Top Banner
“Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trials Click on any item to view associated materials Biographies of Speakers The Honorable Sherry R. Fallon, Magistrate Judge, U.S. District Court Michael Biek, Ph.D., Trial Consultant, Decision Quest Program Materials * The Psychology of Decision-making and ADR The Pros and Cons of Admitting Liability The View from the Jury Box * The forms included herein are samples only and may not be appropriate for any particular matter. 1
43

“Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

Sep 15, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

“Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trials

Click on any item to view associated materials Biographies of Speakers

The Honorable Sherry R. Fallon, Magistrate Judge, U.S. District Court Michael Biek, Ph.D., Trial Consultant, Decision Quest

Program Materials *

The Psychology of Decision-making and ADR The Pros and Cons of Admitting Liability The View from the Jury Box

* The forms included herein are samples only and may not be appropriate for any

particular matter.

1

Page 2: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

Sherry Ruggiero Fallon was appointed as a U. S. Magistrate Judge for the District of Delaware

on April 25, 2012. She is formerly a Partner of the Wilmington, Delaware law firm of Tybout

Redfearn & Pell, with a jury trial practice in all Delaware State and Federal Courts. Magistrate

Judge Fallon’s primary areas of practice included insurance defense, insurance coverage and

bad faith litigation, toxic tort, product liability, retailers’ premises liability, construction

litigation, creditors’ claims in commercial bankruptcies, and defense of employment litigation

claims. Prior to becoming a Magistrate Judge, she had been a member of Tybout, Redfearn &

Pell since 1985.

Magistrate Judge Fallon earned her Bachelor of Arts Degree from the University of

Pennsylvania in 1983. She is a 1986 Graduate of the Delaware Law School of Widener

University, where she was a member of the Moot Court Honor Society, the Delaware Journal of

Corporate Law and Phi Delta Phi Legal Fraternity.

Magistrate Judge Fallon has been admitted to practice in the States of Delaware and New Jersey,

since 1986. Magistrate Judge Fallon is admitted to practice before the United States District

Courts for the Districts of Delaware and, New Jersey, and is admitted in the Third Circuit Court

of Appeals.

Magistrate Judge Fallon is a member of the Delaware, New Jersey and American Bar

Associations. Magistrate Judge Fallon has been a member of the American Board of Trial

Advocates (ABOTA), an organization for civil trial lawyers, in which membership is by

invitation, from 2003 through the present. Magistrate Judge Fallon served as President of the

Delaware Chapter of ABOTA, from 2009-2010. Magistrate Judge Fallon is a former member of

the Defense Research Institute, Defense Counsel of Delaware and the Delaware Claims

Association. Following her appointment as a Magistrate Judge, she became a member of the

Rodney Inn of Court and the Technology Inn of Court. Magistrate Judge Fallon is a frequent

lecturer on various aspects of civil litigation, trial practice and insurance coverage.

Page 3: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

MICHAEL A. BIEK, PH.D. Director State College, PA 814.867.4080 [email protected] Dr. Michael Biek has over 18 years of experience as a trial consultant, and he has advised clients in over 800 cases. His work includes pre-trial studies, which help attorneys develop case themes, and statistical studies in support of venue motions and jury selections. Dr. Biek is an adept courtroom strategist, and he often advises clients throughout trial in order to help them with their arguments before triers of fact. As a jury psychologist, Dr. Biek has particular expertise in the fields of intellectual property, contracts, securities, employment, product liability, professional malpractice, insurance and pharmaceutical cases. His research allows the trial team to make a wide-range of decisions; in addition to aiding clients inside the courtroom, he often helps achieve favorable out-of-court resolutions. EDUCATION: Texas A&M University, Ph.D., Psychology, 1992.

Texas A&M University, M.A., Psychology, 1989.

University of California – Santa Barbara, B.A., Psychology, 1986. SELECTED PUBLICATIONS: “The Psychology of Decision-making and ADR,” 2007.

“Juror Attitudes Toward Corporate America,” Voir Dire, 2003.

“The Pros and Cons of Admitting Liability,” 2001.

“Voir Dire in Medical Malpractice Cases,” Litigating Medical Malpractice Claims, 2001.

“The Psychology of Demonstrative Evidence,” Interview by Richard Herrmann, Esquire. InRe: The Journal of the Delaware State Bar Association, 2001.

“No Crying Over Spilled Coffee,” Pennsylvania Business Central, 2000.

“Working Knowledge, Cognitive Processing, and Attitudes: On The Determinants of Bias,” with W. Wood and S. Chaiken. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1996. .

Page 4: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

MICHAEL A. BIEK, PH.D. Director Page Two SELECTED SPEAKING ENGAGEMENTS: “Juror and Judicial Decision-making,” Presentation to Philadelphia Common Pleas Criminal and Civil Judges, Philadelphia, January 19, 2011.

“Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases,” Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, 2008.

“The Impact of Technology on the Jury,” Widener University School of Law, 2008.

“Best Practice in Jury Trials,” Widener School of Law – Bridge The Gap, 2008.

“The Impact of Technology on the Jury & the Effective Use of Courtroom Technology,” Paralegal Superconference Philadelphia, 2007.

“Trials Approaches to Jury Persuasion,” National Employment Lawyers Association Presentation – Trial Tactics: Jury Considerations and Effective Graphic, 2007.

“A View From the Jury: Medical – Legal Issues Facing CHOP Practitioners,” Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, 2007.

“Juror’s Reactions to Technology in the Courtroom,” Widener Law School, 2007.

“Jury Voir Dire: Can Our System Be Improved?” Delaware Trial Lawyers Association, 2007.

“Witnesses: Can’t Live With ‘em,” Pennsylvania Bar Institute, 2007.

“Witness Preparation: Keeping the Jury in Mind,” Philadelphia Attorney General’s Office, 2006.

“Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases,” American Physicians Assurance Corporation, 2006.

“Impact of Technology on the Jury,” Widener School of Law, 2006.

“The View From the Jury Box,” Pennsylvania Trial Association, 2005.

“Pros and Cons of Admitting Liability,” Long Term Care Defense Summit, 2005.

“Jurors’ Reactions in Intellectual Property Cases,” 2003.

“Jurors’ Perceptions in Financial Malpractice Cases,” 2003.

“The Impact of Technology on the Jury,” 2003.

“The Jury Psychology of Employment Cases,” 2002.

“Jurors’ Perceptions of Corporate America: Implications for Trial Strategy,” 2002.

“Jury Selection Since 9/11,” Pennsylvania Association of Criminal Defense Lawyers, 2002.

“Case Analysis & Jury Analysis: An Overview of Mock Trials,” with Judge Marvin Halbert (ret.), 2002.

“Voir Dire in Medical Malpractice Cases,” Pennsylvania Bar Institute, 2001.

Page 5: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

MICHAEL A. BIEK, PH.D. Director Page Three “The Strategic Use of Juror Psychology in Litigation,” 2001.

“How Jurors Think,” with Arthur Patterson and Nancy Neufer, 2001.

“Using Focus Groups,” National Institute of Trial Advocacy, 2001.

“Jurors’ Perceptions of Attorneys,” 2001.

“Jury Persuasion: What They Don’t Teach in Law School,” 2001.

“Voir Dire in the Business Litigation Case,” 2001.

“Jurors’ Perceptions of Demonstrative Evidence,” 2000.

“Understanding Juror Psychology and its Role in Positive Outcomes,” 2000.

“What Are They Thinking? Juror Psychology 101,” 1999.

“Jury Dynamics and the Defense Lawyer,” Virginia Association of Defense Attorneys, 1998.

“Sexual Harassment Themes and Jury Issues,” 1998.

“Theme Selection and Jury Selection,” ALI-ABA, 1997.

“Jury Profiling and Behavior,” Pennsylvania Office of the Attorney General, 1997.

Page 6: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

1

CURRICULUM VITA

MICHAEL A. BIEK

ADDRESS

Business: DecisionQuest403 S. Allen Street, Suite 204State College, PA 16801Tel: (814) 867-4080Fax: (814) 867-4086E-mail: [email protected]

PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE

1998 - Director, DecisionQuest

1993 - 1998 Senior Jury Consultant, FTI Consulting, Inc.

1992 - 1993 Lecturer, Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University

EDUCATION

1990 - 1992 Ph.D., Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station.

1986 - 1989 M.S., Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station.

1982 - 1986 B.A., Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara.

PAPERS AND PRESENTATIONS

The Jury Pool Today. Worrall F. Mountain American Inn of Court, Morristown, NJ, May 23, 2011

The Psychology of Juror Decision-making/Witness Preparation. English, Lucas, Priest & Owsley, Bowling Green, KY, April 14, 2011.

Witness Preparation. Bressler, Amery & Ross. Florham Park, NJ, April 12, 2011.

Page 7: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

2

Witness Preparation. Finnegan Henderson Law Firm, Washington, DC, March 29, 2011.

Making the Most of Voir Dire. American Inns of Court, Pittsburgh, PA, February 16, 2011.

Juror Perceptions and Asbestos Cases. HB Litigation Conferences: Trying an asbestos case—how to get the “W,” Philadelphia, PA, January 25, 2011.

Juror and Judicial Decision-making. Presentation to Philadelphia Common Pleas Criminal and Civil Judges, Philadelphia, PA, January 19, 2011.

Jury Selection From a Jury Consultant’s Perspective. Presentation to Zarwin Baum Law Firm, Philadelphia, PA, January 7, 2011.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation to Widener School of Law, Wilmington, DE, January 7, 2011.

Best Practices: Jury Trials. Invited Speaker, The Supreme Court of Delaware, Pre-Admission Conference, Wilmington, DE, October 29, 2010.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation to Widener School of Law, Wilmington, DE, August 20, 2010.

Preparing Witnesses to Testify. Presentation to Wilson Elser Law Firm, New York, NY, July 27, 2010.

Jurors’ Perceptions: An Analysis of Pre- and Post-Trial Juror Attitudes. HarrisMartin’s Pennsylvania Asbestos Litigation Conference, Philadelphia, PA, June 17, 2010.

Is Your Case Right for Focus Group Research? Telephone CLE to the Kentucky Justice Association, June 7, 2010

Jury Selection from a Jury Consultant’s Perspective. Marshall Dennehy Warner Coleman & Goggin, Philadelphia, PA, May 4, 2010.

Technology Impact on the Jury. The National Judicial College Webinar Series on Electronic Discovery, Nationwide, April 28, 2010.

Jury Selection from a Jury Consultant’s Perspective. Thorp, Reed & Armstrong, Pittsburgh, PA, April 19, 2010.

Page 8: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

3

Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter, and Blogging…Oh MySpace”Pennsylvania Bar Institute, Philadelphia, PA, March 24, 2009

Myth Busters: Debunking Trial Myths Through Mock Jury Research. Pepper Hamilton LLP, Philadelphia, PA, January 14, 2010.

Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter, and Blogging…Oh MySpace”Pennsylvania Bar Institute, Philadelphia, PA, November 10, 2009

Best Practices: Jury Trials. Invited Speaker, The Supreme Court of Delaware, Pre-Admission Conference, Wilmington, Delaware, November 6, 2009.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation to Widener School of Law, Wilmington, DE, August 13, 2009.

Jury Selection & Voir Dire. CLE presented to Margolis Edelstein, Philadelphia, PA, August 3, 2009.

Maximizing Your Client’s Recovery in Challenging Times: A Jury Consultant’s Perspective. Pennsylvania Bar Institute, Philadelphia, PA, June 23, 2009, Pittsburgh, July 22, 2009.

The Psychology of Jurors, Damages & Voir Dire. Anapol Schwartz. Philadelphia, PA, June 18, 2009.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, June 4, 2009.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Babst, Balland, Clements & Zomnir, P.C. Pittsburgh, May 21, 2009.

Jury Psychology & Employment Cases. Littler Mendelson, P.C. Philadelphia, May 12, 2009.

Using Pre-Trial Jury Research to refine Case Strategies. Pennsylvania Bar Institute – 15th

Annual Employment Law Institute. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, April 16, 2009.

Preparing Witnesses. Hunton & Williams, Richmond, Virginia, April 6, 2009.

Voir Dire – Making the Most of It. Delaware County Trial Lawyers Association. Media, Pennsylvania, April 2, 2009.

Page 9: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

4

Voir Dire – Making the Most of It. Selective Insurance – Advanced Trial Skills Seminar. Cherry Hill, New Jersey, April 2, 2009.

Preparing Witnesses to Testify. Greenbaum, Rowe, Smith & Davis, LLP, Iselin, New Jersey, March 17, 2009.

Impact of Technology: What are Jurors Thinking? Greensfelder, Hemker & Gale, P.C., St. Louis, MO, February 19, 2009.

The Trucking Industry Defense Association, Advanced Seminar. Invited speaker. Miami, February 12, 2009.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, January 15, 2009.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation to Widener School of Law, Harrisburg, Pennsylvania, January 7, 2009.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, December 18, 2008.

An Ounce of Prevention: Preventing Employment Litigation and Other Workplace Legal Problems. Invited panelist, Pennsylvania Bar Institute 2008 Employment Law Institute West, Pittsburgh, November 18, 2008.

Best Practices: Jury Psychology. Invited Speaker, The Supreme Court of Delaware, Bridge the Gap Program, Wilmington, Delaware, November 14, 2008.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, November 6, 2008.

The Psychology of Jurors. CLE Presentation to Sunoco Legal Department. Philadelphia, October 13, 2008.

The Psychology of Jurors. CLE Presentation to Schnader Harrison. Philadelphia, September 3, 2008.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Delaware Paralegal Association. Wilmington, Delaware, August 15, 2008.

Jurors and Damages. CLE Presentation to Marshall Dennehey, Philadelphia, August 5, 2008.

Page 10: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

5

Preparing Witnesses. CLE Presentation to Fox Rothschild, Philadelphia, June 19, 2008.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Delaware Paralegal Association. Wilmington, Delaware, June 12, 2008.

The Psychology of Jurors. CLE Presentation to Morris James, LLP. Wilmington, Delaware, June 10, 2008.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, April 30, 2008.

The Psychology of Jurors. CLE Presentation to Schnader, Harrison, Segal & Lewis, LLP. Philadephia, Pennsylvania, April 24, 2008.

How to Manage a Reduction in Force. Invited panelist, Pennsylvania Bar Institute 14th

Annual Employment Law Institute, Philadelphia, April 18, 2008.

Preventing Employment Litigation and Other Workplace Legal Problems. Invited panelist, Pennsylvania Bar Institute 14th Annual Employment Law Institute, Philadelphia, April 18, 2008.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, April 4, 2008.

Preparing Witnesses. CLE Presentation to Marshall, Dennehey, Warner, Coleman & Goggin, Philadelphia, March 18, 2008.

Preparing Witnesses. CLE Presentation to Pepper Hamilton, LLP, Pittsburgh, March 6, 2008.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, January 12, 2008.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation to Widener School of Law, Harrisburg, Pennsylvania, January 4, 2008.

Juror Psychology and Witnesses: Avoiding Pitfalls through Effective Preparation. CLE Presentation to the Pennsylvania Attorney General’s Office. Harrisburg, Pennsylvania, December 12, 2007.

The Psychology of Jurors. CLE Presentation to Weber, Gallagher, Simpson, Stapleton, Fires & Newby, LLP. Philadephia, Pennsylvania, December 7, 2007.

Page 11: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

6

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. CLE Presentation to Montgomery, McCracken, Walker & Rhoads, LLP paralegals, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, November 20, 2007.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Invited Speaker, The Supreme Court of Delaware, Bridge the Gap Program, Wilmington, Delaware, October 26, 2007.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. In-House Paralegal SuperConference, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, October 22, 2007.

Jury Psychology & Employment Cases. Presentation to the National Employment Lawyers Association, New York City, October 10, 2007

Voir Dire & Jury Selection. Presentation to the Montgomery Count Inn of Court. Blue Bell, Pennsylvania, October 9, 2007.

Jurors and Damages. CLE Presentation to Wilson, Elser, Moskowitz, Edelman & Dicker, LLP. Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, September 12, 2007.The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, August 16, 2007.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, The Children’s Hospital of Pennsylvania, in-house seminar “Medical Legal Issues Facing the CHOP Practitioner.” June 23, September 8, November 10, 2007.

Witnesses: Can’t live with ‘em. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s “Preparing Your Witness to Testify,” Pittsburgh, PA, January 23, 2007

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, January 5, 2007.

Jury Psychology and Medical Malpractice Cases. ALI-ABA Litigating Medical Malpractice Claims. Boston, September 29, 2006.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Presentation at the Mid-Atlantic eLaw Technology Conference, April 21, 2006.

Jury Psychology & Medical Malpractice Cases. Invited speaker, American Physicians Assurance Corporation claims management conference, March 22, 2006.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, January 6, 2006.

Page 12: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

7

Admitting Liability in Nursing Home Cases. Keynote speaker, Long-term Care Defense Counsel Summit, Sioux Falls, SD, September 28, 2005.

Reaching the Jury in a Commercial Damages Case. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s “Damages in Commercial Litigation,” Pittsburgh, PA, July 22, 2005.

The Business of Jury Selection: The Quest for Diverse Juries. Invited panelist, Pennsylvania Bar Association Litigation Retreat, April 8, 2005.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, January 6, 2005.

Voir Dire in Motor Vehicle Cases. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Trial Lawyer’s Association’s “How to Try the Auto Case,” Harrisburg, PA October 19, 2004 and Philadelphia, PA, October 14, 2004.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, August 20, 2004.

Voir Dire in Medical Malpractice Cases. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s “Jury Issues and Selection in Medical Malpractice Cases,” in Mechanicsburg, PA, April 16, 2004.

Technology in the Courtroom. Invited speaker, Rodney Inn of Court, Wilmington DE, February 10, 2004.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, January 9, 2004.

Jurors’ Reactions in Patent Cases. Presentation to the Shaw Pittman Intellectual Property Practice Group, September 23, 2003.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, August 22, 2003.

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, April 16, 2003.

Juror Attitudes toward Corporate America. Voir Dire, Volume 10, Issue 1, Spring 2003.

Jurors’ Perceptions in Financial Malpractice Cases. Presentation to the Actuarial Claims Conference, New York, January 23, 2003.

Page 13: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

8

The Impact of Technology on the Jury. Guest lecturer, Widener University School of Law, January 10, 2003.

The Jury Psychology of Employment Cases. Presentation to the Employment Law & Litigation Group, New York, October 10, 2002.

Jurors’ Perceptions of Corporate America: Implications for Trial Strategy. Presentation to the July 12, 2002 Litigation Meeting at Dechert, Price & Rhoads, Philadelphia.

Jury Selection Since 9/11. Presentation to the Annual Meeting of the Pennsylvania Association of Criminal Defense Lawyers. April 18, 2002.

Case Analysis & Jury Analysis: An Overview of Mock Trials. Joint presentation with Judge Marvin Halbert (ret.) to the Philadelphia Association of Defense Counsel. March 19, 2002.

Voir Dire in Medical Malpractice Cases. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s “Nuts and Bolts of the Medical Malpractice Case, November 8, 2001 in Mechanicsburg, PA, and November 14, 2001 in Philadelphia.

The Strategic Use of Juror Psychology in Litigation. Presentation to the Annual Meeting of the Nebraska Association of Trial Attorneys, October 19, 2001.

The Pros and Cons of Admitting Liability. Presentation to the Trucking Industry Defense Association, Atlanta, GA, October 11, 2001.

Using Focus Groups. Presentation to the National Institute of Trial Advocacy’s Advanced Advocates course, Washington DC, October 5th, 2001.

Jurors’ Perceptions of Attorneys. Presentation to Schnader, Harrison, Segal & Lewis Litigation Group, July 19, 2001

Jury Persuasion: What They Don’t Teach in Law School. Presentation to Schnader, Harrison, Segal & Lewis summer associates, July 19, 2001.

Jurors’ Reactions in Personal Injury Cases. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s 8th Annual Personal Injury Institute, July 18, 2001.

How jurors think. (co-authored by Nancy Neufer, M.S.) Paper published in course materials for the Pennsylvania Bar Institute’s 8th Annual Personal Injury Institute, July 2001.

Page 14: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

9

The Psychology of Demonstrative Evidence. Interview by Richard Herrmann, Esquire published in InRe:, the journal of the Delaware State Bar Association, May, 2001.

Voir Dire in Medical Malpractice Cases. Paper published in course materials for ALI-ABA’s Litigating Medical Malpractice Claims, May, 2001.

Voir Dire in the Business Litigation Case. Invited panelist. Indiana Continuing Legal Education Forum. March 29, 2001.

No Crying Over Spilled Coffee. Guest columnist. Pennsylvania Business Central, Volume 9, No. 26, December 22, 2000.

Jurors’ Perceptions of Demonstrative Evidence. Presentation to the Minneapolis Institute of Legal Education, Demonstrative Evidence Bringing Life to Trial. November 1, 2000.

Understanding Juror Psychology and its Role in Positive Outcomes. Presentation to the Desmond J. McTighe American Inns of Court, Montgmery County PA, September 21, 2000.

What They Don’t Teach in Law School. Presentation to numerous Philadelphia firms’ summer associates, Summer, 2000.

Mini-Trials. Facilitator/moderator for the Benjamin Franklin American Inn of Court. Philadelphia, PA, March 9, 2000.

What are they thinking? Juror Psychology 101. Presentation to the Bucks County Bar Association. Doylestown, PA, December, 9, 1999.

Jury Dynamics and the Defense Lawyer. Presentation to the Virginia Association of Defense Attorneys. Charlottesville, VA, October 16, 1998.

Voir Dire Symposium. Massachusetts Academy of Trial Attorneys. Boston, MA, October 17, 1998.

Sexual Harassment Themes and Jury Issues. Presentation to Allentown Inns of Court. March 18, 1998.

Theme Selection and Jury Selection. Presentation to ALI-ABA, Litigating Medical Malpractice Claims. November 8, 1997.

Jury Profiling and Behavior. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Office of the Attorney General - Seminar on Torts. July 31, 1997.

Page 15: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

10

The Non-Verbal Message. Invited Panelist. Video-Technology in the Courts conference, sponsored by the State of Delaware, Criminal Justice Council. February 3, 1997.

Jurors’ Attitudes Toward Damages in Civil Lawsuits. Paper prepared for the Pennsylvania Bar Institute. 1997.

No Use Suing Over Spilled Coffee? How the McDonald’s Case has Influenced Jurors’ Attitudes in Civil Lawsuits. Presentation to the State Bar of Michigan - Litigation Section. August, 1996.

Trial of a Patent Case. American Law Institute - American Bar Association. Facultymember. Chicago, September, 1995.

How to Pick a Jury. Presentation to the Pennsylvania Prison Society. August, 1994.

Individual vs. Collective Memory in the Jury Room. Practical Aspects of Memory Conference. Sponsored by The Federal Judicial Center. Invited panelist, July 1994.

The Psychology of Opening Statements. Presentation to the Allegheny County Bar Association, Civil Litigation Section. June, 1994.

Criminal Jury Selection. Invited panelist, 11th Annual Criminal Law Symposium, sponsored by The Pennsylvania Bar Institute. June, 1994

Advances in Jury Selection. With John D. Gilleland, Ph.D., and Ann T. Greeley, Ph.D. Presentation to the Washington D.C. Bar, Litigation Section. March, 1994.

"Not Just The Facts" or "The Complete Communicator". Paper for the Pennsylvania Bar Institute, Young Lawyers Division newsletter, At Issue, Spring, 1994.

Biggens v. Hazen Paper. Analysis of relevant jury considerations. Eighth Annual Labor and Employment Law Conference, 1993.

Jurors' Attitudes in the 90's. Paper prepared for the Pennsylvania Bar Institute, 1993.

Working knowledge and attitude strength. Society for Experimental Social Psychology. San Antonio, TX, 1992 (invited panelist).

How prior knowledge affects processing of persuasive messages. American Psychological Association. Boston, MA, 1990.

RESEARCH AND PUBLICATIONS

Page 16: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

11

Biek, M. (2010). Jury selection in harassment cases: Discovering juror attitudes that reveal bias. Chapter in Litigating the Workplace Harassment Case, Tort Trial & Insurance Practice Section, American Bar Association, 305-320.

Biek, M., Wood, W., & Chaiken, S. (1996). Working knowledge, cognitive processing, and attitudes: On the determinants of bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 547-556.

Wood, W., Rhodes, N.D., & Biek, M. (1995). Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information processing analysis. In R. Petty & J. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences. The Ohio State Symposium on Attitudes (Vol. 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Simpson, J., Gangestad, S., & Biek, M. (1993). Personality and nonverbal social behavior: An ethological perspective of relationship initiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 434-461.

Gangestad, S., Simpson, J., Geronimo, K., & Biek, M. (1992). Differential accuracy in person perception across traits: Examination of a functional hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 688-698.

Samuelson, C. & Biek, M. (1991). Attitudes toward energy conservation: A confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 549-568.

Biek, M. Knowledge and affect as determinants of information processing, attitude stability, and behavior. (unpublished doctoral dissertation, 1992).

Biek, M. Accuracy versus bias in the processing of persuasive messages. (unpublished masters thesis, 1989).

REVIEWING AND EDITING

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

AWARDS

Texas A&M University College of Liberal Arts: Dissertation Fellowship 1991 - 1992American Psychological Association: Science Directorate Award 1990

Page 17: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

12

COURSES TAUGHT

Social PsychologyExperimental Psychology Lab Methods

FIELDS OF INTEREST

Psychology and lawPersuasion/Attitude changePersonality and social behaviorStatistics/Research methodology

[Revised April 5 2011]

Page 18: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

13

TESTIMONY & AFFIDAVITS

May 1997 Commonwealth of PA v. Scher Susquehanna County. Testified briefly regarding methodological issues involved in a change-of-venue survey conducted by FTI.

May 1995 Frederick v. Altoona Hospital, et. al Blair County. Submitted affidavit in support of motion for use of a written juror questionnaire.

Page 19: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

Michael Biek, Ph.D. 403 South Allen Street, Suite 204 State College, Pennsylvania 16801 814 867-4080 [email protected]

The Psychology of Decision-making and ADR

Litigating disputes to a jury is often a stressful experience for everyone involved. Part of this stress comes from the underlying difficulty in predicting how a group of people, often chosen on the basis of having as little knowledge or experience relevant to the dispute as possible, will make decisions about the case at issue. Attorneys and their clients often worry about whether jurors will be able to make rational decisions based on evidence, especially in complex or emotionally charged cases. These concerns affect both plaintiffs and defendants and sometimes lead to a decision to forgo a jury trial in favor of a bench trial or to attempt a resolution through the use of alternative dispute resolution practices.

Are judges likely to be better decision-makers than jurors? This is clearly a complex question, and a complete answer would need to consider factors such as group versus individual decision-making, cognitive abilities and the ability to understand and apply legal constructs (among many other things). This paper will address the issue of systematic errors that people tend to make when evaluating decision options and interpreting information and examine whether judges tend to make the same sorts of errors as laypersons.

Limitations on Jurors / Cognitive Biases

Psychologists have learned that human beings rely on mental shortcuts, known in the jargon as "heuristics," to make complex decisions. Reliance on these heuristics facilitates good judgment much of the time, and helps people deal with what would often be overwhelming amounts of information, but it can also produce systematic errors in judgment. Just as certain patterns of visual stimuli can fool people's eyesight, leading them to see things that are not really there, certain fact patterns can fool people's judgment, leading them to believe things that are not really true. Reliance on heuristics can sometimes create cognitive illusions that produce erroneous judgments.

Research on juries indicates that cognitive illusions can adversely affect the quality of the decisions jurors make. Researchers have found, for example, that juries believe that litigants should have predicted events that no one could have predicted, allow irrelevant or inadmissible information to influence liability determinations, defer to arbitrary numerical estimates, and rely on incoherent methods to calculate damages. Several of the more important heuristics that have been studied in the legal arena are as follows:

Page 20: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

2

Hindsight bias

People overstate their own ability to have predicted the past and believe that others should have been able to predict events better than was possible. Psychologists call this tendency for people to overestimate the predictability of past events the "hindsight bias." It occurs because learning an outcome causes people to update their beliefs about the world. People then rely on these new beliefs to generate estimates of what was predictable, but they ignore the change in their beliefs that learning the outcome inspired. Few judgments in ordinary life require people to assess the predictability of past outcomes, but such judgments are pervasive in the law. This bias frequently operates against defendants because jurors tend to overestimate the likelihood that bad outcomes could have been foreseen (the defendant being blamed for the bad outcome). However, it can also work against plaintiffs in situations where jurors believe that contributory negligence (alleged or not) may have been a factor in a plaintiff’s injury.

Anchoring

When people make numerical estimates (e.g., the fair market value of a house), they

commonly rely on the initial value available to them (e.g., the list price). That initial value tends to "anchor" their final estimates. In many situations, reliance on an anchor is reasonable because many anchors convey relevant information about the actual value of an item. The problem, however, is that anchors that do not provide any information about the actual value of an item also influence judgment. Anchors affect judgment by changing the standard of reference that people use when making numeric judgments. Even when people conclude that an anchor provides no useful information, mentally testing the validity of the anchor causes people to adjust their estimates upward or downward toward that anchor. As a consequence, even extreme, wholly absurd anchors can affect judgment. For example, if you ask people whether the average annual temperature of a city like San Francisco is higher or lower than 550 degrees, virtually everyone who understands the concept of temperature will answer that the average temperature is lower. However, when you then ask them to estimate the true average temperature, their estimates will be significantly higher than the estimates given by people who have not first been asked the anchoring question. Another example from the research is to ask an audience to think of the last 4 digits of their social security number, and then to estimate the number of physicians in New York. The correlation (which should be about 0 on a scale from -1 to 1) between an individual's social security number and their estimate in this situation is around 0.4—far beyond what would be expected by chance. The simple act of thinking of the first number strongly influences the second, even though there is no logical connection between them. Anchoring is relevant in many litigation scenarios, particularly when it comes to parties involved in settlement negotiations and jurors making estimations of damages.

Egocentric biases

People tend to make judgments about themselves and their abilities that are "egocentric"

or self-serving. For example, people routinely estimate that they are above average on a variety of desirable characteristics, including health, driving, professional skills, and likelihood of having a successful marriage. Moreover, people overestimate their contribution to joint activities. For example, after a conversation both parties will estimate that they spoke more than half the time. Similarly, when married couples are asked to estimate the percentage of household tasks they perform, their estimates typically add up to more than 100%.

Page 21: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

3

Egocentric biases occur for several reasons. For one thing, people remember their own actions better than others' actions. Thus, when asked to recall the percentage of housework they perform, people remember their own contribution more easily and, consequently tend to overestimate it. Egocentric biases can be adaptive, but they can also have misleading influences on the litigation process. Due to egocentric biases, litigants and their lawyers might overestimate their own abilities, the quality of their advocacy and the relative merits of their cases. Research demonstrating this asked participants to evaluate a dispute scenario from the perspective of either a plaintiff or defendant and to judge the potential settlement value of the case. Participants evaluating the case from the perspective of the plaintiff predicted that a judge would award significantly more than the defendant-participants predicted. Similarly, when asked to identify what they perceived to be a fair settlement value, plaintiff-participants selected a higher value than the value selected by defendant-participants. These results suggest that self-serving or egocentric biases could be a factor in bargaining impasses.

Framing

When people confront risky decisions such as deciding whether to settle a case or to

proceed to trial, they tend to categorize their decision options as potential gains or losses. This categorization, or "framing," of decision options influences the way people evaluate options and affects their willingness to incur risk. People tend to make risk-averse decisions when choosing between options that appear to represent gains and risk-seeking decisions when choosing between options that appear to represent losses. For example, most people prefer a certain $100 gain to a 50% chance of winning $200 but prefer a 50% chance of losing $200 to a certain $100 loss. Litigation produces a natural frame. In most lawsuits, plaintiffs choose either to accept a certain settlement from the defendant or to gamble, hoping that further litigation will produce a larger gain. Most defendants, by contrast, choose either to pay a certain settlement to the plaintiff or to gamble that further litigation will reduce the amount that they must pay. Thus, plaintiffs often choose between options that appear to represent gains, while defendants often choose between options that appear to represent losses. As such, plaintiffs are more likely to prefer settlement, the risk-averse option, while defendants are more likely to prefer trial, the risk-seeking option.

Representativeness heuristic

When people make categorical judgments (e.g., assessing the likelihood that a criminal

defendant is guilty), they tend to base their judgments on the extent to which the evidence being analyzed (e.g., the defendant's demeanor) is representative of the category. When the evidence appears representative of the category (e.g., defendant is nervous and shifty), people judge the likelihood that the evidence is a product of that category as high (i.e., evidence of guilt). When the evidence being analyzed does not resemble the category (e.g., defendant appears at ease), people judge the likelihood that the evidence is a product of that category as low (i.e., evidence of innocence). Psychologists refer to this phenomenon as the "representativeness heuristic." Although the representativeness heuristic can be useful, it can lead people to discount or ignore more relevant statistical information. In particular, people undervalue the importance of the frequency with which the underlying category occurs in an underlying population, essentially ignoring the "base-rate" of the phenomenon under consideration.

Page 22: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

4

Impediments inherent in the system

Our legal system is based on assumptions about human decision-making, some of which have been demonstrated to be invalid. For example, the system assumes that jurors can disregard inadmissible information when instructed to do so, even though this is psychologically impossible. Indeed, the fact that such information gets pointed out for special attention has been shown to actually increase its impact on juror decision-making in some circumstances. A partial listing of aspects of the system, varying wildly from place to place, that make jurors’ jobs difficult (some of which are being addressed in some jurisdictions) include:

• Not being told the law until near the end of the process. • Not being able to read a written copy of the law. • Not being given instructions in words that have meaning for laypeople. • Not being able to ask questions. • Not being able to take notes. • Verdict questions that are sometimes constructed with no thought to the difficulty

they might pose to laypeople. • Being told that some of the things they have heard and seen are evidence and

others are not. • Distinguishing between questions and statements of attorneys as non-evidence

versus answers, admitted exhibits and other stipulations which are evidence.

Simply put, being a juror is a difficult job, on top of which the rules of the task (while created for sound legal reasons) frequently add to its difficulty. Indeed, our interviews with actual jurors post-trial show that they have often been confused about some of the most basic principles, e.g., that the answers given from the witness stand are evidence. We have asked jurors what they made of testimony and whether it was important to their decision only to hear, “They were just saying that.” For such jurors, if there is not written material backing up the testimony, it does not count as evidence, even though those verbal answers are at the heart of what evidence is supposed to be.

The Superiority of Judicial Decision-making

Given cognitive biases and other limitations on juror decision-making as discussed

above, is it any wonder that parties involved in disputes often wonder whether they would be better off in front of a judge and foregoing a jury trial? One would expect that judges should have an easier time making decisions based on evidence and legal principles and ignoring (or at least minimizing the influence of) tangential or emotional considerations. Judges certainly have a greater amount of knowledge and informational resources available to inform their decision-making and are highly motivated in that they are not interested in seeing their decisions overturned due to error.

But are judges more reliable decision-makers than jurors? Are they less susceptible to

making decisions based on erroneous cognitive processes? Whether or not judges tend to make similar cognitive errors as jurors has not received a great deal of systematic study. Despite the important role that judges play in the courtroom, psychologists have focused most of their research efforts on juries. One study that provides an excellent overview on this topic, as well as experimental data collected from a large population of federal magistrate judges was conducted

Page 23: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

5

by Chris Guthrie, J.D., Jeffrey Rachlinski, J.D., Ph.D. and the Honorable Andrew J. Wistrich, J.D., and published in the May 2001 issue of the Cornell Law Review. Their study examined the responses of magistrate judges to scenarios that were manipulated in order to assess the effects, if any, of the cognitive biases discussed above. The sample of 167 judges comprised nearly one-third of all such federal magistrate judges on the bench at the time the data were collected. The results showed that judges did better than lay jurors in some areas but that judges appeared to be just as susceptible to certain cognitive errors as were jurors. A brief summary of the key results is as follows:

Hindsight bias

Judges in this research were asked to evaluate a hypothetical fact pattern and make

decisions about the likelihood of an outcome of an appeal. They were asked to predict which of three potential actions the court of appeals was most likely to have taken (affirm, vacate or remand) after having been informed, on a random basis, that one of the three actions had been taken. After being given all three possible outcomes and asked to predict which of these decisions the court of appeals was most likely to have made, judges were more likely to predict the outcome that was consistent with the outcome that they were told had occurred; that is, judges who were given a particular outcome were more likely to predict that outcome than judges given the other possible outcomes. This demonstrates hindsight bias because the outcome provided to them should have been irrelevant to their own predictions about what the appeals court was most likely to do.

These results may have important implications for courtroom decision-making. For

example, in a situation where one side succeeds in keeping certain evidence out, the jury may be shielded from that evidence if it was excluded as part of a pre-trial hearing; hence it cannot contribute to a jury finding being based on hindsight. In a bench trial, in contrast, the judge as fact-finder cannot avoid being exposed to such evidence.

Anchoring

To study anchoring effects, judges were asked to assess a damages amount in reaction to

a personal injury case where liability was conceded. They were randomly assigned to a “no anchor” condition and an “anchor condition” in which the only additional information was a statement that the defendant had moved for dismissal based on the argument that the claim did not meet a jurisdictional minimum of $75,000 (a clearly merit-less motion given that the damages described were obviously greater than $75,000, including months of hospital bills, loss of the use of plaintiff’s legs, etc.). Although the $75,000 figure in the anchor condition communicated no useful information, it nevertheless had a significant effect on the damages awarded. Judges in the “no anchor” condition made average awards almost 30% higher than judges in the anchor condition, indicating that the mere presence of the $75,000 figure served as a lower anchor and depressed awards in that group.

These data show how easily anchors can affect the way judges think about damage awards. It appears that plaintiffs' damage requests may have unintended effects on both on juries and judges. The potential for statutory damage caps to anchor judges is another area of potential concern. Although judges might be able to keep juries in the dark about caps on damages, they clearly cannot blind themselves to such caps.

Page 24: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

6

Egocentric biases

In testing this bias, judges in the study were asked to estimate their own reversal rates on appeal, putting themselves into one of four categories as compared to their peers, equivalent to saying whether they were reversed more than 75% of their peers, more than 50% of their peers, more than 25% of their peers or less than 25% of their peers. Results showed that judges are susceptible to egocentric biases. Over 50% of the judges (56.1%) reported that their appeal rate placed them in the lowest quartile; 31.6% placed themselves in the second-lowest quartile; 7.7% in the second-highest quartile, and 4.5% in the highest quartile. In other words, 87.7% of the judges believed that at least half of their peers had higher reversal rates on appeal. This pattern of results differs significantly from what one would expect if judges were unbiased.

It is possible that egocentric biases might prevent judges from maintaining an awareness

of their limitations, and this may make it difficult for litigants to convince judges that they might have been wrong, thereby increasing the hurdles faced by parties in the situation of having to ask a judge to vacate a prior ruling or to set aside a jury’s verdict on the basis of a judicial mistake. Framing

Judges in the study were asked to place themselves in the role of overseeing settlement negotiations. They were given identical economic options as choices, however, for half the decision had been constructed around a “Plaintiff/Gains” perspective whereas the other half received a “Defendant/Losses” perspective. Although the hypothetical litigants in this problem faced settlement offers that were identical in terms of their expected value, the materials created the illusion that the plaintiff faced a choice between potential gains and that the defendant faced a choice between potential losses. The results showed that review from the defense (or risk-seeking) perspective was more likely to lead to an evaluation that the case should not be settled (75%) than review from the plaintiff (or risk-averse) view (60%). From the plaintiff's perspective, settlement seemed relatively more attractive, while from the defendant's perspective, trial seemed relatively more attractive, even though the two perspectives presented identical economic choices. Although this difference was statistically reliable, it should be noted that the 15% difference shown by this sample of judges was much less than that typically seen in studies of non-expert decision-makers (often as high as a 40-50% difference). Although judges were affected by framing effects, it appears that they may be less susceptible to this bias than many other people.

Representativeness heuristic Judges were provided with a scenario asking them to estimate the likelihood that an event occurred due to negligence, given a variety of statistical information about the occurrence rates of various steps in a series of events that led to a plaintiff’s injury. Research with most populations evaluating the same scenario shows that the tendency to ignore base-rate information leads a substantial majority of people to overestimate the extent to which negligence was a factor. Here, 40% of the judges (much higher than other populations) made the correct decision taking into account relevant statistical information. Of the 60% who answered incorrectly, however, a large proportion (40% of the total) did appear to have been affected by the representativeness heuristic. Hence it appears that judges are less affected by this cognitive bias than most other people (although they are still somewhat vulnerable to it). Still, the representativeness heuristic might account for some judges' apparent preference, observed by

Page 25: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

7

some researchers, for individuating evidence (e.g., eyewitness testimony) over statistical evidence (e.g., base rates).

So, Jury versus Bench?

This question is too involved to answer thoroughly within the scope of this brief review. Even so, the research on cognitive biases in information processing does provide some information that is applicable to the consideration of whether to forgo trial by jury. If one of the major arguments being put forth in favor of a bench trial is that judges are less affected by extra-legal thought processes than jurors, the answer appears to that this argument does not carry a great deal of weight. Judges, being human after all, are influenced in their decision-making by many of the cognitive shortcuts and biases as human jurors.

A point that also needs to be considered is that of individual versus group decision-

making. Unfortunately, the prior research does not provide a straightforward conclusion about whether groups make better decisions about disputes than individuals. There is evidence that juror deliberations can result in the lessening of the effect of cognitive biases in some instances, improved memory for trial evidence, increased complex reasoning about the evidence and arguments presented, and reduced variability in decisions. However, there is also evidence that group deliberation may exacerbate biases under certain conditions and can result in more extreme judgments in a process known as group polarization.

It should be pointed out that there is not even agreement among academic researchers about how to determine whether a jury’s or judge’s decision is the “right” one in the first place, and a number of different ways have been used to attempt to define the measure. In addition, given that every case is at least somewhat different from every other (sometimes wildly so), there is never likely to be a “one-size-fits-all” answer. Fortunately for those of us in the world of applied jury psychology (and for litigants themselves), there is an easier way to deal with this; the “right” decision is the one that provides the best possible result for our client. This is a testable question. Research methodologies exist which allow us to test whether a judge or a jury trial is more likely to lead to a better outcome for the client in any particular situation. Customized studies can help to determine whether judges may be better able than juries to focus on a particular narrow legal issue, for instance. In cases where bench trials or ADR approaches are required, e.g. Markman hearings, some contractual disputes, etc., applied research techniques can also be used to test approaches and refine arguments to make sure that the most persuasive case possible is being presented to the final fact-finder. Hence, parties facing a judge versus jury decision can make it supported by evidence as opposed to hunches; parties obligated to litigate in a non-jury format can take advantage of persuasion techniques that are applicable to both juries and judges.

Page 26: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

Copyright DecisionQuest 2001

THE PROS AND CONS OF ADMITTING LIABILITY

Prepared for the Trucking Industry Defense Association 10/11/2001

Michael Biek, Ph.D.

DecisionQuest 403 South Allen Street, Suite 204

State College, PA 16801 814-867-4080

[email protected]

Page 27: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

2

INTRODUCTION

The study of psychology in general, and the study of jury behavior in particular, has

long been used by counsel for both plaintiffs and defendants to sharpen the persuasive

power of their trial presentations, aid in identifying and avoiding potentially biased jurors at

trial, enhance the presentation skills of fact and expert witnesses, and to help educate clients

about the most likely settlement value of cases that should not be tried. It is a basic fact of

litigation that some cases are tougher than others, and it is another fact that jury consultants

generally see the worst cases out there. We don’t often get called in on easily win-able

cases because lawyers with great cases usually don’t need the extra help. We get called in

on cases that are difficult at best, and often downright impossible, and as a result, we have a

great deal of experience studying what happens in cases where liability is admitted, as well

as what happens in very bad cases where liability is not admitted. This brief paper will

summarize some of those findings. Let’s start with the negatives.

CONS

You’re going to lose something. If your case is bad enough that you are considering

whether to admit liability, you may already be ready to accept this, however, convincing a

client that this might be an acceptable possibility is often rather difficult. No one likes to

admit that they were wrong, and defendants in litigation are affected by the same basic

psychological factors as the rest of us. Even so, it is often possible to get defendants to

understand that wins and losses can be relative. Indeed, we frequently consult on cases

where large amounts of money get paid to plaintiffs and the defendants are quite happy

because it wasn’t a great deal more. It is true that an admission of liability, on its face,

Page 28: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

3

sounds like a complete admission of defeat to many attorneys and their clients, but in cases

with bad facts and significant exposure, an admission of liability can actually form the basis

of a relatively successful defense.

PROS

Gaining credibility with the jury. The greatest benefit to an admission of liability is the

potential to gain an enormous amount of credibility with the jury, and in doing so, to enable

them to take your remaining arguments seriously. When the facts of a case are such that a

strong denial of liability is totally contrary to jurors’ interpretations of those facts, it is only

natural that taking such a position leads to juror skepticism of the entire defense case. For

example, imagine a collision case where a tractor-trailer runs a red light at high rate of

speed, and there is no evidence of any attempt by the truck driver to either decelerate or

even swerve to avoid the impact. Unfortunately the truck driver has perished in the collision

and won’t be able to testify as to whether or not he saw the light, the other vehicle, the

impending crash, etc. This is admittedly an extreme example, but it is certainly realistic that

some defendants will insist on trying to find a way to shift liability to the driver of a

passenger vehicle who was obliterated on his or her way through the intersection at the

moment the tractor-trailer came barreling through. When a defendant with such facts

contests liability, it makes it very difficult for jurors to listen seriously to anything the

defense says about causation or damages. Similarly, even in cases where there are some

legitimate issues that the defense might raise regarding liability, if the facts do not fit jurors’

expectations about how the world works, jurors are more likely to distrust the entire defense

case, if they already believe that the defendant is at fault for the incident or occurrence at the

heart of the dispute. Thus, an admission of liability (or at least an appropriate portion of it)

Page 29: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

4

as to the basic occurrence at issue can help jurors understand that the defense is not just

denying everything with no rational basis, and hence might be worth listening to on other

issues such as damages.

Contesting causation. There are many cases where mistakes have been made, but there are

issues about whether or not those mistakes were the cause of the injuries claimed by the

plaintiff. Causation arguments are often difficult, but they are certainly more so if jurors

believe that the defendant is liable and the defendant is still persisting in contesting liability

in the first place. An admission of liability can allow a defendant to say with greater

credibility, “It is true that mistakes were made. The job now is to move past the fact that

mistakes were made and analyze whether or not those mistakes really have anything to do

with what the plaintiff is claiming in this case, and ladies and gentlemen of the jury, the

evidence in this case is clear that the mistakes we made have absolutely no connection to the

injuries claimed by the plaintiff.”

Contesting damages. In an analogous manner to causation, damages often need to be

contested because they were not caused by any errors on the part of the defendant, or

because the claimed damages are wildly inflated and/or speculative. In cases where liability

is contested, many times defendants are reluctant to talk about damages at all for fear that

doing so creates an appearance of an admission of liability. In such instances, damages are

often addressed in a very unappealing (from a jury standpoint) way, e.g. “We believe that

the evidence in this case is clear that we didn’t do anything wrong here, but if you think we

did, then the evidence also shows that the plaintiff’s damages calculations are flawed, and if

we owe anything at all (and our position is that we don’t, but if you think we owe anything

at all), it’s this amount, and not the amount claimed by the plaintiff, and here are all of the

Page 30: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

5

reasons why.” Certainly there are better ways to present alternative damages theories in

cases where liability must be contested. The point for present purposes is that an admission

of liability can enable a defendant to bypass such awkward damages discussions, and argue

with a straight face that, “We regret that this accident occurred, and that our driver was at

fault, and we want to do the right thing here and provide reasonable compensation for these

plaintiffs. We are asking you, the jurors, to help us figure out what is reasonable here, and

we believe that if you carefully examine the plaintiff’s calculations, you will see that their

numbers are unreasonable because of the following facts…”

Focusing on co-defendants. There are plenty of cases in which numerous parties,

sometimes including the plaintiff, have contributed to an accident, and the arguments in such

cases often involve lots of finger-pointing and blame-shifting. We know from our pre-trial

research and our post-trial juror interviews that jurors generally take a dim view of the tone

of such arguments. An admission of an appropriate portion of the liability can help make

one defendant’s arguments that other defendants share in the blame appear more reasonable,

especially if the other defendants are still contesting what appears to be fairly clear liability.

Some cases are so bad that the best strategy may actually be to settle with the plaintiff, thus

removing potentially horrible emotional/physical damages from the mix, and then to go back

as the plaintiff against the other defendants and argue, “We did the right thing here. We

have paid these people for the harm they have suffered. Now we are coming to you because

these other defendants don’t want to pay their fair share. The evidence is clear that this

accident was caused not only by our driver, but by the construction company who failed to

post the appropriate warning signs, by the State Transportation Department who knew this

was an accident-prone area yet still failed to properly oversee the construction, etc.”

Page 31: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

6

Avoiding punitive damages. Along with gaining credibility as discussed above, the other

primary benefit to an admission of liability is the possibility of decreasing juror anger, and

thus avoiding punitive damages. If jurors understand that punitive damages are for sending

a message, they may very well believe that the defendant who “just doesn’t get it” is the

defendant who needs to get such a message. An admission of liability, in a particularly bad

case where punitive damages are a real possibility, allows a defendant to attempt to appear

more reasonable, e.g. “This was a terrible tragedy. We understand that this was

unacceptable, and we have made changes to ensure that it never happens again.” This also

sends the unspoken message being that the defense does not need to be sent a punitive

message in this case.

CONCLUSION

Given the benefits discussed above, admitting liability may be the rational thing to

do in some cases. The difficulty often lies in assessing for which cases this holds true and

for which cases liability should be vigorously defended. How does a defendant know in

advance which cases might benefit from such an admission, and which cases might turn out

disastrously worse if such an admission is made? How does a defendant decide whether an

admission of liability will help jurors apportion a greater share of the blame to a co-

defendant? Questions such as these can be answered by conducting applied psychological

research beforehand. Jury studies, using the proper methodologies and analyzed

appropriately by experienced professionals, can be designed to answer such questions, thus

enabling attorneys and their clients to make informed decisions about whether an admission

of liability is appropriate for any given fact pattern.

Page 32: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

© 2005 by DecisionQuest, Inc. All rights reserved.

THE VIEW FROM THE JURY BOX:

JURORS’ REACTIONS TO TECHNOLOGY IN THE COURTROOM

Michael A. Biek, Ph.D.

DecisionQuest

403 S. Allen Street, Suite 204

State College, PA 16801

814-867-4080

[email protected]

Page 33: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

BACKGROUND

How does anyone really know what jurors are thinking as they watch trial

attorneys do their jobs? While it may be difficult or impossible to predict what is going

through any individual juror’s head at any given moment, there are a number of reliable

findings that have emerged through years of psychological research into understanding

juror behavior. As applied social scientists, we study jury behavior in a variety of ways.

We spend most of our time helping attorneys find effective ways to present complex

disputes to people who often do not have much experience dealing with the subject

matter at issue, or even with the task of analyzing complex information in order to decide

how to resolve a dispute. We conduct research studies every week in which

representative potential jurors are presented with arguments and evidence and asked to

evaluate it. They tell us about what they believe and what they don’t believe and about

what they understand and don’t understand about the facts and arguments being

presented. They tell us about what they like in terms of attorney performance, and they

are not shy about telling us what they dislike. We talk to real jurors after real cases and

they tell us exactly the same sorts of things. In addition, we regularly survey national

samples of the jury-eligible population in order to gather quantitative data about jurors’

attitudes and experiences.

In addition to finding out what jurors think about the disputed issues in litigation,

our research activities allow us to gather information about how jurors respond to the use

of technology and demonstrative evidence in the courtroom. This paper will discuss

some of our findings, as well as provide a brief tutorial on juror psychology and

information processing that is important for understanding how jurors view litigants.

Page 34: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

JUROR PSYCHOLOGY

Jurors process the information and evidence in a lawsuit though the filters of their

attitudes and life experiences. They have certain expectations about how the world

works. Regardless of whether those expectations are accurate or inaccurate, they

influence how jurors perceive the evidence at trial. Social psychological research has

demonstrated conclusively that people are more likely to believe and remember points

that are consistent with the way they view the world, and to reject points that are

inconsistent. For example, if a juror has a pre-existing bias that most big companies treat

employees poorly, that juror will tend to believe and remember the plaintiff’s arguments

in an employment case better than they will believe or remember evidence introduced by

the defense.

Jurors’ attitudes may be more influential in some types of cases than in others.

For example, most jurors will have employment experiences that they use to help them

analyze which side they believe is right in an employment discrimination case, but most

jurors do not have a great deal of experience with antitrust or patent issues. Although

they may have general attitudes such as respect for inventors, or suspiciousness of

complex business deals, most are not able to defend such attitudes with concrete

examples in the jury room. Still, jurors will usually try to bring their personal

experiences to bear on the facts of a case. Thus, jurors will interpret a complex breach of

contract case, perhaps involving numerous interrelated, lengthy contracts, in terms of

their own experiences with the contracts they signed to purchase a car or home.

Because the information in civil trials is often complex, it is important to

understand that human beings are not perfect information processors. They are not able

Page 35: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

to attend to, evaluate, and integrate every piece of information presented to them. As a

result, people tend to use cognitive shortcuts, known to social scientists as heuristics.

This is not necessarily a bad thing. People are able to be incredibly efficient information

processors, which is a basic requirement for living in a world flooded with information.

Heuristic information processing mechanisms are adaptive techniques that allow people

to try to make sense of the information that matters, while cutting through the vast

majority of information that doesn’t.

The example given earlier concerning the bias to accept information that is

consistent with one’s attitudes, while rejecting inconsistent information, is based in part

on the availability heuristic. People tend to be influenced more by attitudes that are

readily accessible in memory. The more times a given concept (e.g., tort reform) is

activated or thought about, the more accessible or retrievable it is from memory. More

importantly, the likelihood that the person’s opinion about that topic will surface

spontaneously and influence their evaluation of new information is increased.

HOW JURORS LISTEN

Research also shows that jurors make sense of the conflicting information that

forms the basis of a lawsuit by constructing a story in order to help understand the

conflict. Such stories provide a cognitive structure that helps jurors to understand and

remember evidence. It is well documented that jurors construct such stories early in the

trial process. Evidence that is consistent with the story created tends to be incorporated

into memory, whereas inconsistent evidence is rejected or forgotten. As noted above,

jurors’ pre-existing attitudes, experiences and expectations are important because they are

Page 36: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

largely determinative of how jurors will evaluate the evidence in a case. These factors

also serve as sources of material to fill in gaps in the story left by the lawyers. It is

important for trial counsel to use the narrative story format in presenting cases, and to do

everything possible to ensure that the story being told is consistent with that created by

jurors. The party that presents a story that does not meet jurors’ expectations as to how

the world works is the party that is going to lose.

THE IMPORTANCE OF DEMONSTRATIVE EVIDENCE

Demonstrative evidence forms an important part of the presentation of a case for

several reasons. When integrated strategically with the trial story, demonstrative

evidence helps reinforce key points, helps keep jurors’ attention, and provides cues that

improve jurors’ retention of information.

A. The boredom factor.

Our research shows that jurors often become bored during even the most

exciting trials. What appears to be a hectic pace for counsel can be

excruciatingly slow for jurors who must sit and wait for things to happen.

Jurors are accustomed to very quick sound-bites and persuasive appeals in

their daily lives (e.g., commercials), and they are used to being able to

exercise some control over the information flow (e.g., the remote). A

major value of demonstrative evidence is that it helps break the boredom

cycle. Anything that gets jurors’ attention is valuable, even if only for that

reason.

Page 37: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

B. Comprehension.

Many concepts are more easily presented in a visual, rather than an oral,

manner. This is particularly true of numbers and relationships among

objects. The use of visual aids can help make the evidence clearer to the

jury, and thus more comprehensible. In addition, research shows that

learning style is an idiosyncratic variable. Some people learn better by

listening; some people learn better by reading or watching. The more

options provided to jurors for learning the issues in the case, and the more

routes to persuasion that are used, the better the odds that counsel will

make points with as many jurors as possible.

C. Retention.

Visual information is retained better than oral information. A clear chart

will be better recalled by jurors than will clear oral testimony. The two

modes of presentation used in combination are more effective than either

when used alone, especially when recall is measured at a later period. In

other words, information is retained longer when a presentation is

composed of both oral and visual elements.

D. Black and white makes it real.

Jurors tend to believe that anything that is written down, printed or exists

on paper must be real. An oral statement is far less credible than a black

and white presentation of this same statement. When we ask jurors in

post-trial interviews about why testimony from a key witness was not

persuasive, they often tell us things like, “The witness was just saying it;

Page 38: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

there was no evidence.” Oftentimes jurors never quite grasp the fact that

in our system, statements from witnesses are evidence, hence they tend to

discount testimony that is not supported by documentation. This

observation leads to the following recommendation: when showing

videotaped testimony at trial, synchronize the footage with a rolling

transcript. The addition of the written words along with the oral testimony

increases the likelihood that jurors will perceive the testimony as

“evidence.” Similarly, instead of simply reading prior testimony to a

witness to impeach their current testimony, use document management

software to show the quoted text to the jury at the same time, in order to

make it real for them.

TECHNOLOGY & THE EFFECTIVE USE OF DEMONSTRATIVE EVIDENCE

A. Timing.

Be careful of confusing the jurors with lengthy oral explanations before

presenting graphic evidence. They may have already closed their minds to

the issue you are trying to present to them in graphic form. Also, be

careful not to explain the evidence in so much detail that they are no

longer interested in the graphic evidence when you present it to them.

Prepare jurors to be receptive to graphic evidence by letting them know

you are about to present it, before actually presenting demonstrative

exhibits.

Page 39: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

B. The presentation.

As noted above, you must prepare jurors to view the evidence. You must

explain that it is coming, what it is about, and how you want them to view

it. Then present the graphic evidence and lead them through it. You must

tell them what the demonstrative shows, rather than letting them draw

their own, possibly incorrect, conclusions.

C. Taking advantage of retention.

Because jurors retain visual evidence longer than oral evidence, it is

important to refer back to prior graphics as you proceed through your case.

These pieces of graphic evidence can become visual milestones, or anchor

points to hang your case on, that will be easily remembered by jurors.

D. Pacing.

Pacing is related to timing, as discussed earlier, but the point to remember

here is that the pace you go through a complex document or other exhibit

is crucial. Go too slow and you will lose jurors’ attention and tax their

patience. Go too fast and you risk losing them altogether, or possibly even

creating the impression that you are rushing them because you don’t want

them to see the “whole picture.” This is especially important when you

are highlighting or otherwise drawing attention to an isolated portion of a

document. If counsel appears to skip over material, for instance,

paragraphs before and after the paragraph of interest, jurors may view the

key paragraph as “out of context,” and therefore as not credible.

Page 40: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

WHAT JURORS TELL US

The following are some of the more important things jurors tell us relevant to

technology, demonstrative evidence, and trial conduct in general.

A. Teach me.

Jurors want to do the right thing. They want to make correct, fair and just

decisions. In order to do that, they need to understand the facts and issues.

Even though this sounds obvious, the fact that attorneys must serve as

teachers as well as advocates is often forgotten or neglected. Especially in

complex litigation, the ability to teach jurors, and make them feel like they

really understand the issues, is crucial. The side that is able to get jurors

to say, “They made me feel like I really understood it,” is at a tremendous

advantage. The effective use of technology to present demonstrative

evidence, such as charts, lists of issues, timelines, representations of

complex processes, etc., can provide a significant advantage in this effort.

B. Keep it simple, but don’t condescend.

Jurors are generally smarter and more capable than they are given credit

for. To the extent that they make “mistakes” in decision-making, it is

often due to the fact that presentations by counsel and witnesses are

disjointed or full of jargon. Often the legal instructions and interrogatories

are incomprehensible to lay people. Graphic evidence can help

tremendously in simplifying complex material without making jurors feel

inferior. Likewise, computer animations or re-creations, or even relatively

simple physical models, can be helpful in summarizing lengthy testimony

Page 41: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

in a way that is much easier to understand than trying to put together the

point that is supposed to be drawn from a witness’ answers.

C. Don’t play tricks or games.

Many jurors are cynical about attorneys’ motivations. They understand

that it is an adversarial system, and they expect attorneys to fight hard on

behalf of their clients. This leads many jurors to expect bad behavior;

therefore, they are attuned to anything that strikes them as suspicious. As

noted earlier, jurors often suspect that demonstrative evidence is being

used to mislead them. Although there are many technological tools now

available to manage and present evidence, care must be taken to provide

jurors with context, and to make them feel that they are getting the “whole

picture,” in order for them to trust demonstrative evidence. If jurors

suspect for an instant that portions of a document are being called out and

highlighted in order to draw attention away from something else in the

document, use of such technologies can have a detrimental effect on

jurors’ perceptions.

D. Be professional.

Jurors want attorneys to be consummate professionals. They expect a

thorough command of the facts and physical materials, and they resent

having their time wasted. An attorney who has frequent difficulty

managing exhibits, e.g., “If the witness will please turn to Tab 15 in

Binder 3…I’m sorry…Tab 12 in binder 2,” is at a disadvantage. Jurors

appreciate the attorney who is readily able to find and use documents and

Page 42: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

exhibits, who gets to the point, and who makes things clear without being

overly repetitive. Competent handling of demonstrative evidence goes a

long way toward generating a perception of competence generally. The

use of document management software and electronic presentation

systems can help attorneys meet jurors’ expectations for a smooth and

organized presentation.

E. We are not afraid of technology.

We find that jurors are much less afraid of technology than many counsel,

or even than counsel’s clients. Jurors are accustomed to getting their

information from television and computer screens. The fear that a party

using technology will be perceived as “too slick” is unfounded. In fact,

we virtually never hear jurors express this view; rather, they will often

make negative comments about the side that eschews technology, e.g.

“They were old-school.” Some clients worry that using technology may

make it appear that they have too many resources, or perpetuate a “David

vs. Goliath” dynamic. Here is a tip: jurors already believe that you have

resources, and they expect you to use them. No jury is fooled by (or

sympathetic toward) an attorney who appears to be trying a case on a

shoe-string budget. Jurors expect technology to be used as part of modern

presentations. The fact that new software tools allow instant, random

access to digitized documents and videotape, as well as the means to

manipulate such evidence on the fly (highlighting, enlargement, etc.)

allows attorneys to use demonstratives quickly, simply and effectively. In

Page 43: “Substantive Best Practices” Best Practice in Jury Trialsmedia.dsba.org/PreAdmitMaterials/29-Best Practices-Jury... · 2016. 11. 7. · Social Media & the Jury. “Facebook, Twitter,

some cases, counsel can even forgo the production of traditional court

boards, because the same content can be easily displayed on screens or

monitors around the courtroom. Rather than boxes and boxes of

documents cluttering up the courtroom, it is becoming more frequent to

see counsel tables adorned only with laptop computers. Jurors are

perfectly comfortable with this. It is crucial, however, that counsel using

such technology be comfortable with it as well. Having a system freeze

up, or even to be awkward to use, can be a disaster. Use technical people

who know what they are doing, have back-up options available, and

practice extensively before using high-tech tools in front of a Judge or

jury. All of the above notwithstanding, it is important to vary the media to

keep jurors’ attention. Also, remember that “low-tech” exhibits can still

be among the most effective. An attorney who can teach the jury by

summarizing key points on a flip chart as they go often spontaneously

produces the key exhibit that wins the case.