Top Banner
Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control Gordon B. Moskowitz a, , Peizhong Li b a Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, USA b School of Public Administration, Hohai University, Nanjing, China abstract article info Article history: Received 9 July 2009 Revised 18 August 2010 Available online 26 August 2010 Keywords: Stereotyping Goals Implicit goals Goal shielding Control Stereotype activation Inhibition Stereotype activation is often described as beyond control, unable to be prevented by willing it or engaging the self-regulatory system. Four experiments illustrate that this initial stage of the stereotyping process is controllable. Stereotypes are shown to be implicitly inhibited as part of a goal shielding process. In each experiment, egalitarian goals are triggered through a task in which participants contemplate a past failure at being egalitarian to African American men. This is followed in each experiment by a task that measures stereotype activation/inhibition using reaction times to words (either control words or stereotype-relevant words) that follow the presentation of either faces of Black or White men. The rst two experiments examine participants with egalitarian goals versus those with a control goal, whereas the last two experiments examine people with egalitarian goals versus those whose egalitarian strivings have been satised (by contemplating success at being egalitarian). Only participants with egalitarian goals exhibit stereotype inhibition, and this occurs despite the fact that they lack awareness of the inhibition and lack the conscious intent to inhibit stereotypes at the time the response is made. © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Ironically, it is often the case that the conscious effort associated with goal pursuit and action control undermines achievement. The very act of trying is one's undoing. For example, thinking hard about losing weight can make weight-loss more difcult (one is focused more on food, especially fattening foods, than when not dieting at all). The current research illustrates how lack of consciousness in a goal pursuit can help us attain what we want. This is illustrated in the domain of controlling oneself from stereotyping or pursuing egalitarian goals. Consciously trying to not stereotypemay succeed, but can be quashed by a number of problems associated with how the goal is pursued. One may have a bad strategy for pursuing the goal (such as waiting until one recognizes oneself as biased before taking action; a bad strategy because such recognition rarely occurs). One may have a good strategy that is implemented poorly (detecting one's stereotypes and attempting to take them into account, but not adequately weighting their inuence). One may have a strategy that increases stereotyping as an ironic and undesired effect of processes that accompany the steps one takes when pursuing the goal (such as suppressing a stereotype only to see it return more powerfully later in time; e.g., Wegner, 1994). A nonconscious goal to not stereotype can yield success free of these pitfalls of conscious control. Our experiments reveal that stereotypes are inhibited when one encounters a member of a stereotyped group. This is a proactive form of control because it prevents a stereotype from ever being retrieved from memory, despite perceivers having categorized the person to a social group. It occurs prior to awareness of either the goal or stereotype activation. It requires no effortful expenditure of mental resources (cf., Fiske, 1989), nor for one to be aware of the bias, nor for one to consciously try to prevent bias from having inuence (correct- ingfor the stereotype). Stereotyping others is goal-driven We begin with two statements about stereotyping that are likely intuitively obvious, and clear to experts and nonexperts alike. First, a stereotype is knowledge that exists in the mind of an individual that is associated with a group of people, knowledge that is learned from, and shared with, others in the culture. Second, stereotyping is a process that unfolds in phases (e.g., Kunda & Spencer, 2003): In an initial phase, the stereotype is pulled from memory due to its association with a category (e.g., old, fat, Black, Jew, woman) that has recently been used to identify a person. In the next phase, this perceptually readystereotype is used to help one understand others and plan behavior toward those others. These two assertions lead to perhaps less obvious third and fourth statements. The third is that, regarding when stereotypes are pulled from memory, it is far more common and silent (we rarely recognize this has happened) than people know (e.g., Devine, 1989). The fourth is that stereotypes are triggered and used for a reason; they serve a goal. As such, they have Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103116 This research was supported by NSF grant BCS-0213693 to Gordon B. Moskowitz. Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (G.B. Moskowitz). 0022-1031/$ see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.014 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp
14

Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

Mar 30, 2023

Download

Documents

Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

j ourna l homepage: www.e lsev ie r.com/ locate / jesp

Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form ofstereotype control☆

Gordon B. Moskowitz a,⁎, Peizhong Li b

a Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, USAb School of Public Administration, Hohai University, Nanjing, China

☆ This research was supported by NSF grant BCS-021⁎ Corresponding author.

E-mail address: [email protected] (G.B. Moskowitz)

0022-1031/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Aldoi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.014

a b s t r a c t

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 9 July 2009Revised 18 August 2010Available online 26 August 2010

Keywords:StereotypingGoalsImplicit goalsGoal shieldingControlStereotype activationInhibition

Stereotype activation is often described as beyond control, unable to be prevented by willing it or engaging theself-regulatory system. Four experiments illustrate that this initial stage of the stereotyping process iscontrollable. Stereotypes are shown to be implicitly inhibited as part of a goal shielding process. In eachexperiment, egalitarian goals are triggered through a task in which participants contemplate a past failure atbeing egalitarian toAfricanAmericanmen. This is followed ineachexperimentbya task thatmeasures stereotypeactivation/inhibition using reaction times to words (either control words or stereotype-relevant words) thatfollow the presentation of either faces of Black or White men. The first two experiments examine participantswith egalitarian goals versus those with a control goal, whereas the last two experiments examine people withegalitarian goals versus those whose egalitarian strivings have been satisfied (by contemplating success at beingegalitarian). Only participantswith egalitarian goals exhibit stereotype inhibition, and this occurs despite the factthat they lack awareness of the inhibition and lack the conscious intent to inhibit stereotypes at the time theresponse is made.

3693 to Gordon B. Moskowitz.

.

l rights reserved.

© 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction

Ironically, it is often the case that the conscious effort associatedwithgoal pursuit and action control undermines achievement. The very act oftrying is one's undoing. For example, thinking hard about losing weightcan make weight-loss more difficult (one is focused more on food,especially fattening foods, than when not dieting at all). The currentresearch illustrates how lack of consciousness in a goal pursuit can helpus attain what we want. This is illustrated in the domain of controllingoneself from stereotyping or pursuing egalitarian goals.

Consciously trying to “not stereotype” may succeed, but can bequashed by a number of problems associated with how the goal ispursued. One may have a bad strategy for pursuing the goal (such aswaitinguntil one recognizes oneself as biasedbefore taking action; a badstrategy because such recognition rarely occurs). One may have a goodstrategy that is implemented poorly (detecting one's stereotypes andattempting to take them into account, but not adequately weightingtheir influence). Onemay have a strategy that increases stereotyping asan ironic andundesired effect of processes that accompany the stepsonetakes when pursuing the goal (such as suppressing a stereotype only tosee it return more powerfully later in time; e.g., Wegner, 1994).

A nonconscious goal to not stereotype can yield success free of thesepitfalls of conscious control. Our experiments reveal that stereotypes are

inhibitedwhen one encounters amember of a stereotyped group. This isa proactive form of control because it prevents a stereotype from everbeing retrieved frommemory, despiteperceivershaving categorized theperson to a social group. It occurs prior to awareness of either the goal orstereotype activation. It requires no effortful expenditure of mentalresources (cf., Fiske, 1989), nor for one to be aware of the bias, nor forone to consciously try to prevent bias from having influence (“correct-ing” for the stereotype).

Stereotyping others is goal-driven

We begin with two statements about stereotyping that are likelyintuitively obvious, and clear to experts and nonexperts alike. First, astereotype is knowledge that exists in themind of an individual that isassociated with a group of people, knowledge that is learned from,and shared with, others in the culture. Second, stereotyping is aprocess that unfolds in phases (e.g., Kunda & Spencer, 2003): In aninitial phase, the stereotype is pulled from memory due to itsassociation with a category (e.g., old, fat, Black, Jew, woman) that hasrecently been used to identify a person. In the next phase, this“perceptually ready” stereotype is used to help one understand othersand plan behavior toward those others. These two assertions lead toperhaps less obvious third and fourth statements. The third is that,regarding when stereotypes are pulled from memory, it is far morecommon and silent (we rarely recognize this has happened) thanpeople know (e.g., Devine, 1989). The fourth is that stereotypes aretriggered and used for a reason; they serve a goal. As such, they have

Page 2: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

2 Some of that research has focused on the question of shifting the type of categorythat is used to identify a person (by manipulating gaze, target ambiguity, fame, skintone, etc.). Activating a stereotype requires first identifying a person as a member of a

104 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

been likened to tools pulled from a “cognitive tool box” when peopleare encountered (e.g., Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Macrae & Bodenhausen,2000).

If stereotype activation is a tool that serves a goal, what goal does itserve? Given that stereotyping contributes to discrimination andprejudice (e.g., Allport, 1954), a natural initial answer to this questionthat received empirical attention was that stereotypes service goals,such as the desire to feel superior to others and have positive identityassociated with one's social group (e.g., Tajfel & Turner, 1979); thewish to attain and maintain social, physical, political, and economicpower over others (e.g., Jost, 2001; Sherif, 1958; Sidanius & Pratto,1999) and to protect oneself (and loved ones) from what seems alien,strange, and threatening (e.g., Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, &Sanford, 1950). But stereotyping is found even among people who donot fear others, wish to dominate, or need to boost self-esteem (e.g.,Devine, 1989).

Lippmann (1922) introduced the term stereotype in describing acognitive process used by people because they have a goal of beingefficient and economical with their time and mental energy when tryingtomake sense of (understand) theworld around them.Meaningmust beattained quickly and without much strain, given both the press of theexternal world (the speed of life, the multitude of things to process) andthe limits of the human processing system.1 From Allport's (1954)principle of least effort, up to and beyond Gilbert and Hixon's (1991)assertion that “people rely strongly on prior information to ease theburden of ongoing perception” (p. 514), stereotyping has been describedas in the service of a goal to attain meaning; to knowwhat thoughts andacts are appropriate. We shall refer to this as an epistemic goal.

Heider (1944) described this goal as a central pursuit, a “causal drive,”that guides much of daily life. Because of its chronic and habitual pursuit,the epistemic goal comes to operate without consciousness. Moskowitz(1993) and Neuberg and Newsom (1993) illustrated that one need notconsciously intend to a) categorize or understand another person, b) beefficient, or c) arrive at closure quickly. The epistemic goal is silentlypursued, routinely. Stereotyping is an efficient, nonconscious processprecisely because the goal it serves is not conscious to the individual.

Proactive versus reactive stereotype control

Given the efficient nature of both the stereotyping process and thegoals served by this process, how can stereotyping be controlled? Untilrecently, control in generalwas seenas a consciousprocess,with intendedresponding equated with effort (Wegner & Bargh, 1998). This is onereason stereotype control has traditionally been depicted as a processwhereby a) a stereotype is first triggered, b) the possibility for bias thenexists, c) this possibility is detected by the person, and d) at that point agoal to prevent bias, or overturn it if it has already occurred, is pursued. Infact, until recently, stereotype activation was seen as inescapable—anecessary by-product of the categorization process (Allport, 1954; Bargh,1999; Brewer, 1988; Devine, 1989). Such a view, by definition, precludesthe possibility of stereotypes not being activated.

However, many perceiver-based and target-based factors are nowknown to disrupt stereotype activation. Perceiver-based factors includecognitive load (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991), holding counterstereotypicexpectancies (Blair & Banaji, 1996), beliefs (Lepore & Brown, 1997),associative learning (Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, Hermsen, & Russin,2000), chronic motivations (e.g., Glaser & Knowles, 2008; Moskowitz,Gollwitzer, Wasel, & Schaal, 1999; Plant & Devine, 1998), and thedirection of gaze (Macrae, Hood, Milne, Rowe, & Mason, 2002). Target-based factors also prevent stereotype activation—typicality of a target'sface, fameof a target, skin tone, category ambiguity (is theperson clearly

1 As proof of this efficiency, researchers have pointed to the functionality ofstereotype use, such as the increased efficiency produced in one's thought when one isstereotyping (e.g., Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, & Jetten, 1994; Sherman, 2001).

awoman, Black, etc.?), andwhether a target's other roles/categories aresalient and used in categorization. Activation is not inevitable.

Yet, even as research has dispelled the notion of stereotype activationas inevitable, the ability to regulate this cognitive activity through themotivational system has been relatively ignored. Time and again,researchers stop short of claiming that the triggering of stereotypes canbewillfully stoppedand instead call for the importanceofnoticing implicitstereotypes so that one can then exert conscious attempts to curtail theirinfluence. Stereotype control is still largely seen as rooted in dual processmodels (e.g., Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Posner & Snyder,1975). These models describe one set of psychological processesinexorably giving rise to specific outputs, but a separate set of processesmaybe incompatiblewith thoseoutputs anddrawonconscious control toinhibit those outputs from influencing how one ultimately responds.Regarding stereotype control, a first process silently activates thestereotype. A second process, such as the recruitment of explicit goalsthat are incompatible with the use of stereotypes, prevents the already-activated stereotype from influencing responding.

This is a reactive strategy of stereotype control. Goals allow one tocorrect-for (e.g., Devine, Monteith, Zuwerink, & Elliot, 1991) ordecontaminate (e.g., Wilson & Brekke, 1994) cognition from the biasinginfluence of an already-activated stereotype, overriding it with a moreappropriate response (“putting the brakes on prejudice,” e.g., Monteith,Ashburn-Nardo, Voils, & Czopp, 2002). A rich tradition of research hasillustrated this approach to be a highly effective strategy (e.g., Devine,1989; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Monteith, 1993; Wegner & Erber, 1992),even impacting a U.S. Supreme Court ruling regarding employerculpability for unintended workplace discrimination. Granting itsimpact, we argue that this conception of stereotype control is toolimiting and can be extended by moving beyond dual process notions.

Control over stereotyping is not only attainable through subsequentintervention of the conscious will to overturn activated stereotypes.Control can be exerted on stereotype activation at the first step of theprocess. This is a proactive strategy of control, one focused at the level ofbasic social–cognitive functioning, arguing that goals disrupt theactivation of stereotypes. This focus on the role of the control systemdistinguishes our approach from existing research on the prevention ofstereotype activation described above (cognitive load, associativelearning, gaze).2

What is often lost when discussing stereotypes is the fact that theyare the product of a goal pursuit, and are, as such,willed, or wanted. Notrealizing one stereotypes does not, mean one does not, or that suchunnoticed stereotypes are unintentional. Indeed, explicitly wanting tonot stereotype does not preclude having an unconscious goal that relieson stereotyping. Epistemic goals, for example, are compatible withstereotype activation—the goal controls stereotyping, with control hereexisting in the form of heightening the response tendency. Given thislogic, it should also be true that control in the form of weakening theresponse tendency is also possible if one's goals are incompatible withstereotypes. Onehas thepower to efficientlynot stereotypebyexercisingthe same self-regulatory system that at times promotes stereotyping.The cognitive process serves a goal, and whether stereotype activationoccurs is thus an issue of what goal the individual is silently pursuing.We examine whether an egalitarian goal that is incompatible withstereotyping will inhibit, not activate, stereotypes. Any person can notstereotype,without even being aware of exerting control, dependent on

group, and such research reveals that shifting how a person is categorized affects how(and if) the person is stereotyped. Some of that research has focused onpostcategorizing processes where stereotype activation is replaced as one's dominantreaction to the category due to new learning experiences—by forging new associationsto the category or developing counterstereotypic expectancies. None of that researchaddresses how shifting a given individual's goals impacts the activation of stereotypes.

Page 3: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

105G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

what goals are triggered. Stereotyping is a default response only insofaras goals compatible with stereotyping are one's default goals.

Unconscious goals and stereotype control

The recent proliferation of research on unconscious goals (e.g.,Bargh, 1990; Custers & Aarts, 2005a; Förster, Liberman, & Friedman,2007; Kruglanski et al., 2002; Moskowitz, Li, & Kirk, 2004; Shah, 2005)has made a proactive approach to stereotype control tenable. It was awatershed moment in goals theory when Chartrand and Bargh (1996)found that goals are not always consciously selected. Rather, a goalstructure, just like any other cognitive structure, can be primed.Evidence of goal priming is now overwhelming (for reviews, see,Dijksterhuis, Aarts, & Chartrand, 2007;Moskowitz & Gesundheit, 2009).Once a goal is primed, to what extent are the steps taken thatmove onetoward goal completion also occurring outside of consciousness?

Compensatory respondingA goal specifies an as-yet-unattained end state. The steps or

responses taken that move the person toward that end are compensa-tory—they compensate for the failure of not reaching the goal (e.g.,Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982). Traditionally, the study of goals focusedon explicit steps taken to help one produce outcomes specified by explicitgoal states. However, compensatory responses are now known toinclude not only overt actions that are in the service of the goal but alsocognitive operations and automatic processes.

Examples of such compensatory cognition include the following: Agoal (such as to be egalitarian) directs selective attention to goal-relevant stimuli in the environment that one is not consciously able todetect (Moskowitz, 2002; Moskowitz, Li, Ignarri, & Stone, in press). Agoal (to choose between various products or to solve a problem) leadsone to implicitly ruminate onmaterial related to thegoal and strengthenmemory traces for information, evenwhen one has consciously stoppedthinking about the task (Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, & Van Baaren,2006; Koole, Smeets, van Knippenberg, & Dijksterhuis, 1999). A goal (toformattributions) leads one to implicitly follow rules of inference (Jones& Davis, 1965) and infer traits (Moskowitz, 1993). A goal (to suppressthoughts) leads one to implicitlymonitor cognitive processing to detectunwanted thoughts and prevent them from entering consciousness(Postman, Bruner, & McGinnies, 1948; Wegner, 1994). A goal (to dobusiness in a specific location) facilitates processing of goal-relevantinformation (features associated with the desired location, e.g., Aarts,Dijksterhuis, & Midden, 1999). And a goal (to memorize) impacts howinformation is clustered in memory (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1996).3

Goal shieldingKruglanski et al. (2002) argue that goals, as representations, exist

among a system, or network, of related representations. Movementtoward one goal impacts standing on another (e.g., Fishbach & Trope,2008; Shah & Kruglanski, 2002). Goals may be compatible and facilitateeach other. Goals may also compete and result in the inhibition of acompeting goal to “shield” a focal goal (e.g., Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai,Barndollar, & Trötschel, 2001; Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2003;Shah, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002). Goal shielding describes thegeneral set of processes by which a given goal is promoted throughcognitive operations that inhibit distractions to the goal and facilitatethe detection and processing of goal-relevant stimuli (as well as othergoals) that are compatible with the focal goal.

3 These selected examples highlight the implicit and explicit nature of both goalpriming and the goal operations performed when regulating a goal. They illustrate thatthe role of consciousness in one is independent of the role of consciousness in theother. Implicitly primed goals can have implicit and explicit effects. Implicit operationscan arise from both implicit and explicitly selected (primed) goals (Moskowitz &Ignarri, 2009).

For example, Shah (2003) illustrated that priming a goal that wasrelated to a focal goal improved task performance on the focal goal. Shah(2003) further illustrated that incompatible goals produce an inhibitoryeffect on the accessibility, commitment, and pursuit of one of the goals.Whenagoal incompatiblewithverbalfluencywasprimedprior toa taskassessing verbal fluency, the goal system shielded the primed goal byinhibiting the goal of verbal fluency. Aarts, Custers, and Holland (2007)also illustrated inter-goal inhibition. Participants were primed with thegoal of socializing while they had been pursuing an incompatible goal –studying. Goal shielding emerged in that people with studying goalsinhibited the goal to socialize. Goal shielding research shows thatgoals reside in a hierarchically-organized network with inhibitory andfacilitative associative links among goals that are, respectively,incompatible or compatible.

Stereotype inhibitionResearch on goal priming, goal shielding, and compensatory

cognition each inform stereotype control. Goals incompatible withstereotyping can be primed and held by the person outside of his/herawareness. Such goals, despite not being consciously detected, canguide how one responds, activating and inhibiting associated goals,shaping both behavior and cognition. When lower-order goals thatimplicate the activation of stereotypes are compatible with higher-order goals, then goal shielding should facilitate stereotype activation.When lower-order goals that implicate the activation of stereotypesare incompatible with higher-order goals that denounce or rejectstereotyping, then stereotypes should be inhibited.

Consider an example of such goal shielding relating to a Whiteperceiver encountering an African American man. The person istypically perceived in the context of a high-order goal to categorize interms of race, gender, or age. A category (Blackman) once activatedwilllead to heightened accessibility of category-relevant information, withthe retrieval of that information also under the direction of goals. High-order goals, such as to understand the person's behavior, trigger lower-order goals, such as to understand behavior using the least effortpossible, and to retrieve stereotypes to facilitate fast, effortless,prediction. These goals determine which knowledge, from amongmany possible types of category-relevant knowledge, is retrieved andactivated. It facilitates the retrieval and use of social stereotypesassociated with the group.

However, the processing of these same stereotypes can be inhibitedif goals incompatiblewith stereotype use, such as egalitarian goals, are inplace. It has already been shown that chronic egalitarian goals inhibitstereotypes (Moskowitz et al., 1999). Inhibition due to chronic goals isaccompaniedby other formsof goal shielding, such as strengthening theassociation between the goal and group members (Moskowitz,Salomon, & Taylor, 2000) and the scanning of the environment forgoal relevant items (Moskowitz, 2002;Moskowitz et al., in press). Thus,if a category (e.g., Blackman) activates an egalitarian goal, the nature ofthe spreading activation from the category is altered relative to whenthe category is not associated with such a goal. Egalitarian goalactivation is often incompatible with the goal of retrieving stereotypes,and should, under those conditions, activate lower-order goals such asthe goal to inhibit stereotypes.

Experiment 1

The current experiment examines if egalitarian goals that are notchronically held lead to control of stereotype activation by triggeringassociated goal shielding operations that include the inhibition ofstereotypes. Egalitarian goals are triggered in this experiment by askingparticipants to reflect on a failure experience. Many models of goalselection (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981; Lewin, 1936; Powers, 1973;Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) reveal that a goal is triggered when onecontemplates failure in the goal domain; by a person detecting adiscrepancybetween their actual responses and a desired response. This

Page 4: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

106 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

discrepancy is said to produce a psychological tension that impels theorganism to reduce the tension and approach the standard.4 Such adiscrepancy is sometimes introduced via negative feedback (e.g., Kooleet al., 1999; Spencer, Fein, Wolfe, Fong, & Dunn, 1998). In the currentresearch, as in other research (e.g., Monteith, 1993; Moskowitz, 2002),the discrepancy is introduced by having participants reflect on howtheir past behavior is discrepant with a desired goal (in this case, thegoal of being egalitarian to Black men).

In past research where goals are triggered by discrepancy detection,the discrepancy is typically salient to the individual, and he/she is thenplaced in a situation that clearly affords an opportunity to behave in amanner that addresses the failure (e.g., Gollwitzer, Wicklund, & Hilton,1982;Wicklund &Gollwitzer, 1981). For example,Monteith (1993) hadparticipants write how they should versus would act toward aminority(triggering a discrepancy). They then respond to a task explicitly aboutthatminority group. In such situations, the goal is the focus of consciousattention as one is responding. The compensatory responses are drivenby awareness of the connection between the discrepancy-arousingand -reducing tasks.

But, a response can be “compensatory” without the individual beingaware of its association to a goal (or of the goal itself). Such nonconsciousgoal pursuit is examinedhere. It is evidenced in twoways. First, byhavinggoal activation temporally distanced fromthe response (so thatwhen theresponse is made the goal is accessible, but the person is not aware ofthis). Second, by making the response one that is not explicitly linked tothegoal in themindof the individual. This is achievedbyusingan implicitmeasure of stereotype accessibility as the response in question.Participants responded to words that were either relevant or irrelevantto the stereotype of Black men following faces of either Black or Whitemen. A robust finding in the psychological literature is that responses arefacilitated to concepts that have been activated and slowed to conceptsthat have been inhibited. Using this response time logic, Experiment 1illustrates that stereotypes are activated when White participantsencounter a Black man unless an egalitarian goal has been triggered.Egalitarian goals instead initiate stereotype inhibition.

Method

Research participantsParticipants were 42 White students at Lehigh University who

participated to partially fulfill a requirement in their introductorypsychology course. All were native English speakers.

Materials

Goal status manipulation. A paper-and-pencil task that was describedas a goals survey was given to participants. The survey contained 26items (e.g., equality, freedom, intelligence, respect for tradition,humility, egalitarian). Participants were asked to rate the importanceof each goal on a Likert scale, with values ranging from 1 (not at allimportant) to 9 (extremely important). When finished, they turnedthe page and read the instructions for the next task.

These instructions informed participants each person in theexperiment was focusing on one goal from the survey. Half weretold they were to focus on egalitarian goals. Their task was to write a

4 In various literatures, this tension has been referred to as a self-discrepancy (e.g.,Higgins, 1989), a feeling of incompleteness (e.g., Wicklund, & Gollwitzer, 1982), afailure (e.g., Carver, & Scheier, 1981), and a lack of affirmation (e.g., Steele, 1988).Thus, inherent to having a goal is a state of negative feedback that informs one, viamonitoring processes, that a desired state is not reached. What is required to bemonitored is both one's current standing in relation to the desired standard as well asone's rate of progress in movement toward that standard (Carver, & Scheier, 1998).Detecting a discrepancy, though traditionally discussed as an explicit/consciousrecognition of one's standing regarding the pursuit of a goal, need not requireconsciousness (see Custers & Aarts, 2007; Wegner, 1994).

description of an instance in which they failed to live up to the idealspecified by an egalitarian goal, and to do so by relaying an eventrelating to African American men. The instructions stated theexperiment was not interested “in fleeting moments of negativethoughts towards others, but for you to reflect on instances in your lifewhen you truly performed in a way that led you to violate theegalitarian ideal.” An egalitarian goal was defined for them as:

Egalitarian goals are part of the “American Creed” and involve actingfair andopen-mindedly, being tolerant of others despite the fact theydiffer from you, and treating people equally regardless of theirethnicity, religion, gender, race, physical appearance.

The remaining half of the participants were each asked to similarlydescribe a failure, but in the domain of “respect for traditions.” Respect fortradition was defined as: “acting in amanner that recognizes the value ofestablished practices and institutions, honoring these traditions, and con-tinuing to practice and observe the rituals associatedwith the traditions.”

Since these tasks were of a personal nature, participants werereminded responses would be anonymous and that they should betruthful, even if admitting things they do not like.

Computer. The computer was a Dell Dimension 8200 with a Pentium 4processor and 256 MB RAM attached to a 17-inch Dell E770 ColorDisplay monitor.

Pictures used as primes. Photographs were drawn from a pool of 180black-and-white yearbook pictures of White and Black men. On thecritical trials half of the photos depict White, while the other halfBlack, male individuals. Filler trials contained White male faces.

Target words. Eight words relevant to the stereotype of AfricanAmericans and eight neutral words matched for length, frequency,and valence were used on critical trials. Attributes were selected asrelevant or irrelevant to this stereotype based on the research ofMoskowitz et al. (2000), Devine (1989), and Devine, and Elliot (1995).Stereotype-relevant attributes (crime, stupid, lazy, violent, prison, poor,hostile, and welfare) and control attributes (annoying, nervous,indifferent, cruel, selfish, death, and nosy) were all negative in valence.

Reaction time task. The task contained 24 practice trials and 96 trials(divided into two blocks of 48 trials). Each trial contained two phases—apriming phase, where pictures (primes) were presented along withirrelevant stimuli, and a response phase, where participants performed alexical decision task. The first phase consisted of a male face presentedsimultaneously with a pair of letter-strings. These pairs of letter-stringswere relevant to the cover story. Participants were told the task was todetermine if the strings in the pair were identical by pressing buttonsmarked “same” versus “different” on the keyboard. One-quarter of thetrials contained a pair of identical words, one-quarter contained identicalletter strings that were not words, one-quarter contained two separatewords, and one-quarter contained two separate letter strings. Faces wereostensibly presented as a distracter to the “focal” task. Participants wereinstructed to attempt to ignore them and to perform the stringidentification task as quickly as possible. Faces and string-pairs remainedon the screen until a response was made or until 1000 ms elapsed(whichever comes first). From among these 96 trials, there were 16designated as critical trials. On critical trials, the type of face wasmanipulated—eight contained Black and eight containedWhite faces. Theremaining 80 trials were filler trials with 90%White and 10% Black faces.Phase 1 served as a way to subtly present (by having them be ignored)primes. Responses to the letter-strings pairs were not of interest. Theresponse of interest was made during the next phase of the trial.

Phase 2 of a trial commenced 10 ms after a response was made inphase 1. It contained a single string of letters, and participants were

Page 5: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

107G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

instructed to respond to each string bymaking a lexical decision—is this aword in the English language? To respond, participants were asked topress either a buttonmarked “yes” or “no” on the keyboard. On half of thetrials the letter-string containedaword, the remaininghalf hadnonwords.Critical trials contained eight stereotype-relevant and eight stereotype-irrelevant words. On these critical trials, half of the words followed Blackfaces from phase 1 of the trial and half followedWhite faces.

Procedure and designUpon arrival at the laboratory, participants were taken to a private

room to work on a first task. They were informed that while they hadagreed to participate in a perception experiment, theywould first have anopportunity to perform an unrelated task to help a graduate studentpreparing a project examining how people remember positive versusnegative information about the self. When finished with this task, theywere to seal the forms in an envelope and get the experimenter, whowould take them to a separate room for the perception experiment. Theywere thengiven the goal statusmanipulation andanempty envelope, andthe experimenter exited.

Upon finishing the task, participants were next brought to a cubiclewith a desk and computer. Task instructions were presented on themonitor. The task was described as having multiple trials, with each trialconsisting of two phases. Phase 1 was said to contain three itemspresented simultaneously: A face,which they are to ignore, and two letterstrings presented one on top of the other. The task was to decide if theletter strings were identical. Phase 2 contained a string of letters, andthe taskwas todecide if the letters comprisedanEnglish-languageword—a lexical decision task (LDT). For each phase, they were instructed torespond as accurately and quickly as possible. When finished with all thetrials they were debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

The experiment had a Goal (egalitarian, tradition) by Prime Type(Black, White) by Word Type (stereotype-relevant, stereotype-irrele-vant) mixed-factorial design. Goal was a between-participant variable,Prime Type and Word Type were within-participant variables.

Results and discussion

If stereotypes are activated by the faces/primes, people should befacilitated in their ability to respond to words that are relevant to thestereotype. Thus, seeing a Black face on phase 1 of the trial shouldmakeone quicker to respond to such words on the lexical decision taskpresented at phase 2. Stereotype control is evidenced by this effectdisappearing so that stereotypicwords no longer are responded tomorequickly, and instead, due to spreading inhibition, are responded tomoreslowly following faces of Black men (Moskowitz et al., 1999).

Two independent coders rated each of the statements produced byparticipants on the “goal status manipulation” task. Each statement wasrated on a 7-point scale ranging from responses that did not attempt todescribe what was asked (a score of 1) to detailed answers with clearexamples of a failure (a score of 7). People with scores less than 3 (eightparticipants) were excluded from analyses. Interrater reliability was high(r=.70, pb .001). In addition, ratings on the Likert scales to the items“egalitarian,” “equality,” “fairness,” and “tolerance” were examined todetermine if participants valued being egalitarian. The goal shielding logicrequires the goal is not one participant's devalue or dislike. Participantswere considered to value the goal if they averaged a score of six or higheron these items. Across all experiments reported in this paper, noparticipants were excluded based on this assessment (all valuedegalitarianism).5 For thecomputerizedportionof theexperiment, reaction

5 This measure did not assess whether the goal was chronically held by theseparticipants. It merely assessed whether this goal, along with many others, was onethe person deemed valuable and desirable. Previous research (Moskowitz et al., 1999;Moskowitz et al., 2000) has found that only approximately 5% of students are chronicfor this particular goal. Thus, our participants are not selected because they hold aspecific chronic orientation, but are merely Americans who express that they valueconcepts that are specified as part of the American creed.

timesmore than 3 standard deviations from themean (5.2% of responses)were considered outliers and eliminated from the analyses. No partici-pants expressed suspicion the words to which they responded wererelated to the faces.

To most clearly illustrate processes of inhibition and activation,difference scores were created for each type of word. Reaction time to agiven word following a White face was subtracted from the reactiontime to the same word following a Black face. Inhibition would thus berepresented by positive difference scores (slower to words—largerresponse latencies—that followBlack faces),while activation/facilitationwould be represented by negative difference scores (faster to words—smaller response latencies—that follow Black faces). Difference scoresfor stereotype-relevant and stereotype-irrelevant words were submit-ted to a Word type×Goal (egalitarian, respect for traditions) mixed-factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA). As predicted, the only effect toemerge was a reliable interaction, F(1,32)=15.67, pb .001 (which alsoappears asa three-way interaction if difference scoresarenot computed,and reaction times are submitted to a Goal Status×Prime Type×WordType mixed-factorial ANOVA; see below).

To examine the specific prediction regarding activation and inhibition,comparisons of the difference scores were made between the goalconditions for each word type. No reliable effects emerged whenexamining difference scores computed for control words—the differenceas a function of prime type in response to these words is not significantlydifferent between people who had egalitarian goals triggered and thosewho had respect for tradition goals triggered. However, for stereotype-relevantwords, there is an influenceofprimetypeonresponding, and thatinfluence is in opposite directions for people in the egalitarian versustradition conditions. For people in the egalitarian condition, reactionstimes following Black faces are slower than reaction times followingWhite faces; the reverse pattern is seen in the tradition condition (seeFig. 1). Participants with egalitarian goals have a mean difference scorereliably larger (M=86.12) than themean difference score (M=−42.88)of participants with tradition goals, t(32)=3.56, pb .05. These effectsreflect inhibition and facilitation respectively. To further illustrate this, thedifference score for stereotype-relevantwordswere comparedagainst thedifference scores for control words. Egalitarian goal participants hadreliably slower difference scores to stereotype-relevant words comparedto the response times to controlwords, t(16)=4.07, pb .01. This indicatesa significant slow-down in responding to stereotype-relevant wordswhen they follow Black faces that does not occur for control words.Participants with respect for tradition goals show facilitation tostereotype-relevant words when they follow Black faces such that thedifference score is significantly smaller for stereotype-relevant wordscompared to control words, t(16)=-2.20, pb .05.

Fig. 1. Goal effects on response times (RTs): Stereotype-relevant words following Blackfaces minus stereotype-relevant words following White faces in Experiment 1.

Page 6: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

108 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

These analyses can also be approached by examining raw scores(opposed to difference scores). Consistent with the hypothesis, a PrimeType×Goal ANOVA revealed no reliable main effects (pN .4) orinteraction (pN .1) when examining reaction times to stereotype-irrelevant words. Importantly, when examining stereotype-relevantwords, the expected interaction between Goal and Prime Type wasreliable, F(1,32)=12.67, pb .01. Participants who had tradition goalsundermined had response times to stereotype-relevant words thatwere reliably faster when they followed African American faces(M=646 ms) than when they followed White faces (M=689 ms),t(16)=−2.08, pb .05. Thus, as predicted, they exhibited stereotypeactivation. However, participants whose egalitarian goals had beenundermined exhibited inhibition. Stereotype-relevant words followingAfrican American faces (M=746 ms) yielded response times reliablyslower than those following White faces (M=660ms), t(16)=2.88,pb .05.

Stereotype activation and inhibition were exhibited, with the typeof processing initiated dependent on the type of goal the individualhad accessible at the time of the reaction time task. One potentialargument against the inhibition exhibited here being an implicitresponse is that the LDT and the prime were separated in time by a fullsecond, which is enough time for conscious control to intervene.However, during targeted questioning in the debriefing, no partici-pants expressed any conscious intent to inhibit stereotypes on thetask, nor saw any of the tasks performed during the computerizedportion of the experiment as related to the egalitarian goals they hadundermined earlier in the session. The reaction time task was notconsciously seen as a way to address an egalitarian goal or as havinganything to do with stereotyping.

A second potential argument might assert that the stereotypeinhibition exhibited here is not a specific reaction to a goal. Rather thangoal shielding, in which a specific compensatory response is associatedwith the category “African American,” participants might be insteaddisplaying a generalized behavioral inhibition. African American primesmight have triggered the behavioral inhibition system (BIS; e.g.,Monteith et al., 2002). This could occur if the undesired behaviorwritten about in the first task led participants at that time to adopt astrategy of being cautious and to respond more slowly whenever anAfrican American is encountered. This would cause them to respondslower to items following Black faces on the computer task. Thisargument does not explain the current findings adequately becausereaction times were not generally slowed following faces of AfricanAmericans. Reaction times to irrelevant stimuli (in both phase 2 and 1)did not differ as a function of the primes. The inhibition displayed isspecific to stereotype-relevant words, suggesting a goal-specific,compensatory response.

Experiments 2A and 2B

Experiment 2 sought to replicate the goal shielding effect ofExperiment 1 while directly addressing the two concerns just discussed.Rather than separating the prime and LDT tasks by a full second anddescribing them as two, separate tasks in the trial, the two events weredescribed as part of one phase of a trial with a stimulus onset asynchrony(SOA) of only 100 ms.

To rule out that a generalized behavioral inhibition was beinginitiated as an explicit response to the failure, experiment 2 used amethod of assessing inhibition in which faster (rather than slower)responses evidenced inhibition. The task was to indicate whether twowords presented simultaneously were written in the same color. If thewords are related in semantic content to a stereotype, and if thatstereotype is accessible, the color-naming responsewill be slowed. If thestereotype has been inhibited, then there is reduced interference fromthe semantic content and the color-naming response will be facilitated.This task is modeled after the Stroop (1935) task, and slower responsetimes indicate increased activation rather than inhibition.Goal shielding

in the form of stereotype inhibition would be exhibited by the exactopposite response than expected if inhibition was due to the operationof the BIS——following African American faces responses should befaster, but only responses to specific target words (stereotypic words).

Experiment 2A

The purposes of this experiment are to introduce a new reactiontime paradigm in which slower responses indicate increasedaccessibility while faster responses indicate inhibition and to examinewhether this paradigm can be used to adequately measure stereotypeactivation.

Method

Research participants. Participants were 18 White students at LehighUniversity who participated to partially fulfill a requirement in theirintroductory psychology course. All were native English speakers.

MaterialsComputer. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.Pictures used as primes. Identical to those used in Experiment 1.Target words. The words were identical to those used in the LDT of

Experiment 1. Unlike the LDT, on a given trial, words in the currenttask were presented in pairs. Because there are eight words of eachtype, four sets of randomly paired words were produced for eachword type.

Reaction time task. The experiment included 18 practice trials and 96trials. Each trial started with a photograph displayed for 100 ms. Theimage (aman's face) appeared randomly in one of the four quadrants ofthe screen (upper left, upper right, lower left, and lower right). Theparticipants were informed by the instructions that the picture wouldserve as a gaze-fixation cue for the location of a word pair. On each trial,the image was followed immediately by a pair of words in the sameposition. Reactions to the word pair were the focal task. Participantswere asked to indicate if the two words were written in the same ordifferent color as quickly and accurately as possible. Words remainedon the screen until a response was made by pressing keys marked“different” or “same.” The position of the photograph and word pairwas selected randomly on each trial. After the response was recorded,feedback information was displayed on the screen, informing theparticipant of his/her accuracy and reaction time.

There were 16 critical trials. Eight had pairs of stereotypic words(four following Black, four following White, faces). Eight had pairs ofcontrol words (four following Black, four following White, faces). Eachwordpair appeared in two trials, once following aphoto of awhitemale,once followingaphoto of a blackmale. Critical trialswere alwayspairs ofdifferent words in different colored font. The remaining trials had pairsof “filler words.” On filler trials, half of the trials had word pairs printedin the same color, half had word pairs in different colors.

Procedure and design. The experimenter took each participant to asmall room containing a desk and a computer where they were towork on a perception task in private. Each participant was informedthe task had multiple trials, with each trial consisting of a pair ofwords written in color. The task was to determine if the colors of thewords were the same or different (a color comparison task). After allthe trials had been completed, each participant was debriefed,thanked and dismissed.

The experiment had a Prime Type (Black, White) by Word Type(stereotype-relevant, control) factorial design. Prime Type and WordType were within-participant variables.

Results and discussionIf stereotypes are activated by the faces, then people should have

attention directed to words that are consistent with the stereotype.

Page 7: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

Fig. 2. Goal effects on response times (RTs): Stereotype-relevant words followingWhite faces minus stereotype-relevant words following Black faces in Experiment 2B.

109G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

With attention displaced from the color-naming task toward readingthe words, responses to the color-naming task would be slowed. Thus,stereotype activation is evidenced by slower reaction times tostereotypic words following Black faces, but no differences betweenstereotypic and control words following White faces.

Tomost clearly illustrate activation, difference scores were created foreach type of word. Reaction time to a given word-pair following a Blackface was subtracted from the reaction time to the same word-pairfollowing a White face. Stereotype activation would be represented bynegative difference scores (slower to words—longer response latencies—that follow Black faces). Difference scores for stereotype-relevant wordswere reliably smaller (M=−81.59) thandifference scores for stereotype-irrelevant words (M=15.34), t(17)=3.51, pb .01.

These analyses can also be approached by examining raw scores.Reaction times were submitted to a Prime Type (Black face, Whiteface)×Word Type (stereotype-relevant, control) within-participantfactorial ANOVA. As predicted, a reliable interaction was found, F(1,17)=12.35, pb .01. To examine the hypothesis, separate t-testswere conducted for each combination of word type and prime type.Responses to stereotype-relevant words following Black faces wereslower (M=585 ms) than responses to the same words followingWhite faces (M=503 ms), t(17)=2.274, pb .05. No such differencesas a function of prime emerged on responses to pairs of control words(pN .23). Reaction times were slowed to the color-naming task onlywhen stereotypic word content followed a Black face, indicating thestereotype had been activated.

Experiment 2B

After having established the utility of the reaction time paradigmfor assessing stereotype activation, the manipulation of egalitariangoals was introduced in Experiment 2B.

Method

Research participants. Participants were 38 White students at LehighUniversity who participated to partially fulfill a requirement in theirintroductory psychology course. All were native English speakers.

MaterialsGoal status manipulation. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.Computer. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.Reaction time task. Identical to that used in Experiment 2A.Pictures used as primes. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.Target words. Identical to that used in Experiment 2A.

Procedure and design. Each participant was taken to a room to work onthe first task in private. They were informed that while they hadagreed to participate in a perception experiment, they would firsthave the opportunity to perform an unrelated task to help a graduatestudent preparing a project examining memory for positive versusnegative information about the self. When finished, they were to sealthe forms in an envelope and get the experimenter. They next movedto another private room for their so-called perception experiment,where they completed a task identical to that in Experiment 2A.Whenfinished with this task they were debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

The experiment had a Goal (egalitarian, tradition) by Prime Type(Black, White) by Word Type (stereotype-relevant, stereotype-irrele-vant) mixed-factorial design. Goal was a between-participant variable,Prime Type and Word Type were within-participant variables.

Results and discussionStereotype activation is evidenced by slower reaction times to

stereotypic word-pairs following Black faces, but no differencesbetween stereotypic and control word-pairs following White faces.Stereotype control is evidenced by this effect disappearing so that

stereotypic words no longer displace focused attention, even whenBlack faces have been presented as primes.

Two independent coders rated each of the statements produced byparticipants on the first task following the same rating scale used inExperiment 1. Based on these ratings six participants (three in each goalcondition) were eliminated from the analyses. Interrater reliability washigh (r=.72, pb .001). In addition, reaction times more than threestandard deviations from themean (4.8% of responses)were consideredoutliers and eliminated from analyses. No participants expressedsuspicions that word-pairs were related to the faces.

To most clearly illustrate activation and inhibition, difference scoreswere created for each type of word following the same procedure usedin Experiment 2A. Difference scores for stereotype-relevant andstereotype-irrelevant words were submitted to a Word type×Goal(egalitarian, respect for traditions) mixed-factorial analysis of variance(ANOVA). As predicted, the only reliable effect was the interaction, F(1,32)=12.67, pb .01.

To examine the specific prediction regarding activation andinhibition, comparisons of the difference scores were made betweenthe goal conditions for each word type. No reliable effects emergedwhen examining difference scores computed for control words.However, for stereotype-relevant words, there was an influence dueto one's goals. For people in the egalitarian condition, reactions timesfollowing Black faces are faster than when following White faces; thereverse pattern is seen in the tradition condition (see Fig. 2). Participantswith egalitarian goals have a mean difference score reliably larger(M=86.73) than the mean difference score of participants withtradition goals (M=−103.94), t(32)=3.496, pb .01. These effectsreflect inhibition and facilitation respectively. To further illustrate this,the difference score for stereotype-relevant words was comparedagainst difference scores for control words. Egalitarian goal participantshave reliably slower difference scores to stereotype-relevant wordsthan control words, t(16)=3.46, pb .01. Participants with respect fortradition goals show facilitation to stereotype-relevant words thatfollow Black faces such that the difference score is significantly smallerfor stereotype-relevant compared to control words, t(16)=−2.06,pb .05.

These analyses can also be approached by examining raw scores.Consistent with the hypothesis, a Prime Type by Goal ANOVA revealedno reliablemain effects (pN .52) or interaction (pN .37)when examiningreaction times to stereotype-irrelevant words. Importantly, whenexamining reaction times to stereotype-relevant words, the expectedinteraction between Goal and Prime Typewas reliable, F(1,32)=12.21,pb .01. Participants who had tradition goals undermined had responsetimes to stereotype-relevant words that were slower when followingAfricanAmerican faces (M=585ms) versusWhite faces (M=482 ms),

Page 8: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

110 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

t(16)=2.85, pb .05. As predicted, stereotypeswere activated. However,inhibition was exhibited when egalitarian goals had been undermined.Response times to stereotype-relevant words were faster followingAfricanAmerican faces (M=510ms) versusWhite faces (M=597 ms),t(16)=−2.14, pb .05.

Evidence for implicit control of specific stereotypes as opposed tobroad inhibition

Experiments 1 and 2 provide converging evidence, using twoseparate reaction time procedures, that African American faces do notlead to a generalized inhibitiondue to theBIS, they cause (amongpeoplewith egalitarian goals) specific compensatory responses in the form ofinhibiting specific semantic content–stereotypes and stereotypes alone.The evidence against the BIS triggering a generalized responseinhibition is the fact that such a processwould be indicated by a generalslow down in responding to all stimuli that followed the face of anAfrican American. Yet, the inhibition is specific to stereotype-relevantitems. Further, in Experiment 2, inhibition is evidenced by fasterresponding to the color-naming task, not a response slow down. Thus,the exact opposite response pattern than that predicted by a BIS effectwas found—following African American (and not White) facesresponses to stereotypic words (and not control words) were faster.This occurred only if egalitarian goals were induced.

The unconscious nature of this control is evidenced by the fact thatparticipants areunaware the taskhas todowith stereotypingor its controlandundertake no explicit strategy to exert control. In addition, the controloccurs quickly, at a SOA too fast for explicit control. Fazio, Jackson,Dunton,andWilliams (1995) described tasks inwhich responses to a stimulus areinitiatedwithin approximately a half-second of perceiving the stimulus asuseful for measuring implicit processing because conscious control takeslonger than this to be engaged.

The nature of inhibition: Concept incompatibility or goal shielding?

Experiments 1 and 2 illustrated the implicit inhibition of a stereotype.Is this due to an egalitarian goal? The goal conditions differed in thatwhileeach introduced a goal through a failure experience, only one introducedthe concept of egalitarianism. Perhaps the concept “egalitarian” isantithetical with the concept of “stereotyping” and what the data revealis the inhibition that typically accompanies interconstruct incompatibility.Glucksberg, Newsome, and Goldvarg (2001) illustrated that, when ametaphor is used to refer to apersonas a “pool shark,” the concept “shark”in its typicalmeaning as an aquatic creature is actively inhibited. The sametype of inhibition of incompatiblemeanings between concepts is found inidiom comprehension. A concept that is used in one context with onemeaning inhibits the incompatible meanings.

Interconcept inhibition is also seen in social psychology. Kunda andThagard (1996) parallel–constraint–satisfaction theory posits inhibitionof competing thoughts during concept activation. Kunda, Sinclair, andGriffin (1997, p. 721) offer an example: “Aggressive may activate bothpunch and argue. The context inwhich aggressive is activatedmay serve tonarrow its meaning. Thus, courtroommay activate arguewhile deactivat-ing punch.” Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg (1996), as well as Macrae,Bodenhausen, and Milne (1995), illustrated such inhibition of conceptsincompatiblewith a stereotype.Macrae et al. demonstrated that the sameperson can trigger the activation and deactivation of different traitsdependent on what stereotype is primed. A Chinese woman viewed in acontext thatpromotes categorizingher as awoman leads to theprimingofgender stereotypes, not stereotypes of Chinese people. The same womanviewed in a context that promotes categorizing her as Chinese producesthe opposite result—the stereotype of Chinese people is primed, andgender stereotypes are no longer accessible.

Perhaps in Experiments 1 and 2 participants are illustrating thisfunctioning of the lexical system: the primed semantic concept“egalitarian” is inhibiting incompatible semantic items (the stereotype)

rather than the stereotype being inhibited due to goal shielding. The nexttwo experiments illustrate that priming the concept “egalitarian” is notsufficient to initiate inhibition. Indeed, under some conditions, priming“egalitarian” promotes stereotype activation. The next two experimentsillustrate that the inhibitionversus triggeringof stereotypesdependsuponwhether the goal to be egalitarian is triggered (as opposed to mereconcept activation).

Experiment 3

The hypothesis that inhibition results from priming the semanticconcept “egalitarian” is pitted against a goal shielding hypothesis.Goal accessibility is differentiated from concept accessibility in anumber of ways (see Förster et al., 2007; Martin & Tesser, 2009). Onefeature that defines accessible goals is they possess a tension state.And once a tension state is addressed, goal operations are eliminatedor halted (e.g., Bargh et al., 2001; Koole et al., 1999; Liberman &Förster, 2000; Moskowitz, 2002; Spencer et al., 1998). Martin andTesser (2009) refer to this as the “persistence until” feature of a goal—pursuit persists until the goal is successfully moved toward, at whichpoint goal activity ceases. Successful movement toward the goalproduces a state of affirmation and with affirmation goal-relevantresponding shuts down (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981, 1998). This canbe used to distinguish goal accessibility and concept accessibility,which does not have such tension states. Thus, unlike conceptaccessibility which fades with time (Higgins, Bargh, & Lombardi,1985), goal accessibility persists over time if the goal is not addressed.And unlike concept accessibility which increases as one thinks about aconcept, goal accessibility can decrease as one thinks about a conceptif that thought resolves the tension state.

Liberman and Förster (2000) provided an illustration of this fact inan experiment in which a goal-completion procedure was shown toshut down the monitoring system thus eliminating the tension and itsconsequent effects. Participants with an implicitly triggered goal whowere allowed to satisfy the goal showed no evidence of goal-relevantresponding.When there was no opportunity to satisfy the implicit goal,participants persisted in their goal-relevant responding (see also Barghet al., 2001). Förster, Liberman, and Higgins (2005) similarly found thatthere was a stronger tension state when a goal had not been fulfilled, asillustrated by participants being faster at recognizing goal-relevantwords in a lexical decision task. However, after having completed thegoal, these same words were responded to more slowly.

Goal shielding in stereotype control can be evidenced by utilizing thispersistence until feature of goals. If the inhibition observed in ourexperiments is due to incompatibility between the concept “egalitarian”and semantic content associated with a stereotype, then increasedaccessibility of the concept “egalitarian” will trigger greater inhibition. Ifthe inhibition observed in our experiments is due to goal shielding, adifferent prediction can be made. The persistence until logic argues thatdecreased activation of the goal “egalitarian” can be produced by anincreased focuson theconcept “egalitarian” if that focus is on resolving thetension associated with the goal. Thinking about egalitarianism whilecontemplating one's successes at it would, ironically, heighten accessibil-ity of the concept “egalitarianism” while simultaneously lowering theaccessibility of the goal to be egalitarian. This is ironic because thinkingabout success at the goal would eliminate the stereotype inhibition thataccompanies the goal, thus restoring stereotype activation to the verypeople contemplating success at being egalitarian. In contrast, thinkingabout failure at being egalitarian primes the goal and triggers stereotypeinhibition.

The persistence until logic in Experiment 3 followed the procedure ofMoskowitz (2002) to manipulate egalitarian goals versus the concept“egalitarian.” Activation of the concept “egalitarian” in half of the partici-pants was achieved by having them contemplate a success at beingegalitarian. These participants were contrasted against participants whohad egalitarian goals activated by contemplating a failure at being

Page 9: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

111G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

egalitarian. We predict that thoughts of failure should lead to goalaccessibility, which persists until the goal is met, thus increasing stereo-type inhibition. Thoughts of success should trigger the same concept, yetfail to trigger agoal (or tension state), and thus fail to initiate inhibitionasacompensatory response. Activation and inhibition were assessed using areaction time procedure similar to Moskowitz et al. (1999).

Method

Research participantsThirty-eight White undergraduates participated to fulfill a re-

quirement in their introductory psychology course. All were nativeEnglish speakers.

Materials

Goal status manipulation. This was identical to Experiment 1 with oneexception. Rather than half the participants describing lack of respectfor traditions, they affirmed their sense-of-self as egalitarian. Theydescribed success at being egalitarian toward African Americans. Theinstructions asked not to focus on “fleeting moments of positivethoughts, but to reflect on instances in life when you truly performedin a way that led you to uphold the egalitarian ideal.”

Target attributes. The attributes were identical to those used in Experi-ment 1, withminor additions. Tenwords of each typewere included. Thismeant the additionof two stereotype-relevantwords (showy, dangerous)and two stereotype-irrelevant words (tiresome, cheap).

Pictures used as primes. Identical to those used in Experiment 1.

Apparatus. The experiment was presented on a Macintosh Powerbook180 attached to an “Applecolor High-Resolution RGB” monitor.Participants indicated responses for the recognition measure andthe lexical decision task on the Powerbook's keyboard. The “J” keywaslabeled “yes,” the “F” key was labeled “no” for all participants. Theprogram was written in Macprobe.

Reaction time task. The task was described as a memory experiment inwhich two images would be presented (one in a red, one in a blue,border). The image in the red borderwas to bememorized and comparedagainst another image that would shortly appear to determine if theywere amatch. The image in the blue borderwas to be ignored. In addition,a lexical decision task occurred in the short interval between imagepresentation and the memory test.

The task began with 20 practice trials followed by 140 trials withan identical format. Each trial began with a fixation cross lasting500 ms as a sign that the trial was about to start, followed by a blankscreen for 500 ms. Two pictures were then presented simultaneouslyfor 200 ms. One picture was in a red border, the other in a blue one,with location on the screen of each border-type (right or left of wherethe fixation cross appeared) randomly determined on each trial. Aftera 15-ms interval following the disappearance of the images from thescreen, a letter string appeared. Participants were to decide if it was aword by pressing keys marked “yes” and “no” on the keypad. A 500-ms interval after the lexical decision was followed by an image. Thetask was to indicate whether the image was the same as the red-bordered image from that same trial by pressing keys marked “yes”and “no.” After a 3-s pause, the next trial commenced.

There were 40 critical trials, 20 where African American and 20where White faces were presented in red borders. On these trials theletter strings were words (attributes) randomly paired with thepreceding, red-bordered images (given the constraint that 10 stereo-type-relevant and10 irrelevantwordshad tobepairedwith each type offace/prime). The program randomized the order of the trials and thepairing of specific faces with specific words within each trial.

Procedure and designEach participant was taken to a room to work on the first task in

private following the procedure of Experiment 1. Next, they moved toanother private room for their so-called perception experiment. Uponcompletion, they were debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

The experiment had an Egalitarian Goal (affirmed, undermined)by Prime Type (Black, White) by Word Type (stereotype-relevant,stereotype-irrelevant) mixed-factorial design. Goal was a between-participant variable, Prime and Word Type were within-participantvariables.

Results and discussion

The hypothesis was that African Americans faces would activatestereotypes of African Americans, unless one had the goal to beegalitarian, in which case the same faces would trigger inhibition.Participants affirmed in their status as egalitarian individuals were notpredicted to have the goal to be egalitarian triggered and should havefaster responses to stereotype-relevant words following AfricanAmerican versus White faces (activation). People whose status asegalitarians were undermined were predicted to have egalitariangoals triggered and should have slower responses to stereotype-relevant words following African American faces (inhibition).

Reaction times from the lexical decision task were submitted to aGoal (affirmed, undermined)×Prime (African American, Whiteface)×Attribute (stereotype-relevant, irrelevant) mixed-factorialANOVA. Reaction times more than 3 standard deviations from themean (5.5% of responses) were treated as outliers and eliminatedfrom analyses. No participants expressed suspicion that responses tothe words were related to the images that preceded them.

As predicted, a reliable three-way interaction was found, F(1,36)=5.94, pb .03. Separate Goal×Prime ANOVAs were conducted for eachword type. If an egalitarian goal was able to disrupt stereotype activation,wewould observe a Goal×Prime interaction in examining reaction timesto stereotype-relevant words. However, no such interaction wouldemerge when examining responses to stereotype-irrelevant words.Consistent with the hypothesis, no reliable main effects or interactionsemerged in examining reaction times to stereotype-irrelevant words.

Importantly, when examining reaction times to stereotype-relevant words, the interaction was reliable, F(1,36)=4.90, pb .04.As predicted, participants who had their egalitarian goals affirmedexhibited stereotype activation—facilitated responses to stereotype-relevant words following stereotype-relevant, but not stereotype-irrelevant, primes. When stereotype relevant words followed AfricanAmerican faces (M=664 ms), response times were reliably fasterthan when they followed White faces (M=744 ms), t(19)=3.53,pb .01. However, participants who had egalitarian goals underminedfailed to show this facilitation effect. For these participants, whenstereotype relevant words followed African American faces(M=738 ms) response times did not reliably differ from thosefollowing White faces (M=758 ms), t(19)=1.35, p=.19.

In addition, participants did not reliably differ in responses tostereotype-relevant words following White faces as a function ofwhether egalitarian goals were affirmed or undermined (F=.18,p=.68), but responded reliably faster to stereotype-relevant wordsfollowing African American faces when egalitarian goals had beenaffirmed versus undermined (F=7.8, pb .01).

In summary, people who contemplated successfully being egalitar-ian did not have egalitarian goals regulating their responding—a fairlytypical, stereotype activation effect emerged when a Black face wasseen. In contrast, peoplewhohad contemplated failure at egalitarianismhad a goal activated and displayed the evidence of goal shielding. Theseindividuals controlled stereotype activation, regulating goal pursuit inthe preconscious.

In this regard, Experiment 3 replicates a point previously demon-strated by Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2003) and Moskowitz (2002).

Page 10: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

112 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

Exposure to a goal concept does not necessitate there be downstreamconsequences on behavior and cognition. Simply thinking about thegoal of being egalitarian may trigger the semantic meaning associatedwith it, but goal-relevant responses are only initiated when there is anenergy to pursue the goal triggered aswell. Thoughts of success at beingegalitarian did not supply this energy required for compensatorycognition to occur. It also did not trigger inhibition of stereotypes as theparallel–constraint–satisfaction theory could predict (if these conceptswere semantically linked as oppositional constructs). Instead, stereo-type activation emergedwhen African American faceswere detected bythe perceptual system.6

The procedure used in this experiment, unlike the previous twoexperiments, did not find evidence for active inhibition. Stereotypeactivation was controlled when egalitarian goals were accessible, butreaction times were not slowed to the stereotype-relevant wordsfollowing African American faces relative to White faces. There areseveral possible reasons for this. The competing goal of memorizingfaces was active as participants responded to the LDT. The goal ofmemorizing faces could have interfered with the inhibitory processesassociated with the goal of being egalitarian. Or, it could be that thesegoals are not incompatible, but that the critical LDT, being anintermediary task performed while awaiting an upcoming task, hadvariance in responding introduced thatmasked the ability to detect anyinhibitory processes that occurred. Finally, it could be that negativepriming conditions must be created for inhibition to be detected in thereaction time task. In this procedure, the target faces were the stimulibeing attended to in the memory task. Moskowitz et al. (1999) had thetarget faces in the blue borders, not being attended to (but ignored).Perhaps inhibition would be found if such a procedure had been used.Since Experiment 1 relied on a procedure where participants were toignore faces, and that procedure proved reliable for showing inhibition,Experiment 4 returned to that procedure.

Experiment 4

Experiment 4 uses the persistence until logic once again, this timefirst introducing the goal to be egalitarian to all participants. In this way,the goal is in place, and it will subsequently either persist or be shutdownas a function of the task that follows. Failure to adequately addressthe goal will lead the goal to persist and stay accessible, resulting instereotype inhibition. In contrast, goal attainment would eliminate thetension and shut down goal shielding, thus causing stereotypeinhibition to cease (and stereotype activation to occur). This allowsfor a stronger test of the goal shielding hypothesis than the manipula-tionused inExperiment3because the competinghypothetical process—concept activation—makes a diametrically opposing prediction. Itpredicts that once the concept “egalitarian” is primed, this activationfades if one subsequently does not think about the concept. And with itfades inhibition of the opposing concepts (stereotypes). Conversely,accessibility of the concept ‘egalitarian’ should increase, as wouldinhibition, if one subsequently thinks about the concept. Goal shieldinglogic dictates the exact opposite effects of thinking, versus not thinking,about success at egalitarianism, as described above.

To control for the fact that thinking about success at being egalitarianresults in a self-affirmation as well as results in goal attainment,participants who do not attain the goal also contemplate a self-affirmation; however, it is in a domain irrelevant to egalitarianism. Thus,each group of participants is given an egalitarian goal, and each thenthinks about an affirmation. What is manipulated to induce goalcompletion versus incompletion is whether the affirmation is specific toegalitarianismor irrelevant to egalitarianism. Such a procedurewill also

6 This was not a “licensing effect” as described by Monin and Miller (2001) sinceparticipants did not know the reaction time task afforded them an opportunity tostereotype. They were also not afforded the time to make a conscious response.

allow for a test of whether affirmation processes in generalwill alleviatethe tension state associatedwith a goal orwhether the goal persists untilthe specific goal-related discrepancy has been addressed.

Method

Research participantsParticipants were 36 White students at Lehigh University who

participated to partially fulfill a requirement in their introductorypsychology course. All were native English speakers.

Materials

Goal status manipulation. The packet of materials began identically tothat used in Experiment 1—a set of values to be rated with Likert scaleswas followed by the task of describing a failure at pursuing one of theitems from the set. All the participants, in a fashion identical to thatfollowed by half the participants in Experiment 1, describe a failure atbeing egalitarian. Following this task, all participants experience thepositive affect associatedwith affirming the sense of self but are dividedinto twogroups. Half of the individuals nextwrite about success at beingegalitarian in a fashion identical to Experiment 3. This group has hadtheir egalitarian goal affirmed. The remaining half write about success atrespecting traditions (as described in Experiment 1). This group has anegalitarian goal that is still incomplete.

Computer. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.

Reaction time task. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.

Pictures used as primes. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.

Target words. Identical to that used in Experiment 1.

Affect measure. Participants indicated how they felt after completingthe tasks on the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark,& Tellegen, 1988). Feelings (e.g., upset, distressed, proud) were ratedon a 5-point scale (1=very slightly or not at all; 5=extremely).

Procedure and designEach participant was taken to a room and told they would work on

the first task in private following the procedure used in Experiment 1.Next, they moved to another private room for their so-calledperception experiment. Before the task began, they completed theaffect measure and then the computerized task. When finished, theywere debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

The experiment had a Goal (incomplete, affirmed) by Prime Type(Black, White) by Word Type (stereotype-relevant, stereotype-irrele-vant) mixed-factorial design. Goal was a between-participant variable,Prime Type and Word Type were within-participant variables.

Results and discussion

If stereotype inhibition is due to incompatibility between theconcept “egalitarian” and concepts associated with African Americanstereotypes, then continued activation of the concept “egalitarian”will maintain inhibition. Conversely, decreased accessibility of theconcept “egalitarian” (such as when thoughts are focused on conceptsunrelated to egalitarianism) will decrease inhibition and stereotypeactivation will return. Goal shielding predicts an opposite pattern.Thoughts about success at being egalitarian will eliminate goalaccessibility and goal shielding (and inhibition). However, if thoughtsare unrelated to egalitarianism, the egalitarian goal will not fade. And,as part of the goal shielding process, stereotype inhibition persists.

Two independent coders rated statementsmade by participants onthe first task. People failing to adequately describe a failure or success

Page 11: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

113G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

at the dimensions in question were excluded from analyses. Eachstatement was rated on a 7-point scale ranging from responses thatdid not attempt to describe what was asked (a score of 1) to peoplewho gave detailed answers with clear examples of the failure/success(a score of 7). Two participants were eliminated from the analyses forhaving scores less than 3. On the computer task, reaction times morethan 3 standard deviations from the mean (3.9% of responses) weretreated as outliers and eliminated from analyses. No participantsexpressed suspicion that the words were related to the faces.

To examine inhibition and activation, difference scores were createdfor each type of word. Reaction time to a given word following a Whiteface was subtracted from the reaction time to the same word following aBlack face. Inhibition would thus be represented by positive differencescores (slower to words—larger response latencies—that follow Blackfaces), while activation/facilitation would be represented by negativedifference scores (faster towords—smaller response latencies—that followBlack faces). Difference scores for stereotype-relevant and -irrelevantwords were submitted to a Word type×Goal (incomplete, affirmed)mixed-factorial ANOVA. As predicted, a reliable interaction emerged,F(1,31)=11.50, pb .01.

To examine the predictions regarding activation and inhibition,comparisons of difference scores were made between goal conditionsfor each word type. No reliable effects emerged when examiningdifference scores computed for controlwords. The difference as a functionof prime type in response to these words is not significantly differentbetween people who had egalitarian goals affirmed and those for whomthe goal was incomplete (still accessible). However, for stereotype-relevant words, there was an influence of prime type on responding andthat influence is in opposite directions for people whose goals wereaffirmed (and thus shut down) versus thosewhose egalitarian goalswereincomplete (and still accessible). In the incomplete condition, there isinhibition displayed—reactions times following Black faces are slowerthan reaction times following White faces. The reverse (stereotypeactivation)pattern is seenamongpeoplewhoseegalitariangoalshadbeenaffirmed (see Fig. 3). Participantswith an incomplete egalitarian goal havea mean difference score (M=55.02) reliably larger than the meandifference score (M=−39.56) of participants with affirmed egalitariangoals, F(1,31)=34.2, pb .01.

These analyses can also be approached by examining raw scores.Consistent with the hypothesis, a reliable three-way interactionemerged, F(1,31)=11.50, pb .01. Goal by Prime Type ANOVAs were

Fig. 3. Goal effects on response times (RTs): Stereotype-relevant words following Blackfaces minus stereotype-relevant words following White faces in Experiment 3.

conducted for each word type. As predicted, the interaction examiningreaction times to stereotype-irrelevant words is not reliable (pN .79).Importantly, for reaction times to stereotype-relevant words, theexpected interaction between Goal and Prime Type was reliable,F(1,31)=34.82, pb .01. Participants whose egalitarian goals wereaffirmed had response times to stereotype-relevant words that werereliably fasterwhen they followedAfricanAmerican (M=488ms) versusWhite faces (M=528 ms), t(15)=−3.96, pb .01. This facilitated reactiontime to words following Black faces does not occur for control words(p>.8). In addition, response times to stereotype-relevant wordsfollowing Black faces (488 ms) were reliably faster than reaction timesto control words (515 ms) following Black faces, t(16)=−4.58, pb .01.Thus, as predicted, these participants exhibited stereotype activation.

However, people whose egalitarian goals had not been affirmed(incomplete participants) exhibited inhibition. Stereotype-relevantwords followingAfrican American faces (M=562 ms) yielded responsetimes reliably slower than those followingWhite faces (M=507 ms), t(16)=4.44, pb .01. In addition, response times to stereotype-relevantwords followingBlack faces (562 ms)were reliably slower than reactiontimes to control words (526 ms) following Black faces, t(16)=−4.58,pb .01. Comparing across goal conditions, the only comparison wherereaction times differ is to stereotype-relevant words following Blackfaces. There are no differences for stereotype-relevant words followingWhite faces, or stereotype-irrelevant words following either face type(p'sN .34). Thus, as predicted, these participants inhibited stereotypes.

These results illustrate that affirmed participants have stereotypesactivated upon detecting a Black face, whereas incomplete participantshave stereotypes inhibited. This occurs despite the fact that affirmedparticipants have thought more about the concept “egalitarian” andhave stronger accessibility for the concept. The incomplete participantsinhibit despite not having consciously thought about egalitarianismsince the initial stage of the experiment. Thepattern offindings suggeststhe manipulations trigger a goal that is then either shut down oraccessible. As such, goal shielding is either shut down or remains active.This occurs without awareness that the reaction time task is relevant tostereotyping, without being conscious a goal is accessible during thetask,7 without conscious intent to inhibit stereotypes or pursueegalitarian goals in the task, and without time to consciously reflecton such issues (prior to a response) when faces are seen.

These findings also replicate an important point about the nature ofgoal affirmation recently reported by Moskowitz et al. (in press). Whattype of success “shuts down” compensatory responding? Self-comple-tion theory (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) suggests the affirmationmust occur in the specific domain in which a discrepancy was detected.However, self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988) says any restoration ofthe self-system, an increase in self-esteem, should resolve the tension,portrayed instead as a threat to the global self-system. Our findingsreveal that the tension associated with being non-egalitarian to Blackmen is not satisfied by global affirmation, but only by affirming the self-as-egalitarian. This is not to say global self-affirmation does not resolvetension states. However, wewould argue this would only occur if one ispursuing the goal of affirming the global sense of self. Researchillustrating the successful resolution of tension states resulting fromprocesses of global affirmation (e.g., Koole et al., 1999; Spencer et al.,1998; Steele, 1988) typically create tension by challenging self-esteemat a somewhat broad level (providing feedback suggesting one is notintelligent enough to lead a successful life, or that one is a bad person, ornotwell received by others). Devastations to the global self-system suchas these would be resolved (goal completion) by restorations of that

7 Although the goal was perhaps consciously selected and aware to the person whilewriting their essays, it has receded from consciousness while performing thecomputerized task. Thus, despite initial consciousness, the goal's heightenedaccessibility directs responding at another point in time unknowingly.

Page 12: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

114 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

global self-system. However, more specific goals should not be resolvedby global affirmations.

This finding is consistent with the research of Galinsky, Stone, andCooper (2000) who found that the tension associated with cognitivedissonance was only seemingly reduced by a global self-affirmation. Thattension state (and dissonance) was easily reintroduced if one's globalaffirmation strategy was subsequently challenged. Participants thenadopted a strategy of goal completion—addressing the specific threatdirectly. Perhaps the ease of regenerating the dissonance is a sign that thetension had never really dissipated at the implicit level and could havebeen detected by implicit measures such as the reaction time tasks weemploy. Our results reveal that below conscious awareness, such tensionsare not resolved, as Galinsky et al. (2000) suggest.

General discussion

Despite the fact stereotyping often occurs outside of awareness, sotoo do processes of control. Stereotypingmay not require one to think(consciously), but one also need not think (consciously) to control it.Indeed, the logic of the ubiquity of stereotype activation is turned onitself. Because stereotype activation is an unconscious response that isin the service of one type of goal (onewhere stereotyping is required),it is controlled by exercising the same self-regulatory system thatmakes stereotype activation efficient. These experiments illustratesuch a proactive form of stereotype control—preventing a stereotypefrom ever being activated upon categorizing a person as a member ofa stereotyped group. One can control thought, even low-levelprocesses of activation/inhibition (even if one is not consciouslyaware of the goal).

Shifting from types of people to all people

Since the so-called “New Look” (e.g., Bruner, 1957), it has beenargued that goals direct what is perceived, even when the items beingperceived are subliminally presented. However, the “New Look” andrelatedwork focused on chronic states, not goals that are adopted in themoment (even unknowingly primed in the moment). Prior researchextended that approach to stereotyping, showing that chronic goalsdetermine if stereotypes are activated or inhibited (Moskowitz et al.,1999). Our experiments move the question away from such individualdifferences to ask whether any person can adopt a goal to inhibitstereotypes without awareness or conscious intent to inhibit thestereotype at the time such inhibition occurs.

We apply goal shielding logic to control of stereotype activation.People are associated with many goals, some compatible and someincompatible to each other. Some goals promote stereotyping, someoppose it. An African Americanman provides one with the opportunityto be fair and egalitarian toward a member of a stereotyped group. Healso represents an opportunity to pursue an alternative goal of quicklyand efficiently categorizing him, a goal that may involve lower-ordergoals of recruiting stereotypes to ease the process. Each of these goals-in-conflict can be triggered, each triggering stereotype-relevantprocessing outside of awareness.

Our research finds that when egalitarian goals are accessible, thepresence of an African American man triggers that goal and itsassociated operations. This will include inhibition of incompatiblegoals and stereotypes (as well as heightened accessibility of theegalitarian goal, e.g., Moskowitz, 2002, and a readiness to detect goal-relevant people in the environment, e.g., Moskowitz et al., in press).An individual can control stereotyping without knowing a stereotypeor a goal exists. Consciousness is not required. One's wants, evenimplicit wants, can direct thoughts. Consistent with research outsidethe domain of stereotyping (e.g., Bargh & Huang, 2009; Dijksterhuis etal., 2006; Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005), it is possible that at timesthe implicit pursuit of one's goals is evenmore efficient and successfulthan the conscious pursuit.

Not all egalitarian goals are equal

The current experiments required participants to define egalitarian asantithetical to stereotyping by forcing them towrite essays in which theyreported having acted in a stereotypic fashion. However, not all peoplewill, outside of laboratorymanipulations that control the definition of thegoal, associate egalitarianism as necessarily being antithetical withstereotypes. Two general forms of egalitarian goals are represented inthe culture. One stresses egalitarianism being defined by explicitly payingno mind to culture, race, religion, ethnicity, etc. Color-blind is the termused to describe strivings marked by evaluating people based on meritwith avoidance of categories. A second stresses egalitarianism beingdefined by diversity. Multiculturalism is the term used to describeegalitarian strivings marked by cultural and ethnic diversity being thedesired end (Richeson&Nussbaum, 2004). These two forms of egalitariangoal pursuits are often at odds in terms of the cognitive and behavioralmeans they suggest be implemented en route to fairness (Vorauer,Gagnon, & Sasaki, 2009).Multiculturalism requires one take note of groupmembership while a color-blind approach requires ignoring it. Multicul-tural and color-blind goals thus impact stereotypingdifferently because ofthese differing ends that are specified.

Multicultural goals lead perceivers to attend to group membership,categorize others into groups, and drawon stored representations aboutthe group—stereotypes. This goal, however, also involves recognizingand valuing positive group differences. It requires open-mindednessand for one to consider a wide range of qualities—positive as well ascounter-stereotypes—that stand in contrast to the traditional negativestereotypes. Multicultural goals, rather than being antithetical withstereotyping, control stereotype activation by facilitating it. Wolsko,Park, Judd, and Wittenbrink (2000) found such effects. In a task whereparticipants estimate what percentage of Whites and Blacks possesscertain attributes, participants in the multicultural group stereotyped(both positive and negative) Blacks more thanWhites, while there wasno difference between the color-blind and control groups. They alsofound themulticultural group had amore accurate understanding of thedifferences that exist between Blacks and Whites.

Moskowitz and Ignarri (2009) suggest that because color-blind goalsrequire one to ignore categories, this approach narrows the mind-setwith which a perceiver approaches interpersonal perception, andshould thus be able to control stereotype activation in two distinctways. First, by preventing a category from ever being activated (andhence no activation of the stereotype linked to the category). Second, bythe category being activated, and that category triggering fairness goalsthat inhibit, rather than activate, stereotypes through the goal shieldingprocesses we have here illustrated. These two forms of control arereviewed next.

Control despite categorization versus control by blocking categorization

Stereotype activation involves the intermediary step of categoriz-ing the person as “a person” and as a member of a group to which thestereotype is associated. Categorization itself is a multistep process inwhich preattentive processes locate features, and those features thenget matched against existing categories. Livingston and Brewer(2002) argued that not even the categorization of people into relevant“person groups”must occur automatically but requires one has a goal.Further, people are complex and multifaceted targets of perception,and as such, the way one categorizes (which category is selected) isgoal-based. For example, giving a perceiver the goal of reacting to aperson in terms of race inhibits categories such as gender andoccupation (e.g., Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 1996; Macrae et al.,1995). In contrast, goals that promote categorizing to occupationalgroups may promote lawyer, professor, and fireman stereotypes butinhibit racial stereotypes also associated with, for example, Blackfiremen (Taylor, 1981). Goals can promote category use, but in sodoing inhibit competing categories.

Page 13: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

115G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

The main point of our experiments is not “goals specify whichcategories, from among many possible categories, are triggered.” Noris it that “stereotypes of Blacks can be blocked if one categorizes theperson instead according to gender or occupation.” It is that given aspecific category is activated, people may be perceived in a fashionthat does not implicate a stereotype associated with that category ifgoals inhibit such processing. Stereotype control is accomplished evenwhen categorization has occurred, despite categorization; goals directprocessing such that stereotypes are inhibited and other knowledgeassociated with the category is retrieved instead.

Conclusion

Stereotype control is not only something we consciously perform tooverturn, or react to, unwanted thoughts. It is somethingweproactivelyengage, outside of conscious awareness, to help produce desiredcognition in the first place, even inhibiting unwanted thoughts beforethey occur. This distinction between reactive and proactive control isperhaps more recognizable outside the domain of psychology. Forexample, British Petroleum recently took a reactive strategy to itsdrilling operations in the Gulf of Mexico by ignoring several preventa-tive measures that could have been undertaken in advance of anaccident. Instead, they waited for an oil spill and then applied attemptsat control, perhaps only after substantial damage had already accrued. Aproactive strategy would have anticipated problems with drilling andeliminated the likelihood of a spill ever happening. Proactive strategiesare difficult forpeople to contemplate because theyultimately result in anegative event; success at the goal is nothing happening (such as aglobal computer meltdown not happening due to Y2K preparations, adeep depression being avoided because of economic bailouts, terroristacts failing to materialize from proactively passing and enforcingantiterrorism laws, and oil spills at rigs that never happen).

Returning to the psychology of stereotyping, both approaches areviable, each with limitations. Reactive strategies require being aware ofstereotypes, yet stereotypes are hard to detect even when one is notmotivated to deny they exist. They also require knowing how toeliminate bias and having the desire and ability to implement thatstrategy, with each of these components often lacking (e.g., Wilson &Brekke, 1994). Proactive strategies require commitment to a goal, whichmay not always be present. They also are at the mercy of theenvironment and the multitude of competing goals that might gettriggered and rotate the egalitarian goal out of one's current concerns(e.g., Shah, Hall, & Leander, 2009). However, the possibility for control isevenmore pervasive and efficient than researchers have often assumed.And in the case of stereotyping, proactive control produces a negativeevent (no stereotype activation) that is easy to contemplate because itcan be seen. It does not result in nothing happening, but somethingmeasurable in the form of stereotype inhibition and other forms of goalshielding.

Just as with stereotype activation, stereotype control is functional.It has a purpose, or it serves a goal. Not stereotyping can be just asefficient and functional as stereotyping.

References

Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). The nonconscious cessation of goalpursuit: When goals and negative affect are coactivated. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 92, 165−178.

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the library: Environment, situationalnorm, and social behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84, 18−28.

Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A. P., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goalachievement of interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. EuropeanJournal of Social Psychology, 29, 971−979.

Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). Theauthoritarian personality. New York: Harper & Row.

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of thought and behavior. In

E. T. Higgins &R.M. Sorrentino (Eds.),Multiple affects frommultiple stages. Handbook of

motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, Vol. 2. (pp. 93−130). NewYork: The Guilford Press.

Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against controllability of automaticstereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in socialpsychology. New York: The Guilford Press.

Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). Theautomated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014−1027.

Bargh, J. A., &Huang, J. Y. (2009). The selfish goal. In G. B.Moskowitz, & H. Grant Halvorson(Eds.), The psychology of goals (pp. 127−150). New York: The Guilford Press.

Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotypepriming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1142−1163.

Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R.S.Wyer (Eds.), Advances in Social Cognition, Vol. 1. (pp. 1−36). Hillsdale, N.J.: L.Erlbaum.

Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123−152.Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory

approach to human behavior. New York: Springer.Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York:

Cambridge University Press.Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression formation

goals: Nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(464), 478.

Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005a). Beyond priming effects: The role of positive affect anddiscrepancies in implicit processes of motivation and goal pursuit. In W. Stroebe &M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, Vol. 16. (pp. 257−300)Hove: Psychology Press.

Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2007). Goal-discrepant situations prime goal-directed actions ifgoals are temporarily or chronically accessible. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin, 33, 623−633.

Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlledcomponents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5−18.

Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The PrincetonTrilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1139−1150.

Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J. (1991). Prejudice with andwithout compunction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 817−830.

Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., & Chartrand, T. L. (2007). Automatic behavior. In J. A. Bargh(Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mentalprocesses. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M., Nordgren, L., & Van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the rightchoice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005−1007.

Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1996). The knife that cuts both ways:Facilitated and inhibited access to traits as a result of stereotype activation. Journalof Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 271−288.

Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automaticactivation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013−1027.

Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not untotemptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 84, 296−309.

Fishbach, A., & Trope, Y. (2008). Implicit and explicit counteractive self control. In J. Y.Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science. New York: TheGuilford Press.

Fiske, S. T. (1989). Examining the role of intent: Toward understanding its role instereotyping and prejudice. In J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought(pp. 253−283). New York: Guilford.

Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, fromcategory-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motiva-tion on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psychology, Vol. 23. (pp. 1−74)New York: Academic Press.

Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Friedman, R. S. (2007). Seven principles of goal activation: Asystematic approach to distinguishing goal priming from priming of non-goalconstructs. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(3), 211−233.

Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilledgoals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 220−239.

Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective taking: Decreasing stereotypeexpression, stereotype accessibility and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 78, 708−724.

Galinsky, A. D., Stone, J., & Cooper, J. (2000). The reinstatement of dissonance andpsychological discomfort following failed affirmations. European Journal of SocialPsychology, 30, 123−147.

Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application ofstereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509−517.

Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicit motivation to control prejudice. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 164−172.

Glucksberg, S., Newsome, M. R., & Goldvarg, Y. (2001). Inhibition of the literal: Filteringmetaphor-irrelevant information during metaphor comprehension. Metaphor andSymbol, 16(3), 277−293.

Gollwitzer, P. M., Wicklund, R. A., & Hilton, J. L. (1982). Admission of failure andsymbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 43, 358−371.

Heider, F. (1944). Social perception and phenomenal causality. Psychological Review, 51,358−374.

Higgins, E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs causepeople to suffer? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology,Vol. 22. (pp. 93−136)New York: Academic Press.

Page 14: Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control

116 G.B. Moskowitz, P. Li / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 (2011) 103–116

Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. (1985). Nature of priming effects oncategorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,11, 59−69.

Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process inperson perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology,Vol. 2. (pp. 219−266)New York: Academic Press.

Jost, J. T. (2001). Outgroup favoritism and the theory of system justification: Anexperimental paradigm for investigating the effects of socio-economic success onstereotype content. In G. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: ThePrinceton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 89−102).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (tostereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotype associations onstereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871−888.

Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation ofrumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77,111−125.

Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun,W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D.(2002). A theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psychology, Vol. 34. (pp. 331−378). San Diego: Academic Press.

Kunda, Z., Sinclair, L., & Griffin, D. (1997). Equal ratings but separate meanings:Stereotypes and the construal of traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,72(4), 720−734.

Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come tomind andwhen do theycolor judgment? A goal-based theory of stereotype activation and application.Psychological Bulletin, 129, 522−544.

Kunda, Z., & Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions fromstereotypes, trait, andbehaviors:A parallel constraint satisfaction theory. Psychological Review, 103, 284−308.

Lepore, L., & Brown, R. (1997). Category and stereotype activation: Is prejudiceinevitable? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 275−287.

Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.Liberman, N., & Förster, J. (2000). Expression after suppression: A motivational

explanation of postsuppressional rebound. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 79, 190−203.

Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Macmillan.Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming? Cue-based

versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 82(1), 5−18.

Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking categoricallyabout others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93−120.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Milne, A. B. (1995). The dissection of selection inperson perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 69, 397−407.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but backin sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,808−817.

Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C., & Mason, M. F. (2002). Are youlooking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychological Science, 13, 460−464.

Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (2009). Five markers of motivated behavior. In G. B.Moskowitz & H. Grant (Eds.), The Psychology of Goals (pp. 257−276). New York:The Guilford Press.

Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 33−43.

Monteith, M. J. (1993). Self-regulation of prejudiced responses: Implications forprogress in prejudice reduction efforts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65, 469−485.

Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Czopp, A. M. (2002). Putting the brakeson prejudice: On the development and operation of cues for control. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 83, 1029−1050.

Moskowitz, G. B. (1993). Individual differences in social categorization: The effects ofpersonal need for structure on spontaneous trait inferences. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 65, 132−142.

Moskowitz, G. B. (2002). Preconscious effects of temporary goals on attention. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 38, 397−404.

Moskowitz, G. B., & Ignarri, C. (2009). Implicit Volition and Stereotype Control.European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 97−145.

Moskowitz, G.B., Li, P., Ignarri, C., & Stone, J. (in press). Compensatory cognitionassociated with egalitarian goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.

Moskowitz, G. B., & Gesundheit, Y. (2009). Goal priming. In G. B. Moskowitz & H. Grant(Eds.), The psychology of goals (pp. 203−233). New York: The Guilford Press.

Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. (1999). Preconscious control ofstereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 77, 167−184.

Moskowitz, G. B., Li, P., & Kirk, E. (2004). The implicit volitionmodel: On the preconsciousregulation of temporarily adopted goals. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in ExperimentalSocial Psychology (Volume 36 (pp. 317−413). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Moskowitz, G. B., Salomon, A. R., & Taylor, C. M. (2000). Implicit control of stereotypeactivation through the preconscious operation of egalitarian goals. Social Cognition,18, 151−177.

Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal Need for Structure: Individualdifferences in the desire for simple structure. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 65, 113−131.

Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond withoutprejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811−832.

Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso(Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55−85).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Postman, L., Bruner, J. S., & McGinnies, E. (1948). Personal factors as selective factors inperception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43, 142−154.

Powers, W. T. (1973). Feedback: Beyond behaviorism. Science, 179, 351−356.Richeson, J. A., & Nussbaum, R. J. (2004). The impact of multiculturalism versus color-

blindness on racial bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 417−423.Sassenberg, K., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Don't stereotype, think different!

Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 41, 506−514.

Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of significant othersimplicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84,661−681.

Shah, J. Y. (2005). The automatic pursuit andmanagement of goals. Current Directions inPsychological Science, 14, 10−13.

Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedentsand consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 83,1261−1280.

Shah, J. Y., Hall, D., & Leander, N. P. (2009). Moments of motivation: Margins ofopportunity in managing the efficacy, need, and transition of striving. In G.Moskowitz & H. Grant (Eds.), Goals (pp. 234−254). New York: The Guilford Press.

Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Priming against your will: How accessiblealternatives affect goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38,368−383.

Sherif, M. (1958). Superordinate goals in the reduction of intergroup conflict. AmericanJournal of Sociology, 63, 349−356.

Sherman, J. W. (2001). The dynamic relationship between stereotype efficiency andmental representation. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: ThePrinceton Symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 177−190).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchyand oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Spencer, S. J., Fein, S., Wolfe, C. T., Fong, C., & Dunn, M. A. (1998). Automatic activation ofstereotypes: The role of self-image threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,24, 1139−1152.

Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity ofthe self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 21.(pp. 261−302). New York: Academic Press.

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal ofExperimental Psychology, 18, 643−662.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In S.Worchel, & W. G. Austin (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations(pp. 33−47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Taylor, S. E. (1981). The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In J. H. Harvey (Ed.),Cognition, social behavior, and the environment (pp. 189−212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Vorauer, J. D., Gagnon, A., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Salient intergroup ideology andintergroup interaction. Psycholical Scence, 20(7), 838−845.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of briefmeasures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 54, 1063−1070.

Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101,34−52.

Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In D. Gilbert,S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., vol. 1,pp. 446–496). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903−912.

Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1981). Symbolic self-completion, attemptedinfluence, and self-deprecation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 89−114.

Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction:Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 116,117−142.

Wolsko, C., Park, B., Judd, C. M., &Wittenbrink, B. (2000). Framing interethnic ideology:Effects of multicultural and color-blind perspectives on judgments of groups andindividuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 635−654.