Top Banner
Chapter 9 Knowledge
52

Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Dec 17, 2015

Download

Documents

Candace Elliott
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Chapter 9

Knowledge

Page 2: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Some Questions to Consider

• Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,” by looking up its definition?

• How are the properties of various objects “filed away” in the mind?

• How is information about different categories stored in the brain?

• Can young infants respond to the categories “cat” and “dog”?

Page 3: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Knowledge

• Concept: mental representation used for a variety of cognitive functions

• Categorization is the process by which things are placed into groups called categories

Page 4: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Why Categories Are Useful

• Help to understand individual cases not previously encountered

• “Pointers to knowledge”

– Categories provide a wealth of general information about an item

– Allow us to identify the special characteristics of a particular item

Page 5: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Knowing that something is in a category provides a great deal of information about it.

Page 6: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Definitional Approach to Categorization

• Determine category membership based on whether the object meets the definition of the category

• Does not work well

• Not all members of everyday categories have the same defining features

Page 7: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Different objects, all possible “chairs.”

Page 8: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Definitional Approach to Categorization

• Family resemblance

– Things in a category resemble one another in a number of ways

Page 9: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Prototype = “typical”

• An abstract representation of the “typical” member of a category

• Characteristic features that describe what members of that concept are like

• An average of category members encountered in the past

• Contains the most salient features

• True of most instances of that category

Page 10: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Results of Rosch’s (1975a) experiment, in which participants judged objects on a scale of 1 (good example of a category) to 7 (poor example): (a) ratings for birds; (b) ratings for furniture.

Page 11: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• High-prototypicality: category member closely resembles category prototype

– “Typical” member

– For category “bird” = robin

Page 12: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Low-prototypicality: category member does not closely resemble category prototype

– For category “bird” = penguin

Page 13: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Strong positive relationship between prototypicality and family resemblance

• When items have a large amount of overlap with characteristics of other items in the category, the family resemblance of these items is high

• Low overlap = low family resemblance

Page 14: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Typicality effect: prototypical objects are processed preferentially

– Highly prototypical objects judged more rapidly

• Sentence verification technique

Page 15: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Results of E.E. Smith et al.’s (1974) sentence verification experiment. Reaction times were faster for objects rated higher in prototypicality

Page 16: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Typicality effect: prototypical objects are processed preferentially

– Prototypical objects are named more rapidly

Page 17: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Prototype Approach

• Typicality effect: prototypical objects are processed preferentially

– Prototypical category members are more affected by a priming stimulus

– Rosch (1975b)

• Hearing “green” primes a highly prototypical “green”

Page 18: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Procedure for Rosch’s (1975b) priming experiment. Results for the conditions when the test colors were the same are shown on the right. (a) The person’s “green” prototype matches the good green, but (b) is a poor match for the light green.

Page 19: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Exemplar Approach

• Concept is represented by multiple examples (rather than a single prototype)

• Examples are actual category members (not abstract averages)

• To categorize, compare the new item to stored examples

Page 20: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Exemplar Approach

• Similar to prototype view– Representing a category is not defining it

• Different: representation is not abstract– Descriptions of specific examples

• The more similar a specific exemplar is to a known category member, the faster it will be categorized

Page 21: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Exemplar Approach

• Explains typicality effect

• Easily takes into account atypical cases

• Easily deals with variable categories

Page 22: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Prototypes or Exemplars?

• May use both

• Exemplars may work best for small categories

• Prototypes may work best for larger categories

Page 23: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Levels of categories for (a) furniture and (b) vehicles. Rosch provided evidence for the idea that the basic level is “psychologically privileged.”

Page 24: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Left column: category levels; middle column: examples of each level for furniture; right column: average number of common features, listed from Rosch, Mervis et al.’s (1976) experiment.

Page 25: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Evidence that Basic-Level Is Special

• People almost exclusively use basic-level names in free-naming tasks

• Quicker to identify basic-level category member as a member of a category

• Children learn basic-level concepts sooner than other levels

• Basic-level is much more common in adult discourse than names for superordinate categories

• Different cultures tend to use the same basic-level categories, at least for living things

Page 26: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

A Hierarchical Organization

• To fully understand how people categorize objects, one must consider

– Properties of objects

– Learning and experience of perceivers

Page 27: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Results of Tanaka and Taylor’s (1991) “expert” experiment. Experts (left pair of bars) used more specific categories to name birds, whereas nonexperts (right pair of bars) used more basic categories.

Page 28: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Concepts are arranged in networks that represent the way concepts are organized in the mind

Page 29: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Collins and Quillian (1969)

– Node = category/concept

– Concepts are linked

– Model for how concepts and properties are associated in the mind

Page 30: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Collins and Quillian’s (1969) semantic network. Specific concepts are indicated in blue. Properties of concepts are indicated at the nodes for each concept. Additional properties of a concept can be determined by moving up the network, along the lines connecting the concepts. For example, moving from “canary” up to “bird” indicates that canaries have feathers and wings and can fly.

Page 31: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Cognitive economy: shared properties are only stored at higher-level nodes

• Exceptions are stored at lower nodes

• Inheritance– Lower-level items share properties of higher-level

items

Page 32: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: The distance between concepts predicts how long it takes to retrieve information about concepts as measured by the sentence verification technique. Because it is necessary to travel on two links to get from canary to animal (left), but on only one to get from canary to bird (right) it should take longer to verify the statement “a canary is an animal.”

Page 33: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Spreading activation– Activation is the arousal level of a node– When a node is activated, activity spreads

out along all connected links– Concepts that receive activation are

primed and more easily accessed from memory

Page 34: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Lexical decision task

– Participants read stimuli and are asked to say as quickly as possible whether the item is a word or not

Page 35: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Myer and Schvaneveldt (1971)

– “Yes” if both strings are words; “no” if not

– Some pairs were closely associated

– Reaction time was faster for those pairs

• Spreading activation

Page 36: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Criticism of Collins and Quillian

– Cannot explain typicality effects

– Cognitive economy?

– Some sentence-verification results are problematic for the model

Page 37: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Semantic Networks

• Collins and Loftus (1975) modifications

– Shorter links to connect closely related concepts

– Longer linkers for less closely related concepts

– No hierarchical structure; based on person’s experience

Page 38: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Semantic network proposed by Collins and Loftus (1975). (Reprinted from A.M. Collins & E.F. Loftus, “A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing.” From Psychological Review, 82, pp. 407-428, Fig. 1. Copyright © 1975 with permission from the American Psychological Association.

Page 39: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Assessment of Semantic Networks

• Is predictive and explanatory of some results, but not all

• Generated multiple experiments

• Lack of falsifiability

– No rules for determining link length or how long activation will spread

– Therefore, there is no experiment that would “prove it wrong”

– Circular reasoning

Page 40: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• How do neurons that can only either signal or not signal (on-off) represent knowledge?

• Model on digital circuitry – 1 bit can only be on or off– Group them together and get many more

patterns (1 byte: 00110011)

• Our neurons group together to form a network to represent concepts

• A network of nodes and links but operates very differently from semantic networks

Page 41: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• “Neuron-like units”

– Input units: activated by stimulation from environment

– Hidden units: receive input from input units

– Output units: receive input from hidden units

Page 42: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: A connectionist network showing input units, hidden units, and output units. Incoming stimuli activate the input units, and signals travel through network, activating the hidden and output units. Note that this is an extremely simplified version of a connectionist network. Networks used in research on connectionism contain many more units and more complex connections between units.

Page 43: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• Parallel distributed processing

• Knowledge represented in the distributed activity of many units

• Weights determine at each connection how strongly an incoming signal will activate the next unit

Page 44: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• How learning occurs

– Network responds to stimulus

– Provided with correct response

– Modifies responding to match correct response

Page 45: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• Error signal

– Difference between actual activity of each output unit and the correct activity

Page 46: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• Back-propagation: error signal transmitted back through the circuit

• Indicates how weights should be changed to allow the output signal to match the correct signal

• The process repeats until the error signal is zero

Page 47: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: Learning in a connectionist network. Bars represent activity in the eight representation units. Notice how the pattern of activation changes as learning progresses.

Page 48: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• Slow learning process that creates a network capable of handling a wide range of inputs

• Learning can be generalized

Page 49: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

The Connectionist Approach

• Graceful degradation: disruption of performance occurs gradually as parts of the system are damaged

Page 50: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Categories in the Brain

• Different areas of the brain may be specialized to process information about different categories

– Double dissociation for categories “living things” and “nonliving things”

Page 51: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Categories in the Brain

• Categories are represented by distributed activity

– More similar patterns of brain activity for categories with similar features

– Category-specific neurons

Page 52: Chapter 9 Knowledge. Some Questions to Consider Why is it difficult to decide if a particular object belongs to a particular category, such as “chair,”

Caption: (a) Testing procedure for determining if monkeys can categorize cats and dogs. (b) Activity recorded from a neuron in the monkey’s IT cortex during the testing sequence. This neuron responds more to a 60 percent dog stimulus than to a 60 percent cat stimulus during presentation of the sample. (c) Activity recorded from a neuron in the monkey’s PF cortex during the testing sequence. This neuron responds better to the dog stimulus during the delay and test periods.