Top Banner
1 Designing Persuasive Destination Web Sites: A Mental Imagery Processing Perspective 1. Introduction Persuasive Web sites are those that can influence the attitudes of Web site users (Morosan & Fesenmaier, 2007). Web sites’ influence on product attitudes has been confirmed by empirical research (Macias, 2003; Lee, Gretzel & Law, 2010). Strong attitudes that are resistant to change are especially important in the context of tourism destinations, for which online information is abundant (Xiang, Wöber & Fesenmaier, 2008), competition for the attention of consumers is fierce (Gretzel, Fesenmaier, Formica & O’Leary, 2006), and expectation formation is difficult due to the complexity and experiential nature of the destination product (Nelson, 1970). Consumers planning a vacation are also likely to encounter traveler reviews and other forms of social media (Xiang & Gretzel, 2010) that can challenge the brand image portrayed by the official destination Web site. Further, research has found that users typically make very quick judgments about destination Web sites (Kim & Fesenmaier, 2008); therefore, there is a great practical need from the destination marketing point of view to identify those features of destination Web sites that can effectively persuade and encourage users to elaborate on the portrayed advertising messages so they can form resistant attitudes. Recent studies have especially addressed the importance of virtually transporting the consumers of travel information to the destination to support the formation of concrete expectations (Rozier-Rich & Santos, 2010; Lee et al., 2010). It is indeed a major concern of providers of tourism products to encourage quasi-trial experiences to support travel decision- making processes (Stamboulis & Skayannis, 2003). Since actual product trial is impossible, being able to vividly imagine what a destination is like is the next best alternative if the goal is to form concrete expectations (Goossens, 1995). Oh, Fiore and Jeong (2007) emphasize that
38

Designing Persuasive Destination Web Sites: A Mental ...326682/UQ326682OA.pdf · Designing Persuasive Destination Web Sites: A Mental Imagery Processing Perspective . 1. Introduction

Oct 18, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • 1

    Designing Persuasive Destination Web Sites: A Mental Imagery Processing Perspective

    1. Introduction

    Persuasive Web sites are those that can influence the attitudes of Web site users

    (Morosan & Fesenmaier, 2007). Web sites’ influence on product attitudes has been confirmed by

    empirical research (Macias, 2003; Lee, Gretzel & Law, 2010). Strong attitudes that are resistant

    to change are especially important in the context of tourism destinations, for which online

    information is abundant (Xiang, Wöber & Fesenmaier, 2008), competition for the attention of

    consumers is fierce (Gretzel, Fesenmaier, Formica & O’Leary, 2006), and expectation formation

    is difficult due to the complexity and experiential nature of the destination product (Nelson,

    1970). Consumers planning a vacation are also likely to encounter traveler reviews and other

    forms of social media (Xiang & Gretzel, 2010) that can challenge the brand image portrayed by

    the official destination Web site. Further, research has found that users typically make very quick

    judgments about destination Web sites (Kim & Fesenmaier, 2008); therefore, there is a great

    practical need from the destination marketing point of view to identify those features of

    destination Web sites that can effectively persuade and encourage users to elaborate on the

    portrayed advertising messages so they can form resistant attitudes.

    Recent studies have especially addressed the importance of virtually transporting the

    consumers of travel information to the destination to support the formation of concrete

    expectations (Rozier-Rich & Santos, 2010; Lee et al., 2010). It is indeed a major concern of

    providers of tourism products to encourage quasi-trial experiences to support travel decision-

    making processes (Stamboulis & Skayannis, 2003). Since actual product trial is impossible,

    being able to vividly imagine what a destination is like is the next best alternative if the goal is to

    form concrete expectations (Goossens, 1995). Oh, Fiore and Jeong (2007) emphasize that

  • 2

    destination choice is influenced by the mental images the tourist forms based on the expected

    experience at the destination. Further, Miller and Stoica (2003) have demonstrated that

    consumers' choices of vacations may be significantly influenced by mental imagery processing.

    Mental imagery processing refers to high elaboration processing and involves the

    representation of nonverbal information in working memory (MacInnis & Price, 1987). When

    travelers engage in mental imagery processing, they experience the destination in their mind’s

    eye. Such processing is believed to help in learning about a product (Elliott, 1973). Research has

    also found that it helps in the persuasion process. For instance, MacInnis and Price (1990) have

    pointed out that mental imagery has a positive influence on consumer attitudes and leads to

    greater confidence that events will unfold as expected. Past research has revealed that imagery

    can be stimulated by various external advertising elements such as pictures, concrete words,

    sound effects, and instructions to imagine (Babin & Burns, 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1992; Lutz &

    Lutz, 1977; MacInnis & Price, 1987; Miller & Marks, 1997). Recently, Lee et al. (2010)

    identified sensory descriptions on destination Web sites as textual elements that encourage

    mental imagery processing. Building on this existing research, the study presented in this paper

    tests the ability of textual, auditory, and pictorial features on destination Web sites to foster

    strong and resistant attitudes by eliciting mental imagery processing.

    2. Theoretical Background 2.1 Mental Imagery Processing and Persuasion

    Mental imagery processing can be conceptualized as “a mode of information processing

    which includes sensory representations (images) in working memory that are used in the same

    way as perceptions of external stimuli” (Goossens, 2000, p. 306). Similarly, it is identified by

  • 3

    Lutz and Lutz (1978) as “a mental event involving visualization of a concept or relationship”

    (p.611). Mental imagery theory assumes that we mentally re-present in our minds something

    that was presented to us before by experience or that we imagine based on information available.

    According to Burns, Biswas and Babin (1993), this mental imagery can be experienced in

    different sensory modalities and can vary in terms of vividness and quantity. Research on

    imagery has provided strong evidence that elaborate imagery processing may positively

    influence decision outcomes (Babin & Burns, 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1990).

    From the perspective of consumer research, a consumer’s mental image of a tourism

    product can be the main source of information available to enhance expectations and facilitate

    purchasing decisions (Walters, Sparks, & Herington, 2007). When imagery is encouraged

    through vivid product information, it strongly influences consumers’ attitudinal judgments

    (McGill & Anand, 1989). Most importantly, several researchers have found that attitudes

    formed based on mental imagery processing tend to be stronger, more stable over time, and more

    resistant to persuasion because of its high elaboration quality (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Petty,

    Haugtvedt, & Smith, 1995). Considering these findings, it is critically important for tourism

    marketers to understand what types of stimuli on a destination Web site efficiently induce mental

    imagery so that they can design persuasive sites that can successfully compete in the tourism

    information space.

    2.2 Narrative Information Processing

    While the impact of pictures on imagery processing has been studied extensively in

    consumer research (e.g. Paivio, 1971; Rossiter, 1978; Babin, Burns & Biswas, 1992; Burns,

    Biswas & Babin, 1993; Babin & Burns, 1997), little empirical research exists that deals with the

    effects of text formats on mental imagery processing. Brewer (1988) claims that text genre can

  • 4

    indeed be an important factor in encouraging mental imagery processing and Lee et al. (2010)

    also found textual qualities of destination Web sites to matter. Thus, there is a need to further

    examine the relations between textual genre and mental imagery.

    A text genre very common in tourism promotion is the narrative (Rozier-Rich & Santos,

    2010). Narrative in a broad sense refers to anything recounted or retold; in a narrower sense it

    describes something that is told or recounted in the form of a story (Denning, 2000). It is the

    latter that is of specific interest in the context of persuasion. Delgadillo and Escalas (2004)

    identified two required structural features that distinguish narratives from other text genres:

    chronology and causality. The first refers to events described in the narrative as being organized

    with respect to a temporal sequence. Second, text elements are structured in a way that builds

    relationships among characters and objects, thus allowing for causal inference. Similarly, Stein

    and Albro (1997, p. 12) suggest that stories are comprised of “goal-directed action-outcome

    sequences” called “episode schemata,” that is, they start from initial events that result in a

    response from a character’s physical condition, psychological state, or both. These responses

    develop goals that lead to courses of action and certain outcomes (Pennington & Hastie, 1986).

    The specific structure of narratives influences the way in which they are processed.

    According to Packer and Jordon (2001), narratives allow the human mind to “collapse

    boundaries of space and time, drawing attention to previously undetected connections, creating

    links between disparate ideas and elements (p. 174).” Some researchers claim that this is the

    case because the structure of narratives resembles the associative way the human mind stores

    knowledge (Schank & Abelson, 1995). They are also inherently entertaining (Brewer, 1988).

    Green and Brock (2002) stress that mental imagery may be facilitated by narratives because they

    allow for deep immersion and, thus, “transportation” into the story. Transportation into a

  • 5

    narrative world is conceptualized as a distinct mental process, which may mediate the impact of

    narratives on beliefs, and may derive its force from individuals’ general ability to create vivid

    mental images (Green & Brock, 2000; Escalas, 2004).

    Narrative processing has indeed been linked to persuasion. Padgett and Allen (1997)

    contend that narrative advertising would be the most effective way to communicate, especially in

    the context of an experiential product such as tourism. This argument has also been made by

    Mattila (2000). Escalas (2007) finds that self-referenced narratives enable consumers to generate

    a positive evaluation of an advertised product no matter what level of argument strength they

    include. When narrative ads are presented in the form of stories, potential customers are likely to

    envision functional consequences and derive symbolic meanings to interpret the advertisement

    (Padgett & Allen, 1997). Phillips and McQuarrie (2010) also point out that the greater the

    transportation into a story, the greater the belief that the world within the story is true, since there

    will be less critical examination of the ideas that were presented in the story world.

    In the context of tourism, Gretzel (2006) suggests that “travel stories help us understand

    and make meaning of our travel experiences and encourage us to relive and reflect on trips, as

    well as integrate travel experiences with the rest of our experiences and knowledge” (p. 175).

    Adaval and Wyer (1998) found that vacations at unfamiliar destinations were more positively

    evaluated by consumers who were exposed to advertising information in a narrative format

    rather than in a list of attributes. According to research by Tussyadiah and Fesenmaier (2008),

    the narrative structure of travel blogs allows readers to feel empathy and associate the

    experiences of the blogger with their own. A connection between the reading of travel narratives

    and narrative transportation was established by Rozier-Rich and Santos (2010).

  • 6

    While tourism-related print and TV advertising make extensive use of sensory

    descriptions and narrative texts, destination Web sites often contain functional lists of, for

    example, attractions or accommodation establishments (Gretzel & Fesenmaier, 2002). The

    question is whether including a greater amount of narrative text on destination Web sites will

    increase the persuasiveness of these Web sites.

    2.3 Processing Pictures and Sounds

    As mentioned above, pictures have been studied extensively in the context of mental

    imagery processing (Rossiter 1978; Shepard, 1967; Kisielius & Sternthal, 1984). In addition,

    Lutz and Lutz (1977, 1978), Alesandrini and Sheikh (1983), and Rossiter and Percy (1983)

    investigated the effects of various types of pictures specifically on recall and attitude. Babin et al.

    (1992) found a picture superiority effect which demonstrated that visual information would be

    remembered over verbal information. In the context of destination marketing, Olson,

    McAlexander, and Roberts (1986) found that pictures presented in destination advertisements

    have an influence on a consumer’s perception of the vacation experience through the association

    of variety of pictures with certain types of experiences. Furthermore, Miller and Stoica (2003)

    indicated that photographic images of beach scenes effectively stimulated mental imagery

    processing. More recently, Walters, Sparks and Herington (2007) asserted that the presence of

    more concrete pictures affects the extent of elaboration and the quality of consumers’

    consumption visions. Adaval and Wyer (1998) noted that the addition of pictures to a narrative

    format can encourage readers to imagine the sequence of events, and can facilitate the

    construction of a representation to be used as a basis for judgment. In a more recent study they

    reconfirmed that a picture presented in combination with narrative text can cause a verbal event

    description to be more vivid, help the recipients to enhance perceptual links between events, and

  • 7

    increase the story’s coherence (Adaval, Isbell, & Wyer, 2007). On the other hand, Goossens

    (1995) claimed that using pictorial images may smother individuals’ imagination as they become

    over-reliant on the featured image as an information source rather than elaborating on their own

    mentally created images.

    With regard to the effects of sound on mental imagery, several researchers found that the

    inclusion of sound effects increased mental imagery processing (Ferrington, 1994; Miller &

    Marks, 1992, 1997). Lee et al. (2010), however, did not identify any significant sound effects.

    Interestingly, Frick (1984) discovered that if processing was evoked visually with a set of items

    followed by a series of auditory items, recall was better than if both sets of items were elicited by

    auditory or visual modes alone. With regard to mixing sound and narrative text, Kerr (1999)

    claimed that sounds add critical information to narrative text. Even more persuasively,

    Verhallen, Bus, and De Jong (2006) showed that stories accompanied by multimedia features

    including video, sounds and music were able to stimulate people to construct meaning and gain a

    deeper understanding of the story line. Specifically, presenting the story with sounds can result

    in a listener’s greater ability to process the story and to construct a more coherent mental

    representation of the story events (Verhallen et al., 2006). These findings are consistent with

    dual coding theory.

    Dual coding theory developed by Paivio (1971) points out that people learn better when

    stimuli include related verbal and pictorial information compared to verbal information alone or

    pictorial information alone. He also indicated that information presented via the pictorial channel

    is more salient and better remembered than information presented through the verbal channel

    (Paivio, 1991). Richardson (1999) also pointed out that information is likely to be retrieved

    more accurately when it is encoded using dual codes rather than just one code. If one code is

  • 8

    forgotten, the other code can still facilitate the retrieval of all the information. Lukosius (2004)

    claimed that the more codes (e.g. picture, sound, text, touch, etc.) used, the better the recall.

    Multimedia learning theory developed by Mayer (1997) suggests that multimedia presentations

    can foster focused attention and immersion into the content and, consequently, can make it more

    meaningful. The ability of destination Web sites to include both visual, verbal and auditory

    stimuli should therefore contribute to their persuasiveness.

    Along with multimedia learning theory and dual coding theory, media richness theory,

    which was initially posited by Daft and Lengel (1986), also asserts that multiple cues embedded

    in messages support information processing tasks. Rich media are especially important in

    ambiguous communication contexts (Daft, Lengel & Trevino, 1987; Dennis & Kinney, 1998). In

    the context of online consumer behavior, a recent study noted that the perceived richness of

    online stores may have a significant impact on consumers’ intentions to buy online (Brunelle &

    Lapierre, 2008). However, a priori statements of one medium being richer than another are

    inappropriate as they neglect the specific communication context (Dennis & Valacich, 1999).

    Furthermore, D’Ambra (1995) asserted that the measures provided by Daft et al., (1987) were

    highly unreliable and unidimensional. Due to these reasons, this study adopted dual coding

    theory and multimedia learning theory as theoretical frameworks to guide the research.

    2.4. Communication Effects of Mental Imagery Processing

    Mental imagery plays a significant role in mediating ad-evoked feelings and attitudes

    (Bone & Allen, 1990, 1992; Burns et al., 1993; Mitchell, 1986; Mitchell & Olsen, 1981). Since

    mental imagery is self-generated cognitive processing, it can be expected to be more personally

    relevant (Escalas, 2007). Thus, it may result in self-generated persuasion and stronger attitudes

    (MacInnis & Jaworski, 1989). Importantly, mental imagery can lead to self-sell and therefore

  • 9

    greater belief that the formed attitude is correct (Tormala & Petty, 2004). The literature also

    infers stronger attitudes as a result of mental imagery processing from the fact that it involves

    high elaboration processing (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Petty et al., 1995). Petty and Krosnick

    (1995) noted that there are two dimensions of strong attitudes: the first is related to the

    persistence of an attitude (stability), which is the degree to which an attitude remains unchanged

    over an extended period. The other is resistance: the attitude’s ability to withstand an attack.

    Research by Petty and Cacioppo (1986) indicates that strong attitudes are more likely to come to

    mind faster, persist over time, resist counter persuasive attempts and guide behavior more than

    weak attitudes. Several researchers have also found a connection between attitude certainty and

    resistance to persuasion (Bassili, 1996; Krosnick & Abelson, 1992; Swan, Pelham, & Chidester,

    1988; Pomerantz, Chaiken, & Tordesillas; 1995; Tormala & Petty, 2002). Such resistance to

    persuasion is of great importance in an online travel planning context, where new and potentially

    negative information about a destination is only a click away.

    3. Conceptual Framework and Hypotheses

    While there is strong theoretical and, in part, also empirical support for the positive

    effects of narrative, pictorial and auditory stimuli on mental imagery processing and for mental

    imagery processing leading to stronger attitudes, no empirical study has so far tested these

    relationships in a comprehensive model. A comprehensive model is important to simultaneously

    test the effects and also to identify interaction effects. Further, most of the research on which the

    theories are based was conducted using print advertising. In contrast, the current study seeks to

    examine whether these assumptions hold true in the context of multimedia environments such as

    destination Web sites.

  • 10

    As illustrated in Figure 1, specific Web site features (text information in narrative format,

    presence of picture and presence of sound) are posited as predictor variables, while

    communication effects in the form of attitude strength, attitude confidence, and attitude

    resistance are assigned as dependent variables. Mental imagery serves as a mediator to

    accentuate or attenuate the effects of the predictor variables on the dependent constructs. Overall,

    the conceptual framework indicates that an imagery-eliciting Web site feature is assumed to

    evoke mental imagery, which in turn is expected to lead to stronger attitudes, i.e., attitudes that

    are more resistant to counter-persuasion attacks.

    As described in the literature review, narratives, pictures and sounds enrich information,

    foster immersion, and encourage a deeper and more extensive processing of content. Therefore,

    the following hypotheses are proposed:

    H1: Narrative text on a destination Web site has a positive influence on mental imagery

    processing.

    H2: Pictures on a destination Web site positively affect mental imagery processing.

    H3: The presence of sound on a destination Web site encourages mental imagery

    processing.

    According to the cognitive theory of multimedia learning (Mayer 1997), a story

    supplemented with multimedia such as pictures or sounds may foster a deeper understanding of

    information. Additionally, research by Verhallen et al. (2006) suggests that presenting pictures

    or sounds can increase the ability to process a story and construct a more coherent mental

  • 11

    representation of story events. Stories can uniquely incorporate pictures or sounds to enhance

    perceptual links between events and to increase the story’s coherence (Adaval et al., 2007). Thus,

    there is strong support for interaction effects to take place among the mental imagery inducing

    stimuli.

    H1a: The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing will be even greater

    when pictures are present.

    H1b: The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing will be even greater

    when sounds are present.

    H1c: The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing will be the greatest

    when pictures and sounds are both present.

    H2a: The positive impact of pictures on imagery processing will be greater when sound

    is present.

    The reviewed literature suggests that mental imagery processing is a form of cognitive

    processing that is elaborate and focused and very influential when it comes to attitude formation.

    Consequently, when customers surf destination Web sites, imagery processing allows them to

    form more concrete expectations and engage in greater information elaboration, which will likely

    instill confidence and make their attitudes stronger and more resistant to change.

    H4: Mental imagery processing leads to positive communication effects, namely greater

    attitude strength and confidence.

    H5: Attitude strength and confidence effects generated through mental imagery

    processing significantly influence attitude resistance to change.

    4. Research Methodology

  • 12

    An experiment was designed to examine the influence of Web site design features

    including narrative texts, presence of pictures, and sound effects on attitude strength, confidence

    and resistance, with the assumption that this influence is mediated by mental imagery processing

    induced by these Web site elements.

    4.1 Design

    The study used a 2 (narrative text vs. expository text) × 2 (picture vs. no picture) × 2

    (sound vs. no sound) full factorial between-subjects design to determine the main and interaction

    effects of the influence of narrative content, pictures, and sounds on mental imagery processing

    and ultimately attitude strength, confidence and resistance. The Web site designed for the

    purpose of the experiment showed a fictitious island destination. Only the homepage of the Web

    site was visible, with the links indicated on the page not being active to achieve controlled

    exposure to the text/picture/sound elements. There were a total of eight experimental conditions.

    The contents and lengths of the text included on the sites were the same, only the text structure

    varied. Narrative text and expository text differ in that narrative text portrays a sequence of

    events, includes protagonists, and connects parts of the text by means of suggesting causal

    relationships (Wolfe, 2005). In contrast, expository text is descriptive, does not refer to

    protagonists, and does not seek to connect the individual text elements (Figure 2). The narrative

    text condition is presented in Figure 3. The Web site shown to the subjects only included the

    logo and text in the no-picture condition. The picture condition (Figure 3) included a series of

    four revolving pictures of island, beach, and ocean scenes. In the sound condition, the Website

    included ocean sounds (crashing waves and seagulls). All conditions were pre-tested to ensure

    their appropriateness.

  • 13

    4.2 Measures 4.2.1 Mental Imagery. To measure the mental imagery construct, the present study adapted an

    imagery scale developed by Miller, Hadjimarcou, and Miciak (2000). This mental imagery scale

    consists of four dimensions: 1) vividness; 2) quantity; 3) valence; and, 4) modality. Quantity and

    modality were measured using seven-point rating scales ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7

    (Strongly agree), including three items for quantity and two items for modality (Table 1). Only

    taste and scent were tested as modalities as sound and visual stimuli were provided in some of

    the conditions and touch was not applicable in the context of the study. The vividness and

    valence scales were measured using seven-point semantic differential scales, including five items

    for vividness and five for valence (Table 1). The items were preceded by the introductory

    statement “The mental imagery I experienced was…”.

    4.2.2 Attitude Scales. As dependent variables, the current study explored attitude strength,

    confidence, and resistance of attitude to counter-arguments. The scales for attitude strength and

    confidence were adapted and modified from Bizer, Tormala, Rucker, and Petty (2006) using

    seven-point rating scales (Table 1). Attitude strength was measured with four items and

    confidence with two. In order to measure attitude resistance, subjects were exposed to a negative

    destination review that contradicted the initial destination description. Attitude strength was

    measured before as well as after the exposure to the reviews. Accordingly, an attitude resistance

  • 14

    measure was produced by subtracting the attitude strength score of each item in time two from

    attitude strength scores in time one.

    4.3 Participants

    This study intended to obtain a minimum of 25 responses per experimental cell; thus, at

    least 200 participants were needed. The goal of the study was to test a theoretical model rather

    than to describe occurrences of a phenomenon in a population; also, influences of visual, textual

    and auditory stimuli on mental imagery processing are based on very basic cognitive processes in

    which social characteristics should not play a role. Thus, a student sample was deemed to be

    appropriate. Problems arising from potential gender differences were avoided by using random

    assignment to the experimental conditions. A $3 gift card was given to each participant as an

    incentive to join the study. The recruitment effort resulted in 252 responses. However, seven

    students could not complete the survey due to technical problems, and six students completed the

    survey but indicated the same response for every question. Consequently, a total of 239 subjects

    were included in the analysis.

    4.4 Procedure

    When subjects arrived at the research lab, they were greeted by a research assistant and

    informed that an online travel agency was interested in developing a newly created destination

    Website. Next, they were seated at a computer and randomly assigned to an experimental

    condition based on the order of their appearance. The subjects were instructed to carefully look

  • 15

    at the Website and read the vacation destination description provided. After being exposed to the

    Website for two minutes, they were presented with an online, self-administered questionnaire

    asking them to evaluate the Website and rate their attitude toward the destination. The Web-

    based survey then presented a negative review of the destination. The participants were asked to

    indicate their attitudes toward the destination again after their exposure to the negative review.

    After completing the survey, all subjects were debriefed and compensated.

    4.5 Manipulation Checks

    To make sure that the manipulations were reliable and effective, this study conducted

    several manipulation checks during pre-tests by asking pre-test subjects about the believability of

    the Website and the destination review as well as the suitability of the texts, pictures and the

    sound. More specifically, 15 graduate students and 20 students from the same subject pool as the

    main study were involved in a series of pre-tests to evaluate the validity and score reliability of

    the scales used in this study as well as to test whether subjects could successfully distinguish the

    narrative text from the expository text; pre-testing determined that this was the case. During the

    main study, manipulation checks aimed to make sure that the subjects actually paid attention to

    the manipulations. First, all subjects in the picture conditions stated that they saw pictures.

    Second, all of those in the sound conditions indicated that they had heard sound.

    4.6 Analysis

    The hypothesized relationships between independent, mediator, and dependent measures

    were tested by means of structural equation modeling (SEM) using Amos 5.0 (Arbuckle, 2003).

    To address problems with large sample size requirements, unreliability, and non-normal or

    coarsely measured item-level data, parceling was used (Landis, Beal, & Tesluk, 2000). The idea

  • 16

    of creating and using item parcels was originally introduced by Cattell (1956), and further

    explored by Cattell and Burdsal (1975). Parceling is usually done by summing or averaging

    several items that presumably measure the same construct (Meade & Kroustalis, 2005). For the

    current study, a total of six parcels were created for each dimension underlying a latent construct.

    First, under the mental imagery construct, quantity, modality, vividness and valence parcels were

    created by averaging the item scores. In the context of the communication effect construct,

    attitude strength and attitude confidence parcels were produced. However, attitude resistance

    was measured using its four items separately since the construct consisted of only one dimension.

    5. Results

    5.1 Measurement Model

    The originally proposed measurement model was estimated using maximum-likelihood

    estimation. As suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999) and Quintana and Maxwell (1999), several

    indices were used to assess goodness of fit for the model. The initial test of the measurement

    model resulted in poor model fit (Chi-Square/df = 4.1; CFI = .95; NFI = .93, RMSEA =.10). In

    particular, the mental imagery construct exhibited very poor construct reliability, as indicated by

    the fact that the four parcel indicators loaded inappropriately onto the latent construct.

    Additional analyses suggested that vividness and valence variables were causing the problems.

    Therefore, an alternative model was estimated that only included quantity and modality as

    dimensions of mental imagery processing. As discussed by other researchers (Gefen, Straub &

    Boudreau, 2000; Chin & Todd, 1995; MacClallum, Roznowski & Necowitz, 1992), the dropping

    of measurement items to improve model fit should be executed with caution as it can generate an

    overfitting of the model to the data. As recommended in the literature (MacCallum et al. 1992),

  • 17

    only a few model modifications were made to fix the relatively severe model-fit problems. Also,

    appropriateness from a theoretical point of view was a major decision criterion. The overall

    model fit improved: Chi-Square/df=3.0, which is appropriate (Kline, 1998); CFI = .97; NFI

    = .95; and RMSEA =.09; consequently, the alternative model was adopted. The discriminant

    validity of the constructs was tested by calculating the squared roots of average variance

    extracted (AVE). As shown in Table 2, all AVE scores were higher than the threshold of .5 as

    recommended by Fornell & Larcker (1981) for all three constructs (mental imagery= .65;

    communication effect= .84; attitude resistance= .84). Second, the convergent validity of the

    constructs was examined. All items had factor loadings higher than .5, which suggests good

    convergent validity (Nunnally, 1967). Composite reliability was computed to assess the internal

    consistency of the constructs. As shown in Table 2, the composite reliability values of all of the

    constructs exceeded the minimum .60 suggested by Nunnally and Bernstein (1994).

    5.2 Structural Model

    The overall model fit and the regression paths were estimated to determine if the

    proposed model described the data well. As shown in Table 3, the model fit is acceptable. The

    normalized chi-square statistic indicated an adequate fit (Kline, 1998). Further, the

    recommended threshold is 0.9. for NFI, CFI and GFI (Brown, 2006), and RMSEA values less

    than .08 are seen as indicating satisfactory fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

  • 18

    The squared multiple correlation coefficients (SMC; R2) for indicator variables were

    assessed. SMCs lie between 0 and 1 (the closer to 1, the more of the latent construct can be

    explained by the model). The results reveal that the model does not explain mental imagery

    (R2=.07) and attitude resistance (R2=.10) very well. However, it explains a large portion of the

    communication effects construct (R2= .72).

    Figure 4 shows the hypothesized structural model paths. Most of the model paths yielded

    significant parameter estimates. However, in the context of the impact of multimedia stimuli

    (e.g., narrative text, pictures, and sound) on mental imagery processing, only pictures showed a

    significant effect (β=.23, p < .01). These results provide support for Hypothesis 2, but not for

    Hypotheses 1 and 3. Additionally, the interaction effects were not significant. Thus, the results

    of the structural model did not support H1a, H1b, H1c, and H2a.

    Most importantly, Figure 4 reveals that the mental imagery construct strongly influenced

    the communication effects construct (β=.85, p < .01), which is represented by attitude strength

    and attitude confidence. In other words, evoked mental imagery leads to positive

    communication effects, which supports Hypothesis 4. In addition, the results presented in Figure

    4 show that attitude strength and confidence have a significant impact on attitude resistance

    (β=.32, p < .01). This implies that the stronger the attitude the more resistant it is to negative

    stimuli. This finding supports Hypothesis 5. In addition, this study confirmed full mediation of

    mental imagery processing as the modification indices for the model did not suggest the need for

    modeling additional relationships among the constructs.

  • 19

    6. Implications

    The purpose of this study was to generate insights into the nature of imagery-evoking

    Website features including narrative text, pictures, and sounds, and to explore the relationship

    between these Web site features and communication effects that are encouraged through mental

    imagery processing. The overarching goal was to see whether certain Website features can

    induce changes in attitudes that are resistant to subsequent persuasion attempts.

    6.1 Theoretical Implications

    Prior research has found that mental imagery processing can be evoked by various

    external advertising stimuli (Babin & Burns, 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1992; MacInnis & Price,

    1987; Miller & Marks, 1997; Lutz & Lutz, 1977). Most of this research has focused on print

    advertising and has usually only included one stimulus, which does not allow for interaction

    effects to be tested. Mental imagery research in the context of Websites has mainly dealt with

    the effects of object interactivity (Schlosser, 2003; Macias, 2003) and sensory information (Lee

    et al., 2010). Thus, the current study has contributed to mental imagery research by

    simultaneously testing three different stimuli in the context of a Website. Further, in the tourism

    field very little research has been conducted on the effects of features (Lee & Gretzel, 2008) and

    particularly pictures (Jeong & Choi, 2004) included in destination Websites. Thus, the findings

    certainly contribute to this body of literature as well.

    The most important finding of the study is that mental imagery processing indeed not

    only leads to very strong attitudes but also greater confidence in attitudes. This issue had not

  • 20

    been addressed in the context of tourism nor in the context of online information processing . In

    the present study, mental imagery processing was confirmed as an important element of

    persuasive communication in the context of travel planning, even if the persuader is not a human

    communicator or a traditional advertising medium but a Website. Consequently, more emphasis

    needs to be placed on studying mental imagery processing in the context of tourism marketing

    and travel decision-making as well as online communication environments.

    With respect to attitude resistance, this study also successfully supports past research

    indicating that strong attitudes significantly influence attitude resistance (Haugtvedt & Petty,

    1992; Petty et al., 1995). Based on these findings, it can be stated that once subjects form strong

    attitudes and confidence in the destination through mental imagery processing, they are more

    likely to resist negative reviews of the destination. Given that very little research has been

    conducted regarding attitude resistance in the realm of tourism marketing while the likelihood of

    tourism consumers being exposed to reviews is increasing, the findings of this study are an

    important contribution to tourism marketing research.

    6.2 Practical Implications

    With the growing amount of information available on the Internet and the increasing

    number of destination options available to travel consumers, it will be ever more important to

    create persuasive Website designs that can help consumers learn about destinations and form

    strong attitudes about them. Moreover, in the context of successful tourism marketing, with the

    rocketing amounts of consumer-generated contents that include personal stories, pictures, and

    sound available on tourism Websites, a persuasive Website effectively instills confidence in

    consumers and helps them form attitudes that are more resistant to counter-arguments. Also,

  • 21

    given the many benefits for marketing that can be assumed to result from imagery processing, it

    appears to be critical for online tourism marketing to be better informed about the effectiveness

    of imagery processing and the stimuli that can encourage it. When consumers evaluate

    experience products, they may have a higher degree of uncertainty compared to other physical

    products. Running a simulation-like scenario in one's mind can facilitate the process of

    destination decision making and lead to more confidence in the decision. While customers

    consume information on travel Websites, imagery processing enables them to engage in greater

    information elaboration and to form more certain expectations (Goossens, 2000). Tourism

    marketers should make an effort to help travel planners to imagine what it would be like to

    experience the destination, which can lead to positive attitudes and build confidence.

    Furthermore, the generated high levels of certainty foster resistance to subsequent negative

    persuasion, which is of high practical relevance given the likely exposure to social media content

    in travel information searches (Xiang & Gretzel, 2010).

    These findings can help tourism marketers understand how critical it is to invest in

    careful Website design and also to evaluate the effectiveness of particular designs. The current

    study especially emphasizes the importance of pictures. While many tourism marketers

    intuitively know that they are important, this study shows that they can make a significant

    difference; thus, decisions to place pictures on Websites need to be made with an understanding

    that changes will affect the Website’s persuasiveness. In general, Websites are often designed by

    practitioners based on past experience, existing examples, or technological considerations. The

    research presented in this paper argues very strongly for a more scientific approach to Website

    design.

  • 22

    6.3 Limitations and Future Research

    The current study should be considered in light of its limitations. First, while the

    manipulations were extensively pre-tested there always remains the question of whether they

    were strong enough. Second, using just a beach destination for this experiment may constrain

    the generalizability of the results found in this study. The use of Websites for a broader array of

    tourism products such as hotels, events and festivals, and other destinations may generate

    different results in terms of mental imagery processing. Third, the use of student subjects was

    limiting in that students can be different from older and less educated individuals in terms of

    their experience or proficiency in interpreting information (James & Sonner, 2001). However,

    given that the research aim of the current study was to focus on theoretical explanation rather

    than the generalization of the study to a population, using student subjects was appropriate

    (Sternthal, Tybout & Calder, 1994; Peterson, 2001; Walters, Sparks & Herington, 2007).

    Moreover, as is typical for experiments, the context was artificially created and did not represent

    a real travel planning scenario, which can influence motivations to process the information.

    Further, the problems in measuring the vividness and valence of mental imagery processing

    suggest that a stable measure has yet to be developed to assess these dimensions. The lack of

    visible effects could have been due to only measuring the quantity and modality of the mental

    imagery.

    Future research in this area should be extended to include a greater variety of

    potentially imagery-evoking Website features. For example, it may be interesting to investigate

    how interactive or customized features such as interactive maps or chatting with travel experts on

    the Website influence mental imagery processing. On the other hand, presenting just pictures to

    subjects without any other conditions, or pictures with narration in the form of audio instead of

  • 23

    narrative text, or videos that combine moving pictures with narrative and sound may result in a

    different level of mental imagery processing. Also, varying the length of the narratives and the

    type and quality of sound might produce different results. Additionally, while instructions to

    imagine have been widely tested for evoking mental imagery processing, they have not been

    tested in online environments, and thus can be a subject for future research. Further, the

    experiment was conducted using traditional computer screens. However, Fogg, Booker and Don

    (2004) assert that recently developed mobile phones are more compelling platforms for

    narratives. They claim that mobile phones are highly personal devices, thus people are more

    likely to experience content from those phones in a way that is more personal and influential than

    content from a computer. If people are indeed more receptive to persuasive messages through

    mobile phones, it will be valuable to study mental imagery processing while using these other

    types of platforms, which are increasingly used in the context of travel and tourism.

    Several issues need to be discussed with regard to the text manipulation. First, all

    subjects were exposed to a text in either expository or narrative form. Thus, even though the

    findings of this study revealed that the narrative versus expository text did not influence mental

    imagery processing, it is highly possible that the information contained in the text by itself might

    have evoked mental imagery processing. The proposed structural model showed that the picture

    condition significantly affected mental imagery, but indicated a small standardized coefficient

    β=0.23, which means that the pictures had a small influence on mental imagery. Nevertheless,

    many of the subjects reported mental imagery. It needs to be asked what generated this mental

    imagery. Indeed, both text conditions contained a large number of sensory words. Previous

    research has found that the proper mixture of sensory information presented on Websites can

    strongly evoke mental imagery, which can greatly influence consumer attitudes and behavioral

  • 24

    intentions (Lee et al., 2010; Miller et al., 2000; Schlosser, 2003). Another aspect that might have

    to be considered is the length of the text as it was rather short to avoid burdening the study

    subjects. In real trip planning contexts, individuals are highly motivated to process information

    and will probably consume more text, which might make effects more visible.

    Of the stimuli included in the study, only pictures significantly affected mental imagery

    processing. The result is consistent with existing research (Babin et al., 1992; Kisielius &

    Sternthal, 1984; Paivio, 1971; Rossiter, 1978; Shepard, 1967; Hirschman & Solomon, 1984;

    Starch, 1966) that finds pictures to more efficiently evoke mental imagery processing than other

    stimuli. However, the insignificant results for the other modalities are inconsistent with research

    that claims that narrative texts and sounds support mental imagery processing. More research is

    needed to examine whether the specific context (i.e. destinations) is the reason for this or if other

    types of manipulations are needed to evoke effects.

    In addition, the current study revealed that sound does not influence mental imagery.

    Some researchers claim that when imagery processing and auditory perception compete for the

    same resources, elaboration of information can be reduced (Unnava, Agarwal, & Haugtvedt,

    1996). In other words, they assert that imagery generated by subjects selectively interfered with

    the processing of externally provided information (Unnava et al., 1996). For example, while

    conducting the experiment for this study, subjects were informed that they would see the

    Website of an island destination. At that time, subjects might have generated auditory imagery

    by themselves before they actually heard waves and seagull sounds. In this case, based on

    Unnava et al. (1996), the interference between self-generated auditory imagery and actual sounds

    can adversely affect cognitive elaboration. Not enough research currently exists to address this

    issue in a definitive manner.

  • 25

    REFERENCES

    Adaval, R., Isbell, L. M., & Wyer, R. S. (2007). The impact of pictures on narrative- and list-based impression formation: A process interference model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 352-364.

    Adaval, R., & Wyer, R. S. Jr. (1998). The role of narratives in consumer information processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 207-245.

    Alesandrini, K. L., & Sheikh A. A. (1983). Research on imagery: Implications for advertising. In A.A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: current theory, research and application (pp. 535-556). New York: John Wiley.

    Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). AMOS user’s guide, version 5.0. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Co. Babin, L.A. & Burns, A.C. (1997). Effects of print ad pictures and copy containing instructions

    to imagine on mental imagery that mediates attitudes. Journal of Advertising, 26(3), 33-44.

    Babin, L. A., Burns, A. C., & Biswas, A. (1992). A framework providing direction for research on communications effects of mental imagery-evoking advertising strategies. Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 621-628.

    Basilli, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 637-653.

    Bizer, G. Y., Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Memory-based versus on-line processing: Implications for attitude strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 646-653.

    Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1990). The effect of imagery processing and imagery content on behavioral intentions. Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 449-454.

    Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1992). Generation and consequences of communication evoked imagery. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 93-104.

    Brewer, W. F. (1988). Imagery and text genre. Text, 8, 431-439. Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The

    Guildford Press. Brunelle, E., & Lapierre, J. (2008). Testing media richness theory to explain consumers’

    intentions of buying online. In Proceedings of 10th International Conference of Electronic Commerce, Innsbruck, Austria.

    Burns, A. C., Biswas, A., & Babin, L. A. (1993). The operation of visual imagery as a mediator of advertising effects. Journal of Advertising, 22(2), 71-85.

    Cattell, R. B. (1956). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: World Book Co.

    Cattell, R. B., & Burdsal, C. A. (1975). The radial parcel double factoring design: A solution to the item-vs.-parcel controversy. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 10(2), 165-179.

    Chin, W.W., & Todd, P.A. (1995). On the use, usefulness and ease of use of structural equation modeling in MIS research: a note of caution. MIS Quarterly, 19(2), 237-246.

    Daft, R.L., & Lengel, R.H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32, 554-571.

    Daft, R.L. Lengel, R.H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media selection and manager performance:implicayions for information systems. MIS quarterly, September, 355-366.

  • 26

    D’Ambra, J. (1995). A field study of information technology, task equivocality, media richness, and media preference. PhD Dissertation, University of New South Wales.

    Delgadillo, Y., & Escalas, J. (2004). Narrative word-of-mouth communication: Exploring memory and attitude effects of consumer storytelling. Advances in Consumer Research, 31, 186-192.

    Denning, S. (2000). The Springboard: how storytelling ignites action in knowledge-era organizations. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.

    Dennis, A.R., & Kinney, S.T. (1998). Testing media richness theory in the new media: the effects of cues, feedback, and task equivocality. Information System Research, 9(3), 256-274

    Dennis, A.R., & Valacich, J.S. (1999). Rethinking media richness: towards a theory of media synchronicity. In Proceedings of the 32nd Hawaii International Conference on Systems Sciences, 1-10.

    Elliot, L. (1973). Imagery versus repetition encoding in short- and long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 270-276.

    Escalas, J. E. (2004). Image yourself in the product: Mental simulation, narrative transportation, and persuasion. Journal of Advertising, 33(2), 37-48.

    Escalas, J. E. (2007). Self-referencing and persuasion: Narrative transportation versus analytical elaboration. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 421-429.

    Ferrington, G. (1994). Audio design: Creating multi-sensory images for the mind. Journal of Visual Literacy, 14(1), 61-67.

    Fogg, B. J., Booker, A., & Don, A. (2004). Persuading people via computer-based narratives, Persuasive Technology Lab, Stanford University. Available at http://captology.stanford.edu/

    Fornell, C.R. & Larcker, D.F. (1981). Structural Equation Models with Unobserved Variables and Measurement Error. Journal of Marketing Research. 18, 39-50.

    Frick, R. (1984). Using both an auditory and a visual short-term store to increase digit span. Memory and Cognition, 12, 507-514.

    Gefen, D., Straub, D. W., & Boudreau, M. C. (2000). Structural equation modeling and regression: guidelines for research practice. Communications of the Association for Information Systems. 4(7), 1-70.

    Goossens, C. F. (1995). External information search: Effects of tour brochures with experiential information. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 3(3):89-107.

    Goossens, C. F. (2000). Tourism information and pleasure motivation. Annals of Tourism Research, 27(2), 301-321.

    Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701-721.

    Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 315-341). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate, Inc.

    Gretzel, U. (2006). Narrative design for travel recommender systems, In D. R. Fesenmaier, H. Werthner, & K. Wöber (Eds.), Travel destination recommendation systems: Behavioral foundations and applications (pp. 171-179). London: CAB International.

    Gretzel, U., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2002). Building narrative logic into tourism information systems. IEEE Intelligent Systems, November/December, 59-61.

  • 27

    Gretzel, U., Fesenmaier, D. R., Formica, S., & O’Leary, J. T. (2006). Searching for the Future: Challenges Faced by Destination Marketing Organizations. Journal of Travel Research, 45(November), 116-126.

    Haugtvedt, C. P., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Personality and persuasion: Need for cognition moderates and persistence and resistance of attitude changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 308-319.

    Hirschman, E., & Solomon, M. (1984). Utilitarian, aesthetic and familiarity responses to verbal versus visual advertisements. Advances in Consumer Research, 11, 426-443.

    Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fix indexes in covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.

    James, W. L., & Sonner, B. S. (2001). Just say no to traditional student samples. Journal of Advertising Research, 41 (October), 63-71.

    Jeong, M., & Choi, J. (2004). Effects of picture presentations on consumer’s behavioral intentions on the Web. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 17(2/3), 193-204.

    Kerr, B. (1999). Effective use of audio media in multimedia presentations. In Proceedings of the Mid-South Instructional Technology Conference, Murfreesboro, TN, March 28-30.

    Kim, H., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2008). Persuasive Design of Destination Web Sites: An Analysis of First Impression. Journal of Travel Research, 47(3), 3-13.

    Kisielius, J., & Sternthal, B. (1984). Detecting and explaining vividness effects in attitudinal judgments. Journal of Marketing Research, 21, 54-64.

    Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

    Krosnick, J. A., & Abelson, R. P. (1992). The case for measuring attitude strength in surveys. In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions (pp. 177-203). New York: Russell Sage.

    Landis, R. S., Beal, D. J., & Tesluk, P. E. (2000). A comparison of approaches to forming composite measures in structural equation models. Organizational research methods, 3(2), 186-207.

    Lee, W., & Gretzel, U. (2008). Use and perceived usefulness of destination Website features. In Hara, T. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 13th Annual Graduate Education and Student Research Conference in Hospitality and Tourism, Orlando, FL, January 3-5, 2008, pp. 142-150.

    Lee, W., Gretzel, U., & Law, R. (2010). Quasi-Trial Experiences through Sensory Information on Destination Web Sites. Journal of Travel Research, 49(3), 310-322.

    Lukošius, V. (2004). Mental imagery in advertisements: More than meets the eye? American Marketing Association, Boston, MA, August 6-9.

    Lutz, K. A., & Lutz, R. J. (1977). Effects of interactive imagery on learning: Application to advertising. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62(4), 493-498.

    Lutz, K. A., & Lutz, R. J. (1978). Imagery-eliciting strategies: Review and implications of research. Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 611-620.

    MacCallum, R.C., Roznowski, M., & Necowitz, L.B. (1992). Model modifications in covariances structure analysis: the problem of capitalization on chance. Psychological Bulletin, 111(3), 490-504.

    Macias, W. (2003). A Preliminary Structural Equation Model of Comprehension and Persuasion of Interactive Advertising Brand Web Sites. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 3(2), 36-48.

  • 28

    MacInnis, D. J., & Jaworski, B. J. (1989). Information processing from advertisement: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of Marketing, 53, 1-23.

    MacInnis, D. J., & Price, L. L. (1987). The role of imagery in information processing: Review and extensions. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 473-491.

    MacInnis, D. J., & Price, L. L. (1990). An exploratory study of the effects of imagery processing and consumer experience on expectations and satisfactions. Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 41-47.

    Mattila, A. S. (2000). The role of narratives in the advertising of experiential services. Journal of Service Research, 3(1), 35-45.

    Mayer, R. E. (1997). Multimedia learning: Are we asking the right questions. Educational Psychologist, 32, 1-19.

    McGill, A. L., & Anand, P. (1989). The effect of imagery on information processing strategy in a multi-attribute choice task. Marketing Letters, 1(1), 7-16.

    Meade, A. W., & Kroustalis, C. M. (2005). Problems of item parceling with CFA: Test of measurement invariance. Paper presented at the 20th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Los Angeles, CA.

    Miller, D. W., Hadjimarcou, J., & Miciak, A. (2000). A scale for measuring advertisement-evoked mental imagery. Journal of Marketing Communication, 6, 1-20.

    Miller, D. M., & Marks, L. J. (1992). Mental imagery and sound effects in radio commercials. Journal of Advertising, 21, 83-93.

    Miller, D. M., & Marks, L. J. (1997). The effects of imagery-evoking radio advertising strategies on affective responses. Psychology and Marketing, 14(4), 337-360.

    Miller, D. W., & Stoica, M. (2003). Comparing the effects of a photograph versus artistic renditions of a beach scene in a direct response print ad for a Caribbean resort island: A mental imagery perspective. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 10(1), 11-21.

    Mitchell, A. A. (1986). The effect of verbal and visual components of advertisements on brand attitudes and attitude toward the advertisement. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(June), 12-24.

    Mitchell, A. A., & Olson, J. C. (1981). Are product attribute beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand attribute? Journal of Marketing Research, 18(August), 318-332.

    Morosan, C., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2007). A Conceptual Framework of Persuasive Architecture of Tourism Websites: Propositions and Implications. In Sigala, M., Mich, L., & Murphy, J. (Eds.), Information and Communication Technologies in Tourism 2007, pp. 243-254. Vienna, Austria: Springer Verlag.

    Nelson, P. (1970). Information and consumer behavior. Journal of Political Economy, 78 (March/April), 311-329.

    Nunnally, J.G., (1967). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Nunnally, J. C. & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory, New York: McGraw-Hill. Oh, H., Fiore, A. M., & Jeoung, M. Y. (2007). Measuring experience economy concepts:

    Tourism applications. Journal of Travel Research, 46, 119-132. Olson, J., McAlexander, J., & Roberts, S. (1986). The impact of the visual content of

    advertisements upon the perceived vacation experience. In W. Joseph, L. Moutinho, & I. Vernon (Eds.), Tourism services marketing: Advances in theory and practice, vol.2 (pp. 260-269).Cleveland State University: American Marketing Association.

  • 29

    Packer, R. K., & Jordan, K. (2001). Multimedia: from wagner to virtual reality. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Padgett, D., & Allen, D. (1997). Communicating experiences: A narrative approach to creating service brand image. Journal of Advertising, 26(4), 49-62.

    Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of

    Psychology, 45, 255-287. Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1986). Evidence evaluation in complex decision making. Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 242-258. Peterson, R. A. (2001). On the Use of College Students in Social Science Research: Insights

    from a Second-Order Meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(3), 450-461. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances

    in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123-205. Petty, R. E., Haugtvedt, C. P., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Elaboration as a determinant of attitude

    strength: Creating attitudes that are persistent, resistant, and predictive of behavior. In R. E. Petty & J. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. (pp. 93-130), Hillsdale, NJ; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Phillips, B. J., & McQuarrie, E.F. (2010). Narrative and persuasion in fashion advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (October), 368-392.

    Pomerantz, E. M., Chaiken, S., & Tordesillas, R. S. (1995). Attitude strength and resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 408-419.

    Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1999). Implications of recent development in structural equation modeling for counseling psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 27, 485-527.

    Richardson, J. T. E. (1999). Imagery. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Rossiter, J. R. (1978). Visual imagery ability as a mediator of advertising response. Advances in

    Consumer Research, 5, 621-629. Rossiter, J. R., & Percy, L. (1983). Visual communication in advertising. In R. J. Harris (Ed.),

    Information processing research in advertising (pp. 83-125). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Rozier-Rich, S., & Santos, C. A. (2010). Processing promotional travel narratives. Tourism Management, accessible online doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2010.03.009

    Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1995). Knowledge and memory: The real story. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., (Ed.), Knowledge and memory: The real story. Advances in social cognition, 8, pp.1-85. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Schlosser, A. E. (2003). Experiencing products in the virtual world: The role of goal and imagery in influencing attitudes versus purchase intentions. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(3), 184-198.

    Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6, 156-163.

    Stamboulis, Y., & Skayannis, P. (2003). Innovation strategies and technology for experience-based tourism. Tourism Management, 24, 35-43.

    Starch, D. (1966). How does the shape of ads affect relationship? Media/Scope, 10, 83-85.

  • 30

    Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R. (1997). Building complexity and coherence: Children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches, (vol.1, pp. 5-44). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Sternthal, B., Tybout, A., & Calder, B. (1994). Experimental design; generalization and theoretical explanation. In Bagozzi, R. (Ed.), Principles of Marketing Research (pp.195-223), , Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

    Swan, W. B., Pelham, B. W., & Chidester, T. R. (1988). Change through paradox: Using self-verification to alter beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 268-173.

    Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn’t kill me makes me stronger: The effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1298–1313.

    Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Resistance to persuasion and attitude certainty: The moderating role of elaboration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1446-1457.

    Tussyadiah, I. P. & Fesenmaier, D.R. (2008). Marketing places through first-person stories-an analysis of Pensylvania roadtripper blog. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 25(3), 299-311.

    Unnava, H. R., Agarwal, S., & Haugtvedt, C. P. (1996). Interactive effects of presentation modality and message-generated imagery on recall of advertising information. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 81-88.

    Verhallen, M. J. A. J., Bus, A. G., & De Jong, M. T. (2006). The promise of multimedia stories for kindergarten children at risk. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 410-419.

    Walters, G., Sparks, B., & Herington, C. (2007). The effectiveness of print advertising stimuli evoking elaborate consumption visions for potential travelers. Journal of Travel Research, 46(3), 24-34.

    Wolfe, M. B. W. (2005). Memory for narrative and expository text: Independent influences of semantic associations and text organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 359-364.

    Xiang, Z., Wöber, K., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2008). Representation of the Online Tourism Domain in Search Engines. Journal of Travel Research, 47(2), 137-150.

    Xiang, Z. & Gretzel, U. (2010). Role of Social Media in Online Travel Information Search. Tourism Management, 31(2), 179-188.

  • 31

    Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Each Construct

    Items in Scale Internal Consistency Construct

    Name Item No. Item Wording Mean SD

    Cronbach’s Alpha

    Quantity

    Q12a Many images came to my mind 5.23 1.34

    0.92 Q12b A lot of images came to my mind 4.96 1.45

    Q12c I experienced various images in my mind 5.02 1.41

    Modality Q12d I imagined tastes 3.38 1.71

    0.82 Q12e I imagined scents 3.72 1.85

    Vividness

    Q13a Vivid-Vague 2.77 1.49

    0.93 Q13b Clear-Unclear 2.90 1.44 Q13c Sharp-Dull 3.34 1.60

    Q13d Intense-Weak 3.69 1.61 Q13e Well-defined-Fuzzy 3.28 1.61

    Valence

    Q13f Pleasant - Unpleasent 2.20 1.21

    0.96

    Q13g Good-Bad 2.37 1.26 Q13h Nice-Awful 2.38 1.30 Q13i Likable-Not likeable 2.21 1.23

    Q13j Positive-Not positive 2.08 1.16

    Attitude Strength-Time 1

    Q14 How good would the destination be for a pleasure trip? 6.12 1.02

    0.94 Q15 How positive would you say is your impression of the destination? 5.88 1.01

    Q16 How much do you think you would like this destination? 6.02 1.05

    Q17 How favorable is your impression of the destination? 5.86 1.05

    Attitude Confidence

    Q18 How certain are you of your attitude toward this destination? 5.40 1.23 0.84

    Q19 How sure are you that your current impression of the destination is correct? 4.85 1.39

    Attitude Strength-Time 2

    Q20 How good would the destination be for a pleasure trip? 3.68 1.48

    0.95 Q21 How positive would you say is your impression of the destination? 3.46 1.36

    Q22 How much do you think you would like this destination? 3.72 1.54

    Q23 How favorable is your impression of the destination? 3.47 1.32

  • 32

    Table 2. Alternative Measurement Model Analysis

    Latent Variable Indicators Factor Loadings Construct Reliability

    Average Variance Extracted

    (AVE)

    Mental Imagery Quantity .72

    .60 .65 Modality .58

    Communication Effect

    Attitude Strength .96 .82 .84

    Attitude Confidence .69

    Attitude Resistance

    Attitude Resistance 1 .76

    .90 .84 Attitude Resistance 2 .78

    Attitude Resistance 3 .86

    Attitude Resistance 4 .94

  • 33

    Table 3. Model-Fit Indices for Structural Model Model-Fit Statistics χ

    2 df χ2/df CFI GFI NFI RMSEA

    132.49 65 2.03 .93 .93 .89 .065

  • 34

    Figure 1. Conceptual Framework

    Website Characteristics

    Narrative Text vs. Expository Text

    Presence of Picture vs. No Picture

    Presence of Sound vs. No Sound

    Mental Imagery

    Processing

    H1

    H3

    H2

    H4

    Attitude Strength

    & Confidence

    Attitude Resistance

    H5 H5

  • 35

    Figure 2. Experimental Condition: Expository Text Only

  • 36

    Figure 3. Experimental Condition: Narrative Text, Picture, No Sound

  • 37

    **denotes significance at the 0.5 level

    Figure 4. Hypothesized Structural Model.

    Mental Imagery

    Q M

    e e

    Communication Effects AC

    AS e

    e

    Attitude Resistance AR3

    AR2 e

    e

    AR1 e

    AR4 e

    Sound

    Narrative × Picture

    Narrative × Sound

    Picture

    Narrative

    Narrative Picture Sound

    Picture × Sound

    .07 (ns)

    .23**

    .00 (ns)

    - .05 (ns)

    .07 (ns)

    .06 (ns)

    - .07 (ns)

    .76 .81

    .88

    R 2 = .72

    R 2 = .10

    .92 .72 .58

    R 2 = .07

    .95

    .70

    .85** .32**

    × ×

    d

    d

    d

  • 38

    Appendix 1.

    Intercorrelations Among Proposed Indicators 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

    1. AtRes1 - 2. AtRes2 .39** - 3. AtRes3 -.66** -.48** - 4. AtRes4 -.65** -.36** .78** - 5. Quan .54** .39** -.66** -.70** - 6. Modality .40** .42** -.56** -.50** .66** - 7. Vividness .20** -.03 -.05 -.14* .23** -.01 - 8. Valence .19** .04 -.12 -.16* .30** .13* .72** - 9. AttiS .25** .24** -.37** -.28** .23** .17** .00 -.01 - 10. AttiC .03 .11 -.01 .01 -.01 .02 .10 .05 -.15*

    Note. N = 239. 1=AtRes(Attitude resistance) ,2= AtRes2 , 3= AtRes3 , 4=AtRes4 , 5=Quantity ,6=Modality, 7= Vividness , 8=Valence 9=Attitude Strength , 10=Attitude Confidence * p < .05. ** p < .01.

    2. Theoretical Background2.2 Narrative Information Processing2.4. Communication Effects of Mental Imagery Processing

    3. Conceptual Framework and Hypotheses4.1 Design4.2 Measures4.3 Participants4.4 Procedure4.5 Manipulation Checks5.1 Measurement Model5.2 Structural Model6.1 Theoretical Implications