Top Banner
Project Title Effects of Model Facial Expression & BMI in Health Advertising on Consumer Intent to Achieve Health Goals: An Eye-Tracking Study Names of Principal Applicant and Co-Applicants (please also include the names of the institutions and email addresses) Dr Kerrie Bertele, University of Hertfordshire Dr Ariadne Kapetanaki, University of Hertfordshire Dr Paul Connell, Stoneybrook University Introduction Globally, an estimated 1.46 billion adults are overweight and 502 million are obese (Swinburn et al. 2011). People with excess weight are prone to getting type 2 diabetes, heart disease and certain cancers (Flegal at al. 2007). Obesity can also affect individuals’ self-esteem and mental health (Krishen and Worthen 2011). Social marketing campaigns that include mass media advertising to promote healthy eating are among the initiatives proposed by the World Health Organisation in their portfolio of actions to tackle obesity (WHO/Europe, 2008). In this vein, developed countries have allocated significant budgets to promote healthy eating behaviours through advertising. Despite the huge public spending on programs and campaigns to enhance healthy eating behaviors, obesity rates reflect citizen’s persistence in making unhealthy choices. The prevalence of obesity is continually increasing and despite public expenditure to tackle the problem “no national success stories have been reported in the past 33 years” (Ng et al. 2014). Limited research examines the role of advertising promoting healthy eating. Effective antismoking advertising has received substantial attention, focusing on the message valence and intensity (Reardon et al. 2006), the use of graphic images (Andrews et al. 2014) and the effectiveness of message themes (Pechmann et al. 2003). The relationship between advertising and obesity has also been heavily studied, but the focus has been on the impact of food advertising on rising obesity levels (Beales and Kulick 2013), particularly of advertising targeted at children (Harker, Harker and Burns 2007). Some notable exceptions examining effective health campaigns include Kees, Burton and Tangari’s (2010) investigation of
22

Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Jun 26, 2019

Download

Documents

vuphuc
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Project Title

Effects of Model Facial Expression & BMI in Health Advertising on Consumer Intent to Achieve Health Goals: An Eye-Tracking Study

Names of Principal Applicant and Co-Applicants (please also include the names of the institutions and email addresses)

Dr Kerrie Bertele, University of HertfordshireDr Ariadne Kapetanaki, University of Hertfordshire Dr Paul Connell, Stoneybrook University

Introduction

Globally, an estimated 1.46 billion adults are overweight and 502 million are obese (Swinburn et al. 2011). People with excess weight are prone to getting type 2 diabetes, heart disease and certain cancers (Flegal at al. 2007). Obesity can also affect individuals’ self-esteem and mental health (Krishen and Worthen 2011). Social marketing campaigns that include mass media advertising to promote healthy eating are among the initiatives proposed by the World Health Organisation in their portfolio of actions to tackle obesity (WHO/Europe, 2008). In this vein, developed countries have allocated significant budgets to promote healthy eating behaviours through advertising. Despite the huge public spending on programs and campaigns to enhance healthy eating behaviors, obesity rates reflect citizen’s persistence in making unhealthy choices. The prevalence of obesity is continually increasing and despite public expenditure to tackle the problem “no national success stories have been reported in the past 33 years” (Ng et al. 2014).

Limited research examines the role of advertising promoting healthy eating. Effective antismoking advertising has received substantial attention, focusing on the message valence and intensity (Reardon et al. 2006), the use of graphic images (Andrews et al. 2014) and the effectiveness of message themes (Pechmann et al. 2003). The relationship between advertising and obesity has also been heavily studied, but the focus has been on the impact of food advertising on rising obesity levels (Beales and Kulick 2013), particularly of advertising targeted at children (Harker, Harker and Burns 2007). Some notable exceptions examining effective health campaigns include Kees, Burton and Tangari’s (2010) investigation of the role of temporal framing on consumer’s health goals. The aim of this research is to identify the optimal portrayal of models in health advertising in order to enhance health advertising effectiveness. Attentional avoidance is an effective defense against emotive, distressing or threatening stimuli, and reduces accessibility of the message (Hansen, Hansen and Shantz, 1992) and ad persuasiveness (Keller and Block 1997), therefore capturing consumer attention is an important goal of health advertisers. This project involves two eye-tracking experiments: Study 1 considers the direct impact of model facial expression (Serious x Fake Smile x Duchenne Smile) on the consumer attention and intent to diet, and questions whether this is mediated by visual attention to the advertisement. The second study investigates the interaction of facial expression x BMI. Participant hunger, gender, their BMI and self-esteem are controlled. Self-esteem is often linked with body image satisfaction; lower self-esteem is related to higher levels of body image dissatisfaction (Krishen and Worthen 2011). Those with higher body image dissatisfaction are more likely to want to change their body, and may be more influenced by the experiment stimuli. The findings are aimed at informing effective advertisements to enhance consumer intent to eat healthily. If every adult reduces their BMI by just

Page 2: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

1%, about 100,000 new cases of cancer would be prevented (National Cancer Institute, 2012). Small steps taken to address obesity can therefore have an important impact.

Report of Activities

Empirical Research completed:To date two eye-tracking experiments have been carried out.Study 1

The first study was a three-way between-subjects design investigating model facial expression (serious x fake smile x Duchenne smile) on consumer intent to achieve health goals and visual attention. This experiment was conducted with students from a UK university (Brown and Richardson 2012), with a minimum of 29 participants per condition (n = 89). This sample size was consistent with studies adopting a similar eye-tracking experimental design (Hollitt et al. 2010). Participants were given £5 compensation for taking part in the study. This study involved a four step process:

First a photo-shoot was carried out with a model recruited to capture the required facial expressions. This was completed in the film studies department at the University of Hertfordshire by a professional photographer. Next the facial expressions were pre-tested via an online questionnaire (n=68). The facial expression manipulation was tested for valence of expression and genuineness of expression. Results are detailed in the method and results sections below. The public service advertisement stimuli were then created using photoshop. Finally the main eye-tracking experimental study was carried out.

Study 2Study 2 was a 3 x 2 between-subjects factorial design investigating the impact of facial

expression (serious x fake smile x Duchenne smile) x model BMI (low x high) on visual attention and intent to diet. This study was conducted with a combination of students at a UK university as well as participants at a local shopping mall, to enhance the heterogeneity of the sample (n = 151). There were a minimum of 25 participants in each of the six conditions, consistent with similar eye-tracking research studies (Hoover et al. 2010). Participants were compensated with either a £5 voucher to spend in the shopping mall (mall consumers) or £5 cash (University students).

This study involved two stages. First the same stimuli as in Study 2 were manipulated using photoshop to develop high model BMI/low model BMI versions of the advertisements. Next the main eye-tracking experimental study was carried out.

Data Analysis & Write up

The questionnaire and eye-tracking data was cleaned, analysed and written up. It was prepared for a journal article submission and conference presentation.

Journal Submission

This research was written up and submitted to the Journal of Advertising Special Issue: Effective Health Messages in Advertising. It was sent to reviewers and we are waiting on feedback.

Page 3: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Planned Activities for the Future

Data Collection:We plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of

Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking experimental study investigating the impact of model appearance on consumer health goals focusing on an alternative variable to Study 1 & Study 2. Alternatively we will carry out qualitative research in the form of interviews to get a deeper insight into the impact of models in public service advertisements on consumer intent to live healthily.

Conference Attendance: We look forward to presenting our project at the British Academy of Management

Conference in September 2015, and then going on to collect further data and spread our results to the consumer psychology research area in Europe (EMAC 2016) and North America (ACR 2016).

Were the Research Aims and Objectives met?

The overarching aim of this research was:

To identify optimal portrayal of models in health advertising in order to determine effective health advertising strategies and take a step towards tackling the global obesity crisis.

To this end, specific objectives include:

1. Investigate the impact of facial expression (serious, smiling (forced), smiling (spontaneous)) in health food advertising on consumer affective responses and intentions to adopt a healthy lifestyle.

Facial expression is known to impact perceived physical attractiveness, yet no study has examined the influence of facial expression on individual response to health marketing. This represents an important contribution.

2. Identify the extent to which the impact of facial expression on consumer behaviour intentions is mediated by visual attention to elements of the ad.

This project hypothesises that the persuasiveness of health food advertising portraying models will be mediated by visual attention to the advertisement

3. Investigate the impact of model BMI on consumer affective responses and intentions to adopt a healthy lifestyle

4. Identify the extent to which the impact of model BMI on consumer affective and behavioural responses is mediated by visual attention to elements of the ad.

5. Determine the interaction effect of model BMI and facial expression on consumer affective and behavioural responses.

6. Examine whether this interaction effect is mediated by visual attention to elements of the advertisement.

7. Identify the moderating impact of relevant individual difference variables (e.g. BMI, self-esteem, self-control) on consumer response to different health advertisements.

8. Explore the coping strategies (e.g. attentional avoidance, selective accessibility) used by individuals sharing certain individual difference variables (e.g. High BMI, Low self-esteem) when presented with distressing or threatening health advertisements (e.g. morbidly obese/extremely thin imagery)

The primary aim and objectives 1-7 have been met by Study 1 & Study 2.

Page 4: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Findings from the two experiments offer an important contribution in relation to illustrating optimal model presentation strategies in health advertising. The first study indicates that, for female consumers, a Duchenne smiling model is optimal. Specifically, alongside stimulating a higher intent to diet than the serious and fake expressions, a Duchenne smiling model leads to higher attention to the verbal message than a serious expression. Emotional contagion theory supports the role of portraying a Duchenne smile to enhance audience response (Hatfield, Cacioppo and Rapson 1994).

The results provide a tale of caution for advertisers who employ distressing and graphic images when warning against the consequences of obesity. Not only can distressing imagery lead to attentional avoidance (Brown and Richardson 2012), but it can fail to garner the positive consumer emotions spurred by smiling models which are in turn attributed to positive behaviour intent (Ekman, Davidson and Friesen 1990). While intent to diet is not affected by the model BMI, evidence from the second study overwhelmingly supports the role of model BMI in capturing visual attention to the advertisement. Higher model BMI is more effective in gaining visual attention when combined with a serious or Duchenne smiling model, and is found to be most effective when the target audience is overweight. This is consistent with social comparison theory, explaining how individuals compare themselves with models that are similar to them and avoid comparisons with heavier or thinner models as a means of self-enhancement (Festinger 1954). Given that public campaigns are attempting to change behaviours and reverse obesity levels, higher BMI individuals are likely to be an important target audience.

The preliminary evidence from these experiments suggests that advertisers should consider both the model facial expression and BMI when designing public health advertisements. The facial expression has an important significant impact on consumer intent to diet, and the model BMI has a strong influence on visual attention. Combining findings from the two studies, a Duchenne smiling model with a high BMI is optimal to both capture visual attention and enhance behavioral intentions.

Objective 8 [Explore the coping strategies (e.g. attentional avoidance, selective accessibility) used by individuals sharing certain individual difference variables (e.g. High BMI, Low self-esteem) when presented with distressing or threatening health advertisements (e.g. morbidly obese/extremely thin imagery)] had not been met. This will be achieved via a third study post the duration of the grant.

Limitations or challenges encountered

Methodological limitations:The stimuli employed in the experiments include a female model only, which could have

impacted how the male participants responded to the stimuli. It also prevented an analysis of the impact of advertisement-audience gender congruence on the impact of the model presentation strategies on attention and behaviour intent, which represents an interesting avenue for future research. Second, in an attempt to increase the external validity of the study, the health advertisement stimuli were placed among four real life advertisements; for cars, washing up liquid, and perfume. Post-experiment discussions with some participants revealed that some of them believed they were going to be questioned on the content of the advertisements, which may have led to increased focus on the branded advertisements, diluting the impact of our manipulated stimuli on their responses. Finally, intent to diet was measured with a two-item scale (Brown and

Page 5: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Richardson 2012), alternative scales measuring intentions to behave in a manner consistent with the ad message (e.g. Chandran and Menon’s (2004) five item scale) may offer additional insight.

Sample recruitment challenge:We proposed recruiting 120 students for Study 1, we were only able to recruit 90

participants (89 valid responses). Given the time required to complete this offline study it is a challenge to recruit participants. In Study 2 we proposed recruiting 120 participants. For this study we increased the number of experimental conditions from three to six conditions. Ideally we would have recruited a minimum of 180 participants; however we were only successful in recruiting 150 participants. We managed to meet the minimum acceptable sample size for eye-tracking experimental research (Hollitt et al. 2010). In order to increase our sample size we went on to collect a further 14 participants offering £10.

Personal challenges:Two of the researchers (PI Dr Kerrie Bertele and co-investigator Dr Ariadne Kapetanaki) went

on maternity leave during the course of the grant duration, leaving less time to complete the project than was forseen on application of the grant. This left only 9 months as opposed to 12 months to complete our objectives, which proved very challenging.

Analysis of Methods

The methodology adopted was a series of eye-tracking experiments. Two studies have been completed:

Study 1Study 1 was a three-way between-subjects design investigating model facial expression

(serious x fake smile x Duchenne smile) on consumer intent to achieve health goals and visual attention. This experiment was conducted with students from a UK university (Brown and Richardson 2012), with a minimum of 29 participants per condition (n = 89). This sample size is consistent with studies adopting a similar eye-tracking experimental design (Hollitt et al. 2010). Participants were given £5 compensation for taking part in the study.

Stimuli Development.The first step was to develop three stimuli with the required facial expressions. A model was

recruited and a professional photo-shoot was conducted. The model was paid £100 for participating in the study. The model was shown images of the facial expressions by the researchers and three photographs were selected from a sample of 100 photographs. Following the photo-shoot three advertisements were created and a verbal advertisement copy was added ‘Eat healthily to avoid obesity and live better’. Both positive (‘live better’) and negative (‘avoid obesity’) framing are included in this slogan to rule out the potential confounding impact of the valence of the information framing (Levin, Schneider and Gaeth 1998).

FIGURE 1PHOTO SHOOT IMAGES

Page 6: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Pre-test.To test the validity of the facial expression manipulation a questionnaire was conducted

using an online consumer panel (61.8% male, 96% from the US, 88.2% third level educated). Participants were paid for their participation (n = 68). Each participant was randomly exposed to one of the three facial expressions followed by an identical questionnaire. The questionnaire included Izard’s (1977) differential emotions scale (DES), which proposes a set of ten basic emotions - interest, enjoyment, surprise, fear, sadness, anger, shame, guilt, disgust, and contempt - each captured with three distinct items. We are particularly interested in whether the model displays enjoyment (items: delighted/happy/joyful) or sadness (items: downhearted/sad/discouraged). It is expected there will be no significant difference in the other emotions and that they should be relatively low. The manipulation of the fake vs. Duchenne smile was tested by asking how genuine the smile of the model was perceived. This was measured on a seven-point Likert scale anchored by extremely fake/extremely genuine (Krumhuber and Kappas 2005).

Design and Procedure.The experiment involved calibrating the participant’s pupils to capture the eye-tracking data.

They were then allowed to process through the study at their own pace. To increase external validity the manipulated advertisements were placed randomly among four other neutral advertisements (e.g. perfume, washing powder, car). All of the advertisements were black and white to avoid the confounding impact of color (Gorn et al. 1997). Each participant was randomly assigned to one of the three experimental conditions. The five advertisements were followed by an identical questionnaire. A Tobii X2-30 eye-tracker was used to collected visual attention data including gaze duration (milliseconds) and fixation density for the whole ad and specific ad elements (Hollitt et al. 2010). Both duration of gaze -representing longer gaze duration achieved by refixations of the pupil (Russo and Leclerc 1994)- and fixation density - the number of pupil fixations on a given area of the advertisement which is conceptually equated to attention to that area of the stimulus (Feiereisen, Broderick, and Wong 2008)- are used to assess visual attention in this paper. The Tobii X2-30 eye-tracker is a small piece of hardware attached to the base of a monitor. It has a frequency of 30 Hertz, meaning the screen is refreshed 30 times per second. A 24” monitor is used to present the images, consistent with real-life magazine sizes. It captures highly accurate gaze-position data in real-life

Page 7: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

conditions – allowing participants to move their head freely and accurately in varying light conditions.

Study 2The second study introduces the role of model BMI on consumer responses. Study 2 was a 3

x 2 between-subjects factorial design investigating the impact of facial expression (serious x fake smile x Duchenne smile) x model BMI (low x high) on visual attention and intent to diet. This study was conducted with a combination of students at a UK university as well as participants at a local shopping mall, to enhance the heterogeneity of the sample (n = 151). There were a minimum of 25 participants in each of the six conditions, consistent with similar eye-tracking research studies (Hoover et al. 2010). Participants are compensated with either a £5 voucher to spend in the shopping mall (mall consumers) or £5 cash (University students).

Stimuli Development.The first step of Study 2 is manipulating the model BMI using Photoshop. Following this

manipulation six advertisement stimuli were created (three facial expressions x two model body sizes) with the same slogan as in Study 1.

FIGURE 2MANIPULATED PHOTO SHOOT IMAGES (e.g. SERIOUS EXPRESSION)

Design and Procedure.Study 2 followed an identical procedure as the first study. Participants were sat at the

monitor, their eyes were calibrated, and they progressed through the study at their own pace. As in Study 1 the manipulated advertisement was randomly placed among four other neutral advertisements. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the six experimental conditions, and an even breakdown of gender and BMI was sought across each condition. The same questionnaire was employed in Study 2. An additional dependent variable was included – we offered participants to help themselves to a large bowl of M&M’s following exposure to the advertisement stimuli. This was weighed and recorded in order to determine actual consumption of unhealthy foods (Boland,

Page 8: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Connell and Vallen, 2013). In Study 2 a finer measure of participant BMI was recorded, participants were asked to calculate and report their exact BMI.

Analysis of Results

Study 1 Results:

The sample was recruited from a UK university (n = 89), 66.3% are female. The BMI range included 16.9% <18.5 BMI, 62.9% 18.5-24.9 BMI, 16.9% 25-29.9 BMI, and 3.4% >30 BMI. Participant BMI, hunger, gender and self-esteem are controlled for throughout the analysis. A univariate ANCOVA investigating the impact of model facial expression on consumer intent to diet is not significant. We find no significant impact of the Duchenne smile (M Duchenne = 5.05) versus the fake smile (M Fake = 5.01) or the serious expression (M Serious = 5.36) on consumer intent to diet (M Serious vs. M Duchenne, F (1, 60) = 1.310, p = 0.257; M Serious vs. M Fake, F (1, 59) = 0.031, p = 0.861; M Duchenne vs. M Fake, F (1, 59) = 1.271, p = 0.265).

Given the model in the advertisement is female and male participants may not experience the same level of involvement with the advertisement (Diedrichs and Lee 2010), we look at the impact of model facial expression on consumer intent to diet for the female participants only (n = 59). For females hypothesis 1 is supported, the Duchenne smile (M Duchenne (female) = 5.59) is significantly more effective than the fake smile (M Fake (female) = 4.92) and the serious expression (M Serious (female) = 4.88) in enhancing intent to diet (Figure 3) (M Serious (female) vs. M Duchenne (female), F (1, 39) = 5.162, p = 0.030; M Duchenne (female) vs. M Fake (female), F (1, 38) = 4.028, p = 0.053); M Serious (female) vs. M Fake (female), F (1, 41) = 0.001, p = 0.978).

A similar analysis for male participants indicates no significant difference between serious (M Serious (male) = 5.44), fake smile (M Fake (male) =5.22) and Duchenne smile (M Duchenne (male) =5.04) on intent to diet (M Serious (male) vs. M Duchenne (male), F (1, 21) = 1.722, p = 0.208; M Serious (male) vs. M Fake (male), F (1, 18) = 0.632, p = 0.441; M Duchenne (male) vs. M Fake (male), F (1, 21) = 0.134, p = 0.719). Univariate ANCOVA analysis comparing each of the three facial expressions to each other reveals no significant difference of the facial expression on consumer gaze duration or fixation density to the whole advertisement, or the verbal ad message, model face, eyes, or mouth. Repeating this analysis for female participants only indicates the hypothesis 2 is partially supported. The descriptive statistics reveal that the Duchenne smile leads to highest gaze duration and fixation count for both the whole advertisement and each specific advertisement element. The Duchenne smile is significantly more effective than the serious expression in enhancing visual attention to the model mouth (M Duchenne (female) = 0.651secs vs. M Serious

(female) = 0.124secs, F (1, 39) = 6.293, p = 0.017), as and close to significance for fixation count to the verbal message (M Duchenne (female) = 15.61 vs. M Serious (female) = 11.10, F (1, 39) = 3.949, p = 0.055). We find no significant differences in the serious vs. fake smile or the Duchenne vs. fake smile for visual attention measures.

Using the same control variables, 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals with 5,000 bootstrap samples are conducted to investigate the mediating impact of visual attention to the model mouth and verbal message on the effect of facial expression (Duchenne vs. serious) on female intent to diet (Preacher and Hayes 2008). Because the bootstrap includes zero for both gaze duration to the model mouth (-.1437 to .1253) and fixation count to the verbal ad copy (-.0609 to .2262) we can conclude that hypothesis 3 is rejected, and a direct only effect (non-mediation) of model facial expression on female intent to diet exists.

Page 9: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

FIGURE 3IMPACT OF FACIAL EXPRESSION ON FEMALE INTENT TO DIET

4.884.92

5.59

Consumer Intent to Diet

Study 2 Results:

The sample was 57.2% female, participant BMI ranged from 14.2-36.8, (M participant BMI = 24.02), 52.6% of the sample had a BMI ≤23.9 and 47.4% of the sample had a BMI of ≥24. We controlled for hunger and self-esteem throughout this study. When the moderating influence of gender and participant BMI was not under investigation, these two variables were also controlled.

A univariate ANCOVA analysis shows no significant interaction of participant BMI x model BMI on consumer intent to diet (F (1, 152) = .616, p = 0.434). Further stepwise comparisons indicate the model BMI does not significantly impact intent to diet for either low BMI participants (F (1, 80) = 0.700, p = 0.405) or high BMI participants (F (1, 72) = 0.140, p = 0.710), therefore hypothesis 4 is rejected. Controlling for gender, hunger and participant BMI we find that model BMI does however have an impact on actual consumer consumption of chocolate snacks following exposure to the advertisement which is close to significance (F (1, 152) = 3.009, p = 0.085). A low model BMI (M M&M

consumption (low model BMI) = 7.43) increased participant consumption compared to a high model BMI (M M&M

consumption (high model BMI) = 4.21). The BMI of the participant has no significant impact on the consumption of chocolate snacks (F (1, 152) = 0.004, p = 0.948). Univariate ANCOVA analysis reveals that model BMI has an important impact on consumer visual attention to the health advertisement. Overall, we find that consumer visual attention to the whole advertisement (both gaze duration and fixation density) is higher for high versus low model BMI (Table 1). A closer investigation into visual attention to specific advertisement elements indicates that it is the visual attention to the body, rather than the face, that lends to higher visual attention to the advertisement with the high versus low model BMI.

TABLE 1VISUAL ATTENTION ACROSS LOW V. HIGH MODEL BMI

Serious Fake Smile Duchenne Smile

p=0.05p=0.03

Page 10: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Visual Attention* Low Model BMI High Model BMI

F P

M SD M SD

Gaze duration to Ad 4.554 2.23 5.3714 2.04 5.071 0.026

Fixations on Ad 14.80 6.92 17.14 6.63 4.147 0.044

Gaze duration to Verbal 2.7209 1.45 3.1801 1.44 3.563 0.061

Gaze duration to Face .9509 0.84 1.0305 0.71 0.229 0.633

Gaze duration to body .3764 0.57 .5639 0.54 3.852 0.052

Fixation Count for Verbal 9.53 4.82 10.43 4.72 1.257 0.264

Fixations on Face 2.39 2.05 2.72 1.67 0.825 0.365

Fixations on Body 1.11 1.54 1.75 1.67 5.765 0.018

*Gaze = seconds, Fixations = frequency count

Considering the female participants only (n = 87), the impact of model BMI on visual attention remains important; high model BMI enhances female attention (both gaze duration and fixation density) to the whole ad, the verbal ad copy, and fixations on the face (Table 2). Table 3 illustrates the interaction of model BMI x model facial expression. A univariate ANCOVA analysis indicates no significant interaction for each visual attention measure. A closer analysis at each facial expression individually indicates that for the serious expression (gaze duration and fixation density) and Duchenne smile (gaze duration) high model BMI leads to higher attention to the advertisement than low model BMI. Specifically, for both the serious and Duchenne smile, attention is captured by the verbal advertisement message. For the fake smile overall visual attention to the advertisement is not impacted by the model BMI, although the results indicate that high model BMI leads to higher fixation density on the body than low model BMI for the fake smile condition.

TABLE 2VISUAL ATTENTION ACROSS LOW V. HIGH MODEL BMI (FEMALES)

Visual Attention Low Model BMI High Model BMI

F P

M SD M SD

Gaze duration to Ad 4.054 1.70 5.345 1.69 10.929 0.001

Fixations on Ad 13.52 5.30 17.37 5.14 10.098 0.002

Gaze duration to Verbal 2.408 1.06 3.278 1.33 10.725 0.002

Gaze duration to Face 0.800 0.77 0.997 0.60 1.299 0.258

Gaze duration to body 0.353 0.62 0.563 0.55 2.072 0.154

Fixation Count for Verbal 8.66 3.72 10.65 4.40 4.679 0.033

Fixations on Face 2.00 1.58 2.86 1.66 4.837 0.031

Fixations on Body 1.07 1.58 1.79 1.74 3.276 0.074

Page 11: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

TABLE 3INTERACTION OF MODEL BMI x FACIAL EXPRESSION ON ATTENTION

Visual Attention

Serious Fake Smile Duchene SmileLow BMI

High BMI F, p

Low BMI

High BMI F, p

Low BMI

High BMI F, p

Gaze duration to Ad

3.836 5.388 5.784, 0.020

5.531 5.570 .106, .747

4.256 5.138 4.313, .044

Fixations on Ad

12.52 17.54 6.025, 0.018

17.92 17.46 ..002, .986

13.84 16.37 3.543, .067

Gaze duration to Verbal

2.269 3.061 5.320, 0.026

3.571 3.271 .156, .694

2.288 3.210 8.430, .006

Gaze duration to Face

0.846 1.030 .361, .551

1.169 1.063 .026, .873

0.829 0.995 .471, .496

Gaze duration to body

0.464 .698 .618, .436

0.348 0.634 4.012, .051

0.318 0.342 .042, .839

Fixations on Verbal

8.00 10.35 4.514, .039

12.23 10.65 .694, .409

8.24 10.29 3.397, .072

Fixations on Face

2.24 2.69 .051, .822

2.96 2.62 .129, .721

1.96 2.88 3.406, .072

Fixations on Body

1.32 2.04 .877, .354

1.08 2.04 4.647, .036

0.92 1.13 .469, .497

Page 12: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

To test hypothesis 5 the interaction of participant BMI x model BMI is examined. A univariate ANCOVA analysis indicates no significant interaction for each visual attention measure. The results presented in Table 4 illustrate partial support for hypothesis 5. For participants with a low-normal BMI, the model BMI does not impact their visual attention to the advertisement. However, for participants with normal-high BMI the high BMI model (M high model BMI = 5.74) leads to higher gaze duration to the advertisement than the low BMI model (M low model BMI = 4.486). More specifically, participant’s enhanced visual attention was focused on the body of the model in the advertisement.

TABLE 4INTERACTION OF PARTICIPANT BMI x MODEL BMI ON ATTENTION

Visual Attention

Low Participant BMI

F, p

High Participant BMI

F, p

Low Model BMI

High Model

BMI

Low Model BMI

High Model BMI

Gaze duration to Ad

4.607 4.983 .799, .374 4.486 5.740 4.561, .036

Fixations on Ad 14.88 16.32 .913, .342 14.70 17.92 3.485, .066Gaze duration to Verbal

2.734 3.068 1.212, .275 2.704 3.286 2.107, .151

Gaze duration to Face

1.002 0.861 .699, .406 0.884 1.192 2.239, .139

Gaze duration to body

0.415 0.477 .253, .617 0.327 0.647 6.254, .015

Fixations on Verbal

9.47 10.27 .561, .456 9.61 10.59 .580, .449

Fixations on Face

2.58 2.38 .288, .593 2.15 3.05 3.220, .077

Fixations on Body

1.26 1.43 .267, .607 0.91 2.05 9.169, .003

Impact of Research to the wider business and management academic community

In terms of academic impact, this research makes two contributions, both theoretically and methodologically. First, this research is addressing two previously unexplored areas: consumer response to model BMI in health advertising and the role of model facial expression on consumer responses to health advertising. Prior research has considered the impact of model size on consumer self-esteem and affective responses (Smeesters et al. 2009), but no research has captured the impact of this on health goals, or whether attentional avoidance mediates this relationship. Attentional avoidance to threatening or distressing images has been explored for anti-alcohol messages (Brown and Richardson 2012), this project builds on this research in the context of obesity and health goal activation. To the researcher’s knowledge, no research has considered the impact of model facial expression on consumer responses to either obese/distressing or thin/aspirational model portrayal in health advertisements. Given the link between smiling and perceived happiness (Otta et al. 1996) it could be expected that portraying a thin, smiling model may be more effective than a determined/non-smiling model in health advertisements. However this link has not been shown. Nor has whether consumer BMI moderates the interaction effect of model BMI and facial expression on their intent to adopt healthy behaviours. Secondly, this project is employing a novel methodology which is effective in capturing objective, automatic and unconscious responses to health advertisement stimuli. Adopting this methodology allows us to investigate the mediating

Page 13: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

impact of participant attention on consumer self-reported attitudinal and behavioural responses, and their actual food consumption. This research is of interest to researchers in consumer psychology, dietetics and nutrition, advertising and health marketing.

A second impact pathway is social in nature. The rising rates of obesity and direct link between obesity and such diseases as diabetes, cancer and heart disease underlie the need for better understanding how consumers responds to health advertisement stimuli. The findings are relevant to public health campaigns, and offer recommendations in how to more effectively portray individuals in their health advertisements. Understanding how to present models in health advertisements can have a direct impact on (a) audience intent to make healthy choices, and therefore (b) the effective use of taxpayer funded public campaigns. Given the huge cost of such campaigns as the NHS Change4Life campaign, it is essential that advertising imagery is effective in stimulating health goals. The topical nature of this project means that the findings are also of interest to newspaper media, and every effort will be made to share them with news media in the UK.

Further research opportunities the project has highlighted

Feedback from our journal submission indicates there is scope to develop theoretical research on the link between visual attention and social comparison (specifically related to an individual’s body mass index) as well as visual attention and emotional contagion. There is of yet no research exploring these links and this provides a fruitful avenue for theoretical papers, as well as offering a strong grounding to explain our empirical findings. This is work that the three authors intent to carry out in the next academic year.

References and Bibliography – 2 pages maximum

Bavelas, Janet B., Alex Black, Charles R. Lemery and Jennifer Mullett (1986), "" I show how you feel": Motor mimicry as a communicative act," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 (2), 322.

Bellman, Steven, Anika Schweda and Duane Varan (2010), "The residual impact of avoided television advertising," Journal of Advertising, 39 (1), 67-82.

Boland, Wendy Attaya, Paul M. Connell and Beth Vallen (2013), "Time of day effects on the regulation of food consumption after activation of health goals," Appetite, 70, 47-52.

Brown, Stephen and Emma Locker (2009), "Defensive responses to an emotive anti-alcohol message," Psychology and Health, 24 (5), 517-528.

------ and Miles Richardson (2012), "The Effect of Distressing Imagery on Attention to and Persuasiveness of an Antialcohol Message A Gaze-Tracking Approach," Health Education & Behavior, 39 (1), 8-17.

Chandran, Sucharita and Geeta Menon (2004), "When a day means more than a year: Effects of temporal framing on judgments of health risk," Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 375-389.

Diedrichs, Phillippa C. and Christina Lee (2010), "GI joe or average joe? The impact of average-size and muscular male fashion models on men's and women's body image and advertisement effectiveness," Body Image, 7 (3), 218-226.

Duchenne, G. B. (1990). The mechanism of human facial expression or an electro-physiological analysis of the expression of emotions, R. Andrew Cuthbertson., ed. & trans., New York: Cambridge University Press, (Original work published 1862).

Ekman, Paul (1989), "The Argument and Evidence about Universals in Facial Expressions of Emotion," in Handbook of social psychophysiology, Wagner, Hugh Ed, and Antony Ed Manstead, John Wiley & Sons, 143-164.

Page 14: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Feiereisen, Stephanie, Veronica Wong and Amanda J. Broderick (2008), "Analogies and mental simulations in learning for really new products: The role of visual attention," Journal of Product Innovation Management, 25 (6), 593-607.

Festinger, Leon (1954), "A theory of social comparison processes," Human relations, 7 (2), 117-140.Frank, Mark G., Paul Ekman and Wallace V. Friesen (1993), "Behavioral markers and recognizability

of the smile of enjoyment," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64 (1), 83.Grabe, Shelly, L. Monique Ward and Janet Shibley Hyde (2008), "The role of the media in body image

concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies," Psychological bulletin, 134 (3), 460.

Hansen, Christine H., Ranald D. Hansen and David W. Shantz (1992), "Repression at encoding: discrete appraisals of emotional stimuli," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63 (6), 1026.

Harker, Debra, Michael Harker and Robert Burns (2007), "Tackling obesity: Developing a research agenda for advertising researchers," Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 29 (2), 39-51.

Harris, Jennifer L., John A. Bargh and Kelly D. Brownell (2009), "Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior," Health Psychology, 28 (4), 404.

Hatfield, Elaine, John T. Cacioppo and Richard L. Rapson (1994), Emotional contagion, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hollitt, Sarah, Eva Kemps, Marika Tiggemann, Elke Smeets and Jennifer S. Mills (2010), "Components of attentional bias for food cues among restrained eaters," Appetite, 54 (2), 309-313.

Hoover, Alison, Natalie Ceballos, Oleg Komogortsev and Reiko Graham (2010), "Effects of hunger and body mass index on attentional capture by high and low calorie food images: An eye-tracking study," Journal of Vision, 10 (7), 256-256.

Izard, Carroll E. (1977), Human emotions, New York: Plenum Press.Joshi, Ramona, C. Peter Herman and Janet Polivy (2004), "Self-enhancing effects of exposure to thin-

body images," International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35 (3), 333-341.Kees, Jeremy, Scot Burton and Andrea Heintz Tangari (2010), "The impact of regulatory focus,

temporal orientation, and fit on consumer responses to health-related advertising," Journal of Advertising, 39 (1), 19-34.

Keller, Punam Anand and Lauren G. Block (1997), "Vividness effects: A resource-matching perspective," Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (3), 295-304.

Kessels, Loes T.E., Robert A.C. Ruiter and Bernadette M. Jansma (2010), "Increased attention but more efficient disengagement: neuroscientific evidence for defensive processing of threatening health information," Health Psychology, 29 (4), 346.

Krishen, A. and Dominique Worthen (2011), "Body image dissatisfaction and self-esteem: A consumercentric exploration and proposed research agenda," Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction, and Complaining Behavior, 24, 90-106.

Krumhuber, Eva and Arvid Kappas (2005), "Moving smiles: The role of dynamic components for the perception of the genuineness of smiles," Journal of nonverbal behavior, 29 (1), 3-24.

Levin, Irwin P., Sandra L. Schneider and Gary J. Gaeth (1998), "All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76 (2), 149-188.

Lundqvist, Lars-Olov and Ulf Dimberg (1995), "Facial expressions are contagious," Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 203-211.

Maclnnis, Deborah J. and Bernard J. Jaworski (1989), "Information processing from advertisements: Toward an integrative framework," The Journal of Marketing, 53 (4), 1-23.

Martin, Mary C. and James W. Gentry (1997), "Stuck in the model trap: The effects of beautiful models in ads on female pre-adolescents and adolescents," Journal of Advertising, 26 (2), 19-33.

Page 15: Bertele for website...  · Web viewWe plan to carry out a third study based on feedback from our submission to the Journal of Advertising. We will either complete a third eye-tracking

Meyers-Levy, Joan and Prashant Malaviya (1999), "Consumers' processing of persuasive advertisements: An integrative framework of persuasion theories," The Journal of Marketing, 63 (special issue), 45-60.

National Cancer Institute (NCI) (2012) Obesity and Cancer Risk, available at: http://www.cancer.gov/cancertopics/factsheet/Risk/obesity (accessed August 25, 2014).

Ng, Marie, Tom Fleming, Margaret Robinson, Blake Thomson, Nicholas Graetz, Christopher Margono, Erin C. Mullany, Stan Biryukov, Cristiana Abbafati and Semaw Ferede Abera (2014), "Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013," The Lancet, 384 (9945), 766-781.

Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew F. Hayes (2008), "Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models," Behavior Research Methods, 40 (3), 879-891.

Schoenewolf, Gerald (1990), "Emotional contagion: Behavioral induction in individuals and groups," Modern Psychoanalysis, 15(1), 49-61.

Soussignan, Robert (2002), "Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and autonomic reactivity: a test of the facial feedback hypothesis," Emotion, 2 (1), 52-74.

Surakka, Veikko and Jari K. Hietanen (1998), "Facial and emotional reactions to Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles," International Journal of Psychophysiology, 29 (1), 23-33.

Tiggemann, Marika and Janet Polivy (2010), "Upward and downward: Social comparison processing of thin idealized media images," Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34 (3), 356-364.

Tourangeau, Roger and Phoebe C Ellsworth (1979), "The role of facial response in the experience of emotion," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (9), 1519-1531.

WHO/Europe (2008), Action plan for food and nutrition policy 2007-2012, Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.