Top Banner
ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT FIT: AN EYE TRACKING STUDY by Lijuan Pi A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Fashion Studies Spring 2011 Copyright 2011 Lijuan Pi All Rights Reserved
132

ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

May 22, 2022

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT FIT:

AN EYE TRACKING STUDY

by

Lijuan Pi

A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial

fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Fashion Studies

Spring 2011

Copyright 2011 Lijuan Pi

All Rights Reserved

Page 2: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT FIT:

AN EYE TRACKING STUDY

by

Lijuan Pi

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Sharron J. Lennon, Ph.D.

Professor in charge of thesis on behalf of the Advisory Committee

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Marsha A. Dickson, Ph.D.

Chair of the Department of Fashion and Apparel Studies

Approved: __________________________________________________________

George H. Watson, Ph.D.

Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Charles G. Riordan, Ph.D.

Vice Provost for Graduate and Professional Education

Page 3: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to thank everyone who offered help in my master study and

research. First, I like to express my sincere appreciation to my advisor, Professor

Sharron J. Lennon for her generous support, continuous direction, and endless

encouragement in my master study. I would like to thank Professor Jaehee Jung for her

valuable comments in my proposal and research. I also would like to thank Professor

Kelly Cobb for her encouragement and kindly help from the origination of the research

topic. I would like to thank my friend Rich Burns and his advisor Professor Sandra

Carberry at University of Delaware for offering me this valuable access to eye-tracker.

I also would like to thank all the other professors and staffs in Dept. of Fashion and

Apparel Studies for giving me all kinds of help in the past two years.

Finally, I would like to thank my family and my friends who give me the

mental power to complete my study.

Page 4: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................... vi

LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................... vii

ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................ viii

Chapter

1. INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................................... 1

Statement of Problem ......................................................................................... 2

Purpose of the Study ........................................................................................... 6

Definition of Terms .......................................................................................... 10

2. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ................................................................................... 12

Online Shopping for Apparel ........................................................................... 12

Online Visual Product Presentation Techniques .............................................. 13

Self-Discrepancy Theory .................................................................................. 15

Body Cathexis ................................................................................................... 18

Body Image Discrepancy .................................................................................. 19

Visual Attention, Eye Movement and Eye Tracker .......................................... 21

Negative Psychological State on Visual Attention ........................................... 24

Consumers’ Concern with Fit ........................................................................... 25

Confidence in Fit Judgment .............................................................................. 27

Purchase Intention ............................................................................................ 29

Body Cathexis and Body Image Discrepancy ................................................... 31

Body Cathexis, Body Image Discrepancy and Visual Perception .................... 32

Visual Perception of Garment Fit and Concern with Fit .................................. 36

Concern with Fit and Confidence in Fit Judgment ........................................... 38

Concerns with Fit and Purchase Intent ............................................................. 39

Confidence in Judgment and Purchase Intention.............................................. 39

Effect of Body Sites on Visual Perception ....................................................... 40

Page 5: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

v

3. METHOD ................................................................................................................. 42

Sample .............................................................................................................. 42

Stimuli .............................................................................................................. 42

Experimental Design ........................................................................................ 44

Instruments ....................................................................................................... 45

Procedure .......................................................................................................... 51

4. ANALYSIS OF RESULTS ...................................................................................... 56

Demographics ................................................................................................... 58

Descriptive Statistics for Variables .................................................................. 58

•Body Cathexis ........................................................................................ 58

•Body Discrepancy .................................................................................. 59

•Visual Attention ..................................................................................... 61

•Concern with Fit..................................................................................... 62

•Confidence in Judgment......................................................................... 63

•Purchase Intention .................................................................................. 64

Hypotheses Testing ........................................................................................... 65

5. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS .................................................................... 84

Discussion ......................................................................................................... 85

Conclusions and Implications ........................................................................... 96

Limitations ...................................................................................................... 101

Appendix A- Garment Stimuli ................................................................................... 102

Appendix B- Body cathexis scale and Body image discrepancy scales ..................... 106

Appendix C- Concern with Fit, Confidence in Judgment, and Purchase Intent ......... 109

Appendix D- Permission for using photos of human model ...................................... 110

Appendix E- University of Delaware IRB Approval .................................................. 111

References .................................................................................................................. 112

Page 6: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

vi

LIST OF TABLES

Table 4.1 Statistic analysis ......................................................................................... 57

Table 4.2 Demographics: Age, Weight, and Height ................................................... 58

Table 4.3 Descriptive Statistics for Body cathexis and sub-items .............................. 59

Table 4.4 Descriptive Statistics for Body self-discrepancy ........................................ 61

Table 4.5 Descriptive Statistics for duration and numbers of fixations ..................... 62

Table 4.6 Descriptive Statistics for concern with fit .................................................. 63

Table 4.7 Descriptive Statistics for Confidence of judgment ..................................... 64

Table 4.8 Descriptive Statistics for Purchase intent ................................................... 64

Table 4.9 Linear Regression Results for Hypotheses 2 .............................................. 66

Table 4.10 Linear Regression Results for Hypotheses 3 .............................................. 68

Table 4.11 Linear Regression Result for Hypothesis 4 ................................................ 70

Table 4.12 Linear Regression Result for Hypothesis 5 ................................................ 71

Table 4.13 Linear Regression Result for Hypotheses 6 ................................................ 72

Table 4.14 Linear Regression Result for Hypotheses 7 ................................................ 72

Table 4.15 One-way repeat measures ANOVA Result for Hypothesis 8 ..................... 75

Table 4.16 Means of duration of fixations and number of fixations ............................ 76

Table 4.17 Summery of Hypotheses Testing Results ................................................... 81

Page 7: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

vii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 2.1 Theoretical framework of current study ..................................................... 41

Figure 3.1 An example of stimulus divided into AOIs ................................................ 48

Figure 3.2 An example of “hot-spot” graph ................................................................ 49

Figure 3.3 An example of “scan-path” graph .............................................................. 50

Figure 3.4 Tobii eye tracker (www.tobii.com) ............................................................ 54

Figure 3.5 An example of a questionnaire item divided into AOIs ............................. 55

Figure 4.1 Hypotheses tested in this study .................................................................. 56

Figure 4.2a Effect of body sites on duration of fixations in Front view ....................... 78

Figure 4.2b Effect of body sites on duration of fixations in back view ........................ 78

Figure 4.3a Effect of body sites on number of fixations in Front View ....................... 79

Figure 4.3b Effect of body sites on number of fixations in back view ......................... 80

Figure 5.1 Construction details and draping effects might draw attention ................. 96

Figure A.1 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 2 ......................................... 102

Figure A.2 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 4 ......................................... 103

Figure A.3 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 6 ......................................... 104

Figure A.4 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 8 ......................................... 105

Page 8: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

viii

ABSTRACT

The current online apparel retailers are not able to provide enough visual

information about garment fit to online apparel shoppers, even though various visual

product presentation techniques have been developed. The present study was designed

to analyze consumers’ visual perception of garment fit in order to provide managerial

suggestions for online apparel retailers. This study used self-discrepancy theory

(Higgins, 1987) as a theoretical base. The purposes of this study were to examine the

effect of subjective factors (body image discrepancy, body satisfaction) and objective

factors (body sites) on consumers’ visual perceptions (duration of fixations and

number of fixations) of garment fit, and to examine the effect of visual perception on

consumers’ concern with fit judgment, confidence in fit judgment, and purchase

intention for the garment. Forty-five college women participated in this study for extra

credit and incentives. Eight photos (front view and back view of blazers of four sizes

worn by a human model) were created as visual stimuli. Participants’ duration time

and number of fixations on the visual images of blazers when making judgments of fit

of blazers were measured by an eye tracker. Participant’s body image discrepancy,

body satisfaction, concern with fit, confidence in fit judgment, and purchase intention

were also measured. Using descriptive statistics, one-way univariate analyses of

variance and simple regression analyses, the results of present study indicated: 1) there

was a significant relationship between female college consumers’ body image

discrepancy and their body satisfaction-Hypothesis 1 was supported; 2) female

consumers’ body image discrepancy and body satisfaction significantly predicted their

Page 9: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

ix

visual attention over the garments (how long and how often they looked at the

garments) when making garment fit judgments-Hypotheses 2 and 3 were supported; 3)

no relationships was found between female consumers’ visual attention and their

concern with garment fit-Hypothesis 4 was not supported; 4) there was no significant

relationship between female consumers’ concern with garment fit and their confidence

in fit judgments-Hypothesis 5 was not supported; 5) female consumers’ concern with

fit was a significant predictor of their purchase intent for the garment- Hypothesis 6

was supported; 6) there was no significant relationship between female consumers’

confidence in fit judgments and purchase intention- Hypothesis 7 was not supported;

7) Consumers attended to some human body sites more than others -Hypothesis 8 was

supported.

Page 10: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

1

Chapter 1

INTRODUCTION

The online retailing market is fast growing and profitable. According to

the US Department of Commerce, total online retail sales were $134.9 billion for

2009, and online retail sales for the fourth quarter of 2009 increased 14.6% from that

of the fourth quarter of 2008 (US Census Bureau News, 2010). Online retail in both

the US and Western Europe is expected to maintain double-digit growth over the next

five years, according to the forecasts by Forrester Research Inc (Forrester Research,

2010).. Apparel is one of the three product categories that dominate the online retail

market; the others are consumer electronics and consumer computer peripherals. In

2009, online apparel retail sales were $12 billion, accounting for 4.9% of total apparel

retail sales.

Although online apparel shopping offers some benefits, such as

convenience and diverse product categories (Bhatnagar, Misra, & Rao, 2000), there are

a number of obstacles preventing consumers from buying apparel products online. One

of the obstacles is the inability to characterize the product accurately (Hammond &

Kohler, 2000). Many of the important characteristics of a garment, such as color, feel,

and fit, are difficult to communicate “virtually” online, which impedes consumers’

decision making process when they purchase apparel from online stores.

Page 11: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

2

Currently, consumers make their purchase and size-selection decisions

based on two dimensional (2D) photos of garments, videos, and sizing charts; they are

not able to try clothes on when they shop online (Citrin, Stern, Spangenberg, & Clark,

2003). Concerns with fit and size of garments have become serious issues and are the

biggest sources of perceived risk of online apparel shopping (Beck, 2003). The

difficulty in presenting fit information adequately and the inability to try on garments

contributes to a high return rate in online apparel retailing (Horrigan, 2008).

According to Beck (2003), the return rate for apparel bought online was 14%, about

twice as high as return rates for other products bought on the Web. High return rates

increase the cost of selling merchandise for online retailers due to restocking and

reselling costs (Mollenkopf, Rabinovich, Laseter, & Boyer, 2007).

Statement of Problem

To improve the ability to present fit and size information and thereby

reduce the return rates of apparel products, computer aided design (CAD) companies,

such as “Optitex.com” and “My virtual model.com,” have developed virtual-try-on

techniques for the apparel industry, enabling visualization of garments on three

dimensional (3D) avatars. Virtual try-on is defined as a computer simulation that

enables customers to select their garments, and try them on 3D virtual models (Volino,

Cordier, & Magnenat-Thalmann, 2005). The online virtual model is a visual tool that

can improve the ability to represent garment information (color, design, texture, and

fit) and simulate the garment’s look on a consumer’s body (Istook, 2008). Fit

information is the most important type of information that online retailers want to

Page 12: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

3

present to consumers through the virtual model (Ashdown & DeLong, 2007). Lands’

End adopted the virtual try-on service from My Virtual Model (www.mvm.com) in

1998, in an attempt to provide more fit information to online shoppers.

In addition, with the development of the 3D body scanner (TC2, 2010), 3D

body measurements of consumers can be easily obtained. Based on digital 3D body

measurements, more accurate virtual models and garment draping effects can be

created and presented on a computer screen through the use of some computer

algorithms (Istook & Hwang, 2001).

However, most of these virtual models are not as effective as people

imagined. Now, online retailers, such as Lands’ End, have removed the virtual try-on

service from their websites. Usually, these virtual models were developed from a

technical perspective since the developers were computer scientists. However, a

consumer point of view needs to be considered with respect to buying apparel online

(Kim, 2009), because clothing is the "skin" one chooses to wear to project one's self-

image to the public and hence is intimately tied to one's sense of self (Hammond &

Kohler, 2000).

Little research has analyzed consumers’ responses toward virtual models.

In Kim’s (2009) study, questionnaires and an interview were used to access

consumers’ fit evaluation of virtual pants on a virtual model and real pants on a real

body. The results showed that the visual information from the virtual model provided

participants’ an overall idea and perception of the pants fit, but some aspects of the

virtual model made the image illustrating the pants fit inaccurate, such as locations of

tightness and looseness. Lim (2009) did similar research comparing people’s

Page 13: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

4

evaluation of images of virtual garments worn by an avatar to the evaluation of real

garments worn by human models. Her results indicated most respondents preferred

real garments on the real body. Although Kim (2009) indicated that the locations of

tightness and looseness are important for consumers’ evaluation of fit, she did not

obtain information about the specific body locations and how important they are in fit

judgments and in confidence of fit judgments.

Apparel fit judgments are closely linked to peoples' feelings about

themselves and the image they wish to project (Hammond & Kohler, 2000; Kim &

Damhorst, 2010). Body image is defined as a mental image of one’s body and how

individuals perceive their own bodies (Garner & Garfinkel, 1981). Thus, it is

reasonable to expect that apparel fit judgments may be associated with body image and

one way to assess body image is through ratings of body satisfaction. Feather, Ford,

and Herr (1997) investigated 503 female collegiate basketball players’ perceptions of

body and garment fit satisfaction; the result showed that these female athletes’

satisfaction with the fit of their uniform parallels their satisfaction with their body.

Researchers have found that clothing has the potential to improve an individual’s body

image because women evaluate their bodies higher when clothed rather than unclothed

(Markee, Carey & Pedersen, 1990).

Ashdown and Delong (2007) mentioned that the current evaluation and

analysis of garment fit is mainly based on expert judgment and live models’

anthropometric data and also examined the issue of fit of women’s ready-to-wear

(RTW) from the perspective of garment makers. Only a few studies have analyzed

garment fit from the consumers’ perspective. For example, LaBat and DeLong (1990)

Page 14: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

5

analyzed the body cathexis (body satisfaction) and the satisfaction with fit of clothing

of 107 female consumers using survey methodology. The results indicated that the

body satisfaction scores were slightly lower for consumers’ lower body, and also

confirmed a significant relationship between the consumers’ satisfaction with fit and

their satisfaction with their body. Ashdown and DeLong (1995) studied consumers’

tactile perceptions of apparel ease by conducting experiments with a set of computer-

generated pants, the patterns of these pants were created and graded using software.

The results revealed that the people’s perceptible fit variation could be quite small, and

individuals vary in their tolerance for fit variations at different locations on the body.

These studies investigated consumers’ general attitudes about fit and their tactile

perception of fit when they try on or think about clothes.

When a female consumer tries on a garment, her judgment of fit is based

on visual feedback (Delong, Kim, & Larntz, 1993) and tactile perception (Ashdown &

Delong, 1995). The tactile perception could result from the nerves in the surface of the

skin and the deep pressure sensations felt by the nerves in muscles and joints

(McBurney & Collins, 1977). The pressure sensation may result from an overall

feeling of pressure on the whole body and the separate feedback from localized body

sites. In order to learn about fit, these various tactile and visual responses need to be

considered separately. How important is the visual feedback in the fit judgment

process? To answer this question, consider what consumers do when trying on clothes

in the fitting room. One important and inevitable step is to observe their figures in a

mirror. Without the visual feedback obtained from mirror, we would not be able to

make a confident judgment about the garment fit. “Our clothing … is ordinarily felt as

Page 15: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

6

an integral part of ourselves” (Knoblich, Thornton, Grosjean, & Shiffar, 2006, p.34).

We could infer the important role of visual feedback in the perception of clothing fit

from its role in human body perception. Kinsbourne and Lempert (1989) found that

congenitally blind children may mistakenly represent some parts of their bodies when

they are asked to reproduce models of their own bodies with plasticine, which means

that subjective perceptions of one’s own body may be distorted in an absence of

vision.

Purpose of the Study

In the online shopping context, consumers cannot obtain any tactile

sensation from the virtual model, although consumers have the need to touch apparel

products (Workman, 2009). They can only infer a tactile perception from the verbal

descriptions and visual presentations of the textile on the website (Kim & Lennon,

2008).

We expect virtual models to reflect the characteristics of real garments and

real bodies as accurate and realistically as possible. Based on the realism concepts

proposed in Hagen (1986), Ferwerda (2003) identified three varieties of realism in

computer graphics to evaluate how accurately a natural scene was represented by

computer generated images: Physical realism, photo realism, and functional realism.

Physical realism means the computer generated image could provide the same visual

stimulation as the natural scene, which is almost impossible to achieve based on

current computer techniques; photo realism means that the virtual image is able to

produce the same visual response as the natural scene; functional realism means that

Page 16: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

7

the image provides as much visual information as the natural scene (Ferwerda, 2003).

In addition, Ferwerda also proposed two criteria to measure the functional realism of

computer graphics: (1) accuracy, which means that natural object’s physically

measurable properties are correctly represented in the image; (2) fidelity, which means

that the image could provide enough information so that the viewers are able to

perform tasks with the image as they do in the real world. Kim (2009) used these two

criteria to evaluate the effectiveness of virtual models in representing the fit of pants.

She compared 37 participants’ evaluations of fit of pants on a virtual model (generated

by their body-scanning data) and the fit of pants on their bodies. The result showed

that although virtual models have the ability to accurately present the overall visual

information of how pants look on a human body, some aspects of the virtual

simulation made the pants fit image inaccurate. To increase the accuracy and provide

more functional information from the virtual image, computer scientists developed

algorithms for rendering the virtual image (Ferwerda, Westin, Smith & Pawlicki,

2004). However, these algorithms are very expensive and need computational time,

hence it is impossible to use these algorithms with online virtual models. To save

computational time, a visual attention-based rendering algorithm has been developed

to improve the realism of virtual images. This means that only those important cues in

people’s visual attention are rendered with high accuracy and fidelity (Sundstedt,

Debattista, Longhurst, Chalmers & Troscianko, 2005). Researchers have found that a

human’s response to visual stimuli involves two stages; the first is an essentially

parallel stage which occurs within the first glimpse of the stimuli and provides a global

impression; the second stage is to serially examine the detailed information using eye

Page 17: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

8

movements (Todd & Kramer, 1993). According to previous studies in human visual

attention (Torralba, Oliva, Castelhano, & Henderson, 2006; Treisman, 1982), when a

visual stimulus is presented to people, some important visual cues are serially selected

and used to construct the visual perception, while others are ignored. Based on the

knowledge of selective human visual attention and the assistance from some high-tech

equipment (i.e., an eye tracker), researchers can identify important cues in women’s

visual perception of garment fit. These important cues will help scientists develop a

perception-based rendering algorithm to improve the accuracy and fidelity of virtual

models, and finally makes the virtual model more realistic and more effective in

conveying the information of fit and size. Hence, the current study might have future

commercial applications in online retailing.

The purposes of this study were to: 1) examine the effect of body sites on

female consumers’ visual perceptions of a garment; 2) examine the relationship among

female consumers’ body image discrepancy, body satisfaction and visual perception of

garment fit (focus on how long and how often they look at the garments); 3) examine

the relationship among female consumers’ visual perception, concern with garment fit,

confidence in fit judgment, and purchase intent. Generally, the study analyzed the

effect of the subjective factors (body image discrepancy, body satisfaction) and

objective factors (body sites) on consumers’ visual perception (duration of fixations

and number of fixations) of garment fit and how the visual perception affects

consumers’ purchase decision making process.

Eight hypotheses were investigated in this study:

Page 18: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

9

Hypothesis 1- Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

negatively related to their total body satisfaction;

Hypothesis 2a -Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total duration of fixations over the garment when making

judgments of fit;

Hypothesis 2b -Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total number of fixations over the garment when making

judgments of fit;

Hypothesis 3a - Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total duration of fixations over the garment when making judgments of

fit;

Hypothesis 3b - Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total number of fixations over the garment when making judgments of

fit;

Hypothesis 4a - Duration of fixations on human model will be negatively

related to concern with fit;

Hypothesis 4b - Number of fixations on human model will be negatively

related to concern with fit;

Hypothesis 5 - Consumers’ concern with garment fit will be negatively

related to their confidence in fit judgments;

Hypothesis 6 - Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to

their purchase intent;

Page 19: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

10

Hypothesis 7 - Consumers’ confidence with fit judgments will be

positively related to their purchase intent;

Hypothesis 8a - There is a significant difference among the body sites of

the human model on duration of fixations when judging the garment fit;

Hypothesis 8b - There is a significant difference among the body sites of

the human model on number of fixations when judging the garment fit.

Definition of Terms

Terms used in this study are defined as follows:

Body cathexis: Body cathexis is people’s satisfaction or dissatisfaction for

various parts of their body (Rosen & Ross, 1973).

Body image discrepancy: Derived from self-discrepancy theory (Higgins,

1987), it is the differences between aspects of an individual’s real self (i.e. perceived

body shape or size) and idealized self (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe & Tantleff-Dunn,

1999, p. 134).

Concern with fit: “the subjectively determined expectations and amount

of risk perceived by a shopper in relation to the fit and size of the garment in

contemplating a particular purchase decision” (Kim & Damhorst 2010, p. 242).

Confidence of judgment: The strength of belief about the quality of a

judgment or choice (Sniezek, 1992).

Duration of fixation: In this study, how long a participant’s eyes fixate on

an area.

Page 20: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

11

Eye Tracker: It is a measurement device used for measuring eye

movement (Duchowski, 2007).

Fixation: It is the eye movement that stabilizes the retina over a stationary

object of interest and it corresponds to the desire to maintain one’s gaze on an object

of interest (Duchowski, 2007).

Number of fixations: In this study, how many times a participant’s eyes

fixate on an area.

Purchase intention: It reflects “what we think we will buy” (Blackwell

Miniard, and Engel, 2001, p. 283).

Visual attention: Visual attention means people focus their mental

capacities on the selection of sensory input so that the mind can successfully process

the visual stimulus of interest (Duchowski, 2007).

Page 21: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

12

Chapter 2

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

The present study analyzed consumers’ visual perceptions of garment fit in

order to provide managerial suggestions for online apparel retailers. At the beginning

of this chapter, online apparel shopping and visual product presentation techniques

will be discussed as contexts of this study. Self-discrepancy theory will be used as the

theoretical framework of this study. The important concepts, body cathexis, body

image discrepancy, concern with fit, visual perception, confidence of judgment, and

purchase intent, will also be discussed. Eye movement as an observable and

measureable factor which reflects people’s visual perception process will be reviewed.

Online Shopping for Apparel

The growth in the online retailing sector has become the major driving

force for a lot of multichannel retailers. In 2009, the online retail sales have reached

$134.9 billion, and have increased 14.6% in the fourth quarter (US Census Bureau

News, 2010). A Forrester Research report expected that online retail in both the US

and Western Europe would continue a double-digit growth (Forrester Research, 2010).

Apparel is one of the three popular product categories for online shopping; the others

are consumer electronics and consumer computer peripherals. In 2009, 4.9% of total

apparel retail sales came from online retailing and the amount of online sales reached

$12 billion. Consumers use the internet as an important source of information for

Page 22: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

13

buying apparel, because they enjoy some characteristics of online shopping, such as

convenience, unlimited product variety, time-saving nature, and easy accessibility

(Kim, Kim & Lennon, 2007).

In the online apparel shopping context, consumers are not able to try

clothes on before they receive the apparel products. Lack of information about size

and fit of apparel was a major reason why early online apparel sales lagged behind

books and music (Elliot & Fowell, 2000). The concerns with fit and size of garments

have become serious issues and remain important sources of perceived risk of online

apparel shopping (Beck, 2003). In apparel retail websites, attractive visual presentation

of apparel products is considered to be high task-relevant information for online

shoppers (Kim, Kim, & Lennon, 2007), because they depict the characteristics of

apparel products, such as color, texture, and fit, and help consumers make confident

purchase judgments (Eroglu, Machleit, & Davis, 2001). In research by Then and

Delong (1999), a large portion of respondents mentioned that they would like to see a

garment displayed on a human model to see how the garment fit the model’s body.

Online Visual Product Presentation Techniques

Therefore, online apparel retailers have tried innovative technologies to

enhance visual product presentation and provide size and fit information. According to

Retail Forward’s report in 2001, 3D images, virtual models, digital images, and

zooming technology were introduced to display apparel products and provide fit

information. Although Fan, Yu and Hunter (2004) mentioned 3D rotational images

and virtual models as promising trends and believed these new technologies could help

Page 23: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

14

consumers make confident judgments about garment fit, these is still much room for

these new technologies to be improved. Landsend.com removed their MVM (My

Virtual Model) technology from their retail website after using it only for a few years

because the online virtual model was barely used by their customers. Kim (2009) and

Lim (2009) did similar lab experiments, which tested consumers’ attitude and

cognitive responses to computer generated virtual models and found similar results

that consumers preferred the real garments on real human models more than 3D virtual

garments worn by virtual models when they need to make the judgment about garment

fit.

Although some online retailers use online virtual models to provide ways

to virtually present 3D garment information to consumers (Ha, Kwon, & Lennon,

2007), they face the same problem of how to increase the realism of the virtual

presentation or how to display a virtual object in an accurate way. Consumers expect

virtual models to reflect the characteristics of real garments and real bodies as

accurately and realistically as possible. Ferwerda (2003) proposed two criteria to

measure the functional realism of computer graphics: (1) accuracy, which means that

the natural object’s physically measurable properties are correctly represented in the

image; (2) fidelity, which means that the image provides enough information so that

the viewers are able to perform tasks with the image as they do in the real world. Kim

(2009) used these two criteria to evaluate the effectiveness of virtual models in

representing the fit of pants. She compared 37 participants’ evaluations of the fit of

pants on a virtual model (generated by their body-scanning data) and the fit of pants on

their bodies. The result showed that although virtual models have the ability to

Page 24: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

15

accurately present the overall visual information of how pants look on a human body,

some aspects of the virtual simulation made the pants fit image inaccurate. To increase

the accuracy and provide more functional information from the virtual image,

computer scientists have developed algorithms for rendering the virtual image

(Ferwerda et al., 2004).

However, these algorithms need extensive computational time, hence it is

inconvenient to use these algorithms with online virtual models. To save

computational time, a visual attention-based rendering algorithm has been developed

to improve the realism of virtual images. A visual attention-based rendering algorithm

is one for which only the areas in a visual image, on which people tend to place a large

amount of visual attention, are rendered with high accuracy and fidelity. The areas that

receive intense visual attention can be identified by using some attention capture

devices, such as an eye tracker (Sundstedt, Debattista, Longhurst, Chalmers &

Troscianko, 2005). Therefore, one of the goals of the current study is to identify and

analyze the areas in the visual image of garments, on which women are inclined to

place a large amount of visual attention when they are evaluating fit of a garment on

their bodies. The result of this study may help computer scientists to develop better

perception-based rendering algorithms and create a virtual model with better accuracy

and fidelity.

Self-Discrepancy Theory

The major theoretical framework of this study is self-discrepancy theory.

Higgins (1987) proposed self-discrepancy theory that “different types of self-

Page 25: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

16

discrepancies represent different types of negative psychological situations that are

associated with different kinds of discomfort”. Self-discrepancy is defined as the gap

between those characteristics possessed by the actual self and those characteristics

people would like to possess (ideal self). The characteristics could be physical

appearance, academic performance, social popularity, and so on (Jung, Lennon &

Rudd, 2001). Self-discrepancy theory assumed that individuals had a tendency to

compare their perceived selves with an idealized self and this comparison process

could lead to negative psychological states and corresponding behavior (Thompson,

1990; Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). Researchers found that when the discrepancy

between one’s ideal body and actual perceived body becomes larger it may be

associated with psychological depression and may lead to eating disorders (Connell,

Ulrich, Brannon, Alexander, & Presley, 2006; Jacobi & Cash, 1994). Cash and

Szymanski (1995) mentioned that individuals were inclined to attain a match between

their actual self-concept and internal ideal. Rudd and Lennon (1994) also suggest that

people have the tendency to use coping strategies to reduce the self-discrepancy.

The self-discrepancy has been measured by a number of researchers.

Strauman and Higgins (1987) developed the Self-Discrepancy Questionnaire.

Individuals were asked to list the traits related to their actual self and to rate the extent

to which they and their ideal self possesses the trait. The ideal rating was compared

with the actual rating to generate a discrepancy score. Fitzgibbon, Blackman and

Avellone (2000) used a figure rating scale which contained nine schematic figures of

women to measure 389 participants’ actual perceived size and their ideal size.

Participants were asked to select their actual size and their ideal size from the

Page 26: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

17

schematic figures. Cash and Szymanski (1995) developed the widely used Body Image

Ideal Questionnaire (BIQ) for accessing self-ideal discrepancies. The BIQ asked

participants to rate the discrepancy between ideal selves and actual selves on eleven

attributes, such as facial features, skin complexion, height, weight, chest size, hair

thickness, muscle tone and definition, body proportions, physical strength, and

physical coordination.

In the present study, self-discrepancy theory is used to explain consumers’

fit evaluation process in an online apparel shopping context. Previous studies have

found that apparel fit judgments are closely linked to peoples' feelings about

themselves and the image they wish to project (Hammond & Kohler, 2000; Kim &

Damhorst, 2010). When a female consumer tries to buy a garment, she needs to

evaluate how the garment will fit her body, such as “Does the garment fit me or look

good on me”, and also think about if the garment presents a proper image to others,

such as “Do I look positive with this garment”. Obviously, this female consumer not

only needs to perceive herself in terms of her body but also needs to compare her

perceived self appearance with an imagined ideal self. In an online shopping context,

the consumer’s self-ideal comparison process is “visualized” mainly in the consumer’s

imagination with some external assistance (i.e. visual image of garment and verbal

instructions), while in an offline shopping context, this comparison could be visualized

in a mirror, or be perceived by feeling the tightness or looseness of the garment. Kim

and Damhorst (2010) used self-discrepancy to examine the relationships among online

consumers’ body image discrepancy, body dissatisfaction, apparel involvement,

concerns with fit and size of garments, and purchase intentions. The results confirmed

Page 27: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

18

that perceived body image discrepancies were related to body dissatisfaction and

influenced online consumers’ concerns with fit and size of garments. In the current

study, participants’ visual attention to pictures of garments worn by a human model for

an online shopping scenario was analyzed; the result might provide some clues about

how online consumers “visualize” this self-ideal comparison process.

Body Cathexis

Body cathexis is defined as people’s satisfaction or dissatisfaction with

various parts of their body. (Mahoney & Finch, 1976; Rosen & Ross, 1973; Secord &

Jourard, 1953). The original body cathexis scale in Secord and Jourard (1953)

contained 46 items to measure the satisfaction or dissatisfaction toward physical

characteristics on a seven-point Likert scale. Rosen and Ross (1973) developed the

most frequently used body cathexis scale to measure an individual’s body cathexis on

a nine-point Likert scale. The other frequently used instruments to measure people’s

satisfaction toward their body is the Body Areas Satisfaction Scale (BASS), a subscale

of the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire (MBSRQ: Cash &

Strachan, 1999; Cash, 1995). LaBat and DeLong (1990) used the scale developed by

Rosen and Ross (1973) to study relationships between consumers’ satisfaction with fit

and body cathexis. Their results showed that body cathexis was an important factor

related to consumers’ satisfaction with fit. The researchers also found that women

were dissatisfied with the fit of RTW (ready to wear clothing) for the lower body (pant

length, crotch, thigh, hip, and buttocks) and relatively satisfied with upper body fit.

Page 28: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

19

Likewise, they were more satisfied with their upper bodies and less satisfied with their

lower bodies.

Markee, Carey, and Pedersen’s (1990) study showed that clothing

functions to improve an individual’s body cathexis because women were more

satisfied with their bodies when they wore clothes. Apparel fit could also have an

effect on consumers’ body cathexis. Apparel companies use an idealized body type to

develop and present their products. When women do not see themselves fitting that

particular standard, they blame their bodies rather than the clothes for improper fit

(LaBat & DeLong, 1990). It is reasonable to expect that body cathexis could affect the

consumers” evaluation of fit and even have an effect on consumers’ visual perception

of garments.

Body Image Discrepancy

Cash (1990) defined body image as the perception of one’s body and the

feelings about the perception. A more recent view is that body image is a

psychological construct that includes perceptual, affective, and cognitive and

behavioral components (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999, p. 9).

The perceptual component measures tap how people perceive themselves in terms of

their bodies, such as weight and body shape (Brown, Cash, & Mikulka, 1990; Cash,

1994; Cash & Szymanski, 1995; Jung & Lennon, 2003). The affective component taps

the feelings (i.e. upset, distressed, or anxious) about one’s own appearance; the

cognitive component reflects the interpretation of certain appearance features; a

behavioral aspect might include the avoidance of situations that related to body image

Page 29: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

20

(i.e. swimming, wearing revealing clothes) or performance (e.g., exercising, dieting) of

certain behaviors related to body image (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe & Tantleff-

Dunn, 1999, p9).

Body image discrepancy was derived from self-discrepancy theory

(Higgins, 1987), which assumed that individuals have the tendency to compare their

real selves (i.e. perceived body shape or size) with an idealized self (i.e. the ideal body

shape or size they aspire to). The result of this comparison process is thought to lead to

a discrepancy between the perceived self and the ideal self (Thompson, Heinberg,

Altabe & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999, p134). Researchers have confirmed the existence of

body image discrepancy and also found that body image discrepancy could be an

important predictor of body image dissatisfaction and eating disorders (Altabe &

Thompson, 1992; Cash & Deagle, 1997; Chen & Swalm, 1998; Jacobi & Cash, 1994).

To tap body image discrepancy, Cash and Szymanski (1995) developed

the Body-Image Ideals Questionnaire (BIQ). The BIQ scale measures body image

discrepancy and includes two aspects: (1) the extent to which an individual believes

that his/her physical characteristics match his/her physical ideals, and (2) the

importance associated with having or attaining those ideals. Cash (1994) indicated that

body image discrepancy could influence people’s affective, cognitive and appearance

related behaviors. Kozar and Damhorst (2009) sampled 281 women between the ages

of 30 and 80 to examine the relationship between women’s age identity, body image

discrepancy, and tendency to compare themselves with fashion models. The result

indicated that respondents who reported a larger difference between their cognitive and

actual ages tended to have larger perceived appearance discrepancy and lower body

Page 30: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

21

satisfaction, and were more willing to compare themselves with fashion models. Kim

and Damhorst (2010) found that the discrepancy between the perceived body of a

consumer and the body of an ideal apparel model depicted on a retail website

increased the consumer’s body dissatisfaction and hence influenced the concern with

fit and loyalty intention.

Visual Attention, Eye Movement and Eye Tracker

When people attend to visual stimuli they focus their mental capacities on

the selection of sensory input so that the mind can successfully process the visual

stimulus of interest (Rayner, 1998; Duchowski, 2007). There are many models and

terms developed for describing visual attention. The basic words “where” and “what”

roughly describe the selective nature of visual attention (Duchowski, 2007). The

“where” represents the visual selection of specific regions of interest from the entire

visual stimulus for detailed inspection (Von Helmholtz, 1925). The “what” of visual

attention corresponds to the detailed inspection of the spatial region through a

perceptual channel (James, 1981). For example, when considering a visual image,

people are attracted to certain regions in the image over others. The located image

features may attract people’s attention to the places “where” they need to look, and

then they may identify “what” detail is present at those locations. The dual “what” and

“where” feature-driven view of vision is a simplistic but useful way to analyze visual

attention. To gain insight into viewers’ visual attention, analysis of viewers’ eye

movement is one of the commonly used methods. The measurement device most often

Page 31: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

22

used for analyzing human’s visual attention is an eye-tracker, which can measure

viewers’ eye movement when they see a visual stimulus.

Fixations are the most frequently analyzed eye movement in various eye

tracking applications (Ju & Johnson, 2010; Nguyen, Isaacowitz, & Rubin, 2009) and

visual attention studies (Pieters & Wedel, 2004). Fixation is an eye movement that

stabilizes the retina over a stationary object of interest; it corresponds to the desire to

maintain one’s gaze on an object of interest (Duchowski, 2007). Fixation is an eye

movement that will be analyzed in this study. According to Jacob and Karn (2003), the

most commonly recorded eye tracking metrics are: number of fixations over the whole

image, number of fixations over a specific visual area, fixation duration time of overall

image, fixation duration time of a specific visual area. Both the number of fixations

and fixation duration time are indicators of visual attention load (longer duration and

larger numbers of fixations indicate an increase in visual attention) (Duchowski,

2007). In the present research, the numbers and duration time of fixations over a static

photo of human models will be examined to identify the body sites that receive the

most attention when consumers make fit judgments.

An eye tracker is a measurement device used for measuring eye movement

There are four categories of eye movement measurement methods: Electro-Oculo-

Graphy (EOG), sclera contact lens coil, Photo-Oculo-Graphy (POG), and video-based

pupil and corneal reflection (Duchowski, 2007). The first three methodologies have

been developed and widely used for decades and are invasive measurements. The

video-based eye tracker utilizes relatively inexpensive cameras to measure the visible

features of the eye, such as iris-sclera boundary, and it is the most non-invasive

Page 32: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

23

method of assessing eye tracking. By using an eye tracker, eye movements of people

can be generally recorded to ascertain their attention patterns over a visual stimulus.

The eye tracking technology provides objective and quantitative evidence of a person’s

visual and attention processes (Duchowski, 2007). According to Duchowski, the eye

tracker has been widely used in diverse domains related to human visual attention,

such as psychology, linguistics, advertising, computer science, and even

ophthalmology.

For example, in the advertising area, eye tracking can provide insight into

how the consumer disperses visual attention over different forms of advertising. In a

recent study of eye movements over advertisements, Wedel and Pieters (2000)

reported that fixations to the picture and the brand systematically promoted accurate

brand memory but that text fixations did not. Results showed that the more

information that was extracted from an ad during fixations, the shorter the time for

recollection of the brand. An interesting example in ophthalmology is that people’s eye

movements are recorded by a video-based eye tracker to find the facial cues used by

people when they make judgments about how old or tired a face appears (Nguyen,

Isaacowitz, & Rubin, 2009). The results demonstrated that participants spent the most

time looking at the eye region, which means people tend to use the eye region as first

criteria for their judgments of oldness and tiredness. A recent application in fashion

research is in the study of Ju and Johnson (2010), who used use eye tracking

technology to measure to what extent young women, focused their attention on models

in fashion advertisements. The results suggested that young women did pay most

Page 33: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

24

attention to the model because they tend to compare themselves to the models depicted

in the advertisements.

The motivation for utilizing an eye tracker in the present study is to record

and analyze consumers’ visual fixations on the visual image of a garment on a human

model when they make judgments of garment fit. The result of this study may provide

some implication for understanding how consumers perceive visual information for

making fit judgments when they buy clothes online.

Negative Psychological State on Visual Attention

Schwarz, Bless and Bohner (1991) proposed that people in a negative

psychological state tended to use more cognitive effort to process relevant information

than people in a positive psychological state, because they were more motivated than

their counterparts. In the present study, this theory was applied to explain the effect of

body image discrepancy and body dissatisfaction on consumers’ visual attention for

judging garment fit. According to self-discrepancy theory, females who had large body

image discrepancy and were less dissatisfied with their body might experience a

negative psychological state. In addition, Kuo, Hsu and Day (2009) used the duration

of fixations and numbers of fixation obtained from eye-tracking technology to indicate

people’s cognitive effort in processing information for making judgments or decisions.

The results indicated that respondents under negative mood tended to fixate longer and

more frequently at the visual image in making a judgment than their counterparts

under positive mood. Hence, it is reasonable to expect that those females who had

large body image discrepancy and less body satisfaction would have the longer

Page 34: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

25

fixation duration and a larger number of fixations on the human model when they

made judgments of fit of a garment.

Consumers’ Concern with Fit

Fit refers to how well the garment conforms to the 3D human body. Good

fit is crucial to consumer satisfaction. Fit may be evaluated at many different points in

the product life cycle for apparel manufacturers, retailers, and consumers (Brown &

Rice, 2000; Goldsberry, Shim, & Reich; 1996). Five classic elements for evaluating fit

are: (1) grain, (2) set, (3) line, (4) balance, and (5) ease (Brown & Rice, 2000; Erwin,

Kinchen, & Peters, 1979). For apparel manufacturers, all areas of the garment must be

examined when evaluating the fit of a garment. Some body sites are more important,

such as shoulder, bust, neckline, collar, lapel, armscyes, sleeves, waistline, hips,

crotch, and garment length (Brown & Rice, 2000). For a female consumer, her

judgment of fit is based on visual feedback (Delong, Kim, & Larntz, 1993) and tactile

perceptions (Ashdown & Delong, 1995). The tactile perception could come from the

pressure sensations felt by the nerves in skin, muscles and joints (McBurney &

Collins, 1977).

In an online shopping context, it is impossible to obtain tactile sensory

information based on current technology. Hence, the visual image of the garment has

become the major source for consumers to visualize the proper fit of a garment during

online shopping. Hence, when consumers purchase garments online, they have high

levels of concern with size and fit, because they lack some necessary sensory

information (e.g., pressure sensation) to evaluate garment fit. They make purchase

Page 35: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

26

judgments based on the limited visual and verbal information about the garments

presented on the website. The lack of necessary information about size and fit has

become a major perceived risk for purchasing apparel online (Forsythe, Liu, Shannon,

& Gardner, 2006).

Researchers defined online apparel shoppers’ concerns with fit and size

“as the subjectively determined expectations and amount of risk perceived by a

shopper in relation to the fit and size of the garment in contemplating a particular

purchase decision” (Kim & Damhorst 2010, p. 242). They also developed an

instrument for measuring concern with fit and size, which included 5 factors: concerns

with overall appearance, concerns with inability to try on in online shopping, concerns

with projecting a correct impression, concerns with unavailability of size, concerns

with imagining fit/size in online shopping. The concern with fit and size scale was

tested among 348 female college students using a web-based survey, the results

showed that two factors (concern with overall fit and concern with imagining fit/size

in online shopping) had negative relationships with consumers’ online purchase

intention. The concern with overall fit included the item, “The garment may not look

good on me”; the concern with imagining fit/size in online shopping included item

“Shopping in the website, I may have a hard time picturing myself wearing the

garment”. These items suggest that the lack of necessary visual information displaying

how garments look on consumers’ bodies was the major source of perceived risk for

buying clothes online. Hence, there is a need for online retailers to develop new visual

presentation techniques to visualize how well garment fit conforms to a consumer’s

body and how garments of various sizes drape differently on consumers’ bodies. This

Page 36: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

27

new technology may decrease consumers’ concern with fit and size, increase their

confidence shopping online, and finally increase their willingness to purchase online.

Confidence in Fit Judgment

Sniezek (1992) stated that confidence in judgment reflected an

individual’s strength of belief about the quality of a judgment or choice. People’s

confidence or uncertainty about a judgment determines whether the people would act

upon the judgment (Sniezek & Henry, 1989). Laroche, Kim and Zhou (1996) revealed

that consumers’ confidence about their selection of a brand had a positive impact on

their intention to purchase a product from that brand. Peterson and Pitz (1988)

demonstrated that confidence and uncertainty of a judgment or selection were affected

by the available information. When the amount of information was manipulated within

subjects, the confidence in judgment or selection increased as more information

become available. When the amount of information was manipulated between

subjects, the effect of amount of information on confidence in judgment was not

observable. According to Sicilia and Ruiz (2010), the amount of information has

effects on consumers’ selection and attitudes toward the product presented on the

website. In terms of online apparel shopping, Park, Stoel and Lennon (2008) indicated

that consumers relied on the visual and verbal information presented on the website to

make their purchase decision in online shopping.

Urbany, Dickson, and Wilkie (1989) divided consumer’s confidence into

two categories, knowledge confidence and choice confidence. Knowledge confidence

means how certain a consumer is regarding what is known about the attributes of a

Page 37: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

28

product. Choice confidence represents how confident a consumer is to make a choice

among alternatives. Based on this conceptualization, the confidence in judgment of

garment fit would include two aspects, how certain an individual is regarding her

knowledge of fit and how confident an individual is that the selected garment would fit

the body. For apparel manufacturers and apparel researchers, panels of expert judges

and trained paraprofessional judges have frequently been used to judge garment fit

(Ashdown & O'Connell, 2006). The standard knowledge framework accepted by

apparel manufacturers and researchers for judging fit was usually developed based on

the five components of fit: ease, line, grain, balance, and set (Erwin, Kinchen, &

Peters, 1979). However, there is little research about what kind of judging rule is

generally used by consumers. Since apparel consumers usually learn information about

products from apparel manufacturers and sales people (Häubl & Trifts, 2000), they

may have adopted part of theses rules from daily interaction with apparel salespersons.

According to Urbany et al. (1989), confidence in choice is related to the perceived

information for making a judgment. Ashdown and O'Connell (2006) mentioned that

experts made judgments of fit by visually assessing a garment on a body; the visual

perceptual assessment was a common strategy used to judge the fit of apparel products

in apparel product development and research. Hence, the confidence in choice in

garment fit judgments should be highly related to the visual information perceived

from the garment. In an online shopping context, the visual presentation of the

garment is the major source of visual information for judgment of garment fit. Even

though some websites have size charts which can help consumers select the right size

of a garment, consumers still need to picture the size of the garment in their minds

Page 38: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

29

based on the text information of the size charts. Some consumers might experience

difficulty in picturing the size of the garment in their minds (Kim & Damhorst, 2010).

Obviously, the perceived visual information used in fit judgments in online apparel

shopping is very limited. Hence, online shoppers feel uncertain about their garment fit

judgments and have a high level of concern with garment fit. Finally, consumers’

willingness to buy garments online could be affected (Rosa, Garbarino & Malter,

2006).

Purchase Intention

Purchase intention is the last stage in a consumer’s decision-making

process for an apparel product (Chen-Yu & Kincade, 2001). Purchase intention

reflects “what we think we will buy” (Blackwell, Miniard, & Engel, 2001, p. 283). In

the online apparel shopping context, the brand familiarity, and apparel information

presented on a website could affect consumers’ purchase intention for apparel (Kim,

Fiore, & Lee, 2007;Park & Stoel, 2005; Szymanski & Hise, 2000). Researchers also

found that consumers’ perceived risk toward the product and online retailer influenced

consumers’ intention to shop online (Jarvenpaa & Todd, 1997). Further, apparel

products were considered to be more risky than books and software by consumers in

online shopping (Bhatnagar et al., 2000). The previous studies suggest that inability to

try on apparel in an online shopping context was one of the major reasons for

perceived risk which in turn affected consumers’ purchase intention.

Previous research has found that consumers’ willingness to purchase

garments online could be affected by the methods used to present product information,

Page 39: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

30

such as visual image, verbal message, and rotating garment display. Chen-Yu and

Kincade (2001) found that online consumers made a purchase decision by gathering

information about garment quality and fit through what was displayed on the screen.

Eroglu, Machleit, and Davis (2001) proposed an online shopping model that suggested

that verbal information about size, price, customer service, and visual image affected

consumers’ purchase decisions. Furthermore, Chau and Tam (2000) argued that visual

images were more effective and efficient in communicating information compared to

verbal messages in the online shopping context. Researchers found that providing a

better visual display could reduce perceived risk and increase consumers’ intention to

purchase (Park, Lennon, & Stoel, 2005). Then and DeLong (1999) found that a large

percent of their respondents preferred that apparel be displayed on a realistic human

model rather than displayed on a mannequin or a flat surface so they could visualize

the garment silhouette. Park, Stoel and Lennon (2008) examined the effect of a

rotational display on consumers’ response; the results indicated that the rotational

product display decreased perceived risk and increased purchase intent.

Khakimdjanova and Park (2005) suggested that online retailers could increase

consumer’s interest in purchasing by adding visual features to their website, such as a

whole view of the model, alternative body shapes for a mannequin and alternative

human model poses. Therefore, product information, such as verbal information, a

visual image, displaying garments on human model, and rotating images could be

important for reducing consumers’ perceived risk and facilitating online shopping.

Previous studies also found that consumer’s beliefs and attitudes toward

online shopping and their previous online shopping history had significant effects on

Page 40: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

31

their current intention to buy products from an online retailer (Lee & Park, 2009). The

more positive people’s previous experience, the higher the possibility they would like

to engage in online shopping activities (Szymanski & Hise, 2000; Park, Lennon, &

Stoel, 2005).

In addition, the Internet has also been widely used by consumers as a

source for product information (McQuitty & Peterson, 2000). Based on survey data

collected from 706 respondents who had who resided in 15 U.S. metropolitan areas

and had access to a computer at home or work, researchers found that consumers’

intention to search for product information online was not only a strong predictor of

online purchase intention but also a mediator between purchase intention, attitude

toward online shopping and previous Internet purchase history (Shim, Eastlick, Lotz &

Warrington; 2001).

Body Cathexis and Body Image Discrepancy

Body cathexis reflects people’s satisfaction or dissatisfaction for the body.

Body image discrepancy means the differences between individual’s real perceived

body and an idealized one. According to self-discrepancy theory, the discrepancy

between a real self and an idealized self could lead to negative psychological state and

corresponding behavior (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). Since

people’s body cathexis is a kind of psychological state toward their body, it is

reasonable to infer that people’s body image discrepancy could predict their body

cathexis. The larger body image discrepancy might lead to higher level of body

dissatisfaction. Previous research confirmed that body image discrepancy could be an

Page 41: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

32

important predictor for body dissatisfaction and eating disorders (Jacobi & Cash,

1994; Altabe & Thompson, 1992). Williamson, Gleaves, Watkins and Schlundt (1993)

even conceptualized body dissatisfaction as the discrepancy between self and ideal

body size estimates. A sample of 110 women were instructed to estimate their current

body size and their ideal body size by making a selection from nine silhouettes of

different body sizes (from very thin to very obese) on separate cards. Individuals’ body

dissatisfaction was measured by a Body Dissatisfaction Scale (Garner, Olmstead &

Polivy, 1983). The results showed that measures of self-ideal body size discrepancy

were positively correlated with measures of body dissatisfaction. Kim and Damhorst

(2010) used the Body Image Ideal Questionnaire (Cash & Szymanski, 1995) to

measure 348 college women’s body image discrepancy and tapped their body

dissatisfaction in items borrowed from Heinberg and Thompson (1995). Their result

indicated that consumers’ perceived body image discrepancy was positively related to

body dissatisfaction. This rationale led to the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 1: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

negatively related to their total body satisfaction.

Body Cathexis, Body Image Discrepancy and Visual Perception

Garment fit may be closely related to body cathexis, because garments

have the potential to improve the people’s body shape and enhance their perception

and feelings about the body (Markee, Carey & Pedersen, 1990; Petrie, Tripp, &

Harvey, 2000; Wendel & Lester,1988). Consumers’ fit satisfaction might be related to

their body satisfaction. LaBat and DeLong (1990) indicated that body cathexis is an

Page 42: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

33

important predictor for consumers’ satisfaction toward garment fit. The researchers

found that female consumers were more satisfied with their upper bodies than with

their lower bodies; correspondingly, their satisfaction with the fit of RTW for the

upper body was higher than their satisfaction with fit for lower body.

In addition, previous studies found that consumers’ perceptions of garment

fit are closely related to people’s feelings about themselves and the image they wish to

project (Hammond & Kohler, 2000; Kim & Damhorst, 2010). Consumers not only

need to perceive the visual information about how the garment conforms to the body,

they also think about if the garment would present a proper image to others. Self-

discrepancy could be used to explain this internal visualization process. In a real-store

environment, a female consumer usually perceives the visual information about how

the garment is displayed on her body from a mirror and internally compares this visual

image with an imagined ideal image about how this garment should look on her body.

In an online apparel shopping context, the consumer perceives the visual information

of the garment itself, uses the visual information to picture an image in her mind about

how the garment would display on her body, and finally compares this image with the

ideal look she would like to project. Based on self-discrepancy theory, Kim and

Damhorst (2010) found that individuals who had a large discrepancy between their

real body and ideal body were more dissatisfied with their bodies and finally felt a

higher degree of concern with fit and size of garments. Their instrument included the

items, such as “The garment may not look good on me” and “Shopping in the website,

I may have a hard time picturing myself wearing the garment”. Thus, this research

Page 43: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

34

demonstrates that consumers’ visual perception of garment of fit is related to their

body image discrepancy.

Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that individuals who have a large

body image discrepancy and higher level of body dissatisfaction might need to look

longer to obtain enough information to assess themselves in a garment and hence their

visual perception process for a garment might take more time than those with less

discrepancy and less dissatisfaction. With the assistance of an eye-tracker, researchers

can study people’s visual perception of visual stimuli by accurately measuring the

visual attention over them (Keith, 2009). The eye tracker can capture and measure the

fixations, which is an eye movement that stabilizes the retina over a stationary object

of interest. The number of fixations and the duration of fixations reveal how much

visual attention is placed on visual stimuli. Longer duration and more fixations reflect

an increase in visual attention.

Some researchers use the eye tracker to study consumer behavior. For

example, Pieters and Warlop (1999) used eye tracking data to analyze consumers’

perceptual process of brand choice. The research found that consumers accelerated

visual information acquisition under high time pressure, because they decreased the

duration of eye fixations on the stimuli. Ju and Johnson (2010), who used use an eye

tracker to measure to what extent young women, focused their attention on models in

fashion advertisements. They found that young women who tended to compare

themselves to the models depicted in the advertisements paid most attention to the

model.

Page 44: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

35

Kuo, Hsu and Day (2009) used the duration of fixations and numbers of

fixations obtained from eye-tracking technology to indicate people’s cognitive effort in

processing information for making judgments or decisions. They found that

respondents under negative mood were prone to take more cognitive effort (longer

duration of fixations and greater number of fixations) in making a judgment than their

counterparts under positive mood. According to self-discrepancy theory, consumers

who have a large body image discrepancy and a high level of body dissatisfaction may

experience a negative psychological state. Hence, it is reasonable to infer that

consumers’ negative emotion induced by a large body image discrepancy and a high

level of body dissatisfaction would lead to longer durations and more fixations on the

visual display of a human model wearing a garment when consumers evaluate the fit

of the garment. Therefore, the following hypotheses were developed.

Hypothesis 2a: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total duration of fixations over the human models when

making judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look longer at the human model if

they havea large body image discrepancy.)

Hypothesis 2b: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total number of fixations over the human model when

making judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look more often at the human model

if they have a large body image discrepancy.)

Hypothesis 3a: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total duration of fixations over the human model when making

Page 45: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

36

judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look longer at the human model if they are

less satisfied with their bodies.)

Hypothesis 3b: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total number of fixations over the human model when making

judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look more often at the human model if they

are less satisfied with their bodies.)

Visual Perception of Garment Fit and Concern with Fit

Online shoppers perceive visual information about garment fit from the

visual display of garments presented on websites. Park, Stoel and Lennon (2008)

indicated that visual display of garments was a crucial factor for consumer’s selection

of garments in the online shopping context. Then and Delong (1999) found that

consumers preferred a visual image of a garment displayed on a human model over a

visual image of a garment displayed on a mannequin or a flat surface. They also found

that consumers felt less uncertain about purchasing apparel online when the garment

was displayed on a human model. The results imply that consumers may perceive

more information about fit from the picture of a garment wore by a human model,

which could reduce consumers’ perceived risk for buying garment online.

Kim and Damhorst (2010) defined concern with fit as the perceived risk

related to fit of a garment when making a purchase decision. The researchers

constructed this concept with five aspects: concerns with overall appearance, concerns

with product performance, concerns with unavailability of size, concerns with

projecting a correct impression, and concerns with imaging fit online. They found that

Page 46: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

37

only two aspects, concern with overall appearance and concern with imagining fit

online was negatively related to consumer’s apparel purchasing decision. The results

may imply that the consumers who have difficulty imagining their overall appearance

based on the visual information perceived from the visual display of a garment might

need more information to feel confident about choosing a well fitted garment online.

Park and Stoel (2002) suggested that sensory (i.e., visual and tactile) information such

as garment draping effect and fabric construction was helpful to reduce perceived risk

for online apparel shoppers. Consumers who obtained more visual information from a

visual display of a garment on the website would have less concern with fit when

buying clothes online. Hence, it is reasonable to propose that consumers who look

longer and more often at the visual display of a garment might obtain more

information and reduce their concern with fit.

Based on the previous discussion, an eye tracker could help researchers

analyze the process of perceiving visual information from visual stimuli. The number

of fixations and the duration of fixations indicate how much visual attention is loaded

on visual stimuli. Longer duration and more fixations reveal people place more

attention on a visual image and might obtain more information from the visual image.

Therefore, the following hypotheses were developed.

Hypothesis 4-a: Duration of fixations on a human model will be

negatively related to concern with fit (i.e., Consumers who look longer at the photos

of a human model wearing blazers have less concern with fit.)

Page 47: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

38

Hypothesis 4-b: Number of fixations on a human model will be

negatively related to concern with fit (i.e., Consumers who look more often at the

photos of a human model wearing blazers have less concern with fit.)

Concern with Fit and Confidence in Fit Judgment

Concern with fit was defined as the perceived risk related to fit of a

garment when making a purchase decision (Kim & Damhorst, 2010). Based on the

definition of knowledge confidence and choice confidence in Urbany et al. (1989),

consumers’ confidence in fit judgment refers to how certain an individual is regarding

her knowledge of fit and how confident she believes the selected garment would fit the

body. Researchers found that consumers’ confidence for selecting a product was

related to consumers’ previous shopping experience (Laroche et al., 1996), the amount

of information available about the product (Peterson & Pitz, 1988; Urbany et al.,

1989), and the perceived risk relevant to purchasing the product (Cox & Stuart, 1964;

Bennett & Harrell, 1975). Perceived risk researchers (Cox & Stuart, 1964) mentioned

that the perceived risk for buyers was closely related to the degree of confidence the

consumer felt about his/her ability to judge the outcome of making a purchase. In the

case of buying garments online, consumers need to judge the fit of the garment before

they buy it. Hence the perceived risk for buying a garment would be related to the level

of confidence a consumer has in his/her ability to make a correct fit evaluation.

Therefore, the following hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 5: Consumers’ concern with garment fit will be negatively

related to their confidence in fit judgments.

Page 48: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

39

Concerns with Fit and Purchase Intent

Concern with fit is an important issue for online apparel shoppers, because

they are not able to try the garments on before they purchase. Thus, they are not able to

obtain enough information about how the garment will look on their body and whether

the garment would present the proper image they desire (Rosa, Garbarino & Malter,

2006). The lack of enough information about garment fit increases the perceived risk

for online apparel shopping. Hence, concern with fit was conceptualized as

subjectively perceived risk related to fit of a garment when making a purchase decision

(Kim & Damhorst, 2010). These researchers found that the online apparel shoppers’

concern with fit was negatively related to consumers’ intention to make a purchase

online. Other researchers found similar results that the concern with fit of garments

due to the inability to examine the garment online had a negative impact on

consumers’ online shopping behavior (Kim & Lennon, 2010; Rosa et al., 2006). The

concern with garment fit inhibits the consumer from making a purchase decision.

Therefore, the following hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 6: Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to

their purchase intent.

Confidence in Judgment and Purchase Intention

A certain number of researchers have indicated that consumers’

confidence in judgment about attributes of products or brands affect their purchase

intent (Cox & Stuart, 1964; Howard & Sheth, 1969; Peterson & Pitz, 1988; Urbany et

al. 1989) Laroche, Kim and Zhou (1996) stated that intention to buy a specific product,

such as cough medications, will be positively affected by a consumer's knowledge

Page 49: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

40

confidence and choice confidence toward a product. Bennett and Harrell (1975) used

survey methodology to examine the role of consumers’ confidence to judge attributes

of a product in the formation of their purchase intention; the results demonstrated a

positive relationship between people’s confidence in their ability to judge attributes of

a product and intention to purchase the product. Fit is an important attribute of a

garment which is necessarily considered by consumers before making a purchase

(Ashdown & O'Connell, 2006). Consumers judge garment fit based on tactile

feedback, how the garment feels on the body, and visual feedback, how the garment

looks on the body (Ashdown & Delong, 1995). When consumers buy clothes online,

they judge garment fit based on the visual and verbal information presented on the

website (Park et al., 2008). Being afraid to buy a badly fitted garment prevents people

from buying clothes online (Beck, 2003). Hence, consumers’ confidence of fit

judgment would be an important predictor for their intention to purchase clothes.

Therefore, the following hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 7: Consumers’ confidence with fit judgments will be

positively related to their online purchase intent.

Effect of Body Sites on Visual Perception

Human visual attention researchers found that people are more easily

attracted by salient features in a visual image, such as vivid color, contour, shape, and

texture (Kanwisher & Driver, 1992). A garment hanging on a human body may display

various winkles; wrinkles at some specific body sites, such as bust and chest, are

important features for apparel experts to judge the fit of the garment (Brown & Rice,

2001). This implies that some body sites may offer salient features which could draw

Page 50: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

41

more attention in people’s visual perception of fit. In addition, researchers also find

that people had different body cathexis scores over the body sites, which means people

are more satisfied with some specific body sites than the others. Labat and Delong

(1990) investigated 107 female college students and found that these consumers were

more satisfied with their upper body sites, such as neck and shoulder, and less satisfied

with their lower bodies, such as hip and buttock. Furthermore, these female

consumers’ satisfaction with the fit of RTW garments was correspondingly varied.

Since consumers’ perception of garment fit is based on tactile response and visual

information (Ashdown & Delong, 1995), people’s satisfaction with specific body sites

may affect this perception process. The following hypotheses were developed.

Hypothesis 8-a: There is a significant difference among the body sites

of the human model on duration of fixations.

Hypothesis 8-b: There is a significant difference among body sites of the

human model on number of fixations.

Figure 2.1 Theoretical framework of current study

Body image

Discrepancy

Body

Satisfaction

Visual Attention

Fixation

Duration

Fixation

Number

Concern

with Fit

Confidence

of Judgment

Purchase

Intention H1

H2

H3

H4 H5

H6

H7

Body

Sites

H8

Page 51: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

42

Chapter 3

METHOD

Sample

Data were collected from a convenience sample of female college students

from a medium-sized mid-Atlantic university. The students were recruited by making

announcements on classes. The recruiting announcements were made in three classes

of fashion majors. The course instructor offered some extra credit for students who

participated in this research. The researcher made individual appointments for each

volunteer participant to complete the research tasks. College students were chosen as

participants, because they use the internet frequently and they are likely customers for

online apparel retailing (Lee & Lin, 2005). Most college students tend to use the

Internet for gaining product information before purchasing a product and also use the

Internet to purchase products (Kim et al., 2007; Yoo, Lee, & Park, 2010).

Stimuli

A business blazer was chosen as the garment stimulus, because it covers

most of the body, is more fitted than other types of garments worn on the upper body,

and fits closer to the body than some other apparel items, such as sweaters or t-shirts.

Some researchers have used the business blazer as a typical garment silhouette for

understanding size (Ashdown, & Dunne 2006), to assess perception of garment

Page 52: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

43

proportion (Delong, Kim, & Larntz, 1993), and to assess fit preference (Alexander,

Connell, & Presley, 2005). For example, Delong et al. (1993) tested 172 undergraduate

women’s responses to twelve sets of blazer silhouettes varying proportionally in

components of lapel, yoke, and pocket details. The research results indicated that

participants detected differences in proportions of garment details within the context of

blazer silhouettes and preferred proportions adjusted to smaller sizes.

For the current study, four business blazers in the same color (dark grey),

fabric (made from 80% wool, 19% cotton, and 1% spandex), and style (notched lapel)

but different sizes (2, 4, 6 and 8) were purchased from a major fashion brand that

targets young female consumers. This set of blazers was selected so that the only

variation among the blazers was the size. The purpose of using four different sizes of

blazers in this study is to simulate the situation in a real fitting room where consumers

try on garments of different sizes and observe how the garments look from the mirror.

A size 4 female college student was recruited to model the four blazers in photographs.

She wore the same white blouse and black high heel pumps with each size of blazer.

No jewelry and other accessories were worn by the model to avoid introducing other

factors that could affect participant responses. One front-view photo and one back-

view photo were taken for each size of the blazer which was worn by the model. The

distance between the model and the camera was fixed, so that the four blazers could be

displayed in the same area of the photos. Totally, eight photos were taken. In these

photos, the model’s face was removed, in order to reduce the impact of facial

appearance of the model on people’s perceptions of garments. For example,

researchers have found that consumers who had more positive beliefs about a model's

Page 53: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

44

facial appearance indicated higher intention to purchase the model's clothing, and

perceived the model's clothing to be more fashionable (Kozar & Damhorst, 2008;

Kozar, 2010). In addition, the size of the blazers was labeled in the four photos. To

remove the impact of size number on participants’ fit judgments, the numbers (2, 4, 6,

and 8) were replaced with the symbols ( ).The eight photos as visual stimuli

were uploaded to the eye tracker (See Appendix A).

Experimental Design

The whole experiment consisted of three parts. The first part was to test

whether and how each participant’s visual attention to the stimulus garment would

vary across the body sites (e.g., neck, shoulder, bust, waist, arm, abdomen).

Participants’ visual attention (in terms of duration of fixations and number of

fixations) on all the body sites of the human model was recorded by an eye tracker,

when they were asked to make fit judgments of the blazers. Four different sizes of

blazers were used to simulate the scenario that consumers try on garments of different

sizes in a real fitting room. Hence, the body-sites were a within-subjects factor. In this

part of experiment, the participants’ visual attention was the dependent variable, the

body sites of human model was used as the independent variable. Participants were

also requested to complete the measures of concern with fit, confidence of fit

judgment, and purchase intention, after they viewed the photoes of each blazer. Scores

on these measures were used to test whether participant’s overall visual attention was

related to their concern with fit, and to examine the relationship among these variables.

Page 54: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

45

In the second part of the experiment, participants completed measures of

body cathexis and body discrepancy. Scores on these measures were used to examine

the relationship between the two variables and to examine whether these variables

could be related to participants’ visual attention on blazers.

Instruments

The Body cathexis scale (See Appendix B) was adapted from the body

cathexis scale used in the study of Labat and Delong (1990). The body cathexis scale

used in Labat and Delong’s (1990) research used a nine-point Likert-type scale (1=

very dissatisfied, 9=very satisfied) measuring feelings of satisfaction with 22 body

sites for upper body, lower body, and total body. In Labat and Delong’s (1990)

research the researchers examined the correlation between female consumers’ body

cathexis and their satisfaction with fit of ready-to-wear garments. The body cathexis at

eight body sites (bust, arm, back, shoulder, hip, thigh, buttock, and abdomen) was

found to have a significant correlation with consumer’s fit satisfaction at

corresponding body sites. This scale was widely used to assess participants’ feeling

about the self and their satisfaction/dissatisfaction with their bodies (Markee, Carey &

Pedersen, 1990; Pisut & Connell, 2007) The reliability of this scale was reported to be

0.83 (Shim, Kotsiopulos, & Knoll, 1991). In the present study, the body cathexis scale

(See. Table 3.1) measured individual’s satisfaction or dissatisfaction with nine body

sites (neck, shoulder, bust, back, waist, abdomen, hip, buttock, and arm). The nine

upper body sites were chosen, because the business blazer is worn on the upper body

and was the garment stimuli used in the current research, and individuals’ body

Page 55: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

46

satisfaction with the upper body was the research focus. In addition, seven upper body

sites (bust, arm, back, shoulder, hip, buttock, and abdomen) have been confirmed to be

related to fit satisfaction (Labat & Delong, 1990). Another two body sites (neck, waist)

were important in the judgment of garment (i.e., blazer) fit were on the upper body

(Pisut & Connell, 2007). The overall body cathexis score was calculated by summing

together the cathexis scores for the nine body sites. Higher cathexis scores indicated a

higher level of body satisfaction.

Body Image Discrepancy Scale (See Appendix B). The discrepancy

between participants’ perceived bodies and ideal bodies was measured by a scale

adapted from Body-image Ideals Questionnaire items (Cash & Szymanski, 1995). The

original BIQ assessed internalized ideals and included 10 appearance characteristics

(height, skin complexion, hair texture and thickness, facial features, muscle tone and

definition, body proportions, weight, chest size, physical strength, and physical

coordination). For each characteristic, participants were asked to think of their ideal

for each characteristic (how they wish they were) and then to evaluate how well they

actually match that ideal.

For the present research, the BIQ scale was modified to tap respondent’s

self-ideal discrepancy on nine body sites (neck, shoulder, bust, arm, back, waist, hip,

buttock, and abdomen). The nine relevant body sites were chosen because female

consumers’ perceptions of these body sites either was confirmed to be significant

predictors of their satisfaction with fit of garments (Labat & Delong, 1990), or played

important roles in the process of judging fit of a garment (Ashdown & O'Connell,

2006). Discrepancy between the ideal status and actual status of a body site was

Page 56: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

47

measured using a 4-point scale: 0 (exactly as I am), +1 (almost as I am), +2 (fairly

unlike me), and +3 (very unlike me). Importance of each body site was also measured

using a 4-point scale: 0 (not important), 1(somewhat important), 2 (moderately

important), and 3 (very important). The final body image discrepancy was calculated

by multiplying the discrepancy score for each body site by the importance scores of the

same body site and then summing the weighted scores for all nine body sites. Higher

scores indicate higher levels of body image discrepancy. The internal consistency of

original BIQ was reported to be 0.77 in Cash and Szymanski (1995)’s study.

Measurement of visual attention. Participant’s visual attention was

captured by the Tobii ET-17 eye tracker, which is a computer with a camera built into

the monitor. Two major parameters of visual attention, the number of visual fixations

and duration of fixations were measured. Duration time, measured in milliseconds,

refers to the amount of time that a person’s eye stays on a specific area. Number of

fixations refers to the number of times the eye located or hit on the same area.

Duration time and number of fixations are used to indicate how much visual attention

is placed on specific body sites. In order to output the duration time and number of

fixations on each body site, the software of the eye tracker allowed the researcher to

divide the visual stimulus (photo of human model wearing blazers) into areas of

interest (AOI) based on the relevant body sites, including neck, shoulder, bust, waist,

arm, back, abdomen, hip, and buttocks (See Figure 3.1). The specific areas of concern

are the same as assessed using the BIQ and body cathexis. The output of the eye

tracker software included the “hot-spot” graph (See Figure 3.2), “scan-path” graph

(See Figure 3.3), and AOI data files. The number of fixations and duration times were

Page 57: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

48

respectively calculated for each AOI and saved in AOI data files by the eye tracker

software. In addition, participant’s overall number of fixations and duration time of the

entire body of human model are also part of the AOI data of the eye tracker.

Figure 3.1 an example of stimulus divided into AOIs

Page 58: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

49

Figure 3.2 an example of “hot-spot” graph

Page 59: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

50

Figure 3.3 an example of “scan-path” graph

Concern with fit and size scale (See Appendix C). Kim and Damhorst

(2010) developed a scale for tapping consumers’ concerns with fit and size, which

included five latent factors: Concerns with body image and overall appearance,

concerns with product performance, concerns with unavailability of size, concerns

with projecting a correct impression, and concerns with uncertainty about the sizing

system. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the latent factors ranged from .79 to .93.

The study found consumers’ concerns with fit and size of garments was affected by

their body satisfaction, body image self-discrepancy, and influenced their loyalty

intention. In the present research, the three items used in the study of Kim and

Damhorst (2010) were adapted to measure consumers’ concern with fit. The items

include “The blazer of this size may not fit me”, “The blazer of this size may not look

good on me”, and “I may have a hard time picturing myself wearing the garment”. The

Page 60: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

51

three items used a five-point Likert format with endpoints of 1= strongly disagree and

5= strongly agree.

Confidence in Judgment scale (See Appendix C). The overall confidence

in fit judgment was measured by five-point scale items (1=not confident/certain at all,

5=very confident/ certain) modified from the two-item confidence in judgment scale

used in (Laroche, Kim & Zhou, 1996). The reliabilities of the two-item measures were

reported from 0.81 to 0.89 for different brands. In the present study, the two items

were “how confident you are about your evaluations of fit of garments” and “to which

extent you are certain about this size of blazer is fit for you”

Purchase intention scale (See Appendix C). Previous studies have shown

that consumers’ body image (Alexander et al., 2005; Rosa et al., 2006; Shim et al.,

1991) and concern for fit (Kim & Damhorst, 2010) contributes to variation in purchase

intention for apparel products. One purchase intent item was adopted from the scale

used in Kim and Lennon (2007) to measure individual’s intention to purchase a blazer

after viewing the visual image of the garment wore by a human model. The item “I

would like to buy a blazer of this size from this retailer” used a five-point Likert

format with endpoints ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Procedure

The eight photos of four blazers (See Appendix A) were uploaded to the

eye tracker (Tobii ET-17) as the visual stimuli. The Tobii ET-17 eye tracker is a table-

mounted video-based tracker, which includes a computer monitor equipped with a

camera illuminated by an infrared light source and image process hardware and

Page 61: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

52

software (See Figure 3.4). The camera and the infrared light source were located inside

the monitor, which was mounted on a table surface about two feet in front of the

participant. The eye tracker is capable of measuring a viewer’s eye movement by

capturing and calculating the relative position between the corneal reflection and pupil

center (Duchowski, 2007).

Participants completed the experimental task individually. The whole

experiment consisted of two parts. In the first part the eye movement of each

participant was recorded when she viewed the visual stimuli and perceived

information to judge the fit of blazers of four sizes. In addition, the items that

measured participant’s concern with fit, confidence in fit judgment, and purchase

intent were also presented on the computer screen immediately following presentation

of the visual stimuli. Participants rated their concern with fit, confidence in fit

judgment and purchase intention for all the four blazers. In the second part of the

experiment, participants completed measures of body cathexis and body image

discrepancy using a paper-based survey. Participant’s body cathexis and body image

discrepancy were only measured once.

Before the experiment started, each participant chose a 5-digit random

number as her identification number in this research. This identification number was

used to combine the data collected from the two parts of the experiment and also

protect participant’s privacy.

First, participants entered identification numbers to the eye-tracker, and

then were instructed to do a calibration to make sure the measurement of the eye

movements was accurate. In the calibration process, the participant was asked to stare

Page 62: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

53

at a blue round spot and visually follow the track of the blue spot. At the same time,

the participant’s eye movement was automatically recorded and analyzed by the eye

tracker. The calibration succeeded only when participant’s eye movement was very

close to the track of the blue spot. After completing the calibration, the real experiment

started. Participants were given the following instructions on the computer monitor:

“Please imagine this situation. You need to buy a blazer for a job

interview. However, you are too busy to try some blazers in local stores. One of your

friends has the similar body size as you, and she went to the store to try the clothes for

you. She sent you some photos that were taken when she tried on four blazers of

different sizes. Now, you are going to view these photos and think about if the blazers

will properly fit you. The four sizes of the blazers are size , size , size , and

size . Each size has a front view and a back view. After viewing the photos, you

need to finish a short questionnaire about this blazer. The questionnaire will be

presented on the computer screen; you need to complete the questionnaire using your

eyes. Stare at the option which you believe to be correct about two seconds long, so

that the computer can automatically recognize your choice. Please press the “space”

bar to start the experiment.”

Participants read the instructions and pressed the space bar to browse the

photos of the blazers. After participants finished the ratings of the photos of the

blazers, a short questionnaire appeared on the screen. The questionnaire included three

items for rating each participant’s concern with the fit of the blazer on her body; two

items tapped confidence in her fit judgments regarding the blazers; and one item

assessed purchase intention. Participants completed the items by staring at the selected

Page 63: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

54

choice for about two seconds, so that the computer was able to recognize a

participant’s choice. Specifically, each option was divided into an AOI (See Figure

3.5) in the software of eye tracker. The software was able to calculate the duration time

and fixation number on each AOI and finally recognize participant’s choice by

comparing the duration time and fixation numbers of AOIs. For each blazer,

participants completed the same items about her concerns with fit, confidence in fit

judgments, and purchase intention.

In the second part of this study, participants were asked to complete a

paper-based questionnaire (see Appendix A), which included the scales for measuring

participants’ body cathexis and body image discrepancy. Participants also wrote down

the 5-digit identification number on the questionnaire, so that the answers from the

questionnaire could be linked with the eye tracking recording obtained in the first part

of the study.

Figure 3.4 Tobii eye tracker (www.tobii.com)

Page 64: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

55

Figure 3.5 an example of a questionnaire item divided into AOIs

Page 65: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

56

Chapter 4

ANALYSIS OF RESULTS

SPSS computer programs were used for statistical data analysis.

Descriptive statistical analysis was conducted for all variables. One-way repeated-

measures univariate analysis of variance was conducted to examine if people fixated

longer and more often (in terms of longer duration and larger number of the fixations)

at some body sites (i.e., shoulder) than other body sites (i.e., neck). Simple linear

regression was used to test relationships among body satisfaction, body discrepancy,

concern with fit, confidence in judgment, and purchase intent (See Figure 4.1, See

Table 4. 1). A .05 level of significance was set for all analyses of dependent variables.

Figure 4.1 Hypotheses tested in this study

Body image

Discrepancy

Body

Satisfaction

Visual Attention

Fixation

Duration

Fixation

Number

Concern

with Fit

Confidence

of Judgment

Purchase

Intention H1

H2

H3

H4 H5

H6

H7

Body

Sites

H8

Page 66: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

57

Table 4.1 Statistic analysis

Hypotheses & Variables Statistical Test

H1 IV: Body Image discrepancy Simple Linear regression to test the

relationship between body image

discrepancy and body cathexis DV: Body Cathexis

H2 IV: Body Image discrepancy Simple Linear regressions to test the

relationship of body image discrepancy

to duration of fixations and number of

fixations

DV: Duration of Fixations

Number of fixations

H3 IV: Body Cathexis Simple Linear regressions to test the

relationship of body cathexis to

duration of fixations and number of

fixations

DV: Duration of Fixations

Number of fixations

H4 IV: Duration of Fixations

Number of fixations

Simple Linear regression will be used

to test the relationship among duration

of fixations, number of fixations and

concern with fit, because duration of

fixations and number of fixations were

highly correlated.

DV: Concern with fit

H5 IV: Concern with fit Simple Linear regression to test the

relationship between concern with fit

and confidence in fit judgment DV: Confidence in fit judgment

H6 IV: Concern with fit Simple Linear regression to test the

relationship between concern with fit

and purchase intention DV: Purchase intention

H7 IV: Confidence of judgment Simple Linear regression to test the

relationship between confidence in fit

judgment and purchase intention DV: Purchase intention

H8 IV: Body sites (i.e. neck) One-way repeated-measures

ANOVA to test if people fixated longer

and more often at some body sites (i. e.

shoulder) than other body sites (i.e.

neck).

DV: Duration of Fixations

Number of fixations

Page 67: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

58

Demographics

The demographic data included self-reported age, weight and height. The

mean age of participants (N = 45) was 21 years, with a range of 17 to 25 years. All

participants were female undergraduate students who majored in either fashion design

or fashion merchandising. The mean weight was 115lbs with a range of 98lbs to 155

lbs. The height ranged from 5ft 5in to 5 ft 9in, with an average of 5ft 6in (see Table

4.2).

Table 4.2 Demographics: Age, Weight, and Height

Demographics

of participants

Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean SD

Age 17 25 21 2.31

Weight (lbs) 98 155 115 4.52

Height (ft) 5 ft 5in 5 ft 9in 5 ft 6in 3.94

Descriptive Statistics for Variables

•Body Cathexis

Participants’ body cathexis was measured using the scale revised from the

body cathexis scale used in Labat and Delong (1990). The original body cathexis scale

used in Labat and Delong’s research (1990) accessed satisfaction with 19 body sites. In

the present study, satisfaction with 9 relevant body sites (neck, shoulder, bust, back,

arm, waist, abdomen, hip, and buttock) was summed to form an indicator of overall

Page 68: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

59

body cathexis. The nine body sites were selected because they were highly related to

blazer fit and blazers were chosen as the stimuli in this study. Scores could range from

9 to 81. Higher scores indicate more satisfaction with body parts. The actual range of

overall satisfaction scores was from 30 to 75 (M = 52.05). Among the individual body

sites, participants’ satisfaction scores were relatively high for the neck (M = 6.89); and

relatively low for the abdomen (M = 4.97) (see Table 4.3).

Table 4.3 Descriptive Statistics for Body cathexis and sub-items

Measures Range Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean S.D.

Total Body Cathexis 9-81 30 75 52.05 11.03

Cathexis of Neck 1-9 4 9 6.89 1.78

Cathexis of Shoulder 1-9 3 9 6.87 1.73

Cathexis of Bust 1-9 1 9 5.61 2.31

Cathexis of Back 1-9 3 9 5.89 1.64

Cathexis of Arm 1-9 1 9 5.32 2.13

Cathexis of Abdomen 1-9 1 9 4.97 2.32

Cathexis of Waist 1-9 1 9 5.76 2.11

Cathexis of Hip 1-9 1 9 5.05 1.75

Cathexis of Buttock 1-9 2 9 5.68 1.85

•Body Discrepancy

The discrepancy between participants’ actual and ideal bodies was

measured by a revised version of Body-image Ideals Questionnaire items (Cash &

Szymanski, 1995). The original BIQ assessed individual’s self-ideal discrepancy on 10

Page 69: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

60

appearance characteristics (height, skin complexion, hair texture and thickness, facial

features, muscle tone and definition, body proportions, weight, chest size, physical

strength, and physical coordination). For the present research, the BIQ scale was

modified to tap respondent’s self-ideal discrepancy on nine body sites (neck, shoulder,

bust, arm, back, waist, hip, buttock, and abdomen). The nine relevant body sites were

chosen because female consumers’ perception of these body sites played important

roles in the process of judging fit of a garment (Ashdown & O'Connell, 2006). In the

present study, body discrepancy scale measured participants’ discrepancies between

their ideal and real status of each body site using a 4-point scale: 0 (exactly as I am),

+1 (almost as I am), +2 (fairly unlike me), and +3 (very unlike me). The importance of

each body site toward their appearance was also measured using a 4-point scale: 0 (not

important), 1(some what important), 2 (moderately important), and 3 (very important).

The final body self-discrepancy was calculated by multiplying the discrepancy scores

by the importance scores and then summing the scores.

Hence, the final body self-discrepancy score had a possible range from 0

to 81. Higher scores indicate greater discrepancy. In the present study, participants’

body self-discrepancy scores ranged from 6 to 39 (M = 19.76). The results indicated

that the participants had a small discrepancy between their ideal body and actual body.

Among the individual body sites, neck had a relatively low discrepancy score (M =

0.33); abdomen had a relatively higher discrepancy score (M = 3.96) (see Table 4.4).

Page 70: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

61

Table 4.4 Descriptive Statistics for Body self-discrepancy

Measures Range Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean S.D.

Total Body discrepancy 0-81 3 39 19.6 9.57

Discrepancy on Neck 0-9 0 2 .33 1.78

Discrepancy on Shoulder 0-9 0 6 .84 1.73

Discrepancy on Bust 0-9 0 9 2.64 2.31

Discrepancy on Back 0-9 0 9 1.27 1.64

Discrepancy on Arm 0-9 0 9 3.02 2.13

Discrepancy on Abdomen 0-9 0 9 3.96 2.32

Discrepancy on Waist 0-9 0 9 2.53 2.11

Discrepancy on Hip 0-9 0 9 2.51 1.75

Discrepancy on Buttock 0-9 0 9 2.53 1.85

•Visual Attention

Two major parameters of visual attention, the number of visual fixations

and duration of fixations on visual stimuli were recorded by the eye tracker in less than

20 minutes. Duration time, measured in milliseconds, refers to the amount of time that

a person’s eye stays on a specific spot. Number of fixations refers to the number of

times the eye located or hit on the same spot. In this study, the visual stimuli (front-

view and back-view photos of a human model wearing blazers of different sizes) were

divided into nine areas of interest (AOI) based on the relevant body sections, including

neck, shoulder, bust, waist, arm, back, abdomen, hip, and buttock (See Appendix A).

Participant’s duration time and number of fixations for the AOIs were hypothesized to

be significantly different from each other, for example, waist as an AOI might draw

significantly longer duration and more fixations than abdomen as an AOI. Participant’s

overall duration time and number of fixations were hypothesized to be affected by

Page 71: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

62

participant’s body image ideals discrepancy, and body satisfaction, and influence

participants’ concern with fit.

The duration and number of fixations on the AOIs of front-view and back-

view photos of each blazer (size 2, 4, 6, and 8) were summed respectively. Photos of

blazer of size 4 had the longest duration of fixations, ranged from 2252 to 16882 (M =

6271) milliseconds, and the largest number of fixations, ranged from 9 to 92 (M =

34.9). Photos of blazer of size 8 had the shortest duration of fixations, ranged from

1656 to 15553 (M = 5307), and the largest number of fixations, ranged from 7 to 66

(M = 22.0) (see Table 4.5). The results indicated that the blazer of size 4 received the

most visual attention from participants while the blazer of size 8 received the least

visual attention.

Table 4.5 Descriptive Statistics for duration and numbers of fixations

Visual

stimuli

Duration of fixations* Number of fixations

Min. Max. Mean S.D. Min. Max. Mean S.D.

size 2 2252 16882 6271.8 3322.8 8 53 24.6 10.6

size 4 2292 21128 9430.4 4711.6 9 92 34.9 17.2

size 6 1853 14136 6244.8 2960.2 9 48 24.3 9.9

size 8 1656 15553 5307.4 3386.5 7 66 22.0 12.7

*Duration of fixation was measured in milliseconds.

•Concern with Fit

The Concern with Fit scale, which included three items adapted from

Rosa, Garbarino, Malter (2006) and Kim and Damhorst (2010), measured consumers’

about whether the size of garments fit their bodies and looked good on their bodies.

Page 72: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

63

The three items used a 5-point scale. The scores for the three items were summed and

averaged to form an index yielding a range from 1 to 5. Higher scores indicate

participants had a higher level of concern with the fit of the blazer of a specific size.

Among the 4 sizes of the blazers, participants indicated high level of

concern toward the size 8 blazer (M = 3.8) and low level of concern toward the size 4

blazer (M = 2.2) (see Table 4.6).

Table 4.6 Descriptive Statistics for concern with fit

Measures Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean S.D.

Concern with fit for size 2 1 5 3.0 1.7

Concern with fit for size 4 1 5 2.2 1.1

Concern with fit for size 6 1 5 3.0 1.4

Concern with fit for size 8 1 5 3.8 1.2

•Confidence in Judgment

Confidence in judgment scale, which includes two items revised from

Laroche, Kim and Zhou (1996), measures consumers’ certainty regarding the fit of a

garment and consumers’ certainty regarding choosing the best-fitting garment. The

original two items used in Laroche et al. (1996) tapped consumers’ confidence in

brand judgment. In the present study, these items were modified to access consumers’

confidence in garment fit judgment. The two items used a 5-point scale. The scores of

the two items were averaged to yield a potential range from 1 to 5. Higher scores

indicate higher certainty. Participants’ responses toward the four sizes of blazers were

very close (see Table 4.7).

Page 73: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

64

Table 4.7 Descriptive Statistics for Confidence of judgment

Measures Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean S.D.

Confidence of judgment for size 2 1 5 3.9 1.1

Confidence of judgment for size 4 1 5 4.0 .78

Confidence of judgment for size 6 1 5 3.7 .92

Confidence of judgment for size 8 2 5 4.0 .75

•Purchase Intention

Respondents reported their willingness to buy the blazer from online

retailers. The item was adopted from Kim and Lennon (2007) with a range from 1 to 5.

Higher scores indicate higher willingness to make a purchase from online retailers.

Participants indicated a relatively high level of purchase intent to buy a blazer of size 4

(M = 3.5) and relatively low level of purchase intent toward the blazer of size 8 (M =

2.3) (see Table 4.8).

Table 4.8 Descriptive Statistics for Purchase intent

Measures Female participants

N = 45

Min. Max. Mean S.D.

Purchase intent for size 2 1 5 2.7 1.3

Purchase intent for size 4 1 5 3.5 .99

Purchase intent for size 6 1 5 2.7 1.2

Purchase intent for size 8 1 5 2.3 1.2

Page 74: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

65

Hypotheses Testing

Hypothesis 1: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

negatively related to their total body satisfaction.

Linear regression was used to examine the relationship between body

satisfaction and body image discrepancy. The analysis indicated that body image

discrepancy was a significant predictor of body satisfaction, (1,43) 26.4, .001F p .

Body image discrepancy accounted for 38% ( 2 .38R ) of the variation in body

satisfaction. The two variables were negatively related, .62 . Hence, participants

who had a large discrepancy between their ideal body and actual body felt less

satisfied toward their body. Therefore, H1 was supported.

Hypothesis 2a: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total duration of fixations over the human models when

making judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look longer at the human model if

they have large body image discrepancy.)

Hypothesis 2b: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total number of fixations over the human model when

making judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look more often at the human model

if they have large body image discrepancy.)

For hypothesis 2a, simple linear regression was used to examine the

relationship between total body image discrepancy and total duration of fixations on

the human model. The analysis indicated that body image discrepancy was a

significant predictor for overall duration of fixations on the human model,

(1,43) 9.2, .01F p (for size 2), (1,43) 7.5, .05F p (for size 4),

Page 75: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

66

(1,43) 16.7, .01F p (for size 6), (1,43) 15.8, .01F p (for size 8). Body image

discrepancy accounted for 17~32% ( ) of the variation in overall

duration of fixations on the human model. The two variables were positively

related, .42 ~ .56 . Hence, participants who had a large discrepancy between their

ideal body and actual body looked longer at the human model when they made

judgments of fit. Therefore, Hypothesis 2a was supported.

For Hypothesis 2b, simple linear regression results indicated that the total

body image discrepancy positively predicts the overall number of fixations on the

human model (see Table 4.9). (1,43) 6.01, .05F p (for size 2),

(1,43) 6.42, .05F p (for size 4), (1,43) 14.13, .01F p (for size 6),

(1,43) 17.32, .01F p (for size 8). Body image discrepancy accounted for 14~32%

( 2 .17 ~ .32R ) of the variation in fixations on the human model. The two variables

were positively related, .38 ~ .57 . Therefore, Hypothesis 2b was supported as

well. Participants who had a large self-ideal body discrepancy looked more often at the

human model when they made judgment of fit.

Table 4.9 Linear Regression Results for Hypotheses 2

DV= Duration of Fixation DV= Number of Fixation

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8 Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

IV= Body

image

discrepancy

F 9.21 7.52 16.72 15.84 6.01 6.42 14.13 17.32

.45 .42 .56 .55 .38 .39 .53 .57

.20 .17 .32 .31 .14 .15 .28 .32

.00 .10 .00 .00 .02 .02 .00 .00

Page 76: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

67

Hypothesis 3a: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total duration of fixations over the human model when making

judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look longer at the human model if they are

less satisfied with their bodies.)

Hypothesis 3b: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively

related to the total number of fixations over the human model when making

judgments of fit. (i.e., Consumers will look more often at the human model if they

are less satisfied with their bodies.)

For Hypothesis 3a, simple linear regression analysis was conducted for

each size of blazer to examine the relationship between total body satisfaction and

overall duration of fixations on the human model. The results showed that body

satisfaction significantly predicted the duration of fixations on the human model,

(1,43) 5.23, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 7.81, .01F p (for size 4),

(1,43) 7.42, .05F p (for size 6), (1,43) 4.82, .05F p (for size 8). Total body

satisfaction accounted for 12~18% ( 2 .12 ~ .18R ) of the variation in duration of

fixations. The two variables were negatively related, .42 ~ .34 . Hence,

participants who were less satisfied with their body looked longer at the human model

when they made judgments of fit. Therefore, Hypothesis 3a was supported.

For Hypothesis 3b, simple linear regression analysis (see Table 4.10) was

conducted for each size of blazer to examine the relationship between total body

satisfaction and overall number of fixations on the human model. The results showed

that body satisfaction significantly predicted the number of fixations on the human

model, (1,43) 4.23, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 6.12, .05F p (for size 4),

Page 77: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

68

(1,43) 6.51, .05F p (for size 6), (1,43) 4.22, .05F p (for size 8). Total body

satisfaction accounted for 11~15% ( 2 .11~ .15R ) of the variation in number of

fixations. The two variables were negatively related, .39 ~ .32 . Hence,

participants who were less satisfied with their body looked more often at the human

model when they made judgments of fit. Therefore, Hypothesis 3b was supported.

Table 4.10 Linear Regression Results for Hypotheses 3

DV= Duration of Fixation DV=Number of Fixation

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8 Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

IV= Body

satisfaction

F 5.23 7.81 7.42 4.82 4.23 6.12 6.51 4.22

-.36 -.42 -.41 -.34 -.32 -.38 -.39 -.32

.13 .18 .17 .12 .11 .14 .15 .11

.03 .01 .01 .03 .04 .02 .02 .04

Hypothesis 4-a: Duration of fixations on human model will be

negatively related to concern with fit (i.e., Consumers will have less concern with fit

when they get more information by looking longer at the photos of human model

wearing blazers.)

Hypothesis 4-b: Number of fixations on human model will be negatively

related to concern with fit (i.e., Consumers will have less concern with fit when they

get more information by looking more often at the photos of human model wearing

blazers.)

First, Pearson correlation coefficients between two independent variables,

overall number of fixations and overall duration of fixations, were calculated. The

Page 78: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

69

results showed that these two independent variables were highly correlated, r>.85,

p<.001. The multi-collinear recommendation for computing multiple linear regressions

was violated. Hence, the Hypotheses 4a and 4b were tested individually using simple

linear regression instead of multiple linear regression.

For Hypothesis 4a, simple linear regression was used to examine the

relationship between the total duration of fixations on the human model and concern

with fit. Participants’ duration of fixations on the human model and concern with fit

were repeatedly measured for each size of blazer, the linear regression between these

two variables were repeatedly computed for each size of blazer. The results revealed

no significant relationship between the total duration of fixations and concern with

fit, (1,43) .38, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 2.15, .05F p (for size 4),

(1,43) .17, .05F p (for size 6), (1,43) 3.91, .05F p (for size 8). Total duration of

fixations only accounted for 1~8% ( 2 .01~ .08R ) of the variation in concern with fit

(see Table 4.11). Therefore, Hypothesis 4a was not supported.

For Hypothesis 4b, simple linear regression was calculated to examine the

relationship between the total number of fixations on the human model and concern

with fit. Participants’ total number of fixations on the human model and concern with

fit were repeatedly measured for each size of blazer, the linear regression between

these two variables were respectively computed for each size of blazer. The results

showed no significant relationship between the total number of fixations and concern

with fit, (1,43) .47, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 1.74, .05F p (for size 4),

(1,43) .20, .05F p (for size 6), (1,43) 4.83, .05F p (for size 8). The F ratio

between concern with fit and total number of fixations for all sizes of blazers were

Page 79: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

70

bigger than .05. The overall number of fixations only accounted for 1~10%

( 2 .01~ .10R ) of the variation in concern with fit for all sizes (see Table 4.11).

Therefore, Hypothesis 4b was not supported.

Table 4.11 Linear Regression Result for Hypothesis 4

IV: Duration of Fixation IV: Number of Fixation

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8 Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

DV:

Concern

with fit

F .38 2.15 .17 3.91 .47 1.74 .20 4.70

.09 .218 -.06 -.29 .10 .20 -.07 -.32

.01 .05 .01 .08 .01 .04 .01 .10

.54 .15 .68 .05 .50 .20 .66 .05

Hypothesis 5: Consumers’ concern with garment fit will be negatively

related to their confidence in fit judgments.

For Hypothesis 5, simple linear regression was computed to test the

relationship between the concern with fit and confidence in fit judgment. Participants’

concern with fit and confidence in fit judgments were repeatedly measured for each

size of blazer, hence the linear regression between these two variables were

respectively computed for each size of blazer. The results indicated no significant

relationship between concern with fit and confidence in fit

judgments, (1,43) 1.32, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 1.74, .05F p (for size 6),

(1,43) 2.39, .05F p (for size 8). Noticeably, participants’ concern with fit for blazer

size 4 was significantly related to their confidence in fit judgment for the same size

blazer, (1,43) 18.24, .01F p , .55 . However, the concern with fit for size 2, 6,

Page 80: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

71

and 8 only accounted for 3~5% ( 2 .03 ~ .05R ) of the variation in corresponding

confidence in fit judgment (see Table 4.12). Therefore, Hypothesis 5 was not

supported.

Table 4.12 Linear Regression Result for Hypothesis 5

DV=Confidence in fit judgment

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

IV=Concern with fit

F 1.32 18.24 1.74 2.39

-.17 -.55 -.20 .23

.03 .30 .04 .05

.26 .00 .19 .13

Hypothesis 6: Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to

their purchase intent.

For Hypothesis 6, linear regression results (see Table 4.13) indicated that

concern with fit was a significant predictor for purchase intent,

(1,43) 13.01, .01F p (for size 2), (1,43) 15.32, .01F p (for size 4),

(1,43) 30.71, .01F p (for size 6), (1,43) 32.23, .01F p (for size 8). Concern with

fit accounted for 23~43% ( 2 .23 ~ .43R ) of the variation in purchase intention. These

two variables were negatively related .48 (for size 2), .51 (for size 4),

.65 (for size 6), .65 (for size 8). Participants who were more concerned

about the fit toward a size of garment correspondingly had lower purchase intent for

this size. Therefore, H6 was supported.

Page 81: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

72

Table 4.13 Linear Regression Result for Hypotheses 6

DV= Purchase Intention

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

IV=Concern with

fit

F 13.01 15.32 30.71 32.23

-.48 -.51 -.65 -.65

.23 .26 .42 .43

.001 .000 .000 .000

Hypothesis 7: Consumers’ confidence with fit judgments will be

positively related to their purchase intent.

For Hypothesis 7, simple linear regression was conducted to test the

relationship between confidence in fit judgment and purchase intention. Participants’

confidence in fit judgments and purchase intention were repeatedly measured for each

size of blazer, hence the linear regression between these two variables was respectively

computed for each size of blazer as well. The results indicated no significant

relationship between confidence in fit judgment and purchase

intention, (1,43) 2.43, .05F p (for size 2), (1,43) 1.60, .05F p (for size 6),

(1,43) 2.89, .05F p (for size 8). Noticeably, participants’ confidence in fit

judgment for blazer of size 4 was positively ( .39 ) related to their purchase

intention for the same size blazer, (1,43) 7.93, .05F p . For size 2, 6, and 8,

participants’ confidence in fit judgments only accounted for 4~6% ( 2 .04 ~ .06R ) of

the variation in corresponding purchase intention (see Table 4.14). Therefore, H7 was

not supported.

Table 4.14 Linear Regression Result for Hypotheses 7

DV: Purchase Intention

Page 82: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

73

Size2 Size4 Size6 Size8

IV: Confidence in

fit judgment

F 2.48 7.93 1.60 2.89

.23 .39 .189 -.251

.05 .16 .04 .06

.12 .01 .21 .10

Hypothesis 8a - There is a significant difference among the body sites of

the human model on duration of fixations when judging the garment fit;

Hypothesis 8b - There is a significant difference among the body sites of

the human model on number of fixations when judging the garment fit..

One-way repeated-measures ANOVA were used to examine whether the

duration of fixations and numbers of fixations varied across the body sites (e.g., neck

versus waist). One-way repeated-measures ANOVA tests were conducted for front

view and back view of four sizes of blazers. Specifically, for the front view, the

dependent variables were duration of fixations and number of fixations, while the

independent variable was body site with 7 levels (neck, shoulder, bust, arm, waist,

abdomen, and hip). For the back view, the dependent variables were duration of

fixations and number of fixations, while the independent variable was body site with 6

levels (neck, shoulder, back, arm, waist, and buttock).

For Hypothesis 8a, the one-way repeated-measures ANOVA results

showed that consumers’ duration of fixations significantly varied across the body sites,

F(3.75, 164.831) = 21.41, p < .001(for front view of size 2), F(2.62, 115.41)=54.42, p

1 According to Kerr, Hall and Kozub (2002), when the sphericity assumption was not

met, the “Greenhouse-Geisser” correction was applied to the One-way ANOVA and

changed the degrees of freedom.

Page 83: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

74

< .001 (for front view of size 4), F(2.85, 125.23)=41.00, p < .001 (for front view of

size 6), F(2.52, 111.00)=47.64, p < .001 (for front view of size 8), F(3.46, 152.05) =

17.62, p < .001 (for back view of size 2), F(3.87, 170.41)=27.35 , p < .001 (for back

view of size 4), F(2.85, 125.46)=33.43 , p < .001 (for back view of size 6,) F(3.624,

159.45)=18.36, p < .001 (for back view of size 8). The pairwise comparison results

also indicated that participants’ duration of fixations at bust and waist were

significantly longer than those at other body sites for front views of all sizes. There

was no significant difference between duration of fixations at bust and waist. Hence,

bust and waist are the hot-spots for front view of the blazer, where participants fixated

longest when they made fit judgments. Similarly, duration of fixations at back, waist,

and buttock were significantly longer than those at other body sites for back views of

all sizes. There was no significant difference among back, waist, and buttock. Hence,

back, waist and buttock were the hot-spots for back view of blazer (See Figure 4.2a &

b and Table 4.16). Therefore, H8a was supported.

For Hypothesis 8b, the one-way repeated-measures ANOVA results(See

Table 4.15) showed that consumers’ number of fixations significantly changed across

the body sites, F(3.32, 146.24) = 42.52, p < .001 (for front view of size 2), F(3.43,

151.04) = 52.88, p < .001 (for front view of size 4), F(3.09, 135.76) = 50.17, p < .001

(for front view of size 6), F(2.85, 125.22) = 54.11, p < .001 (for front view of size 8),

F(3.75, 164.83) = 21.41, p < .001 (for back view of size 2), F(3.19, 140.29) = 32.57, p

< .001 (for back view of size 4), F(3.34, 146.94)=37.99, p < .001 (for back view of

size 6,) F(2.52, 111.00) = 47.64, p < .001 (for back view of size 8).

Page 84: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

75

The pairwise comparison results also indicated that participants’ number

of fixations at bust and waist were significantly larger than those at other body sites for

front views of all sizes. There was no significant difference between number of

fixations at bust and waist. Hence, bust and waist are the hot-spots for front view of

blazer, where participants fixated most often when they made fit judgments. Similarly,

number of fixations at back, waist, and buttock were significantly larger than those at

other body sites for back views of all sizes. There was no significant difference among

back, waist, and buttock. Hence, back, waist and buttock were the hot-spots for the

back view of the blazer (See Figure 4.3a & b and Table 4.16). Therefore, H8b was also

supported.

Table 4.15 One-way repeat measures ANOVA Result for Hypothesis 8

Hypothesis 8a

DV = duration of fixations;

IV = body sites with 7 levels (neck, shoulder, bust, arm, waist, abdomen, and hip)

Front

view

Size 2 F(2.33, 102.44) = 31. 19 p < .001

Size 4 F(2.62, 115.41)= 54.42 p < .001

Size 6 F(2.85, 125.23)= 41.00 p < .001

Size 8 F(2.52, 111.00)= 47.64 p < .001

DV = duration of fixations;

IV = body sites with 6 levels (neck, shoulder, back, arm, waist, and buttock).

Back

view

Size 2 F(3.46, 152.05) = 17.62 p < .001

Size 4 F(3.87, 170.41)= 27.35 p < .001

Size 6 F(2.85, 125.46)= 33.43 p < .001

Size 8 F(3.62, 159.45)= 18.36 p < .001

Hypothesis 8b

DV = number of fixations

IV = body sites with 7 levels (neck, shoulder, bust, arm, waist, abdomen, and hip)

Front Size 2 F(3.32, 146.24) = 42.52 p < .001

Page 85: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

76

view Size 4 F(3.43, 151.04)= 52.88 p < .001

Size 6 F(3.09, 135.76)= 50.17 p < .001

Size 8 F(2.85, 125.22)= 54.11 p < .001

DV = number of fixations

IV = body sites with 6 levels (neck, shoulder, back, arm, waist, and buttock).

Back

view

Size 2 F(3.75, 164.83) = 21.41 p < .001

Size 4 F(3.19, 140.29)= 32.57 p < .001

Size 6 F(3.34, 146.94)= 37.99 p < .001

Size 8 F(2.52, 111.00)= 47.64 p < .001

Table 4.16 Means of duration of fixations and number of fixations

Hypothesis

8a

Mean of duration of fixations

neck shoulde

r

bust arm waist abdome

n

hip

Fron

t

view

size 2 53.58 473.96 1319.6

0

462.87 1968.3

3

407.49 307.44

size 4 107.67 876.64 1415.5

1

468.13 2833.8

9

468.91 458.42

size 6 89.96 500.87 1382.9

3

228.0 1379.0

0

217.96 231.16

size 8 38.93 315.40 863.71 199.82 1461.7

6

182.13 165.69

Hypothesis

8a

Mean of duration of fixations

neck shoulder back arm waist buttock

Back

view

size 2 66.02 225.11 515.22 189.98 650.22 543.82

size 4 116.00 346.78 698.33 277.33 950.49 948.36

size 6 10.20 168.87 840.69 137.20 686.29 742.36

size 8 51.84 221.47 596.16 195.76 577.22 653.42

Hypothesis

8b

Mean of number of fixations

Neck Shoulder Bust Arm Waist Abdomen hip

Fron

t

size 2 .27 1.60 5.20 1.98 6.80 1.76 1.24

size 4 .56 3.22 5.67 1.89 8.98 1.82 2.09

Page 86: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

77

view size 6 .42 1.78 5.13 1.09 5.29 1.07 .87

size 8 .18 1.27 3.58 .87 5.80 .80 .73

Hypothesis

8b

Mean of number of fixations

neck shoulder back arm waist buttock

Back

view

size 2 .18 .91 1.84 .82 2.64 2.13

size 4 .44 1.22 2.53 1.09 3.60 3.84

size 6 .04 .64 3.07 .67 2.64 2.98

size 8 .16 .73 2.38 .87 2.40 2.91

Note: unit of duration of fixations is millisecond

Page 87: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

78

Figure 4.2a Effect of body sites on duration of fixations in Front view

Figure 4.2b Effect of body sites on duration of fixations in back view

Page 88: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

79

Figure 4.3a Effect of body sites on number of fixations in Front View

Page 89: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

80

Figure 4.3b Effect of body sites on number of fixations in back view

Page 90: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

81

Table 4.17 Summery of Hypotheses Testing Results

Hypothesis 1: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be negatively related

to their total body satisfaction.

IV= body

image

discrepancy

DV= body

satisfaction

F (1,43) = 26.4 p <

.001

β = -.62 R2 = .38

Hypothesis 2-a: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be positively

related to the total duration of fixations over the human models when making

judgments of fit.

IV = body

image

discrepancy

DV := overall

duration of

fixation

F (1,43) = 9.21a

p < .01 β = .45 R2 = .20

F (1,43) = 7.52b p < .05 β = .42 R

2 = .17

F (1,43) = 16.72c p < .01 β = .56 R

2 = .32

F (1,43) = 15.84d p < .01 β = .55 R

2 =

.31

Hypothesis 2-b: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be positively

related to the total number of fixations over the human model when making

judgments of fit.

IV=body

image

discrepancy

DV= overall

number of

fixations

F (1,43) = 6.01a

p < .05 β = .38 R2 = .14

F (1,43) = 6.42.b p < .05 β = .39 R

2 = .15

F (1,43) = 14.13c p < .01 β = .53 R

2= .28

F (1,43) = 17.32d p < .01 β = .57 R

2= .32

Hypothesis 3-a: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively related to

the total duration of fixations over the human model when making judgments of fit.

IV= body

cathexis

DV= overall

duration of

Fixations

F (1,43) = 5.23a

p < .05 β = - .36 R2 = .13

F (1,43) =7.81 b p < .05 β = -.42 R

2 = .18

F (1,43) =7.42 c p < .05 β = -.41 R

2=.17

F (1,43) =4.82 d p < .05 β = -.34 R

2= .12

Hypothesis 3-b: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively related to

the total number of fixations over the human model when making judgments of fit.

IV= body

cathexis

DV= overall

number of

fixations

F (1,43) = 4.23a

p < .05 β = -.32 R2 = .11

F (1,43) = 6.12.b p < .05 β = -.38 R

2 = .14

F (1,43) = 6.51c p < .05 β = -.39 R

2 =.15

F (1,43) = 4.22d p < .05 β = -.32 R

2= .11

Hypothesis 4a: Duration of fixations on human model will be negatively related to

concern with fit.

Page 91: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

82

IV = overall

duration of

fixations

DV = concern

with fit

F (1,43) = .38a

p > .05 β = .09 R2 = .01

F (1,43) = 2.15b p > .05 β = .218 R

2 = .05

F (1,43) = .17c p > .05 β = -.06 R

2= .01

F (1,43) = 3.91d p > .05 β = -.29 R

2= .08

Hypothesis 4b: Number of fixations on human model will be negatively related to

concern with fit.

IV = overall

number of

fixations

DV = concern

with fit

F (1,43) = .47a

p > .05 β = .10 R2 = .01

F (1,43) = 1.74b p > .05 β = .20 R

2 = .04

F (1,43) = .20c p > .05 β = -.07 R

2 =.01

F (1,43) = 4.70d p > .05 β = -.32 R

2 = .10

Hypothesis 5: Consumers’ concern with garment fit will be negatively related to

their confidence in fit judgments.

IV= concern

with fit

DV= confidence

in fit judgment

F (1,43) = 1.32a

p >.05 β = -.17 R2 = .03

F (1,43) = 18.24b p < .01 β = -.55 R

2 = .30

F (1,43) = 1.74c p > .05 β = -.20 R

2= .04

F (1,43) = 2.39d p > .05 β = .23 R

2= .05

Hypothesis 6: Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to their

purchase intent.

IV= concern

with fit

DV= purchase

intention

F (1,43) = 13.01a

p < .01 β = -.48 R2 = .23

F (1,43) = 15.32.b p < .01 β = -.51 R

2 = .26

F (1,43) = 30.71c p < .01 β = -.65 R

2= .42

F (1,43) = 32.23d p < .01 β = -.65 R

2= .43

Hypothesis 7: Consumers’ confidence with fit judgments will be positively related

to their purchase intent.

IV=

confidence in

judgment

DV= purchase

intention

F (1,43) = 2.48a

p > .05 β = .23 R2 = .05

F (1,43) = 7.93b p < .05 β = .39 R

2 = .16

F (1,43) = 1.60c p > .05 β = .19 R

2=.04

F (1,43) = 2.89d p > .05 β = -.25 R

2= .06

Hypothesis 8-a: There is a significant difference among duration of fixations over

the body sites of the human model when judging the garment fit.

IV=

body

sites (i.e.

neck)

DV= duration

of Fixations

Front

view

F(3.75, 164.83) = 21.41a

p < .001

F(2.62, 115.41)= 54.42b p < .001

F(2.85, 125.23)= 41.00c p < .001

F(2.52, 111.00)= 47.64d p < .001

Page 92: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

83

IV=

body

sites (i.e.

neck)

DV= duration

of Fixations

Back

View

F(3.46, 152.05) = 17.6 a

p < .001

F(3.87, 170.41)= 27.35b p < .001

F(2.85, 125.46)= 33.43c p < .001

F(3.62, 159.45)= 18.36d p < .001

Hypothesis 8-b: There is a significant difference among number of fixations over

the body sites of the human model when judging the garment fit.

IV=

body

sites (i.e.

neck)

DV= number

of fixations

Front

View

F(3.32, 146.24) = 42.52 a

p < .001

F(3.43, 151.04)= 52.88 b

p < .001

F(3.09, 135.76)= 50.17 c p < .001

F(2.85, 125.22)= 54.11 d

p < .001

IV=

body

sites (i.e.

neck)

DV= number

of fixations

b

Back

view

F(3.75, 164.83) = 21.41 a p < .001

F(3.19, 140.29)= 32.57 b

p < .001

F(3.34, 146.94)= 37.99 c p < .001

F(2.52, 111.00)= 47.64 d

p < .001

Note a is for size 2

b is for size 4

c is for size 6

d is for size 8

Page 93: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

84

Chapter 5

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The purposes of this study were to: examine the relationship between

female consumers’ body image discrepancy and body satisfaction, which was

addressed in Hypothesis 1; examine the effects of female consumers’ body image

discrepancy and body satisfaction on their visual perception of garment fit (focus on

how long and how often they look at the garments), which was addressed in

Hypotheses 2 and 3; examine the relationships among female consumers’ visual

perception, concern with garment fit, confidence in fit judgment and the purchase

intent, which were addressed in Hypotheses 4, 5, 6, and 7; examine the effect of body

sites on female consumers’ visual perception of garment fit, which was addressed in

Hypothesis 8. Generally, this research examined the effect of subjective factors (body

image discrepancy, body satisfaction) and objective factors (body sites) on consumers’

visual perceptions (duration of fixations and number of fixations) of garments and the

effect of visual perception on consumers’ concern with fit judgment, confidence in

judgment, and purchase intent for the garment.

Eight hypotheses were investigated in this study: 1) Hypothesis 1-

Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be negatively related to their total body

satisfaction; 2) Hypothesis 2a -Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total duration of fixations over the garment when making

Page 94: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

85

judgments of fit; Hypothesis 2b -Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be

positively related to the total number of fixations over the garment when making

judgments of fit; 3) Hypothesis 3a - Participants’ total body satisfaction will be

negatively related to the total duration of fixations over the garment when making

judgments of fit; Hypothesis 3b - Participants’ total body satisfaction will be

negatively related to the total number of fixations over the garment when making

judgments of fit; 4)Hypothesis 4a - Duration of fixations on a human model will be

negatively related to concern with fit; Hypothesis 4b - Number of fixations on a human

model will be negatively related to concern with fit; 5) Hypothesis 5 - Consumers’

concern with garment fit will be negatively related to their confidence in fit judgments;

6) Hypothesis 6 - Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to their

purchase intent; 7) Hypothesis 7 - Consumers’ confidence in fit judgments will be

positively related to their purchase intent; 8) Hypothesis 8a - There is a significant

difference among the body sites of the human model on duration of fixations when

judging the garment fit; Hypothesis 8b - There is a significant difference among the

body sites of the human model on number of fixations when judging the garment fit.

Discussion

It was predicted in Hypothesis 1 that there is a negative relationship

between college women’s total body image discrepancy and their body satisfaction. As

expected, the results of the current study supported this hypothesis. Female

Hypothesis 1: College women’s total body image discrepancy will be

negatively related to their total body satisfaction.

Page 95: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

86

participants who had a larger discrepancy between their ideal body and their actual

body were prone to be less satisfied with their body. This finding was consistent with

previous studies (Altabe & Thompson, 1992; Jacobi & Cash, 1994; Jung, Lennon &

Rudd, 2001).

According to Higgins, Klein, and Strauman (1987), discrepancy might

exist between the actual self and the idealized self that people would like to be; the

discrepancy may lead to an unpleasant internal psychological state (e.g., dissatisfied

with body) and negative behavior (e.g., eating disorder). When a college woman buys

clothes, she tends to compare the image of her body reflected in a retail store mirror

with the ideal image or expectation that exists in her mind. Attractive physical

appearance is one of the salient attributes people would like to possess (Connell, et al.,

2006). The ideal image in college women’s minds is influenced by the cultural ideals

for beauty, which set the standards for people to evaluate their body and physical

appearance (Irving, 1990; Richins, 1991). Monteath and McCabe (1997) found that

college women are extremely sensitive to cultural ideals of beauty in western culture,

and showed greater levels of body dissatisfaction when their perceived bodies deviated

from the cultural standard.

Page 96: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

87

Hypothesis 2a: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be positively related

to the total duration of fixations over the human models when making judgments of

fit.

Hypothesis 2b: Participants’ total body image discrepancy will be positively related

to the total number of fixations over the human model when making judgments of

fit.

Hypotheses 2a and 2b predicted that participants’ total body image

discrepancy would be positively related to their total duration and number of fixations

over the human model when they make judgments of fit. As expected, these

hypotheses were supported in the current study.

The female participants who had greater body image discrepancy tended to

look longer and more frequently over the human model who wore the garment, when

they were making judgments of fit of the garment. The results are consistent with the

findings in Ju and Johnson (2010), who used the eye tracking technology to measure

the extent to which young women focused their attention on human models in fashion

advertisements. They found that young women who reported themselves frequently

engaged in comparing with fashion models, tended to look longer and more frequently

at human models in fashion advertisements. Monteath and McCabe (1997) indicated

that college women were exposed to cultural ideals (i.e. fashion magazines or

advertisements) and often compared themselves with these idealized images of

thinness and beauty. It is reasonable to expect that women whose perceived bodies

deviated from their idealized bodies would spend longer comparing themselves with

either a fashion model in a magazine or an internalized ideal body in their memory. In

terms of finding a well-fitted garment, they might be more worried about if the

Page 97: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

88

garment on the model’s body looks close to the ideal image which she would like to

present and hence take longer to confirm it.

Hypothesis 3a: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively related to

the total duration of fixations over the garment when making judgments of fit.

Hypothesis 3b: Participants’ total body satisfaction will be negatively related to

the total number of fixations over the garment when making judgments of fit.

Hypotheses 3a and 3b predicted that participants’ total body satisfaction

would be negatively related to their total duration and number of fixations over the

human model when they making judgment of fit.

The female participants who were less satisfied with their bodies tended to

look longer and more frequently over the human model, when they were making

judgments of garment fit. This result could be explained from the perspective that

internal affective states influence visual information processing. Kuo, Hsu and Day

(2009) used the duration of fixations and number of fixations obtained from eye-

tracking technology to indicate people’s cognitive effort in processing information for

making judgments or decisions. They found that respondents under negative mood

were prone to make more cognitive effort (longer duration of fixations and greater

number of fixations) in making a judgment than their counterparts under positive

mood. Schwarz, Bless and Bohner (1991) proposed that people in a bad mood exerted

more cognitive effort in processing relevant information than those in a good mood,

because they were more motivated than their counterparts. It is reasonable to infer that

those women who were less satisfied with their body might go through a relatively

Page 98: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

89

negative psychological state when they were making a fit judgment of a garment, and

hence fixate longer and more frequently on the human model.

Hypothesis 4a: Duration of fixations on human model will be negatively related to

concern with fit (i.e., Consumers who fixate longer at the photos of human model

wearing blazers have less concern with fit.)

Hypothesis 4b: Number of fixations on human model will be negatively related to

concern with fit (i.e., Consumers fixate more often at the photos of human model

wearing blazers have less concern with fit.)

Hypotheses 4a and 4b predicted that female consumers, who fixated

longer and more frequently on the visual image of the human model for evaluating fit

of a garment, would have less concern with fit. Consumers’ concern with fit refers to

the perceived risk related to fit. When consumer can gain more information about fit of

a garment, the perceived risk about fit or concern with fit would be reduced (Bhatnagar

et al., 2000; Forsythe et al., 2006; Park et al., 2005). According to visual attention

literature, people tend to fixate on the informative areas of a visual stimulus

(Rayner, 2009), and the information from fixations were integrated in visual short-

term memory (VSTM) (Hollingworth, Richard & Luck, 2008). People might gain

more information about the fit of a garment, when they fixate longer and more

frequently on the visual image of a human model wearing the garment. Previous

researchers (Kim & Lennon, 2010; Park & Stoel, 2002) found that the amount of

information could reduce the perceived risk for online shopping. Because concern

with fit was defined as perceived risk about fit for online shopping (Kim & Damhorst,

2010), it is reasonable to postulate that consumers might have less concern with fit

Page 99: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

90

after they obtain more information about fit by fixating longer and more often on the

visual image of a human model wearing the garment.

However, these hypotheses were not supported for all sizes of blazers

tested in this study. The overall duration of fixations and number of fixations had no

significant relationship with concern with fit for all sizes of blazers. There are several

reasons for failing to support these hypotheses. First, only three items, “The blazer of

this size may not fit me”, “The blazer of this size may not look good on me”, “I may

have a hard time picturing myself wearing the garment” were adopted from the

original concern with fit scale used in Kim and Damhorst (2010)’s research. (The

reason is too many items presented on the screen of an eye tracker might make

participants get bored and sabotage the reliability of the whole experiment.) These

three items may be inadequate to assess consumers’ entire concern with fit. Second,

people might have different efficiency in perceiving visual information. Although

some participants fixated at the visual stimuli longer and more often, they are not able

to gain larger amount of information. Researchers found that experts tended to be

more efficient in extracting useful information for making a decision than amateurs

because they used less fixation and shorter duration on a visual image (Humphrey &

Underwood, 2009). Some participants might know more about how to judge fit of

garment, because they majored in apparel design, hence they could fixate less often on

the visual image and still extract enough information and be less concerned about fit.

In addition, other factors might also interrupt the possible relationship between

consumers’ visual attention and concern with fit, such as mood, apparel involvement,

Page 100: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

91

and patience. Lastly, the sample size of this study may be too small to include enough

variation for supporting this hypothesis.

Hypothesis 5: Consumers’ concern with garment fit will be negatively related to

their confidence in fit judgments.

Hypothesis 5 predicted that consumers who were more concerned with fit

of a garment would be less confident with their fit judgment of the garment. However,

the hypothesis was not supported. In the current study, the construct of confidence in

fit judgment included two aspects: the knowledge confidence and choice confidence.

The knowledge confidence refers to participants’ confidence about their previous fit

evaluation, and the choice confidence is about how certain the participants are that the

garment would fit their bodies. The concern with fit taps the perceived risk for buying

a fit garment. The confidence of judgment includes the item related to consumers’ past

shopping experiences, while the concern with fit only taps the current potential risk.

The timing discrepancy might make these two variables unrelated to each other. It is

possible that consumers who were confident about their fit evaluation in the past

shopping experiences but still think it is very risky to purchase a garment online.

Secondly, the visual stimuli used in this study were blazers, which are

relatively expensive. Although consumers thought the risk for buying a blazer online is

financially large, they might still be confident about their fit judgment. Hence, the

confident of fit judgment was not able to predict the concern with fit. Finally, the

sample size of this study may be too small to generate a significant regression result.

Hypothesis 6: Consumers’ concern with fit will be negatively related to their

purchase intent.

Page 101: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

92

Hypothesis 6 predicted that consumers who were more concerned with fit

of a garment would be less likely to buy this garment. As expected, the hypothesis was

supported. This result is consistent with previous studies (Kim & Damhorst, 2010,

Rosa et al, 2006). Concern with fit is one of the major reasons for the high return rate

of clothes bought online, because the garments cannot be inspected by consumers

before the purchase (Beck, 2003). Concern with fit refers to the perceived risk related

to fit of a garment. Because consumers evaluate fit of garment based on the visual and

tactile feedback, the lack of such information increases the perceived risk for online

apparel shopping and hence reduces purchase intention.

This result provides implications for online apparel retailers. The result

indicated that when consumers choose clothes online, they are worried about if the

garment would look good on them and they might have difficulty in visualizing the fit

of the garment. Hence, there is a need for online apparel retailers to develop a better

visual presentation tool for apparel to facilitate consumers’ visualization of how the

garment would look on their body to increase their willingness to shop online.

Hypothesis 7: Consumers’ confidence in fit judgments will be positively related to

their purchase intent.

Fit is a critical attribute to be considered when buying a garment

(Ashdown & O'Connell, 2006). When consumers were not able to make a confident

judgment about fit of the garment, they may think it is too risky to buy (Mollenkopf et

al., 2007). Hence, Hypothesis 7 predicted that consumers who were more confident in

their fit judgment of a garment would show a higher level of intention to buy the

garment. However, the hypothesis was not supported. This result is not consistent with

Page 102: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

93

previous research that intention to buy a specific product would be positively affected

by a consumer's confidence in judging the attributes of a product (Cox & Stuart, 1964;

Howard & Sheth ,1969; Laroche et al., 1996; Peterson & Pitz, 1988; Urbany et al.

1989). College students sampled for this study might have less interest in buying a

blazer compared with working professionals, hence their purchase intention could be

very low even though they could confidently judge whether a blazer is properly fitted.

Consumers’ confidence in judgment ratings were influenced by their

previous experiences. Consumers who had bought properly fit clothes online would

be more likely to trust their evaluation of fit of garments; hence they would have high

level of confidence in fit judgment. However, there are some other factors which affect

consumers’ willingness to buy a garment, such as color, style, and fabric. For example,

consumers who were confident about their fit evaluation would not buy a garment, if

they did not like the style. Finally, the sample size of this study may be too small to

generate a significant regression result.

Hypothesis 8-a: There is a significant difference among the body sites of the human

model on duration of fixations. (Consumers fixate longer at some body sites than

other body sites when they are judging fit of a garment.)

Hypothesis 8-b: There is a significant difference among the body sites of the human

model on number of fixations. (Consumes fixate more often at some body sites than

other body sites when they are judging fit of a garment.)

Hypotheses 8a and 8b predicted that there was a significant difference in

the visual attention (measured by duration of fixations and number of fixations) placed

on the various body sites of the human model, which meant consumers tended to fixate

Page 103: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

94

longer and more often at some body sites than other body sites when they were judging

fit of a garment. As expected, these hypotheses were supported. Specifically, in the

front view of human model wearing the garment, the body site waist received

significantly more visual attention (longer duration and larger number of fixations)

than all the other body sites. In addition, the bust was another body site that received a

large number of fixations and that people looked at for a long time. In the back view of

the human model, back, waist and buttocks were the three body sites on which people

placed a lot of attention. The result in current study is explainable in the current

theoretical framework about selective visual attention.

In the current selective visual attention literature, there are two major

theoretical systems about the strategies people use to control their eye movements and

focus their attention: top-down control and bottom-up control (Duchowski, 2007). The

top-down control is typically found in a visual search or visual recognition situation,

which means people are requested to find or identify a specific object in a visual field.

In this situation, the internal representation or the features of the object usually were

stored in the long-term working memory, people’s attention is directed to a specific

portion of a visual field by the signals arising from working memory (Moores, Laiti, &

Chelazzi, 2003). The bottom-up control was widely found in various visual attention

tasks, including reading and visual search (Keith, 2009). Todd and Kramer (1993)

mentioned that people’s visual attention is likely to fixate at the salient features

including edges, corners, special colors, but not plain surfaces.

In the current research, participants were requested to make judgments

about whether the garment would fit by looking at the visual image of a human model

Page 104: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

95

wearing the garment. Apparel experts and researchers have identified five major

features for evaluating garment fit: grain, set, line, balance, and ease. (Erwin, Kinchen,

& Peters, 1979). Although consumers might not know these five rules for evaluating

fit, they have their own knowledge and understanding about the “fit” concept. For

example, “set” was defined as absence of wrinkles on the garment (Erwin et al., 1979),

which may be intuitively adopted by consumers as their criteria for judgment of

garment fit, hence, wrinkles would be an salient feature which attract consumers

attention when they evaluate garment fit. Applying a top-down control view

(Humphrey & Underwood, 2009) to an apparel fit situation, consumers’ knowledge

and understanding about garment fit stored in their memory would likely direct their

visual attention to fixate at some specific area. In current study, the participants tended

to fixate at the waist and bust for evaluating the fit of a blazer, perhaps because their

previous knowledge and understanding about fit drive them to obtain information from

these body sites. Applying a bottom-up control view (Treisman, 1982) to an apparel fit

situation, the body sites, such as the waist and bust, of a blazer contains many

construction details and draping effects, such as button, pocket, hem, and wrinkles,

which are likely to be salient features that attract people’s attention (Figure. 5.1).

Generally, some body sites, such as waist, bust, and buttocks might be

selected as areas of interest in the garment fit evaluation process, on which people

would exert more visual attention, because salient features for fit judgment (i.e.,

wrinkles) often appear in these body sites. This result has implications for the online

apparel retailers. Online apparel retailers could increase the accuracy of the visual

image of the garment at some specific areas such as waist and bust to highlight the

Page 105: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

96

construction details and draping effects, which would attract consumers’ attention and

help them evaluate the fit of the garment. For example, online apparel retailers could

make bust and waist of a blazer have higher zooming rate while other body sites (e.g.,

neck and arm) have lower zooming rate.

Figure 5.1 Construction details and draping effects might draw attention

Conclusions and Implications

The results of the present study indicated: 1) there is significant

relationship between female college consumers’ body image discrepancy and their

body satisfaction-Hypothesis 1 was supported; 2) female consumers’ body image

Page 106: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

97

discrepancy and body satisfaction significantly predicted their visual attention over the

garments (how long and how often they looked at the garments) when making garment

fit judgments-Hypotheses 2 and 3 were supported; 3) no relationships between female

consumers’ visual attention and their concern with garment fit-Hypothesis 4 was not

supported; 4) there is no significant relationship between female consumers’ concern

with garment fit and their confidence in fit judgment-Hypothesis 5 was not supported;

5) there is no significant relationship between female consumers’ concern with fit and

purchase intention- Hypothesis 7 was not supported; 6) female consumers’ concern

with fit was a significant predictor of their purchase intent for the garment- Hypothesis

6 was supported; 7) Consumers attend to some human body sites more than others-

Hypothesis 8 was supported.

Fit is a critical issue in buying a closely fitting garment, such as blazer,

especially when the shopping behavior occurs online. Consumers need visual feedback

and tactile feedback to make a judgment about garment fit; however, the tactile

information is unavailable and the visual information is very limited in the context of

online apparel shopping. Consumers obtain the visual information for fit judgments

from the visual display of garments presented on the website and from previous

experience with the brand, while in the real-store environment consumers are able to

perceive the visual information from a mirror. The current study used the eye-tracking

technology to record consumers’ visual attention in terms of fixations when they look

at the visual image of a human model wearing garments of different sizes (to simulate

the situation that consumers look at the mirror when they try on garments of different

sizes). This study analyzed the effects of subjective factors (body image discrepancy

Page 107: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

98

and body cathexis) and objective factors (body sites) on consumers’ visual attention

during the process of perceiving information for making a judgment of garment fit;

examined the impact of consumers’ visual attention on their concern with fit; and

analyzed relationships among consumers’ concern with fit, confidence of judgment

and purchase intention. The results of this study have some managerial implications

for online apparel retailers.

The research finds that consumers’ body image discrepancy was highly

related to their body cathexis, which means people whose perceived body deviates

from their ideal body tend to be less satisfied with their body. This result is also

supported by body image research (Thompson et al., 1999). The research also found

that consumer’s body image discrepancy and body satisfaction could influence their

visual attention in perceiving information for evaluating fit. Consumers who have

large body image discrepancies and are less satisfied with their bodies were prone to

fixate longer and more often at the visual image of a human model when they made a

judgment of fit. According to self-discrepancy theory, consumers who have large body

image discrepancy and high level of body dissatisfaction may experience a negative

psychological state. Kuo, Hsu, and Day’s research (2009) indicated that individuals

under negative emotion would exert more cognitive effort (measured by duration of

fixations and number of fixations) on gathering information to make a decision.

Hence, consumers who have large body image discrepancies and a high level of body

dissatisfaction would expend more cognitive effort (in terms of longer duration of

fixations and larger number of fixations) on visual images of garment to make a

judgment of garment fit. This result also supports the theory that people’s affective

Page 108: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

99

state influences their response to the external environment (Eroglu, Machleit, & Davis,

2001).

The other important finding of this research is that consumers’ visual

attention is drawn to certain body sites of a human model when making a judgment of

fit of a blazer. In the front view of the model, the waist and bust are the two highly

fixated on areas (with long duration and large number of fixations), while waist and

buttocks are highly fixated on in the back view of the model. The reasons that people

fixate longer and more often at these specific areas can be explained using from two

perspectives: top-down and bottom up processing. From the top-down view, fit

knowledge, which consumers learn from previous shopping experience and now stored

in their memory, directs their attention to the body sites (i.e. waist) which may contain

important information for evaluating fit. From the bottom-up view, the visual image of

body sites contain salient features (i.e. edge, corners, special color) which tend to stand

out and draw people’s attention. No matter which perspective is used to explain the

results, some body sites (i.e., waist, bust, buttocks) are very important in consumers’

visual perception of garment fit. Online apparel retailers, who want to facilitate

consumers’ fit judgments, should make the visual display of the garment especially

clear and accurate at these body sites.

The current study did not find a significant relationship between

consumers’ visual attention and concern with fit. Perhaps the instrument used in this

study is not sensitive enough to assess the concept of concern with fit. In future

studies, the researcher could include more items to obtain a better measure of concern

with fit. This research does confirm that consumers’ concern with fit has a negative

Page 109: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

100

impact on their purchase intention. The result supports the previous research that fit is

one of the major reasons that keep consumers away from buying clothes online. For

the online apparel retailers, there is a urgent need to develop new online visualization

tools to reduce consumers’ concern with fit in order to facilitate online apparel

shopping.

Better online virtual models and more efficient online fitting tools could

be the methods to fulfill this goal. Some companies have developed new online fitting

technology. Alcatel-Lucent, Hachette Filipacchi Media U.S., Inc., Samsung, [TC]²,

TelePresence Tech, VisionMAX, and Vidyo have collaborated to develop an

interactive Virtual Personal Stylist platform, which offers an accurate and reliable

online fitting experience with high bandwidth networks. Consumers’ personal avatar

with accurate body measurements can be created in an in-store [TC] ² 3D scanning

booth. Consumers’ scanning profile is then stored in an online data storage and sharing

application (a cloud) and can be retrieved in retail stores via an interactive “mirror,”

kiosk, or mobile device to try on clothes. This platform will allow a consumer to mix-

and-match a large variety of garments in a very short time without repeatedly taking

off their clothes ([TC] ² Technology Communicator, 2011). The findings of the current

study suggest that the draping effect of garments of different sizes needs to be

displayed on the virtual avatar to provide more accurate online fitting information. In

addition, the accuracy (i.e., the amount of pixels per inch) of the visual display of

garments at waist, bust, and buttocks should be increased to draw consumers’ attention

and provide more visual information to facilitate consumers’ judgments of fit.

Page 110: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

101

Limitations

Although the research found some useful results, there are some

limitations in this research. First, many body related variables besides body image

discrepancy and body cathexis should be included in this research. For example, body

boundary aberration was a significant predictor for consumers’ concern with fit in

online shopping (Rao et al., 2006), which means body boundary aberration might also

be an important factor in visual perception of garment fit.

Second, although 4 sizes of garments are used in this research, the visual

attention differences caused by the size differences were not examined. In the present

study, because the within-subjects design was used and the sequence of presenting

garment stimuli was not randomized, there could be an order effect. This order effect

could mask the visual attention differences induced by the differences in garment

sizes. In future study, the garment size effect could be examined using a between-

subject design.

Third, the sample number of this study was small; it might be the reason

why some hypotheses could not be supported in this research. In future research, more

participants should be recruited to make the research more generalizable.

Lastly, the eye tracker used in this study was a desk-mounted setting,

which means participants needed to sit in front of a computer screen to view the visual

stimuli. In the future study, if researchers are able to use a head-mounted eye-tracker,

which is commercially available, they might be able to simulate an online fitting room

more accurately. Participants can wear the head-mounted eye-tracker and inspect their

looks in a mirror. This setting would allow researchers to obtain more accurate results.

Page 111: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

102

Appendices

APPENDIX A- GARMENT STIMULI

Figure A.1 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 2

Page 112: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

103

Figure A.2 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 4

Page 113: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

104

Figure A.3 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 6

Page 114: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

105

Figure A.4 Front-view and back view of blazer of size 8

Page 115: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

106

APPENDIX B - BODY CATHEXIS SCALE AND BODY IMAGE

DISCREPANCY SCALES

Body cathexis scale

Use this 1 to 9 scale to indicate how dissatisfied or satisfied you are with each of the

following areas or aspects of your body:

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Very

Dissatisfied

Neither

Satisfied

Nor

Dissatisfied

Very

Satisfie

d

______ 1. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your neck?

______ 2. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your shoulder

______ 3. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your bust ?

______ 4. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your back?

______ 5. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your arm?

______ 6. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your abdomen

______ 7. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your waist?

______ 8. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your hip?

______ 9. How dissatisfied or satisfied you feel about your buttocks?

Page 116: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

107

Body Image Discrepancy Scale

Use this 0 to 3 scale to indicate how you feel about each areas of your body.

1-a. My ideal neck is:

0 1 2 3

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

1-b. How important to your appearance if your neck is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

2-a. My ideal shoulder is:

0 1 2 3

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

2-b. How important to your appearance if your shoulder is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

3-a. My ideal back is:

0 1 2 3

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

3-b. How important to your appearance if your back is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

4-a. My ideal bust is:

0 1 2 3

Page 117: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

108

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

4-b. How important to your appearance if your bust is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

5-a. My ideal arm is:

0 1 2 3

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

5-b. How important to your appearance if your arm is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

6-a. My ideal waist is:

0 1 2 3

Exactly As I Am Almost As I

Am

Fairly

Unlike Me

Very Unlike

Me

6-b. How important to your appearance if your waist is ideal?

0 1 2 3

Not Important Somewhat

Important

Moderately

Important

Very

Important

Page 118: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

109

APPENDIX C- CONCERN WITH FIT, CONFIDENCE IN FIT JUDGMENT,

AND PURCHASE INTENT SCALES

Concern with fit and size scale

Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statement using a 5

point scale (1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree)

Strongly

Disagree

1

2

3

4

Strongly

Agree

5

__________1. The blazer of this size may not fit me.

__________2. The blazer of this size may not look good on me.

__________3. I may have a hard time picturing myself wearing the garment.

Confidence of Judgment scale

not

confident/certain

at all

1

2

3

4

very

confident

5

______1. Please rate how confident you are about your fit evaluation of garments.

______2. To which extent you are certain about this size of blazer is fit for you.

Purchase intention scale

Strongly

Disagree

1

2

3

4

Strongly

Agree

5

________1. I would like to buy a blazer of this size from this retailer.

Page 119: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

110

APPENDIX D- PERMISSION FOR USING PHOTOS OF HUMAN MODEL

Page 120: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

111

APPENDIX E UNIVERSITY OF DELAWARE IRB APPROVAL

Page 121: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

112

REFERENCES

Alexander,M., Connell, L. J., & Presley, A. B.(2005) Clothing fit preferences of young

female adult consumers. International Journal of Clothing Science and

Technology, 17(1), 52-64.

Altabe, M. N., & Thompson, J. K. (1992). Size estimation vs. figural ratings of body

image disturbance: Relation to body dissatisfaction and eating dysfunction.

International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 397-402.

Ashdown, S. P. & Delong, M. (1995). Perception testing of apparel ease variation.

Applied Ergonomics, 26(1),47-54.

Ashdown, S. P. & Dunne, L. (2006). A Study of Automated Custom Fit: Readiness of

the Technology for the Apparel Industry. Clothing and Textiles Research

Journal, 24(2), 121-136.

Ashdown, S.P. & DeLong, M. (1995). Perception testing of apparel ease variation.

Applied Ergonomics, 26(1), 47-54.

Ashdown, S.P. &DeLong, M. (2007). Size in clothing ready-to-wear. NY.

Ashdown,S.P., & O'Connell, E. K.(2006).Comparison of test protocols for judging the

fit of mature women's apparel. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 24(2),

137-146.

Beck, B. (2003). Key strategic issues in online apparel retailing. Retrieved March 6,

2010, from http://www.techexchange.com/thelibrary/online_fit.html

Bennett, P. D. & Harrell, G. D. (1975). The Role of confidence in understanding and

predicting buyers' attitudes and purchase intentions. Journal of Consumer

Research, 2(2), 110-117.

Bhatnagar, A., Misra, S., Rao, H.R., (2000). On risk, convenience, and Internet

shopping behavior. Communications of the ACM, 43 (1), 98–114.

Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W. and Engel, J.F. (2001), Consumer behavior. The

Dryden Press, Orlando, FL.

Page 122: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

113

Brown, P. & Rice, J. (2001). Ready-To-Wear apparel analysis (Third Edition). Upper

Saddle River, New Jersy: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Brown, T. A., Cash, T. F. & Mikulka, P. J. (1990). Attitudinal body-image assessment:

Factor analysis of the Body Self-Relations Questionnaire. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 55, 135-144.

Cash, T. F. (1990). The psychology of physical appearance: Aesthetics, attributes, and

images. In T.F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development,

deviance and change (pp. 51-79). New York: Guilford Press.

Cash, T. F. (1995). Developmental teasing about physical appearance: Retrospective

descriptions and relationships with body image. Personality and Social

Behavior: An International Journal, 23, 123-130.

Cash, T. F., & Deagle, E.A. (1997). The nature and extent of body-image disturbances

in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A meta-analysis. International

Journal of Eating Disorders, 22, 107-125.

Cash, T. F., & Szymanski, M. L. (1995). The development and validation of the Body-

Image Ideals Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 466-477.

Cash, T.F., & Strachan, M.D. (1999). Body images, eating disorders, and beyond. In

R. Lemberg (Ed.), Eating disorders: A reference sourcebook (pp. 27-36).

Phoenix, AZ: Oryx Press.

Cash, T.F., Novy, P.L., & Grant J.R. (1994). Why do women exercise? Factor analysis

and further validation of the Reasons for Exercise Inventory. Perceptual &

Motor Skills, 78, 539-544.

Chau, P.Y.K., Tam, G.A.K.Y., 2000. Impact of information presentation modes

on online shopping: an empirical evaluation of a broadband interactive

shopping service. Journal of Organizational Computing and Electronic

Commerce, 10 (1), 1–22.

Chen, W. & Swalm, R. (1998). Chinese and American college students’ body-image:

Perceived body shape and body affect. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 87, 395-403.

Page 123: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

114

Chen-Yu, H. J., and Kincade, D. H. (2001). Effects of product image at three stages of

the consumer decision process for apparel products: Alternative evaluation,

purchase and post purchase. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management,

5(1), 29-43.

Citrin, A., Stern, D., Spangenberg, E., Clark, M. (2003). Consumer need for tactile

input. An internet retailing challenge. Journal of Business Research, 56(11),

915-922.

Connell, L. J., Ulrich, P. V., Brannon, E. L., Alexander, M. & Presley, A. B. (2006).

Body shape assessment scale: Instrument development for analyzing female

figures. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 24, 80-95.

Cox, D. F. & Stuart, U. R. (1964). Perceived risk and consumer decision making-

the case of telephone shopping. Journal of Marketing Research, 1, 32-39.

Delong, M., Kim, S. H., & Larntz, K. (1993). Perceptions of garment proportions by

female observers. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 76, 811-819.

Duchowski, A. T. (2007). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. London:

Springer.

Elliot, S., Fowell, S., 2000. Expectations versus reality: a snapshot of consumer

experiences with Internet retailing. International Journal of Information

Management, 20, 323–336.

Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K. A., & Davis, L. M. (2001) Atmospheric qualities of online

retailing: A conceptual model and implications. Journal of Business Research,

54, 177-184.

Erwin, M., Kinchen, L., & Peters, K. (1979). Clothing for moderns (6th ed.).

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Fan, J., Yu, W. & Hunter, L. (2004). Clothing appearance and fit: Science and

technology. Boca Raton: CRC Press LLC.

Feather,B., Herr,D., & Ford, S.(1997). Black and white female athletes' perceptions of

their bodies and garment fit. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 15(2):

125 -128.

Page 124: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

115

Ferwerda, J.A (2003) Three varieties of realism in computer graphics. Proceedings

SPIE Human. Vision and Electronic Imaging, 3, 290-297.

Ferwerda, J. A., Westin, S. H., Smith, R. C., Pawlicki, R. (2004). Effects of rendering

on shape perception in automobile design. ACM Symposium on Applied

Perception in Graphics and Visualization, 1, 107-114.

Fitzgibbon, M. L., Blackman, L. R., & Avellone, M. E. (2000). The relationship

between body image discrepancy and body mass index across ethnic groups.

Obesity Research, 8(8), 582-589.

Forsythe, S., Liu, C., Shannon, D., & Gardner, L. C. (2006). Development of a scale to

measure the perceived benefits and risks of online shopping. Journal of

Interactive Marketing, 20, 55-75.

Forrester Research. (March, 2010). Forrester forecast: Double digit growth for online

retail in US and Western Europe. Retrieved April 13, 2010 from

http://www.businesswire.com/portal/site/home/permalink/?ndmViewId=.

Garner, D. & Garfinkel, P.(1981). Body image in anorexia nervosa: Measurement

theory and clinical implications. International Journal of Psychiatry and

Medicine, 2 (11), 263-284.

Garner, D.M., Olmstead, M.A., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a

multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia.

International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15-34.

Goldsberry, E., Shim, S. & Reich, N. (1996). Women 55 years and older: Overall

satisfaction and dissatisfaction with the fit of ready-to-wear. Clothing and

Textiles Research Journal, 14(2), 121-132.

Ha, Y., Kwon, W., & Lennon, S. J. (2007). Online visual merchandising (VMD) of

apparel web sites. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 11(4), 477-

493.

Hagen, M. (1986). Varieties of Realism. Cambridge University Press.

Hammond, J. & Kohler, K.(2001) E-commerce in the textile and apparel industries. In

tracking a transformation: E-commerce and the terms of competition in

Industries. The BRIE-IGCC Economy Project. Brookings Institute.

Page 125: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

116

Häubl, G. & Trifts, V. (2000). Consumer decision making in online shopping

environments: The effects of interactive decision aids. Marketing Science,

19(1), 4-21.

Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of

thinness and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of

Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 325-338.

Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect.

Psychological Review, 94(3), 319-340.

Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1987). Self-concept discrepancy theory: A

psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression

and anxiety. Social Cognition, 3, 51-76.

Hollingworth, A., Richard, A. M., & Luck, S. J. (2008).Understanding the function

of visual short-term memory: Transsaccadic memory, object correspondence,

and gaze correction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 163-

181.

Horrigan, J. B. (2008, February). Online shopping: Internet users like the convenience

but worry about the security of their financial information. Retrieved April 25,

2010, from http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2008/Online-Shopping.aspx

Howard, J. A. & Sheth, J. N. (1969). The theory of buyer behavior. New York: John

Wiley and Sons, Inc.

Humphrey, K., & Underwood, G. (2009). Domain knowledge moderates the influence

of visual saliency in scene recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 100,

377–398.

Irving, L. (1990). Mirror images: Effects of the standard of beauty on the self- and

body-esteem of women exhibiting varying levels of bulimic symptoms.

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 230-242.

Istook, C.(2008). Three-dimensional body scanning to improve fit, In C. Fairhurst

(Eds.), Advances in apparel production, Cambridge, England : Woodhead

Publishing. 94-116

Page 126: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

117

Istook, C., & Hwang, S. (2001). 3D body scanning systems with application to the

apparel industry. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 5(2), 120-

132.

Jacob, R. J. K., & Karn, K. S. (2003). Eye tracking in Human-Computer Interaction

and usability research: Ready to deliver the promises, In J. Hyona, R. Radach,

& H. Deubel (Eds.), The mind’s eye: cognitive and applied aspects of eye

movement research (573-605). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Jacobi, L., & Cash, T.F. (1994). In pursuit of the perfect appearance: Discrepancies

among self- and ideal-percepts of multiple physical attributes. Journal of

Applied Social Psychology, 24, 379-396.

Jarvenpaa, S.L., Todd, P.A., 1997. Consumer reactions to electronic shopping on the

Word Wide Web. International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 1 (2), 59–

88.

Ju, H. W., & Johnson, K. K. P. (2010). Fashion advertisements and young women:

Determining visual attention using eye tracking. Clothing and Textiles

Research Journal, 28(3), 159-173.

Jung, J., & Lennon, S. J. (2003). Body image, appearance self-schema, and media

images. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 32(3), 27-51.

Jung, J., Lennon, S. J., & Rudd, N. A.. (2001). Self-schema or self-discrepancy?

Which best predicts body image? Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 19,

171-184.

Kanwisher, N. & Driver, J. (1992).Objects, Attributes, and Visual Attention: Which,

what, and where. Psychological Science, 1(1), 26-31.

Kerr, A. W., Hall, H. K., Kozub, S. A. (2002). Doing statistics with SPSS.

Trowbridge, Wiltshire: The Cromwell Press.

Khakimdjanova, L., and Park, J.H.(2005). Online visual merchandising practice of

apparel e-merchants. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 12, 307-

318.

Page 127: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

118

Kim, Dong-Eun (2009). Apparel fit based on viewing of 3D virtual models and living

models. International Textile and Apparel Association 2009 Preliminary

Conferences. Retrieved from

http://dha.design.umn.edu/programs/grad/documents/Poster_DongEunKim-

1_000.pdf

Kim, H., & Lennon, S. J. (2010). E-atmosphere, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral

responses. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 14(3), 412-428.

Kim, H.J.and Damhorst, M. L. (2010). The relathionship of body-related self-

discrepancy to body dissatisfaction, apparel involvement, concerns with fit and

size of garments, and purchase intention in online apparel shopping. Clothing

and Textiles Research Journal, 28(4), 239-254.

Kim, J., Fiore, A. M., & Lee, H.-H. (2007). Influences of online store perception,

shopping enjoyment, and shopping involvement on consumer patronage

behavior toward an online retailer. Journal of Retailing and Consumer

Services, 14(2), 95-107.

Kim, J-H., Kim, M., & Lennon, S. J. (2007). Information components of apparel retail

websites: Task relevance approach. Journal of Fashion Marketing and

Management, 11(4), 494-510.

Kim, M., & Lennon, S. J. (2008). The effects of visual and verbal information on

attitudes and purchase intentions in Internet shopping. Psychology &

Marketing, 25, 146-178.

Kinsbourne, M., & Lempert, H. (1980). Human figure representation by blind

children. Journal of General Psychology, 102, 201–209.

Knoblich, G., Thornton,I., Grosjean, M. & Shiffar,M. (2006). Human body perception

from the inside out, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

Kozar, J. M. (2010). Women’s responses to fashion media images: a study of female

consumers aged 30–59. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 34(3),272-

278.

Kozar, J. M., and Damhorst, M. L.(2009). Comparison of the ideal and real body as

women age: Relationships to age identity, body satisfaction and

importance,and attention to models in advertising. Clothing & Textiles

Research Journal, 27(3), 197-210.

Page 128: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

119

Kozar,J. M. & Damhorst, M. L.(2008). Older women's responses to current fashion

models. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 12(3),338-350.

Kuo, F. Y., Hsu, C. W. & Day, R. F. (2009). An exploratory study of cognitive effort

involved in decision under Framing: An application of the eye-tracking

technology. Decision Support System, 48, 81-91.

LaBat, K. L. & DeLong, M. R. (1990). Body cathexis and satisfaction with fit of

apparel. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 8(2), 43-48.

Laroche,M., Kim, C., Zhou,L. (1996). Brand familiarity and confidence as

determinants of purchase intention: An empirical test in a multiple brand

context. Journal of Business Research, 37, 115-120.

Lee, E. J., and Park, J.K.(2009). Online service personalization for apparel shopping.

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 16, 83-91.

Lee, G.-G., & Lin, H.-F. (2005). Customer perceptions of e-service quality in online

shopping. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 33(2),

161-176.

Lim, H. S. (2009). Three dimensional virtual try-on technologies in the achievement

and testing of fit for mass customization. Unpublished Doctor dissertation ,

North Carolina State University, Raleigh.

Mahoney, E. R. & Finch, M. D. (1976). The dimensionality of body-cathexis. Journal

of Psychology, 92, 277-279.

Markee, N., Carey, I., & Pedersen, E.(1990). Body cathexis and clothed body cathexis:

Is there a diff erence? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1239-1244.

McBurney, D. & Collins, V., 1977. Introduction to sensation/perception, Prentice-Hall

Inc., Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

McQuitty, M. A., & Peterson, R. T. (2000). Selling home entertainment on the

Internet: An overview of a dynamic market place. Journal of Consumer

Marketing, 17(3), 233-248.

Mollenkopf, D., Rabinovich, E., Laseter, T. & Boyer, K. (2007) Managing internet

product returns: A focus on effective service management. Decision Sciences,

38(2), 2007

Page 129: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

120

Monteath, S., & McCabe, M. (1997). The influence of societal factors on female body

image. Journal of Social Psychology, 137(6), 708-727.

Moores, E., Laiti, L., & Chelazzi, L.(2003). Associative knowledge controls

deployment of visual selective attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 182-189.

Nguyen, H. T., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Rubin, P. A. D. (2009). Age and fatigue related

markers of human faces: An eye tracking study. Opthamology, 115, 355-360.

Park, J. H., & Stoel, L. (2002). Apparel shopping on the Internet: Information

availability on US apparel merchant web sites. Journal of Fashion Marketing

and Management, 6(2), 158-176.

Park J. H., Lennon, S. J., & Stoel, L. (2005). Online product presentation: Effects on

mood, perceived risk, and purchase intention. Psychology & Marketing, 22(9),

695-719.

Park, J. H., Stoel, L., & Lennon, S. J. (2008). Cognitive, affective, and conative

responses to visual simulation: The effects of rotation in online product

presentation. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7, 72-87.

Park, J., & Stoel, L. (2005). Effects of brand familiarity, experience and information

on online apparel purchase. International Journal of Retail & Distribution

Management, 33(2), 148-160.

Peterson, D. K., and Pitz, G. F. (1988). Confidence, uncertainty, and the use of

information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and

Cognition, 14, 85-92.

Petrie, T. A., Tripp, M. M., & Harvey, P. (2002). Factorial and construct validity of the

body parts satisfaction scale-revised: An examination of minority and

nonminority women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 213–221.

Pieters, R. and Warlop, L. (1999).Visual attention during brand choice: The impact of

time pressure and task motivation. International Journal of Research in

Marketing, 16, 1–16.

Pieters, R., & Wedel, M. (2004). Attention capture and transfer in advertising: Brand,

pictorial, and text-size effects. Journal of Marketing, 68(2), 36-50.

Page 130: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

121

Pisut, G. & Connell, L. J. (2007). Fit preferences of female consumers in the USA.

Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 11(3), 366-379.

Rayner Keith (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and

visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457-

1506.

Rosa, J. A., Garbarino, E. C., & Malter, A. J. (2006). Keeping the body in mind: The

influence of body esteem and body boundary aberration on consumer beliefs

and purchase intention. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 79-91.

Rosen, G., & Ross, A. (1973). The relationship of body image to self concept.

Unpublished paper, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh.

Rudd, N. A., & Lennon, S. J. (1994). Linkages between attitudes toward gender roles,

body satisfaction, self-esteem and appearance management behaviors in

women. Family and Consumer Science Research Journal, 23, 94-117.

Schwarz,N.,Bless,H. & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion: affective states

influence the processing of persuasive communications. Advances in

Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 161–199.

Secord, P. F. and Jourard, S. M. (1953). The appraisal of body-cathexis: Body cathexis

and the self. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 17(5), 343-347.

Shim, S., Eastlick, M. A., Lotz, S. L., & Warrington, P. (2001). An online pre-

purchase intentions model: The role of intention to search. Journal of

Retailing, 77(3), 397-416.

Shim, S., Kotsiopulos, A. & Knoll, D. S. (1991). Body cathexis, clothing attitude, and

their relations to clothing and shopping behavior among male consumers.

Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 9(3), 35-44.

Sicilia, M. & Ruiz, S. (2010). The effect of web-based information availability on

consumers' processing and attitudes. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 24, 31–

41.

Slyke, C.V., Comunale, C.L., Belanger, F., 2002. Gender differences inperceptions of

Web-based shopping. Communications of the ACM, 45 (7), 82–86.

Page 131: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

122

Sniezek, J. A. (1992). Groups under uncertainty: An examination of confidence in

group decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 52, 124-155.

Sniezek, J. A., and Henry, R. A. (1989). Accuracy and confidence in group judgment.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 1-28.

Spitz, H., Stark, L., & Noton, D (1971). Scanpaths and pattern recognition. Science,

New Series, 173(3998), p. 753

Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1987). Automatic activation of self-discrepancies

and emotional syndromes: When cognitive structures influence affect. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1004-1014.

Sundstedt, V., Debattista, K., Longhurst, P., Chalmers, A., & Troscianko, T. (2005).

Visual attention for efficient high-fidelity graphics. Spring Conference on

Computer Graphics. Retrieved from http://

www.cs.bris.ac.uk/home/veronica/Documents/SCCG05_sundstedt.pdf

Szymanski, D.M. & Hise, R.T. (2000). E-satisfaction: an initial examination. Journal

of Retailing, 76(3), 309-322.

[TC]² (2010, March). [TC]² [On-line]. Available: Retrieved March 16, 2010, from

http://www.tc2.com/products

[TC]² Technology Communicator.(2011). Virtual personal stylist concept using

[TC]²’s 3D scanning showcased at NG CONNECT AT CES 2011. Retrieved

from http://www.tc2.com/newsletter/2011/012611.html

Then, N.K. & DeLong, M.R., 1999. Apparel shopping on the web. Journal of

Family and Consumer Sciences, 91 (3), 65–68.

Thompson, J. K. (1990). Body image disturbance: Assessment and treatment.

Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.

Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting

Beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance.

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Page 132: ANALYSIS OF CONSUMERS’ VISUAL PERCEPTION OF GARMENT …

123

Todd, S., & Kramer, A. F. (1993). Attentional guidance in visual attention. Paper

presented at 37th Annual Meeting of Proceedings of the Human Factors and

Ergonomics Society.

Torralba, A., Oliva, A., Castelhano, M. S., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Contextual

guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of

global features in object search. Psychological Review, 113, 766-786.

Treisman, A. (1982). Perceptual Grouping and Attention in Visual Search for Features

and for Objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 8(2), 194-214.

Urbany, L. E., Dickson, P. R., Wilkie, W. L.(1989). Buyer uncertainty and information

search. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 208-215.

US Census Bureau News (2010). Quarterly retail e-commerce sales 4th quarter 2009.

Retrieved from: http://www.census.gov/retail/mrts/www/data/html/09Q4.html

Volino, P., Cordier,C. & Magnenat-Thalmann, N. (2005). From early virtual garment

simulation to interactive fashion design. Computer-Aided Design, 37, 593-608.

Wendel, G. & Lester, D. (1988). Body-cathexis and self-esteem. Perceptual & Motor

Skills, 67, 538.

Williamson, D. A., Gleaves, D. H., Watkins, P. C., and Schlundt, D. G. (1993).

Validation of self-ideal body size discrepancy as a measure of body

dissatisfaction. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 15(1),

57-68.

Workman, J. E. & Lentz, E. S. (2000). Measurement specifications for manufacturers’

prototype bodies, Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 18(4), 251-259.

Yoo, W. S., Lee, Y. J., & Park, J. K. (2010). The role of interactivity in e-tailing:

Creating value and increasing satisfaction. Journal of Retailing and Consumer

Services, 17(2), 89-96.