Top Banner
This article was downloaded by: [University of Alabama at Tuscaloosa] On: 25 August 2014, At: 17:21 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Discourse Processes Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hdsp20 Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories Mina Lee a , Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen a & David R. Roskos-Ewoldsen a a Department of Psychology , University of Alabama Published online: 11 Dec 2008. To cite this article: Mina Lee , Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen & David R. Roskos-Ewoldsen (2008) Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories, Discourse Processes, 45:6, 519-544 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638530802359566 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
27

Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

May 13, 2023

Download

Documents

doug gibler
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

This article was downloaded by: [University of Alabama at Tuscaloosa]On: 25 August 2014, At: 17:21Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Discourse ProcessesPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hdsp20

Applying the Landscape Model to ComprehendingDiscourse From TV News StoriesMina Lee a , Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen a & David R. Roskos-Ewoldsen aa Department of Psychology , University of AlabamaPublished online: 11 Dec 2008.

To cite this article: Mina Lee , Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen & David R. Roskos-Ewoldsen (2008) Applying the Landscape Model toComprehending Discourse From TV News Stories, Discourse Processes, 45:6, 519-544

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638530802359566

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) containedin the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of theContent. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon andshould be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable forany losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use ofthe Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematicreproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in anyform to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

Discourse Processes, 45:519–544, 2008

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 0163-853X print/1532-6950 online

DOI: 10.1080/01638530802359566

Applying the Landscape Modelto Comprehending Discourse

From TV News Stories

Mina Lee, Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen, andDavid R. Roskos-Ewoldsen

Department of Psychology

University of Alabama

The Landscape Model of text comprehension was extended to the comprehension

of audiovisual discourse from text and video TV news stories. Concepts from the

story were coded for activation after each sequence, creating a matrix of activations

that was reduced to a vector of the degree of total activation for each concept.

In Study 1, the degree vector correlated well with participants’ ratings of how

much the sequence made them think of each concept. In Study 2, the degree

vector, vectors based on the number of activations, and the degree of co-activation

were used to predict participants’ recall. The model predicted recall for the text

version well, but only moderately well for the video version. The Landscape Model

was modified using Dual Code Theory by coding and analyzing audio and visual

information as separate components. It predicted students’ recall well, indicating

its robustness as a model of discourse processing.

Although most studies of discourse comprehension focus on the written or

spoken word, much of the discourse we process is visually based. Face-to-

face conversation involves gestures and facial expressions in addition to the

interlocutors’ shared visual scene; discourse on television or in movies typically

has visuals in the background or foreground, in addition to showing speakers’

gestures and facial expressions. In all these cases, the visual context can be

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen,

Department of Psychology, University of Alabama, 348 Gordon Palmer Hall, Box 870348,

Tuscaloosa, AL 35487-0348. E-mail: [email protected]

519

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 3: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

520 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

important for understanding the discourse. In this study, we examine comprehen-

sion of discourse from television news stories as a focused way of understanding

the relation between linguistic and visual processing more generally.

When people watch a television news story, one of their implicit goals is

to process and understand (i.e., comprehend) the story that they are watching,

much as it is when they read a newspaper story. As discourse research has

shown, comprehending a story involves integrating past knowledge and new

knowledge into a coherent mental model so that the story is meaningful to the

reader or viewer. Research on the reading and comprehension of a written text

has consistently shown that people construct mental models of what they are

reading (Albrecht & O’Brien, 1993; Bower & Morrow, 1990; Garnham, 1997;

Gyselinck & Tardieu, 1999; O’Brien & Albrecht, 1992; Radvansky & Zacks,

1991), as well as mental representations of situations and what is occurring

in those situations (Morrow, Greenspan, & Bower, 1987; Zwaan & Radvansky,

1998). Just as text comprehension relies on the construction of a mental model,

it makes sense that viewing visual stories also involves the construction of a

mental model.

What is known about the nature of such models and how they are constructed?

The focus of most prior research on mass media has not focused on processing,

although there has been some research on the effects of structural changes

in visual information on perception or comprehension of a story (e.g., edits,

panning and zooming, camera angles). For example, one particular area of focus

is an assessment of the effects of formal features of television programs on

processing information. Unlike a print story, a televised story benefits from

its presentational structure. Structural features such as quick scene shifts or

strange camera angles are defined as having perceptual salience as long as those

features attract immediate attention. The salient structural features of television

programs are hypothesized to keep viewers aroused (Singer, 1980). However,

salient structural features do not automatically lead to attention and further

comprehension. Some researchers argue that the comprehension process, rather

than salience, guides attention (Anderson & Lorch, 1983; Collins, 1983; Gunter,

1987; Schmitt, Anderson, & Collins, 1999)—that is, attention to a television

program is not a reactive response to salient elements of a story but is a

comprehension-driven process.

Another area of focus is in the literature on children’s television, where

the visual elements of the story have different effects on memory than the

verbal elements. Television visuals change how children recount a story they

saw, compared to reading or hearing the story. Specifically, children who saw

a story on TV described more visual elements of the story than children who

read or heard the same story (Beagles-Roos & Gat, 1983; Meringoff, 1980),

and they retold the story in a different order than did children who read the

story (Hoffner, Cantor, & Thorson, 1988). The visual presentation of stories

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 4: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 521

also results in more memory errors in children, such as intrusions of extraneous

information (Hayes, Kelly, & Mandel, 1986).

We argue that a fruitful way to think about visual media effects is to ap-

proach visual media as an extension of verbal discourse (Roskos-Ewoldsen,

Roskos-Ewoldsen, & Carpentier, 2002; Yang, Roskos-Ewoldsen, & Roskos-

Ewoldsen, 2004) and to use the research tools of discourse psychologists to

examine how people mentally represent or process discourse in media stories

(Roskos-Ewoldsen et al., 2002). In so doing, we follow in the tradition of a few

quantitative studies that have explored this issue. For example, in Livingstone’s

(1987, 1989) research on soap operas, participants who regularly viewed a soap

opera rated their agreement with statements about its story line, where the state-

ments represented different perspectives depending on the characters involved.

Livingstone (1987, 1989) found that viewers’ comprehension of a soap opera was

tied to their perceptions of the characters within the soap opera (see also Cohen,

2002). For example, characters were seen as varying along three dimensions:

moral–immoral, mature–immature, and traditional–modern. In another study,

Magliano, Dijkstra, and Zwaan (1996) explored whether sources of information

in a film—mise en scene (i.e., costumes, lighting, placement of actors and props

within a scene, etc.), montage (i.e., edits within a film), dialogue, and music—

in narrative films, such as Moonraker (Broccoli & Gilbert, 1979), influenced

whether and when viewers made predictive inferences. Generally, they found that

when these sources of information were available, viewers were more likely to

make accurate predictive inferences than when they were not available. Finally,

Magliano, Miller, and Zwaan (2001) explored the role of changes in different

dimensions within a feature-length movie on perceptions of events in the movie.

Included were changes in time, changes in where the action was occurring, and

changes in the location of characters. When there was a change along one or

more of these dimensions, participants were more likely to say that a new event

had begun.

In this study, we focus on whether models of discourse processing of text can

be extended to processing of discourse in visual media. Specifically, we tested

a well-known model of discourse processing, the Landscape Model (van den

Broek, Risden, Fletcher, & Thurlow, 1996; van den Broek, Risden & Husebye-

Hartmann, 1995; van den Broek, Young, Tzeng, & Linderholm, 1999). As with

the more general mental models framework, the Landscape Model is concerned

with how people generate a coherent understanding of a story. Specifically, it

focuses on how concepts within a story are activated in memory while people

read a text story. The model gets its name from the observation that various

concepts are activated by a story to varying degrees across time (i.e., across the

sentences of the story).

What is unique about the Landscape Model as a model of text comprehension

is that it focuses on coherence by looking at the relation between the online

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 5: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

522 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

processing of a story and the memorial representation of that story. By looking

at a participant’s memory for a text, the model takes advantage of the well-

established finding that greater levels of activation of a particular concept result

in greater memory for that concept. Thus, by using the theory’s predictions for

how active various concepts are in memory, one can test whether those concepts

are indeed more likely to be remembered.

In addition, the model recognizes several sources of activations and asso-

ciations in the comprehension process. It assumes that there are four general

sources of concept activation while attending to a story (van den Broek et al.,

1996; van den Broek et al., 1999). First, the immediate environment will activate

concepts in memory. Specifically, concepts within the current sentence (e.g.,

in a book) or a scene (e.g., in a movie) will be activated. Second, because

activation dissipates across time (Higgins, Bargh, & Lombardi, 1985), concepts

from the immediately preceding sentence or scene should still be activated,

albeit at a lower level of activation. Third, concepts from earlier in the story

may be reactivated when they are necessary for maintaining the coherence of the

story. Fourth, world knowledge that is necessary for understanding the story will

be activated. According to the model, the cognitive representation of the story

will reflect these four sources of activation. The model seems to capture one’s

mental representation because it predicts participants’ memory for text-based

stories very well (van den Broek & Gustafson, 1990).

APPLYING THE LANDSCAPE MODEL TO

TV NEWS STORIES

Our strategy was to follow the methods originally used to test the Landscape

Model. In these studies, the stories used to test the Landscape Model have tended

to be short; for example, in one study (van den Broek et al., 1996), the stimulus

story was 13 sentences long and included only 26 concepts. In branching out

to the visual realm, we similarly began relatively simply with a TV news story.

A TV news story has both a video version and a transcript that can serve as

a text-only version. The two different forms of the TV news story (i.e., video,

text) enable a direct comparison of text and video processing, as well as a com-

parison of the text version with previous research using text. However, there are

several complexities involved in preparing a TV news story for testing, including

determining the meaningful units of analysis and identifying the concepts.

To address these complexities, extensive discussion and piloting went into

establishing the structure of the video news story itself, from which a theoretical

landscape of activations was created. In addition, we ensured that the theoret-

ically driven model captured readers’ online processing of concepts in the TV

news story. However, the main thrust of the study was to investigate how well the

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 6: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 523

Landscape Model (i.e., the theoretically derived activations) predicted recall for

the video version of the TV news story. To preview, the model predicted the text

version very well, but predicted recall for the video version only moderately well.

We then modified the Landscape Model based on Dual Code Theory (Paivio,

1971, 1986, 1991; Sadoski & Paivio, 2001) so that verbal and visual components

of the story were treated separately, and this modification worked well.

Preparing the Stimulus

Video news story. The news story chosen for the study was an excerpt

from the TV show Daybreak, which appears on Cable News Network (CNN).

The story aired on April 4, 2002, and was about a robot exhibition that had

been held in Japan at that time. The story highlights Japan’s latest robotics

innovations, including robots that save lives by detecting land mines; robots

that mimic human expressions, language, and talents; and robots that serve a

therapeutic purpose by responding to human touch. The news clip was 2 min,

10 s long. The transcript of the story was obtained from CNN.

Constructing meaningful units of analysis. The first step in preparing

the stimulus was to decide on the meaningful units of analysis. With text, this is

straightforward because sentences or clauses typically serve as meaningful units.

With TV news, the task is trickier because a meaningful unit in the transcript

does not always correspond with a break in the visual elements of the news story.

For example, the first two sentences of the transcript are, “Move over Madonna?

Maybe not quite yet.” These would typically be treated as two different units in

a text-based story. However, it was impossible to create a clean break between

these two sentences in the video news story. Either the video version clipped the

sentencesshort so that theend ofonesentenceand thebeginning of thenext sentence

seemed cut off too abruptly, or they appeared in both clips, creating redundancy.

As a result, to determine meaningful units in the video version, we classified

units using a combination of sentence structure and edits, such as changes in

camera angle and changes in scene. For example, between the first two sentences

and the third sentence (i.e., “These new entertainment robots made by Sony

aren’t about to win a Grammy, but they are talented.”), there was a shift in

the camera shot from the news reporter reporting from a studio news set to the

robots in action at the exhibition, with the reporter’s voice-over. In this case, the

first two sentences were designated as the first sequence in the story, and the

third sentence was designated as the second sequence. Based on these criteria,

the three authors plus two other graduate students familiar with the Landscape

Model mutually agreed on 19 meaningful units. The same units were used for

the text version of the story because we were interested in a comparison of a

text version and a video version (Appendix A).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 7: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

524 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

Identifying concepts. The next step, following procedures for testing the

Landscape Model, was to identify the concepts that occurred in the story. Con-

cepts were defined as words or images that seemed important for understanding

the story. Identifying the concepts in this news story was made difficult by the

complexity of the story compared with single sentences. To begin the task, the

first author read the transcript, writing down all concepts that came to mind.

Then, the five researchers separately read the transcript while looking at the

original list of concepts; additional concepts were noted. Through subsequent

discussions among the authors, concepts were added, deleted, or combined.

Some concepts became word phrases rather than single words because the

single words did not seem to capture the information in the story and, more

important, if all single concepts had been included there would have been well

over 100 concepts. As an example, the sentences, “Aside from singing and

dancing, they can recognize human faces and names” and “This one recognizes

and follows different colored objects,” produced the compound concepts of

can sing/dance/entertain and can recognize objects, among others. The five

researchers agreed on 54 concepts for the text version (Appendix B).

For the video version, visual information, including background visuals, was

added to the list of concepts using the same procedures as for the text version.

Again, we collapsed single concepts into compound concepts. For example,

we used dog-like-robot rather than dog and robot separately. Including visual

information resulted in a longer list of 79 concepts, agreed on by all five

researchers (Appendix B).

Creating Theoretical Activation Matrices

Coding for degree of activation. Two graduate students, who were oth-

erwise not involved in the project, coded the activation levels of the concepts.

Each coder independently rated both the video and text versions of the story.

The text version appeared as 19 sequences of sentences, each on a separate

page. The video version was edited so that there was a 10 sec blank screen after

each sequence. This allowed the rater to pause the video cassette recorder for

the ratings that followed each sequence. No specific order of video and text was

specified.

After reading or watching each sequence, the coders rated each of the con-

cepts (54 for text and 79 for video) for activation using the 5-point rating scheme

introduced by van den Broek et al. (1996). Using this coding scheme, a concept

was assigned a 5 if it was either explicitly mentioned in the dialogue or text

or was visually salient in the scene. A concept was assigned a 4 if it aided in

creating a coherent understanding of the sequence or was causally related to what

was occurring. A concept that acted as an enabler was assigned a value of 3.

Enablers are concepts that enable actions to occur within a scene. For example,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 8: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 525

a stairway could be an enabler in the scene in which the robots demonstrate

that they are mobile. Finally, a concept that could be inferred from a scene or

dialog and text was assigned a value of 2. If a concept did not fit into any of the

aforementioned categories, it was assigned a 0. A 1 is not used in this coding

scheme to allow for dissipation of inferred concepts (dissipation is described

later). For the video version, coders had a separate page of concepts for each

sequence. For the text version, each coder received a packet with the sequences

and concepts to be rated. Specifically, the first sequence was on the first page,

followed by a page of the concepts to be rated. This was followed by a page

with the second sequence, which was followed by the list of concepts to be

rated, and so on. The coders were instructed not to return to a previous page

once they began to code the concepts.

Constructing activation matrices and vectors. An activation matrix was

constructed for each version of the story (video, text) and for each coder. The

matrix represented the amount of activation (i.e., rating) each concept received

after each sequence. Thus, the video version resulted in a 19 � 79 matrix of

activations, and the text version resulted in a 19 � 54 matrix for each coder.

These matrices were then modified because the Landscape Model assumes

that the activation of a concept will dissipate across subsequent sequences if

it is not reinstated. Thus, those concepts that were not reactivated in the next

sequence were assigned a value equal to one half of their value from the previous

segment. The activation levels of these concepts were again reduced to 0 during

the next sequence if they were not reactivated based on the story. If the concept

was reactivated in the next sequence, it received a rating based on the current

sequence; that is, the ratings from one sequence to the next were not additive.

Figures 1 and 2 show the activation matrices for the text and video versions of

the story, respectively.

The matrices were reduced to concept activation vectors by adding the ac-

tivation level of each concept across sequences, as dictated by the Landscape

Model of text comprehension (van den Broek et al., 1996). This resulted in a

1 � 54 vector for the text version and a 1 � 79 vector for the video version.

Again, there were two vectors per version—one for each coder.

Reliability of activation vectors. To assess the reliability of the activation

vectors, we focused on these activation vectors for the simple reason that they

are the primary predictors of story recall (see Study 2). The vectors for each

rater were correlated in both the text version and the video version to determine

an interrater reliability. For the text version, the correlation was r D :92 .p <

:01/I and for the video version, it was r D :76 .p < :01/: In addition, there

were no significant differences in overall ratings between the coders for each

of the versions. These high correlations and lack of mean differences provide

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 9: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

526 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

FIGURE 1 Concept activations for the text version of the news story (number of con-

cepts D 54).

FIGURE 2 Concept activations for the video version of the news story (number of con-

cepts D 79).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 10: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 527

evidence that the coding scheme was reliable; therefore, we concluded that the

activation vectors, and by inference the matrices, were reliable. Finding that the

text version afforded more reliable activation ratings than the video version is not

surprising because some people are more likely than others to notice background

visual information. As a consequence, however, the lower reliability of the video

version compared to the text version may result in lower predictive power of

the Landscape Model for predicting story recall.

Theoretical matrices and vectors of activations. The coders’ matrices

were averaged and then reduced to vectors for subsequent analyses. Specifi-

cally, their activations for each sequence-concept combination were averaged

separately for the text and video versions, and then collapsed to two vectors—

one for each version.

STUDY 1: ESTABLISHING EXTERNAL VALIDITY OFTHE THEORETICAL MATRIX OF ACTIVATION

The next step was to establish that there was a relation between the theoretical

activations, based on the matrices formed by the coders, and actual participants’

activations (i.e., empirical activations) for both the text and video versions of

the story. This relation is important for two reasons. First, it provides external

validity for the theoretical matrix. A strong correlation between the theoretical

matrices and the empirical matrices would indicate that the theoretical activa-

tion matrix is adequately capturing the participants’ mental models. Second, it

provides further evidence that the coding scheme, and therefore the activation

matrix, is reliable.

Method

Participants. Thirteen students were recruited from introductory mass com-

munication courses. They received either extra credit or credit toward a course

requirement for their participation. Seven of the students were assigned to the

text version of the story, and 6 were assigned to the video version. None of

the participants in this study reported having seen the news story previously. A

power analysis (Faul & Erdfelder, 1992) indicated that 6 to 7 participants would

be enough to detect a correlation of r D :76 (from the interrater reliability of the

video version), with alpha D .05 and power of around .80 (.76, .84). However,

the more important power analysis used the number of concepts as the unit of

analysis. In this case, 54 and 79 are enough units to detect an effect size as

small as r D :40: From this perspective, the critical question is whether 6 to 7

participants would be enough to establish reliable findings. It was, as seen later.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 11: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

528 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

Materials and procedure. The text and video versions of the story were

used. After each sequence, participants rated on an 11-point scale how much the

sentence or scene made them think of each of the concepts. The scale ranged

from 0 (not at all) to 10 (a lot), and was modeled after van den Broek et al.’s

(1996) scale. Participants in the video version received a packet of 19 concept

lists, 1 for each sequence. Participants in the text version received a packet with

sequences and concept lists interwoven, and were instructed not to return to a

previous page once they had begun rating the concepts. The procedure lasted no

longer than 1 hr.

Results and Discussion

Empirical activation matrices. The ratings for each sequence-concept com-

bination were averaged across participants. This resulted in two averaged acti-

vation matrices—one for the video version and the other for the text version. It

was not necessary to modify the empirical matrices to account for dissipation

because of the way the rating scale is designed—that is, if a person is thinking

less about a concept on its nC1 sequence, he or she will assign it a lower value.

Reliability of empirical matrices. The matrices generated by each partici-

pant were reduced to concept activation vectors by adding the activation level of

each concept across sequences. Activation matrices were used because they are

the primary predictors of recall (Study 2). To assess the internal reliability of the

matrices, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for both the text and video versions,

with the concepts as the cases and the participants as the items. Reliability was

calculated in this way because analyzing simple correlations (and means) for 6

to 7 students was unwieldy. For the text version, Cronbach’s alpha was .89; and

for the video version, it was .83. These results indicate high internal reliability

for the two matrices, and shows that 6 to 7 participants were enough to obtain

stable data.

Relation between theoretical and empirical activation matrices. To

analyze the relation between the theoretical and empirical activation matrices,

the participants’ matrices were averaged and then reduced to vectors by adding

across sequences. Specifically, their ratings for each sequence-concept com-

bination were averaged separately for the text and video versions, and then

collapsed to two vectors—one for each version. Next, the theoretical vectors

were correlated with the empirically derived vectors. For the text version, the

correlation was r D :66 .p < :01/I and for the video version, it was r D :70

.p < :01/:

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 12: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 529

Discussion. These are strong correlations, and they indicate that the the-

oretical activation matrices based on the coders’ ratings captured the empirical

activation matrices based on participants’ thoughts about the concepts. In other

words, the theoretical activation matrices have external validity. This is important

because the theoretical model is being used to predict participants’ recall of the

video news story, and a strong relation between the theoretical and empirical

models bodes well for this task.

STUDY 2: PREDICTING RECALL

In this main study, the theoretical activation matrices are used to predict free

recall of the news story, thus determining how well the Landscape Model of

text comprehension applies to a story that has substantial visual components.

As a comparison, a study using the Landscape Model to predict free recall for

a text story found that 64% of the variance in recall was accounted for by the

theoretical activation matrix (van den Broek et al., 1996). This level is used as a

guide to judge whether the Landscape Model is applicable to TV news stories.

Three attributes of the activated concepts are used by the Landscape Model

to predict memory for the news story: the number of times a concept is acti-

vated, the degree to which a concept is activated, and the associations formed

between simultaneously activated concepts. According to the model, each of

these attributes reflects a unique aspect of the mental representation of a story.

First, the number of times a concept is activated within a story is expected to

influence recall of that concept because a concept that is activated fewer times

within the story will result in lower memory for that concept than when the

concept is activated more times in the story. Second, the degree to which a

concept is activated across all the scenes of the story is expected to influence

recall of that concept because concepts with more overall activation are more

likely to be recalled than concepts with lower levels of overall activation. Finally,

associations between concepts that are activated simultaneously should influence

the likelihood that a concept is recalled. This is because two concepts that are

activated simultaneously will be linked in memory; it is assumed that their

activation will have an effect such that the concepts that have more linkages in

memory are more likely to be recalled than those concepts with fewer linkages

in memory.

Method

Participants. Thirteen students who had not participated in Study 1 were

recruited from introductory mass communication courses. They received either

extra credit or credit toward a course requirement for their participation. Seven

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 13: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

530 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

of the students were assigned to the text version of the story, and 6 were assigned

to the video version. None of the participants in Study 2 reported having seen

the news story previously.

Materials and procedure. Participants watched the unedited version of the

video news story or read the unedited version of the text presented in transcript

form. Next, they completed an unrelated task for 10 min to avoid memory

recency effects. Finally, participants wrote down as much of the story as they

could recall. They were not told before reading or viewing the story that they

would have to recall the story. They were given as much time as they needed

during recall. The entire procedure lasted 30 min.

Results and Discussion

Recall data. Overall, participants recalled a low number of concepts from

the text-based version of the story .M D 8:00 out of 54 concepts, SD D 3.51)

than from the video version of the story .M D 12:67 out of 79 concepts, SD D

3.56), t.11/ D 2:37; p D :04: Although it appears that the research participants

had better memory for the video version of the story, there were more concepts

in this version. When the proportion of possible items was analyzed, there was

no difference in recall between the text version .M D 0:15; SD D 0.65) and

the video version of the story .M D 0:16; SD D 0.45), t.11/ D :03; p D :98:

Creating the recall vector. Two new coders not otherwise involved in

the project coded the recall data. Each coder coded each participant’s written

response for the number of times each of the concepts (54 concepts for text, 79

concepts for video) was included in the response. Thus, each participant had a

vector of the number of times each concept was recalled. After discussion, the

coders achieved 100% agreement for both the text and video versions of the

story for each participant.

To create the set of recall vectors, the number of times each concept was

recalled was added across all participants, resulting in two recall vectors—one

for the text version and one for the video version.

Creating three types of activation vectors. Three sets of theoretical

activation vectors were formed using the procedures by van den Broek et al.

(1996). Each set contained a vector for the text version and a vector for the

video version. The first set of vectors comprised the theoretical activation matrix

described earlier. This vector set is called the degree of activation vector set. The

other two sets of vectors used these activation matrices as a base. For one set,

activation was recoded in an all-or-none fashion based on whether a concept

received any activation during each of the 19 sequences. A concept received

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 14: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 531

a 1 if there was any activation and a 0 if there was none. These 1s and 0s

were summed across sequences, resulting in a number of activations vector set

representing the total number of times each concept was activated. The final set

of vectors measured how much co-activation occurred among the concepts. This

co-activation represents a particular concept’s associative strength with other

concepts. To calculate the amount of co-activation between a given concept and

all other concepts, the following formula was used, based on van den Broek

et al. (1996):

S.x; y/ D

IX

iD1

YX

yD1

Axi Ayi

S.x; y/ is the strength of co-activation between a concept x and all other

concepts y; i represents the sequence .I D 19/; Ax is the activation of concept

x, and Ay represents the activations of each of the other concepts y .Y D 54�1

for text and Y D 79 � 1 for video). In other words, for a given concept, one

begins with the first sequence and multiplies the activation of the concept with

each of the other concepts’ activations from that sequence separately, and then

sums the products. Once this is completed, the co-activations are summed across

sequences. This two-step procedure is repeated for each of the concepts. The

resulting vector set is called the association vector set.

Correlations among the activation vectors. Correlations among these

vectors were high. For the text version, the correlations were r D :83 (degree,

number), r D :96 (degree, association), and r D :82 (number, association): all

ps < .01, N D 54: For the video version, they were r D :85 (degree, number),

r D :99 (degree, association), and r D :86 (number, association): all ps < .01,

N D 79:

Predicting recall. The following analyses are a direct test of the model’s

hypothesis that the activation of a concept while watching the news story con-

tributes to the formation of a stable memorial representation of the story, which

can be recalled at a later time (van den Broek et al., 1996). Two regression

equations were computed—one for the text version and one for the video

version. In both cases, the recall vector was the criterion variable; and the degree,

number, and association vectors were treated as the predictor variables. All three

were entered into the analysis simultaneously because of the multicollinearity of

the vectors (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). Multicollinearity influences

the interpretation of individual betas such that the relative influence of each

component cannot be determined. However, this is not perceived to be a problem

because the focus of the study is how well the Landscape Model predicts recall

for the story and not the relative influence of the components of the model.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 15: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

532 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

For the text version, the model predicted recall well .R2 D :86; p <

:01/; F.3; 50/ D 98:61; p < :001: The beta weights for degree, number, and

association were 1.47, �0.57, and �0.14, respectively.1 Caution should be used

in interpreting these beta weights because of the multicollinearity. Consistent

with past research, the Landscape Model does an excellent job of predicting

participants’ memory for a text-based story. For the video version, the model

did not fare as well, although it still predicted 32% of the variance in recall

.R2 D :32; p < :01/; F.3; 75/ D 11:81; p < :001:2 Beta weights for degree,

number, and association were 1.61, �0.43, and �0.71, respectively. Because of

the multicollinearity among the predictor variables here and elsewhere, caution

must be used when interpreting these weights.

Although the Landscape Model does an acceptable, reliable job of predicting

participants’ memory for the TV news story, its predictive ability is much higher

for the text story than for the video story. This result raises the question of

whether the Landscape Model can be revised to predict the video recall data

better.

Before modifying the model, however, one can ask whether it is simply

misspecifying the visual components. Perhaps if only the concepts that the two

versions had in common were tested, the Landscape Model would predict recall

equally well. To test this, we used only the 54 concepts that both the text

and the video versions had in common. In many respects, these versions were

very similar. First, although the video version had higher overall activation than

the text version in degree, number, and association (all ps < .05), the degree,

number, and association vectors for the two versions were correlated: rs = .63,

.62, and .63, respectively; all ps < .001. Second, in terms of the average number

of times each concept was recalled, the correlation between the two versions

was high .r D :93; p < :001/: Further, the means were statistically equivalent:

text, M D 0:24; SD D 0.56; and video, M D 0:32; SD D 0.71; t.53/ D

1:91; p D :06; although there is a trend for higher recall in the video version

than in the text version. Regression analyses revealed a different story. A new

regression analysis for the reduced 54-concept video version produced R2 D :45;

1Another way to analyze the data is to conduct a forward stepwise regression, which adds the

variable that accounts for the most variance first; and if another accounts for additional variance, it

is added next. In this analysis, degree predicts R2D :75 of the variance .ˇ D 1:35/; F.1; 52/ D

156:72; p < :001I and number accounts for another R2D :10 .ˇ D �0:58/; Fchange.1; 51/ D

35:89; p < :001—for a total of R2D :85; F.2; 51/ D 148:88; p < :001: Association did not

account for any additional amount of variance.2A forward stepwise regression analysis shows that degree predicts R2

D :27 of the variance

.ˇ D 0:91/; F.1; 77/ D 29:01; p < :001I and number accounts for another R2D :04 .ˇ D �0:44/;

Fchange.1; 76/ D 4:78; p < :03—for a total of R2D :32; F.2; 76/ D 17:61; p < :001: Association

did not account for any additional amount of variance.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 16: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 533

F.3; 50/ D 13:42; p < :001—higher than for the 79-concept video version but

still far below that for the text version .R2 D :86/: The beta weights for degree,

number, and association were 0.89, �0.46, and 0.16, respectively. Therefore,

although recall for the concepts that both versions had in common was the same

and the theoretical models were similar, the model for the video story still did

not predict recall as well as the model for the text story. In fact, when the model

for the text version is used to predict recall for the video version, the model

predicts recall quite well, R2 D :83; F.3; 50/ D 81:40; p < :001 (ˇs D 1.57,

�0.51, and �0.30, respectively).

One could argue that we should stop here. However, this would miss the

critical point that online (i.e., sequence by sequence) processing of video news

stories does not capture comprehension of the video story nearly as well as

online processing of text news stories does. It is important to understand what

was being processed while participants watched the video and why the model

did not predict their recall well. Further, many videos rely more heavily on

visual information than does a news story. In these cases, meaning often results

mostly from the visuals, and coding text only would not capture that. Therefore,

we pursued the question of how the online processing of video news stories

differed from the online processing of text news stories, even when the concepts

were identical. We believe that the answer derives from the basic differences in

processing text and pictures or images or, more specifically, representing verbal

and visual information in memory (e.g., Kosslyn, 1994; Paivio, 1986; Palmer,

1999; see also Baggett, 1989; Farah, 1989; Levie, 1987; Moliter, Ballstaedt, &

Mandl, 1989).

A primary theory that focuses on the independence of visual and verbal

information is Dual Code Theory (Paivio, 1971, 1986, 1991; Sadoski & Paivio,

2001). According to Dual Code Theory, there are two types of representa-

tions in memory—one verbal and the other nonverbal. Within each type of

representation, concepts are represented as nodes (i.e., logogens in the verbal

system and imagens in the nonverbal system) and are connected to each other

in an associative structure that differs for verbal and nonverbal representations.

Further, verbal and nonverbal representations are independent of each other,

and the activations of concepts that are dually coded are additive. For example,

assume that the visual element of the story involves a robot going up some stairs

and that there is a reporter voice-over saying, “Robots can be taught to climb

stairs.” The activations of the concept “robot” from the verbal and nonverbal

representations would be added together to create a higher level of activation

of the concept “robot” than either the verbal element or the nonverbal element

of the story alone. Conversely, activation of the concept “reporter” would not

involve adding activation from both the verbal and nonverbal codes because the

reporter is only heard during this segment. Its activation would derive only from

the verbal system. As a result, dually coded information is predicted to have

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 17: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

534 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

higher levels of overall activation that, in turn, leads to better memory for the

information.

In our study, it could be the case that recall participants in the video group

comprehended and remembered only the verbal aspects of the news story, and

that is why their recall matched the text group so well. In this case, a verbal-

only coding of the 79 concepts would suffice for determining recall for the

video version. On the other hand, the recall participants in the video group may

have comprehended and remembered both visual and verbal aspects of the news

story, but the verbal aspects of the news story outweighed its visual aspects;

this possibility cannot be tested with the current data because the expert coders

rated the visual and verbal aspects of the news story simultaneously. A third, but

unlikely, possibility is that the visual components of the news story outweighed

the verbal information in terms of predicting recall. Perhaps the reason that the

text version of the Landscape Model predicted the 54 concepts so well is because

these concepts had strong verbal components in the news story. However, the text

version cannot capture the concepts that are only visual, and maybe a visual-only

model would predict recall for the entire 79 concepts better than a verbal-only

model.

To test these possibilities, it is necessary to code the verbal aspects of the

news story separately from the visual aspects. In doing so, we incorporated

two key aspects of Dual Code Theory. First, to capture the separate nature of

verbal and nonverbal representations, we coded activation for visual information

(visual only) separately from verbal information, which in our case is audio

in nature (audio only). Second, reflecting the additive nature of activation in

the two representational systems, we used each type of activation as separate

predictors in a new regression equation, using the video recall data described

earlier as the criterion variable.

There were two other aspects of Dual Code Theory we did not include to keep

the analysis as simple as possible, at least to begin with. First, we decided not

to include qualitatively different kinds of associations for verbal and nonverbal

information. Instead, co-activations for verbal and nonverbal information were

calculated in the same manner. Second, we chose to include only co-activations

within a type of representation (i.e., associations within the verbal and nonverbal

systems), rather than including co-activations across types of representations

(i.e., referential connections).

Creating audio-only and visual-only activation vectors. The coders from

Study 1 recoded the video news story to obtain the separate visual-only and

audio-only activation matrices for the news story. For the visual-only version,

coders watched a sequence of the news story with the volume off, paused

the news story during the blank screen at the end of the sequence, and then

coded how much each of the 79 concepts was activated by the visual images in

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 18: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 535

that sequence. This was repeated for each of the 19 sequences. For the audio-

only version, coders listened to a sequence in the audio story, with the visual

information blocked. They paused the story at the end of each sequence, and

then coded how much each of the 79 concepts was activated by the sound portion

of each segment. There was no order specified for coding the visual and audio

information. There was a lag of around 1 month between the original coding

and the present coding.

Two sets of activation matrices were created—one for the audio-only version

of the news story and the other for the visual-only version. Within each set

were the two coders’ activation matrices. The coders’ activation matrices were

modified for the dissipation of activation over time and then averaged. This

resulted in a single degree of activation matrix for the audio-only version and

another for the visual-only version. These matrices were each reduced to vectors

by summing the activations across sequences.

The activation matrices and, consequently, the activation vectors showed

different configurations of activation (Figures 3 and 4, audio only and visual

only, respectively). A correlation between the visual-only and audio-only de-

gree vectors was reliable but moderate in strength—r D :36; p D :001—

corroborating the differences between audio and visual online processing seen

in Figure 2. For the number and association vectors, the correlations between

the visual-only and audio-only versions were r D :07 .p D :27/ and r D :32

.p D :002/; respectively.

Predicting recall. The participants’ recall vector from the video version

of the news story was used as the criterion variable, and the six activation

vectors were used as the predictor variables in a regression analysis. As in

the original analysis, the six predictor variables were entered into the analysis

simultaneously because of the multicollinearity of some of the vectors, and

interpreting individual beta weights for each variable requires caution. The new

model predicted recall very well: R2 D :73; F.6; 72/ D 32:51; p < :001:3 The

beta weights for all predictor variables except visual-only number and audio-

only association were reliably different than zero (audio only: 0.87, �0.26, and

0.04; visual only: 2.72, 0.06, and �2.52; all ps < .04). The new model was

on par with a previous test of the Landscape Model for recall of text material

.R2 D :64I van den Broek et al., 1996), and accounted for much more of the

variance in recall of the video version of the news story than the original model

3A forward stepwise regression analysis shows that audio-only degree predicts 64% of the

variance, F.1; 77/ D 138:36; p < :001I and visual-only degree accounts for another 4%,

�F.1; 76/ D 10:77;p D :002: Visual-only association added another 2.2%, �F.1; 75/ D 5:79;

p D :02I and audio-only number added 1.9%, �F.1; 74/ D 5:27; p D :02: Visual-only number

and audio-only association did not account for any additional amount of variance. For the total

model, F.4; 74/ D 49:67; p < :001:

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 19: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

536 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

FIGURE 3 Concept activations based on audio-only information of the video news story

(number of concepts D 79).

FIGURE 4 Concept activations based on visual-only information of the news story

(number of concepts D 79).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 20: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 537

.R2 D :32/: In comparison, the original model accounted for R2 D :86 of the

variance in the text version of the news story.

To test the three possibilities described earlier, we performed three additional

regression analyses. First, we used the audio-only vectors to predict recall. In

this case, R2 D :66; F.3; 75/ D 48:87; p < :001 (ˇs D 1.10, �0.26, and

�0.08 for degree, number, and association vectors, respectively). For the second

possibility, we entered the audio-only vectors into the equation first, followed

by the visual-only vectors. The audio-only vectors produced the same results

as the first hypothesis, of course; but the visual-only vectors added reliably

to the variance accounted for, �R2 D :07; �F.3; 72/ D 6:13; p D :001:

For the overall model, R2 D :73; F.6; 72/ D 32:51; p < :001 (audio-only

ˇs D 0.87, �0.26, and 0.04; visual-only ˇs D 2.72, 0.06, and �2.52). For the

third hypothesis, we used the visual-only vectors to predict recall: R2 D :33;

F.3; 75/ D 12:18; p < :001 (ˇs D 5.20, �0.04, and �4.71). It appears that

participants’ memorial representation primarily reflected the verbal information

in the video. Their memorial representation also reflected the visual information,

but this information was overwhelmed by the verbal information.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

The results of these studies suggest that the mental models approach, and the

Landscape Model in particular, provide a viable way to study how viewers

process and comprehend video stories like TV news stories. However, video

stories, unlike text stories, are harder to study using the Landscape Model,

in part because there are no simple units to study. Unlike a written or auditory

story that is told in sentences, video stories often have no such easily identifiable

units of meaning. Instead, structural features, such as camera angles and scene

changes, need to be incorporated when identifying the units of meaning in the

story. However, a change in camera angle does not always signal the end of one

unit and the beginning of the next. For example, a dialog between two people

may have several changes in camera angle and yet one would not call each

camera angle a unit of information (Lang, Bradley, Park, Shin, & Chung, 2006).

However, these difficulties can be overcome, at least with TV news stories.

In the two studies presented here, we investigated how well models of text

comprehension could be extended to comprehension of media stories that contain

both visual and verbal components—in particular, TV news stories. Empirical

findings have been accumulating in the mass media literature on factors that

affect comprehension of and memory for print news stories including visual

presentation styles (Gunter, 1987; Gunter, Furnham, & Griffiths, 2000), prior

knowledge of topics (Robinson & Davis, 1986), and gender and age (Robinson

& Levy, 1986). However, there has been very little research on how people build

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 21: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

538 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

mental models of a visual media story (Roskos-Ewoldsen et al., 2002). The

Landscape Model, as an example of the broader category of mental models, has

been used successfully to capture the online processing of text, thus predicting

comprehension of and subsequent recall for text news stories (van den Broek

et al., 1996, 1999).

In these studies, we adapted the Landscape Model for use with TV news

stories to test its robustness as a model for comprehension of a video story. We

created theoretically based landscapes of activation for text and video versions

of a TV news story, based on the Landscape Model of text comprehension. The

theoretically driven landscapes were found to capture the moment-to-moment

thoughts of participants as they read or watched the TV story (Study 1). In

Study 2 we used the theoretically based landscapes to predict recall of the

text and video versions of the TV story. For the text version, the landscape

of activation accounted for an impressive 86% of the variance in participants’

recall. However, for the video version of the story, the landscape accounted

for only 32% of the variance in recall. Although 32% is still remarkable, it

is clearly not as impressive as is the variance in recall for text stories (86%

here, 64% in van den Broek et al., 1996). The discrepancy between the text and

video versions suggested that comprehension of TV news stories differs from

the comprehension of text stories.

The question is, how are they different? Our results indicated that our par-

ticipants paid most attention to the verbal aspects of the news story. To the

extent that our participants are typical, this suggests that the visual aspects

of a TV news story are represented in memory but are overwhelmed by the

verbal representations. However, these representations are not identical. This

is supported by the findings when our expert coders coded the verbal (i.e.,

audio) information separately from the visual information, and each kind of

information was treated as a separate variable in predicting recall for the video

story. These independent ratings implemented Dual Code Theory, which is a

theory of memory positing that there are two different representational codes in

memory—one for verbal information and one for nonverbal information (Paivio,

1986, 1991; Sadoski & Paivio, 2001). When visual and verbal information were

treated separately, they accounted for 73% of the variance in recall. This result is

on par with a previous study of text comprehension (van den Broek et al., 1996).

A follow-up question is why the original theoretical coding of the video did

not predict recall of the video story. The answer appears to be that the expert

coders were paying more attention to the visual aspects of the video story than

those who were recalling the video story. In our view, this discrepancy reflects

a bottleneck in the experts’ coding of activations, rather than a bottleneck in

the activations themselves. The discrepancy is similar to the discrepancy in

investigations of iconic memory (Sperling, 1960). When participants were asked

to recall all of the alphanumeric characters within a very briefly presented matrix

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 22: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 539

of characters (whole report method), recall was poor (25%). However, when

participants were cued to report a specific part of the matrix (partial report

method), recall was much higher (75%). Regarding our results, it was likely

difficult for our coders to report immediately both visual and auditory activations

simultaneously (whole report); but when they could focus on a particular type

of information (partial report), their coding adequately captured activations.

Participants who watched the news story and later recalled the story were able

to consolidate both sources of information into longer term memories in both

the verbal and nonverbal systems, resulting in the ability to recall both verbal

and nonverbal information, albeit to different extents (i.e., audio only captured

66% of the variance in recall, whereas visual only added an additional 7% of

the variance). Of course, this answer is speculative, and more research is needed

to test it.

Although the verbal and visual online processing of the video story we used

predicts recall for the video story well, TV news stories are similar in many ways

to text stories and to discourse found in textbooks. Discourse in TV news stories,

like text discourse, has an introduction and then a series of facts connected

by transitions, followed by a conclusion. There is little inferencing occurring

other than to which object a pronoun refers (e.g., “it” refers to “the robot”). In

particular, there is little reason to predict future events or news stories (forward

inferencing) and almost no reason to use current information to understand

previous news stories (backward inferencing). As a result, perhaps it should not

be surprising that a combination of verbal and visual online processing was able

to predict recall well.

However, it remains to be seen how well the online processing suggested

by the Landscape Model predicts more complex video stories, such as TV

dramas or movies. In these situations, there is reason to predict future events

or reinterpret past events. Thus, inferencing, both forward and backward is

much more important. Further, not only is a story (or several stories) told

within a single episode of a TV series, and a single movie within a genre

of movies, but stories cross episodes or movies within a genre—that is, there

is intertextual discourse that needs keeping track of, in addition to intratextual

discourse (Roskos-Ewoldsen, Roskos-Ewoldsen, Yang, & Lee, 2007; see also

Eco, 1990, 1992). In addition, many dramas have emotional content that is not

obviously incorporated into the Landscape Model. For some episodes or series,

this may be very important for predicting recall; for others, it may not be.

It may be that that people process different genres of stories differently (e.g.,

Zwaan, 1994); or, it may be that movies require a higher level of organization

than simple unit-by-unit activations, such as clustering activations into events,

to capture recall. We suspect that the event indexing model is a good place to

start identifying these higher order events (Zwaan, Langston, & Graesser, 1995;

Zwaan, Radvansky, Hilliard, & Curiel, 1998).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 23: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

540 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

REFERENCES

Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and

global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19,

1061–1070.

Anderson, D. R., & Lorch, E. P. (1983). Looking at television: Action or reaction? In J. Bryant & D.

Anderson (Eds.), Children’ understanding of television: Research on attention and comprehension

(pp. 1–34). New York: Academic.

Baggett, P. (1989). Understanding visual and verbal messages. In H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.),

Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures (pp. 101–124), Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Beagles-Roos, J., & Gat, I. (1983). Specific impact of radio and television on children’s story

comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 128–137.

Bower, G. H., & Morrow, D. G. (1990). Mental models in narrative comprehension. Science, 247,

44–48.

Broccoli, A. R. (Producer), & Gilbert, L. (Director). (1979). Moonraker. [Videotape]. London, UK:

Eon Productions Ltd./Les Artistes Associes. [Distributed by MBM/UA Home Video].

Cohen, J. (2002). Deconstructing Ally: Exploring viewers’ interpretations of population television.

Media Psychology, 4, 253–277.

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression: Correlation

analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Collins, W. A. (1983). Cognitive processing in television viewing. In E. Wartella & D. C. Whitney

(Vol. Eds.), Mass communication review yearbook (pp. 195–209). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Eco, U. (1990). The limits of interpretation. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Eco, U. (1992). Interpretation and overinterpretation/Umberto Eco with Richard Rorty, Jonathan

Culler, Christine Brooke-Rose; edited by Stefan Collini. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Farah, M. J. (1989). Knowledge from text and pictures: A neuropsychological perspective. In

H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures (pp. 59–71).

Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). G Power: A Priori Post–Hoc, and Compromise Power Analysis

for MS–DOS (Version 2.0) [Computer program]. Bonn, FRG: Bonn University, Department of

Psychology.

Garnham, A. (1997). Representing information in mental models. In C. Martin (Ed.), Cognitive

models of memory (pp. 149–172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gunter, B. (1987). Poor reception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Gunter, B., Furnham, A., & Griffiths, S. (2000). Children’s memory for news: A comparison of

three presentation media. Media Psychology, 2, 93–118.

Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1999). The role of illustrations in text comprehension: What, when,

for whom, and why? In S. R. Goldman & H. van Oostendorp (Eds.), The construction of mental

representations during reading (pp. 195–218). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Hayes, D. S., Kelly, S. B., & Mandel, M. (1986). Media differences in children’s story synopses:

Radio and television contrasted. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 341–346.

Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. L. (1985). Nature of priming effects on categorization.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 59–69.

Hoffner, C., Cantor, J., & Thorson, E. (1988). Children’s understanding of a televised narrative:

Developmental differences in processing video and audio content. Communication Research, 15,

227–245.

Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.

Lang, A., Bradley, S. D., Park, B., Shin, M., & Chung, Y. (2006). Parsing the resource pie: Using

STRTs to measure attention to mediated messages. Media Psychology, 8, 369–394.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 24: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 541

Levie, W. H. (1987). Research on pictures: A guide to the literature. In D. M. Willows & H. A.

Houghton (Eds.), The psychology of illustration (Vol. 1, pp. 1–50). New York: Springer.

Livingstone, S. M. (1987). The implicit representation of characters in “Dallas”: A multidimensional

scaling approach. Human Communication Research, 13, 399–420.

Livingstone, S. M. (1989). Interpretive viewers and structured programs. Communication Research,

16, 25–57.

Magliano, J. P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A. (1996). Generating predictive inferences while viewing

a movie. Discourse Processes, 22, 199–224.

Magliano, J. P., Miller, J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2001). Indexing space and time in film understanding.

Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 533–545.

Meringoff, L. K. (1980). Influence of the medium on children’s story apprehension. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 72, 240–249.

Moliter, S., Ballstaedt, S., & Mandl, H. (1989). Problems in knowledge acquisition from text and

pictures. In H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures (pp. 3–

35). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models during

narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165–187.

O’Brien, E. J., & Albrecht J. E. (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental

model. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, 18, 777–784.

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. Oxford, England: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University

Press.

Paivio, A. (1991). Images in mind: The evolution of a theory. Hertfordshire, England: Harvester

Wheatsheaf.

Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT

Press.

Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, R. T. (1991). Mental models and the fan effect. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 940–953.

Robinson, J. P., & Davis, D. K. (1986). Comprehension of a single evening’s news. In J. P. Robinson

& M. R. Levy (Eds.), The main source: Learning from television news (pp. 107–132). Beverly

Hills, CA: Sage.

Robinson, J. P., & Levy, M. R. (1986). Comprehension of a week’s news. In J. P. Robinson & M. R.

Levy (Eds.), The main source: Learning from television news (pp. 87–105). Beverly Hills, CA:

Sage.

Roskos-Ewoldsen, D., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Carpentier, F. (2002). Media priming: A synthesis.

In J. B. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.) Media effects in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 97–120).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., Yang, M., & Lee, M. (2007). Comprehension of

the Media. In D. R. Roskos-Ewoldsen and J. L. Monahan (Eds.), Communication and social

cognition: Theory and methods (pp. 319–348). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Schmitt, K. L., Anderson, D. R., & Collins, P. A. (1999). Form and content: Looking at visual

features of television. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1156–1167.

Singer, J. (1980). The power and limitations of television: A cognitive-affective analysis. In P.

Tannenbaum (Ed.), The entertainment functions of television (pp. 31–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Mono-

graphs, 74 (Issue X, Whole No. 498).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 25: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

542 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

van den Broek, P., & Gustafson, M. (1990). Comprehension and memory for texts: Three generations

of reading research. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative com-

prehension, causality, and coherence (pp. 15–34). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,

Inc.

van den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C., & Thurlow, R. (1996). A “landscape” view of reading:

Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In B.

Britton & A. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 165–188). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartmann, E. (1995). The role of readers’ standards for

coherence in the generation of inferences during reading. In R. R. Lorch & E. J. O’Brien (Eds.),

Sources of coherence reading (pp. 353–373). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The Landscape Model of reading:

Inferences and the online construction of a memory representation. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R.

Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Yang, M., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. (2004). Implications of the Landscape

Model of text memory for brand placement. In L. J. Shrum (Ed.), The psychology of entertainment

media: Blurring the lines between entertainment and persuasion (pp. 79–98). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Zwaan, R. A. (1994). Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 920–933.

Zwaan, R. A., Langston, M. C., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). The construction of situation models in

narrative comprehension: An event-indexing model. Psychological Science, 6, 292–297.

Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory.

Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.

Zwaan, R. A., Radvansky, G. A., Hilliard, A. E., & Curiel, J. M. (1998). Constructing multidimen-

sional situation models during reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 199–220.

APPENDIX A

Nineteen Sequences of the News Clip

1. Move over Madonna? Maybe not quite yet.

2. These new entertainment robots made by Sony aren’t about to win a

Grammy, but they are talented.

3. Aside from singing and dancing, they can recognize human faces and

names.

4. This is ASIMO. Honda’s latest pride and joy.

5. It moves with greater flexibility and spontaneity than any other robot on

earth.

6. It also learns. One museum already uses it as a tour guide.

7. These robots are part of Japan’s second ever robot expo, Robodex.

8. Its goal: To show off Japan’s latest robotics innovations and encourage

engineers to share ideas.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 26: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

LANDSCAPE MODEL 543

9. Some robots now under development in Japan are meant to save lives.

This one detects land mines.

10. Still others mimic humans, this one with facial expressions.

11. This one recognizes and follows different colored objects.

12. Yet another has been taught to play the flute with special sensitivity in

its robotic lips, tongue and fingertips.

13. Many speak at least some Japanese.

14. “Konichiwa.” “Konichiwa.”

15. Robots may not yet take over our jobs—at least in our lifetime—but they

are likely to enhance our lives both at home and at work in ways we

still can’t predict. And Japan wants to be at the forefront of the robot

revolution.

16. A healing robot made to look like a baby seal is already used for therapy

with senior citizens and in pediatric wards in Japan.

17. It responds to human touch. Like a real pet, it has been clinically proven

to reduce stress without the hygiene issues of real animals.

18. Robot expo organizers hope to inspire a new generation of robot inven-

tors.

19. “I’d like to create a robot that can communicate with human beings in

a warm way,” says this university student.” “I think robots will change

human culture. It will be like coexisting with beings from another planet.”

APPENDIX B

Concept List

Alien-Beings

ASIMO

Can-reduce-stress

Can-dance/sing/entertain

Can-not-predict-ways

Can-recognize-objects

Can-recognize-human-face/name

Clinically-proven

Coexist-with humans

Communicate-with-humans

Detects

Engineers

Enhance-lives

Forefront

Honda

Inspire

Inventors

Japan

Japanese

Konichiwa

Landmines

Learn

Like a pet

Mimics-human/facial-expression

Moves-with-flexibility/spontaneity

Madonna

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4

Page 27: Applying the Landscape Model to Comprehending Discourse From TV News Stories

544 LEE, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN, ROSKOS-EWOLDSEN

Museum

No-hygiene-issues

Not-as-talented/skilled-as-human

Not-in our lifetimes

Not-real-animal

Organizers

Pediatric-wards

Play-flute

Pride

ROBODEX

Robot

Robot that-heals

Robot-expo

Robotic-innovations

Saves-lives

Seal-responds-to touch

Senior-citizens

Sensitive-robot-fingertips/lips/tongue

Share-ideas

Show-off robots

Sony

Speaking

Talented

Therapy

Under-development

University-student

Used-as-tour-guide

Will-change-culture

Concept Presented Only in the Video Version

of the News Story

Can-move-eyes/head

Crowd

Disc-like attachment

Disc-sweeping movement

Disgust/dislike

Dog-like-Robot

Exhibition hall

Gesturing robot

Going-down-stairs

Holding/patting-seal

Man

Microphone

Raise-lip

Reporter

Robot-with-woman-face

Robot blinking-eyes

Robot-doll

Robot-face/fingers

Robot-machine

Small-red-ball

Tracking-objects

Walking

Waving-robot

White-baby-seal robot

Woman

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f A

laba

ma

at T

usca

loos

a] a

t 17:

21 2

5 A

ugus

t 201

4