Top Banner
© European Delirium Association 2012 www.europeandeliriumassociation.com Annals of Delirium July 2012 Editorial A Delirium Tipping Point Why does delirium continue to be persistently under recognised and mismanaged across the health care spectrum? What reasons do we have to explain the fact that despite sound ethical, empirical and economical arguments this highly preventable condition continues to be ignored by healthcare planners? Perhaps most perplexing, how can such a devastating disease remain invisible to the general public for so long? Conspicuous by their absence are patient and carer delirium support groups. These are the questions we, as an emerging group of passionate clinicians, researchers and educators within the European Delirium Association, wrestle with on a daily basis. The answers are undoubtedly complex and to use a favourite word amongst delirium cognoscenti, "multifactorial". Arguably a toxic mix of the complexity of the illness together with an, as yet, comparatively less well understood pathophysiology may account for the lack of ownership encountered at so many different levels. In trying to piece together the pieces of this jigsaw puzzle and answer the big questions I am reminded of the efforts of the committed group of clinicians and researchers led by Blessed and Roth studying dementia in the late 60s and early 70s. The challenge was great however by a series of small but significant steps, all of which increased our understanding of dementia, a tipping point was reached. Tackling the issue of complexity, in this edition of Annals, Dr Emma Vardy offers a personal insight into the problems associated with applying the Confusion Assessment Method in the acute setting. The challenges in unravelling the pathophysiology of delirium are well known and in a cutting edge article Dr Colm Cunningham considers the value of animal models in basic sciences. Importantly, he is not afraid to honestly answer the key questions as we try to link bench to clinical findings. So as we increase our understanding of delirium, how near are we to reaching a delirium tipping point? The challenges remain in terms of recognition and raising awareness however two recent events that I have encountered provide hope that we may not be far away. Firstly, whilst visiting a Dignity award winning Trust in Birmingham, I was introduced to a simple yet effective initiative called, ”Another cup of tea and a slice of cake". This very English innovation involves trying to reduce dehydration and malnutrition as well as increase stimulation on wards. With military precision volunteers, managed by an activities coordinator, facilitate a posh tea party within each ward bay complete with china plates, fine tea and cake. When asked what the driver for this was staff were quite clear it was about preventing delirium through good hydration and nutrition. The collective response made me wonder whether finally staff recognise the importance of delirium prevention and crucially link delirium to dignity.
16

Annals of Delirium July 2012

Oct 26, 2014

Download

Documents

The Annals of Delirium is published by the European Delirium Association
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Annals of Delirium July 2012

 

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Annals of Delirium July 2012 Editorial

A Delirium Tipping Point

Why  does  delirium  continue   to  be  persistently  under  recognised  and  mismanaged   across   the   health   care   spectrum?  What   reasons   do   we  have   to   explain   the   fact   that   despite   sound   ethical,   empirical   and  economical  arguments  this  highly  preventable  condition  continues  to  be  ignored  by  healthcare  planners?  Perhaps  most  perplexing,  how  can  such  a  devastating  disease  remain  invisible  to  the  general  public  for  so  long?   Conspicuous   by   their   absence   are   patient   and   carer   delirium  support  groups.  

These   are   the   questions   we,   as   an   emerging   group   of   passionate  clinicians,   researchers   and   educators   within   the   European   Delirium  Association,   wrestle   with   on   a   daily   basis.   The   answers   are  undoubtedly   complex   and   to   use   a   favourite  word   amongst   delirium  cognoscenti,  "multifactorial".  Arguably  a  toxic  mix  of  the  complexity  of  the  illness  together  with  an,  as  yet,  comparatively  less  well  understood  pathophysiology  may  account   for   the   lack  of  ownership  encountered  at  so  many  different  levels.      

In  trying  to  piece  together  the  pieces  of  this  jigsaw  puzzle  and  answer  the  big  questions  I  am  reminded  of  the  efforts  of  the  committed  group  of   clinicians   and   researchers   led   by   Blessed   and   Roth   studying  dementia   in   the   late   60s   and   early   70s.   The   challenge   was   great  however   by   a   series   of   small   but   significant   steps,   all   of   which  increased  our  understanding  of  dementia,  a  tipping  point  was  reached.  

 

 

 

Tackling   the   issue   of   complexity,   in   this   edition   of   Annals,   Dr   Emma  Vardy   offers   a   personal   insight   into   the   problems   associated   with  applying   the  Confusion  Assessment  Method   in   the   acute   setting.   The  challenges   in   unravelling   the   pathophysiology   of   delirium   are   well  known   and   in   a   cutting   edge   article   Dr   Colm   Cunningham   considers  the   value   of   animal   models   in   basic   sciences.   Importantly,   he   is   not  afraid  to  honestly  answer  the  key  questions  as  we  try  to  link  bench  to  clinical  findings.        

So  as  we  increase  our  understanding  of  delirium,  how  near  are  we  to  reaching  a  delirium  tipping  point?  The  challenges  remain   in   terms  of  recognition   and   raising   awareness   however   two   recent   events   that   I  have  encountered  provide  hope  that  we  may  not  be  far  away.    

Firstly,  whilst  visiting  a  Dignity  award  winning  Trust  in  Birmingham,  I  was  introduced  to  a  simple  yet  effective  initiative  called,  ”Another  cup  of  tea  and  a  slice  of  cake".  This  very  English  innovation  involves  trying  to   reduce   dehydration   and   malnutrition   as   well   as   increase  stimulation  on  wards.  With  military  precision  volunteers,  managed  by  an  activities  coordinator,   facilitate  a  posh  tea  party  within  each  ward  bay  complete  with  china  plates,   fine  tea  and  cake.    When  asked  what  the  driver   for   this  was  staff  were  quite  clear   it  was  about  preventing  delirium   through   good   hydration   and   nutrition.   The   collective  response   made   me   wonder   whether   finally   staff   recognise   the  importance   of   delirium   prevention   and   crucially   link   delirium   to  dignity.  

 

Page 2: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

The  second  delirium  story  of  hope  occurred  recently  when  assessing  an   OSCE   examination   station   for   medical   students.   The   task   for   the  students  was  to  explain  a  diagnosis  of  delirium  to  a  distressed  relative.    As   expected   they   struggled   with   describing   delirium   indicators  confidently.  However,   since   assessment   drives   learning,   the   fact   that  delirium  was   a   core   part   of   the   exam   process   represents   significant  progress  for  tomorrows  doctors.  Even  more  encouraging  was  a  chance  remark  from  the  seasoned  role  player  playing  the  part  of  the  relative  of   a   delirious   patient.   Reflecting   back   on   the   session   she   specifically  mentioned   how   real   and   important   the   scenario   was.   Finally   it  appears   the   reality   that   delirium   is  more   than   an   epiphenomenon   is  being  recognised.    

Of  course  these  are  but  two  stories  of  hope  but  we  should  take  heart  from  them.  If  we  continue  on  our  quest  for  greater  understanding  and  couple   it   with   raising   delirium   on   the   public   agenda  we   can   remain  confident   that   a   larger   number   of   such   positive   encounters   will  emerge.  Different   challenges  will   present   themselves  however  under  recognition  may  no  longer  be  chief  amongst  them.  

Dr  Andrew  Teodorczuk  

A new animal model with potential to reconcile and dissect cholinergic and neuroinflammatory hypotheses of delirium

Colm Cunningham, Delirium Basic Research Group, Trinity College Dublin

It   has   been   recognized   for   some   time   that   existing   memory  impairment   is   one   of   the   biggest   risk   factors   for   the   subsequent  occurrence   of   delirium.   However,   it   is   not   clear   how   this   prior  impairment  interacts  with  the  typical  acute  precipitants  of  delirium,  to  induce  the  deficits  observed  in  patients.  This  is  just  one  among  many  

questions  in  delirium  pathophysiology  that  remain  unanswered.

The   lack   of   animal  models   relevant   to   delirium  pathophysiology   has  been   one   of   the   significant   gaps   in   the   delirium   research   field   and  recent   studies   by   our   laboratory  have   tried   to   address   this   problem,  using   new   mouse   models   to   replicate   aspects   of   prior   degenerative  pathology  and  superimposed  systemic  inflammation  (Field  et  al,  2012;  Murray   et   al.,   2012).   Significantly,   we   showed   in   the   first   of   these  studies,   that   mild/moderate   systemic   inflammation   in   a   normal  animal  is  insufficient  to  induce  working  memory  deficits  but  when  the  same  inflammatory  challenge  is  made  in  animals  with  prior  pathology,  these  animals  now  show  acute  and  transient  working  memory  deficits  (Murray  et  al.,  2012).  This  ‘prior  pathology’  consists  of  robust  synaptic  loss   in  the  hippocampus  and  thalamus,   induced  by  the  ME7  model  of  prion  disease,  and  microglia  that  are  primed  by  the  primary  pathology  to  produce  exaggerated  responses  to  subsequent  inflammatory  insult  (Cunningham   et   al.,   2005).   Many   researchers   might   argue   that   this  prion   disease   model   is   not   directly   relevant   to   more   common  predisposing   factors   for   delirium,   such   as   Alzheimer’s   disease   and  age-­‐associated  cognitive  impairment.  While  this  is  an  understandable  view,   what   these   data   show,   unequivocally,   is   that   where   there   is  existing  pathology  or  ‘vulnerability’  in  particular  regions  of  the  brain,  systemic   inflammation   can   selectively   disrupt   function   in   those  regions  more  easily  than  it  can  in  normal  animals.  As  such,  it  provides  a   plausible   recapitulation   of   the   observation   that   systemic   infection  can   produce   profound   CNS   effects   in   some   patients   while   leaving  others   unaffected.   Understanding   this   dichotomy   is   central   to  elucidating  delirium  pathophysiology.  Useful  mechanistic  information  will  emerge  from  this  model.  

Making  a  better  model  

Nonetheless,  there  are  good  reasons  to  try  to  take  this  observation  out  of   the   prion   disease   setting   and   replicate   it   in   a   neuropathological  

Page 3: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

setting  not  involving  prions  and  in  a  neuroanatomical/neurochemical  setting   that   is   relevant   to   delirium   as   it   is   observed   in   the   geriatric  population.   Prominent   hypotheses   of   delirium   pathophysiology  include  both  hypocholinergic  and  neuroinflammatory  hypotheses  and  whether  these  are  parallel  and/or  interacting  is  largely  unknown.    

Last  month  in  The  Journal  of  Neuroscience  we  published  new  findings  (Field   et   al.,   2012)   showing   that   systemic   inflammation   induced   by  gram-­‐negative   bacterial   endotoxin   (LPS)   induces   working   memory  deficits,  but  only  in  animals  with  prior  pathology  in  the  basal  forebrain  cholinergic   nuclei   (BFCN).   The   BFCN,   which   comprises   the   medial  septum,   the  diagonal  bands  and  the  nucleus  basalis   (see  Figure  1),   is  the   source   of   most   acetylcholine   in   the   forebrain   and   this   area  degenerates   markedly   during   Alzheimer’s   disease.   To   specifically  lesion   this   region   we   used   the   ribosomal   toxin   saporin,   covalently  linked  to  an  antibody  directed  against  the  p75  neurotrophin  receptor  that   is   highly   expressed   on   basal   forebrain   cholinergic   neurons.  We  showed   that   intracerebroventricular   injection   of   this   toxin   (p75NTR-­‐saporin)  at  low  doses  could  induce  approximately  20%  destruction  of  the   cholinergic   cells   of   the  medial   septum  without   obvious   effect   on  working  memory  function.  However,  if  these  animals  were  allowed  to  recover   for  40  days  post-­‐surgery,   and   then  challenged  with   systemic  LPS,   only   those   animals   with   prior   lesions   showed   acute   working  memory   deficits.   Many   of   our   previous   predictions   about   the  interaction  of  prior  pathology  and  systemic  inflammation  were  based  on   the   original   demonstration   of  microglial   priming   and   subsequent  exaggerated   inflammatory  responses  to  systemic   inflammatory   insult  (Cunningham   et   al.,   2005).   However,   40   days   after   p75NTR-­‐saporin  lesions,   microglia   did   not   show   exaggerated   inflammatory   cytokine  induction   after   systemic   inflammation.   Thus   the   acute   working  memory  deficits  appear  to  occur  in  the  absence  of  microglial  priming.  

This  independence  of  microglial  priming  does  not  imply  a  lack  of  CNS  inflammatory   response   to   systemic   LPS,   it   simply   means   that  

microglia  appear   to   respond  equally   to   systemic   inflammation   in   the  lesioned   and   normal   animals.   This   implies   that   the   ‘vulnerability’   in  these   animals   represents   a   neuronal   susceptibility   to   disruption   of  function.   We   showed,   using   the   acetylcholine   muscarinic   receptor  antagonist   scopolamine,   that   the  T-­‐maze  working  memory   task  used  in   these   studies   was   indeed   dependent   on   cholinergic   function   and  further  showed  that  treatment  with  the  acetylcholinesterase  inhibitor  donepezil,   1   hour   after   LPS,   protected   against   the   working   memory  deficit  observed.  Collectively  these  data  show  that   the   loss  of  20%  of  cholinergic  neurons  of  the  basal  forebrain  cholinergic  system  does  not  robustly  affect  cognitive  function  under  normal  conditions,  but  leaves  these   animals   vulnerable   to   significant   cognitive   disruption   upon   an  acute   systemic   inflammatory   insult.   The   data   also   show   that  preserving  acetylcholine  levels  using  an  acetylcholinesterase  inhibitor  is  protective   in   this   setting,  which  may  have   relevance   for   the   recent  trial  of  rivastigmine  for  ICU  delirium  (discussed  below).    

 

Figure  1  

 

 

Page 4: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

 

We   believe   that   these   findings   have   significant   implications   for  delirium,   particularly   in   the   setting   of   existing   cognitive  

impairment/dementia   and   that   this   limited   cholinergic   lesion  model  will  be  an  extremely  useful  tool  in  delineating  molecular  pathways  to  dysfunction   that   are   relevant   to   delirium.   However,   it   is   clear   that  many   delirium   researchers   are   hesitant   about   the   application,   to  clinical   delirium,   of   data   arising   from   these   animal   models   and   a  number  of  pertinent  questions  do  arise.  

Q Can we really say that these mice are delirious?

Q Is this a hippocampal phenomenon?

Q What, therefore, is the role of the prefrontal cortex?

The  occurrence  of  working  memory  deficits  using  a  task  that  has  been  shown   to   be   hippocampal-­‐dependent   has   made   it   easy   to   ignore   or  dismiss   these  data  as  not   relevant   to  delirium.  We  do  not   imply   that  delirium   is   a   hippocampal   phenomenon.   Firstly,   we   cannot   yet   say  what   other   regions   of   the   brain   are   also   recruited   by   this   task.  Secondly,  the  induction  of  these  cognitive  deficits  by  recapitulation  of  key   etiological   factors   for   delirium   must   be   emphasized:   there   are  simply   no   other   studies   that   have   even   attempted   to   address   the  multi-­‐factorial  nature  of  dysfunction  during  delirium  and  as  such,  the  data  from  our  studies  on  the  interaction  of  systemic  inflammation  and  prior  CNS  pathology  have  a  utility  for  delirium  research  irrespective  of  whether   the   cognitive   impairments   observed   constitute   delirium  per  se.   Thirdly,   it   is   important   to   stress   that  mice   and  men   are   different  and  while  we   cannot   say   that   these  mice   are  definitely  delirious  nor  can  we  assert  that  they  are  not.  However,  we  can  say  that  they  have  an  acute   onset   and   transient   attentional/working   memory   deficit,  induced   by   a   systemic   inflammatory   challenge,  which   occurs   only   in  mice   with   prior   cholinergic   pathology.   Asking   the   most   relevant  questions   of   mice   during   acute   episodes   of   sickness,   in   which  appetitive   and   exploratory   drives   are   profoundly   suppressed   is  challenging.   The   water-­‐motivated,   working   memory,   T-­‐maze  

Figure  1  

Prior  pathology  and  superimposed  systemic   inflammation   interact  to  produce   acute   and   transient  working  memory  deficits:   towards  reconciling   cholinergic   and   inflammatory   hypotheses   of   delirium.  This   scheme   shows   the   basal   forebrain   cholinergic   nuclei   (BFCN),  illustrating   the   septohippocampal   pathway,   the   ventral   diagonal  band  projections  to  the  amygdala  and  piriform  and  insular  cortices  and   the   nucleus   basalis   projection   to   the   frontal,   parietal   and  temporal   cortices   (after   Wolff   1991).   Intracerebroventricular  injection   (left   panel)   of   the   ribosomal   toxin   saporin,   linked   to   an  antibody  against  the  p75  neurotrophin  receptor,  which  is  enriched  on   BFCN   cells,   induces   selective   neuronal   death   in   these   regions.  This  approach  facilitates  targeted,  selective  and  limitable   lesioning  of  this  area  and  consequently  decreased  cholinergic  tone  (shown  in  grey,  right  panel)  in  these  projection  fields.  In  lesioned  animals,  LPS  induces   acute   and   transient  working  memory   deficits,   resembling  delirium.   Neither   lesion   nor   LPS   alone   were   sufficient   to   induce  such   deficits.   LPS-­‐induced   CNS   inflammatory   responses   were  equivalent   in   lesioned   and   non-­‐lesioned   animals   and   the  acetylcholinesterase   inhibitor   donepezil   could   block   these   deficits  (Field   et   al.,   2012).   Thus   systemic   inflammation   in   ‘vulnerable’  animals   may   be   used   to   interrogate   aspects   of   delirium  pathophysiology  

Page 5: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

represents   a   task   that   is   tailored   to   assess   correct   and   incorrect  responses  in  a  task  reliant  on  both  attention  and  working  memory,  in  a   manner   that   is   not   confounded   by   sickness-­‐induced   performance  deficits  such  as  suppressed  activity,  slower  response  time,  suppressed  appetite   etc.   In   order   to   successfully   solve   the   T-­‐maze   task,   animals  must  attend   to   the  maze  exit  on   first  entry,  be  attentive   to   the  body-­‐turn   they   made   to   escape   the   maze   and   recall   this   choice   just   30  seconds   later,   in   order   to   make   the   opposite   turn   to   find   the   new  location  of  the  exit  on  re-­‐exposure  to  the  maze.  Whether  the  deficit  is  attentional  or  purely  working  memory   is  difficult   to  dissect,   but   it   is  undeniably  distinct  from  longer  term,  or  reference,  memory.  We  have  clearly  shown  that  mice  are  not  impaired  on  that  type  of  hippocampal-­‐memory   after   LPS   treatment   (Cunningham   et   al.,   2009).   This   is  consistent  with  data  showing  that  patients  with  delirium  were  able  to  access   previously   learned   information   but   showed   impairments   on  tasks   involving   online   processing   of   novel,   trial   specific,   information  (Brown  et  al.,  2011).  Thus   there  are   important  parallels  between  the  type   of   cogntive   impairments   seen   in   delirium   and   those   in   animals  with   cholinergic   “vulnerability”:   we   are   not   simply   describing   a  generalised  hippocampal  deficit.  

As   for   the   relative   contribution   of   the   basal   forebrain,   the  hippocampus  and   the  prefrontal   cortex,   it   appears   that   the  dose  and  route  of  p75NTR-­‐saporin  used   in   the  current  study  appeared   to   target  the   septal   neurons   that   project   to   the   hippocampus  more   than   they  affected   the   nucleus   basalis   neurons   that   project   to   the   prefrontal  cortex.   This   latter   path   and   the  prefrontal   cortex   in   general,   are   less  developed   in  mice   than   in   humans   and   this   is   a   limitation   of  mouse  research  for  many  fields  of  neuroscience,  including  delirium.  This  does  not  invalidate  the  use  of  mice  to  investigate  the  mechanisms  by  which  systemic   inflammation   and   prior   CNS   pathology   interact.   It   is  worth  noting   that   most   neuropsychological   and   even   neurodegenerative  conditions,   including   depression,   schizophrenia,   Alzheimer’s   disease  and   Parkinson’s   disease,   are   studied   using   models   that   do   not  

recapitulate  several  key  features  of  these  conditions.  This  is  the  reality  in  developing  and  using  animal  models  and  the   important   thing   is   to  focus  on  the  questions  that  these  models  can  answer.  

Q Despite the long-standing cholinergic hypothesis of delirium, there are now data showing that acetylcholinesterase inhibition is not helpful in ICU delirium. Can these findings be reconciled with the current data?

The   clinical   study   of   van   Eijk   and   colleagues   (2010)   found   no  protective  effect  of  rivastigmine  against  delirium  in  the  ICU.  However,  our   data   are   most   relevant   to   patients   where   there   is   existing  cholinergic   vulnerability,   as   is   frequently   the   case   in   Alzheimer’s  disease   and   in   some   other   age-­‐related   CNS   pathologies.   If   the  individual  has  neurodegeneration   in   the  cholinergic  system  and   then  suffers  a  moderate   systemic   inflammatory   insult   such  as   infection  or  surgery,   it   is   plausible   that   boosting   cholinergic   function   would  protect  against  the  deleterious  effects  of  this  inflammation,  as  we  have  shown   to   be   the   case   in   our  mouse   study.   However,   in   ICU   patients  who  have  suffered  severe  trauma  or  severe  sepsis,  we  believe  that  it  is  highly   unlikely   that   bolstering   a   single   neurotransmitter   system  will  be   able   to   correct   the   marked   divergence   from   homeostasis   in  multiple   systems,   including   severe   inflammation,   impaired   tissue  perfusion   and   hypoxia,   blood   brain   barrier   breakdown   and   multi  organ   dysfunction.   Obviously   the   multiple,   often   quite   divergent,  routes   to  delirium  will   influence   correct   treatment   strategies.  We  do  not   propose   that   cholinergic   dysfunction   is   the   ‘final   common  pathway’   in   delirium  pathhophysiology   but  we   believe   that   our   data  demonstrate  that  systemic  inflammation  and  cholinergic  vulnerability  interact   to   induce   acute   attentional   and/or  working  memory  deficits  and   in   that   scenario,   bolstering   cholinergic   function  was   found   to  be  beneficial.   This   clearly   has   relevance   for   delirium,   at   least   in   the  geriatric   population.   The   fact   that   donepezil   offered   only   partial  protection   even   in   animals   that   had   a   known   cholinergic   deficit  

Page 6: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

already   actually   predicts   that   cholinergic   enhancement   would   be  unlikely   to   be   beneficial   in   settings   such   as   ICU   delirium.   Therefore  this   study   and   the   results   of   the   rivastigmine   trial   are   by   no  means  incompatible  and  this  animal  model  allows  us  to  ask  simple  questions  in  this  domain  and  to  provide  empirical  evidence  that  can  contribute  to   supporting   or   rejecting   clinical   hypotheses.   We   now   know   that  moderate   systemic   inflammation   can   produce   dysfunction   in   an  already   compromised   cholinergic   system   but   cannot   do   so   in   intact  animals.  Severe  trauma  and  sepsis  is  likely  to  lead  to  CNS  dysfunction  by   multiple   routes,   against   which   cholinesterase   inhibition   is  apparently   powerless.   Just   as   it   is   too   simplistic   to   assume   that  hypocholinergic   function   is   the   final   common   pathway   to   delirium  simply  because   robust   cholinergic   inhibition   can  produce   a  delirious  state,   so   is   it   unwise   to   reject   the   importance  of   cholinergic   function  just  because  cholinesterase  inhibition  did  not  protect  against  delirium  in  one  profoundly  sick  population.    

Q Since the deficits observed here are independent of microglial priming, does this rule out a role for this phenomenon in delirium?

What   these   data   show   is   that   loss   of   cholinergic   input   is,   of   itself,  sufficient   to   make   neurons   proximal   to   the   terminal   fields   of  cholinergic   neurons   susceptible   to   dysfunction   upon   exposure   to  inflammatory  mediators.  This  means   that  priming   is  not  essential   for  the   acute   dysfunction.   However,   priming   leads   to   higher   levels   of  inflammatory   mediators   being   produced   in   the   areas   of   prior  pathology   and   thus   their   presence   would   be   predicted   to   further  exacerbate   the   effects.   Therefore,   there   are   both   inflammatory   and  neuronal  vulnerabilities   to  consider.  There  are   two  main   factors   that  might  have  influenced  microglial  priming  in  this  model:  the  time  post-­‐surgical   injection   of   p75NTR-­‐saporin   and   the   degree   of   loss   of  cholinergic  input.  

1)   Time   post-­‐surgery:   CNS   pathology   primes  microglia   and  we   have  previously   shown   that   this   can   last   up   to   28   days   post   axonal  degeneration   (Palin  et   al,   2008).   It   is   clearly  no   longer  present  at  40  days  post-­‐insult  in  the  current  study  and  we  must  investigate  whether  priming  actually  occurs  and  how  long  it  lasts  in  the  current  model.  2)  Loss   of   cholinergic   tone:   acetylcholine   can   directly   suppress  macrophage/microglial  activation  (Tracey  et  al.  2007).  We  show  that  a  limited   cholinergic   lesion   (<20%)   does   not   prime   the   microglia.  However,   one   might   predict   that   larger   lesions   will   be   required   to  remove   sufficient   acetylcholine   to   lift   this   suppression   of   microglia  activity.   This   is   also   under   investigation.   The   prediction   is   that   if  cholinergic  dysfunction  is  sufficient  for  acute  deficits,  in  the  absence  of  microglial   priming,   then   the   additional   factor   of   microglial   priming  will  only  add  to  the  severity  and  duration  of  these  deficits.  

Concluding remarks

In  considering  the  value  of  this  and  other  animal  models,  purportedly  relevant   to   delirium,   clinical   researchers  must   be  measured   in   their  expectations.   It   may   be   difficult   to   recapitulate   delirium   in   a  mouse  and  even  if  one  can  achieve  this  it  will  be  extremely  difficult  to  verify  that  mice   are   indeed   delirious.  What  we  must   focus   on   is   recreating  key   etiologies   that   lead   to   delirium   in   the   clinic,   demonstrating   that  these   factors   interact   in   ways   that   are   consistent   with   clinical  observations   and   then   using   these   models   to   delineate   mechanisms  and  to  make  predictions  for  ongoing  clinical  studies.    

In   other   words,   we   must   focus   on   what   these   models   do   provide  rather   than   on   what   they   do   not   provide.   Our   recent   studies   show  unequivocally  that  systemic  inflammation  induces  acute  and  transient  attentional/working  memory  deficits,  which  are  relevant  to  delirium,  in   animals   with   prior   pathology   in   the   basal   forebrain   cholinergic  

Page 7: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

system.  Whether  or  not  these  animals  fit  all  the  criteria  for  delirium,  in  as  much  as  anyone  can  definitively  say  what  these  criteria  should  be  in  a   mouse,   they   offer   a   unique   tool   to   investigate   the   interaction  between  cholinergic  and  neuroinflammatory  routes  to  acute        

 neuropsychological   dysfunction   and   this   already   represents   a  significant  advance  on  previous  knowledge  in  this   field.   In  particular,  this  model   allows   us   to   interrogate  whether   it   is  microglial   priming,  hypocholinergia   or   some   other   aspect   of   neuronal   susceptibility,   or  indeed   all   three   that   create   the   susceptibility   to   systemic   infection-­‐induced  dysfunction   in   vulnerable   individuals.   The  data   arising   from  such  models  should  obviously  form  part  of  the  discussion  on  delirium  pathophysiology.  

Full reference for citation

Prior   Pathology   in   the   Basal   Forebrain   Cholinergic   System  Predisposes   to   Inflammation-­‐Induced   Working   Memory   Deficits:  Reconciling   Inflammatory   and   Cholinergic   Hypotheses   of   Delirium.  The  Journal  of  Neuroscience,  May  2,  2012  •  32(18):6288  –  6294  

Robert  H.  Field,  Anna  Gossen,  and  Colm  Cunningham  This article is freely available (open access) through The Journal of Neuroscience References Fiel  Murray,  C.,  Sanderson,  D.J.,  Barkus,  C.,  Deacon,  R.M.,  Rawlins,   J.N.,  Bannerman,   D.M.   &   Cunningham,   C.   (2012)   Systemic   inflammation  induces  acute  working  memory  deficits  in  the  primed  brain:  relevance  for  delirium.  Neurobiol  Aging,  33,  603-­‐616  e  603.  

Field,  R.H.,  Gossen,  A.  &  Cunningham,  C.  (2012)  Prior  pathology  in  the  basal  forebrain  cholinergic  system  predisposes  to  inflammation  induced  working  memory  deficits:  reconciling  inflammatory  and  

cholinergic  hypotheses  of  delirium.  Journal  of  Neuroscience,  32  6288-­‐6294.  

Cunningham,  C.,  Wilcockson,  D.C.,  Campion,  S.,  Lunnon,  K.  &  Perry,  V.H.  (2005)  Central  and  systemic  endotoxin  challenges  exacerbate  the  local  inflammatory  response  and  increase  neuronal  death  during  chronic  neurodegeneration.  J  Neurosci,  25,  9275-­‐9284.  

Cunningham,  C.,  Campion,  S.,  Lunnon,  K.,  Murray,  C.L.,  Woods,  J.F.,  Deacon,  R.M.,  Rawlins,  J.N.  &  Perry,  V.H.  (2009)  Systemic  inflammation  induces  acute  behavioral  and  cognitive  changes  and  accelerates  neurodegenerative  disease.  Biol  Psychiatry,  65,  304-­‐312.  

Brown,  L.J.,  Ferner,  H.S.,  Robertson,  J.,  Mills,  N.L.,  Pessotto,  R.,  Deary,  I.J.  &  MacLullich,  A.M.  (2011)  Differential  effects  of  delirium  on  fluid  and  crystallized  cognitive  abilities.  Arch  Gerontol  Geriatr,  52,  153-­‐158.  

van  Eijk,  M.M.,  Roes,  K.C.,  Honing,  M.L.,  Kuiper,  M.A.,  Karakus,  A.,  van  der  Jagt,  M.,  Spronk,  P.E.,  van  Gool,  W.A.,  van  der  Mast,  R.C.,  Kesecioglu,  J.  &  Slooter,  A.J.  (2010)  Effect  of  rivastigmine  as  an  adjunct  to  usual  care  with  haloperidol  on  duration  of  delirium  and  mortality  in  critically  ill  patients:  a  multicentre,  double-­‐blind,  placebo-­‐controlled  randomised  trial.  Lancet,  376,  1829-­‐1837.  

Palin,  K.,  Cunningham,  C.,  Forse,  P.,  Perry,  V.H.  &  Platt,  N.  (2008)  Systemic  inflammation  switches  the  inflammatory  cytokine  profile  in  CNS  Wallerian  degeneration.  Neurobiol  Dis,  30,  19-­‐29.  

Tracey,  K.J.  (2007)  Physiology  and  immunology  of  the  cholinergic  antiinflammatory  pathway.  J  Clin  Invest,  117,  289-­‐296  

Page 8: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Personal experience of the complexity of diagnosing delirium

 Dr  Emma  Vardy  

I   am   a   Consultant   Geriatrician   and   Associate   Clinical   lecturer.     My  clinical   role   involves   elderly   medical   inpatient   work   as   well   as  providing  an  older  peoples  liaison  service  on  the  general  medical  and  medical   assessment   wards.     Until   recently   my   research   interest   has  focussed   on   dementia,   mainly   Alzheimer’s   disease.     More   recently   I  have   become   more   interested   in   delirium   at   both   a   clinical   and   a  research  level.    This  opinion  piece  describes  my  journey  into  delirium:  

The   continuing   development   of   any   healthcare   professional   involves  being  open  to  the  fact  that  there  is  always  something  to  learn,  even  on  subjects   about   which   you   thought   you   knew   a   lot!     I   shall   describe  here  my  recent  re-­‐evaluation  of  my  skills  in  the  diagnosis  of  delirium.  

Through  preparing  a  protocol  for  a  piece  of  clinical  research  I  started  to   explore   the   ‘gold   standard’   method   for   diagnosis   of   delirium.    Perhaps   to   be   expected  was   the   fact   that   there  were   of   course   tests  that   I   had   not   any   experience   of,   such   as   the   Delirium   Rating   Scale  (DRS).    I  rapidly  became  aware  that  with  research  into  delirium  comes  a   variety   of   methods   for   delirium   assessment.     First   there   are   a  number   of   different   tests   and   combinations   of   tests.     They   differ   in  terms   of   goal,     that   is   some   are   diagnostic,   others  measure   severity.    Secondly   methodologies   differ   in   both   the   frequency   and   timing   of  application   of   these   tests.     Added   to   this   is   the   fact   that   currently  available   methods   come   in   different   formats   ,   for   example   the  confusion  assessment  method  (CAM)  and  the  CAM-­‐ICU,  in  recognition  of   the   fact   that   most   definitely   one   size   does   not   fit   all.       As   my  knowledge  of  methods  of  diagnosis  of  delirium  broadened  I  started  to  question   what   I   had   previously   seen   as   the   ‘basics’   in   diagnosing  delirium,  not  something  I  had  originally  expected.  

Many   of   us   will   be   using   tools   such   as   the   confusion   assessment  method   (CAM),   and   this   comes   recommended   both   in   the   NICE  guidelines   (guidance.nice.org.uk)   and   those   of   the   British   Geriatrics  Society   (www.bgs.org.uk).    This   is   certainly   the   test  with  which   I   am  most   familiar.    The  CAM  consists  of  4   simple  questions,  but   are   they  really  so  simple?    The  CAM  is  easily  accessible,  not  always  so  readily  available  are  details  of  how  to  assess  each  of  the  criterion.      Inattention  and   disorganised   thinking   are   signs   that   take   careful   and   detailed  assessment,   this  question   requires   a   yes  or  no   in   terms  of  diagnosis,  but   actually   eliciting   this   feature   is  more   complex.     The  waters   have  become   a   little   clearer   for   me   on   my   recent   discovery   of   the   CAM  training  manual  (http://hospitalelderlifeprogram.org).    

My   own   personal   professional   development   in   the   diagnosis   of  delirium   has   led   me   to   evaluate   my   own   practice   but   also   my  expectations   of   the   junior   doctors   I   work  with.     I   often   ask   them   to  apply   the   CAM   in   the   medical   inpatient   setting.     But   through   re-­‐evaluating  my  own   skills   I   have   started   to   consider   just   how  much   I  have   perhaps   presumed   of   some   of   my   juniors   in   the   past.     I   have  considered  whether  the  junior  doctors  that  I  work  with  have  ever  had  any  comprehensive   training  on  applying   the  CAM?    Have   I  ever  even  asked?     In  retrospect   I  have  probably  presumed  skills   that   the   junior  members,    and  dare  I  say  some  more  senior  members  of  the  team,  just  don’t   have.     What   about   the   time   element?   Eliciting   these   signs  properly   takes   time  but   is  not  something   that  can  often  be  pondered  over   by   our   junior   doctors,   for   example,     whilst   on   call.     In   fact   in  recognition   of   this   the   NIHR   Health   technology   assessment  programme   has   just   put   out   a   call   for   triage   tools   for   delirium    (http://www.hta.ac.uk/funding/standardcalls/11_143cb.pdf).     This  recognition  of  a  need  for  a  simple  and  easy  to  apply  diagnostic  tool  is  particularly  timely,  as  the  detection  of  delirium  becomes  increasingly  important,  with   progress   in   terms   of   the   dementia   strategy   in   acute  hospitals.  

Page 9: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

The   rewards   of   a   good   diagnosis   of   delirium   abound,   leading   to  identification   and   treatment   of   underlying   cause,   reduced  morbidity  and  mortality  and  increased  understanding  and  support  for  carers  and  relatives.    But  it  is  important  that  the  assessment  for  delirium  is  done  well   and   that   delirium   is   correctly   diagnosed.     There   are   various  examples  in  the  literature  of  poor  detection  of  delirium.    Perhaps  less  evident  are  the  consequences  of  incorrectly  diagnosing  delirium.    Over  the   course   of   my   career   two   examples   come   to   mind.     One   was   a  patient   who   had   more   than   one   admission   with   ‘delirium’.     Brain  imaging   after   the   second   or   third   repeat   admission   revealed  underlying  brain  metastases.    The   second  case   relates   to  a   lady  with  an  ‘acute  confusion  state’.    Brain  imaging  was  performed,  after  a  delay,  revealing   an   extradural   haematoma  which  was   operated   upon.     The  safe-­‐guard   to   ensuring   such   cases   are   not  missed   are   present   in   the  NICE  guidelines,  which  state  at  the  end  of  the  treatment  algorithm  that  there   should  be   re-­‐evaluation   for  underlying   causes.    However   these  examples   are   a   sobering   reminder   of   the   importance   of   ensuring  accuracy   of   diagnosis   and   following   a   procedure   of   re-­‐evaluation   of  diagnosis.  

In  summary  my  research  into  methods  of  diagnosis  of  delirium  led  me  to   re-­‐evaluate   my   own   clinical   practice   but   also   reflect   on   my   own  presumptions  of  others  knowledge.    There  may  be  new  and  improved  methods  for  diagnosing  delirium  on  the  horizon,  perhaps  more  readily  deliverable.   But   in   the   interim   perhaps   those   of   us   at   the   coal   face  need   to   reflect   on   what   our   colleagues,   particularly   junior   doctors,  may  find  hard  about  making  a  diagnosis  of  delirium  and  seek  to  raise  clinical  standards  overall.  

Dr  Emma  Vardy  

Newcastle   upon  Tyne   hospitals  NHS   foundation   trust   and  Newcastle  University  

Report on 2nd Annual ADS Meeting, June 3-5, Indianapolis, Indiana

The   second   annual   American   Delirium   Society   meeting   was   well  attended;   we   were   very   pleased   that   this   year   we   had   123  participants;  a  33%  increase  over  last  year’s  meeting.      Delegates  came  from   China,   Canada,   US   (20   states),   Norway,   Portugal,   Ireland,  England,   Australia   and   Denmark.     Speakers   from   several   continents  were  included  and  the  program  was  full  enough  that  parallel  sessions  were   held.       A   very   broad   range   of   speakers   and   topics   were  presented,   demonstrating   delirium’s   ascendance   as   a   highly   valued  and   increasingly   well   studied   aspect   of   geriatric   care.     ADS’   new  president   Malaz   Boustani,   MD,MPH   provided   an   address   during  opening  night   reception  which   explored  ways   that   drugs   targeted   to  symptomatic   relief   or   disease   modification   of   delirium   could   be  assessed  and  regulated  on  a  global  scale.        The  president  elect,  Karin  Neufeld,  MD,MPH  and  new   treasurer,  Ann  Gruber-­‐Baldini,  PhD,  were  introduced,   as   were   new   ADS   Board   members.       A   number   of   new  collaborations   were   developed   in   the   course   of   this   meeting,   an  achievement  that  all  of  us  in  ADS  heartily  applaud  and  endorse!  

 On   June   4th,   a   session   on   pathophysiology   and   biomarkers   was  initiated  by  Edward  Marcantonio,  MD,  who  reviewed  technical  aspects  of  biomarker  research.      Other  speakers  included  Alasdair  MacLullich,  MB/ChB,  PhD,Stacie  Denier,  MD,  James  Root,  PhD,  and  Malaz  Boustani,  MD,  MPH,   and   included   studies   relating   to   CSF,  MRI/structural     ICU,  and  surgical  aspects  of  delirium.    The  parallel  session  was  devoted  to  delirium  measurement,   and   was   introduced   by   Paula   Trzepacz,   MD,  who   presented   updates   on   her   collaborative   work   on   the  phenomenology  of  delirium.    This  has  provided  a  replicable  pattern  of  symptomatology  that  may  inform  studies  of  delirium  pathophysiology.    Other   work   on   screening,   measurement,   and   operationalization   of  delirium  measurement  was  presented  by  Laura  Hoofring,  MSN,  ARNP-­‐PMH,   Jean-­‐David   Gaudreau,   MD,   Edward  Marcantonio,   MD,   and   Ann  

Page 10: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Gruber-­‐Baldini,   PhD.     The   final   session   of   the   day   explored  postoperative  delirium;  this  was  initiated  by  Gregory  Crosby,  MD,  who  discussed  aspects  of  vulnerability  in  the  aging  CNS,  and  was  followed  by   work   on   baseline   cognitive   function   and   its   impact   on   post-­‐operative  outcomes,  the  assessment  of  delirium/emergent  delirium  in  the   PACU,   brain   dysfunction   in   critical   care,   the   impact   of   analgesic  agents   in   the   postoperative   setting,   and   the   method   of   “ABCDE  bundle”  in  addressing  and  maximizing  postoperative  care  complicated  by  delirium.    Speakers  included  Thomas  Robinson,  MD,  Karin  Neufeld,  MD,   MPH,     Yoanna   Skrobik,   MD,   and   Michele   Balas   PhD,RN,   CRNP,  CCRN.    

The   second   and   final   day   included   morning   sessions   on   long   term  outcomes   of   delirium   and   clinically   specific   aspects   of   the   care   of  delirium.      The  outcomes  session  included  a  study  by  Daniel  Davis,  MD,  which   involved   a   large   retrospective   chart   review  with   implications  that   the   pathophysiology   of   Alzheimer’s   Dementia   may   be   distinct  from  that  of  delirium.    Other  speakers  included  Dimitry  Davydow,  MD,  Alasdair   MacLullich,   MD,   ChB,   PhD.     Talks   on   the   psychological  sequelae  of  delirium  as  well   as   the   impact  of  pre-­‐operative  cognitive  dysfunction  on  delirium  outcomes  were  presented  by  O.  Bienvenu  MD,  PhD    and  Frederick  Sieber  MD.    A  comprehensive  literature  review  of  pharmacological  prevention  strategies  in  delirium  by  Jose  Maldonado,  MD   completed   the   session.     The   symposium   on   clinically   specific  aspects  of  delirium  care  included  a  session  by  James  Rudolph,  MD,MS  on   a   comprehensive   approach   to   delirium   identification   and  management   in   acute   settings   (the   “Delirium   Toolbox”),   a   highly  practical   and   effective   session   on   ways   to   de-­‐escalate   agitated   and  irrational   patients   (the   T-­‐A-­‐DA   method,   representing   “tolerate-­‐anticipate-­‐don’t   agitate”)   which   involved   acting   the   parts   of   patient  and  nurse  by   the   speakers,   Joseph  Flaherty,  MD  and  Sharon  Gordon,  PhD,   and   sessions   on   medications   presenting   risk   to   patients   with  delirium   by   Noll   Campbell,   PharmD,     the   role   of   sleep   in   the  presentation   and   management   of   delirium   in   the   ICU   by   Wes   Ely,  

MD,MPH,   and   techniques   of   early   mobilization   for   patients   with  critical   illness,   by  Amy  Pawlik,  PT,DPT,  CCS  and  Cheryl  Estbrook  OT.      The  final  session  presented  clinical  trial  updates  provided  by  Wes  Ely  (“MIND-­‐USA”),   a   new   delirium   assessment   tool   for   emergency   room  use  (Jin  Han,  MD),  and  other  work  by  Simon  Mears,  MD,  Laura  Sands,  PhD,   MA,   Babar   Khan,   MD,   MS,   Don-­‐Zin   Wang   MD,   PhD,     Jeff  Silverstein,  MD,   and  Gideon   Caplan,  MB,  MS,   and   included   studies   of  new   agents   and   pharmacological   approaches   to  minimizing   delirium  in  different  settings;  Dr  Caplan’s  study  reported  new  findings  on  CN  S  blood  flow  abnormalities  in  patients  with  delirium.    

Keynote   speakers   included   Sharon   Inouye,   MD,  MPH,   who   spoke   on  the  future  of  research  in  delirium,  including  an  emphasis  on  the  need  to  codify  and  simplify  diagnostic  categories  of  delirium  in  the  ICD.    The  Delirium   Champion   Award   2012   was   presented   to   Dr   Inouye   in  recognition   of   her   outstanding   contribution   to   the   field.     Ann  Kolanowski,  PhD,  RN  spoke  about  the  vital  role  nurses  play  in  delirium  assessment   and   intervention,   and   provided   updates   on   the  methodology  of  dissemination  of  delirium  teaching  for  nurses.      There  were  16  posters  accepted  for  presentation.  

Next  year’s  conference  will  be  in  Indianapolis,  Indiana,  in  June    -­‐  actual  date   to   be   confirmed     shortly.     Preparations   are   being   made   for   a  conference  in  Baltimore,  MD  area  for  the  following  year  (2014).        We  sincerely  hope  that  this  year’s  meeting  was  helpful  and  gratifying  for  attendees,  and  that  next  year’s  will  be  even  better!  

Barbara  Kamholz,  MD  

Board  Member,  EDA  and  ADS  

Page 11: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Raising delirium consciousness using the media

Dr   Valerie   Page,   Consultant   Anaesthesia   and   Critical   Care,  Watford  General  Hospital,  

Dr   Daniel   Davis,   Research   Fellow,   Institute   of   Public   Health,  University  of  Cambridge  

Tony  Jameson-­‐Allen,  Accredited  Knowledge  Management  Consultant  and   Director,   Evolution   Networking   Ltd,   UK.   www.evolution-­‐networking.com  

Introduction  “The  greatest  trick  the  devil  ever  played  was  convincing  the  world  that  he  did  not  exist.”  Charles  Baudelaire  

Delirium   occurs   in   one   in   five   general   hospital   patients.   It   remains  underappreciated   and   under   researched.   The   European   Delirium  Association   remit   includes   campaigning   at   local,   national   and  international  levels  in  order  to  influence  policy.  The  area  of  advocacy  has  remained  under  addressed.  

There   needs   to   be   an   increased   awareness   of   delirium   clinically,  politically   and   publicly.   This   project   assessed   the   effectiveness   of  using   traditional   advertising,   websites   and   social   media   to   achieve  this.  The  aim  was  to  discover  how  best  to  use  traditional  and  modern  forms  of  media  to  lobby  on  behalf  of  patients  with  delirium.  

Methods  Stage  1:  Selected   websites   and   social   media   resources   relating   to   delirium  were   identified   (see   table   1).     The   effectiveness   was   judged   by   the  number  of  times  a  webpage  was  viewed.    

Stage  2:  A   banner   advert   was   purchased   for   the   UK   national   newspaper  Guardian  website   directing   readers   to   the   UK   ICU   delirium  website;  the  number  of  clicks  resulting  from  this  advert  was  collected.  (Figure  1).  

Results  

The   web   pages   had   large   variations   in   number   of   times   they   were  viewed.  The  most  successful  was  the  Wikipedia  delirium  page  with  an  average   of   nearly   44,000   per  month.   Encouragingly   the   UK   Youtube  clip   was   activated   43   400   times   in   less   than   4   years.     (Figure   2)    Conversely   the   Guardian   banner   advert   generated   an   astoundingly  low  total  of  12  responses  from  over  25,000  appearances  

 

Resource   Date  started   Outcome/’Hits’  

Wikipedia  Delirium  Page   Dec  2002   527  317  last  12  months  

Youtube   ICU   delirium  clip  

Aug  2008   43   500   views   since  launch  

ICU  delirium  website   Aug  2008   Average  1000  visits  per  month  

Delirium  videocasts*   April  2009   3468  views  since  launch  

EDA**  website   July  2009   73  556  page  views  

EDA**  facebook  group   June  2011   49  members  

Guardian   web   banner  advert  

July  2011   21   clicks   from   18   000  appearances  

Page 12: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Discussion  

Heightened  media  attention  to  an  issue  has  been  shown  to  pressurise  policy   makers   to   generate   immediate,   short-­‐term   solutions   to   the  problem   which   then   shift   to   long-­‐term   solutions.   Delirium   needs   a  simple  but  powerful  device  as  a  means  of  disseminating  knowledge  on  the  impact  on  patient  outcomes  –  death  and  dementia.  

After  the  massacre  of  schoolchildren  in  Dunblane,  Scotland,  Parliament  was   introducing   a   law   to   ban   certain   firearms,   but   not   .22   handguns.  The   team,  with   no  money   and   no   time,   decide   to   act.   They   discovered  that  Robert  Kennedy  had  been  assassinated  with  a  .22  gun.  Using  a  stock  photograph   of   Kennedy,   they   had   a   poster   printed   to   go   on   a   lorry  parked  outside  Parliament  on  the  day  of  the  debate.  "If  a  .22  handgun  is  less   deadly,"   it   asked,   "why   isn't   he   less   dead?"   An   amendment   was  added  to  include  the  banning  of  .22  guns.  

Regarding  promotional   campaigns  Ed   Jones  of  Saatchi  &  Saatchi   said  "Your   message   has   to   be   really   powerful   and   life-­‐transforming,  otherwise   you   are  wasting   your  money."   There   is   no   doubt   that   the  facts  of  delirium  are  as  powerful  as  cancer  or  HIV  yet  still  the  subject  does   not   feature   in   the   public   or   to   a   lesser   extent   the   clinical  consciousness.  On   the  BBC  health  website   cancer   is   covered   in   great  depth  with  a  number  of  helpful  links;  delirium  does  not  even  feature.    

There   is   growing   interest   in   use   of   Facebook   for   disseminating  information   and   establishing   informal   networks.   It   has   been  demonstrated   that   spending  money   on   a   single   advert  was   nowhere  near  as  effective  as  entries  on  high  profile  social  media  based  websites  such   as  Wikipedia   and  Youtube.   Currently   the   Internet   can   be   freely  utilized   to   educate   and   influence   politically,   clinically   and   publicly.  There   is   growing   interest   in   use   of   Facebook   for   disseminating  information  and  establishing  informal  networks.  

 

This   project   was   a   means   in   discovering   how   best   to   use   different  forms   of   media   to   lobby   on   behalf   of   patients   with   delirium.   The  findings   suggest   that   currently   the   use   of   responsive,   informal,   and  contemporary   means   will   gain   greater   exposure   to   delirium   than   a  traditional   advertising   campaign.   It   would   be   reasonable   to   assume  that   will   generate   a   higher   awareness   amongst   clinicians   and   the  public.  Further  work   is  needed   to  discover  how   to  promote  delirium  publicly,  build  on  the  momentum  that  has  been  established,  to  manage  the   response   and   to   capitalize   on   any   media   opportunities   as   they  arise.  

Figure  1  Banner  advertisement  for  ICU  delirium  website    

 

 

Page 13: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Figure  2  CAM-­‐ICU  demonstration  on  Youtube  

 

*“Haloperidol prophylaxis decreases delirium incidence in elderly patients after noncardiac surgery: A randomized controlled trial”

W  Wang,  HL  Li,  DX  Wang  ,  et  al,  Crit  Care  Med  2012:  40  (3);  731-­‐739  

Earlier   this   year,   a   Chinese   group   published   the   results   of   a   study  examining  the  potential  role  of  haloperidol  for  prophylaxis  in  surgical  patients  admitted  to  critical  care*.    The  study  was  carefully  designed,  using   a   prospective,   randomised,   double   blind   placebo   controlled  method  conducted  across  two  sites.    Subjects  were  older  patients  (>65  years   old),   post   non-­‐cardiac   surgery   and   admitted   to   ICU   post  operatively.     Exclusion   criteria   were   rigorous,   the   entire   study   was  safety  conscious  and  this  meant  that  the  doses  of  haloperidol  used  in  the  active  arm  were  very  conservative.    A  0.5mg  bolus  of  haloperidol  was  followed  by  a  12  hour  infusion  running  at  0.1mg  of  haloperidol  an  hour.    The  total  prophylactic  dose  of  haloperidol  given  was  thus  1.7mg  versus  the  control  arm,  where  patients  received  a  matching  volume  of  saline.    Usual  sedation  (propofol  or  midazolam)  was  used  and  titrated  to   RASS   -­‐2   to   +1,   with   daily   sedation   breaks   for   intubated   patients.    Postoperative   pain   was   managed   with   patient   controlled   analgesia  administered   intravenously   or   epidurally,   with   supplemental  analgesia   being   given   as   fentanyl   boluses   +/-­‐   a   fentanyl   infusion.    Multicomponent   interventions   to   reduce   modifiable   delirium   risk  factors  were  used  in  all  patients.    Delirium  assessments  were  carried  out  once  a  day  in  the  early  evening  using  CAM-­‐ICU.    The  primary  end  point  was  delirium  incidence  in  the  first  7  days  post-­‐operatively,  with  multiple  secondary  endpoints.  

The  final  analysis  was  made  on  229  patients  in  the  haloperidol  group  compared  with  228  patients  in  the  placebo  group.    Overall  the  groups  were   well   matched   on   baseline   characteristics.     There   were   some  statistically   significant   differences   noted   in   perioperative   variables,  the  duration  of  anaesthesia  was  longer  in  the  haloperidol  group  (5.51  

Page 14: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

vs   4.81   hours,   p=0.003),   the   duration   of   surgery   was   longer   in   the  haloperidol   group   (4.51   vs   3.79   hours,   p=0.001)   and   the   total  intraoperative   infusion   volume  was   greater   in   the  haloperidol   group  (2700ml   vs   2550ml,   p=0.048).     All   other   variables   were   not  statistically  different,  including  the  total  doses  of  opioid  analgesics  and  sedatives  administered.  

 

The  primary  outcome  results  showed  an  incidence  of  delirium  in  the  7  days  post   surgery   of   15.3%   in   the  haloperidol   group   compared  with  23.2%   in   the   placebo   group   which   translates   into   an   NNT   of   13   to  prevent   one   case   of   delirium.     Much   of   the   benefit   appears   to   be  confined   to   patients   who   received   intra-­‐abdominal   surgery,  representing   about   three   quarters   of   the   cohort.     The   difference   in  delirium   incidence   in   the   rest   of   the   cohort   (intra-­‐thoracic   surgery,  spinal   and   extremital   surgery   and   superficial   surgery)   was   not  apparently   numerically   large,   nor   statistically   significant.     This   gives  rise   to   the   obvious   question   (‘why?’),   however   the   authors   do   not  speculate.  

Overall,   the   reported   ICU   length   of   stay  was   short,  with   a  median   of  21.3   hours   in   the   haloperidol   group   and   23.0   hours   in   the   placebo  group   (p=0.024).     When   looking   at   just   patients   who   developed  delirium,   the   intervention   resulted   in   a   reduced   ICU   length   of   stay  (19.6  hours  haloperidol  group,  41.4  hours  placebo  group,  p=0.006).  

The  reported  time  to  onset  of  delirium  was  quite  long,  on  average  6.2  days   in   the   haloperidol   group   and   5.7   days   in   the   placebo   group  (p=0.021),  however,  this  analysis  included  all  the  patients  who  did  not  suffer   delirium,   which   when   expressed   as   a   mean,   would   make   the  onset  of  delirium  time  very  long.    As  a  bonus,  because  of  the  reduced  incidence   of   delirium   in   one   group,   it   also   makes   the   difference  statistically   significant.     When   more   realistically   analysing   only  

patients   who   suffered   delirium,   the   mean   time   to   onset   of   delirium  was   found   to   be   1.7   days   (haloperidol   group)   vs   1.5   days   (placebo  group)   and   was   not   statistically   significant.     The   mean   number   of  delirium  free  days  was  5.7  (haloperidol  group)  vs  5.6  (placebo  group)  and  was  also  not  statistically  significant.    So  the  intervention  reduces  the  numbers  of  patients  experiencing  delirium,  but  not  the  time  to  the  onset  of  delirium  for  those  who  suffer  from  it.  

There  was  no  difference   in  hospital   length  of   stay  overall   (11.0  days  for   both   groups,   p=0.255),   or   between   the   groups   in   patients   who  went   on   to   develop   delirium   (14   days   haloperidol   group   vs   12   days  placebo  group,  p=0.512).  

In   terms   of   overall   safety,   there   were   no   differences   between   the  groups   with   respect   to   arrhythmia’s,   extended   QTc   interval,  extrapyramidal   symptoms,   RASS   or   mortality.     The   intervention   is  clearly  safe.  

Overall,  this  study  shows  that  a  small  prophylactic  dose  of  haloperidol  reduces  the  incidence  of  delirium  in  the  first  7  days  for  older  patients  admitted   to   ICU   after   non-­‐cardiac   surgery   (NNT=13)   and   that   the  effect  is  largely  confined  to  patients  receiving  abdominal  surgery.    The  intervention  reduces  median  ICU  length  of  stay  in  delirious  patients  by  21.8   hours   which   is   both   clinically   meaningful   and   statistically  significant.     There  was   no   effect   on   the   time   to   onset   of   delirium   in  delirious  patients  or  any  effect  on  hospital  length  of  stay,  either  overall  or   for   the  delirious  subgroup.    The  haloperidol  dose  used  resulted   in  no   adverse   effects   and   is   therefore   safe.     The   authors   conclude   that  further  investigation  is  warranted.  

 

 

Page 15: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

Editor’s Choice

The   relationship   between   delirium   duration,   white   matter   integrity,  and   cognitive   impairment   in   intensive   care   unit   survivors   as  determined   by   diffusion   tensor   imaging:   The   VISIONS   prospective  cohort   magnetic   resonance   imaging   study*     Morandi   A,   Rogers   BP,  Gunther   MLet   al   for   the   VISIONS   Investigation   (VISualizing   Icu  SurvivOrs   Neuroradiological   Sequelae).     Crit   Care   Med.   2012  Jul;40(7):2182-­‐2189.      

The   association   between   brain   volumes,   delirium   duration,   and  cognitive   outcomes   in   intensive   care   unit   survivors:   The   VISIONS  cohort  magnetic   resonance   imaging   study*     Gunther  ML,  Morandi   A,  Krauskopf  E  et  al  for  VISIONS  Investigation  (VISualizing  Icu  SurvivOrs  Neuroradiological   Sequelae).     Crit   Care   Med.   2012   Jul;40(7):2022-­‐2032.  

Neuroimaging   offers   a   means   to   help   unravel   the   pathogenesis   of  delirium.      The  Vanderbilt  group  in  Nashville  published  2  papers  in  the  July  edition  of  Critical  Care  Medicine  which  describe  MRI   findings  on  47   patients   who   survived   critical   illness.       These   papers   are   worth  looking   up   firstly   because   they   add   to   the   ever   increasing   body   of  evidence   that   suggest   delirium   results   in   actual   brain   damage;  secondly   they   have   excellent   descriptions   how  MRI   scanning   can   be  used  to  determine  pathological  changes  in  the  brain.      

The   first   study   demonstrated   an   association   between   delirium  duration  and  white  matter  disruption  using  MRI  with  diffusion  tensor  imaging   (DTI).     White   matter   consists   mostly   of   myelinated   axons,  white   matter   integrity   is   required   to   control   consciousness   and  attention.     The   use   of   MRI   with   DTI   provides   investigators   with  quantitative   assessment   of   the   integrity   of   white   matter   and   white  matter   tracts   –   fractional   anisotropy.       This   study   provided  

preliminary   data   to   inform   the   hypothesis   connecting   white   matter  integrity  with  delirium  duration  and  long-­‐term  cognitive  impairment.    

The   second   paper   showed   that   longer   duration   of   delirium   is  associated  with  smaller  brain  volumes  up  to  3  months  after  discharge,  and   that   smaller   brain   volumes   are   associated   with   long-­‐term  cognitive   impairment  up  to  12  months.    They  did  point  out   that   they  could   not   rule   out   that   patients   had   smaller   brain   volumes   before  admission.       The   ventricle-­‐to-­‐brain   ratio   as  determined  by  MRI   is   an  excellent   indicator   of   generalised   brain   atrophy.       The   authors  illustrate   their   findings   by   comparing   scans   of   2   previously   fit,  cognitively   intact   women   in   their   mid-­‐forties   only   one   of   which  suffered  ICU  delirium  (figure  2).  

Acute   confusional   States   in   the   elderly-­‐diagnosis   and   treatment.    Lorenzl   S,   Füsgen   I,   Noachtar   S.     Dtsch   Arztebl   Int.   2012  May;109(21):391-­‐400.  Epub  2012  May  25.      

This   is   a   useful   overview   of   delirium   in   the   elderly   from   Deutsches  Ärzteblatt  International  (even  if  it  does  not  have  delirium  in  the  title!)    It  is  free  to  download.  

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3371633/?tool=pubmed  

Mental   Illness   –   Comprehensive   Evaluation   or   Checklist?   P   McHugh  and  P  Slavney  The  New  England  Journal  of  Medicine  2012;  366:  1853-­‐55  

This   excellent   opinion   piece   discusses   some   of   the   issues   relating   to  the  Diagnostic  and  Statistical  Manual  of  Mental  Disorders  -­‐  DSM.    The  following  quote  hits  the  nail  on  the  head  regarding  delirium.  

Page 16: Annals of Delirium July 2012

©  European  Delirium  Association  2012    www.europeandeliriumassociation.com    

“Identifying   a   disorder   by   its   symptoms   does   not   translate   into  understanding   it.     Clinicians  need   some  heuristic   concept   of   its   nature,  grasped  in  terms  of  cause  or  mechanism,  to  render  it  intelligible  and  to  justify  their  actions  in  practice  and  research”.  

Free  to  read  http://www.nejm.org/doi/full/10.1056/NEJMp1202555    

Valerie  Page  

[email protected]    

News

October  Delirium  Congress  The   European   Delirium   Association   annual   congress   is   in   Bielefeld,  Germany  from  October  18th  to  19th.    It  is  always  a  quality  meeting  so  if  you  only  go  to  one  meeting  a  year  -­‐  make  it  this  one!  

Further  details:  www.eda2012bielefeld.org  

European  Survey  –  you  can  help  The   EDA   has   decided   to   conduct   a   survey   to   help   understand   the  range   of   opinions   among  mailing   list   members   on   various   aspect   of  delirium   care.   We   greatly   appreciate   your   expertise   and   we   believe  that   this   survey   will   provide   a   significant   advancement   in   our  understanding   of   current   practice.   The   findings  will   be   presented   at  EDA  Bielefeld  2012,  and  submitted  for  publication  in  a  peer-­‐reviewed  journal.  

You  can  find  the  survey  here:  http://tiny.cc/gm2nbw    

 

EDA  membership  benefits  

•                            Access  to  the  member’s  only  section  of  the  EDA  website  •                            Reduction  in  the  annual  EDA  conference  registration  fee    •                            Eligibility  to  become  EDA  Board  Member  •                            Opportunities  to  contact  and  exchange  ideas  with  delirium                                          experts.  

The   annual   fee   is   30   Euros.     This   fee   will   be   used   to   support  development   of   the   website,   the   costs   of   online   Board   meetings  (around   200   Euros,   twice   per   year),   to   underwrite   our   annual  meeting,  and  for  other  costs.  All  accounts  will  be  published  annually  

 Payment   may   be   by   a   standing   order   or   by   a   cheque   sent   to   John  Young,  Treasurer  The  bank  account  details:  Royal  Bank  of  Scotland,  East  Parade,  Leeds,  LS1  5PS,  UK.  Account  no:  10147495    Sort  Code:    16-­‐23-­‐17  IBAN&  SWFT  No:    BG64RBOS16231710147495    John  Young,  Treasurer  EDA,    Professor  of  Elderly  Care  Medicine  Academic  Unit  of  Elderly  Care  and  Rehabilitation  Bradford  Institute  for  Health  Research  Temple  Bank  House  Bradford  Royal  Infirmary  Duckworth  Lane  Bradford,  UK,  BD9  6RJ