that the gap in social competence in

children with ASD relative to their peers is not impacted by intelligence.

For the control group, IQ was significantly correlated with social skills as

measured by the SSIS but not the Vineland, with SSIS scores decreasing with higher IQ.

These results are consistent with other studies using the SSIS or its predecessor - the SRS

(McKown et al., 2007) and are also found in the standardization sample of the SSIS. The

reason for this apparent trend is not well-established. It may be that parents of more

intelligent children have higher social expectations or underestimate positive social

behaviours (Lupowski, 1989). Nevertheless, as mentioned previously, researchers may

want to consider using the Vineland instead of the SSIS as a measure of social

competence as the ratings on the Vineland Socialisation domain do not appear to be

associated with IQ in neurotypical children.

IQ was not used as a covariate in this study for several reasons. First, it had

different associations across groups, and thus does not fulfill methodological requirement

as a covariate. Second, IQ was not associated with either measures of social competence

for the ASD group and was not a significant contributor to social competence in the

regression analyses. Furthermore, researchers advise against using IQ as covariate in

studies involving neurodevelopmental populations, and particularly when other

neurocognitive factors, such as EF, are being examined (Dennis et al., 2009; Miller &

Chapman, 2001). Dennis et al. (2009) argue that IQ is always confounded by and

Page 120: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

109

inseparable from the ASD condition, and that controlling for IQ removes variability in

the outcome measure that is directly related to the underlying skill or component being

measured.

Consistent with prior studies, 37% of children in the ASD group in this study

were taking psychostimulant medications. Such medications are typically prescribed to

reduce symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity and attention (Nickels et al., 2008). The

percentage of children with ASD taking psychostimulant medication in this study is

similar to other studies, which have reported rates between 31% and 52% (Nickels et al.,

2008; Pearson et al., 2012; Tureck et al., 2013). The high prevalence of medication use in

this group may reflect the notion that inattention and impulsivity are a common feature of

the disorder (Frazier et al., 2001; Sinzig et al., 2009; van der Meer et al., 2012). In

general, medication use was not significantly correlated with performance on any SEL or

EF task, or on any parent ratings of EF and social competence on the Vineland. However,

children with ASD on medication were rated lower on the SSIS social skills by their

parents when compared to children with ASD not taking medication. Prior research

looking at the effects of medication on social skills for children with ASD has been

inconsistent. Some studies demonstrated that children with ASD on psychostimulant

medications had poorer social skills (Turygin, Matson & Tureck, 2013; Yerys et al.,

2009) whereas others found increased social skills (Handen et al., 2010; Jahromi et al.,

2009; Pearson et al., 2013; RUPP, 2005). One explanation for these discrepancies is that

measures of social functioning vary across the studies, and that different measures are

likely tapping different aspects of social functioning. For example, the SSIS focuses more

on aspects of compliance and cooperation than the Vineland, and these abilities are often

Page 121: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

110

decreased in children who have difficulties with attention and impulsivity (Barkley &

Benton, 2013). This is consistent with recent findings that children with ASD are often

prescribed stimulant medications to manage externalising behaviours (Tureck, Matson,

Turygin & Macmillan, 2013).

Consistent with previous studies, children with ASD in this study were diagnosed,

on average, at age 6 (range: 3 to 10 years) (Daniels & Mandell, 2012; Maenner et al.,

2013; Mandell, Novak & Zubritsky, 2005). As some researchers suggest that children

who receive ASD diagnoses at earlier ages suffer from more impairment than children

diagnosed later, (Daniels & Mandell, 2012; Maenner, 2013), age of diagnosis was

included in this study as a child-based demographic factor. To the researcher’s

knowledge, this is the first study to explore the relationship between age at diagnosis and

multiple measures of social abilities in high functioning children with ASD. Age at

diagnosis was not significantly correlated with social competence for children with ASD

in this study, a finding inconsistent with previous studies (e.g., Daniels & Mandell, 2012;

Maenner, 2013). While the reasons for this are unclear, it may be that age of diagnosis

matters more for children with ASD with lower IQ and less for children with ASD with

average or above average IQ. To further assess this, researchers could study the

relationship between age at diagnosis and social competence in children with ASD across

a broader range of IQ. Alternatively, this finding may reflect the focus on social ability

rather than deficits in this study. In the previously cited studies, age at diagnosis was

related to severity level, as measured by number of diagnostic criteria met in all three

diagnostic categories (social interactions, communication, and restricted

interests/repetitive behaviours). While it may be that children diagnosed earlier have

Page 122: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

111

more clinical symptoms, it may also be that those diagnosed at a later age have specific

difficulties which are equally as disadvantageous to social functioning. For example,

although children who are diagnosed at a later age tend to have fewer symptomatic

deficits, they are more likely to have specific deficits in conversational ability,

idiosyncratic speech, and relating to peers (Daniels & Mandell, 2012; Maenner, 2013),

which may clearly have a significant impact on social competence. The results of this

study thus demonstrate the importance of assessing social competence at the level of the

individual when designing interventions for children with ASD.

In summary, for the ASD group, age at diagnosis and IQ were not significantly

correlated with social competence; medication use correlated significantly with one

measure of social skills. In the control group, the results varied with the measure of social

functioning. The latter results suggest that neurotypical children's’ social adaptive

behaviours, as measured by the Vineland - which includes how well they adapt to and

cope with different demands and situations, how well they relate to others and manage in

play or extracurricular activities – was not associated with age or IQ. Neurotypical

children's social skills as measured by the SSIS – which includes behaviours related to

cooperation, responsibility, communication, engagement with others and self control –

increased with age and decreased with higher IQ.

6.3 Social Emotional Learning

Children with ASD performed significantly below the children in the control

group on most measures of SEL. Children with ASD scored lower on measures assessing

the ability to recognize emotional tone from voice, understand the perspectives of others,

Page 123: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

112

interpret social cues in communication, and find solutions to common social conflicts and

situations.

The significant differences in performance between the two groups on the four

SEL measures is consistent with the plethora of studies that have demonstrated that

children with ASD, as a group, perform worse than typically developing peers on

measures of ToM (Nilsen & Fecica, 2011; Wellman & Liu 2004) and pragmatic language

(de Villiers, Szatmari & Yang, 2014; Lam, 2014). Although the group differences

observed in this study on the ToM task seem to support the notion of a general deficit in

the ability of children with ASD to take others' perspective, these results should be

interpreted with caution since almost one-half of the children (41%) in the ASD group

successfully answered all ToM questions. While the scores may be inflated by the

relatively low demands of the Strange Stories task used in this study, high success rates

have also been reported in other studies in this population using multiple ToM tasks of

varying complexity (Begeer, Malle, Nieuwland & Keysar, 2010; Peterson et al., 2009).

Thus, the general usefulness of including targeted ToM training in interventions for

children with ASD (e.g., Stichter et al., 2012) might be questioned if a large percentage

of children do not actually demonstrate deficits in this area (Rao et al. 2008; Reichow and

Volkmar 2010). However, given the range of success of children with ASD on the ToM

task in this study, particularly as some children with ASD were not able to answer any

questions correctly, clinicians may want to consider administering measures, such as the

Strange Stories task, prior to planning individualised interventions. This would allow

clinicians to determine which children would be more likely to benefit from targeted

ToM training. In terms of problem solving, the findings are consistent with the literature

Page 124: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

113

that children with ASD perform more poorly than neurotypical children (Demopoulos et

al., 2013; Meyer et al., 2006; Ziv, Hadad & Khateeb, 2013). These findings were robust

across age, stimuli used, and means of evaluation. For example, children with ASD

performed more poorly than neurotypical peers when responding to questions based on

story vignettes, pictures or videos and regardless of whether number of solutions,

effectiveness of responses or style of response (e.g. passive or assertive) was used as the

target score.

The finding that children in the ASD group in this study were significantly worse

at recognizing affect in voices than the control group is important for two reasons. First,

there is much less research in this area as most studies focus on facial affect recognition

in the ASD population. These results add support to the growing literature that children

with ASD have deficits in voice affect recognition (Lindner &Rosen, 2006; Oerlemans et

al., 2013; Peppe, McCann, Gibbon, O'Hare & Rutherford, 2007). These results are

inconsistent with a few studies that did not find differences between children with ASD

and neurotypical children (Mazefsky & Oswald, 2007; Paul et al., 2005). However, in

one of these studies (Paul et al., 2005) both groups obtained near-ceiling scores,

suggesting that the task used of matching voices to only two emotions might have been

too simple to differentiate between the groups. In contrast, using the more complex

DANVA task in this study may have led to better detection of differences between

children with ASD and controls. Second, by using parallel tasks of both facial and voice

affect recognition, this study showed that children with ASD were able to adequately

identify varying levels of the four basic emotions (happiness, sadness, anger and fear) in

faces but not in voices. This highlights the importance of examining various modalities

Page 125: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

114

when assessing emotion recognition in children with ASD. Future research should

consider examining voice affect recognition in addition to other less explored areas, such

as recognizing affect in gait and posture (Doody & Bull, 2013). It may be that there are

specific rather than global deficits in affect recognition in ASD which are worthy of

further study.

Children with ASD in this study performed similarly to the control group on

measures of facial affect recognition and empathy. These findings support the notion that

children with ASD can accurately identify emotions through facial expressions. Despite

ample research dedicated to this topic, there are inconsistencies throughout the literature.

Some studies found significant deficits in facial affect recognition (Lindner & Rosen,

2006) whereas others have not (Robel et al. 2004). Several recent reviews have suggested

that the group differences found in the literature may be related to the particular emotions

or modalities used in the studies rather than a global deficit in affect recognition for

children with ASD (Harms et al., 2013; Uljarevic & Hamilton, 2012). Findings from this

study support this idea, in that children in the ASD group were able to identify the four

basic emotions as well as the control group. As such, children with ASD do not appear to

have a global deficit in facial affect recognition. Future studies may want to include more

facial expressions, such as jealousy and pride, to explore whether facial affect recognition

is still intact with more complex emotions. In this study, the DANVA was chosen as the

measure of facial affect because it allows for of the identification of varying levels of

emotional intensity. However, the DANVA also provides children with four choices of

possible emotions from which to identify the facial affect. It may be that this structure

helped children be more successful on this task. As such, this ability to identify emotions

Page 126: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

115

may not be able to translate to more natural situations, where this choice-component is

not typically provided. From a clinical perspective, since children with ASD are able to

recognize facial emotions when provided the vocabulary, it may be important to offer this

same support in other settings such as school. For example, when assisting with a social

situation or teaching children with ASD, it may be more effective to ask 'Is Jimmy

feeling sad or angry' rather than 'how is Jimmy feeling'. The use of static pictures,

however, has been criticized for a lack of ecological validity in studies of facial affect

recognition, but this criticism may not apply to the ASD population. For example, while

recent studies demonstrated that neurotypical children change their visual scanning

patterns based on the nature of the stimuli, children with ASD maintained the same

viewing pattern regardless of whether looking at static pictures or being involved in face

to face interactions (Horlin et al., 2013). In addition, children with ASD rely on specific

facial features to identify emotions whereas neurotypical children rely on more global,

configural-based strategies (e.g., Behrmann et al., 2006). The inclusion of eye tracking

methods in future studies may help to delineate further the factors associated with facial

affect recognition in children with ASD.

Interestingly, the ASD group scored similarly to the control group on the empathy

measure in this study. The general consensus in the literature is that individuals with

ASD lack empathy (Lai, Lombardo, Chakrabarti, Baron-Cohen, 2013; Wing, Gould &

Gillberg, 2011). For example, the DSM-IV specifically includes a lack of awareness or

failure to offer comfort when others are hurt or upset as diagnostic criteria for ASD

(APA, 2004). However, there are few studies dedicated to this topic and of those, the

results are inconsistent (Dziobek et al., 2007; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2013). The mixed

Page 127: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

116

results appear to be related to two factors. First, they may be due to the inclusion of

children with a range of IQ, since children with lower IQ tend to demonstrate more

impairment in empathy (Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse & Allen, 1998; Scambler et al.,

2007). Second, it may be related to the idea that different measures are tapping different

aspects of empathy. For example, there is reason to believe that the deficits for children

with ASD may lie in one type of empathy-cognitive empathy but not in another type of

empathy- affective empathy (Dziodek et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2010; Rogers et al., 2007;

Schwenck et al., 2012). Whereas most research with this population focuses on cognitive

empathy – the ability to infer someone else's feelings, the current study specifically

examined affective empathy – the congruent response to someone else's emotional

distress. As ToM tasks are often purported to measure components of cognitive empathy,

an affective empathy measure was chosen for this study to reduce overlap between the

ToM and empathy measure. Results from this study support the recent conceptualisations

that children with ASD have specific rather than global deficits in empathy, where they

are stronger on tasks of affective empathy than cognitive empathy (Scheeren et al., 2013).

In sum, compared to the control group, children with ASD generated less effective

solutions to everyday problems, had more difficulty taking the perspective of others and

interpreting social cues, and were less adept at both recognizing emotions by tone of

voice and appraising the effectiveness of a response based on social context. However,

they were equally skilled at recognizing basic facial emotions and congruently

responding to others' distress or emotions.

Page 128: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

117

6.4 Executive Functions

This study contributed to the literature by using both performance-based and

parent measures of EF to provide a more comprehensive examination of executive

functioning in children with ASD. The ASD group scored more poorly than the control

group on all but two performance-based measures of EF. More specifically, children with

ASD performed significantly worse on visual attention, auditory attention, visual working

memory, planning, and auditory inhibition. While prior literature has demonstrated mixed

results, comparisons are difficult due the wide range of tasks and stimuli used to tap

similar underlying EF skills (Pellicano, 2012). In addition, most previous studies focus on

isolated EF tasks. Nevertheless, the results of this study are consistent with others which

have simultaneously examined a range of EF in this same age range (Corbett et al., 2009;

Narcizi et al., 2013; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2010; van Rijn et al., 2013). Moreover, the

finding of significant differences between groups when the direct assessment of EF was

conducted under optimal conditions (i.e. limited distractions; breaks when needed; one-

on-one with an adult) is clearly suggestive of significant deficits in EF in ASD.

The use of both auditory and visual modalities in this study allows for a more

comprehensive look at attention and inhibition in children with ASD. Children with ASD

performed more poorly on both tasks of sustained attention. Although diminished

attention to social stimuli is well-documented for children with ASD (Riby & Hancock,

2008), the present results suggest that difficulties with sustained attention are also

evident with non-social stimuli presented in both visual and oral formats. In terms of

inhibition, the ASD group performed worse than the control group on auditory inhibition

but not visual inhibition, findings consistent with another recent study using the same

Page 129: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

118

task with boys with ASD (Narzisi et al., 2013). It is possible that the ASD group

performed well on the visual inhibition task because it was computer-administered as

researchers have reported that non-social administration of tasks attenuate deficits found

on person-administered tasks (Ozonoff & Strayer, 2001; Russell, Hala & Hill, 2003).

However, since there were significant group differences on the two other computer-

administered tasks in this study, this factor likely did not account for the findings. The

better performance on visual inhibition is more likely related to the complexity of the

tasks. The visual inhibition task is simpler in that it requires only one response

(withholding a dominant response).whereas the auditory inhibition task is a dual

processing task that requires two responses (withholding a dominant verbal response

while saying the opposite word). These results are consistent with recent studies that have

reported differential performance across task complexity for children with ASD

(Sanderson & Allen, 2013) and provide support for selective inhibitory deficits in

children with ASD.

This study included a measure of affective decision making, an area of

functioning commonly referred to as 'hot' EF. On this task, complexity was decreased by

using two decks instead of four (Crone et al., 2005) to reduce possible effects of random

responding often reported in young typically developing children (Duijvenvoorden,

Jansen, Bredman, &. Huizenga, 2012; Lambek et al., 2010; Skogli et al., 2013). Marbles

were used to provide a visual representation of the wins and losses to reduce demands on

working memory (Crone et al., 2005). Although few studies have examined affective

decision making in children with ASD, the lack of differences found between the ASD

and control group on this task is consistent with the sparse literature with both children

Page 130: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

119

(Faja, 2013; South et al., 2006, 2011) and adolescents (Johnson et al., 2006; Yechiam et

al., 2010). However, these results should not necessarily be considered evidence of intact

affective decision making in this population for several reasons. The whole premise of

this task is that children who have better affective decision making are able to

strategically adjust their decisions based on previous wins and losses in highly motivating

situation that have an element of risk. However, other studies have demonstrated that

children and teens with ASD are able to obtain similar overall scores as typical peers

despite using a frequent switching strategy not related to outcome or feedback (Johnson

et al., 2006; South et al., 2008; Yechiam et al., 2010) and having stronger autonomic

responses to feedback (Faja et al., 2013). Additionally, the role of motivation is also a

critical aspect of this task as it is measuring the ability to make decisions when there is

some sort of personal gain. It has been postulated that the instructions to make choices for

a donkey rather than for themselves might not be motivating enough (Skogli et al., 2013).

However, to increase motivation in this study, participants were told they would receive a

reward if they were successful at this task (in fact they all received a prize after this task).

Behavioural observations of high frustration and repeated comments made by children

with ASD when losses occurred suggest that this element of deception provided a strong

motivational component. Therefore, given the strong negative reactions the researcher

observed in participants throughout this task and the previous findings that adolescents

with ASD had similar overall scores to their peers despite not adjusting their responses

based on the feedback, it is possible that equal performance between groups in this study

was not actually indicative of their ability to make better decisions in emotionally-

charged situations. Analysing strategy use while measuring physiological reactions

Page 131: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

120

during an affective decision making task may afford more valuable information regarding

the impact of emotion on decision making and shed some light on the nature of 'hot' EF in

this population.

Parent-ratings of EF can provide useful information regarding the application of

EF under more emotion-laden circumstances and were examined in this study. The

results indicated that parents of children with ASD reported significantly more deficits

compared to the control group on the BRIEF, which is consistent with the literature

(Kalbfleisch &Loughan, 2012; Kenworthy et al., 2008; 2009; 2010; Rosenthal et al.,

2013; Troyb et al., 2013). By virtue of reliance on typical day to day behaviours, the

BRIEF targets multiple abilities and thus has been criticized for its inability to isolate

discrete EF processes while also possibly measuring non-EF behaviours (Isquith et al.,

2013; Kenworthy et al., 2009). Although the results of this study may thus not provide

information about specific EF impairments, it does demonstrate the striking deficits in the

application of EF in daily tasks for children with ASD. Studies of EF in children should

consider using parent ratings of EF along with performance-based measures as they are

likely tapping two different yet important aspects of EF. Performance-based measures of

EF typically capture optimal performance and provide information regarding efficiency

of various self-regulatory processes whereas parent-ratings provide more information

regarding the engagement and application of these processes in day-to-day and less

structured situations (Toplak et al., 2013).

In summary, by using both performance-based and parent ratings of EF, this study

found that children with ASD had deficits in specific aspects of EF as well as significant

difficulties regulating their own behaviours in day to day activities and achieving age-

Page 132: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

121

expected goals in daily tasks. Specifically, the ASD group was significantly worse than

the control group at sustaining attention to verbal and auditory information, strategic

planning, holding visual information in mind for short periods, and simultaneously

inhibiting and replacing dominant responses. Children with ASD performed well on a

single-level inhibition task and an affective decision-making task, although further study

is required to examine whether these findings reflect intact abilities in these areas. Parent

ratings of EF demonstrated a breadth of deficits across all areas of daily functioning. The

results also indicated considerable variability on both SEL and EF measures in the group

of children with ASD. Although the ASD group performed significantly poorer on most

measures of SEL and EF and had a higher percentage of children within the clinically

impaired range, it is noteworthy that some children with ASD performed as well as the

control group on many SEL and EF measures. For example, the maximum scores were

similar for the ASD and control groups on all SEL measures, except pragmatic language

and social problem solving. In terms of EF, the maximum scores were similar in both

groups on all measures, except planning and auditory inhibition. Since different children

obtained minimum and maximum scores on different tasks, the higher scores in the ASD

group are not due to the same subgroup of children performing better across all tasks.

6.5 Model of Social Competence

It was hypothesized that EF and SEL domains would be significantly correlated

with each other and with social competence for children with ASD and the control group.

Furthermore, it was hypothesized that both SEL and EF would predict social competence

and that the relationship between EF and social competence would be partially mediated

by SEL. Separate exploratory factor analysis was conducted to examine the relationship

Page 133: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

122

amongst SEL and EF tasks to determine whether proposed domains could be used in an

overall model of social competence. Since performance-based and parent-rated EF are

arguably distinct measures of EF with weak inter-correlations, they were included in

separate models (Toplak et al., 2013).

6.5.1 Factor analysis of SEL domains. An existing model of social competence

was initially used as a basis for examining SEL skills (McKown et al., 2009; 2013.). The

McKown model proposes three SEL domains: Nonverbal Awareness, Social Meaning,

and Social Reasoning. In this model, Nonverbal Awareness is assessed using tasks that

measure affect recognition, Social Meaning is assessed using tasks that measure

understanding and interpretation of others’ communication, and Social Reasoning is

assessed using measures of social problem solving. McKown et al. specifically used

measures of pragmatic language, ToM, empathy, and the Test of Problem Solving with

their community sample, and measures of pragmatic language, social language

comprehension, and social vignettes with their clinical sample. In a recent study,

McKown et al. (2013) provided data to support a two-factor model of SEL. In this model,

facial, voice, gait and posture affect recognition made up the Nonverbal Awareness

domain, while measures of pragmatic language, social problem solving, and ToM make

up the Social Understanding domain.

The results of this study support McKown’s two-factor model of SEL for the ASD

group. The results of factor analysis for the ASD group produced two factors: Nonverbal

Awareness – the ability to infer others' emotions based on nonverbal cues; Social

Understanding – the ability to identify, monitor and appraise the effectiveness of

responses to social problems based on contextual cues social and the understanding of

Page 134: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

123

others' intentions. The results indicated that for children with ASD, the abilities to

interpret social information and to effectively resolve social problems are intricately

linked and should be considered as one domain of SEL. Although the social problem

solving measure targets the ability to reason about social situations and conflict

situations, it also likely involves the interpretation of the intentions underlying others’

words and actions, and thus having these tasks under one domain instead of two is likely

more reflective of the joint processes involved in understanding social information.

Additionally, results suggests that these two SEL domains are also robust for children

with ASD and that they can also be reliably measured using tasks commonly found in

clinical settings.

For the control group, the results of the factor analysis produce a three-factor

model: Nonverbal Awareness – comprised of facial and voice affect recognition; Social

Understanding - comprised of pragmatic language and social problem solving; Empathy -

comprised of the affective empathy task. Consistent with the McKown model, pragmatic

language and social problem solving were retained within the same domain for the

control group. Contrary to the McKown model, ToM did not correlate well with the other

SEL tasks and therefore was not included in any domain. It is unclear why the ToM task

did not correlate well with the other SEL tasks for the control group. It could be that the

task was simplified too much by using only five vignettes from Happe's Strange Stories,

in comparison to the twelve used in the McKown studies (2007; 2009; 2013), as

evidenced by the large proportion of children in the control group (86%) who obtained

perfect scores on this task. Nevertheless, the pragmatic language task also required the

ability to understand others' intentions. McKown’s two-factor model including Social

Page 135: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

124

Awareness and Social Understanding was also initially a good fit for the control group in

this study, but the decision was made to retain a third factor. A third domain comprising

the empathy task was retained for the control group because the model with the Empathy

domain was superior to the model without, and because of the importance of empathy in

most conceptualisations of social competence. It is important to note that the Empathy

factor was retained with some reservations, however, given the low correlations between

Empathy and the other two factor domains. In their latest analysis, McKown et al. (2013)

did not indicate why they excluded empathy as a critical contributor to children's ability

to interpret the meaning of social information, but it is possible that empathy would be

better categorized as part of another important contributor to social competence.

Alternatively, it is possible that the self-report questionnaire used in this study assessed

the child's beliefs and expectations about the experience of empathy, which likely reflects

societal norms and social desirability (Michalskaa, Kinzler & Decety, 2013). However,

given the high correlations with parent ratings of their child's empathy, this is unlikely.

In summary, this study demonstrated that two domains – Nonverbal Awareness

and Social Understanding – are significant contributors to SEL for both children with

ASD and neurotypical children. These two dimensions have repeatedly demonstrated

their importance to SEL and should be included when assessing children's social

comprehension skills. Empathy also appears potentially important, particularly with

neurotypical children.

6.5.2 Factor analysis of EF domains. In this study, EF is conceptualised as a

multidimensional construct that includes multiple lower order skills related to self-

regulatory processes (McCloskey, 2011; Zhou et al., 2012). An integrated model of EF -

Page 136: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

125

as the broader domain of subcomponents and processes that exert control over a person's

cognitive, behavioural and emotional regulation - was chosen because of recent evidence

demonstrating the importance of considering the separate but integral contributions of

cognitive, inhibitory and emotional aspects of EF to a child's social functioning (Barkley,

1997; Blair, Zelazo & Greenberg, 2005; Hongwanishkul et al., 2005; Jahromi et al, 2008;

McCloskey, 2011). Such an integrated approach to EF expands upon the McKown model

of social competence and includes a wider conceptualisation of self-regulation, subsumed

under the realm of EF.

The hypothesized EF model proposed that Cognitive Regulation (attention,

working memory, and planning), Behavioural Regulation (inhibition), and Emotional

Regulation (affective decision making) were fundamental components of EF. The results

of the factor analysis for the ASD group produced these three domains, with some

modifications: Cognitive Regulation – comprised of visual attention, auditory attention

and visual working memory; Behavioural Regulation - comprised of auditory inhibition

time, auditory inhibition errors and planning; Emotional Regulation – comprised of

affective decision making. There are several possible explanations for why the measures

from the Behavioural Regulation domain were not associated as expected. First, the

visual inhibition task was dropped as it did not correlate well with any other measures. As

mentioned previously, this task, which only required withholding a dominant response,

was a more simple inhibition task and was more associated with the visual attention task,

which also used the same computerized stimuli. It may be that this more simple inhibition

task relied more on visual attention than inhibiting a response because it did not require

the second step of replacing with a non-dominant response. Second, planning was

Page 137: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

126

included in this domain as it correlated most with tasks of auditory inhibition. The

planning measure used in this study - Tower of London - is generally considered a more

complex EF task and is robustly associated with inhibition in adults (Gonzalez et al.,

2010; Miyake et al., 2000). However, the relationships between these EF tasks are

inconclusive for children, with some studies reporting significant correlations (Lehto et

al., 2003) but not others (Bull, Espy & Senn

, 2004). Using the same measures as in this

study, Lehto and colleagues (2003) found a significant association between planning and

auditory inhibition while also failing to find a significant association between planning

and auditory attention. Results suggest that children's performance on the planning task

may have been related to their ability to delay their initial response (i.e. inhibition) in

order to plan their moves (Albert & Steinberg, 2011). Finally, the identification of

affective decision making as its own distinct but related domain of EF is consistent with

the sparse literature examining affective decision making in children with ASD (South

and colleagues, 2011).

In summary, three major domains of EF were distinguished in ASD: (1) those

classically related to higher order cognition or cognitive control; (2) those related to

inhibiting behaviour; and (3) those related to the coordination and control of emotional

behaviour, supporting the notion that Cognitive, Behavioural and Emotional Regulation

are different dimensions of EF. The findings further support the popular position that EF

should be considered simultaneously uniform and diverse rather than as a single entity

(Lehto et al., 2003; Miyake et al., 2000). The findings that performance on seven EF

tasks was best accounted for by three separate domains represents diversity. The finding

that there were significant correlations among the three EF domains represents unity.

Page 138: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

127

Importantly, the findings indicated that the study of these three EF domains can be

applied to children with ASD. In addition, the examination of EF through the domains of

Cognitive, Behavioural and Emotional Regulation may allow for integration of other

important areas related to children's self-regulation, such as temperament, emotional

intelligence, and effortful control (Berkman et al., 2012; Hofmann et al., 2012; Jahromi et

al., 2013; Rueda et al., 2005; Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013; Zhou et al., 2012).

Importantly, the separate domains of EF may allow for more accessible intervention

targets and research is clearly needed to determine whether these domains respond to

targeted treatment in ASD.

The three hypothesized EF domains did not emerge from factor analysis in the

control group. Instead, a three-factor model consisting of Visual Executive Control –

made up of visual attention, visual working memory, and visual inhibition; Inhibition -

made up of auditory inhibition time and errors; and Auditory Attention – made up

auditory attention, was retained for the control group. The factor domains likely emerged

differently for the control group for several reasons. First, visual attention and auditory

attention were not significantly correlated. The findings that almost one-half the control

group (47%) obtained perfect scores on the auditory attention task compared to none on

the visual attention task also suggest that they were tapping different aspects of attention.

Since the TEC battery is relatively new, there are no normative studies which have

compared performance on visual attention to alternate measures of auditory attention. For

the control group, the visual attention task was only significantly correlated with the other

two TEC measures using the same visual stimuli. The identification of a distinct Visual

Executive Control domain for the control group is, however, consistent with the results

Page 139: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

128

from the analyses of the standardization sample for the TEC. These three tasks all loaded

on the same factor in both the 5 to 7 year old and the 8 to 18 year old normative samples,

as they all appear to tap an element of selective visual attention.

Second, the planning task did not correlate significantly with any other EF

measures for the control group and thus was not retained in any domain. While it was

expected that planning would correlate highly with working memory because of the

evidence of an association between the ability to hold information in mind (working

memory) while thinking of different responses while planning (Ardila, 2013), there are

mixed results regarding the processes related to planning tasks in neurotypical children.

Albert & Steinberg (2011) examined age-related differences in strategy use on the Tower

of London in a large sample of 10 to 30 year olds and found that working memory and

inhibition were only associated with performance on the planning task in late adolescence

and adulthood. Furthermore, they suggested that the Tower of London task used in this

study may not be a good measure of effortful planning for neurotypical children because

the solutions for the more simple tasks require little planning and more complex tasks are

commonly approached using a trial and error strategy. To further clarify the nature of

effortful planning in both groups, studies comparing the strategies that children with ASD

and neurotypical children use during the planning task are needed. Whereas it has been

reported in the literature that total move scores is the most indicative of planning

(Culbertson & Zillner, 2001), this may be not an accurate representation of strategic

planning in children. Specifically, measuring how much time before making the first

move and how long it takes them to complete each trial would shed some light on this

issue. Furthermore, incorporating measures of initiation and execution time in addition to

Page 140: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

129

total correct score – as is commonly done in the adult literature (Gonzalez et al., 2010) -

will be very important to sorting out whether children with ASD rely on different

processes for successful completion of this task.

Lastly, the affective decision making task did not load on any factor for the

control group. The measure of affective decision making was included in the study to

specifically explore its role in social competence of children with ASD, as this literature

is sparse. However, the lack of significant correlations between affective decision making

and other EF measures for the control group is consistent with findings in the literature

that have also demonstrated clear distinctions between 'hot' and 'cool' EF tasks in

neurotypical children (Barraclough, Conroy, & Lee, 2004; Crone & vanderMolen, 2004;

Crone et al., 2005; Hooper, Luciana, Conklin & Yarger, 2004; Skogli et al., 2013; van

Liejenhorst, Crone & Bunge, 2006). It is noteworthy that children with ASD performed

as well as the control group on this task but that performance was only related to other

aspects of EF for the ASD group. This again suggests that children with ASD may rely

on different strategies to obtain similar overall scores. For example, it is possible that

children with ASD rely more on sustained attention and inhibition for completion of this

task. This would be consistent with recent findings that affective decision making was

significantly correlated with the parent rated behavioural inhibition scale for the ASD

group but not the control group (South et al., 2011). As mentioned previously, future

research should investigate strategy use and its relationship to other EF tasks in children

with ASD.

It is important to note that the generally low correlations between different EF

tasks in the current study for both the control and ASD groups are consistent with the

Page 141: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

130

literature. Low and statistically nonsignificant intercorrelations among EF tasks have

repeatedly been reported in children (Huizinga et al., 2006 Lehto et al., 2003),

adolescents (Lehto et al., 1996) and adults (Miyake et al., 2000). In their study of 108

neurotypical children, Lehto and colleagues (2003) reported low intercorrelations among

14 EF measures. These low correlations likely contribute to the myriad of discrepant or

inconclusive findings within the EF literature (Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013).

Furthermore, it is also fairly common to find differential group correlations when

comparing children with ASD to neurotypical peers (Demopoulus et al., 2013; Dyck et

al., 2006; Gepner et al., 2001; Oerlemans, 2013; Peppe et al., 2007), yet this is rarely the

focus of discussion. Unfortunately, most studies focus on group differences and do not

report correlations between measures (Barron-Linnankoski et al., 2014; Reinvall et al.,

2013; Semrud-Clikeman , Goldenring Fine & Bledsoe, 2013; van Rijn et al., 2013), or

else report combined group correlations (Demopoulus et al., 2013) or only those for the

ASD group (Faja et al., 2013).

6.5.3 Parent rated EF. As expected, the parent ratings of EF were not

significantly correlated with performance-based measures of EF for the control group

(Toplak et al., 2013) and only significantly correlated with two out of eight performance-

based EF measures (auditory attention and visual inhibition) for the ASD group. These

results emphasize the importance of using parent-ratings of EF as complementary rather

than equivalent measures of EF in research and clinical practice. Unfortunately, this has

not been common in the literature, with several studies relying solely on the BRIEF

(Kalbfleisch & Loughan; Rosenthal et al., 2013; Stichter et al., 2012), and others

referring to BRIEF scores as equivalent to the broad construct of EF (e.g., Jahromi, Bryce

Page 142: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

131

& Swanson, 2013; Kloosterman, Kelley, Parker & Craig, 2014; Scheeren, Koot &

Begeer, 2012). Interestingly, Gomez-Guerrero et al. (2011) found significant correlations

between BRIEF parent ratings and the TEC visual inhibition task in a group of eight to

twelve year old children with ADHD. Moreover, the finding that the TEC visual

inhibition task was significantly correlated with all BRIEF composite scores despite no

group differences on this measure, has several implications. It may be that the TEC visual

inhibition task is sensitive to the application of EF in daily tasks. However, it is also

possible that the basic abilities of sustaining visual attention and withholding a dominant

response are related to the overall regulation of behaviour in everyday context for

children.

A three-factor model of parent rating of EF was hypothesized, based on previous

clinical developmental research (Gioia et al., 2002). The results of the factor analysis

produced an identical solution of two domains for both groups: Metacognition and

Behavioural Regulation. The relationship between the three domains of the performance-

based EF and the two domains of the parent-rated EF was subsequently examined. For

the ASD group, the Behavioural Regulation domain was significantly correlated with the

performance-based Cognitive Regulation domain. This is an interesting finding as it

indicates that for children with ASD, performance on higher-order cognitive EF tasks is

related to everyday use of inhibitory control, flexibility, and modulation of emotions,

which has specific clinical implications for intervention. For the control group, there were

no significant correlations between performance-based and parent-rated EF domains.

Page 143: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

132

6.6 Relationship between SEL and EF Domains

The relationship between factor analysis derived SEL domains of Nonverbal

Awareness and Social Understanding and the EF domains of Cognitive Regulation,

Behaviour Regulation, and Emotion Regulation were explored for children with ASD.

The results demonstrated some significant relationships between SEL and EF domains in

the ASD group, findings consistent with the previous literature (Ahmed & Miller, 2011;

Oerlemans et al., 2013). Specifically, two SEL domains – Social Understanding and

Nonverbal Awareness – were significantly correlated with two EF domains - Cognitive

Regulation and Behavioural Regulation. For the children with ASD in this study, the

ability to sustain basic attention, keep things in mind while doing another task, and inhibit

primary responses in favour of a dominant one are related to how well they can recognize

affect in others, how effectively they can problem solve social situations, interpret social

cues, and take the perspective of others. The relationship between affective decision

making and SEL tasks has not been previously been explored in children with ASD.

While it was hypothesized that the Emotion Regulation domain would be significantly

related to SEL since 'hot' EF is purported to extend the EF construct to everyday social

decision making, which is typically conducted in the presence of motivational or

emotional influences (Zelazo & Carlson, 2012), this was not found. This lack of

relationship between Emotion Regulation and SEL domains of Social Understanding and

Nonverbal Awareness could be related to the strategies used to complete SEL tasks. It

may be that children with ASD approach social comprehension tasks in a more logical

and rational manner which are free from or absent of emotional or motivational

influences. This is clearly an area for further study.

Page 144: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

133

For the control group, the relationships between the factor analysis derived SEL

domains of Nonverbal Awareness, Social Understanding, and Empathy and the EF

domains of Visual Executive Control, Inhibition, and Auditory Attention were also

explored. This is the first study to explore the relationship between a wide range of SEL

and EF tasks in neurotypical children. Visual Executive Control (visual attention; visual

working memory; visual inhibition) was significantly correlated with Social

Understanding (pragmatic language; social problem solving) and Nonverbal Awareness

(facial and voice affect recognition). The results suggest that for neurotypical children,

the ability to sustain visual attention, keep images in mind and inhibit primary responses

are related to how well they can recognize affect in others, effectively problem solve

social situations, and interpret social cues. It is not clear why the Inhibition or Auditory

Attention domains did not correlate with any of the SEL domains. In contrast to children

with ASD, it is possible that socially competent neurotypical children do not engage or

rely on these skills to succeed in social comprehension tasks. These differences between

groups have been reported in the literature. In one study of 267 children and adolescents,

facial and voice affect recognition was significantly correlated with inhibition and verbal

working memory for the ASD but not the control group (Oerlemans et al., 2013).

The relationship between the factor analysis derived SEL and parent rated BRIEF

domains was also examined for both groups. For children with ASD, there were no

significant associations between either Metacognition or Behavioural Regulation and any

SEL domain. For the control group, the parent rated Behavioural Regulation domain was

significantly correlated with the SEL domains of Social Understanding and Nonverbal

Awareness. The contrast between the two groups is interesting. These results suggest that

Page 145: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

134

for neurotypical children, but not children with ASD, the ability to inhibit behaviour and

modulate emotions flexibly in everyday settings appears related to their understanding of

social situations and ability to recognize emotions.

6.7. SEL and EF in Relation to Social Competence

The results of this study supported the hypothesis that SEL and EF domains

would be significantly correlated in the ASD group. The further hypothesis that SEL and

EF domains would both be significantly correlated with social competence was only

partially supported. Specifically, two EF domains were related to Social Competence, but

there were no significant associations between any SEL domains and Social Competence

for the ASD group. The lack of association between SEL and Social Competence was

unexpected, particularly given the numerous publications outlining the specific deficits of

the ASD population in relation to SEL skills (Chasson & Jarossiewicz, 2014) and the

presumed importance of SEL skills in children's ability to navigate the social world that

is emphasized throughout the ASD literature. Furthermore, the lack of association

between SEL and social competence in children with ASD was surprising given the

robust outcomes in the McKown studies that found that all three SEL domains –

Nonverbal Awareness, Social Meaning, and Social Reasoning –predicted significant

variance in social competence across community and clinical samples. The measures

used in the McKown studies were comparable or identical to those used in the current

study and the age range of the participants in the McKown studies, albeit a bit wider was

similar to that of the current study. There are, however, some important differences. First

of all, McKown et al. had much larger sample sizes, including 186 community-recruited

neurotypical children and 119 clinic-referred children. Second, their clinical sample

Page 146: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

135

included children with various DSM-IV diagnoses, such as ADHD, ASD and mood and

anxiety disorders, as well as some who did not meet formal criteria for any clinical

diagnosis. As such, their clinical group did not demonstrate overall deficits in SEL as the

children in the current study; furthermore, the clinical group in the McKown studies

demonstrated higher mean scores than their own control group on ToM and pragmatic

language, and equivalent scores on all three measures of social problem solving. As such,

clinical implications are potentially less meaningful when children with different

diagnoses are grouped together. Therefore, since McKown et al. (2007; 2009) combined

children with ASD with other groups of children who did and did not have other DSM

diagnoses, their results may not accurately reflect clinical populations. Third, the

McKown studies (2009; 2013) used Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), a sophisticated

data analysis technique with potentially greater power to detect relationships that may be

missed, even in the same data set, using bivariate correlation analyses (Wilcox, 2001).

This is evident in McKown's own studies where nonverbal awareness was found to be

significantly predictive of social competence only after the data were re-analysed using

SEM. The sample size in the current study was simply too small for a more sophisticated

data analysis.

Although McKown et al. demonstrated the predictive utility of SEL in relation to

social competence in a diverse clinical sample, the relationship may not apply in the same

manner with a specific ASD sample. It is possible that the lack of relationship between

SEL and social competence emphasizes the distinction between 'knowing' which involves

the ability to provide correct answers on social comprehension tasks and 'doing' which

involves the application of these skills in more complex social situations. Since no

Page 147: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

136

information was gathered regarding the children's past or present interventions, it is also

possible that direct teaching of these skills, as is often done in both social skills groups

and individual treatment, prepared children to answer social comprehension questions,

thus providing children with the correct knowledge to correctly answer questions

regarding various social scenarios, but not the skills to apply the knowledge in social

situations. Alternatively, it may be that the children who were more successful on the

SEL tasks were using a more cognitive-based strategy, which helped them perform better

on the task but which may not necessarily translate to more real-world scenarios and

situations, as tapped by the social competence measures. Therefore, future research may

want to use role-play scenarios and more thoroughly evaluate what processes are used to

arrive at the answer. Finally, although SEL is not associated with the aspects of social

competence measured in this study, it may be related to other aspects of social

functioning. For example, it is possible that SEL is associated with problem behaviours.

Although this was not measured in this study, SEL domains predicted problem

behaviours in the first McKown (2007) study.

Two domains of EF - Cognitive Regulation and Emotion Regulation - were

significantly correlated with Social Competence in the ASD group. This finding suggests

children with ASD who have better selective and sustained attention, working memory,

and affective decision making are also more socially competent. There has been some

research examining the relationship between inhibition and social functioning in children

with ASD and most has measured deficits rather than competence. Some studies failed to

find a relationship between inhibition and social impairments (Bishopp & Norbury, 2005)

or social skills (Kentworthy et al., 2009) whereas other have reported a significant

Page 148: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

137

association between inhibition and social impairments (Joseph & Tager-Flusberg, 2004).

Certainly, the association between inhibition and behaviour problems has received the

most attention with neurotypical children and those with ADHD and there is some

consensus that poor inhibition is associated with behaviour problems for these

populations (Nigg et al., 1998; Tilman, Brocki, Sorensen & Lundervold, 2013; White,

Jarrett & Ollendick, 2013). The results of this study add to the literature by demonstrating

that although children with ASD performed significantly worse than the control group on

measures of Behavioural Regulation, this deficit was not associated with Social

Competence. Further evaluation with alternate measures of behavioural inhibition should

be conducted to explore whether other aspects of Behavioural Regulation may be

associated with Social Competence. For example, using delay of gratification tasks, tasks

of behavioral persistence (e.g. puzzle box; Zhou et al., 2012), the Touch Your Toes task

(Ponitz et al., 2008), and/or the slow-down motor task (Lisonbee, Pendry, Mize &

Gwynn, 2010).

In contrast, for the control group, two SEL domains – Social Understanding and

Empathy - were significantly correlated with Social Competence. These findings suggest

that having congruent emotional responses to others' distress, the ability to understand

and apply the social meaning of language, and problem solve common social situations

were related to children's social skills and social adaptive behaviours in the control group.

As mentioned previously, the lack of significant correlations between Nonverbal

Awareness and Social Competence is consistent with McKown (2007) and others that

examined the four basic emotions (Adolf, Sears & Piven, 2001; Castelli, 2005; Gross,

2004; Harms et al., 2010). Furthermore, the Nonverbal Awareness domain from the

Page 149: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

138

McKown et al. (2009) community sample included measures of gait and posture, as well

as facial and voice affect recognition. It may be that these additional tasks are more

highly associated with social competence. On the other hand, EF did not relate to social

competence of children in the control group. Few studies have evaluated the association

between EF and social functioning in neurotypical children in this age range, and of those

that did, most focused on problematic behaviours rather than social competence (Huyder

et al., 2013; Thorell et al., 2004; Vuontele et al., 2013). For example, Ciairano and

colleagues found that inhibition predicted problematic but not cooperative behaviours in

7 to 12 year old neurotypical children. While it may be that better EF performance does

not relate to prosocial behaviours and that worse EF performance is related to

problematic behaviours, simultaneous investigations of pro-social and problematic

behaviours are needed to clarify this relationship for neurotypical children. In addition,

since the EF task in this study were specifically chosen because of expected sensitivity to

deficits in children with ASD, there are many other components of EF which were not

evaluated. The inclusion of other components of EF, such as processing speed, flexibility,

and/or updating and shifting, may render different results. Furthermore, the tripartite EF

model used in this study was chosen for its potential to inform specific ASD

interventions. That is, more simple tasks were chosen in an attempt to minimize shared

variance and to isolate specific underlying skills that could potentially be targets for

intervention. Inclusion of more complex EF tasks, such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting

Task or Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, may have been more sensitive to variations

in social competence for neurotypical children.

Page 150: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

139

Parent ratings of the Metacognition and Behavioural Regulation domains of EF

were significantly correlated with Social Competence within both groups. It is important

to note that these results may have been influenced by shared-rater bias since parents

concurrently completed the BRIEF and social competence measures. The potential exists

for parents to provide answers on both the BRIEF and Social Competence questionnaires

based on the same behaviour because all three measures gather information based on a

child’s behaviour in real life settings.

6.8 Predictors of Social Competence

An important aim of this study was to examine whether EF and SEL predicted

social competence. The hypothesis that EF and SEL would predict Social Competence in

children with ASD and children in the control group received partial support (see Figures

7-10). For the control group, the SEL model significantly predicted 28% of the variance

in Social Competence. One domain, Social Understanding, and IQ were significant

predictors of social competence, whereas age, Nonverbal Awareness, and Empathy did

not significantly contribute. In a separate regression analysis, EF domains did not

contribute significantly to Social Competence. These findings suggest that targeting SEL

skills, particularly teaching social problem solving, rules of conversation, and the

importance of adjusting language based on context should be considered in social skills

interventions. In contrast, the performance-based EF model significantly predicted 28%

of the variance in Social Competence in the ASD group, with the Cognitive Regulation

domain and age being significant predictors. These contrasting results are theoretically

and clinically important: SEL domains, which were impaired in the ASD group, did not

predict Social Competence, but EF did. Most notably, this study demonstrated that for

Page 151: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

140

children with ASD, Cognitive Regulation processes make an important contribution to

Social Competence, providing an important basis for targeted clinical interventions.

These results are consistent with other studies that have found that EF is involved in

social abilities (Diamantopoulou et al., 2007; Rinsky & Hinshaw, 2011; Wahlstedt et al.,

2008). The significant contribution of age demonstrated that increasing age predicted

lower social competence for children with ASD.

Analyses with parent ratings of EF were conducted separately because of the

weak correlations with performance-based EF found in this study and the idea that parent

ratings and performance-based EF measures evaluate different facets of EF. Parent

ratings of EF predicted significant variance in Social Competence for both the ASD and

control group. One domain of the BRIEF - Behavioural Regulation - along with IQ,

predicted 31% of variance for the control group. The BRIEF Behavioural Regulation

domain – along with age, predicted 54% of variance in Social Competence for children

with ASD. The BRIEF Metacognition domain did not contribute to significant variance

to Social Competence for either group. These results are not surprising since the

Behavioral Regulation domain captures children's ability to maintain appropriate

regulatory control of their behavior and emotional responses, and some questions on the

measure are specifically related to aspects of social relatedness. Nevertheless, there are

important implications for these findings. The BRIEF Metacognition domain, which was

impaired in the ASD group, did not significantly predict Social Competence in this group

whereas the Behavioural Regulation domain did. These results support the premise that

parent ratings of EF can provide valuable information regarding children's Social

Competence. Furthermore, it suggests that those working in clinical settings should pay

Page 152: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

141

Figure 7. The relationship between social competence, SEL

and EF for children with ASD

Page 153: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

142

Figure 8. The relationship between social competence, SEL

and parent-rated EF for children with ASD.

Page 154: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

143

Figure 9. The relationship between social competence, SEL

and EF for the control group

Page 155: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

144

Figure 10. The relationship between social competence, SEL

and parent-rated EF for the control group

Page 156: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

145

particular attention to the parent ratings on the Behaviour Regulation domain as it may be

a good gauge of how children are managing socially. Additionally, clinicians should not

dismiss or discount adequate parent ratings on the Metacognition domain as they may not

be representative of the child's Social Competence.

Previous studies have primarily emphasized performance-based EF 'profiles' of

children with ASD (e.g. Barron et al., 2014; Troyb et al., 2013) as a means of assessing

clinical impairment. The results of this study extend this approach and highlight the

important relationship between EF and social competence. Future studies should explore

how this relationship is manifested in order to further understand the processes involved

with the social competence of children with ASD. The finding that the parent rated

Behavioural Regulation domain was significantly correlated with performance-based

measures of Cognitive Regulation and that both were significantly predictive of Social

Competence is important as it provides important information about how EF impacts

Social Competence. It has been proposed that behavioural regulation is a precursor to

cognitive regulation (Ardila, 2013; Gioia et al., 2000; MacKenzie, 2013), but further

research is needed to explore the nature of this relationship. Since the results

demonstrated that EF does not impact social competence through SEL skills (as initially

hypothesized), researchers might explore other ways in which EF impacts social

competence. The relationship between performance-based Cognitive Regulation and

parent-rated Behavioural Regulation also has important clinical implications and provides

support for interventions that specifically target both areas. The Self-Regulation Program

for Awareness and Resilience in Kids (SPARK: MacKenzie, 2013) is noteworthy because

it uses a progressive approach that specifically targets behavioural regulation before

Page 157: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

146

targeting cognitive regulation through a variety of explicit and practical strategies. The

age of the child should also be considered when implementing intervention strategies.

There is some evidence that cognitive regulation strategies benefit older children more

than younger ones (Riccio & Gomes, 2013). Determining whether different EF

interventions are more effective at different stages in development will be instrumental to

clinical practice and to addressing the increasing gap in social competence between

children with ASD and neurotypical peers throughout childhood.

The results of the current study thus suggest that both performance-based and

parent-ratings of EF should be included as integral components of assessment and

intervention with children with ASD. Although leading researchers in the field of ASD

have recently recommended systematic implementation of EF interventions for school

aged children with ASD (Szatmari, Charman & Constantino, 2012), more research is

needed to determine the efficacy and effectiveness of this intervention. Targeted

interventions are needed to explore whether gains in Cognitive and Behavioural

Regulation may in fact generalize to everyday behaviours and improve social

competence. In three recent reviews of EF interventions for children, diverse targeted and

multi-modal approaches are identified that have been shown to improve children's EF

(Cicerone, Levin, Malec, Stiss & Whyte, 2006; Diamond, 2012; Riccio & Gomes, 2013).

These include computerized training, biofeedback, verbal mediation, external cueing,

environmental restructuring and mindfulness meditation. The scaffolding of external

strategies is also recommended because the goal is not to train task-specific performance,

but rather the training and internalisation of regulatory cognitive processes (McCloskey,

2011). There is some evidence that one aspect of Cognitive Regulation – working

Page 158: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

147

memory - can be improved with targeted training with long term gains in ability for both

neurotypical children (Diamond & Lee, 2011, 2012) and those with HFASD (de Vries,

Prins, Schmand, & Guerts, 2012). However, even though computerized working memory

training has been effective in improving working memory, the transfer to other areas or

skills has been narrow and thus programs that target more components of EF are

recommended (Diamond, 2012). Multi-modal interventions that target both behavioral

and cognitive regulation, have been found to have positive effects on inhibitory control,

working memory, and cognitive flexibility in typically developing children (Diamond &

Lee, 2011), but research is needed to determine whether these improvements in EF could

also influence social competence. Although the efficacy of EF as a treatment to increase

social competence has not been established, the results of this study provide clear support

of this idea. Furthermore, a review of recent studies concluded that it is children with the

poorest EF who gain the most from EF interventions (Diamond & Lee, 2011).

Interestingly, recent findings also suggest that children with ASD who had higher

baseline social skills are the ones who benefited the most from group social skills

interventions (Chang et al., 2013).

SEL skills are generally considered important in the normal development of social

competence. The results of this study indicated that SEL domains were not significantly

predictive of Social Competence in the ASD group, but nonetheless SEL might still be an

important factor to consider. It is interesting to speculate that incorporating targeted EF

training in more traditional social skills interventions may increase effectiveness. For

example, the SPARK intervention mentioned earlier specifically targets cognitive,

behavioural and emotional regulation for children with ASD (MacKenzie, 2013). The

Page 159: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

148

specific inclusion of visual attention training in SPARK is supported by the results of the

current study, particularly given the speculation that it may be a critical skill for children

with ASD. Cognitive Behavior Intervention (CBI; Bauminger et al., 2002) and Social

Competence Intervention (SCI: Stichter et al., 2010; 2012) have also shown promising

results in reducing problematic behaviours and social deficits by specifically targeting

metacognitive strategies, self-monitoring, and self-regulation in addition to teaching

traditional social skills of emotion awareness and perspective taking. Moreover, it was

found that children with higher self-regulation benefited most from social skills treatment

(Chang et al., 2013), again providing support for the importance of targeting EF skills

prior to or in combination with social interventions.

The finding that both performance-based and parent-ratings of EF predicted

significant variance in the social competence of children with ASD is a novel and

exciting result of this research. However, there is still much variance to be explained in

the social competence of children with ASD. For example, this study was limited to two

areas associated with social competence – SEL and EF – and, as such, is not an

exhaustive examination of potential antecedents of social competence. In trying to adapt

the established McKown model of social competence to specifically incorporate the role

of EF, other potential influential factors were excluded. Temperament, parenting,

siblings, mental health, motor skills, and emotional intelligence are all potential

influential factors but were not examined in this study. The importance of considering

other factors is demonstrated in several recent studies. For example, emotional

intelligence predicted social interactions in a small sample of 16 to 21 year olds with

Asperger Syndrome (Montgomery, Stoesz & McCrimmon, 2012), effortful control was

Page 160: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

149

inversely related to social deficits (Konstantareas & Stewart, 2006; Jahromi et al., 2013)

and associated with prosocial peer engagement in children with ASD (Jahromi et al.,

2013), and negative affect temperament has also been negatively associated with social

adaptive behaviour in ASD (Millea, Shea & Diehl, 2013).

6.9 Study Limitations

This study is limited by several factors. Children with intellectual disability were

excluded and, as such, the results are not generalizable to the entire ASD population.

However, the functioning level specified in this study (IQ > 80) was consistent with

criteria used by other studies of EF in high-functioning children with ASD (e.g.,

Kenworthy et al., 2005; Landa & Goldberg, 2005; Troyb et al., 2013) and still allowed

for a wide range of IQ scores. Girls with ASD were also excluded from this study. Given

the 4.6 to 1 ratio of boys to girls (CDC, 2012), the sample in this study does nonetheless

represent the majority of children with ASD. The decision to exclude girls in this study

was made based on the consistent gender differences reported in the variables of interest

in this study (SEL, EF, social competence) (Boghi et al., 2006; Gentzler, Kerns &

Keener, 2010; Huizinga et al., 2011; Lemon, Gargaro, Enticott & Rinehart, 2001;

Willoughby et al., 2009).

The small sample size limited the strength of the analysis and the ability to use

more robust statistical analyses, such as SEM. The number of participants in this study,

however, is typical of studies examining EF in clinical populations. Attempts were made

to obtain more participants by recruiting children with ASD from two additional health

regions and by attending all available ASD parent meetings within the geographic area.

Page 161: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

150

Previous clinical diagnostic reports were also used to confirm the diagnosis of ASD

rather than secondary confirmation using the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised

(ADI-R; Lord et al. 1994) or Autism Diagnostic Observation System (ADOS; Lord et al.

1999), as is done in some studies. Importantly, other studies that did secondarily confirm

a diagnosis of ASD using the ADOS or ADI-R demonstrated similar results in their

Vineland Socialisation scores as in the present study, indicating that the current sample

was characteristic of the larger samples in these studies (Liss et al., 2001; Topaka et al,

2013). Notably, whereas many studies also used the results from the ADOS or ADI-R as

an outcome measure, the current study used a separate outcome measure that focused on

ability rather than disability. In addition, the data was collected at a single time and

performance on SEL or EF tasks may have thus been influenced by external factors on

the day of assessment. Although the tasks were administered in the same order for both

groups, many children with ASD needed more frequent breaks and thus the impact of

frequent breaks and hence longer sessions on task performance is unknown. However,

observations made by the researcher suggested that the frequent breaks improved rather

than hindered performance.

The use of only parent ratings of social competence may have provided a limited

or biased view of children's social competence. Although multi-method, cross-informant

evaluation of social competence is ideal because it reduces the effects of measurement

factors, particularly parent-related factors, on ratings of problem behaviours (Bennett et

al., 2012), there is substantial research that supports the use of a single informant

approach for social skills and adaptive behaviour. Studies have consistently found no

significant differences between parent and teacher ratings on social skills (Gresham,

Page 162: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

151

Elliot, Cook, Vance & Kettler, 2010; Murray, Ruble, Willis & Molloy, 2009; Vickerstaff

et al., 2007) and on the Vineland (Gagnon, Nagle & Nickerson, 2007; Szatmari, Archer,

Fisman & Streiner, 1994; Voekler, Shore, Hakim-Larson & Bruner, 1997) for children

with ASD. This is in contrast to the low inter-rater agreement generally found among

informants for problem behaviours and psychopathology (Kanne, Abbacchi, &

Constantino, 2009; Salbach-Andrae, Lenz & Lehmkuhl, 2009). High parent-teacher

agreement on social skills ratings suggests that social behaviours are more consistent

across settings for children with ASD. Additionally, the measurement of social

competence via parent ratings has several advantages. Rating scales assess a broad range

of behavior often not observed during direct observation (McConaughy & Ritter, 2005).

Furthermore, available normative data provide a standard for comparing behaviour with a

typically large and representative sample (Gresham & Elliott, 2008; McConaughy &

Ritter, 2005).

6.10 Conclusions and Future Research

This study aimed to clarify the role of SEL and EF in the social competence of

children with ASD. Previous studies in the literature have failed to incorporate a range of

EF components, have not comprehensively explored the relationship between EF and

SEL in relation to social competence, and focused on measures of impairment or

disability rather than social competence. This is the first study to concurrently evaluate

important SEL components, performance-based and parent-rated domains of EF in

children with ASD and a neurotypical control group. In addition, this study added

significantly to the ASD literature by examining the relationship between SEL and EF

domains specifically in relation to social competence.

Page 163: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

152

Results of this study demonstrated that better performance on two SEL domains

(Social Understanding and Empathy) and one parent rated EF domain (Behavioural

Regulation) were significantly correlated with higher Social Competence for children in

the control group. Social Competence was not associated with any performance-based EF

domains in the control group. IQ, the SEL domain of Social Understanding and the

parent-rated EF domain of Behavioural Regulation independently predicted significant

variance in Social Competence for the control group. Performance-based EF did not

predict significant variance in Social Competence for the control group. For children with

ASD, better performance in performance-based Cognitive Regulation and the parent

rated EF domains of Metacognition and Behavioural Regulation were significantly

correlated with higher Social Competence. Social Competence was not significantly

correlated with any SEL factors in the ASD group. Finally, performance-based Cognitive

Regulation, parent rated Behavioural Regulation, and age independently predicted

significant variance in Social Competence for the ASD group. IQ and SEL did not predict

significant variance in Social Competence for children with ASD.

The importance of EF to the social competence of children with ASD was

demonstrated through both performance-based measures and parent ratings.

Performance-based and parent ratings of EF also highlighted the importance of

considering the separate but integral contributions of cognitive, behavioural and

emotional regulation to children's social functioning. Notable results of the current study

are that (1) performance-based cognitive EF domains were related significantly to

everyday behavioral regulation, based on parent ratings; (2) performance-based and

parent ratings of EF significantly predicted social competence; and (3) SEL did not

Page 164: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

153

contribute significant variance in social competence for the ASD group. This is

particularly interesting in light of the contrasting findings for the control group where the

SEL domain of Social Understanding significantly predicted social competence, whereas

no EF domain did. The results also demonstrated that deficits in social competence tend

to increase with age for children with ASD by virtue of a widening gap with that of their

neurotypical peers. These results have important implications for clinical assessment and

intervention in ASD: (1). both performance-based and parent-ratings of EF should be

included as integral components of assessment and intervention; (2) targeted EF

interventions may be an effective means of increasing social competence in children with

ASD, and ultimately improving long-term mental health and behavioural outcomes.

Finally, although SEL domains were not significantly correlated with social competence

in the ASD group in this study, such factors should still be considered an important area

to assess in children with ASD. As it is likely that SEL domains are important to other

areas of social functioning, evaluation of these skills should also be used to help guide

individualised treatment.

Further research is needed to further explore the relationship between EF and

social competence in children with ASD. The finding that Cognitive Regulation accounts

for significant variance in social competence of children with ASD needs to be examined

in studies with a larger sample that can make use of more sophisticated analyses, such as

SEM. Other aspects of self-regulation should also be explored in further studies to

additionally clarify the relationship between EF and social competence. The importance

of assessing multiple integrated components may also allow for even more targeted

intervention strategies (Wasserman & Wasserman, 2013) and is in line with recent

Page 165: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

154

literature strongly encouraging a more integrated conceptualisation of EF and self

regulation (Ardila, 2013; Isquith, Roth & Gioai, 2013; Koziol, 2013). Additionally, more

focus on the developmental emergence and trajectories of Cognitive, Behavioural, and

Emotional regulation is required. Longitudinal studies could help identify whether there

are disparate developmental trajectories or maturational delays for children with ASD

and inform at which age targeted EF interventions may be most beneficial. This would

inform interventions to address EF impairments in early childhood in order to increase

social competence. Although current findings demonstrate the important role of EF in

social competence, more information is needed on how EF impacts social competence

and on what factors indeed influence EF. For example, sociocultural factors, such as

parent scaffolding and parent-child interactions, predict earlier EF development and

better performance on EF measures in neurotypical children (Pellicano, 2012). Exploring

factors that contribute to EF performance in children with ASD may provide another

avenue for intervention, possibly even at a younger age.

Many researchers have alluded to a distinct EF profile in children with ASD

(Happe et al., 2006; Reinvall et al., 2013), but to date the patterns of strengths and

difficulties has not been solidified because of inconsistent results in the literature. It is

unclear whether this is by virtue of the wide array of individual tasks purporting to

measure the same aspects of EF, which are often accompanied by varying complexities

and modalities. Alternatively, it could be that the nature of the heterogeneity within ASD

eludes identifying the types of specific EF profiles that have been demonstrated in

children with ADHD (Holmes et al., 2010) and FASD (Rasmussen et al., 2013). A

current meta-analysis of studies examining EF in high-functioning children with ASD

Page 166: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

155

may help clarify this issue, although the varying measures and complexity levels would

make this a challenging task. Furthermore, children with ASD may use different

strategies that rely on different underlying cognitive processes to complete SEL and EF

tasks. A comprehensive review of correlation studies may prove indispensable to shed

some light on this issue and further explore whether EF tasks are truly differentially

affected in children with ASD. Incorporating strategy measures, specifically examining

process-related elements of various tasks and physiological measures are critical for

future EF research. The accumulating evidence that children with ASD use different

strategies to successfully solve SEL and EF tasks, as well as the different correlations

among age, IQ, SEL, and EF measures also highlight the need for future studies to focus

on the heterogeneity within the ASD population. Exploring why some children are

successful rather than focusing on what is associated with their deficits or impairments

could also provide valuable clinical information. This may help to narrow the striking

disparity between their cognitive abilities and social competence.

Finally, the results of the current study highlight the need to go beyond

comparisons with neurotypical children and examine whether and why EF tasks are

differentially affected in ASD. Despite overall group deficits in SEL, EF and social

competence, the ASD group had scores ranging from the average to impaired range on

most measures. This heterogeneity in SEL and EF is reflective of the reported

behavioural heterogeneity in the ASD population. Given this intra-group heterogeneity, it

is time to go beyond group comparisons and focus on which aspects specifically

influence successful performance within this group, in order to reduce long-term negative

impacts, and improve overall quality of life for children with ASD and their families.

Page 167: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

156

7.0 References

Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: the assessment of pragmatic language. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43 (8), 973-987.

Adams, C., Lloyd, J., Aldred, C. & Baxendale, J. (2006). Exploring the effects of

communication intervention for developmental pragmatic language impairments.

International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 41-66.

DOI:10.1080/13693780500179793

Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information

from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 232–240.

Albert, D. & Steinberg, L. (2011). Age differences in strategic planning as indexed by the

Tower of London. Child Development, 82(5), 1501–1517.

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies

and psychopathology: A meta analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217-237.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

Alessandri, S. (1992). Attention, play, and social behavior in ADHD preschoolers.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20 (3), 289-302. doi:

10.1007/BF00916693

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC : American Psychiatric Association.

Anderson, D., Oti, R., Lord, C. & Welch, K. (2009). Patterns of growth in adaptive social

abilities among children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 37(7), 1019–1034. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9326-0.

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during

childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71-82. No DOI available.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Ardila, A. (2013). Development of metacognitive and emotional executive functions in

children. Journal of Applied Neuropsychology:Child, 2(2), 82-7. doi:

10.1080/21622965.2013.748388

Page 168: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

157

Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False-belief

understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.),

Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical

development, 13-38. New York: Psychology Press.

Auyeung, B., Wheelwright, S., Allison, C., Atkinson, M., Samarawickrema, S. & Baron-

Cohen, S. (2009). The children’s empathy quotient and systemizing quotient: sex

differences in typical development and in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1509–1521. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-

0772-x

Bacon, A., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L. & Allen, D. (1998). The responses of

autistic children to the distress of others. Journal of autism and developmental

disorders, 28(2), 129-142.

Baird, G. (2014). Diverging trajectories from a similar starting point: the outcome in

adults whose autism was diagnosed in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry 55:1, 59–60 doi:10.1111/jcpp.12182.

Barkley, R. A., & Benton, C. M. (2013). Your defiant child: Eight steps to better

behavior. Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.

Barkley, R. (2012). Executive Functions: What They Are, How They Work, and Why They

Evolved, 244 pages. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. (2005). ADHD and the Nature of Self-Control, 410 pages. New York:

Guilford Press ISBN 978-1-57230-250-1

Russel, B. & Benton, C. (2013). Your Defiant Child: Eight Steps to Better Behavior, 2nd

Edition. 257 pages. New York: Guildford Press.

Barnea-Goraly, N., Kwon, H., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Lotspeich L. & Reiss, A. (2004).

White matter structure in autism: preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor

imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 55(3), 323-6. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.022

Baron, R. & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social

psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizing–systemizing theory. The Year in

Cognitive Neuroscience, 1156, 68–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x

Page 169: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

158

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C. & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced

test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or

Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813-822.

Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O. & Ashwin, E. (2009). The Transporters: animation to teach

children with autism to recognise emotions. Autism Spectrum Quarterly, winter,

15-18.

Baron-Cohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of

adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex

differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163-75.doi:

0162-3257/04/0400-0163/0

Barron- Linnankoski, S., Reinvall, O., Lahervuorti, A., Voutilainen, A., Nuuttila-Lahti, P.

& Korkman, M. (2014). Neurocognitive performance of children with higher

functioning autism spectrum disorders on the NEPSY-II. Child Neuropsychology,

in press. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2013.873781

Bauminger, Edelsztein & Morash. (2005). Social information processing and emotional

understanding in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 38(1), 45-61. No DOI available.

Beauchamp, M. & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An integrative framework for the

development of social skills. Psychological Bulletin, 36 (1), 39-64. doi:

10.1037/a0017768

Begeer, S., Koot, H., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M & Stegge, H. (2008). Emotional competence

in children with autism: Diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence.

Developmental Review 28, 342–369.

Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: visual

processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.

Bennett, T., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Duku, E., Vaccarella, L. & Tuff , L. (2013). Theory

of Mind, Language and Adaptive Functioning in ASD: A Neuroconstructivist

Perspective. Journal of Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

22(1), 13-19.

Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. & Rogers, S. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions

in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816-1835. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1996.tb01830.x

Berkman, E., Graham, A. & Fisher, P. (2012). Training self-control: a domain-general

translational neuroscience approach. Child Developmental Perspective, 6(4), 374–

384. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00248.x.

Page 170: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

159

Best, J. & Miller, P. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child

Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. doi: 0009-3920/2010/8106-0002

Bierman, K. (2004). Peer rejection: Developmental processes and intervention

strategies. New York: Guilford.

Blandon, A., Calkins, S. & Keane, S. (2010). Predicting emotional and social competence

during early childhood from toddler risk and maternal behavior. Development

and Psychopathology, 22, 119-132.

Blair, C., Zelazo, P, & Greenberg, M. (2005). The measurement of executive function in

early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 561-571.

Blunden, S., Lushington, K., Lorenzen, B., Martin, J. & Kennedy, D. (2005).

Neuropsychological and psychosocial function in children with a history of

snoring or behavioral sleep problems. Journal of Pediatrics, 146, 780-786. doi:

10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.01.043

Boghi, A.,Rasetti, R., Avidano, F., Manzone, C., Orsi, L., D'Agata, F., Caroppo, P.,

Bergui, M., Rocca, P., Pulvirenti, L., Bradac, G., Bogetto, F., Mutani, R. &

Mortara, P. (2006). The effect of gender on planning: an fMRI study using the

Tower of London task. NeuroImage, 33, 999–1010.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.022

Bohlin, G. & Hagekull, B. (2009). Socio-emotional development: From infancy to young

adulthood. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 592–601.

Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2003). The recognition of facial affect in autistic and

schizophrenic subjects and their first-degree relatives. Psychological Medicine,

33, 907–915.

Boraston, Z. and Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). The application of eye-tracking technology in

the study of autism. The Journal of Physiology, 581: 893–898. doi:

10.1113/jphysiol.2007.133587

Bowers, L., Huisingh, R., & LoGiudice, C. (2005). Test of Problem Solving 3:

elementary. Linguisystems: East Moline, IL.

Boyd, B., McBee, M., Holtzclaw, T., Baranek, G. & Bodfish, J. (2009). Relationships

among Repetitive Behaviors, Sensory Features, and Executive Functions in High

Functioning Autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(4), 959–966.

doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2009.05.003.

Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in

children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 19, 301-315.

Page 171: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

160

Brooks, P. & Ploog, B. (2013). Attention to emotional tone of voice in speech perception

in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 845–857. doi:

10.1016/j.rasd.2013.03.003

Brown, W., Odom, S. & McConnell, S. (2008). Social Competence of Young Children.

Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company: Maryland.

Bruining, H., de Sonneville, L., Swaab, H., deJonge, M., Kas, M., van Engeland, H. &

Vorstman, J. (2010). Dissecting the clinical heterogeneity of autism spectrum

disorders through defined genotypes. PLoS ONE, 5(5). e10887

Bull, R., Espy, K. & Senn, T.

(2004). A comparison of performance on the towers of

london and hanoi in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

45(4), 743–754.

Burt, K., Obradovic, J., Long, J. & Masten, A. (2008). The interplay of social

competence and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing transactional and

cascade models. Child Development, 79 (2), 359–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2007.01130.x

Calkins, S. (2010). Commentary: Conceptual and Methodological Challenges

to the Study of Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology. Journal of

Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 32(1), pp. 92–95. doi:

10.1007/s10862-009-9169-6.

Canivez, G., Konold, T., Collins, J. & Wilson, G. (2009). Construct validity of the

Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence and wide range intelligence test:

Convergent and Structural Validity. School Psychology Quarterly, 24(4), 252-

265. doi: 10.1037/a0018030

Caprara, G., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Bandura, A. & Zimbardo P. (2000). Prosocial

foundations of children's academic achievement. Psychological Science, 11, 302-

306.

Carlson, S. & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and

children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032-1053. doi: 10.1111/1467-

8624.00333

Carlson S., Zayas, V. & Guthormsen, A. (2009). Neural correlates of decision making on

a gambling task. Child Development, 80 (4), 1076–1096. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2009.01318.x

Carter, A. S., Davis, N. O., Klin, A. & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Social development in

autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism

Page 172: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

161

and pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1. Diagnosis, development,

neurobiology, and behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Carthy, T., Horesh, N., Apter, A. & Gross, J. (2010). Patterns of emotional reactivity and

regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Psychopatholy and

Behavior Assessment, 32, 23–36. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9167-8

Cassotti, M., Houde, O. & Moutier, S. (2011). Developmental changes of win-stay and

loss-shift strategies in decision making. Child Neuropsychology, 17 (4), 400–411.

doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.547463

Castellanos, X., Sonuga-Barke, E., Milham, M. & Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing

cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

10(3). doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011

Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardised facial expressions

in autism and normal development. Autism, 9, 428–449.

doi:10.1177/1362361305056082

Centre for Disease Control and Prevention (2012).

http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6302a1.htm?s_cid=ss6302a1_w

Channon, S., Charman, T., Heap, J., Crawford, S., & Rios, P. (2001). Real-life-type

problem-solving in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 31(5), 461–469.

Channon, S. & Watts, M. (2003). Pragmatic language interpretation after closed head

injury: Relationship to executive functioning. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8(4),

243-260. doi: 10.1080/135468000344000002

Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G. & Drew, A. (1997).

Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and

imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781-9.

Christ, S., Holt, D., White, D. & Green, L. (2007). Inhibitory control in children with

autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,

1155–1165. doi:10.1007/s10803-006– 0259-y

Christ, S., Kester, L., Bodner, K. & Miles, J. (2011). Evidence for selective inhibitory

impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder, Neuropsychology, 25,

(6), 690–701. doi: 10.1037/a0024256

Ciairano, S., Visu-Petra, L., & Settanni, M. (2007). Executive inhibitory control and

cooperative behavior during early school years: a follow-up study. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, pp.335–345. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9094-z.

Page 173: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

162

Cohen, D. & Strayer, J. (1996) Empathy in conduct disordered and comparison youth.

Developmental Psychology, 32, 988-998.

Coo, H., Ouellette-Kuntz, H., Lam, M., Yu, C., Dewey, D., Bernier, F., Chudley, A.,

Henneseey, P., Breitenbach, M., Noonan, A., Lewis, M. & Holden, J. (2012).

Correlates of age at diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in six Canadian

regions. Chronic Diseases and Injuries in Canada, 32 (2), 90-100.

Constantino, J. & Gruber, C. (2005). The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Manual. Los

Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Coplan, R. & Weekes, M. (2009). Shy and Soft-spoken: Shyness, Pragmatic Language,

and Socioemotional Adjustment in Early Childhood. Infant and Child

Development, 18, 238–254 doi: 10.1002/icd.622

Corbett, B. & Constantine, L. (2006). Autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

assessing attention and response control with the integrated visual and auditory

continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychology, 12(4–5), 335–348.

Corbett, B., Constantine, L., Hendren, R., Rocek, D. & Olonoff, S. (2009). Examining

executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder and typical development . Psychiatry Research, 166 (2-3),

210-222. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005

Crick, N. & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-

processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin,

115(1), 74-101.

Crone, E., Bunge, S., Latenstein, H. & van der Molen, M. (2005). Characterization of

children’s decision making: sensitivity to punishment frequency, not task

complexity. Child Neuropsychology, 11: 245–263. doi:

10.1080/092970490911261

Crone, E. & van der Molen, M. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision

making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the

ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(3), 251-

279. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2

Crone, E., Vendel, I., & van der Molen, M. (2003). Decision-making in disinhibited

adolescents and adults: insensitivity to future consequences or driven by

immediate reward? Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1625-1641.

Crone, E. & van der Molen, M. (2007). Development of decision making in school-aged

children and adolescents: evidence from heart rate and skin conductance analysis.

Child Development, 78(4), 1288 – 1301.

Page 174: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

163

Culbertson, W. & Zillner, E. (1998). The Tower of LondonDX

: a standardized approach to

assessing executive functioning in children. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 13(3), 285–301.

Culbertson, W. & Zillner, E. (2005). The Tower of LondonDX

: Chicago, IL: Multi-Health

Systems.

da Mata, F., Neves, F., Lage, G., deMoraes, P., Mattos, P., Fuentes, D., Correa, H. &

Malloy-Diniz, L. (2011). Neuropsychological assessment of the decision making

process in children and adolescents: an integrative review of the literature. Revista

de Psiquiatria Clínica; 38(3),106-15.

da Mata, F., Sallum, I., Miranda, D., Bechara, A. & Malloy-Diniz. (2013). Do general

intellectual functioning and socioeconomic status account for performance on the

Children’s Gambling Task? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7(68), 1-9. doi:

10.3389/fnins.2013.00068

Daniels, A. & Mandell, D. (2013). Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum

disorder diagnosis: A critical review. Autism. published online before print June

20, 2013. doi: 10.1177/1362361313480277

de Bruin, E., Ferdinand, R., Meester, S., de Nijs, P. & Verheij, F. (2007). High rates of

psychiatric co-morbidity in PDD-NOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 37, 877–886.

Decety, J., 2010. The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Developmental

Neuroscience 32, 257–267

De Martino, B., Harrison, N., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. (2008). Explaining

enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. Journal of

Neuroscience, 28, 10746–10750.

Demopoulos, C., Hopkins, J. & Davis, A. (2013). A comparison of social cognitive

profiles in children with autism spectrum disorders and attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a matter of quantitative but not qualitative

difference? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1157–1170. doi

10.1007/s10803-012-1657-y

Denckla, M. (2002). The behavior rating inventory of executive function: commentary.

Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 304–306. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.304.13512

Denham, S., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S. &

Queenan, P. (2003). Preschoolers’ emotional competence: Pathway to mental

health? Child Development, 74, 238-256.

Page 175: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

164

Dennis, T. (2010). Neurophysiological markers for child emotion regulation from the

perspective of emotion-cognition integration: current directions and future

challenges. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35 (2). Pp. 212 — 230.doi:

10.1080/87565640903526579

Dennis, T. A., Malone, M., & Chen, C. (2009). Emotional face processing and emotion

regulation in children: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 1–18.

De Villiers, J., Szatmari, P. & Yang, S. (2014). Autism and Asperger Syndrome:

Language Use, In V. Patel, V. Preedy & C. Martin (Eds) Comprehensive Guide to

Autism, 1929-1951

De Waal, F. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy.

Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279-300.

Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive

functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 335-341. doi:

10.1177/0963721412453722

Diamond, A., Barnett, W., Thomas, J. & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves

cognitive control, Science, 318, 1387 - 1389.

Diamond, A. & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function

development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959-964.

Donders, J., DenBraber, D. & Vos, L. (2010). Construct and criterion validity of the

Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in children referred

for neuropsychological assessment after paediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal

of Neuropsychology, 4, 197–209. doi:10.1348/174866409X478970

Donders, J. & Larsen, T. (2012). Clinical utility of the Tower of London—Drexel

University, second edition (TOLDX

) after adolescent traumatic brain injury.

Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(4), 333–342. doi:

10.1080/87565641.2011.646448

Doyle, C. & McDougle, C. (2012). Pharmacologic treatments for the behavioral

symptoms associated with autism spectrum disorders across the lifespan.

Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 263-279.

Duckworth, A & Seligman, M. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic

performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939 - 944. doi:

10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x

Page 176: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

165

Duijvenvoorden, A., Jansen, B., Bredman, J. &. Huizenga, H. (2012). Age-related

changes in decision making: comparing informed and noninformed situations.

Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 192–203. doi: 10.1037/a0025601.

Dziodek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H., Wolf, O. & Convit, A.

(2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger

syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (met). Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 38, 464–473. doi 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x

Eberly, M. & Montemayor, R. (1998). Doing good deeds: An examination of adolescent

prosocial behavior in the context of parent-adolescent relationships. Journal of

Adolescent Research, 13(4), 403-432.

Egeland, J. & Fallmyr, O. (2010). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating

Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF): Support for a distinction between

emotional and behavioral regulation. Child Neuropsychology, 1-12.

Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., .... & Shepard, S. (2003).

The relations of effortful control and ego control to children’s resiliency and

social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 761 – 776.

Eisenberg, N. & Miller, P. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related

behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1). doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000a). Dispositional

emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 136–157.

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T., Fabes, R., Shepard, S. (2000b). The relations

of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing

problem behavior. Child Development, 72, 1112–34.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. & Murphy, B. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative

emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior.

Child Development, 67, 2227–2247.

Eisenberg, N. & Fabes, R.A. (1992). Emotion, regulation, and the development of social

competence. In M.S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology:

Vol. 14. Emotion and social behavior (pp. 119–150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The

role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning:Alongitudinal

study. Child Development, 66, 1360–1384. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1995.tb00940.x

Page 177: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

166

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Murphy, B., Guthrie, I., Jones, S., Friedman, J.,

Poulin, R., & Maszk, P. (1997). Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of

children’s social functioning from regulation and emotionality. Child

Development, 68, 642– 664. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04227.x

Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I.K., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., Murphy, B., Jones, S.,

Poulin, R., & Reiser, M. (2000). Prediction of elementary school children’s

externalizing problem behaviors from attentional and behavioral regulation and

negative emotionality. Child Development, 71, 1367–1382. doi: 009-

3920/2000/7105-0020.

Fabes, R., Eisenberg, N., Jones, S., Smith, M., Guthrie, I., Poulin, R., Shepard, S. &

Friedman, J. (1999). Regulation, emotionality and preschoolers’ socially

competent peer interactions. Child Development, 70(2), 432–442. doi:

10.1111/1467-8624.00031

Faja, S., Murias, M., Beauchaine, T. & Dawson, G. (2013). Reward-based decision

making and electrodermal responding by young children with autism spectrum

disorders during a gambling task. Autism Research, 6, 494–505. doi:

10.1002/aur.1307.

Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I., Kelley, E., Naigles, L., Schultz, R., Stevens, M., Helt, M.,

Orinstein, A., Rosenthal, M., Troyb, E., Tyson, K. (2013). Optimal outcome in

individuals with a history of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

54(2), 195-205. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12037.

Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. London: Sage Publishing, 816pp.

Flood, A. M., Hare, D. J., & Wallis, P. (2011). An investigation into social information

processing in young people with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism, 15, 601–

624.

Flynn, E. (2007). The role of inhibitory control in false belief understanding. Infant and

Child Development, 16(1), 53-69. doi: 10.1002/icd.500

Fombonne, E., Zakarian, Bennett, Meng, McLean-Heywood (2006). Pervasive

developmental disorders in Montreal, Quebec, Canada: Prevalence and links with

immunizations. Pediatrics, 118. doi: e139-e150. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2993

Fountain, C., King, M. & Bearman, P. (2011). Age of diagnosis for autism: individual

and community factors across 10 birth cohorts. Journal of Epidemiological

Community Health, 65, 503-510. doi:10.1136/jech.2009.104588

Frazier, J., Biederman, J., Bellordre, C., Garfield, S., Geller, D., Coffey, B. & Faraone, S.

(2001). Should the diagnosis of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder be

Page 178: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

167

considered in children with Pervasive Developmental Disorder? Journal of

Attention Disorders, 4 (203). doi: 0.1177/108705470100400402

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.

Frith, U., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Perception in autistic children. In D. J. Cohen & A.

M. In Donnellan (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental

disorders (pp. 85-102). New York: John Wiley.

Frith, U. & Happe, F. (1994). Autism: beyond theory of mind. Cognition, 50(1-3), 115-

32.

Frith, U., Happé, F. & Siddons, F. (1994). Autism and theory of mind in everyday

life. Social Development, 2, 108-124.

Frith, U., White, S., Hill, E. & Happe, F. (2009). Revisiting the strange stories: revealing

mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Development, 80(4), 1097–1117. doi:

0009-3920/2009/8004-0012

Fuentes, C., Mostofsky, S. & Bastian, A. (2010). Perceptual reasoning predicts

handwriting impairments in adolescents with autism. Neurology, 75, 1825-1829.

Ganesalingam, K., Sanson, A., Anderson,V., &Yeates, K. (2006). Self-regulation and

social and behavioral functioning following childhood traumatic brain injury.

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 609– 621. DOI:

10.10170S1355617706060796

Ganesingham, K., Sanson, A., Anderson, V. & Yeates, K. (2007). Self-regulation as a

mediator of the effects of childhood traumatic brain injury on social and

behavioral functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,

13, 298–311. doi: 10.10170S1355617707070324

Ganesalingam, K., Sanson, A., Anderson, V. & Yeates, K. (2007a). Self-regulation as a

mediator of the effects of childhood traumatic brain injury on social and

behavioral functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,

13, 298–311. DOI: 10.10170S1355617707070324

Ganesalingam, K., Yeates, K., Sanson, A., & Anderson, V. (2007b). Social problem-

solving skills following childhood traumatic brain injury and its association with

self regulation and social and behavioural functioning. Journal of

Neuropsychology (2007), 1, 149–170. DOI:10.1348/174866407X185300.

Garcia-Nonell, C., Ratera, E., Harris, S., Hessl, D., Ono, M., Tartaglia, N., Marvin, E.,

Tassone, F., Hagerman, R. (2008). Secondary medical diagnosis in fragile X

syndrome with and without autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of

Medical Genetics, 146A, 1911–1916. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.32290

Page 179: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

168

Gertner, B.L., Rice, M.L., & Hadley, P.A. (1994). Influence of communicative

competence on peer preferences in a preschool classroom. Journal of Speech and

Hearing Research, 37, 913–923.

Geurts, H., de Vries, M. & van den Bergh, S. (2013). Executive functioning theory and

autism. In S. Goldstein & J. Naglieri., (Eds.), Handbook of Executive

Functioning. New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5

Geurts, H., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H. & Sergeant, J. (2004). How specific are

executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and

autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45(4), 836–854.

Geurts, H., van der Oord, S. & Crone, E. (2006). Hot and cool aspects of cognitive

control in children with ADHD: decision-making and inhibition. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, doi 10.1007/s10802-006-9059-2

Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Sirian, L., Black, D., & Wagner, A. (2002). Adaptive

Skills and Executive Function in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Child

Neuropsychology, 8(4), pp. 241 — 248. DOI: 10.1076/chin.8.4.241.13504.

Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S. & Kenworthy, L. (2000). BRIEF – Behavior Rating

Inventory of Executive Function: Professional manual. Odessa, Florida:

Psychological Assessment

Gioia, G., Kenworthy, L. & Isquith, P. (2010). Executive functions in the real world:

lessons from Mark Ylvisaker. Journal of Head Trauma and Rehabilitation,25(6),

433-439.

Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Retzlaff, P. & Espy, K. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample.

Child Neuropsychology, 8 (4), 249-257.

Giurak, A., Gooding, M., Madan, A., Kram, J., Miller, B. & Levenson, R. (2009). Do

tests of executive functioning predict ability to downregulate emotions

spontaneously and when instructed to suppress? Cognitive, Affective, &

Behavioral Neuroscience, 9 (2), 144-152. doi:10.3758/CABN.9.2.144

Glascher, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Bechara, A., Rudrauf, D., Calamia, M., Tranel,

D. (2012). Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making

in the prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America, 109(36), 14681–14686.

Goddard, L., Howlin, P., Dritschel, B. & Patel, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory and

social problem-solving in asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 291–300. doi 10.1007/s10803-006-0168-0

Page 180: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

169

Goldberg, M., Mostofsky, S., Cutting, L., Maone, E., Astor, B., Denckla, M. & Landa, R.

(2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with

ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3). doi:

10.1007/s10803-005-3291-4

Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J., Princiotta, D. & Otero, T. (2014). A history of executive

functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct. In S. Goldstein & J. Naglieri

(Eds) Handbook of Executive Functioning. New York: Springer New York. doi:

10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5

Gomarus, K., Wijers, A., Minderaa, R. & Althaus, M. (2009). Do children with ADHD

and/or PDD-NOS differ in reactivity of alpha/theta ERD/ERS to manipulations of

cognitive load and stimulus relevance? Clinical Neurophysiology 120, 73–79.

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.10.017

Gonzalez, G., Bouwmeester, S. & Boonstra, M. (2010). Understanding planning ability

measured by the Tower of London: an evaluation of its internal structure by latent

variable modeling. Psychological Assessment, 22(4), 923–934. doi:

10.1037/a0020826

Gresham, F., Elliot, S., Cook, C., Vance, M. & Kettler, R. (2010). Cross-informant

agreement for ratings for social skill and problem behavior ratings: An

investigation of the Social Skills Improvement System—Rating Scales.

Psychological Assessment, 22(1), 157-166. doi: 10.1037/a0018124

Gresham, F., & Elliott, S. (2008). Rating Scales Manual. SSIS Social Skills Improvement

System. Minneapolis, Pearson.

Gresham, F.M. & Elliot, S.N. (1990). Social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN:

American Guidance Service.

Groen, Y., Gaastra, G., Evans, B. & Tuche, O. (2013). Risky behavior in gambling tasks

in individuals with ADHD – a systematic literature review. PLoS ONE, 8(9), 1-16.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074909

Gross, Thomas (2004). The perception of four basic emotions in human and nonhuman

faces by children with autism and other developmental disabilities. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(5), 469-480.

Gross, J. & Thompson, R. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations, In J.

Gross (Ed.) Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guildford Press. 638

pp

Hadwin, J. & Kovshoff, H. (2013). A review of theory of mind interventions for children

and adolescents with autism spectrum conditions. In S. Baron-Cohen, M.

Page 181: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

170

Lombardo, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. Cohen (Eds) Understanding Other Minds:

Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience, pp 413-428. Chapter 23

Oxford University Press, NY, New York.

Halberstadt, A., Denham, S. & Dunsmore, J. (2001). Affective social competence. Social

Development, 10 (1), 79-118.

Handen, B., Johnson, C. & Lubetsky, M. (2000). Efficacy of methylphenidate among

children with autism and symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 245–255. doi

10.1007/s10803-011-1399-2

Happe, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters'

thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children

and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmentat Disorders, 24(2), 129-154.

Happe, F. & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119 (4), 1377-1400.

doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1377

Happe, F., Ronald, A. & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for

autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10). doi: 10.1038/nn1770

Happe, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in

autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining

profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition 61, 25–39.

doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004

Harms, B. Martin, A. & Wallace, G. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism

spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies.

Neuropsychological Review, 20, 290–322. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6

Hays, J., Reas, D. & Shaw, J. (2002). Concurrent validity of the Wechsler abbreviated

scale of intelligence and the Kaufman brief intelligence test among psychiatric

inpatients. Psychological Reports, 90(2), 355-359.

Hazlett, H., Poe, M., Lightbody, A., Gerig, G., MacFall, J., Ross, A., Provenzale, J.,

Martin, A., Reiss, A. & Piven, J. (2009). Teasing apart the heterogeneity of

autism:Same behavior, different brains in toddlers with fragile X syndrome and

autism. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 1, p. 81–90. doi

10.1007/s11689-009-9009-8

Heatherton, T. & Wagner, D. (2001). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3). doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005

Page 182: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

171

Herba, C., de Bruin, E., Althaus, M, Verheij,F. & Ferdinand, R. (2008). Face and

emotion recognition in MCDD Versus PDD-NOS. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 706–718. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0438-5.

Hill, E. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental

Review, 24, 189-233. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001

Hill, E. & Bird, C. (2006). Executive processing in Asperger Syndrome: patterns of

performance in multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822 -2835. PubMed:

16930637]

Hinnant, B. & Obrien, M. (2007). Cognitive and emotional control and perspective taking

and their relations to empathy in 5-year-old children. The Journal of Genetic

Psychology, 168(3), 301–322

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. & Baddely, A. (2012). Executive functions and self-

regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3). doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

Holmbeck, G. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the

study of mediators and moderators: examples from the child-clinical and pediatric

psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65 (4),

599-610.

Homack, S. & Riccio, C. (2004). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the

Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,

19, 725–743. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.003

Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K., Lee, W. & Zelazo, P. (2005). Assessment of Hot and

Cool Executive Function in Young Children: Age-Related Changes and

Individual Differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617–644

Hooper, C., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. & Yarger, R. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on

the iowa gambling task: implications for the development of decision making and

ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1148-1158.

doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148

Hooper, C., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. & Yarger, R. (2004). Adolescents’ Performance on

the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the Development of Decision Making

and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Developmental Psychology, 40 (6), 1148–

1158. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148

Horlin, C., Falkmer, M., Fitzgerald, P., Leung, D., Ordqvist, A. & Flakmer, T. (2013).

The influence of static versus naturalistic stimuli on face processing in children

with and without Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Research in

Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 1617–1624. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.09.012

Page 183: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

172

Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with

autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 212–229.

Howlin, P., & Moss, P. (2012). Adults with autism spectrum disorders. Canadian

Journal of Psychiatry, 57(5), 275–284.

Howlin, P., Savage, S., Mosss, P., Tempier, A. & Rutter, M. (2014). Cognitive and

language skills in adults with autism: a 40-year follow-up. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry 55(1), 49–58. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12115

Hughes, C. (2002). Executive functions and development: emerging themes. Infant and

Child Development, 11, 201-209.

Hughes, C. Adlam, A., Happe, F., Jackson, J., Taylor, A. & Caspi, A. (2000). Good test-

retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range

of abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), pp. 483-490. No

available doi.

Hughes, C., Dunn, J. & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind

and emotion and executive dysfunction in hard-to-manage preschoolers. Journal

of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 39, 981-994.

Hughes, C. & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social

relations? review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical

development. Social Development, 13(4), 590-619.

Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. & van der Molen, M. (2006). Age-related change in executive

function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia,

44(11), 2017–2036.

Huizenga, H., Crone, E. & Jansen, B. J. (2007). Decision-making in healthy children,

adolescents and adults explained by the use of increasingly complex proportional

reasoning rules. Developmental Science, 10, 814–825.

Hus, V., Bishop, S., Gotham, K., Huerta, M. & Lord, C. (2013). Factors influencing

scores on the social responsiveness scale. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry 54(2), 216–224. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02589.x

Iarocci, G., Yager, J., & Elfers, T. (2007). What gene– environment interactions can tell

us about social competence in typical and atypical populations. Brain and

Cognition, 65, 112–127. DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.01.008

Isquith, P., Roth, R. & Gioia, G. (2013). Contribution of rating scales to the assessment

of executive functions. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 2(2), 125-132, doi:

10.1080/21622965.2013.748389

Page 184: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

173

Izard, C. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Izard, C. E. (2001). Emotional intelligence or adaptive emotions? Emotion, 1, 249–257.

Izard, C., Fine S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B. & Youngstrom, E. (2001).

Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behaviour and academic competence

in children at risk. Psychological Bulletin, 12(1), 18-23. No doi available.

Jahromi, L. & Stifter, C. (2008).Individual differences in preschoolers' self regulation and

theory of mind. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(1), 125-150. doi:

10.1353/mpq.2008.0007

Jahromi, L., Bryce, C. & Swanson, J. (2013). The importance of self-regulation for the

school and peer engagement of children with high-functioning autism. Research

in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 235–246.

Jahromi, L., Kasari, C., McCracken, J., Lee, L., Aman, M. & McDougle, C. (2009).

Positive effects of methylphenidate on social communication and self-regulation

in children with pervasive developmental disorders and hyperactivity. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorder, 39, 395–404.

Jewell, J., Jordan, S., Hupp, S. & Everett, G. (2009). Etiology and Relationships to

Developmental Disabilities and Psychopathology. In J. Matson (Ed) Behavior and

skills in children. Springer: New York. Pp 39-59.

Johnson, S., Yechiam, E., Murphy, R., Queller, S., & Stout, J. (2006). Motivational

processes and autonomic responsivity in Asperger’s disorder: Evidence from the

Iowa gambling task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,

12(5), 668–676. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060802

Johnson, S., Robertson, I., Kelly, S., Silk, T., Barry, E., Dàibhis, A., Watchorn, A.,

Keavery, M., Fitzgerald, M., Gallager, L., Gill, M. & Bellgrove, M. (2007).

Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism

on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2234–2245.

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.019

Jolliffe,T. & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The Strange Stories Test: a replication with high-

functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 395-406. doi: 10.1023/A:1023082928366

Jones, A., Happe, F., Gilbert, F., Burnett, S. & Viding, E. (2010). Feeling, caring,

knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies

and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51

(11), 1188–1197. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x

Page 185: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

174

Jones, C., Pickles, A., Flacaro, M., Marsden, A., Happe, F., Scott, S., Sauter, D., Tregay,

J., Phillips, R., Baird, G., Simonoff, E. & Charman, T. (2011). A multimodal

approach to emotion recognition ability in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52(3), 275–285. doi:10.1111/j.1469-

7610.2010.02328.x

Jones, W. & Klin, A. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism

spectrum: the role of development. Journal of American Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 48(5), 471-473. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f6c0d

Joseph, D. & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-

analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 54-78.

Joseph, R. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and

executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism.

Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137-155.

Kaland, N., Callesen, K., Møller-Nielsen, A., Lykke Mortensen, E. & Smith, L. (2008).

Performance of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-

functioning autism on advanced theory of mind tasks. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 38, 1112–1123. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0496-8

Kalbfleisch, M. & Loughan, A. (2012). Impact of IQ discrepancy on executive function

in high-functioning autism: Insight into twice exceptionality, Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 42, 390–400

doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1257-2

Kenny, D. (2003). “Mediation,” http://www.users.rcn.com/dakenny/mediate.htm/.

Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B., Gutermuth, L. & Wallace, G. (2008). Understanding executive

control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world.

Neuropsychology Review, 18 (4), 320–338. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9077-7.

Kenworthy, L., Black, D., Harrison, B., della Rosa, A., Wallace, G. (2009). Are

Executive Control Functions Related to Autism Symptoms in High-Functioning

Children? Child Neuropsychology,15: 5, 425 - 440. doi:

10.1080/09297040802646983

Kenworthy, L., Case, L., Harms, M., Martin, A. & Wallace, G. (2010). Adaptive

Behavior Ratings Correlate With Symptomatology and IQ Among Individuals

With High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 416–423. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0911-4

Kerr, A. & Zelazo, P. (2004). Development of ‘‘hot’’ executive function: The children's

gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148–157. doi:10.1016/S0278-

2626(03)00275-6

Page 186: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

175

Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N. & Delis, D. (2005). Executive functions in autism and

Asperger’s disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 27, 379-401.doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2703_5

Klin, A., Saulnier, C., Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., Volkmar, F. & Lord. C. (2007). Social

and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with

Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 748–759. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0229-4

Klin, A. & Jones, W. (2009). Heterogeneity and homogeneity across the autism

spectrum: the role of development. Journal of American Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 48(5), 471-473. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f6c0d

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2005). The enactive mind - from actions

to cognition: Lessons from autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen

(Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1.

Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley

& Sons.

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002a). Defining and

quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,

895–908.

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002b). Visual fixation

patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social

competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–

816.

Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Harlan, E. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood:

Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development.

Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.

Konstantareas, M. & Stewart, K. (2006). Affect regulation and temperament in children

with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

36(2), 143-54. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0051-4

Korkman, M., Kirk, S. & Kemp, U. (2007). NEPSY- Second Edition. San Antonio, Texas:

Harcourt Assessment.

Krivitzky, L., Roebuck, T., Roth, R., Blackstone, K., Johnson, C. & Gioia, G. (2011).

Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory and response

inhibition in children with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the

International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 1143–1152.

doi:10.1017/S1355617711001226

Page 187: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

176

Kumpfer, K. (1999). Factors and processes contributing to resilience: the resilience

framework. In M. Glantz & J. Johnson, (Eds), Resilience and Development:

Positive Life Adaptions, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp.

179–224.

Ladd, G. (2005). Children’s Peer Relations and Social Competence. USA: Vail-Ballou

Press, 421 pp.

Lai, M., Lombardo, M., Chakrabarti, B. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2013). Subgrouping the

autism ‘‘spectrum’’: reflections on DSM-5. PLoS Biol 11(4):

doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001544

Lam, Y. (2014). Pragmatic Language in Autism: An Overview. In V. Patel, V. Preedy &

C. Martin (Eds) Comprehensive Guide to Autism, pp 533-550. doi:

10.1177/136236131038780

Lambek, R., Tannock, R., Dalsgaard, S., Trillingsgaard, A., Damm, D. & Thomsen, P. H.

(2010). Validating neuropsychological subtypes of ADHD: how do children with

and without an executive function deficit differ? Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 51(8), 895-904. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02248.x

Landa R. & Goldberg, M. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high

functioning autism: a continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 35( 5), 557-573. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1

Landry, S. & Smith, K. (2010). Early social and cognitive precursors andparental support

for self regulation and executive function: relations from early childhood into

adolescence. In B. Sokol, J. Carpendale, A. Young and G. Iarocci (Eds), Self and

Social Regulation.Oxford University Press: New York, NY. pp 386-417.

Lee, H. & Park, H. (2007).An integrated literature review on the adaptive behavior of

individuals with Asperger syndrome. Remedial and Special Education, 28, (3),

132–139. doi: 10.1177/07419325070280030201

Lehto, J. (1996). Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity?

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 29–50.

Lehto, E., Juujarvi, P., Kooistra, L. and Pulkkinen, L. (2003), Dimensions of executive

functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental

Psychology, 21: 59–80. doi: 10.1348/026151003321164627

Lemerise, E. & Arsenio, W. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and

cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71(1), 107-118.

doi: 0009-3920/2000/7101-0013.

Page 188: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

177

Leonard, M.; Milich, R. & Lorch, E. (2011). The role of pragmatic language use in

mediating the relation between hyperactivity and inattention and social skills

problems. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(2), 567-579.

doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0058)

Lerner, M., Hutchins, T., Prelock, P. (2011). Brief report: preliminary evaluation of the

theory of mind inventory and its relationship to measures of social skills. Journal

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 512–517. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-

1066-z

Levin, I., Weller, J., Pederson, A., & Harshman, L. (2007). Age-related differences in

adaptive decision making: Sensitivity to expected value in

risky choice. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 225–233

Lewis, M., Lamm, C., Segalowitz, S., Stieben, J. & Selazo, P. (2006).

Neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation in children and adolescents.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18(3), pp. 430–443. No DOI available.

Lewis, A., Zinbarg, R. & Durbin, C. (2010). Advances, problems, and challenges in the

study of emotion regulation: A commentary. Journal of Psychopathology and

Behavioral Assessment, 32, 83-91

Lindner, J. & Rosen, L. (2006). Decoding of emotion through facial expression, prosody

and verbal content in children and adolescents with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal

of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 769–777. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-

0105-2

Lipton, M. & Nowicki, S. (2009). The Social-Emotional Learning Framework (SELF): A

Guide for Understanding Brain-Based Social-Emotional Learning Impairments.

The Journal of Developmental Processes, 4(2), pp. 99-115. (No doi available).

Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L. &

Rapin, I. (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42 (2), 261-270. No doi available.

Lopata, C., Smith, R., Volker, M., Thomeer, M., Lee, G. & McDonald, C. (2013).

Comparison of adaptive behavior measures for children with HFASDs. Autism

Research and Treatment, 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/415989

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation

Schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS), Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., & Schopler,

E. (1989). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: A standardized observation

of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 19, 185–212.

Page 189: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

178

Loukusa, S. & Moilanen, I. (2009). Pragmatic inference abilities in individuals with

Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. A review. Research in Autism

Spectrum Disorders, 3(1), 890–904. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2009.05.002

Luebbe, A., Bell, D., Allwood, M., Swenson, L. & Early, M. (2010). Social information

processing in children: Specific relations to anxiety, depression, and affect.

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 386-399. doi:

10.1080/15374411003691685

Lupowski, 1989. Growing up, growing older: social and emotional needs. Roeper

Review, 11, 124-127.

Luteijn, E., Serra, M., Jackson, S., Steenhuis, M., Althaus, M., Volkmar, F. & Minderaa,

R. (2000). How unspecified are disorders of children with a pervasive

developmental disorder not otherwise specified? A study of social problems in

children with PDD-NOS and ADHD. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

9(3,) 168-179. doi: 10.1007/s007870070040

Maenner, M., Schieve, L., Rice, C., Cunniff, C., Giarelli, E., Kirby, R., Lee, L., Nicholas,

J., Wingate, M. & Durkin, M. (2013). Frequency and pattern of documented

diagnostic features and the age of autism identification. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(4), 401-413.

Mairena, M., Di Martino, A., Dominguez-Martin, C., Gomez-Guerrero, L., Gioia, G.,

Petkova, E. & Castellannos, X. (2012). Low frequency oscillations of response

time explain parent ratings of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. European

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry ,21(2), 101-109. doi:10.1007/s00787-011-0237-6

Mandell, D., Novak., M. & Zubritsky, C. (2005). Factors associated with age of diagnosis

among children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics,. 116 (6), 1480 -1486.

doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0185.

Mangeot, S., Armstrong, K., Colvin, A., Yeates, K. & Taylor, G. (2002). Long-Term

Executive Function Deficits in Children With Traumatic Brain Injuries:

Assessment Using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF),

Child Neuropsychology,8(4), 271-284 dx.doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.4.271.13503

Mangham,C., McGrath,P., Reid,G. & Stewart, M. (1995). Resiliency: Relevance to

health promotion. Minister of Supply and Services, Health Canada: Ottawa,

Ontario. Cat No. H39-330/2-19995E.

Matchullis, R. (2012). The relationship between inhibition and social skills in children

with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. University of Calgary.

Unpublished Dissertation.

Page 190: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

179

Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1995a). Competence, resilience, & psychopathology.

In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol 2. Risk,

disorder, and adaptation (pp. 715-752). New York: Wiley.

Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., Neemann, J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., & Garmezy, N.

(1995b). The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through

adolescence. Child Development, 66, 1635–1659. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1995.tb00956.x

Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable

and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children.

American Psychologist, 53, 205–220.

Matson, J., Wilkins, J., Smith, K. & Ancona, M. (2008). PDD-NOS symptoms in adults

with intellectual disability: toward an empirically oriented diagnostic model.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3). doi: 10.1007/s10803-

007-0422-0

Mathersul, D., Palmer, D., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. E., Cooper, N., Gordon, E., & Williams, L.

(2009) Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: II. Core

domains and relationships with general cognition. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(3), pp. 278 -291. DOI:

10.1080/13803390802043619.

Mayeux, L. & Cillessen, A. (2003). Development of social problem solving in early

childhood: stability, change and associations with social competence. Journal of

Genetic Psychology, 164(2), 153-173. No DOI available.

Mazefsky, C., Pelphrey, K. & Dahl, R. (2012). The need for a broader approach to

emotion regulation research in autism. Child Development Perspectives, 6(1), 92–

97. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00229.x

Mazefsky C. & Oswald, D. (2007). Emotion Perception in Asperger’s Syndrome and

High-functioning Autism: The Importance of Diagnostic Criteria and Cue

Intensity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37:1086–1095. doi

10.1007/s10803-006-0251-6

McCabe, P. & Meller, P. (2004). The relationship between language and social

competence: how language impairment affects social growth. Psychology in the

Schools, 41(3), 313-321. doi: 10.1002/pits.10161

McCauley, T., Chen, S., Goos, L., Schachar, R. & Crosbie, J. (2010). Is the behavior

rating inventory of executive function more strongly associated with measures of

impairment or executive function? Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 495-505. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000093.

Page 191: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

180

McCloskey, 2006. Self regulation executive functions definitions, observed behaviors,

and potential interventions. Unpublished manuscript.

McCloskey, G. (2011). Executive Functions: A General Overview. Unpublished

Manuscript.

McCloskey, G., Perkins, L. & Van Divner, B. (2009). Assessment and Intervention for

Executive Function Difficulties. New York, NY: Routledge.

McCloskey, G. & Perkins, L. (2013). Essentials of Executive Function Assessment. New

Jersey, USA: John Wiley & Sons Publishing.

McClure, E., Treland, J., Snow, J., Schmajuk, M., Dickstein, D., Towbin, K., Charney,

D., Pine, D., Leibenluft, E. (2005). Deficits in social cognition and response

flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162,

1644–1651. No doi available.

Mcdonald, N. & Messinger, D. (2012). Empathic Responding in Toddlers at Risk for an

Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

42(8), 1566-73. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1390-y

McKown, C., Gumbiner, L., Russo, N. Lipton, M. (2009). Social-emotional learning

skill, self-regulation, and social competence in typically developing and clinic-

referred children, Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38 (6), 858

- 871.

McKown, C. (2007). Concurrent Validity and Clinical Usefulness of Several Individually

Administered Tests of Children's Social-Emotional Cognition. Journal of Clinical

Child & Adolescent Psychology, 36, (1), 29 - 41. DOI:

10.1080/15374410709336566

McKown, C., Allen, A., Russo-Ponsaran, N. & Johnson, J. (2013). Direct assessment of

children's social-emotional comprehension. Psychological Assessment. Advance

online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0033435

Meyer J. A., Mundy P. C., Van Hecke A.V. & Durocher J. S. (2006) Social attribution

processes and comorbid psychiatric symptoms in children with Asperger

syndrome. Autism, 10(4), 383–402. doi:10.1177/1362361306064435

Miller, S. (2009). Children’s understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological

Bulletin, 135(5), 749-773. doi: 10.1037/a0016854

Mitchell, D., Rhodes, R., Pine, D. & Blair, R. (2008) The contribution of ventrolateral

and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to response reversal. 187(1): 80–87.

doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.08.034.

Page 192: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

181

Miyake, A., Freidman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A. & Wager, T. (2000).

The unity and diversity of executive functions and their

contributions to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: a latent variable analysis.

Cognitive Psychology 41, 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Mundy, P. (2003). Annotation:The neural basis of social impairments in autism: the role

of the dorsalmedial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry 44(6), 793–809. No doi available.

Murphy, B. & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Young children’s emotionality: regulation and social

functioning and their responses when they are targets of a peer’s anger. Social

Development, 6, 18-36.

Murphy, L., Brinkman, T. & McNamara, K. (2007). Sustained attention and social

competence in typically developing preschool-aged children. Early Child

Development and Care, 177(2), 133-149.

Muscara, F., Catroppa, C. & Anderson, V. (2008). Social problem-solving skills as a

mediator between executive function and long-term social outcome following

paediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2, 445-461. doi:

10.1348/174866407X250820

Narzisi, A., Muratori, F., Calderoni, S., Fabbro, F. & Urgesi, C. (2013).

Neuropsychological profile in high functioning autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1895–1909. doi

10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2009). Family–peer linkages: The

mediational role of attentional processes. Social Development, 18(4), pp. 875-895.

doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00510.x.

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2003). Do children’s attention processes

mediate the link between family predictors and school readiness? Developmental

Psychology, 39, 581–593.

Nickels, K., Katusic, S., Colligan, R., Weaver, A., Voigt, R. & Barbaresi, W. (2008).

Stimulant medication treatment of target behaviors in children with autism: a

population-based study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,

29(2), 75.

Nigg, J., Quamma, J., Greenberg, M. & Kusche, C. (1999). A two-year longitudinal study

of neuropsychological and cognitive performance in relation to behavioral

problems and competencies in elementary school children. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology 27(1), 51-63. No doi available.

Page 193: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

182

Nigg, J., Hinshaw, S., Carte, E. & Treuting, J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of

antisocial behavior and comorbid disruptive behavior disorders in children with

ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 468-480. No doi available.

Nilsen, E. & Fecica, A. (2011). A model of communicative perspective-taking for typical

and atypical populations of children. Developmental Review, 31(1), 55-78.

doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.001

Nowicki, S. (1997). A manual for the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2.

Unpublished manuscript: Department of Psychology, Emory University.

Nowicki, S. & Carton, J. (1993). The measurement of emotional intensity from facial

expressions; the DANVA Faces - 2. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 749-751.

Nowicki, S. & Duke, M. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication

of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of

Nonverbal Behavior, 18 (1), 9-36.

Nowicki, S. & Mitchell, J. (1998). Accuracy in identifying affect in child and adult

faces and voices and social competence in preschool children. Genetic, Social,

and Psychological Monographs, 124, 39-59.

Odom, S., McConnell, S. & Brown, W. (2008). Social competence of young children:

Conceptualization, assessment, and influences. In W. Brown, S. Odom & S.

McConnell, (Eds), Social Competence of Young Children: Risk, Disability, and

Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing, pp. 31–59.

Oerlemans, A., van der Meer, J., van Steijn, D., de Ruiter, S., de Bruijn, Y., de

Sonneville, L., Buitelaar, J. & Rommelse, N. (2013). Recognition of facial

emotion and affective prosody in children with ASD (+ ADHD) and their

unaffected siblings. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 1-15. doi:

10.1007/s00787-013-0446-2

O’Hare, A., Bremner, L., Nash, M., Happe, F. & Pettigrew, L. (2009) A clinical

assessment tool for advanced theory of mind performance in 5 to 12 year olds.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(6), 916–928. doi:

10.1007/s10803-009-0699-2

Orsmond, G., Krauss, M. & Seltzer, M. (2004). Peer relationships and social and

recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 245-256.

Owen, A., McMillan, K., Laird, A. & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory

paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human

Brain Mapping, 25, 46-59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

Page 194: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

183

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., Rogers S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-

functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–105.

Ozonoff, S. & McEnvoy, R. (1994). .A longitudinal study of executive function and

theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6,

415–431.

Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in

autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 31(3), 257-263.

Parrila, R., Ma, X., Fleming, D. & Rinaldi, C. (2002). Development of prosocial skills :

final report. Human Resources Development Canada.

http://www.servicecanada.gc.ca/eng/cs/sp/sdc/pkrf/publications/research/2002-

001274/page01.shtml

Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A & Volkmar, F. (2005) Perception and Production of

Prosody by Speakers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 35(2), doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1999-1

Pearson, D., Loveland, K., Lachar, D., Reddoch, S., Mansour, R. & Cleveland, L. (2006).

A comparison of behavioral and emotional functioning in children and

adolescents with Autistic Disorder and PDD-NOS. Child Neuropsychology, 12 (4-

5), 321-333. doi: 10.1080/09297040600646847

Pearson, D., Aman, M., Arnold, L., Lane, D., Loveland, K., Santos, C., Casat, C.,

Mansour, R., Jerger, S., Ezzell, S., Factor, P., Vanwoerden, S., Ye, E., Narain, P.

& Cleveland, L. (2012). High concordance of parent and teacher attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder ratings in medicated and unmedicated children with

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent

Psychopharmacology, 22(4), 284-91. doi: 10.1089/cap.2011.0067.

Pearson, D., Santos, C., Aman, M., Arnold, L., Casat, C., Mansour, R, Lane, D., ... &

Cleveland, L. (2013). Effects of extended release methylphenidate treatment on

ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and associated

behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD symptoms.

Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 23(5), 337-351. Doi:

10.1089/cap.2012.0096

Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence

predict later understanding of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2),

530-544. doi: 10.1037/a0018287

Page 195: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

184

Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young

children: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology, 43(4),

974-990. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.974

Pelphrey, K., Morris, J., McCarthy, G. & Labar, K. (2005). Perception of dynamic

changes in facial expressions of emotion in autism. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 63.

Pelphrey, K., Morris, J., McCarthy, G. & LaBar, K. (2007). Perception of dynamic

changes in facial affect and identity in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective

Neuroscience, 2(2), 140-149. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm010.

Pennington, B. & Ozonoff, S. (1996).Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51-87. No

doi available.

Peppe, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O'Hare, A. & Rutherford, M (2007). Receptive and

expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of

Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1015 –1028. doi:1092-

4388/07/5004-1015

Perry, A., Flanagan, H., Geier, J. & Freeman, N. (2009). The Vineland Adaptive

Behavior Scales in young children with autism spectrum disorders at different

cognitive levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39:1066–1078

doi 10.1007/s10803-009-0704-9

Peterson, C., Garnett, M., Kelly, A. & Attwood, T. (2009). Everyday social and

conversation applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with

autism-spectrum disorders or typical development. European Journal of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(2), 105-15. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-0711-y

Philip, R., Whalley, H., Stanfield, A., Sprengelmeyer, R., Santos, I., Young, A.,

Atkinson, A., CAlders, A., Johnstone, E., Lawrie, S. & Hall, J. (2010). Deficits in

facial, body movement and vocal emotional processing in autism spectrum

disorders. Psychological Medicine, 40, 1919–1929.

doi:10.1017/S0033291709992364

Place, K. & Becker, J. (1991). The influence of pragmatic competence on the likability of

grade-school children. Discourse Process, 14, 227-243. doi

10.1080/01638539109544783

Raggio, D., Scattone, D. & May, W. (2010). Relationship of the Kaufman brief

intelligence test-second edition and the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence

in children referred for ADHD. Psychological Reports, 106(2), 513-518. doi:

10.2466/PR0.106.2.513-518

Page 196: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

185

Rao, P., Beidel, D. & Murray, M. (2008). Social skills interventions for children with

Asperger’s syndrome or high-functioning autism: a review and recommendations.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 353-

361.doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0402-4

Rasmussen, C., McAuley, R. & Andrew, J. (2007). Parental ratings of children with fetal

alcohol spectrum disorder on the behavior rating inventory of executive function

(BRIEF). Journal of FAS International;5(e2), 1-8.

Rasmussen, C. & Bisanz, J. (2009). Executive functioning in children with Fetal Alcohol

Spectrum Disorders: Profiles and age-related differences. Child Neuropsychology,

15(3(, 201-215. doi: 10.1080/09297040802385400

Raykov, T., Marcoulides, G. & Li, C. (2012). Measurement Invariance for Latent

Constructs in Multiple Populations: A Critical View and Refocus. Educational

and Psychological Measurement, 72(6), 954-974. doi:

10.1177/0013164412441607

Razza, R., & Blair, C. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive

function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied

Developmental Psychology 30, pp. 332–343. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.020.

Reddy, L.; Hale, J. & Brodzinsky, L. (2011). Discriminant validity of the Behavior

Rating Inventory of Executive Function Parent Form for children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 26(1), 45-55. doi:

10.1037/a0022585

Riggs, N., Blair, C. & Greenberg, M. (2003). Concurrent and 2-year longitudinal

relations between executive function and the behavior of 1st and 2nd grade

children. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 267-276.

Riggs, N., Greenberg, M., Kusche, C. & Pentz, M. (2006a). The mediational role of

neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention

program in elementary school students: effects of the PATHS curriculum.

Prevention Science, 7, 91-102.

Riggs, N., Jahromi, L., Razza, R., Dilworth, J. & Mueller, U. (2006b). Executive function

and the promotion of social-emotional competence. Journal of Applied

Developmental Psychology, 27, 300-309.

Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Tonge, B. J., Brereton, A. V., & Bellgrove, M. A.

(2002). A neurobehavioral examination of individuals with high-functioning

autism and Asperger’s disorder using a fronto-striatal model of dysfunction.

Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(2), 164-177.

Page 197: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

186

Rinsky, J. and Hinshaw, R. (2012). Linkages between childhood executive functioning

and adolescent social functioning and psychopathology. Child Neuropsychology,

17(4), 368–390. doi:10.1080/09297049.2010.544649

Roberts, R. & Pennington, B. (1996). An interactive framework for examining prefrontal

cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12(1), 105-126. No doi

available.

Robinson, R., Roberts, W., Strayer, J. & Koopman, R. (2007). Empathy and emotional

responsiveness in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents. Social

Development, 16, 3, 555-579.doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00396.x

Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares?

Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism & Developmental

Disorders, 37(4), 709–715.

Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical review. Social

Development, 6, 111-135. No doi available.

Rosenthal, M., Wallace, G., Lawson, R., Wills, M., Dickson, E., Yerys, B., &

Kenworthy, L. (2013). Impairments in real-world executive function increase

from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology,

27(1), 13–18. doi: 10.1037/a0031299

Rueda, M., Posner, M. & Rothbarth, M. (2005). The development of executive attention:

contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 28(2), 573–594.

Rump, K., Giovannelli, J., Minshew, N. & Strauss, M. (2009). The development of

emotion recognition in individuals with autism. Child Development, 80, 1434-

1447.

Research Units on Pediatric Psychopharmacology (RUPP) Autism Network (2005).

Randomized, controlled, crossover trial of methylphenidate in pervasive

developmental disorders with hyperactivity. Archives of General Psychiatry

62(11), 1266–1274

Russell, J., Hala, S. & Hill, E. (2003). The automated windows task: the performance of

preschool children, children with autism and children with moderate learning

difficulties. Cognitive Development, 18, 111-137. doi: 10.1016/S0885-

2014(02)00163-6

Russel, R. & Grizzle, K. (2008). Assessing child and adolescent pragmatic language

competencies: toward evidence-based assessments. Clinical Child and Family

Psychology Review, 11 (1-2), 59-73.

Page 198: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

187

Russo, N. (2002). Executive function and autism: an exploration of the "HotCold"

distinction. Accessed from McGill University Libraries at

http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/-?func=dbin-jump-

full&object_id=79807&silo_library=GEN01

Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. & Burack, J. (2007).

Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with

autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 77–86.

doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007

Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised.

Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services.

Rys, G., & Bear, G. (1997). Relational aggression and peer relations: Gender and

developmental issues. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 87-106.

Saarni, C. (1990). Emotional competence. In IL Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska symposium:

Socioemotional development (pp. 115-161). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska

Press.

Saarni, C. (1997). Coping with aversive feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 45–63.

Saarni, C. (1999). The continuity dilemma in emotional competence. Psychological

Inquiry, 12( 2), 94-96. jstor.org/stable/1449493.

Scambler, D., Hepburn, E., Rutherford, M., Wehner, E & Rogers, S. (2007). Emotional

responsivity in children with autism, children with other developmental

disabilities, and children with typical development. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 553–563 doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0186-y

Sanders, J., Johnson, K., Garavan, H., Gill. M., & Gallagher, L. (2008). A review of

neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autistic spectrum disorders:

Attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 2, 1–16.

Sanderson, C & Allen, M. (2013). The specificity of inhibitory impairments in autism

and their relation to ADHD-type symptoms. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 43(5): 1065–1079. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1650-5

Sanson, A., Hampton, S. & Hill, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social

development: a review. Social Development, 13(1). no doi available.

Sawa, T., Kodaira, M., Oiji, A., Sasayama, D., Iwadare, Y., Ushijima, H., Usami, M.,

Watanabe, K., Saito, K. (2013). Dysfunction of orbitofrontal and dorsolateral

prefrontal cortices in children and adolescents with high-functioning pervasive

Page 199: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

188

developmental disorders. Annals of General Psychiatry,31(1), 1-13. doi:

10.1186/1744-859X-12-31.

Scambler, D., Hepburn, S., Rutherford, M., Wehner, D. & Rogers, A. (2007). Emotional

responsivity in children with autism, children with other developmental

disabilities, and children with typical development. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 553–563.

doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0186-y

Scheeren, A., Koot, H., Mundy, P., Mous, L. & Begeer, S. (2013).Empathic

responsiveness of children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum

disorder. Autism Research, 6, 362-371. doi: 10.1002/aur.1299

Schulte-Rüther, M., Greimel, E., Piefke, M., Kamp-Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., Fink, G.

R., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. & Konrad, K. (2013). Age-dependent changes in the

neural substrates of empathy in autism spectrum disorder. Social cognitive and

affective neuroscience, nst088.

Schwenck, C., Mergenthaler, J., Keller, K., Zech, J., Salehi, S., Taurines, R., Romanos,

M., Schecklmann, M., Schneider, W., Warnke, A. & Freitag, C. M. (2012).

Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: Group‐specific profiles

and developmental aspects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6),

651-659. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.024

Schohl, K., Amy,V., Van Hecke, A., Carson, M., Dolan, B., Karst, J. & Stevens, S.

(2014). A replication and extension of the peers intervention: examining effects

on social skills and social anxiety in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, doi 10.1007/s10803-013-1900-1

Schulz, K., Fan,J., Magidina,O., Marks, D., Hahn, B. & Haperin, J. (2007). Does the

emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with

measures on a non-emotional analog. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22,

151–160. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001

Segrin, C. (2000). Social skills deficits associated with depression. Clinical Psychology

Review, 20, 379–403.

Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Social Competence in Children. New York: Springer.

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Fine, J. & Bledsoe, J. (2013). Comparison among children with

children with autism spectrum disorder, nonverbal learning disorder and typically

developing children on measures of executive functioning. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorder, published online ahead of print. 10.1007/s10803-013-

1871-2

Page 200: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

189

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A. & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive

functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-

predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 40, 1017–1027. doi 10.1007/s10803-010-0951-9

Sergeant, J. Geurts, H. & Oosterlaan, J. (2004). How specific is a deficit of executive

functioning for Attention-Deficit/HyperactivityDisorder? Behavioural Brain

Research, 130, 3-28.

Sergeant, J., Hilde, G. & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive

functioning for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? Behavioural Brain

Research 130, 3–28.

Shea, V. & Mesibov, G. (2005). Adolescents and adults with autism. In F. Volkmar, R.

Paul, A. Klin & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive

Developmental Disorders, pp. 288-312. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York: NY.

Sibley, M., Evans, S. & Serpell, Z.(2010). Social cognition and interpersonal impairment

in young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral

Assessment, 32, 193-202.

Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of

children with Autism, Down Syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs

of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1 Serial No 256), 1-113.

Simonoff, E., Jones, C., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Happé, F. & Charman, T. (2013). The

persistence and stability of psychiatric problems in adolescents with autism

spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2), 186-194.

doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02606.x

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008).

Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence,

comorbidity and associated factors. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 921–929.

Singer, D. & Bashir, A. (1999). What are executive functions and self-regulation and

what do they have to do with language-learning disorders? Language, Speech,

and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 265–273. No doi available.

Singh, S.D., Ellis, C.R., Winton, A.S., Singh, D.D., Leung, J.P. & Oswald, D.P. (1998).

Recognition of facial expressions of emotion by children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Modification, 22, 245-249.

Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M. & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition,

flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and

Page 201: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

190

without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and

Mental Health, 2(1), 4. doi:10.1186/1753-2000-2-4

Siu, A. & Shek, D. (2010). Social problem solving as a predictor of well-being in

adolescents and young adults. Social Indicators Research, 95(3), 393-406. doi

10.1007/s11205-009-9527-5

Skogli, E., Egeland, J., Andersen, P., Hovik, K. & Oie, M. (2012). Few differences in hot

and cold executive functions in children and adolescents with combined and

inattentive subtypes of ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, (ahead-of-print), 1-20.

doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.753998

Smart, D. & Sanson, A. (2003). Social competence in young adulthood, its nature and

antecedents. Family Matters, 64, 4–9.

Smith, L., Hong, J., Seltzer, M., Greenberg, J., Almeida, D., & Bishop, S.. (2010). Daily

experiences among mothers of adolescents and adults with autism spectrum

disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 40(2), 167-178. doi:

10.1007/s10803-009-0844-y

Sobel, M.,Capps, M. & Gopnik, A. (2005). Ambiguous figure perception and theory of

mind understanding in children with autistic spectrum disorders. British Journal

of Developmental Psychology, 23, 159–174. doi:10.1348/026151004X20694

South, M., Ozonoff, S., Suchy, Y., Kesner, R., McMahon, W. & Lainhart, J. (2008).

Intact emotion facilitation for nonsocial stimuli in autism: is amygdala impairment

in autism specific for social information?. Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 14(01), 42-54. doi: 10.10170S1355617708080107

South, M., Dana, J., White, S., & Crowley, M. (2011). Failure is not an option: Risk-

taking is moderated by anxiety and also by cognitive ability in children and

adolescents diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 41(1), 55-65. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1021-z

Sprenger, L., Bühler, E., Poustka, L., Bach, C., Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., Kamp-

Becker, I., & Bachmann, C. (2013). Impact of ADHD symptoms on autism

spectrum disorder symptom severity. Research in Developmental Disabilities,

34(10), 3545-3552. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.028

Stewart, M., McAdam, C., Ota, M., Peppé, S., & Cleland, J. (2013). Emotional

recognition in autism spectrum conditions from voices and faces. Autism, 17(1),

6-14. doi: 10.1177/1362361311424572

Stichter, J., O’Connor, K., Herzog, M., Lierheimer, K. & McGhee, S. (2012). Social

competence intervention for elementary students with aspergers syndrome and

Page 202: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

191

high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42,

354–366 doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1249-2

Strayer, J. (1993). Children's concordant emotions and cognitions in response to observed

emotions. Child Development, 64, 188-201. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1993.tb02903.x

Strayer, J. & von Rossberg-Gempton, I. (1992). The empathy continuum scoring manual.

http://www.tru.ca/faculty/wlroberts/empathycontinuumscoringmanual.pdf

Swanson, J. (2005). The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: A Review. Canadian

Journal of School Psychology, 20 (1-2), 117-128.. doi:

10.1177/0829573506295469

Swedo,S. (2009).

http://www.dsm5.org/ProgressReports/Pages/0904ReportoftheDSMVNeurodevel

opmentalDisordersWorkGroup.aspx - Report of the DSM-5 Neurodevelopmental

Disorders Work

Szatmari, P. (1992). The validity of autistic spectrum disorders: a literature review.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(4), 583-600. doi:

10.1007/BF01046329

Szatmari, P., Charman, T., & Constantino, J. N. (2012). Into, and Out of, the "Valley of

Death": Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(11), 1108-1112doi:

10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.027

Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Boyle, M., Streiner, D., & Duku, E. (2003). Predictors of

outcome among high functioning children with autism and Asperger syndrome.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 520–528.

Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. (2005). How language facilitates the acquisition

of false-belief understanding in children with autism. In J. Astington, & J. Baird

(Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind, pp. 298–318. Oxford University

Press, USA.

Tabachnick B. & Fidell, L. (2009). Using Multivariate Statistics. Boston: Pearson

Education Inc.

Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self‐control predicts good

adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of

personality, 72(2), 271-324. No doi available.

Thompson, R., Lewis, M. & Calkins, S. (2008). Reassessing emotion regulation. Child

Development Perspectives, 2 (3), 124–131.No doi available.

Page 203: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

192

Tillman, C., Brocki, K., Sorensen, L. & Lundervold, A. (2013). A longitudinal

examination of the developmental executive function hierarchy in children with

externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Attention Disorders. Published online

before print May 15, 2013. doi: 10.1177/1087054713488439

Toplak, M., Sorge, G., Benoit, A., West, R. & Stanovich, K. (2010). Decision-making

and cognitive abilities: A review of associations between Iowa Gambling Task

performance, executive functions, and intelligence, Clinical Psychology Review,

30(5), 562–581. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.002

Toplak, M., West, R. & Stanovich, K. (2013). Practitioner Review: Do performance-

based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct?

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54(2), 131–143.

doi:10.1111/jcpp.12001

Travis, L., Sigman, M. & Ruskin, E. (2001). Links between social understanding and

social behavior in verbally able children with autism. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 31( 2), 119-130. No doi available.

Trentacosta, C. & Fine, S. (2010). Emotion Knowledge, Social Competence, and

Behavior Problems in Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-analytic Review.

Social Development, 19(1), 1-29.

Troyb, E., Rosenthal, M., Eigsti, I., Kelley. E., Tyson, K., Orinstein, A., Barton, M. &

Fein, D. (2013). Executive functioning in individuals with a history of ASDs who

have achieved optimal outcomes. Child Neuropsychology, Epub ahead of

print10.1080/09297049.2013.799644

Tureck, K., Matson, J., Turygin, N. & Macmillan, K. (2013).Rates of psychotropic

medication use in children with ASD compared to presence and severity of

problem behaviors. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1377–1382. doi:

dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.08.003

Tur-Kaspa, H. (2004). Social information processing skills of kindergarten children with

developmental learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,

19(1),3-11.No doi available.

Turygin, N., Matson, J. & Tureck,K. (2013). The relationship of attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder to adaptive skills in young

children. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, early 0nline, 1–5. doi:

10.3109/17518423.2013.846947

Uljarevic, M., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal meta-

analysis. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 43(7), 1517-1526. doi:

10.1007/s10803-012-1695-5

Page 204: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

193

Underwood, B. & Moore, B. (1982). Perspective-taking and altruism. Psychological

Bulletin, 91, 143–173.

van der Meer, J., Oerlemans, A., van Steijn, D., Lappenschaar, M., de Sonneville, L.,

Buitelaar, J. & Rommelse, N. (2012). Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching

disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-based

sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

51(11), 1160-1172.

van Duijvenvoorde, A., Jansen, B., Visser, I., Huizenga, H.(2010). Affective and

cognitive decision-making in adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology,

35(5), 539 -554. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494749

Vickerstaff, S., Heriot, S., Wong, M., Lopes, A., & Dossetor, D. (2007). Intellectual

ability, self-perceived social competence, and depressive symptomatology in

children with high-functioning autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37(9), 1647-1664. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0292-x

Vohs, K. & Baumeister, R. (2004). Understanding self-regulation: An introduction. In K.

Vohs & R. Baumeister (Eds), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and

applications (pp. 1–9). New York: Guilford Press.

Vohs, K. & Baumeister, R. (2011). Understanding self-regulation: Handbook of self-

regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 1–9). New York: Guilford

Press.

Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J. & Bryson, S. (2009). Brief report: pragmatic

language in autism spectrum disorder: relationships to measures of ability and

disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 388–393. doi:

10.1007/s10803-008-0618-y

Volden, J., & Phillips, L. (2010). Measuring pragmatic language in speakers with autism

spectrum disorders: Comparing the Children’s Communication Checklist—2 and

the Test of Pragmatic Language. American Journal of Speech-Language

Pathology, 19(3), 204-212. No doi available.

Volkmar, F. & Klin, A. (2005). Issues in the classification of autism and related

conditions. In F. Volkmar, R.Paul, A. Klin & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of

Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, pp. 5–41. New York: Wiley

&Sons.

Volkmar, F., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive

developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1),

135–170. No doi available.

Page 205: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

194

Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Uchiyama, T., Yoshida, Y., Kuroda, M. &

Wheelright, S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without

autism spectrum conditions: cross-cultural stability. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 37, 1823–1832. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0316

Walker, D., Thompson,A., Waigenbaum, L., Goldberg, J., Bryson, S., Mahoney, W.,

Strabriege, C. & Szatmari, P. (2004). Specifying PDD-NOS: A Comparison of

PDD-NOS, Asperger Syndrome, and Autism. 43(2), 172-180. doi:

10.1097/00004583-200402000-00012

Wass, S. V., & Porayska-Pomsta, K. (2013). The uses of cognitive training technologies

in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Autism, published ahead of print,

doi: 10.1177/1362361313499827

Wasserman, T., & Wasserman, L. D. (2013). Toward an integrated model of executive

functioning in children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 2(2), 88-96. doi:

10.1080/21622965.2013.748394

Wasserman, T., & Wasserman, L. (2012). The sensitivity and specificity of

neuropsychological tests in the diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 1(2), 90–99.

doi:10.1080/21622965/2012.702025

Wecker, N., Kramer, J., Wisniewski, A., Delis, D. & Kaplan, E. (2000). Age effects on

executive ability. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 409-414.

Weems, C. & Pina, P. (2010). The assessment of emotion regulation: improving construct

validity in research on psychopathology in youth. Journal of Psychopathology

and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9178-5

Wellman, H. & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory‐of‐Mind Tasks. Child Development,

75(2), 523-541. 0009-3920/2004/7502-0020

Weiss, J. A., Viecili, M. A., Sloman, L., & Lunsky, Y. (2013). Direct and Indirect

Psychosocial Outcomes for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and their

Parents Following a Parent-involved Social Skills Group Intervention. Journal of

the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 22(4), 303. no doi

available.

White, S., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social skills development in children with

autism spectrum disorders: A review of the intervention research. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10), 1858-1868. doi: 10.1007/s10803-

006-0320-x

Page 206: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

195

White, B., Jarrett, M. & Ollendick, T. (2013). Self-regulation deficits explain the link

between reactive aggression and internalizing and externalizing behavior

problems in children. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

35(1), 1-9

Wing, L. (2005). Reflections on opening pandora’s box. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 197-203. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1998-2

Wing, L., Gould, J. & Gillberg, C. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders in the DSM-V:

better or worse than the DSM-IV? Research in developmental disabilities, 32(2),

768-773. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.003

Wolfe, K., Walsh, K., Reynolds, N., Mitchell, F., Reddy, A., Paltin, I. & Madan-Swain,

A. (2013). Executive functions and social skills in survivors of pediatric brain

tumor. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development

in Childhood and Adolescence, 19(4), 370-384,

doi:10.1080/09297049.2012.669470

Wright, B., Clarke, N., Jordan, J. O., Young, A. W., Clarke, P., Miles, J., Nation, K.,

Clarke, L. & Williams, C. (2008). Emotion recognition in faces and the use of

visual context Vo in young people with high-functioning autism spectrum

disorders. Autism, 12(6), 607-626.

Yeates, K., Swift, E., Taylor, H., Wade, S., Drotar, D., Stancin, T. & Minich, N. (2004).

Short-and long-term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury.

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(03), 412-426. doi:

10.10170S1355617704103093

Yeates, K., Bigler, E., Dennis, M., Gerhard, C., Rubin, K., Stancin, R., Taylor G., &

Vannatta, K. (2007). Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: A heuristic

integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. Psychological

Bulletin, 133(3), 535–556. doi : 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.535.

Yechiam E, Arshavsky O, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Yaniv S, Aharon J. (2010). Adapted to

explore: reinforcement learning in autistic spectrum disorder. Brain and

Cognition, 72(2), 317-24. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.005

Yerys, B., Wallace, G., Sokoloff, J., Shook, D., James, J. & Kenworthy, L. (2009).

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms moderate cognition and

behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2(6), 322–

333.

Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C. & Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in

high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63(1). 150-160.

Page 207: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

196

Ylvisaker, M. & Feeney, T. (2002). Executive functions, self-regulation, and learned

optimism in paediatric rehabilitation: a review and implications for intervention.

Pediatric Rehabilitation, 5(2), 51-70. doi: 10.1080/136384902100004189 1

Zachman, L., Huisingh, R., Barrett, M., Orman, J. & LoGiudice, C. (1994). Elementary

Test of Problem Solving Revised: Examiner’s manual. East Moline, IL:

Linguisystems.

Zelazo, P., Qu, L. & Kesek, A. (2010). Hot executive function: emotion and the

development of cognitive control. In S. Calkins & M. Bell (Eds), Child

Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition, 97-111. Washington,

DC, US: American Psychological Association, 261 pp. doi: 10.1037/12059-006

Zelazo, P., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function:

Relations in early development. Young children’s cognitive development:

Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability,

and theory of mind, 71-93.

Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential benefits of mindfulness training in

early childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective.

Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 154-160.

Zelazo, P. & Carlson, S. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and

adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4),

354-360.

Zelazo, P. & Muller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development.

In U. Goswani (Ed), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development,

445-469. Malden: Blackwell Publishing, 761 pp. doi:

10.1002/9780470996652.ch20

Zhou, Q., Chen, S. & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on

Children’s effortful control and executive function: a call for an integrated model

of self‐regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 112-121. doi:

10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00176.x

Ziv, Y., Hadad, B. & Khateeb, Y. (2013). Social Information Processing in Preschool

Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 1-14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1935-3

Page 208: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

197

APPENDIX A: Ethical Approval

Page 209: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

198

Page 210: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

199

APPENDIX B: Recruitment Poster

Project Title: The Role of Executive Function and Social Emotional Leaning Skills in Social Competence of Boys with and without High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder

Overview and Description:

8-12 year old boys with and without high functioning autism spectrum disorder (Asperger's, PDD NOS, high functioning autism) and their parent(s) are being invited to take part in a study of social competence, which is how well we get along with others. Social competence is important because it is associated with academic success and overall mental health. Learning more about which factors best predict social competence may later help us provide useful treatments for children who have social difficulties. This study is designed to examine which factors are most important in getting along with others.

What's involved in the study?

Before you agree to participate, we would like to give you some information about what's involved with this study. Parents will be asked to complete 3 questionnaires – 2 about your child's daily social skills and 1 about Executive Functions (i.e., attention, planning, memory). Children will be asked to complete 10 short tasks. There is no reading, writing or mathematics involved in any of the tasks. Some are computerized activities where the child has to remember objects he saw on the screen, pay attention to instructions and solve problems. Others involve looking at pictures and answering questions about what the characters feel and think. This will take approximately 90 minutes. Breaks and snacks will be provided. To recognize your child's participation, he will be provided with a Cineplex gift certificate for a movie, popcorn and drink.

Please call or email me if you want more information or are interested in participating.

Nathalie Berard Primary Researcher Department of Psychology, University of Regina Supervisors: Lynn Loutzenhiser, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4078)

Dennis Alfano, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4220)

The Research Ethics Board of the University of Regina has approved of this project. If you have any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may contact Dr. Bruce Plouffe, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at the Office of Research Services (AH 505) at 585-4775 or by email to [email protected]. The Research Ethics Board of the Regina Qu'Appelle Health Region has approved of this project.

If you have any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may

contact Dr. Elan Paluck, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at (306) 766-5451.

Page 211: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

200

APPENDIX C: Consent and Assent Forms

INFORMATION AND ASSENT FORM FOR CHILDREN

PROJECT TITLE: The Role of Executive Functions and Social Emotional Learning Skills in Social Competence of Boys with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder.

Student Researcher: Nathalie Berard, MEd, University of Regina: Department of Psychology Supervisors: Dennis Alfano, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4220)

Lynn Loutzenhiser, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4078).

You are being invited to join a study that is looking at which things are most important for

making friends and getting along with others.

What will I be asked to do?

(1) We will meet today for about 1.5 hours, with breaks when needed. You will be asked to complete 10 different activities, including answering questions about what people think and feel in stories, pictures and short social scenarios. You will also be asked to do some work on the computer. There is NO reading, writing or mathematics involved.

(2) You will get one gift certificate for a Cineplex movie theatre with popcorn and a drink to thank you for taking part in the study.

(3) You will be given a copy of this assent form to keep

Page 212: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

201

Are there any risks if I do the study?

No, we do not think that there is any harm to you if you do this study. You may become tired

during the tasks and can take a break at any time. We will also have scheduled breaks where

you will be provided snacks and water or juice.

Are there any benefits if I do the study?

Your participation will help us learn important information, such as what skills are important for

getting along well with others, and how we can help children learn those skills.

Will anybody know how I answered the questions?

It is our job to keep your answers private, which means that we do not talk to anyone else about

what you tell us. All of the information we collect will be kept in a locked cabinet and on a

computer that needs a password, at the University of Regina. Your name will not be stored on

the computer or on any of your answer sheets.

Do I have to participate?

No, you only have to join the study if you want to. You can chose if you want to do it. Also, if

you do choose to do the study you can stop at any time, you can skip any questions that make

you uncomfortable, and/or you can ask that the answers you give not be used in the study.

Nobody will be mad if you decide not to do it, or if you decide to stop doing the study before

you are finished. It is your choice. If you decide to stop doing the study at any point, you will

still receive the gift certificate to the movie theatre.

Who can I talk to if I have any questions?

If you have any questions or have something to say about the study you can call the person

doing the study, Nathalie Berard at (306) 585 4078, or email her at [email protected].

You can also contact the (supervisors) people in charge, Dr.Lynn Loutzenhiser at (306) 585 4820

(email: [email protected]) or Dr. Dennis Alfano, at (306) 585-4220 (email:

[email protected]).

If you have questions about your rights as a person doing it (participant), you may contact the

Chair of the Research Ethics Board at (306) 585-4775 (email: [email protected]).

Page 213: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

202

I AGREE TO PARTICIPATE IN THE STUDY

I UNDERSTAND I CAN STOP AT ANY TIME

______________________________________ ______________________________

Printed name of participant Date

____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of Witness Date

_____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of principal investigator/designated Date

representative-if applicable

The Research Ethics Board of the University of Regina has approved of this project. If you have

any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may contact Dr.

Bruce Plouffe, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at the Office of Research Services (AH 505) at

585-4775 or by email to [email protected].

The Research Ethics Board of the Regina Qu'Appelle Health Region has approved of this project.

If you have any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may

contact Dr. Elan Paluck, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at (306) 766-5451.

Page 214: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

203

INFORMATION AND CONSENT FORM FOR PARENTS

PROJECT TITLE: The Role of Executive Functions and Social Emotional Learning Skills in Social Competence of Boys with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder.

Student Researcher: Nathalie Berard, Doctoral student, University of Regina.

Supervisors: Dennis Alfano, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4220)

Lynn Loutzenhiser, Ph.D., University of Regina (585-4078).

Introduction: You and your child are being invited to take part in a study that is looking at which factors

are most important for making friends and getting along with others.

Purpose: The primary purpose of the study is to find out if and how certain skills predict social competence. Specifically, I am looking at whether social emotional learning (e.g. recognizing facial emotions, solving social problems, understanding others' thoughts and feelings) and executive function (e.g. attention, memory, planning) are associated with children's ability to interact well with others.

Voluntary Participation: Participation in this study is completely voluntary, so it is your decision whether or not you want to take part. To help you decide whether you do or do not want to participate, it is important to understand what this research involves. This consent form will describe the study, the purpose of the research, what will happen during the study, and the possible risks and benefits. If you do decide to take part in this study, you will be asked to sign this consent form to indicate your informed consent to participate. Although, even after you sign you can choose to drop-out at any time, refuse to answer any questions, as well as request that the information collected not be used. Lack of participation will not result in any negative consequences or affect any services you and your child access in the RQHR or any future services you may access.

Who is conducting the study: The Primary Investigator is Nathalie Berard. She is a registered

psychologist who is completing her doctorate in clinical psychology at the University of Regina. This

project is a part of a Ph.D. dissertation required for partial fulfillment of the University of Regina’s

Psychology Ph.D. program.

Procedures: You and your child will come to the University of Regina to participate. First, you will be asked to read and sign an informed consent form. Then your child will be read information regarding the study and be asked to sign an assent form that says that he agrees to participate in the study. Following consent and child assent, you (the parent/guardian) will be asked to complete a short demographic form and three questionnaires asking your view of your child’s daily social skills and interactions with others as well as some executive function abilities (e.g. memory, planning,

Page 215: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

204

attention). Next, your child will be asked to complete a series of questions and tasks related to social situations as well executive function.

These include 10 tasks that involve (1) answering questions about social situations in stories and pictures; and (2) tasks related to executive functions, that involves some work on the computer. There will not be any reading, writing or mathematics. The total time is expected to be approximately 1.5 to 2 hours, however it may take less time, and breaks will be provided when necessary.

In addition, if your child has been diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (Autism; Asperger's; PDD NOS), then you will also be asked to provide written confirmation of the diagnosis or provide permission for Nathalie Berard (principal researcher) to conduct a file review to confirm your child's diagnosis.

Potential Risks: There are no known or anticipated risks to you, or your child, by participating in this research. Your child may become tired during the tasks and can take a break at any time. We will also provide scheduled breaks where your child will be provided snacks and water or juice.

Potential Benefits: No direct benefit can be guaranteed. However, it is anticipated that the findings from this investigation will help us better understand the factors related to social interactions and friendships. Although participants may not benefit directly from this study, it has the potential to greatly improve our understanding of social competence.

Compensation: Children will be given a Cineplex movie and popcorn gift certificate for their participation. They will receive this even if they decide to discontinue the testing. Children will also be given small rewards during some of the tasks, such as fidget toys, bouncy balls; toy cars or pencils/erasers.

Confidentiality: Any information gathered during the data collection process is strictly confidential and will be used for research purposes only by the University of Regina. All information collected will be made anonymous. The electronic file will not contain any identifying information. The consent and assent forms (containing the participants' names) will be kept separate from the participant responses in a locked cabinet. Participant names will not be put on the demographics forms, questionnaires or response sheets. All of the information that we collect will be stored on a lab computer (requiring an access code) at the University of Regina in the Behavioural Neuroscience Research lab for 5 years, after which time the electronic files will be deleted and paper material will be shredded. This research does NOT involve a psychological assessment or intervention for your child and results will not be documented in any RQHR records or files.

Right to Withdraw: As a reminder, your participation as well as your child's participation is voluntary and you and your child can answer only those questions that you are comfortable with. You may withdraw from the research project for any reason, at any time without explanation or penalty of any sort. Should you wish to withdraw, you may exit the study at any time and any information obtained will be permanently deleted from our data collection. Your right to withdraw data from the study will apply until data has been analyzed. After this it is possible that some form of research dissemination will have already occurred and it may not be possible to withdraw your data.

Page 216: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

205

Participant Consent to Participate

I have read and understand the information provided for the study as described herein.

I have had the opportunity to have my questions answered.

I agree to participate as well as permit my child to participate in this study.

I agree to provide written confirmation of my child's diagnosis or allow the principal researcher to conduct a file review to confirm my child's diagnosis.

I understand that I am not giving up my legal rights as a result of signing this consent form.

I understand that I can withdraw from this study at any time and for any reason.

I have been given a copy of this form.

I AGREE TO PARTICIPATE IN THE STUDY AND

(1) complete an information sheet and 3 questionnaires

______________________________________ ______________________________

Printed name of participant's parent Date

______________________________________ ______________________________

Signature of participant's parent Date

____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of Witness Date

_____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of principal investigator/designated Date

Page 217: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

206

representative-if applicable

(2) allow my child to participate in the study

______________________________________ ______________________________

Printed name of participant's parent Date

______________________________________ ______________________________

Signature of participant's parent Date

____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of Witness Date

_____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of principal investigator/designated Date

representative-if applicable

Page 218: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

207

IF MY CHILD HAS A DIAGNOSIS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER,

I ALSO AGREE TO

(4) either provide confirmation of diagnosis through a letter or report, or allow the researcher to complete a file review to confirm diagnosis through a file review.

______________________________________ ______________________________

Printed name of participant's parent Date

______________________________________ ______________________________

Signature of participant's parent Date

____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of Witness Date

_____________________________________ ________________________

Signature of principal investigator/designated Date

representative-if applicable

Page 219: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

208

Contact Information:

If you have any questions, feedback or comments about the research study or the results of the

research study, please feel free to contact the Principal Investigator, Nathalie Berard, or her

direct supervisors, Dr. Lynn Loutzenhiser or Dr. Dennis Alfano.

Primary Investigator: Nathalie Berard Department of Psychology University of Regina 3737 Wascana Parkway Regina, SK S4S 0A2 Phone: (306) 585 4078 Email: [email protected] Research Supervisor: Lynn Loutzenhiser, Ph.D., R. D. Psych.

Professor Department of Psychology University of Regina 3737 Wascana Parkway Regina SK S4S 0A2

Phone: (306) 585-4180 Email: [email protected] Research Supervisor: Dr. Dennis Alfano, Ph.D., R. D. Psych.

Professor Department of Psychology University of Regina 3737 Wascana Parkway Regina SK S4S 0A2

Phone: (306) 585-4220 Email: [email protected]

The Research Ethics Board of the University of Regina has approved of this project. If you have

any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may contact Dr.

Bruce Plouffe, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at the Office of Research Services (AH 505) at

585-4775 or by email to [email protected].

The Research Ethics Board of the Regina Qu'Appelle Health Region has approved of this project.

If you have any questions or concerns about your rights as a research participant, you may

contact Dr. Elan Paluck, Chair of the Research Ethics Board at (306) 766-5451.

Page 220: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

209

APPENDIX D: Visual Schedule Provided to Children

Faces

Voices

Computer

Pictures & Questions

Computer

Pictures & Questions

Speed Game

Stories

Bead Game

Listening

I Feel Questionnaire

Hungry Donkey

Puzzles & Definitions

Page 221: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

210

APPENDIX E: List of Measures

SEL Variables Measures

Facial affect recognition DANVA-2: Child faces

Linguistic affect recognition DANVA-2: Child Paralanguage

Theory of mind Strange Stories (Happe, 1994)

Empathy Bryant Empathy Continuum

Pragmatics Test Of Pragmatic Language (TOPS-2)

Social problem solving Test of Problem Solving (TOPS-3)

EF Variables

Measures

Auditory Attention NEPSY-II: Auditory attention & response set

Planning Tower of London

Visual Attention

Working Memory

Test of Executive Control (TEC)

TEC: n-back

Inhibition NEPSY-II Inhibition TEC: Go/No-Go task

Affective Decision Making Hungry Donkey Task

Parent EF Ratings Behavior Ratings Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF)

Social Competence

Social Skills Inventory System (SSIS)

Vineland Adaptive Bhaviour Scales 2 (VABS2): Socialisation Domain

Page 222: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

211

APPENDIX F: Demographic Questionnaire

Dear Parent,

Thank you for volunteering to participate in our study. Please complete the following personal information. This confidential information will be kept separate from the parent and teacher questionnaires and your child's responses, and will be locked in a filing cabinet at the University of Regina.

PARENT'S NAME:

CHILD'S NAME:

CHILD'S DATE OF BIRTH:

DOES YOUR CHILD HAVE A DIAGNOSIS?

WHAT IS THE DIAGNOSIS:

AGE OF CHILD AT TIME OF DIAGNOSIS:

NAME OF MEDICATION (if any):

DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY

Regina, Saskatchewan

Canada S4S 0A2

phone: (306) 585-4221

fax: (306) 585-5429

email:

[email protected]

Page 223: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

212

PARENT INFORMATION

What is the highest degree or level of school you have completed?

o No high school diploma

o High school diploma or the equivalent (for example: GED)

o College graduate

o Bachelor's degree

o Master's degree

o Doctorate degree (e.g. Ph.D.; Ed.D)

o Other: Please indicate

What is your total household income?

o Less than 24 999

o 25 000 – 50 000

o 50 000 – 75 000

o 75 000 – 100 000

o 100 000 – 125 000

o 125 000 – 150 000

o More than 150 000

Page 224: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

213

APPENDIX G: Record Forms for SEL measures:

Page 225: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

214

Answer Sheet for DANVA2 - Child Faces & Voices

1. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 17. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

2. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 18. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

3. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 19. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

4. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 20. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

5. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 21. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

6. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 22. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

7. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 23. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

8. Happy Sad Angry Fearful 24. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

9. Happy Sad Angry Fearful

10.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

11.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

12.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

13.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

14.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

15.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

16.Happy Sad Angry Fearful

Page 226: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

215

Answer Key For Child Faces 2

Item Emotion Intensity

1 Angry Low

2 Happy High

3 Happy Low

4 Fearful Low

5 Sad High

6 Sad High

7 Angry High

8 Happy High

9 Angry Low

10 Sad Low

11 Fearful Low

12 Happy Low

13 Sad High

14 Angry Low

15 Fearful Low

16 Happy High

17 Sad Low

18 Fearful High

19 Fearful High

20 Angry High

21 Sad Low

22 Fearful igh

23 Happy Low

24 Angry High

Page 227: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

216

ANSWER KEY FOR CHILD VOICES 2.

Item Emotion Intensity

1 Happy High

2 Sad Low

3 Angry Low

4 Angry High

5 Happy Low

6 Fearful Low

7 Angry High

8 Sad High

9 Fearful High

10 Happy High

11 Sad Low

12 Happy Low

13 Fearful High

14 Angry High

15 Sad High

16 Fearful Low

17 Happy High

18 Sad Low

19 Angry Low

20 Fearful High

21 Angry Low

22 Fearful Low

23 Happy Low

24 Sad High

Page 228: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

217

Bryant Empathy Questionnaire

It makes me sad to see a boy who can’t find anyone to play with

0 1 2 3

I get upset when I see a boy being hurt

0 1 2 3

Seeing a boy who is crying makes me feel like crying

0 1 2 3

I get upset when I see an animal being hurt

0 1 2 3

I really like to watch people open presents, even when I don’t get a present myself

0 1 2 3

Some songs make me so sad I feel like crying

0 1 2 3

Sometimes I cry when I watch TV

0 1 2 3

0 - not at all true

1- a little true

2- pretty much true

3- very much true

Page 229: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

218

Happe Strange Stories

Lie (Dentist)

John hates going to the dentist because every time he goes to the dentist he needs a

filling, and that hurts a lot. But John knows that when he has toothache, his mother

always takes him to the dentist. Now John has bad toothache at the moment, but when his

mother notices he is looking ill and asks him ‘‘Do you have toothache, John?’’. John says

‘‘No, Mummy’’.

1. Is it true what John says to his mother?

2. Why does John say this?

White Lie (Hat)

One day Aunt Jane came to visit Peter. Now Peter loves his aunt very much, but today

she is wearing a new hat; a new hat which Peter thinks is very ugly indeed. Peter thinks

his aunt looks silly in it, and much nicer in her old hat. But when Aunt Jane asks Peter,

‘‘How do you like my new hat?’’ Peter says, ‘‘Oh, it’s very nice’’.

1. Was it true what Peter said?

2. Why did he say it?

Sarcasm (Picnic)

Sarah and Tom are going on a picnic. It is Tom’s idea, he says it is going to be a lovely

sunny day for a picnic. But just as they are unpacking the food, it starts to rain and soon

they are both soaked to the skin. Sarah is angry. She says ‘‘Oh yes, a lovely day for a

picnic alright!’’

1. Is it true what Sarah says?

2. Why does she say this?

Joke (Haircut)

Daniel and Ian see Mrs. Thompson coming out of the hairdressers 1 day. She looks a bit

funny because the hairdresser has cut her hair much too short. Daniel says to Ian, ‘‘She

must have been in a fight with a

lawnmower!’’

1. Is it true what Daniel says?

2. Why does he say this?

Double Bluff (Bat)

Simon is a big liar. Simon’s brother Jim knows this, he knows that Simon never tells the

truth! Now yesterday Simon stole Jim’s bat and Jim knows Simon has hidden it

somewhere, though he can’t find it. He’s very upset. So he finds Simon and he says

‘‘Where is my bat? You must have hidden it either in the cupboard or under your bed,

because I’ve looked everywhere else. Where is it, in the cupboard or under your bed?’’

Simon tells him the bat is under his bed.

Page 230: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

219

1. Was it true what Simon told Jim?

2. Where will Jim look for his bat? 3. Why will Jim look there for his bat?

Happe Strange Stories Scoring Procedure

LIE(DENTIST)

Incorrect: Physical

He didn’t have toothache

Incorrect: Psychological

He’s just saying it for a joke

Correct: Partial psychological state

He thinks it’s really sore and he’s ill

He doesn’t like going to the dentist

He doesn’t like the dentist

He hates going to the dentist

Correct: Physical state

It hurts when he gets a filling

It’s sore when he goes to the dentist

He’s got toothache/He had toothache

He needs to go to the dentist

It hurts when he gets the toothache

Because they hurt

Because when you get a filling it’s sore

So that he doesn’t have to go to the dentist

Correct: Psychological state full and accurate answer

He doesn’t want to go to the dentist

He doesn’t want a filling

He doesn’t want to get a filling because it hurts

He doesn’t want to get hurt

He knows that it’ll hurt a lot

WHITE LIE (HAT)

Incorrect: Physical

It’s got a(u)nts on it

It looked nice

Psychological

He liked the hat

He wanted one

He liked the old hat

Incorrect:Psychological

Page 231: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

220

The lady asked him

It looked horrible

Partial psychological state

He didn’t want to get a row

He didn’t want to get into trouble

He didn’t like the hat

He loved his aunt

Psychological state full and accurate answer

To make his auntie feel that he likes it

He didn’t want his auntie to think that he didn’t like it

He didn’t want her to get sad/to make his auntie sad

He didn’t want to hurt her feelings

He didn’t want to upset his auntie

So his auntie wouldn’t be offended

He didn’t want to tell her he hated it

He didn’t want to be rude

He didn’t want to be nasty to her

He wanted to make his auntie feel good

He wanted to make his auntie happy

SARCASM (PICNIC)

Incorrect: Physical

Because she says it’s a lovely day but it’s not

If they’re eating sandwiches they might get wet

So that she could have a picnic

Incorrect: Psychological

She was exaggerating

She likes rain

She thinks it’s a sunny day

She wanted it to be sunny

She really wanted to go on a picnic

She wanted Tom to get wet

She was pretending

She didn’t want to hurt Tom’s feelings/make him sad

Because Tom thought she’d give him a row

Partial psychological state

She was cross

She was trying to be funny

People say stuff like that when they’re angry

She was annoyed with Tom

She’s angry

She’s copying what Tom says but it isn’t really

She’s joking

Page 232: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

221

Physical state

It was sunny and it started raining

It’s raining

Because Tom said it was going to be sunny but it wasn’t

The boy thought it was going to be a lovely day but it wasn’t

Psychological state full and accurate answer

She’s being sarcastic

JOKE (HAIRCUT)

Incorrect: Physical

Because her hair is cut like the grass

She doesn’t wear nice clothes

She went to the hairdresser and got her hair cut

Incorrect: Psychological

He doesn’t want to upset her

He thinks that’s what it looks like

He doesn’t like it, he’s just pretending

He thought it looked like she’d got it cut by a lawn mower/had a fight with a lawnmower

He’s lying

He was being sarcastic

Physical state

Because her hair is funny

Because her hair looks quite short/hair is too short

Because her hair is too short for a woman, she’s in the army!

Because her hair got cut too short

Because she doesn’t look nice

Because it looks like a lawn mower has cut her hair

Because Mrs. T looks like a boy

Partial psychological state

He didn’t like her hair

Psychological state full and accurate answer

He wants to make his friend laugh/impress his friend

He wants to make fun of her

To have a laugh/to have a joke/be funny

To slag off Mrs. T

It’s a/for a joke

To act smart

He’s being silly in front of his friend

Double Bluff (Bat)

Incorrect: Physical

Page 233: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

222

He said it was under the bed or in the cupboard

Because it’s a good hiding place

He would look under the bed because he would see the lumps on it

I’d look everywhere just in case, may be in the shed

Incorrect: Psychological

He can’t remember

He thinks in there or in the cupboard

I think it’s broken and in the bin

I think it’s in the cupboard because he wouldn’t have hid it under

his bed in case his mum found it

He wants to play ping-pong/find it

He doesn’t know Simon never tells the truth

He doesn’t know where it is

Physical state

Because Simon said so/told him

Partial psychological state

It wont be there because Simon lied

Simon never tells the truth

Because it’s the opposite from what Simon says

Because Simon is a liar/always lies

Simon never tells the truth

Psychological state full and accurate answer

Because he doesn’t believe Simon

Jim knows Simon is a liar/always lies

Page 234: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

223

Hungry Donkey Record Sheet

Response Options

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40

DECK A

DECK B

Page 235: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

224

Response Options

41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80

DECK A

DECK B

Page 236: Executive Function, Social Emotional Learning, and A ...ourspace.uregina.ca/bitstream/handle/10294/5735/Berard_Nathalie... · Nathalie Catherine Marie Berard, candidate for the degree

225

Response Options

81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90

DECK A

DECK B

Response Options

91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100

DECK A

DECK B