Top Banner
ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263 Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23 http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf 1 Analysis of the risky attitude of driver candidates by graphical models VEYSEL YILMAZ a,* , H.ERAY ÇELİK a CENGİZ AKTAŞ a Eskişehir Osmangazi University, Faculty of Arts and Science, Department of Statistics, 26480 Eskişehir, Turkey * Corresponding author. Tel.: + 90 222 2393750; fax: +90 222 2393578. E-mail address: [email protected] ; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] Abstract There are some good scientific studies conducted over risk-taking and risky behaviors of reckless drivers; however, there are only a few studies related to attitudes displayed by candidate drivers towards the traffic. The risky attitude of drivers is usually established by determining their intentions about speed, safety belt use, and drunk driving. In this study, the attitudes of the driver candidates towards drunk drivers and the use of safety belt in downtown traffic were investigated from the perspective of gender, age, and educational level by using graphical models. Graphical models provide a flexible tool for representing complex relations among variables. These relations are marginal or conditional independencies and directed or symmetric association. Each variable in a chain graph is represented by a node and some pairs of nodes are connected by edges which indicate dependencies of subject-matter interest whenever it represents a substantive research hypothesis. According to the results of statistical analysis, while female driver candidates have a risky attitude regarding safety belt use and male driver candidates have a risky attitude regarding drunk driving. Keywords: Risky driving, Driver candidates, Graphical models
22

Analysis of the Risky Attitude of Driver Candidates by Graphical Models

Sep 26, 2015

Download

Documents

Baluse Loredana

Analysis of the risky attitude of driver candidates by graphical models
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    1

    Analysis of the risky attitude of driver candidates by graphical models

    VEYSEL YILMAZ a,*

    , H.ERAY ELK a

    CENGZ AKTA

    a Eskiehir Osmangazi University, Faculty of Arts and Science, Department of Statistics, 26480

    Eskiehir, Turkey

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: + 90 222 2393750; fax: +90 222 2393578.

    E-mail address: [email protected] ; [email protected]; [email protected];

    [email protected]

    Abstract

    There are some good scientific studies conducted over risk-taking and risky behaviors of

    reckless drivers; however, there are only a few studies related to attitudes displayed by

    candidate drivers towards the traffic. The risky attitude of drivers is usually established by

    determining their intentions about speed, safety belt use, and drunk driving. In this study, the

    attitudes of the driver candidates towards drunk drivers and the use of safety belt in downtown

    traffic were investigated from the perspective of gender, age, and educational level by using

    graphical models. Graphical models provide a flexible tool for representing complex relations

    among variables. These relations are marginal or conditional independencies and directed or

    symmetric association. Each variable in a chain graph is represented by a node and some pairs

    of nodes are connected by edges which indicate dependencies of subject-matter interest

    whenever it represents a substantive research hypothesis. According to the results of statistical

    analysis, while female driver candidates have a risky attitude regarding safety belt use and male

    driver candidates have a risky attitude regarding drunk driving.

    Keywords: Risky driving, Driver candidates, Graphical models

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    2

    INTRODUCTION

    In the years of 2005, 2006 and 2007; 570025, 664540 and 749456 traffic accidents occurred

    respectively in Turkey and following numbers were recorded as casualty traffic accidents out of

    these accidents 3195, 3365, and 3459 respectively. In Turkey, 5400 people were killed and

    188,383 were injured in road traffic accidents in 2007. In Turkey, 502746, 680384 and 419137

    individuals obtained drivers licenses in 2003, 2004, and the first six months of 2005,

    respectively. According to the first six month data from 2005, 16561564 people have drivers

    licenses. Considering that on average, 500,000 trainees a year work towards earning a drivers

    license, it is a necessity to equip these candidate drivers with accurate cultural attitudes and

    knowledge regarding traffic. Total highway network of Turkey is 63899 km. Density of Total

    Network in meters per sq . Km is 82 meters. The number of vehicles being used is 12227393.

    When we considered the population of Turkey, we may say that Number of Cars per 1000

    Persons is 84 and Number of Cars per 100 Family is 34. On average, around 9000 accidents

    occur in Turkey every year due to drunk-driving. Accidents associated with drunk drivers make

    up 15% of all accidents. The death rate is the highest for drunk drivers at the ages ranging from

    18 to 24 in all accidents happening in Turkey. This shows a parallelism to the traffic accidents

    caused by speeding. Traffic problem of Turkey annually on average results in loss of 8-9 billion

    dollars for country economy. When we compare Turkey with other countries in terms of traffic

    accidents, we see that the result requires taking more urgent measures. While the death rate in

    accidents per 100 million vehicle-kilometers is 0.9, 1.1, and 1.6 respectively, such rate is 20 in

    Turkey (Traffic Education & Research Center, Report 2005-2007).

    The population in Turkey increases gradually, but there is a considerable amount of migration

    from the countryside to the towns because of health, education, and economics related issues.

    This influx results in traffic problems that grow day by day. An established traffic culture does

    not exist in the Turkish society and traffic education is not provided with an emphasis to close

    the educational gap caused by the high rates of migration. However, there is a common belief

    that such an education should be school-focused, rather than being a lifetime process. The

    whole community should be aware of the need for traffic education, and the necessary

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    3

    measures should be taken to determine drivers attitudes before they hit the roads, and to

    remove the generally accepted negative tendencies and attitudes.

    Studies mentioned above are conducted on active drivers. Traffic education and teaching a

    traffic culture for candidates before they go out to traffic must be evaluated within a complete

    framework. The attitudes and the behaviors of drivers do not come into existence as soon as

    they go into the traffic. The subject carries his/her positive or negative attitude, obtained through

    socializing, to traffic modified by the traffic education they receive. The knowledge of the

    attitudes of drivers about these issues before becoming an active driver can provide an

    opportunity to build an education system. In this study, the attitudes of driver candidates

    towards the drunk driving and the use of safety belt in downtown traffic were investigated from

    the perspective of gender, age, and educational level by using graphical models.

    Attitude is an important factor determining the behavior of an individual .According to Allport

    (1967) attitude is formed as a result of experiences in life and is an emotional and intelligent

    readiness that has got a dynamic or diverting power to affect the behavior of the individual

    towards all objects and situations. Attitude has an active role in the formation and development

    of an action. Also, during their developments, acts may have roles that change the attitudes,

    which form themselves. It has been thought that attitudes form a system that facilitates the

    harmony of the subjects to their environment and have a secret directing power. One of the

    most important terms about driver behavior is the term attitude. Driving styles reflect the

    personal characteristics, attitudes, and motives of the driver (Elander et al., 1993). Before

    getting started with changing the behavior of drivers, their attitudes must be investigated as a

    first step (Forward, 1994). Some investigators in the traffic psychology field consider attitudes

    involve personal characteristics that arise from a wide spectrum of motivators, including

    aggression and risk taking; others investigate specific driver attitudes such as drunk driving or

    speedy driving. On the other hand, some researchers suggest that attitudes should be

    considered in the wider context of social environment or lifestyle (Aberg, 1996).

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    4

    Most recent studies investigating how and when attitudes affect behaviors have been influenced

    by Ajzens Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) (1988, 1991). This socio psychological theory

    offers a model of the relationship among believes attitudes, intentions, and behaviors (Parker et

    al., 1998). Topics such as violation of traffic rules, drunken driving, attitude measurement, and

    perceived risk have been investigated in the framework of Ajzens TBP. The human factor in

    traffic was investigated in several scientific studies and drivers attitudes about traffic were

    clearly shown. The accident tendency, personality predisposed to accidents, risk taking

    tendency, and the driver behavior and attitude were investigated in detail (Parker et al., 1992,

    1995a, 1995b, 1998; Summala, 1996; Yagil, 1998; Iversen and Rundmo, 2002; Ulleberg and

    Rundmo, 2003; Elliott et al., 2003; Iversen, 2004; 2008). These studies had similar findings in

    which the human factor has the highest percentage in accident formation and is found to be the

    most difficult factor to be changed among the three factors (vehicles, human, and environment)

    of the traffic system. Assum (1997) hypothesized that there are no direct relationships between

    institutional attitudes and accidents and found out that drivers with correct attitudes about traffic

    rules have lower accident risks when compared to those of drivers with bad attitudes. Based on

    other studies, it is expected that young male drivers are more likely to violate traffic rules and

    show more risky driving behaviors.

    The tendency of drivers to have a risky attitude is usually determined by finding out their

    attitudes towards fast driving, failure to use a safety belt, and drunk driving. Shinar (1993)

    searched for relations between safety belt use and demographic, socio-economic factors. A

    positive relationship was found between race, marital status, child number, education, income,

    and safety belt usage. Stewart (1993) also investigated safety belt usage within the

    determinants of accident and compared the driver behaviors before and after safety belt usage

    act was released. Li, Kim and Nitz (1999) determined the same relation between safety belt use

    among drivers and passengers. Moreover, in this study, strong relations were found between

    alcohol and safety belt usage. Shin et al. (1999) investigated the relationships between safety

    belt use and the socio-economic status and the ethnicity among high school students and

    concluded that younger respondents view the usage of safety belts, in downtown traffic,

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    5

    useless. Schechtman et al. (1999) studied the relation between alcohol drinking habit, speed

    passion, and safety belt usage and found that alcohol passion is an indicator of risk taking in

    traffic. They further discussed the relation between this habit and safety belt usage. Everett et

    al. (2001), researched the security and risk behaviors of high school students in the USA

    between 1991 and 1997. The results of this study revealed that 17% of the drivers drive after

    alcohol intake and 33% do not use a safety belt. Calisir and Letho (2002) argued that safety belt

    use was affected basically by personal factors such as age, gender, and race. Kim & Kim

    (2003) in their research on safety belt usage, personal factors and severe accident injury,

    showed that the ones who did not use safety belts have more severe injuries or deaths in traffic

    accidents when compared to those of who use safety belts. They also found relations between

    safety belt use and age, gender vehicle type, alcohol use, and speed. Chaudhary et al. (2004)

    found that 29% of the American drivers did not use safety belts; people with high income levels

    comprehended not using safety belts as a high risk, however, youngsters found it low risk.

    In the current study, eleven hypotheses were investigated to find out the relation between

    demographic characteristics and the risky attitude of drivers. Hypotheses are defined as follows:

    H1. There is a relation between attitude regarding safety belt usage in downtown and gender.

    H2. There is a relation between attitude regarding safety belt usage in downtown and age.

    H3. There is a relation between attitude regarding safety belt usage in downtown and the

    education level.

    H4. There is a relation between the attitudes concerning driving under the influence of alcohol in

    downtown and safety belt use.

    H5. A There is a relation between attitudes on drunken driving and gender.

    H6. There is a relation between attitudes on drunken driving and age.

    H7. There is a relation between attitudes on drunken and the education level.

    H8. Drunk driving and gender are conditional independent if age is given.

    H9. Education level and safety belt use are conditional independent if is age given.

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    6

    H10. Education level and drunk driving are conditional independent if the attitude on safety belt

    use in downtown is given.

    H11. Education level and safety belt use are conditional independent if the attitude on drunk

    driving is given.

    METHOD

    Sample

    The study sample consisted of 350 candidate drivers from a pool of 1530 who attended 17

    driver courses, which are implemented under the Ministry of National Education Control, in

    October 2004, in Eskisehir Twenty driver candidates selected randomly from each of 17 driving

    courses constituted the sample of study. During the application of the measurement tool, 350

    questionnaire forms were delivered to candidate drivers. Ninety two candidate drivers did not

    fully or correctly answer and the statistical analyses were performed on the answers from the

    remaining 258 candidate drivers. Table 1 gives the socio-demographic information about the

    participants.

    The variables and their levels were as follows:

    a. Gender (male, female)

    b. Age (18-24 years, 25-31 years, 32 years and older)

    c. Education level (elementary school, high school, university)

    d. I do not think that drunk driving is risky (I agree, I disagree).

    e. I do not think that safety belt use is necessary in downtown traffic (I agree, I disagree).

    Table . The demographical distribution of individuals in the sample

    Statistical Procedure

    In the study, Graphical Log Linear models, which provide an opportunity for numerical and

    graphical interpretation, have been used (For the details and the proof for graphical model see:

    Lauritzen, 1996; Pearl, 1993, 1995a, 1995b, 1998, 2000; Edwards and Kreiner, 1983;

    Whittaker, 1990; Pigeot et al., 2000). Models that have interpretations in term of conditional

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    7

    independence are known as graphical models. The terminology stems from the relationship of

    these models to graph theory. Edwards and Kreiner (1983) give an overview of the use of

    graphical log linear models.

    Graphical models are a form of multivariate statistical analysis in which the structure of

    dependences between variables can be displayed graphically. Note that graphs based on two

    variable effects determine log linear model. Thus pictures of graphical models are worthless

    until after graphical models have been defined. A key feature of this subject is the one-to-one

    correspondence between graphical log linear models and graphs. Every model determines a

    graph and every graph determines a model. The graphical models are represented by an

    undirected independence graph. Graphs consist of vertices and edges as connections between

    selected pairs of are used to formulate hypotheses about relation between variables. A graph

    consists of vertices and edges. Vertices correspond to variables in log linear models. Edges

    correspond to two variable effects. Vertices stand for variables connections representing

    associations. When a missing connection is interpreted as a conditional independence, a graph

    characterizes a conditional independence structure as well (Lauritzen and Wermuth 1989,

    Wermuth and Lauritzen, 1990). For instance, according to the rules of notation, the graph of 4

    independent vertices, shown in Figure 1, shows the shorthand notation [AB], [BCD] models,

    M= U + UA + UB + UC + UD + UAB + UBC + UBD + UCD + UBCD (1)

    where U is the general effect; UA, UB, UC and UD are the main effects ; UAB, UBC, UBD and UCD

    are the two variable interactions and UBCD is the three variable interaction. From Figure 1, it can

    be inferred. Given variable B, variable A is conditional independent of variables C and D. The

    model shows shorthand conditional independence notation [A (C, D) B]. Graphical

    models are determined by their two variable interactions. The basic idea is that any graphical

    model containing all of the terms UAB , UAC and UBC must also include UABC . A model is

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    8

    graphical if, whenever the model contains all two variable terms generated by a higher order

    interaction, the model also contains the higher order interaction.

    Figure 1 The independence graph of the log-linear model [AB], [BCD]

    The best model may be selected either using a forward or a backward approach like in

    regression analysis. In a forward selection approach, one starts with the independence model

    and then progressively adds association parameters that significantly increase the fit of the

    model. It first starts by testing all possible first order interactions; it then selects the one that

    produces the most important and significant increase in fit. If it finds one, it is added to the

    model. The other firstorder interactions are again tested, and terms are added if they are

    significant. The procedure continues with higher order interactions until no other terms can be

    added, yielding a final model that best fits the data. In backward selection, one starts from the

    saturated model. It first tries to remove higher order interaction terms; if a term is removed, it

    does not yield a significant difference. The procedure is repeated until the removal of a term

    yields a significant difference. If the test with eliminating an edge comes to a solution like p,

    since we cannot refuse the edge, there wont be any testing in the further iterations. (Yilmaz et

    al., 2005).

    RESULTS

    The Eliminate Backwards method was used to reach a suitable model and to test the

    hypotheses. The reason for this is the availability of a saturated model in the initial analysis. For

    that reason, a saturated model was used for the first model. The result of the first step is given

    below in Table II.

    First step:

    Table II. The first step results of the analysis done to reach the most proper model

    As seen in the table, the edge with the highest p value and lowest chi-square value is ad edge.

    For this reason, ad edge is removed from the saturated model. At the end of the first step,

    [bcde],[ abce] preliminary model was reached. The graphic of the model is shown below:

    Figure 2. The [bcde],[ abce] model graph

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    9

    The second step: ce: 2 =14.8799 (df=12;p=0.2481). The ac and ae edges were left in the

    second step. Since P value of ac edge was found to be higher than the level of significance, this

    edge is removed from the preliminary models [bcde], [abce]. Subsequently, second preliminary

    model would be [bcde],[ abe]. The graphic related to this model is given below:

    Figure 3. The [bcde] ,[abe] model graph

    The second step: ac: 2 =18.8850 (df=12;p=0. 0.0913) and ae: 2 =16.3964 (df=8;p=0.0370).

    Only the ce edge was left in the third step. Since P value of the ce edge was found to be higher

    than the level of significance, this edge is removed from the preliminary models [bcde], [abe].

    Subsequently, the last model would be [bde], [bce],[abe]. This model can be expressed by

    conditional liberty terms as [ ebda ,/ ] [ dbec ,/ ]. The graphic related to this model is

    given below:

    Figure 4. The [bde], [bcd],[ abe] model graph

    Table III. The results of the hypotheses

    The hypotheses, the results, and the graphics are shown in Table III. All hypotheses except H3

    and H5 were accepted. No significant relation between education level and safety belt use and

    drunk driving was found. However, relations between the variables could be determined

    conditionally if the levels of the age were known. Besides, if the attitude about safety belt use is

    known, the conditional relation between gender and alcohol was determined. Sixty four percent

    of the men reported that they can drive while they are drunk. This rate was 54.9% in the

    women. Odds ratio was found to be 0.66 between these two variables. This means that there

    are 66 male candidates who consider alcohol as a risk with respect to 100 women who do so.

    The only independent variable that is related to alcohol and safety belt use was found to be the

    age variable. 60.3% of the subjects between18-24 ages regarded drunk driving as risky, on the

    other hand 58.8% of the subjects between 25-31 ages regarded drunk driving as risky. 34.8% of

    the subjects older than 32 years reported drunken driving as a risk. The odds ratio of attitude to

    alcohol between 18-24 and 25-31 ages was found to be 0.97. The odds ratio between 18-24

    and 32 + ages and 25-31 and 32 + was found to be 1.23 and 1.34, respectively. From this point

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    10

    of view, the attitude about alcohol is similar between 18-24 and 25-31 ages, but the 32+ age

    group do not see drunk driving as a risk.

    A significant relation was found between gender and safety belt use. 65.5% of the men

    considered safety belt usage unnecessary in downtown while 77% of women found it

    unnecessary. The odds ratio of this statement was 1.76. This statement can be interpreted as

    there are 176 women thinking safety belt use unnecessary while there are 100 men thinking it

    necessary. The odds ratio for safety belt use attitude was found to be 0.7, 10.7, and 15.2 for the

    age groups 18-24 and 25-31, 18-24 and 32 +, 25-31 and 32+, respectively. Especially, the 32+

    age group has more attitudes about the unnecessary of safety belt use in downtown when

    compared to other groups. The percentages of the subjects that declare safety belt use

    unnecessary were 69% for 18-24 years, 62.7% for 25-31 years, and 60.5% for 32+ years.

    DISCUSSION

    In the current study, men of 32+ years of age and women who attend a driving course are found

    to be risky driver candidates. While women have worse attitudes than men in safety belt use,

    men tend to drink and drive more frequently than women. If safety belt use is accepted as a

    personal flaw, it can be considered that they endanger only themselves. However, men who

    drive under the influence of alcohol endanger not only themselves but also other drivers,

    pedestrians and other people in the same vehicle as well. From this point of view, it is not wrong

    to say that men have more dangerous attitudes than women. Using these results one can

    extrapolate that men of 32+ years of age have dangerous attitudes in traffic. When the safety

    belt use percentages are examined in Hawaii, Australia, Canada, USA, and Turkey as being

    0.81, 0.91, 0.87, 0.58, and 0.21, respectively, it is very clear that the drivers in Turkey have bad

    habits about this issue.

    An individuals behaviors should be identified and understood through his/her attitudes. Like

    many of the behaviors, the attitudes have also gained through learning. For these reasons,

    before investigating drivers behaviors and attitudes in traffic, inferences may be made as to

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    11

    they were involving an attitude toward to traffic before and during candidate driving process.

    Individuals traffic education and teaching them traffic culture before they drive in the traffic

    should be discussed in an integrated frame. Drivers behaviors and attitudes toward traffic do

    not emerge immediately after they begin to drive in the traffic. While the individual is being

    socialized, he/she carries his/her attitudes culturally gained toward traffic together with traffic

    education gained as a response tendency in the traffic system which he/she actively takes

    place. To determine candidate drivers attitudes on this subject before being active drivers

    would provide opportunity to create an accurate training system or a process

    The relation between demographic characteristics of the drivers and the risky attitude are

    determined scientifically in the studies that exist in the literature. For this reason, these risky

    attitudes should be determined during education and should be transformed into positive

    attitudes bearing in mind that these candidates will be active drivers in the near future. It will be

    useful to explain the harms of speeding, drunk driving, and the necessity of safety belt use with

    the aid of visual communication devices and statistics in the lessons during the driving course

    program.

    REFERENCES

    Allport, G.W. (1967) Attitudes, readings in attitude theory and measurement, Ed. Martin

    Fishbein. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc, pp.1-14.

    Aberg, L., (1996) The role of perceived risk of detection in the Theory of Planned Behavior.

    International Conference on Traffic and Transport Psychology, 22-25 May, Valencia,

    Spain.

    Ajzen, I.,(1988) Attitudes, Personality and Behaviour. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

    Ajzen, I., (1991) The theory of planned behavior. Organizational, Behavior and Human Decision

    Processes, Vol 50, pp.179 -204.

    Assum, T. (1997) Attitudes and road accident risk. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol

    (29),No.2, pp.153 159.

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    12

    Calisir, F., Lehto M.R. (2002) Young drivers decision making and safety belt use. Accident

    Analysis and Prevention, Vol 34, pp.793805.

    Chaudhary, N.K., Salomon, M.G., Cosgrove, L.A. (2004) The relationships between perceived

    risk of being ticketed and self-reported seat belt use. Journal of safety Research, Vol 35,

    pp.383-390.

    Driver Education in Turkey and World. T C General Directorate of Security Head of Traffic

    Services Traffic Education and Research Center Report 2005-2007, Ankara, Turkey.

    Edwards, D., Kreiner, S. (1983) The analysis of contingency tables by graphical models.

    Biometrika, Vol 70, pp.553-562.

    Elander, J., West, R., French, D., (1993) Behavioral Correlates of Individual Differences in

    Road-Traffic Crash Risk: An Examination of Methods and Findings, Psychological

    Bulletin, Vol 113, pp.179-294.

    Elliott, M.A., Armitage, C.J., Baughan, C.J., (2003) Drivers compliance with speed limits: an

    application of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol 885,

    pp.964-972.

    Everett, S.A., Shuls, R.A., Barrios, L.C., Sacks, J.J., Lowry, R., Oeltmann, J. (2001) Trends and

    Subgroup differences in Transportation-Related Injury Risk and Safety behaviors Among

    High School Students, 1991-1997. Journal of Adolescent Healthy, Vol 28, pp. 228-234.

    Forward, S., (1994) Theoretical Models of Attitudes and the Prediction of Drivers Behaviour,

    Uppsala Psychological Reports, No.434, Department of Psychology, Uppsala Unv.,

    Sweden.

    Iversen, H., Rundmo, T. (2002) Personality, risky driving, and accident involvement among

    Norwegian drivers. Personality and Individual Differences, Vol 33, pp.1251-1263.

    Iversen, H. (2004) Risk-taking attitudes and risky driving behavior. Transportation Research

    Part F, Vol 7, No.3,135-150.

    Kim, S., Kim K. (2003) Personal, temporal and spatial characteristics of seriously injured crash-

    involved seat belt non-users in Hawaii. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol 35, pp.121

    130.

    Lauritzen, S.L. (1996) Graphical models. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    13

    Lauritzen, S. and Wermuth, N. (1989) Graphical models for associations between variables,

    some of which are qualitative and some quantitative. Annals of Statistics, Vol 17, pp.31-

    57.

    Li, L., Kim, K., Nitz, L. (1999) Predictors of safety belt use among crash-involved drivers and

    front seat passengers: adjusting for over-reporting. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol

    31, pp.631-638.

    Parker, D., Manstead, A.S.R., Stradling, S., Reason, J.T. (1992) Determinants of intention to

    commit driving violations. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol 24, pp.117131.

    Parker, D., Reason, J.T., Manstead, A.S.R.,, Stradling, S.G., (1995a) Driving errors, driving

    violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics, Vol 385, pp.1036-1048.

    Parker, D., West R., Stradling, S., Manstead, A.S.R., (1995b) Behavioral Characteristic and

    Involvement And Different Types of Traffic Accident. Accident Analysis and Prevention,

    Vol 27,No.4, pp.571 - 581.

    Parker, D., Lajunen T., Stradling, S. (1998) Attitudinal predictors of interpersonally aggressive

    violations on the road. Transportation Research Part F, Vol 1, pp.11-24.

    Pearl, J. (1993) Causal diagrams for empirical research. Biometrika, Vol 82, pp.669-710.

    Pearl, J. (1995a) Graphical models, causality and intervention. Statistical Sciences, Vol. 8,

    pp.266-269.

    Pearl, J. (1995b) Causal inference from indirect experiments. Artificial Intelligence in Medicine,

    Vol 7, pp.561-582.

    Pearl, J. (1998) Graphs, causality, and structural equation models. Sociological Methods and

    Research, Vol 27, pp. 226-284.

    Pearl, J. (2000) Causality: models, reasoning, and inference. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Pigeot, I., Blauth, A., Bry, F. (2000) Interactive analysis of high-dimensional association

    structures with graphical models. Metrika, Vol 51,No1, pp.53-65.

    Schechtman, E., Shinar, D. , Comton R.C. (1999). The relationship between drinking habits

    and safe driving behaviors. Transportation Research Part F, Vol 2, pp.15-26.

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    14

    Shin, D. Hong, L. , Waldon, I. (1999) Possible causes of socioeconomic and ethnic differences

    in seat belt use among high school students. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol 31,

    pp.458-496.

    Shinar, D. (1993) Demographic and socioeconomic correlates of safety belt use. Accident

    Analysis and Prevention, Vol 25 6, pp.745-755.

    Stewart J.R. (1993) Seat belt use and accident involvement: a comparison of driving behavior

    before and after a seat belt law. Accident Analysis and Prevention, Vol.25,No6, pp.757-

    763.

    Summala, H. (1996) Accident risk and driver behavior. Safety Science, Vol.22, pp.103-112.

    Ulleberg, P., Rundmo, T. 2003. Personality, attitudes and risk perception as predictors of risky

    driving behavior among young drivers. Safety Science, Vol.41, pp.427-443.

    Wermuth, N. and Lauritzen, S. (1990) On Substantive research hypotheses, conditional

    ndependence graphs and graphical chain models. Journal of the Royal Statistics Society,

    Series B, Vol.51, pp.21-72.

    Whittaker, J. (1990) Graphical models in applied multivariate statistics, New York: John Wiley,

    Sons.

    Yagil, D. (1998) Gender and age-related differences in attitudes toward traffic laws and traffic

    violations. Transportation Research Part F Vol.1, pp.123-135.

    Yilmaz, V., Cangur, S., elik, H.E., (2005) Gender difference and earthquake experience effects

    on earthquake victims. Personality and Individual Differences, Vol.39, pp.341-348.

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    15

    Fig. 1 The independence graph of the log-linear model [AB], [BCD]

    A B

    C

    D

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    16

    Figure 2. The [bcde],[ abce] model graph

    (d) (c)

    (e) (b)

    (a)

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    17

    Figure 3. The [bcde] ,[abe] model graph

    (d) (c)

    (e) (b)

    (a)

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    18

    Figure 4. The [bde], [bcd],[ abe] model graph

    ,

    (d) (c)

    (e) (b)

    (a)

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    19

    Table I. The demographical distribution of individuals in the sample

    Variable n %

    Age

    1824 184 71.3

    2531 51 19.8

    32 + 23 8.9

    Gender

    Male 145 56.2

    Female 113 43.8

    Education Level

    Elementary school 71 27.5

    High school 74 28.7

    University 113 43.8

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    20

    Table II. The first step results of the analysis done to reach the most proper model

    Interaction 2

    Degree of

    free p

    ab 37.0896 22 0.0231

    ac 23.7370 18 0.1638

    ad 14.9771 13 0.3088

    ae 21.4303 13 0.0648

    bc 149.8777 28 0.0000

    bd 53.1245 19 0.0000

    be 38.0345 19 0.0059

    cd 36.6492 17 0.0038

    ce 28.8983 19 0.0676

    de 22.8647 12 0.0289

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    21

  • ALATOOACADEMIC STUDIES; ISSN:16945263

    Volume 5 Number 2 Year 2010; P:14-23

    http://www.iaau.edu.kg/aas/AAS5-2.pdf

    22

    Table III. The Results of the hypotheses

    Hypotheses Symbol Graphic Result

    H1: ae a e

    confirmed

    H2: be b e

    Not confirmed

    H3: ce c e

    Not confirmed

    H4: de d e

    confirmed

    H5: ad a d

    Not confirmed

    H6: bd b d

    confirmed

    H7: cd c d

    confirmed

    H8: bda /

    confirmed

    H9: bec /

    confirmed

    H10: edc /

    confirmed

    H11: dec /

    confirmed