Top Banner
37? N8/4 Ao. 38<2£ BODY IMAGE AS MEDIATED BY AGE, SEX, AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS DISSERTATION Presented to the Graduate Council of the University of North Texas in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY By Caren C. Cooper, B.A., M.A. Denton, Texas December, 1993
134

37? N8/4 Ao. 38

Jan 01, 2022

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

37? N8/4

Ao. 38<2£

BODY IMAGE AS MEDIATED BY AGE, SEX,

AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS

DISSERTATION

Presented to the Graduate Council of the

University of North Texas in Partial

Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

By

Caren C. Cooper, B.A., M.A.

Denton, Texas

December, 1993

Page 2: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

37? N8/4

Ao. 38<2£

BODY IMAGE AS MEDIATED BY AGE, SEX,

AND RELATIONSHIP STATUS

DISSERTATION

Presented to the Graduate Council of the

University of North Texas in Partial

Fulfillment of the Requirements

For the Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

By

Caren C. Cooper, B.A., M.A.

Denton, Texas

December, 1993

Page 3: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

Cooper, Caren C., Body Image as Mediated bv Age. Sex,

and Relationship Status. Doctor of Philosophy (Counseling

Psychology), December, 1993, 127 pp., 4 tables, 4

illustrations, references, 92 titles.

Traditionally, body image research has focused on young

women. However, there are indications of cultural shifts

which extend physical appearance pressures to both men and

women, as well as to middle-aged and older adults. Two

hundred and ten subjects were administered objective body

image measures including the Figure Rating Scale, the Body

Shape Questionnaire, and the Multidimensional Body-Self

Relations Questionnaire, as well as projective measures

including the Holtzman Inkblot Technique and the Draw-A-

Person. The NEO-Five Factor Inventory and the Social

Anxiety Subscale were also used to explore variables which

might covary with body image.

A 3 X 2 X 2 Multivariate Analysis of Covariance

(MANCOVA) was utilized with social desirability as the

covariate. Results revealed two strong multivariate main

effects for age and sex, as well as an interaction between

age and relationship status.

With regard to age, middle-aged and older adults

experienced greater body dissatisfaction, while younger

adults experienced greater body shape concern. Older adults

Page 4: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

placed more importance on appearance and paid more attention

to their health and signs of illness, while younger subjects

placed more importance on appearance and fitness and were

more satisfied with their fitness level.

With respect to sex, women experienced greater body

dissatisfaction, more body shape concern, placed more

importance on physical appearance, and were more weight

focused than men. On the other hand, men placed more

importance on fitness and were more satisfied with their

appearance, fitness level, specific body areas, and health

status than women.

With regard to the age by relationship status

interaction, it was found that younger subjects tended to be

more insecure about their appearance if they were single,

while middle-aged and older subjects tended to be more

insecure about their appearance if they were involved in a

committed relationship.

The findings of the present investigation were

discussed in relation to possible contributing factors.

Directions for research were also suggested.

Page 5: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

LIST OF TABLES vi

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS v

Chapter

I. INTRODUCTION 1

The Psychology of Physical Appearance Cultural Standards for Physical Appearance The Role of Body Image in Psychosocial

Development Body Image Disturbance: Eating Disorders

and Bodybuilding Gender and Body Image Age and Body Image Relationship Status and Body Image Statement of the Problem Hypotheses

II. METHOD 28

Subj ects Instruments Procedure Statistical Analyses

III. RESULTS 44

Supplementary Multivariate Analysis of Covariance for the Draw-A-Person

Exploratory Correlational Analyses

IV. DISCUSSION 86

Summary Limitations of the Study Implications

APPENDICES • 106

REFERENCES 116

111

Page 6: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Descriptive Information for the Present Subject Sample 30

2. Observed and Adjusted Means for the Multivariate Analysis of Covariance 45

3. Observed and Adjusted Means for the Multivariate Analysis of Covariance for the Draw-A-Person 80

4. Correlations Among Primary, Objective Dependent Variables 85

IV

Page 7: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS

Figure Page

1. Age group and mean penetration scores for single and involved subjects 112

2. Age group and mean human scores for single and involved subjects 113

3. Age group and mean extraversion scores for single and involved subjects 114

4. Age group and mean openness scores for single and involved subjects 115

Page 8: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Every epoch has had different tolerances for weight and

body shape. It has only been within the twentieth century

that these tolerances have dramatically narrowed. Only a

century ago, people preferred being overweight as it

symbolized wealth and status (Schwartz, 1986; Seid, 1989).

In the early 1900s, the weight scale was invented and,

within a few years, people could be readily quantified at

the general store or in the privacy of their home. During

this period, the cultural tolerance for fat began to

tighten. In 1936, an estimated 100,000 people were taking

Dinitrophenol (a derivative of Benzine) for weight

reduction. In 1948, Dexedrine (an amphetamine) was the

recommended drug of choice for weight-reduction.

By the 1950s, the American culture's lack of tolerance

for fat was well-established as evidenced by the founding

and dramatic growth of weight control groups such as Weight

Watchers and Overeaters Anonymous, and the dramatic success

of Jack LaLanne's exercise television show (Schwartz, 1986).

The 1960s and 1970s witnessed the exponential growth of the

women's movement and a primacy began to be placed on a lean

Page 9: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

and androgynous appearance (Schwartz, 1986; Wooley & Wooley,

1986). Fat came to symbolize the dependent caretaking woman

of past years (Wooley & Wooley, 1986).

The 1980s introduced an obsession with exercise (Seid,

1989) . Health clubs increased exponentially in numbers of

facilities and members (Seid, 1989). The 1980s also

introduced the formal diagnosis of anorexia nervosa and

bulimia nervosa (American Psychiatric Association [APA],

1980; 1987). These eating disorders afflict primarily women

and have been described as reaching epidemic proportions

(Barnett, 1986; Dickstein, 1989). Many researchers cite

this rapid growth as due to intense cultural pressures to be

lean and thin (Rodin, 1992). As Seid (1989) and Yates

(1991) explain, a person's body shape has come to be equated

with one's level of health, status, success, and self-

esteem.

The Psychology of Physical Appearance

According to Cash and Pruzinsky (1990), the past two

decades have witnessed the emergence of a psychology of

physical appearance. This psychology involves the study of

how a person's physical attributes and somatic self-

perceptions affect his or her life (Cash, 1990). The

psychology of physical appearance can be divided into two

areas of focus: (a) the concern with the external,

objective attributes of physical appearance and their

personal and social implications for a person's

Page 10: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

psychological development and experience; and (b) the

concern with internal, subjective representations of

physical appearance and bodily experience. This latter area

is often referred to as body image (Cash & Pruzinsky, 1990).

Cash (1990) refers to these two areas of focus as the

social-image and self-image perspectives. Traditionally,

the social-image perspective has investigated the effects of

physical attractiveness on social perceptions, interpersonal

interactions, and human development. The self-image

perspective has involved the study of the physical self-

concept and body-image variables. Both the social and self-

images are heavily influenced by sociocultural norms, or

cultural standards, for physical appearance.

Cultural Standards for Physical Appearance

The image of one's body is an immediate and central way

to think of one's self. In American culture, the body has

come to represent a reflection of the self and as a result,

great emphasis is placed upon physical appearance (Fallon,

1990).

Body image is constantly evolving and continuously

modified by cultural trends in physical appearance (Cash,

1990). Within the American culture, these cultural trends

are primarily determined by mass media and have tremendous

influence on body image at both sociocultural and individual

levels (Myers & Biocca, 1992; Raphael & Lacey, 1992;

Sharkey, 1993; Wiseman, Gray, Mosimann, & Ahrens, 1992).

Page 11: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

Images portrayed in mass media are consensually adopted as

the cultural standard for physical appearance (Sharkey,

1993) .

These culturally bound and consensually validated

definitions of what is desirable and attractive play an

important part in the development of body image. Social

comparison theory states that, in the absence of an

objective standard of correctness, people will seek other

people as a means of evaluating themselves (Deaux &

Wrightsman, 1988). One's body image then includes his or

her perception of the cultural standard, the extent to which

he or she matches the standard, and the relative importance

that members of the cultural group and the individual place

on that match.

Often, physical appearance is the most readily

available information about a person and conveys basic

information about that person such as gender, race,

approximate age, and possibly socioeconomic status and

occupation. People tend to maintain assumptions and

attitudes about physical aesthetics and attributes and sort

people into a variety of cognitive schemas, consciously and

unconsciously (Cash, 1990). A schema is a cognitive

structure which contains a network of associations that

organize and guide an individual's perception. With respect

to physical appearance, for example, an attractive person

may be associated with intelligence and interpersonal skill.

Page 12: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

Cognitive schemas often lead to appearance-cued

stereotyping. Research in this area focuses on a particular

social goodness stereotype of beauty. Dion, Berscheid, and

Walster (1982) discovered the existence of "what is

beautiful is good" and "what is ugly is bad" stereotypes.

Relative to less attractive counterparts, attractive females

and males are viewed in positive terms such as being

happier, more successful, smarter, more interesting, warmer,

more poised, and more sociable. A recent meta-analytic

review by Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, and Kennedy (1991)

found that regardless of the gender of the perceiver or the

perceived, the strongest components of the "beauty-is-good"

stereotype included social confidence (e.g., outgoing and

likable) and adjustment (e.g., happy and confident). Thus,

physical appearance affects the assumptions about and

reactions from the social environment.

The Role of Body Image in Psychosocial Development

In addition to sociocultural factors, developmental

factors have important effects on a person's body image.

The physical constitution of the developing person and the

person's cognitive and emotional appraisals of his or her

body have been variables of central importance in

developmental theory (Lerner & Jovanovic, 1990).

Within psychoanalytic theory, the stages of

psychosexual development involve bodily zones. The person's

emotional development hinges on external and internal means

Page 13: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

through which appropriate stimulation is provided to these

bodily areas.

Freud assigned importance to body attitudes in his

developmental constructs (Fisher, 1986). He hypothesized

that body feelings are basic to ego formation, and his

theory links personality development to a process involving

the successive shifting of attention and energy investment,

in the growing child, from one body area to another (e.g.,

oral, anal, genital). He constructed a personality typology

that was linked with the degree to which persons continued

inappropriately to invest in certain major body zones.

Thus, the "oral character" was one who as an adult continued

to focus, like the young child, highly on the mouth; the

"anal character" focused on the anal sector of the body.

Freud's descriptions of the developing child were often

rich with body image implications (Fisher, 1986). He

commented on such phenomena as the child's learning to

distinguish events in his or her own body interior from

those outside; the sensations aroused in specific body areas

by socialization experiences (e.g., toilet training,

weaning); anxieties about potential body damage linked to

parental threats; confusion about body structure related to

the discovery of sex differences; and fantasies aroused by

the movement of materials in and out of orifices (Fisher,

1986).

Page 14: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

Freud invented the idea that when growing children

observe or fantasize certain patterned events to be

occurring in their body, such patterns may become paradigms

for constructing images about what the world is like

(Fisher, 1986). For example, the sense of possessing an

overregulated anal sphincter may create an image of life as

a place in which regulation and control are dominant themes.

Throughout Freud's account of the developmental process, the

body as an experienced object is always prominent.

Similar to Freud, Erikson's developmental perspective

also involves a series of psychosocial conflicts which

reflect the intensity of investment in major body zones

(Fisher, 1986; Kaluger & Kaluger, 1984). Like Freud,

Erikson stressed that interaction with body zones establish

life precedents and expectations. However, his

developmental framework proposed fundamental life attitudes

which are associated with attitudes toward specific body

organ systems. For example, feelings of trust and distrust

are linked with early evolving images of the mouth.

Attitudes of autonomy and shame are linked with anal and

muscle images-v while initiative and guilt are associated

with images of the genitals.

In Piaget's developmental stage theory of cognition,

the body also plays a central role in the person's

development. In each of the six phases within the

sensorimotor stage of development, circular reactions

Page 15: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

8

involving the body are elaborated in an increasingly more

cognitively complex manner (Fisher, 1986; Lerner &

Jovanovic, 1990). The body in action, moving in space and

reaching for objects, provides the framework for structuring

the world.

Piaget begins with the assumption that' infants have no

awareness of their own bodies, as such. The infant sees

body action or movement as the raw material of all

intellectual and perceptual adaptation. The body

interacting with objects leads to the organization of

schemas. During infancy, actions are overt and clearly

sensory-motor. Schemata are built up by motor repetition.

Circular reactions, behaviors that produce an event which

lead to repetition of behavior, permit schemata to become

solidly established.

According to Piaget, the child's earliest circular

reactions, primary circular reactions, are to his or her

body. Secondary circular reactions refer to the infant

actively experiencing the effects of his or her behaviors on

external objects (Sroufe, Cooper, & DeHart, 1992). As the

child develops, there is an internalization of these body

actions. This internalization involves the creation of

images that represent sensory-motor sequences. With time,

the images presumably become more schematic and abstract.

These images are rooted heavily in motor activity, that is,

in the movements of one's body (Fisher, 1986). Thus, in

Page 16: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

both the psychoanalytic and Piagetian developmental

theories, the body, as a physical entity and as an object of

the person's emotions and knowledge, plays a central role in

the development of the person's affective and cognitive

processes.

Witkin regarded differentiation as a key dimension of

the body concept (Fisher, 1986). He assumed that the

achievement of a differentiated body concept is a

manifestation of the child's general progress toward

psychological differentiation. Witkin maintained that the

infant experiences self and body as a continuous body-field

matrix. Later, boundaries are formed between body and

environment, and some awareness is developed of the parts of

the body and the interrelation among them. While the

child's early conception of his or her body is relatively

global, it will become more articulated with age such that

there is an impression of the body as having definite

boundaries and of the parts within as being discrete yet

interrelated and joined in a definite structure.

Kohlberg did not directly address body image per se.

However, he developed a cognitive theory of sex role

development that is connected to how children perceive their

body attributes (Fisher, 1986). Kohlberg portrayed sex role

definition as arising from cognitive judgments made by

children about their own bodies and those of others such as

Page 17: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

10

parents. That is, they develop ideas regarding what

attributes are associated with male versus female bodies.

According to Kohlberg, children learn to label their

bodies as male or female, and once they are certain of this

categorization, they begin to think and act in ways that are

congruent with it. They then value positively those objects

and acts consistent with their gender category. The task of

psychosexual differentiation is seen, then, as arising out

of the child's ability to learn that his or her body

possesses physical qualities that are labeled "boy" or

"girl." These qualities include structure, size, and

genital differences. Kohlberg traces children's ideas about

the differences in power and social roles of males and

females to deductions they make on the basis of what they

observe about their own bodies as compared with those of the

opposite sex. The perception of one's own body as being of

a certain sex represents, in Kohlberg's theory, a primary

step in becoming socialized and structuring a life role.

The role of the body in the person's psychological

development has also been stressed in developmental

perspectives that emphasize person-context interactions as

central in processes of change across the lifespan (Lerner &

Jovanovic, 1990). In developmental contextualism, the body

is seen as a key component of the person's organismic

individuality, which evokes differential reactions from

persons in the social context, and thus promoting feedback

Page 18: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

11

to the person. The person's appraisal of his or her own

body is derived from others' reactions (social context) and

the person's cognitive and emotional developments involving

the body (Lerner & Jovanovic, 1990). Furthermore, these

processes contribute to each other; that is, how one thinks

and feels about one's body will influence one's social

relations which will, in turn, influence feedback from the

social context which will influence how one thinks and feels

about one's body. Thus, body image has a dual role of

product and producer in a person's psychosocial development

(Lerner & Jovanovic, 1990). Body image is a product of

social context and psychological development, and is a

producer of the interactions between these two.

Support for this dual role of body image can be seen in

the tests of the "goodness-of-fit" model of person-context

relations (Lerner & Jovanovic, 1990). The goodness-of-fit

model involves the degree to which a person "fits" or meets

the demands of the social context. The feedback between the

person and his/her context is circular. That is, the

person's physical characteristics influence reactions from

others in the social context and the context provides

feedback to the person, which influences the psychological

characteristics and development of that person.

With respect to body image and psychosocial

development, Lerner and Korn (1972) found that the body

images and self-concepts of overweight 5, 10, and 20-year

Page 19: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

12

old males were more negative than those of peers with

average builds. Similarly, male and female late adolescents

who see their bodily characteristics as less interpersonally

attractive or less instrumentally effective have lower self-

esteem than late-adolescent males and females whose body

images are more favorable (Padin, Lerner & Spiro, 1981).

These data may be consistent with the idea noted above that

individual physical appearance leads to feedback from the

social context which, in turn, affects psychological

development.

The level of psychological adjustment that results from

the goodness-of-fit between a person and his or her

environment can be viewed on a continuum which ranges from

normal development to abnormal development. When a person

perceives a discrepancy between his or her physical

appearance and the cultural standard, then body image

disturbance develops. The degree of body image disturbance

may range from mildly to severely debilitating. At the

severe end of the continuum are those people who are

experiencing tremendous pressure to achieve the "ideal"

physical appearance and are willing to take extreme

measures. Two examples of this extreme are persons with

eating disorders and bodybuilders (Yates, 1991).

Body-Image Disturbance: Eating Disorders and Bodybuilders

Studies investigating the relationship between body

image and eating disorders comprise a large portion of the

Page 20: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

13

research on body image. Eating disorders generally refer to

psychological disorders involving gross abnormalities in

eating, however, body-image disturbance is essentially what

distinguishes them from other psychological conditions that

occasionally involve eating abnormalities and weight loss

(Rosen, 1990).

The two predominant eating disorders, which affect

primarily women, are bulimia nervosa and anorexia nervosa.

Bulimia nervosa is often referred to as the "binge-purge"

syndrome and involves eating large quantities of food

followed by purgative behavior such as vomiting, laxatives

or other means (Cullari & Redmon, 1981; Rosen, 1990).

Anorexia nervosa is sometimes referred to as the "self-

starvation" syndrome due to weight reduction being achieved

by a drastic reduction in food intake (Bruch, 1985).

Although there is remarkable agreement that anorexia

and bulimia nervosa involve a pervasive disturbance of body

image, there is considerable variation in the specific body

image disturbance believed to be most characteristic of

these disorders. Several researchers propose that this is

due to the fact that body image is a multidimensional

phenomenon that involves perceptual, attitudinal, and

behavioral features (Cash & Brown, 1987; Garfinkel & Garner,

1982; Rosen, Saltzberg, & Srebnik, 1989; Thompson, 1990;

Williamson, 1990). It is important to note that normal

individuals may also have mild disturbance in their body

Page 21: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

14

perceptions, attitudes, and behaviors. The difference is in

degree, not kind.

With respect to disturbances in perception, individuals

with eating disorders tend to perceive themselves as

unrealistically overweight and as being grossly out of

proportion or protruding at certain body regions, such as

having excessively wide hips (Rosen, 1990). The perceptual

distortion of normal size is often evident by the

unrealistic standards that the person attempts to achieve.

Persons with eating disorders exhibit a mistrust or

disbelief in more conventional standards for comparing their

weight to normal. No matter what feedback the person may

receive about his or her size, the person with an eating

disorder relies on his or her own perceptions and feelings

of being too big (Rosen, 1990).

With regard to disturbances in attitudes, body

dissatisfaction can often be traced to particular body areas

such as the width of the hips or thighs or the protrusion of

the abdomen. Dissatisfaction is expressed in terms of

dislike for the body part, thoughts that the part appears

ugly or disgusting, and beliefs that the part is too fat or

lacking muscle tone (Rosen, 1990). While body

dissatisfaction does not distinguish adult eating disorder

subjects from other weight occupied women (Garner,

Garfinkel, & Olmsted, 1983; Wilson & Smith, 1989), they are

Page 22: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

15

distinguished by the importance they place on weight and

shape for self-worth (Wilson & Smith, 1989).

With respect to disturbances in behavior, negative body

image can also be accompanied by a lifestyle that revolves

around the individual's physical self-consciousness (Rosen,

1990). This can include tendencies to avoid situations that

provoke concern about physical appearance such as avoiding

social situations where the person believes his or her body

will be scrutinized, wearing baggy clothes instead of more

revealing and tight fitting clothes, avoiding physical

intimacy, and eating less. Other habits such as frequent

weighing or inspection in the mirror are also typical.

Overall, body image disturbance contributes to the

development and maintenance of eating disorders (Fairburn &

Garner, 1986, 1988). Among young women in general, body

dissatisfaction was associated with problematic eating

attitudes and habits (Kiemle, Slade, & Dewey, 1987;

Striegel-Moore, McAvay, & Rodin, 1986; Wiedel & Dodd, 1983;

Wolf & Crowther, 1983; Zakin, 1989). Furthermore, body

dissatisfaction was a better predictor of bulimic eating

attitudes and behaviors in teenage girls than self-esteem,

depression, and social anxiety combined, which contribute

little unique variance after body dissatisfaction (Gross &

Rosen, 1988). Among women with bulimia nervosa, the

severity of eating and dieting symptoms is correlated with

Page 23: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

16

body dissatisfaction (Post & Crowther, 1987; Ruderman &

Grace, 1988).

While women tend to feel pressured to achieve a lean

and thin body (Wooley & Wooley, 1986), men may feel

pressured to achieve a lean and muscular body (Allen, 1990;

Glassner, 1989). Most people go on a diet and/or exercise

regimen occasionally. However, only a small portion of the

population becomes anorectic/bulimic or bodybuilders.

Recent clinical observation and research suggest that some

groups of male athletes such as long distance runners

(Bloom, O'Toole, & Chang, 1984; Nudelman, Rosen, and

Leitenberg, 1988; Yates, Leehey, & Shisslak, 1983) and

bodybuilders (Cooper & Lane, 1991) may resemble females with

eating disorders in that both involve body image disturbance

and represent extreme pursuit of the ideal physical

appearance for their gender.

Coinciding with the eating disorders epidemic, the

1980s witnessed dramatic increases in the numbers of long

distance runners. Yates (1991) describes "obligatory"

runners as those persons who pursue running intensely and

their lives and thoughts revolve around the running

activity. They are encouraged and reinforced by their

increase in stamina, leanness, and muscularity. They are as

concerned about weight and body fat as they are about time

and distance (Yates, 1991). Long-distance runners rigidly

restrict their diet even when they are painfully lean and

Page 24: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

17

frequently monitor their caloric expenditure and body fat

ratio.

Similar to long distance runners, the 1980s witnessed

tremendous growth in bodybuilding membership both nationally

and internationally. Bodybuilding is now the sixth largest

international sports federation (Yates, 1991). In contrast

to the goal of physical effectiveness for the runner, the

goal of the male bodybuilder is an ideal physical

appearance, which is more closely aligned with the goal of

females with eating disorders. Although increasing numbers

of men and women are bodybuilding, the majority are male,

especially at the competitive level. Bodybuilding is a

sport where the appearance of the body is essential and

involves the development of muscular hypertrophy, symmetry

and balance in muscular development, and presentation of

muscular development in the form of a posing routine (Fuchs

& Zaichowsky, 1983). It requires strenuous and extremely

disciplined training to achieve the ideal form and the sport

attracts males who wish to be recognized by the appearance

of their bodies (Yates, 1991). Klein (1985, 1987) noted

that male bodybuilders possess an unusual need for mirroring

and are obsessed with developing huge, striated muscles, and

an exceptionally lean appearance. Similar to case reports

of anorectics looking into the mirror and seeing themselves

as extremely overweight and "fat," there are case reports of

Page 25: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

18

competition bodybuilders posing in the mirror and seeing

themselves as extremely underweight and "skinny."

To summarize, persons with eating disorders and

bodybuilders provide evidence of the intensity of

sociocultural pressure to achieve the ideal physical

appearance and represent the more extreme abnormal end of

the psychological adjustment continuum with respect to body

image (Yates, 1991). In these populations, body shape and

appearance is the all-encompassing goal and is pursued at

the expense of normal behavior routines and relationships.

These individuals are dissatisfied with their body image to

the extent that they work obsessively to achieve what they

perceive to be a better goodness-of-fit between person and

social environment. With respect to people who represent

the normal to mildly disturbed points on the adjustment

continuum, what are other factors that may influence body

image?

Gender and Body Image

In addition to sociocultural and developmental

influences on body image, there are other factors which may

have an impact on body image. One such factor is gender.

Most research on body image has focused on women, probably

due, in large part, to the fact that primarily women develop

eating disorders. Recently, however, researchers have begun

to explore body image issues with non-clinical samples of

women.

Page 26: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

19

Historically, women have comprised the larger numbers

of dieters than men (Schwartz, 1986). Fallon and Rozin

(1988) found that 67% of the surveyed college women were

dieting at least some of the time, whereas, only 25% of

college men were dieting. Fallon (1990) reviewed the

literature and found replicated findings that women are more

dissatisfied with their weight and shape than men. Fallon

and Rozin (1986) investigated sex differences in body shape

perceptions and found that women are more likely than men to

describe themselves as overweight, worry about being or

becoming fat, and express greater concern with dieting and

body weight. A 1986 survey also revealed that women tend to

see themselves as overweight even if they are not. They are

less happy with their body shape, and diet more often than

men to achieve the thin ideal (Cash, Winstead, & Janda,

1986). According to Rozin and Fallon (1988), women tend to

equate their body image and self-worth more than men and

are, therefore, more likely to feel distressed about their

body image and put forth more effort to alter their shape.

While the pursuit and preoccupation with body image are

central to the female sex-role stereotype (Rodin,

Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985), some researchers have

suggested that the extent of male body dissatisfaction has

been underestimated (Rodin, 1992). For example, ninety-five

percent of male subjects expressed dissatisfaction with some

aspect of their bodies, and seventy percent of undergraduate

Page 27: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

20

men see a discrepancy between their own body and their ideal

body type, with their greatest dissatisfaction toward chest,

weight, waist, and height (Mishkind, Rodin, Silberstein, &

Striegel-Moore, 1986; Tucker, 1982). Drewnowski and Yee

(1987) found that although more women than men desired to

lose weight, more than half of the normal-weight college

males desired to lose weight. Of the male and female

subjects who wished to lose weight, both groups viewed

themselves as overweight and were dissatisfied with their

body shape. Haynes (1988) found that 85% of normal-weight

college males and females were not satisfied with their

bodies.

Allen (1990) contends that the pressure to achieve an .

ideal body image is being extended to men. According to

Allen (1990), men are adopting the same unrealistic

standards that have been tormenting women for years, and are

defining themselves more by their looks. Women are

pressured to be lean and thin, while men are pressured to be

lean and muscular. The intensity of body image pressures

for both women and men is evidenced by the record number of

dieting and exercise programs currently available, as well

as record number of female and male participants (Yates,

1991).

Age and Body Image

Traditionally, body image research has focused on

adolescent and young adult populations. However, like

Page 28: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

21

gender, body image pressures appear to be no longer reserved

for the young, but for middle-aged and older persons as

well. Middle-aged and older people must come to terms with

the changes in their physical being (Atchley, 1988; Kaluger

& Kaluger, 1984). They must reckon with changes in hair

amount and color, skin changes (e.g., wrinkles and age

spots), decreased energy output, and changing body shape and

weight. Further, this coming to terms with one's bodily

appearance and perceptions must occur within a marketing

context which is placing increasing pressure on the middle-

aged and older person to obtain and maintain a particular

body shape (Allen, 1990). Wantz and Gay (1981) found that

social attitudes about appearance are one of the main

reasons middle-aged and older persons seek cosmetic surgery.

During the mid-life period, body image issues can be

particularly stressful for some persons. Levinson (1977)

believes that mid-life transition period spans four to six

years, reaching its culmination when a person is in the

early forties. About 80% of the Levinson subjects deemed

the mid-life transition as a time of moderate or severe

crisis. Some of the major body image-related experiences

during this period are the realization of bodily decline, a

recognition of one's mortality, and the sense of aging.

Riccio (1989) commented on the massive numbers of "baby

boomers" (aged 35-43) that are flooding the health clubs.

Rozin and Fallon (1988) found that men and women in their

Page 29: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

22

forties and fifties shared similar levels of dissatisfaction

with body shape. Both judge their ideal to be significantly

thinner than their current shape. Riccio (1989) surveyed

this age group regarding aging issues. Results regarding

body dissatisfaction suggested that male and female subjects

were more bothered by their increasing waistlines. Allen

(1990) pointed out that the common spare tire of the middle-

aged male is no longer acceptable in the work arena.

Like middle-aged adults, older adults must contend with

body image issues, but at an even more profound level

(Kaluger & Kaluger, 1984). Ross et al. (1989) found that

healthy elderly persons are more conscious of their external

appearance than younger people. Van-Deusen, Harlowe, and

Baker (1989) found that elderly adults (aged 61+ years)

consistently perceived their body image less positively than

did those aged 60 or younger.

Given the age factor, it is important to note that

there may exist cohort differences in cultural standards

with respect to physical appearance. For example, what was

considered a thin body figure in the 1950s may be different

than the cultural definition of a thin body figure in the

1990s. As people age, they may employ the cultural standard

from past experience and/or they may compare their current

body shape to the body shape they "used to have." That is,

when they employ social comparison processes, they will be

using the cultural standard from their generation.

Page 30: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

23

Relationship Status and Body Image

A potential influential factor on body image, in

addition to gender and age, that has not received attention

in the research literature is a person's relationship status

(i.e., single or involved in a committed relationship). In

the late nineteenth century, only 3.5% of marriages ended in

divorce (Saxton, 1990). Currently, according to the U.S.

National Center for Health Statistics, over 50% of marriages

are ending in divorce (Saxton, 1990). This trend alone

accounts for a significantly larger middle-aged and older

singles population re-entering the dating scene.

Dating for the middle-aged and older person can be a

particularly stressful process, especially for those persons

who separate or divorce after several years in a marriage or

committed relationship. They may feel increased pressure to

work diligently on their body shape, attempting to increase

their physical attractiveness. As found by Cash, Winstead,

and Janda (1986), health clubs are predominantly populated

by single persons who are seeking to increase their

attractiveness to others.

Persons involved in a committed relationship, on the

other hand, may be less motivated to commit to the time and

energy to alter their physical appearance. There may exist

within a committed relationship a level of comfort so that

pressures to pursue and maintain an ideal physical

appearance are minimized. That is, a committed relationship

Page 31: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

24

may serve as a buffer against pressures for an ideal

physical appearance.

Statement of the Problem

Traditionally, research on body image has been

primarily focused on young, female adults. From the

preceding review of the literature, there appear to be

indications of a cultural shift which extends physical

appearance pressures to both men and women, as well as to

middle-aged and older adults. It is suspected that

relationship involvement may protect persons from these

pressures.

If, in fact, this cultural shift is occurring, there

are many potentially negative ramifications. With respect

to age, Allen (1990) contends that the American culture is

placing increasing importance on youth. There is the

perception that a youthful, healthy body correlates with

confidence, success, and power. Older persons tend to

receive more pessimistic evaluations from young individuals

(Rodin & Langer, 1980). Thus, aging persons are in a

predicament in that they live in a social environment which

places a high premium on physical appearance when their

bodies are physically declining. From a contextual

viewpoint, this could result in increased bias against older

adults, which, in turn, could lead to lowered self-esteem,

leading to further bias, etc.

Page 32: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

25

This bias may be especially relevant to women. As a

person gets older, the numbers shift within one's cohort.

That is, there are more women than men (Atchley, 1988). For

women, signs of aging such as wrinkles and gray hair, tend

to be viewed negatively, while, for men, they tend to be

viewed positively (e.g., "distinguished"). Thus, if

cultural pressures for physical appearance extend to older

women, they may be put in a no win situation; that is, due

to physical aging signs and decreased numbers of men, the

possibilities for attracting a relationship partner are

significantly reduced. They are therefore victimized as

both older persons and as women.

With respect to gender, as Allen (1990) stated, men are

adopting the same unrealistic cultural expectations for

appearance that have plagued women for the past three

decades. This extension of unrealistic cultural standards

to men can be seen in the dramatic increases in middle-aged

men seeking cosmetic surgery (Wantz & Gay, 1981), increases

in the number of males with eating disorders such as

anorexia and bulimia nervosa, and, because of increasing

competition, discriminatory hiring/promotion practices due

to an appearance criterion being incorporated into the

workplace (Allen, 1990).

With respect to relationship status, there are

increasing numbers of single people who may feel more

pressure to obtain and maintain a particular body shape than

Page 33: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

26

those people involved in committed relationships. If a

person is single and feels unable to conform to the cultural

standard for physical appearance, self-esteem may decrease,

which, in turn, could lead to fewer dating attempts, leading

to continued decreases in self-esteem, etc.

In addition to the effects of age, sex, and

relationship status, it was anticipated that there would be

an interaction among these factors. Specifically, it was

expected that young, single females would still experience

the most body concern and dissatisfaction as compared to

other groups. However, in light of indicators which suggest

that older and male individuals are experiencing increased

pressure to conform to ideal cultural standards for physical

appearance, it may be that the pressure on these groups is

approaching the intensity experienced by the young, female

population.

Hypotheses

From the present review of the literature, it appears

that body image is no longer an issue reserved for the

young, female, and single individual. It is suggested that

age, sex, and relationship status are mediating factors in

body image. Therefore, it was hypothesized that:

1. For the younger, middle-aged, and older groups,

there would be no differences in general body

dissatisfaction (Figure Rating Scale), amount of body shape

concern (Body Shape Questionnaire),

Page 34: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

27

appearance/fitness/health/illness evaluation and

orientation, body areas satisfaction or weight attitudes

(Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire), body-

concept (Draw-A-Person), or body image-related variables as

measured by the Holtzman Inkblot Technique.

2. For male and female subjects, there would be no

differences on body image variables as measured by the

instruments delineated in the first hypothesis.

3. For single and involved subjects, there would be no

differences on body image variables as measured by the

instruments delineated in the first hypothesis.

4. Young, single, females would be higher on body

image variables as measured by the instruments delineated in

the first hypothesis in spite of the current extension of

the cultural standard for physical appearance to other

groups.

Page 35: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

CHAPTER II

METHOD

Subj ects

Two hundred and ten adult subjects were recruited by

means of an announcement made to university classes, church

organizations, community organizations, and social clubs.

Specifically, subjects were asked to partipate in a body

image study.

To insure that subjects had been consistently exposed

to American mass media from birth through adulthood, all

subjects were born, raised, and had, thus far, lived their

adult years in the United States (US). All but four of the

subjects were white from mid-size to large metropolitan

areas throughout the US.

Subjects were divided into twelve groups based on three

factors: age, sex, and relationship status. Groups

consisted of younger single males (20) and females (20),

younger involved males (20) and females (19), middle-aged

single males (15) and females (16), middle-aged involved

males (15) and females (20), older single males (15) and

females (16), and older involved males (16) and females

(18).

The younger groups consisted of subjects who were 18 to

33 years of age, the middle-aged groups consisted of

28

Page 36: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

29

subjects who were 39 to 55 years of age, and the older

groups consisted of subjects who were 60 and over (Levinson,

1986). Relationship status was dichotomized as single and

involved. Single status included those subjects who were

single, widowed, separated, and divorced. Involved status

included those subjects who were married or cohabitating

with their partner for at least three months.

Table 1 summarizes descriptive information regarding

the subject sample. The average age was approximately 45

years old, the average weight was approximately 165 pounds,

and most subjects rated their health as good or excellent.

Subjects had an average of three years of college and, of

those who were involved in a committed relationship (n =

108), subjects had been together for an average of 23 years.

Only those subjects who were at least 18 years of age,

US citizens, not currently in counseling/therapy, and not

pregnant were allowed to participate. Other exclusion

criteria included childbirth within the last six months, an

episode of major depression within the last year, or a

history of anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, schizophrenia,

or physical trauma/impairment.

Dependent Measures

In the past, body image has been mistakenly understood

as a single, unidimensional construct. Cash and Pruzinsky

(1990) contend there is not an entity known as "The Body

Image." Body image actually refers to body images.

Page 37: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

30

Table 1

Descriptive Information for the Present Subi ect Sample

Variable Mean SD Range

Democrraphic Characteristics:

Age 44.98 19.82 18-83

Weight 166.38 44.24 90-443

Health Status 1.82 .65 1-4

How Long in Relationship 22.72 18.24 1-56

Education 15.19 2.51 3-24

Primary. Obiective Measures:

FRSa:

Actual-Ideal Body

Dissatisfaction 10.78 1.31 7-16

Actual-Cultural Standard

Body Dissatisfaction 11.25 1.72 7-16

BSQb:

Body Shape Concern 75.72 31.02 35-169

MBSR0C:

Appearance Evaluation 3.20 .78 1-4.86

Appearance Orientation 3.62 .61 2.08-4.92

Fitness Evaluation 3.51 .86 1-5

Fitness Orientation 3.13 .58 1.85-4.62

Health Evaluation 3.77 .65 1.17-5

(table continues)

Page 38: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

31

Variable Mean SD Range

Health Orientation 3.49 .60 2.13-5

Illness Orientation 1.98 .49 .75-3.13

Body Areas Satisfaction 3.18 .68 1-4.75

Weight Focus 2.38 .86 1-4.50

Primarv. Proiective Measures:

DAPd:

General Characteristics 17.74 2.12 14-24

Specific Characteristics 20.03 2.88 15-32

HITe:

Barrier 6.92 3.63 0-21

Penetration 7.99 4.26 0-22

Human 22.61 8.84 2-63

Animal 29.08 8.55 9-53

Anatomy 3.54 3.13 0-26

Sex .67 1.23 0-8

Color 13.98 7.99 0-54

Hostility 9.66 4.92 0-28

Secondary. Exploratory Measures:

NEO-FFIf:

Neuroticism 40.98 7.77 18-60

Extraversion 31.83 5.88 17-51

Openness 32.88 5.31 18-48

(table continues)

Page 39: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

32

Variable Mean SD Range

SAS9:

Social Anxiety 10.96 3.18 6-23

Co-Variate:

SDSh:

Social Desirability 3.92 3.38 0-9

aFRS = Figure Rating Scale; bBSQ = Body Shape Questionnaire?

CMBSRQ = Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire;

dDAP = Draw-A-Person; eHIT = Holtzman Inkblot Technique;

fNEO-FFI = NEO Five-Factor Inventory; gSAS = Social Anxiety

Subscale; hSDS = Social Desirability Scale.

Theorists and researchers in the area continually point out

that body perception and experience is multidimensional. At

any given point in time, a person may simultaneously monitor

his or her body's attractiveness, body size/weight,

variations in the size of body parts, etc. Generally,

researchers and clinicians have focused on two aspects of A

appearance-related body image: a perceptual component

(e.g., a subject's perception of his or her body size), and

a subjective component (e.g., a subject's attitudes towards

his or her body size) (Cash & Brown, 1987).

Page 40: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

33

Thus, given that body image is a multidimensional

construct, multiple objective and projective measures were

employed to ascertain various dimensions of body image.

Results from these instruments were of primary interest. In

addition to screening instruments to check for depression

level and social desirability response set,' measures of

personality adjustment were also utilized to explore

variables which might co-vary with dimensions of body image.

Results from these personality instruments were of secondary

importance.

Screening Measures

Beck Depression Inventory. The Beck Depression

Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelsohn, Mock, & Erbaugh,

1961) is a 21-item self-report inventory used to assess the

presence of depressive symptoms. The BDI was used as a

screening instrument. Those subjects demonstrating moderate

to severe depression (total BDI score of 19 or more) were

excluded from the subject pool. In the present study,

approximately 20 subjects were eliminated from the sample

due to moderate to severe levels of depression.

Social Desirability Scale. The Social Desirability

Scale (SDS; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964) was used as an empirical

check on the relationship between social desirability

response set and scores on other instruments. The SDS

consists of 10 questions to which the subject responded true

or false.

Page 41: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

34

Primary. Objective Measures

Demographic Questionnaire. A demographic instrument

was used to obtain individual background information such as

age, sex, relationship status, education, height, weight,

and health status.

Figure Rating Scale. The Figure Rating Scale (FRS;

Fallon & Rozin, 1985) was used to obtain a general

perceptual index of body dissatisfaction. The FRS involves

subjects selecting from nine figures of various sizes,

ranging from very thin to very large figures. Subjects were

asked to choose the figure they thought reflected their

current and ideal body size (actual-ideal). The discrepancy

between these two perceptions was taken as an indication of

level of body dissatisfaction.

In order to assess for cohort differences in the

perception of cultural standards, subjects were also asked

to choose the figure they thought reflected the cultural

ideal. The discrepancy between the subject's perception of

current body size and the cultural standard (actual-cultural

standard) was then used as an additional index of body

dissatisfaction. High scores on either index indicate

greater body dissatisfaction.

Test-retest reliability for a two-week period for six

different ratings ranges from .55 to .71 for males and .60

to .92 for females (Cash & Pruzinsky, 1990).

Page 42: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

35

Body Shape Questionnaire. The Body Shape Questionnaire

(BSQ; Cooper, Taylor, Cooper, & Fairburn, 1987) is a self-

report questionnaire used to assess the amount of concern

about body shape. There are 34 items to which subjects

indicate the frequency with which they react in a particular

way to events, ranging from never (1) to always (6). From

these items, a total BSQ score was obtained. High scores

indicate greater body concern.

Concurrent and discriminant validity have been shown to

be good (Cooper, Taylor, Cooper, & Fairburn, 1987) and

internal consistency is .93 for females (Cash & Pruzinsky,

1990).

Multidimensional Bodv-Self Relations Questionnaire.

The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire

(MBSRQ; Cash, 1990) is a self-report questionnaire used to

assess several subjective aspects of body image and weight-

related variables (Cash & Brown, 1990). The MBSRQ was

developed and normed from a national body-image survey (Cash

et al., 1985, 1986). The 69-item MBSRQ consists of three

components: (a) the Body-Self Relations Questionnaire

subscales (short form; 54 items), (b) the Body Areas

Satisfaction Scale (BASS; 9 items), and (c) the weight

attitude scales (6 items).

Using a 5-point response format ranging from definitely

disagree (1) to definitely agree (5), the BSRQ consists of

three somatic domains: appearance, fitness, and health.

Page 43: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

36

Within each of these somatic domains are items comprising

two composite subscales: (a) evaluation——the extent of

liking, attainment, and satisfaction, and (b) orientation—

the degree of importance of, and attention paid to, the

domain, as well as behaviors related to maintaining or

improving facets of the domain. The six separate scores

plus one additional facet, Illness Orientation from the

BSRQ, was used in the present study. High scores indicate

increased satisfaction with appearance (Appearance

Evaluation), increased importance placed on appearance

(Appearance Orientation), increased satisfaction with

fitness level (Fitness Evaluation), increased attention paid

to fitness (Fitness Orientation), increased satisfaction

with health status (Health Evaluation), increased importance

placed on health status (Health Orientation), and increased

attention paid to signs of illness (Illness Orientation).

The BASS is a 9-item version of the 25-item Body Parts

Satisfaction Scale (Berscheid, Walster, & Bohrnstedt, 1973),

constructed largely on the basis of Bohrnstedt's (1977)

factor analysis of the original instrument and Cash et al.'s

(1985, 1986) survey research. The BASS items are used to

assess satisfaction with face, hair, lower torso, midtorso,

upper torso, muscle tone, weight, and overall appearance.

Each item is rated on a 5-point scale from very dissatisfied

(1) to very satisfied (5). The average of the items results

in a composite body satisfaction index, with Cronbach's

Page 44: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

37

alphas of .79 and .78 for males and females, respectively

(Cash & Brown, 1989). High scores indicate increased

satisfaction with specific body areas.

The final six items of the MBSRQ pertain to weight-

related attitudes. These include statements used to assess

such aspects as fat anxiety, weight vigilance, self-

classified weight, current dieting, and eating restraint.

As with the other sections of the MBSRQ, each item is rated

on a 5-point scale. Scores from each item were used in the

present study. High scores indicate increased weight focus.

For the MBSRQ, internal consistency ranges from .75 to

.91, with mean Cronbach's alpha of .83. Test-retest

reliability after a two-week period ranges from .78 to .94

(Cash & Pruzinsky, 1990).

Primary. Projective Measures

Draw-A-Person. The Draw-A-Person (DAP; Machover, 1952)

is a projective drawing technique which was used to provide

a qualitative measure of a subject's perception of his or

her body-concept (Oster & Gould, 1987). The body-image

projection hypothesis, which assumes that the same sex

figure drawn^is related to the person's internal view of his

or her body, is the most fundamental premise of human figure

drawing (Kahili, 1984).

Dougherty, Cook, and Coppin (1991) developed a scoring

system for the DAP. They grouped 29 criteria into two

sections: general characteristics and specific

Page 45: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

38

characteristics. There are 14 general characteristics which

include shading (anxiety), detail (obsessiveness), and body

distortions. There are 15 specific characteristics which

include trunk, breast, and legs, which are body areas

frequently cited by men and women as unsatisfactory (Rodin,

1992).

Subjects were asked to draw both male and female

figures; however, the DAP scores were derived from the same

sex figure drawn. Interrater reliability was established ^t I

.88. High scores indicate a more negative body concept at a

general and/or specific level.

Holtzman Inkblot Technique. The Holtzman Inkblot

Technique (HIT; Holtzman, 1961) is a projective inkblot test

which was used to assess several relevant aspects of body

image and personality (Form A): Barrier, Penetration,

Human, Animal, Anatomy, Sex, Color and Hostility. The first

two variables, Barrier and Penetration, are theoretically

interrelated with each other and directly related to body

image.

The Barrier concept involves the degree of definiteness

of the body image boundary (Hill, 1972). The body image

boundary can be thought of as a shell that is unconsciously

created to defend against anxiety about interacting with the

social environment. If a person experiences high levels of

body anxiety and dissatisfaction, then it is expected they

will also exhibit high Barrier or body image boundary.

Page 46: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

39

The Penetration concept refers to the vulnerability of

the body image boundary (Hill, 1972). If individuals feel

insecure and/or inadequate with regard to their bodies

(Penetration), they are more susceptible to body anxiety and

dissatisfaction. The more vulnerability experienced, the

more barrier required to defend against anxiety. Thus,

Barrier and Penetration are related in that if Penetration

is high, then Barrier is high and vice-versa. High scores

on these two variables indicate more body image boundary

(Barrier) and increased vulnerability of the body image

boundary (Penetration).

The other six HIT variables may have a possible

influence on body image. Human and Animal variables are

used to assess the salience of interpersonal relationships.

If individuals are more focused on interpersonal

relationships, they may be more susceptible to body concern

and dissatisfaction. Anatomy and Sex variables assess

aspects of subjects' body focus. If one is focused on his

or her body, he or she may be more focused on body shape and

weight. Color and Hostility variables are used to assess

aspects of subjects' emotional activity. If one is higher

in emotionality, he or she may be more likely to experience

body anxiety and dissatisfaction. High scores on these six

variables indicate higher interpersonal relationship

salience (higher Human and lower Animal), increased body

Page 47: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

40

focus (Anatomy and Sex), increased emotionality (Color), and

higher levels of anger (Hostility).

The reliability of the HIT has been established and the

split-half reliability coefficients in the high eighties and

low nineties exist for most variables for immediate and

delayed intrasubject stability (Hill, 1972). Interscorer

consistency has been established with a median value of all

correlations at .86 (Hill, 1972).

Secondary Measures

NEO Five-Factor Inventory. The NEO Five-Factor

Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1985) is a shortened

version of the NEO Personality Inventory which is designed

to measure five personality factors: Neuroticism (N),

Extraversion (E), Openness to Experience (0), Agreeableness

(A), and Conscientiousness (C). Subjects respond to items

on a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to

strongly agree (5).

For the purposes of the present investigation, scales

N, E, and 0 were of primary interest. Scale N refers to

neuroticism, negative affectivity, or nervousness. Neurotic

individuals tend to experience chronic distressing emotions

such as fear, guilt, and frustration and, thus, are possibly

more prone to body concern and dissatisfaction. Scale E is

used to assesses extraversion, warmth, and energy level. If

subjects are more extraverted and outwardly focused, they

may more vulnerable to body concern. Scale O refers to

Page 48: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

41

openness to experience or originality. Open individuals

tend to be imaginative, aesthetically sensitive,

intellectually curious, and attitudinally liberal and, thus,

possibly less bound to cultural norms regarding body image.

High scores on the N, E, and 0 variables indicate increased

levels of negative affectivity, extraversion, and openness

to new experience, respectively.

Social Anxiety Subscale. The Self-Consciousness Scale

(SCS-R; Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975; Scheier & Carver,

1985) is a self-report measure of dispositional self-

attention processes. Of its three subscales, the Social

Anxiety Subscale (SAS) was used to assess level of social

discomfort. The SAS has six items which use a 3-point

Likert scale ranging from a little like me (1) to a lot like

me (3) .

If a person experiences increased social anxiety, he or

she may experience more body anxiety. High scores on the

SAS indicate high levels of discomfort in social settings.

Procedure

Having been recruited via other means, prospective

subjects were contacted by phone and screened with regard to

previously stated exclusion criteria. If eligible for

participation, subjects were briefed regarding the process

of participation and any questions by the participant

addressed. If the subject was willing to participate, an

appointment time was scheduled.

Page 49: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

42

At the time of his or her appointment, each subject was

provided with a consent form which was read and signed by

the person prior to participation. Each was then

administered the two projective techniques (the DAP and HIT)

and then provided with a packet containing the Demographic

Questionnaire, BDI, SDS, FRS, MBSRQ, BSQ, NEO-FFI, and SAS.

After the subject had completed the questionnaire packet, he

or she was given the opportunity to ask any remaining

questions regarding the study.

Statistical Analyses

In the present investigation, there were three

independent variables: age (younger, middle-aged, and

older), sex (male and female), and relationship status

(single and involved). Dependent variables included primary

measures of body image as derived from the FRS, MBSRQ, BSQ,

HIT, and DAP, as well as secondary measures of personality

adjustment (NEO-FFI and SAS).

Statistical analyses included a 3 X 2 X 2 Multivariate

Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) with social desirability as

the covariate. Significant multivariate results were

obtained, thus post-hoc tests were conducted at the

univariate level. For a multivariate main effect for age

and an interaction of age by relationship status, simple

effects tests were conducted.

Due to a substantial number of DAP drawings which were

unscoreable, a separate MANCOVA was conducted for the DAP

Page 50: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

43

with social desirability as the covariate. Significant

multivariate results were obtained, thus post-hoc tests were

carried out at the univariate level. For a multivariate

main effect for age, simple effects tests were conducted.

In addition, exploratory correlational analyses between

demographic and dependent variables, as well as among the

dependent variables were also conducted.

Page 51: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

CHAPTER III

RESULTS

Four main hypotheses were tested utilizing a 3 X 2 X 2

Multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) with social

desirability serving as the covariate. Primary dependent

measures included the Figure Rating Scale (FRS), the Body

Shape Questionnaire (BSQ), the Multidimensional Body-Self

Relations Questionnaire (MBSRQ), and the Holtzman Inkblot

Technique (HIT). Secondary, exploratory dependent measures

included the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) and the

Social Anxiety Subscale (SAS). Table 2 summarizes the

results of this procedure. The minimum cell size was 10 and

cell size ranged from 10 to 21, with an average of 16

subjects per cell.

Hypothesis 1 predicted that, regardless of age group

membership, there would be no differences on body image

variables as measured by the FRS, BSQ, MBSRQ and the HIT.

Results of a 3 (younger, middle-aged, and older) X 2 (male

and female) X 2 (single and involved) MANCOVA failed to

support this hypothesis. In fact, there was a strong,

multivariate main effect for age (F = 5.18, df = 48,308, p <

. 000) .

44

Page 52: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

45

Table 2

Observed and Adjusted Means for Multivariate Analysis of

Covariance

Variable

Observed Adjusted

Mean SD Mean

Primary. Objective Measures:

FRS:

Actual-Ideal

Younger

Single

Male 9.71

Female 10.79

Involved

Male 10.28

Female 11.26

Middle-Aged

Single

Male 10.71

Female 11.00

Involved

Male 10.75

1.19

.79

1.27

1.63

.99

.97

1.29

9.65

10.76

10.22

11.19

10.75

11.10

10.78

(table continues)

Page 53: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

46

Variable

Observed Adjusted

Mean SD Mean

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Actual-Cultural Standard

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

1 1 . 4 5 1 . 0 5 1 1 . 5 2

10.60

11.00

1 0 . 5 3

1 1 . 1 4

9 . 7 1

11.16

1 0 . 3 8

1 1 . 4 2

1.08

1.28

1 . 5 5

. 8 6

1 . 5 9

1.02

1.61

2 . 0 9

1 0 . 5 6

1 1 . 0 7

1 0 . 5 0

11.12

9 . 6 6

1 1 . 1 3

1 0 . 3 4

1 1 . 3 6

1 1 . 1 4 1.10 1 1 . 1 7

(table continues)

Page 54: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

47

Variable

Observed Adjusted

Mean SD Mean

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

BSfi:

Body Shape Concern

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

1 1 . 5 0

1 0 . 9 2

12.10

10.60

1 2 . 3 3

1 0 . 6 7

1 2 . 5 0

6 6 . 1 0

1 0 5 . 1 1

6 6 . 3 9

1 . 4 6

1 . 5 6

. 9 1

1.08

1 . 7 8

1.88

1 . 4 5

1 1 . 5 8

1 0 . 9 4

12.16

1 0 . 5 7

1 2 . 3 9

1 0 . 6 4

1 2 . 4 9

2 0 . 0 3 6 6 . 2 6

3 4 . 8 8 1 0 5 . 1 9

2 5 . 3 8 6 6 . 5 3

(table continues)

Page 55: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

48

Variable

Observed Adjusted

Mean SD Mean

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

9 2 . 5 8 3 4 . 5 6 9 2 . 7 7

5 4 . 4 3

8 7 . 8 8

5 4 . 5 8

9 1 . 2 0

4 6 . 5 0

7 6 . 3 3

6 3 . 8 0

7 4 . 3 6

2 2 . 8 0

2 5 . 1 5

1 7 . 9 1

3 2 . 0 1

1 7 . 1 1

2 5 . 6 0

2 7 . 2 0

2 3 . 2 5

5 4 . 3 3

8 7 . 6 1

5 4 . 1 0

9 1 . 0 0

4 6 . 6 0

7 6 . 1 4

6 3 . 9 0

7 4 . 4 0

(table continues^

Page 56: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

49

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

MBSRO:

Appearance Evaluation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

3 . 5 6

3 . 2 0

3 . 5 6

3 . 1 1

3 . 2 9

2 . 9 9

3 . 4 5

2 . 9 8

. 63

. 9 0

. 7 4

. 9 5

. 33

. 83

. 6 3

.87

3 . 5 6

3 . 2 0

3 . 5 5

3 . 1 1

3 . 2 9

3 . 0 0

3 . 4 6

2 . 9 9

3 . 7 0 . 69 3 . 7 0

(table continues)

Page 57: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

50

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Appearance Orientation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

2 . 8 5

2 . 9 8

2 . 6 8

3 . 6 6

4 . 1 3

3 . 2 9

3 . 6 3

3 . 0 9

3 . 7 9

3 . 2 6

3 . 6 3

. 9 2

. 69

. 72

.61

. 37

. 7 4

. 5 8

. 7 2

. 5 2

. 5 3

. 4 4

2 . 8 5

2 . 9 8

2 . 6 8

3 . 6 6

4 . 1 3

3 . 2 9

3 . 6 4

3 . 0 9

3 . 7 9

3 . 2 6

3 . 6 3

(table continues)

Page 58: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

51

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adj usted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Fitness Evaluation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

3 . 7 8

3 . 9 1

3 . 5 6

3 . 7 1

4 . 1 7

3 . 9 1

4 . 0 9

3 . 3 5

. 5 5

. 7 2

. 38

. 5 8

.66

.82

. 6 8

. 8 4

3 . 7 9

3 . 9 0

3 . 5 6

3 . 7 1

4 . 2 1

3 . 9 3

4 . 1 2

3 . 3 9

3 . 8 1

3 . 3 3

.28

. 8 5

3 . 7 9

3 . 2 8

(table continues)

Page 59: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

52

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Fitness Orientation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

3.61

3.18

3.20

2.72

3.36

2 .60

3 . 66

3.37

3.51

2 . 8 8

.87

.83

.65

. 66

.57

1.00

.41

.54

.54

.57

3.60

3.15

3.22

2 . 6 8

3.37

2 . 6 0

3.66

3.37

3.51

2 . 8 8

(table continues^

Page 60: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

53

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Health Evaluation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

3 . 0 9

3 . 0 2

2 . 9 9

2 . 7 4

3 . 1 7

2 . 8 7

3 . 0 5

2 . 8 7

53

61

.32

,56

34

48

,53

,50

3 . 0 9

3 . 0 2

2 . 9 9

2 . 7 4

3 . 1 7

2 . 8 7

3 . 0 5

2 . 8 7

3 . 9 6

3 . 6 9

. 5 9

. 8 0

3 . 9 7

3 . 7 0

(table continues)

Page 61: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

54

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

3 . 9 5

3 . 4 3

4 . 0 2

3 . 5 3

3 . 8 9

3 . 7 7

3 . 9 0

3 . 3 1

3 . 6 6

3 . 9 2

. 5 1

. 6 7

. 32

.47

. 3 8

. 7 8

. 7 6

.87

. 6 5

. 5 8

3 . 9 6

3 . 4 4

4 . 0 2

3 . 5 2

3 . 8 8

3 . 7 6

3 . 9 1

3 . 3 0

3 . 6 6

3 . 9 2

(table continues)

Page 62: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

55

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Health Orientation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

3 . 3 4

3 . 4 5

3 . 1 9

3 . 2 7

3 . 3 7

3 . 4 4

3 . 3 8

3 . 5 8

. 63

. 5 0

. 7 2

. 5 5

. 5 2

. 83

. 4 9

. 6 7

3 . 3 5

3 . 4 6

3 . 2 0

3 . 2 8

3 . 3 6

3 . 4 3

3 . 3 7

3 . 5 7

3 . 6 0

4 . 0 4

. 3 6

. 5 8

3 . 6 0

4 . 0 3

(table continues)

Page 63: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

56

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Illness Orientation

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

3 . 6 3

3 . 5 7

1 . 9 6

1 . 9 1

1 . 6 7

1 . 9 7

2 . 0 4

2 . 0 8

1 . 8 9

1.86

. 5 3

. 43

,53

.61

,55

,43

. 32

.37

. 42

. 4 2

3 . 6 3

3 . 5 7

1 . 9 7

1 . 9 1

1.68

1 . 9 8

2 . 0 3

2 . 0 6

1.88

1 . 8 4

(table continues^

Page 64: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

57

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Body Areas Satisfaction

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

1 . 9 0

2 . 4 6

2 . 2 2

2 . 0 7

3 . 4 0

3 . 2 6

3 . 4 8

2 . 9 5

,42

,46

,42

,60

. 63

. 67

. 5 5

. 6 9

1 . 9 1

2 . 4 5

2 . 2 2

2 . 0 7

3 . 4 1

3 . 2 6

3 . 4 8

2 . 9 6

3 . 2 4

3 . 1 3

. 48

.61

3 . 2 4

3 . 1 2

(table continues)

Page 65: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

58

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Weight Focus

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

3 . 0 0

3 . 0 1

3 . 4 8

2 . 8 2

3 . 3 8

3 . 1 8

2 . 0 2

2 . 9 6

1 . 8 3

2.61

.84

68

55

74

59

70

. 7 2

. 9 5

. 7 1

. 9 3

3 . 0 0

3 . 0 1

3 . 4 8

2 . 8 2

3 . 3 9

3 . 1 8

2 . 0 3

2 . 9 6

1 . 8 4

2.61

(table continues)

Page 66: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

59

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

1 . 9 3

2 . 7 2

2 . 0 6

2 . 8 0

2 . 1 3

2 . 4 8

2 . 3 2

2 . 3 6

,85

,99

. 8 8

.77

,59

82

,83

,74

1 . 9 3

2 . 7 1

2 . 0 6

2 . 8 0

2 . 1 3

2 . 4 8

2 . 3 2

2 . 3 6

(table continues)

Page 67: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

60

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Primary. Projective Measures;

HIT:

Barrier

Younger

Single

Male 7.33

Female 10.37

Involved

Male 6.78

Female 8.32

Middle-Aged

Single

Male 4.79

Female 7.00

Involved

Male 6.08

Female 8.00

2.92

4.15

3.00

3.43

1.81

2.88

3.55

3.55

7.32

10.36

6.77

8.30

4.79

7.02

6.09

8.01

(table continues^

Page 68: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

61

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adj usted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Penetration

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

5 . 7 0

6 . 6 7

5 . 6 7

5 . 0 7

9 . 3 3

1 1 . 5 8

9 . 1 7

8.21

3 . 4 3

4 . 7 0

3 . 6 0

3 . 2 9

4 . 5 0

5 . 4 7

3 . 1 7

3 . 6 0

5 . 6 9

6 . 6 8

5 . 6 6

5 . 0 7

9 . 4 0

11.61

9 . 2 3

8 . 2 9

6 . 0 0

6 . 7 5

4 . 1 1

3 . 3 2

6 . 0 0

6 . 6 4

Ctable continues)

Page 69: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

62

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Older

Involved

Male

Female

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Human

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

8 . 2 5

7 . 6 0

5 . 3 0

4 . 3 3

8 . 5 3

6 . 8 6

2 1 . 4 3

2 3 . 0 0

2 3 . 3 3

2 9 . 1 6

3 . 6 2

2 . 8 2

3 . 8 6

4 . 4 6

3 . 8 5

4 . 0 9

8 . 3 5

5 . 2 4

7 . 2 2

9 . 4 9

8 . 2 2

7 . 5 2

5 . 3 4

4 . 2 5

8 . 5 8

6.88

2 1 . 4 0

2 2 . 9 9

2 3 . 3 1

2 9 . 1 3

(table continues)

Page 70: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

63

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Animal

Younger

Single

Male

Female

2 3 . 9 3

2 2 . 9 4

1 8 . 7 5

2 2 . 8 5

21.20

2 1 . 8 3

1 9 . 8 0

1 8 . 0 7

2 9 . 1 9

2 8 . 2 6

12.28

7 . 6 6

6 . 4 4

7 . 1 9

8.12

1 4 . 0 3

9 . 2 9

6 . 9 0

1 0 . 0 3

4 . 6 1

2 3 . 9 4

2 2 . 9 8

1 8 . 7 6

2 2 . 8 8

2 1 . 1 9

21.86

1 9 . 7 9

18.06

2 9 . 1 0

2 8 . 2 2

(table continues)

Page 71: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

64

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

2 9 . 6 7

2 7 . 0 0

3 0 . 4 3

2 4 . 9 4

3 1 . 0 0

2 8 . 4 0

2 9 . 2 0

2 9 . 7 5

3 1 . 4 0

3 1 . 5 7

8 . 4 2

7 . 8 5

5 . 9 7

7 . 6 8

6 . 3 5

6 . 5 6

1 1 . 4 6

7 . 0 9

1 2 . 3 9

7 . 6 1

2 9 . 5 8

2 6 . 8 9

3 0 . 4 3

2 5 . 0 9

3 1 . 0 4

2 8 . 5 1

2 9 . 1 5

2 9 . 8 6

3 1 . 3 4

3 1 . 5 4

(table continues!

Page 72: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

65

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Anatomy

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

2 . 5 2

2 . 8 4

4 . 4 4

2 . 4 7

2 . 5 0

4 . 3 8

4 . 2 5

3 . 5 0

2 . 4 8

1.86

2 . 9 4

2 . 0 4

2 . 1 4

3 . 1 0

2 . 7 7

2 . 9 1

2 . 5 4

2 . 8 5

4 . 4 6

2 . 4 9

2 . 4 9

4 . 3 5

4 . 2 4

3 . 4 8

3 . 6 0

2 . 9 2

2 . 7 6

3 . 4 5

3 . 6 1

2 . 9 0

(table continues)

Page 73: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

66

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Sexual

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

4 . 0 0

3 . 2 1

. 52

. 4 2

. 8 3

. 9 5

.14

.94

1.08

. 9 0

3 . 0 2

2 . 2 6

. 7 5

. 7 7

1 . 0 4

2.01

. 3 6

1 . 3 4

1 . 6 8

1 . 8 3

4 . 0 1

3 . 2 2

.57

. 4 5

.88

1.01

.11

.86

1 .06

. 8 4

(table continues)

Page 74: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

67

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Color

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

. 4 0

. 33

. 4 0

. 2 9

1 6 . 1 4

1 6 . 8 4

1 3 . 3 3

1 5 . 9 5

9 . 4 3

1 9 . 0 6

1 . 2 7

. 6 5

. 8 3

.61

9 . 3 8

7 . 2 0

6.61

11.18

4 . 7 0

9 . 6 0

. 43

.27

. 4 3

. 3 0

1 6 . 1 4

1 6 . 8 4

1 3 . 3 4

1 5 . 9 5

9 . 4 3

1 9 . 0 6

(table continues)

Page 75: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

68

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Hostility

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

1 3 . 3 3

16.00

1 1 . 7 0

8 . 8 3

9 . 8 7

1 3 . 5 7

9 . 4 8

7 . 6 3

9 . 0 0

8 . 8 9

7 . 5 1

7 . 8 5

4 . 7 6

4 . 9 7

6 . 6 0

4 . 2 9

4 . 7 3

3 . 8 2

3 . 0 3

3 . 8 6

1 3 . 3 3

16.00

1 1 . 7 0

8 . 8 3

9 . 8 7

1 3 . 5 7

9 . 4 8

7 . 6 3

9 . 0 0

8 . 9 0

(table continues)

Page 76: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

69

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

1 0 . 7 9

1 0 . 5 0

1 0 . 7 5

8 . 8 0

9 . 3 0

8 . 4 2

1 2 . 8 7

9 . 2 9

4 . 1 0

4 . 4 7

5 . 2 8

4 . 2 8

8 . 2 3

5 . 5 7

4 . 1 4

5 . 2 5

1 0 . 7 9

1 0 . 5 0

1 0 . 7 5

8 . 8 0

9 . 3 0

8 . 4 2

1 2 . 8 7

9 . 2 9

(table continues)

Page 77: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

70

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Secondary. Objective Measures:

NEO-FFI:

Neuroticism

Younger

Single

Male 37.95

Female 37.32

Involved

Male 40.83

Female 36.84

Middle-Aged

Single

Male 44.43

Female 38.81

Involved

Male 45.17

Female 41.25

8.25

7.41

6.88

8.80

6.26

5.67

6.19

9.86

37.95

37.31

40.83

36.83

44.43

38.82

45.17

41.26

(table continues)

Page 78: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

71

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adj usted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Extraversion

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

4 4 . 1 0

4 2 . 0 8

4 3 . 7 3

4 3 . 0 0

3 0 . 9 5

2 5 . 5 3

3 2 . 5 0

3 3 . 0 5

1 0 . 7 8

7 . 6 3

3 . 9 4

5 . 0 5

5 . 4 4

5 . 9 3

4 . 5 8

5 . 8 7

4 4 . 1 0

4 2 . 0 9

4 3 . 7 3

4 3 . 0 0

3 1 . 0 3

2 5 . 5 6

3 2 . 5 6

3 3 . 1 4

3 1 . 5 7

3 2 . 6 9

6.21

5 . 4 4

3 1 . 5 3

3 2 . 5 7

(table continues)

Page 79: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

72

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Openness

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

3 1 . 6 7

3 1 . 9 5

3 4 . 7 0

3 4 . 3 3

3 3 . 0 7

3 3 . 3 6

3 2 . 1 0

2 8 . 7 9

3 4 . 0 0

3 3 . 4 7

4 . 6 4

6 . 7 4

7 . 0 7

4 . 2 7

5 . 2 0

4 . 4 0

5 . 9 8

5 . 5 4

4 . 6 4

4 . 4 0

3 1 . 6 3

3 1 . 8 7

3 4 . 7 4

3 4 . 2 5

3 3 . 1 1

3 3 . 3 8

3 2 . 2 2

2 8 . 8 5

3 4 . 1 0

3 3 . 6 1

(table continues^

Page 80: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

73

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

SAS:

Social Anxiety

Younger

Single

Male

3 5 . 7 9

3 2 . 3 8

3 2 . 2 5

2 8 . 5 5

3 8 . 3 0

3 4 . 6 7

3 4 . 0 0

3 6 . 7 9

1 1 . 5 2

2 .86

4 . 9 7

3 . 3 1

5 . 3 7

4 . 1 9

5 . 1 6

4 . 4 6

4 . 6 9

3 5 . 7 2

3 2 . 1 8

3 2 . 2 0

2 8 . 4 1

3 8 . 3 7

3 4 . 5 3

3 4 . 0 7

3 6 . 8 2

3 . 6 1 1 1 . 6 1

(table continues)

Page 81: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

74

Variable

Observed Adjusted

Mean SD Mean

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

* Male

Female

1 1 . 3 2

1 0 . 8 3

1 1 . 9 5

1 0 . 0 0

1 0 . 1 3

1 0 . 0 0

1 0 . 9 0

10.20

1 1 . 5 0

1 0 . 4 7

1 1 . 7 1

3 . 8 3

2 . 6 8

2 . 8 0

2.11

3 . 2 6

2 . 1 7

3 . 1 1

3 . 2 9

2 . 9 7

2 . 9 5

2 . 7 0

1 1 . 3 6

1 0 . 9 1

1 2 . 0 4

9 . 9 5

10.00

10.00

10.80

1 0 . 2 5

1 1 . 4 0

1 0 . 5 2

1 1 . 7 4

Page 82: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

75

Contrary to prediction, differences were found among

subjects of different ages on primary and secondary

measures. Post-hoc univariate analyses with regard to age

produced 15 significant effects. With respect to objective

body image measures, middle-aged subjects experienced more

actual-ideal body dissatisfaction, followed by older

subjects, with younger subjects experiencing less body

dissatisfaction as measured by the FRS (F = 3.71, df =

2,177, p < .026). When the discrepancy between subjects'

actual body size versus the cultural standard was taken into

consideration, older and middle-aged subjects experienced

more body dissatisfaction, with younger subjects again

experiencing less body dissatisfaction (F = 6.87, df =

2,177, p < .001). In contrast, younger subjects exhibited

more body shape concern as measured by the BSQ than middle-

aged and older subjects (F = 6.45, df = 2,177, p < .002).

The MBSRQ revealed older and younger subjects were

higher in appearance orientation than middle-aged subjects

(F = 4.24, df = 2,177, p < .016). Younger subjects showed a

higher fitness evaluation, followed by middle-aged subjects,

with older subjects showing lower fitness evaluation (F =

23.16, df = 2,177, p < .000). Younger subjects were higher

in fitness orientation than middle-aged and older subjects

(F = 11.58, df = 2,177, p < .000). Older subjects were

higher in health orientation (F = 6.45, df = 2,177, p <

Page 83: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

76

.002) and illness orientation (F = 5.13, df = 2,177, e <

.007) than middle-aged subjects and younger subjects.

With regard to the projective measure, the HIT, younger

subjects were higher on barrier (F = 8.10, df = 2,177, E <

.000), and penetration (F = 12.04, df - 2,177, E < .000),

than middle-aged and older subjects. Younger subjects were

higher on human (F = 3.16, df = 2,177, E < .045) than

middle-aged and older subjects. Younger and middle-aged

subjects were higher on color (F = 5.47, df 2, 177, E <

.005) than older subjects.

With regard to the secondary exploratory measure, the

NEO-FFI, older and middle-aged subjects were higher in

neuroticism (F = 7.87, df = 2,177, E < .001) than younger

subjects. Older subjects were higher in extraversion (F =

5.16, df = 2,177, E < .007) than middle-aged subjects and

younger subjects. Further, older subjects were higher in

openness than younger and middle-aged subjects (F = 11.19,

df = 2,177, E < .000) .

Hypothesis 2 predicted that, regardless of sex, there

would be no differences on body image variables as measured

by the FRS, BSQ, MBSRQ, and the HIT. Results from the 3 X 2

X 2 MANCOVA fail to support this hypothesis. As with age,

there was a strong, multivariate main effect for sex (F =

7.23, df = 24,154, £ < .000).

Contrary to prediction, differences were found among

male and female subjects on primary and secondary measures.

Page 84: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

77

Post-hoc univariate analyses with regard to sex produced 15

significant effects. With respect to objective body image

measures, female subjects experienced more actual-ideal body

dissatisfaction (F = 16.14, df = 1,177, p < .000) and more

actual-cultural standard body dissatisfaction than male

subjects (F = 32.80, df = 1,177, £ < .000) as measured by

the FRS. Similarly, female subjects exhibited more body

shape concern as measured by the BSQ than male subjects (F =

51.65, df = 1,177, p < .000).

The MBSRQ revealed male subjects were higher in

appearance evaluation (F = 15.08, df = 1,177, p < .000),

fitness evaluation (F = 23.25, df = 1,177, p < .000),

fitness orientation (F = 13.91, df = 1,177, p < .000),

health evaluation (F = 9.44, df = 1,177, p < .002), and

higher in body areas satisfaction (F = 7.87, df = 1,177, p <

.006) than female subjects. On the other hand, female

subjects were higher in appearance orientation (F = 17.33,

df = 1,177, p < .000) and weight focus (F = 23.39, df =

1,177, p < .000) than male subjects.

With regard to the projective measure, the HIT, females

were higher on color (F = 5.61, df = 1,177, p < .019) and

barrier (F = 8.78, df = 1,177, p < .003) than male subjects.

Male subjects had higher hostility scores than female

subjects (F = 4.22, df = 1,177, p < .041).

With respect to the secondary exploratory measure, the

NEO-FFI, male subjects were higher in neuroticism (F = 6.17,

Page 85: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

78

df = 1,177, £ < .014) and openness (F = 7.93, df = 1,177, E <

.005) than female subjects.

Hypothesis 3 predicted that, regardless of relationship

status, there would be no differences on body image variables

as measured by the FRS, BSQ, MBSRQ, and the HIT. Results

from the 3 X 2 X 2 MANCOVA produced no main effect for

relationship status (F = .91, df = 24,154, E < .588). Thus,

hypothesis 3 was supported.

Hypothesis 4 predicted a three-way interaction such that

young, single females would experience the most body concern

and dissatisfaction relative to other groups. Results from

the 3 X 2 X 2 MANCOVA did not yield a three-way interaction,

thus failing to support hypothesis 4. However, an age by sex

interaction was found (F = 1.62, df = 48,308, £ < .009)

although post-hoc univariate analyses produced no significant

effects. Due to the shared variance among primary dependent

measures, this interaction most likely occurred as an

artifact of such relationships. For example, the correlation

between the BSQ and the Weight Attitudes Scale of the MBSRQ

was .70 and the correlation between the FRS and the BSQ was

.53.

The 3 X 2 X 2 MANCOVA also produced a significant age by

relationship status interaction (F = 2.47, df = 48,308, e <

.000). Post-hoc univariate analyses yielded significant

effects for four variables. The projective measure, the HIT,

revealed two significant effects. As shown in Figure 1 (See

Page 86: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

79

Appendix C), for younger subjects, penetration scores were

higher for single subjects. However, for middle-aged and

older subjects, penetration scores were higher for involved

subjects (F = 5.92, df = 2,177, E < .003). As shown in

Figure 2 (See Appendix C), for younger subjects, human scores

were higher for involved subjects. However, for middle-aged

and older subjects, human scores were higher for single

subjects (F = 3.33, df = 2,177, g < .038).

With regard to the secondary exploratory measure, the

NEO-FFI, two significant effects were found. As shown in

Figures 3 and 4, respectively (See Appendix C), for younger

subjects, extraversion (F = 5.15, df = 2,177, p < .007) and

openness to new experience (F = 9.08, df = 2,177, p < .000)

were higher for involved subjects. However, for middle-aged

and older subjects, extraversion and openness to new

experience were higher for single subjects.

Supplementary Analyses

In a supplementary analysis, DAP variables were analyzed

utilizing a 3 X 2 X 2 MANCOVA with social desirability

serving as the covariate. DAP variables were analyzed

separately due to a substantial number of subjects who drew

heads only which were unscoreable and thus considered missing

data. Cell sizes ranged from 9 to 21 with an average cell

size of 15. Table 3 summarizes the results of this

procedure.

Page 87: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

80

Table 3

Observed and Adjusted Means for Multivariate Analysis of

Covariance for the Draw-A-Person

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Draw-A-Person:

General

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

17.89

16.67

18.00

17.21

17.30

17.33

18.17

17.42

1.82

2.01

1.93

2.01

1.25

1.76

2.13

2.12

17.93

16.68

18.02

17.25

17.29

17.27

18.12

17.39

(table continues^

Page 88: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

81

Variable

Observed

Mean SD

Adjusted

Mean

Older

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Specific

Younger

Single

Male

Female

Involved

Male

Female

Middle-Aged

Single

Male

Female

1 8 . 9 1

1 8 . 2 2

1 8 . 9 3

1 7 . 8 6

2 0 . 7 4

1 9 . 3 8

1 9 . 7 3

1 9 . 1 6

2 . 5 9

2 . 3 9

2 . 7 9

2 . 0 3

1 . 9 7

2 . 9 0

2 . 8 7

2 . 1 4

1 8 . 9 3

1 8 . 2 0

1 8 . 9 6

1 7 . 8 7

2 0 . 7 5

1 9 . 3 9

1 9 . 7 4

1 9 . 1 7

1 7 . 5 4

2 0 . 2 7

1 . 9 8

2 . 4 6

1 7 . 5 3

2 0 . 2 5

(table continues)

Page 89: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

82

Observed Adj usted

Variable Mean SD Mean

Involved

Male 20.25 1.82 20.24

Female 18.95 1.84 18.94

Older

Single

Male 21.18 4.58 21.19

Female 21.00 2.60 20.99

Involved

Male 21.57 3.76 21.58

Female 21.93 3.27 21.93

Results of the 3 X 2 X 2 MANCOVA produced two

significant main effects. A multivariate main effect was

found for age (F = 4.67, df = 4,334, jd < .001) and sex (F =

3.27, df = 2,167, E < .040).

With regard to the age main effect, post-hoc univariate

analyses yielded significant effects for General (F = 3.95,

df = 2,168, p < .021) and Specific (F = 8.62, df = 2,168, p

< .000) DAP variables such that older subjects exhibited a

more negative body concept at a global and specific level

than middle-aged and younger subjects. With respect to the

Page 90: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

83

sex main effect, post-hoc univariate analyses produced a

significant effect for the General DAP variables (£ = 5.75,

df = 1,168, p < .018) such that male subjects showed a more

negative body concept at a global level than female

subj ects.

Exploratory Correlational Analyses

Exploratory correlational analyses were conducted to

examine the relationships between demographic variables and

both primary and secondary dependent variables. Demographic

variables included age, sex, relationship status, education,

health status, and exercise. Few significant correlations

were found. Sex and health status accounted for all but one

significant correlation above r = .30, p < .01.

Sex was negatively correlated with fitness evaluation

as measured by the MBSRQ (r = .31) and positively correlated

with actual-cultural standard body dissatisfaction (FRS, r =

.38, p < .01), weight focus (MBSRQ, r = .35, p < .01) and

body shape concern (BSQ, r = .49, p < .01). Health was also

positively correlated with actual-cultural standard body

dissatisfaction (FRS, r = .35, p < .01) and negatively

correlated with five MBSRQ variables including appearance

evaluation (r = .35, p < .01), fitness orientation (r = .31,

E < .01), health evaluation (r = .50, p < .01), health

orientation (r = .31, p < .01), and body areas satisfaction

(r = .40, p < .01).

Page 91: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

84

Exploratory correlational analyses were also conducted

to examine the relationships among dependent measures. For

the DAP, HIT, NEO-FFI, and the SAS, there were few

significant correlations. The few that were found were less

than r = .03. Most significant correlations were found

among the primary, objective body image measures: the FRS,

BSQ, and the MBSRQ. Table 4 summarizes these results.

Page 92: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

85

w <1)

i—1

A ( 0

• H U >

• P

C

Q )

T 3

C

0 )

0 )

Q

Q )

> • H

- P

0

0 )

• r ~

O

*

> U ( d

B

M

a*

0

c

0

s

«<

CA

c

0

• H

- p

( 0

r H

a > a )

H M

• Q U 0

a

4= W) O ) 3

• - O 0> o

3 u .

t o > » <0

" O 0 ) O t .

CO <

</> t o 4-* 0 ) c c o

-M C —» <u <0 •— <D u

x O

— t 0 <0 > 0> U J

t o t o -M <D C c a>

<0 t o fl) — C <0 • J >

tO c <u a> a — a c .

< o

C D — a> <0 a > Q . LU

<

<D > . a

T J ( 0 O J T

CO CO

— ' <0 ( 0 <-3 3 4-> -M O — ' < •

O

t 0 — ' 3 t 0 -M <U O " D

< ~

O o

o o

o o

a

o o

o o

o o

K > o

o o

o o

o o

> * i n

t o

« - o O f O

o i n

K > i n

v O CM

T J

tO c T J O C • -CO 4-* •M O c

> » c 0 0 t o L .

T J O «+- <D O — — ' t o U

CO *•» t o c O C . •M o

—» tO 3 t 0 C J ( 0 4 - •M tO a> t o — ' ( 0 a>

T J — 3 • - a •-« *-> U O ( 0

• t o i x ; —> <0 — >. CO ( 0 t o t o " O a 3 • - 3 O >» oc

c o l 4-> O +•* CO a 1 3 </) a : ! u o in\ O CO U L | < < c o l CO X

N . i n

( 0 3

( 0 >

U J

Q> O

c ( 0 L . <0 a* a a

c <D

C t 0 U (0 <D a a

<

o o

( M n O

O O

o CM

i n

O <\J * -(Nl ( M

N -

O J • s t

O o

« - 1 ^ « - >4*

NO s f

i n o

N » P O

h -o

> * CNJ

r - O N . N t

s j - N . t -r~ O O

i n r— 0 0 i n o o *4- r o

r o r o r \ j o o i n i n

O r o

CO r v j

&> -4"

r - 4-» (/}

c C c o c o o •r> c o

o • r - 4-»

*J t o •»— •M t o

t o •M •M t o 3 c t o •M c

a> 3 c a>

t o >r— —> a> •»—

> u t o • r - c -

U J O > L . n

o

t o t o U J w

t o

t o t o . c x : t o

V a> •M a>

c c c

4~> t o t o w

>r- 4 ) —*

u . U - 2 C a c —*

( 0 </> t o

3 t o O <0 O <L> u u

> » o > " O o a>

o o 3

Page 93: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

CHAPTER IV

DISCUSSION

To date, the majority of body image research has

focused on young women. Moreover, a large proportion of

these studies have focused on clinical populations such as

women with anorexia and/or bulimia nervosa. The purpose of

the present study was to explore potential influences of

age, sex, and relationship status on body image in a more

comprehensive manner using a nonclinical subject sample.

Hypothesis 1 predicted that, regardless of age group

membership, there would be no differences on body image

measures. Results suggest that subjects do differ with

regard to body image as a function of age.

More specifically, results from the FRS indicated that

middle-aged subjects saw a greater discrepancy between their

actual and ideal body size than older and younger subjects,

while older and middle-aged subjects saw a greater

discrepancy between their actual body size and the cultural

standard for body size than younger subjects. These

findings are consistent with those of Rozin and Fallon

(1988) who found that middle-aged adults were dissatisfied

with their body shape and weight and with those of Van-

Deusen, Harlowe, and Baker (1989) who found that older

86

Page 94: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

87

adults perceive their appearance less positively than

younger adults.

Interestingly, however, while middle-aged and older

adults were more dissatisfied with their appearance, younger

subjects exhibited greater body shape concern (BSQ) than

middle-aged and older subjects. This could reflect the mass

media relentlessly targeting younger audiences (Andersen &

DiDomenico, 1992). This increased level of body concern

could also be reflective of one reason why eating disorders

are much more prevalent in younger groups (Rosen, 1990).

Older adults then are more dissatisfied with their

appearance, but are less concerned about it, while younger

adults are less dissatisfied with their appearance, but are

more anxious about it. It could be that older adults,

although more dissatisfied with their appearance, feel less

able to alter their appearance to meet such unrealistic and

young cultural standards and therefore, worry less about

appearance. Older adults could also feel less attractive

due to cultural stereotypes which equate older age with less

attractiveness (Rodin & Langer, 1980; Sharkey, 1993).

Results from the MBSRQ indicated that older and younger

subjects placed more importance on appearance than middle-

aged subjects. Older adults also paid more attention to

health and signs of illness than younger and middle-aged

subjects. In a culture which places tremendous emphasis on

appearance and health, older adults are likely to be more

Page 95: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

88

sensitive to the discrepancy between themselves and young

cultural standards.

In addition to appearance, younger subjects placed more

importance on fitness and were more satisfied with their

fitness level than middle-aged and older subjects. These

results are consistent with findings which indicate that

people's activity levels differ with age (Gordon & Galtz,

1976). The findings that middle-aged and older adults place

less importance on fitness and are less satisfied with their

fitness level may contribute to the increased body

dissatisfaction reported by these two age groups.

Results from the HIT show that younger subjects were

higher in body image boundary and vulnerability to body

image boundary disturbance than middle-age and older

subjects. These results indicate that younger subjects feel

more insecure about their body image, and as a result, must

utilize defenses to cope with the resulting anxiety. Thus,

young adults are more concerned with their appearance, but

are also more likely to feel insecure about their

appearance.

It was suspected that subjects higher in emotionality

and interpersonal relationship salience would also be higher

in body concern and dissatisfaction. Parallel findings

indicated that younger subjects were higher in emotionality

and interpersonal relationship salience and also higher in

body concern. However, little relationship was found

Page 96: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

89

between body concern and emotionality (r = .04) and

interpersonal relationship salience (r = .08). Middle-aged

subjects were also higher in emotionality than older

subjects and also higher in body dissatisfaction. However,

little relationship was found between body dissatisfaction

and emotionality (r = -.05).

Results from the NEO-FFI indicated that older and

middle-aged subjects were higher in negative affectivity

than younger subjects, while older subjects were higher in

extraversion and openness than middle-aged and younger

subjects. It was suspected that subjects higher in negative

affectivity and extraversion might be more likely to

experience body concern and dissatisfaction, while subjects

higher in openness would be less bound to cultural norms and

experience less body concern and dissatisfaction. Parallel

findings indicated that older and middle-aged subjects were

higher in negative affectivity and also higher in body

dissatisfaction. However, correlational analyses indicated

nonsignficant relationships between body dissatisfaction and

negative affectivity (r = -.24). In addition, while only

older subjects who were suspected to be higher in

extraversion who were also expected to be higher in body

dissatisfaction, no relationship was found (r = -.12),

despite age differences in each construct. However, a

relationship was supported between extraversion and several

MBSRQ variables, in that older subjects higher in

Page 97: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

90

extraversion were less satisfied with their level of fitness

(r = -.29, £ < .05) and health status (r = -.28, p < *05)

and placed less importance on fitness (r = -.44, p < .01).

It was also found that older subjects were more open

and experienced less body concern. Little relationship was

found, however, between openness and body Concern (r =

-.05) .

Hypothesis 2 predicted that, regardless of gender,

subjects would not differ on body image measures. Results

suggest that subjects do differ with regard to body image as

a function of sex.

Specifically, results from the FRS indicate female

subjects saw a greater discrepancy between their actual body

size and their ideal/cultural standard body size than male

subjects. Female subjects also exhibited more concern about

their body shape than male subjects. These results, which

indicate women experience greater body dissatisfaction and

more body shape concern than men, are consistent with the

majority of research findings to date (Cash, Winstead, &

Janda, 1986; Fallon, 1990; Fallon & Rozin, 1986) and may

reflect the fact that women are bombarded with over ten

times as many advertisements and articles promoting thinness

and/or weight loss (Andersen & DiDomenico, 1992).

Results from the MBSRQ indicate male subjects placed

more importance on fitness and were more satisfied with

their appearance, fitness level, specific body areas, and

Page 98: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

91

health status than female subjects. On the other hand,

female subjects placed more importance on physical

appearance and were more weight focused than male subjects.

Due to men placing increased emphasis on fitness and

experiencing a higher fitness level, they may have fewer

weight problems and thus, are less concerned and

dissatisfied with their body. These findings also suggest

that men evaluate themselves more favorably, while women pay

more attention to their physical appearance and, in

particular, weight. Consistent with these findings,

Andersen and DiDomenico (1992) found that mass media targets

body shape change for men and weight loss for women. In

addition, McDonald and Thompson (1992) found that men's

motivation for exercise is more fitness-related, while

women's motivation for exercise is more weight- and tone-

related.

Results from the HIT showed that female subjects were

higher in body image boundary and emotionality than male

subjects, while male subjects exhibited more hostility than

female subjects. The finding that women are more defensive

when it comes to their physical appearance makes sense,

given the degree of body concern and dissatisfaction

exhibited and the intensity with which mass media targets

women (Andersen & DiDomenico, 1992).

As previously stated, it was suspected that subjects

higher in emotionality would be higher in body concern and

Page 99: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

92

dissatisfaction. Results indicated that female subjects

were higher in emotionality and also higher in body concern

and dissatisfaction. However, little relationship between

emotionality and body concern (r = .02) and dissatisfaction

(r < -.06) was found.

It was expected that subjects higher in hostility would

be higher in body concern and dissatisfaction. Results

suggested an inverse relationship between these two such

that male subjects were higher in anger and reported lower

body concern and dissatisfaction than females, however, no

significant relationship was found between hostility and

body concern (r = -.04) and dissatisfaction (r = -.08).

Results from the NEO-FFI indicated that male subjects

were higher in negative affectivity and more open to new

experience than female subjects. It was suspected that

subjects higher in negative affectivity would report more

body concern and dissatisfaction, while subjects higher in

openness would report less body concern and dissatisfaction.

As was the case with hostility, results indicated that male

subjects were higher in negative affectivity and reported

less body concern and dissatisfaction. This inverse

relationship is partially supported by a correlation of -.42

(E < .01) for body concern. Given this inverse

relationship, it may be that, for men, negative affect

serves as a buffer to body image concern. Interestingly,

acknowledging negative affect is frequently cited as a

Page 100: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

93

troublesome issue in therapy with female anorectic and/or

bulimic clients (Bruch, 1985; Garfinkel & Garner, 1982).

As was suspected, male subjects were higher in openness

and reported less body concern and dissatisfaction. Little

relationship, however, was found between openness and body

concern (r = -.09) and dissatisfaction (r = .05).

Hypothesis 3 predicted that subjects would not differ

on body image measures as a function of relationship status.

Results supported this hypothesis suggesting that

relationship status alone exerts little influence on body

image. It was suspected that being involved in a committed

relationship might serve as a buffer against body image

concern and dissatisfaction. However, it was found that

whether a person is single or involved in a committed

relationship had little influence on body image. This could

be due to the fact that the distinction between single and

involved relationships is not as clear-cut as it once was

(Saxton, 1990). Relationships are highly individualized

experiences. Consequently, using an arbitrarily determined

dichotomous classification to explore potential influences

of relationship status on body image may not be able to tap

the potential complexities involved.

Hypothesis 4 predicted that young, single females would

experience higher body image concern and dissatisfaction

relative to other groups. Results failed to support this

Page 101: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

94

hypothesis. However, an age by relationship status

interaction was found.

Interestingly, post-hoc analyses produced no

significant effects for objective body image measures.

However, results from the HIT indicated that younger

subjects felt more insecure about their appearance if they

were single while middle-aged and older subjects felt more

insecure about their appearance if they were involved in a

committed relationship. It appears that for younger

subjects, the stress of dating may increase body image

issues for the single person, while a relationship may

provide a buffer to body image stress for the involved

person. Middle-aged and older involved subjects, who

reported higher levels of body dissatisfaction, may feel

more pressure to maintain a certain body size and shape in

order to maintain their partner's interest.

For younger subjects, interpersonal relationship

salience was higher for involved subjects. However, for

middle-aged and older subjects, interpersonal relationship

salience was higher for single subjects. In spite of the

significant interaction, little relationship was found

between interpersonal relationship salience and body concern

and dissatisfaction (r < .18).

Results from the NEO-FFI indicated that for younger

subjects, extraversion and openness to new experience were

higher for involved subjects. However, for middle-aged and

Page 102: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

95

older subjects, extraversion and openness to new experience

were higher for single subjects. In spite of the

significant interaction with respect to extraversion and

openness, no significant correlations were found between

extraversion and openness and body concern and

dissatisfaction (r < .26).

For the DAP, in spite of a significant amount of

missing data, a separate supplementary analysis yielded the

same two main effects: age and sex. With respect to age,

older subjects exhibited a more negative body concept at a

global and specific level than middle-aged and younger

subjects. These results are consistent with current

findings which suggest that older adults are more

dissatisfied with their physical appearance than younger

adults. For example, Van-Deusen, Harlow, and Baker (1989)

found that older adults view their physical appearance more

negatively than younger adults.

With respect to sex, males exhibited a more negative

body concept at a global level than females. Given the

literature, these results are somewhat surprising as it was

expected that female subjects would more likely exhibit a

negative body concept. Given earlier findings that men

experienced higher levels of projectively assessed hostility

and negative affectivity, it could be that men consciously

report being less concerned and dissatisfied with their

Page 103: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

96

appearance, when they are unconsciously experiencing anger

and frustration resulting in a negative body concept.

Summary

To summarize the findings of the present study, age and

sex have a substantial influence on body image. Age has a

significant influence on body image such that older and

middle-aged adults are more dissatisfied with their

appearance, while younger adults feel more anxious about

their appearance. Older adults place more importance on

appearance and attend more to their health and signs of

illness, while younger adults place more importance on

appearance and fitness and are more satisfied with their

level of fitness.

Sex was also found to have a significant influence on

body image such that women reported more concern and

dissatisfaction with their appearance, placed more

importance on appearance, and were more weight focused.

Men, on the other hand, placed more importance on fitness

and evaluate themselves more favorably in terms of

appearance, fitness level, and health status. At an

unconscious level, however, men experienced a more negative

body image than women.

The interaction of age and relationship status was also

found to exert an influence on body image, in that

insecurity about appearance was higher for younger single

subjects and middle-aged and older involved subjects.

Page 104: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

97

Limitations of the Study

With regard to the sample in this study, there are

several issues to consider. First, there may have been a

selective sampling issue. Subjects were asked to

participate in a body image study; thus, they knew the topic

being investigated in advance and may have elected to

participate based on personal interest and/or curiosity

about the topic.

Second, subjects were predominantly white and all were

United States (US) citizens from birth to adulthood, thus

generalization of these results are limited to white native-

born US citizens. Third, although all subjects were US

citizens, their particular region of origin (e.g., the

northwestern or northeastern region of the US) was not taken

into consideration. Subjects represented the full spectrum

of national regions.

A fourth and somewhat troublesome sampling issue

involved the difficulty of accessing single, middle-aged and

older male subjects. As stated in the literature review,

older, single women significantly outnumber older, single

men (Atchley, 1988). In addition to accessing these two

groups, it was also consistently observed that the single,

middle-aged and older male subjects tended to be more

guarded during interviews than other groups. For example,

single, middle-aged and older male subjects accounted for

the majority of "head only" drawings which reflect some

Page 105: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

98

level of defensiveness and a detachment from affect with a

heavy reliance on intellectualization. Since "head only"

drawings are unscoreable according to the Dougherty, Cook,

and Coppin scoring system, a significant amount of missing

data and reduced cell size was accounted for by these two

groups.

In terms of instruments used in this study, there are

some issues which warrant discussion. First, the Body Shape

Questionnaire (BSQ) is somewhat biased in that it is

oriented towards persons who are concerned about becoming

overweight. For example, several questions are stated in

terms of fear of gaining weight (e.g., "Have you been afraid

that you might become fat?"). Some people, however, want to

gain weight and may have difficulty doing so. "Feeling

skinny" can be a body image issue of equal importance which

is not addressed by the BSQ.

Second, the Holtzman Inkblot Technique (HIT) consists

of 45 inkblot cards to which the subject provides one

response per card. Although the HIT lends itself more

easily to research, the cognitive task required and the

number of cards seem to frustrate and fatigue several older

subjects. Their frustration and fatigue could also

represent defensiveness due to the fact that the HIT is a

somewhat unstructured task and triggers emotional reactions.

Regardless of the reason, older subjects' frustration and

Page 106: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

99

fatigue undoubtedly had an impact on their responses,

particularly on the last half of the cards.

Third, subjects were administered the projective

instruments first so as to obtain purer responses. These

were followed by a questionnaire packet in which the number

of questionnaires seem to overwhelm and fatigue some

subjects, particularly older subjects. As previously

stated, fatigue may have influenced subjects' responses.

In terms of methodological concerns, relationship

status might have been operationalized in a more effective

way. For example, rather than requiring cohabitation, it

might have been more useful to define "involved" to include

anyone who has been dating the same partner for six months

or longer.

Another methodological concern involves the fact that

this was a cross-sectional study. While age differences

were found, cohort effects could be confounding results.

For example, it was found that younger adults were more

concerned about appearance than older adults. Younger

adults live in a culture where appearance receives

tremendous emphasis and they are therefore socialized to be

highly concerned about appearance. Older adults, on the

other hand, may have grown up in a time when less emphasis

was placed on appearance and the cultural standards for

physical appearance were not so extreme. Thus, it may be

Page 107: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

100

that older persons were socialized to be less concerned

about appearance.

Implications

The purpose of the present study was to explore

potential influences of age, sex, and relationship status on

body image. Results indicate that age and'sex exert

considerable influence on body image, while relationship

status combined with age has some influence on body image.

With regard to age, the findings of this study have

important ramifications. First, the acknowledgement that

body image issues do exist for middle-age and older adults,

not just younger adults. Second, middle-aged and older

subjects feel less attractive. Middle-aged and older

subjects are living in a cultural context which places a

high value on physical appearance and in which stereotypes

of beauty belong to the younger population. This can result

in lowered self-esteem for middle-aged and older people,

leaving these groups feelings unattractive, inadequate, and

devalued.

While middle-aged and older groups feel less

attractive, younger persons worry more about physical

appearance. What happens when a young, adult cohort, which

already obsesses about physical appearance to such a high

degree, encounters aging and the inevitable physical decline

that goes with it? Will they experience even more feelings

Page 108: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

101

of unattractiveness, inadequacy, and devaluation than the

current middle-aged and older sample?

Marketing and advertising campaigns certainly reflect

these findings. Printed advertisements geared toward

younger audiences more often employ full body pictures of

young adults, while advertisements geared toward middle-aged

audiences more often show chemical agents (e.g., anti-aging

creams) with which to fight aging and thus, rejection

(Sharkey, 1993).

With regard to sex, results from the present study

have important ramifications for women and men. First,

although not to the extent that women exhibit, body image

issues do exist for men. The difference may be that, at a

conscious level, men do not evaluate themselves as harshly

as women. At an unconscious level, however, men may feel

more negative about their bodies and express more negative

affect (e.g., hostility) than women.

Women, on the other hand, consciously acknowledge body

image issues. This may be due to the fear of becoming

overweight and the pursuit of thinness being more strongly

socialized in females (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore,

1985). Women are socialized from an early age and at many

levels to worry excessively about their appearance and to

critically compare themselves with the unrealistic cultural

standards for attractiveness presented in the onslaught of

mass media. In fact, body concern and dissatisfaction might

Page 109: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

102

be thought of as a normative experience for women (Rodin,

Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985).

Regardless of age or sex, body dissatisfaction in this

sample is due in large part to people comparing themselves

with thinner cultural standards. Modal responses from the

FRS reflect this tendency. Of nine figures, with figure 1

representing the thinnest option from which to choose,

subjects most frequently selected figures 4-5 as their

actual body size, figures 3-4 as their ideal body size,

while figures 2-3 were selected as representative of the

cultural standard for physical appearance.

With regard to relationship status combined with age

influences, the implications are more complex. It appears

that involvement in a relationship serves as a buffer

against body image issues for younger people. On the other

hand, involvement increases body image issues for middle-

aged and older persons. With cultural stereotypes which

dictate that younger is more attractive, it may be that

middle-aged and older persons are feeling less attractive

and potentially worrying about their ability to keep their

partner's interest. Another possible hypothesis is that

middle-aged and older adults are more sensitive to the

possibility of losing a partner and then having to re-enter

the dating scene.

The findings of the present investigation may be useful

for the therapist working with adult populations. First,

Page 110: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

103

these results provide normative information with which to

educate adult clients of all ages and represent both genders

who may be struggling with body image issues. Second, the

findings of this study may help to validate and normalize

body image issues. For example, a newly divorced middle-

aged person may feel less aberrant knowing that middle-aged

persons are often dissatisfied with their appearance.

Third, information from this study is also important to

convey to the general public in that it sensitizes people to

the reality of body image issues which exist across the

lifespan and for both sexes.

With regard to directions for future research, there

are several considerations which warrant discussion. The

present study explored the effects of age, sex, and

relationship status on body image. These three variables

are merely three of numerous potential variables which may

influence body image. The following are a few possibilities

which may add to the knowledge base regarding determinants

of body image.

It is important to note that subjects in the current

study represented several metropolitan areas of the United

States. It can be speculated that persons from different

regions of the country might differ with respect to body

image. Furthermore, given that mass media is more

concentrated in metropolitan areas, people from metropolitan

areas might differ from people from rural areas with regard

Page 111: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

104

to body image. Further investigation is needed to explore

these possibilities.

It is also important for future research to take into

account a person's sexual orientation. According to

Glassner (1989), homosexual men contend with intense

cultural pressures with regard to physical appearance

(Glassner, 1989). On the other hand, homosexual women

report less concern with weight and appearance and are more

satisfied with their bodies than heterosexual women (Herzog,

Newman, Yeh, & Warshaw, 1992).

Another line of inquiry would be to take into

consideration a person's racial/ethnic background with

regard to body image. Most body image research has employed

Eurocentric norms which may not provide an accurate and/or

complete picture of body image for non-white racial/ethnic

groups. For example, Cash and Bond (1992) found that for

African-American women, skin color dissatisfaction was

related to body dissatisfaction.

It might also be important to take into account racial

membership and their stage of racial identity development.

For example, "during the immersion stage of racial identity

development, people immerse themselves in their respective

culture completely and reject the norms of Euro-American

culture (Helms, 1990; Sue & Sue, 1990). Thus, with respect

to body image, persons in the immersion stage of racial

identity development might identify with a different

Page 112: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

105

cultural norm for physical appearance than the typical Euro-

American standards in the United States.

In conclusion, the findings of the present study

confirm the reality that body image issues exist across the

lifespan and for both genders. Due, in large part, to

media-driven cultural standards for physical appearance,

there are tremendous numbers of people who are feeling less

attractive and devalued because they do not "match" the

cultural standard presented in the media. This is an

atrocity and it is hoped that the findings of current study

and future research in this area will bring these issues to

light so that change can be effected and people of different

shapes and sizes, ages and gender can feel better about

their physical appearance and themselves.

Page 113: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

APPENDIX A

CONSENT FORM

106

Page 114: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

107

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS RESEARCH PARTICIPANT

CONSENT FORM

I, , agree to participate in a study regarding body image in adults. The purpose of this study is to examine people's perceptions of their physical appearance.

As a participant, I understand that my involvement in this research project is completely voluntary. I understand that I will be asked to respond to questionnaires relating to my perceptions, attitudes, and behaviors.

I have been informed that any information obtained in this study will be recorded with a code number that will only allow the project director, Caren Cooper, to determine my identity. At the conclusion of this study, the key that relates my name with my assigned code number will be destroyed. Under this condition, I agree that any information obtained from this research may be used in any way thought best for publication or education.

I understand that there is no personal risk or discomfort directly involved with this research. I understand that I am free to withdraw my consent and discontinue participation in this study at any time.

If I have any questions or problems that arise in connection with my participation in this study, I should contact Caren Cooper, project director, at (817) 565-2671.

Research Participant Date

Project Director Date

THIS PROJECT HAS BEEN REVIEWED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS COMMITTEE FOR THE PROTECTION OF HUMAN SUBJECTS (Phone: 817/565-3940).

Page 115: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

APPENDIX B

DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE

108

Page 116: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

109

SUBJECT t:

1. Age:

2. Sex: (1) Male (2) Female

3. Marital/Relationship Status: (1) Single (2) Committed Relationship (living with partner

at least 3 months, but not legally married) (3) Married (How long? ) (4) Separated (5) Divorced (6) Widowed

4. Number of Children:

5. How many years of formal education have you completed? (Circle)

U M H 7 8 9 10 H 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Grade School High School College Graduate School

6. Height: inches

7. Do you consider yourself too short? (1) Yes (2) No

If yes, how much? inches

8. Do you consider yourself too tall? (1) Yes (2) No

If yes, how much? inches

9. Height: pounds

10. Have you lost or gained weight within the last month? "(1) Yes (2) No

If yes, how much? pounds

11. Would you like to lose weight? (1) Yes (2) No

If yes, how much? pounds

12. Would you like to gain weight? (1) Yes (2) No

If yes, how much? pounds

Page 117: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

110

13. Do you smoke? (1) More than one pack of cigarettes per day (2) Regularly, but less that one pack per day (3) Occasionally (4) Never

14. How would you rate your health compared to other people your age?

(1) Excellent (2) Good (3) Pair (4) Poor

15. How often do you exercise? (1) 1-2 times per week (2) 3-5 times per week (3) Everyday (4) Once every two weeks (5) Once a month (6) Less than once a month

Briefly describe exercise activities:

16. Are you a member of any club(s) or organization(s)? (1) Yes (2) No

If yes, which ones?

Page 118: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

APPENDIX C

FIGURES

111

Page 119: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

112

t s H i =>

o z>

• O l

' S r i 5=

• pm*

C O

«3J

W •P 0 0)

• o

3 cn

* d 0) >

*H O

£

(A •P O i

• H M O tfl XX 3 W

<d

s a j o 3 $ u D i | C J | a u a , j u e a y

T5 C (d

0) i H Cn g

• H CO

o u

m <D M o o 05

c o

• H -P <d

-P <D C 0) 04

c <d a) fl

T5 a <d

a 3 o M cn

a) tx> c

a) u 3

• H Cm

• H B

• H in

> i xx ' d a) x a) c

• H

a) <d

-P a a) a)

<w <w

-P c <d o

•H <W • H c

* H w . p

o c 0)

<d

xi o

• H £

in c <d a) SB

-P C 0)

a) <w <4-4

c (d 0

• H <W • H C tx>

•H W

<D M (d

a) <4-1 • H

(A •P CU

•H

0 i f i

XI 3 W

a) 01 o & » (0 c (d a) s

Page 120: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

113

•He* Oi

• OI

s= • ««* CO •

s a j m s u e u n H u c a y

U) •P o 0) • o

3 (A

T3 0) > iH 0

1 •H

*d c fd

0) H 0* C •H W U O %4 (A Q) U 0 0 w

c fd

1 a c <d a) s *0 a <G

a 3 o

tr>

0)

i ?

04 a) u 3 tJ •H Pl»

W 4J 04 •H

O (A A 3 m u <d H *H B •H (ft

>i £

•d a) x a) *d c

•H

0)

(d P C a) M a) <w

•P C a) m a> <w <w •H

«p a <d O •H <U *H c tn •H ia

>i a) rH -P fd C <d b 0 a> •H <w <W <W •H •H c T3 tr» •H CO <a -P

-P •H 0 £ O

(A a) JQ b 3 ed (A ,£5 0) 0 (A

•H O ,C £ > *

(A (A c C td (d 0) a) £ 36

Page 121: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

114

V) -P 0 q> •o % W

13 a) >

s a j Q 3 $ UCJ i sjartcjp:] u e a y

o % •H

'd g (d

0) H

g •H in

u o

in a)

o o (0

g o •H (0 M a) > <d -P X w

g (0 a) n T5 G <d a , 3 o u tr»

a) & <

CO

a) 3 d •H Pm

CO

a •H M O in £ s (0

14 <d H •rl

e •H (0

>i A

a> X a) *d c

0) u <d

4J g a) u

•P g 0) M a) <w <w •H

•P g rd O •H <U

a) • H <W G <W tr> • H • H ' d W >i a> rH n -P (d g

<d 0 a) • H <w <w •rH •H G ' d tr»

• H in (A •P cu -p • H 0 G 0 m 0) JQ

3 <d <n A a) O w

• H 0 £ > £

in (A G G <d <d 0) a) SB as

Page 122: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

115

ai

• Oi

£5 • p-GO •

cu cj?

05 C rd a) SB

* -ai •XJ

a

G D m C O L n G D m C D l O C S D co co oa oa

s a j D 3 $ ssauuadi) ueay

(0 c «d 0) se

m <P •

0 in 0) 4-> •o P4 A •H 3 u (0 0

in T* JQ a) 0 > (0 rH 0 M > <d c rH •H •H

s T5 •H c in <d

>1 •

0) ,Q 4J rH C tP 'd a) a a) •H X a) CO a) u

*3 <W u G •H 0 •H n3 <4-1

a) >i CO rH a> (d -P u G 0 -P (d a c 0 0) a) •H

M w a) •H in u a a) u cn c •H •H c 13 0) a) 04 >1 a) o rH b

•P (d T3 c c cd u (d 0 0)

•H <W a <w <w 3 •H •H 0 c

0> -H in CO •P

a) a tn -P •H < 0 U

G 0 m

• 0) JQ 3

td m 0) u £ a> 3 0 w tJ •H 0 *H ,a 43

>

Page 123: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

REFERENCES

Allen, S. (1990, November 6). They're so vain. The Dallas

Morning News, pp. 1, 4C.

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and

statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.).

Washington, DC: Author.

Andersen, A. E., & Didomenico, L. (1992). Diet vs. shape

content of popular male and female magazines: A dose-

response relationship to the incidence of eating

disorders? International Journal of Eating Disorders,

11, 283-287.

Atchley, R. C. (1988). Social Forces and Aging. Belmont,

CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Barnett, L. R. (1986). Bulimarexia as symptom of sex-role

strain in professional women. Psychotherapy. 23., 311-

315. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., &

Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring

depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 4, 561-

571.

Blumenthal, J. A., O'Toole, L. C., & Chang, J. L. (1984).

Is running an analogue of anorexia nervosa? An empirical

study of obligatory running and anorexia nervosa.

116

Page 124: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

117

Journal of the American Medical Association. 252. 520-

523.

Bond, S., & Cash, T. F. (1992). Black beauty: Skin color

and body images among African-American college women.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 22., 874-888.

Bruch, H. (1985). Four decades of eating disorders. In D.

M. Garner & P. E. Garfinkel (Eds.), Handbook of

psychotherapy for anorexia nervosa and bulimia (7-18).

New York: Guilford Press.

Cash, T. F. (1990) . The Multidimensional Bodv-Self

Relations Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript and

software.

Cash, T. F. (1990). Body, self, and society: The

development of body images. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky

(Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change

(pp. 51-79). New York: Guilford Press.

Cash, T. F., & Brown, T. A. (1987). Body image in anorexia

nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A review of the literature.

Behavior Modification. 11. 487-521.

Cash, T. F., & Brown, T. A. (1989). Gender and body images:

Stereo-types and realities. Sex Roles. 21, 361-373.

Cash, T. F., & Pruzinsky, T. (Eds.). (1990). Body images:

Development, deviance, and change. New York: Guilford

Press.

Page 125: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

118

Cash, T. F., Winstead, B. A., & Janda, L. H. (1986, April).

The great American shape-up. Psychology Today, pp. 30-

43.

Cohn, L. D., & Adler, N. E. (1992). Female arid male

perceptions of ideal body shapes: Distorted views among

Caucasian college students. Psychology of Women

Quarterly. 16. 69-79.

Cooper, C., & Lane, T. (1991). Bodybuilders: Male analogue

of anorexia and bulimia nervosa? Unpublished manuscript.

Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G.

(1987). The development and validation of the Body Shape

Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating

Disorders. 6, 485-494.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO

Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1989a). The NEO-PI/NEO-

FFI manual supplement. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources.

Crowne, D., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive. New

York: Wiley.

Cullari, S., & Redmon, W. K. (1983). Bulimarexia, bulimia,

and binge eating: A bibliography. Professional

Psychology: Research and Practice. 14, 480-485.

Deaux, K., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1988). Social psychology.

Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.

Page 126: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

119

Dickstein, L. J. (1989). Current college environments: Do

these communities facilitate and foster bulimia in

vulnerable students? Journal of College Student

Psychotherapy. 3, 107-133.

Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is

beautiful is good. Journal of Personality of Social

Psychology. 24. 285-290.

Dougherty, L. M., & Cook, D. B. (1991). Development of a

geriatric Draw-A-Person scoring system. Paper presented

at the annual meeting of the American Society on Aging,

New Orleans, LA.

Dougherty, L. M., Cook, D. B., & Coppin, A. (1991).

Development of a projective personality assessment

technique for older adults: The geriatric Draw-A-Person.

Paper presented at the meeting of the Gerontological

Society of America, San Francisco, CA.

Drewnowski, A., & Yee, D. K. (1987). Men and body image:

Are males satisfied with their body weight?

Psychosomatic Medicine. 49, 626-634.

Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Kennedy,

L. C. (199"1) . What is beautiful is good, but...: A

meta-analytic review of research on the physical

attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin. 51.

292-304.

Page 127: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

120

Fairburn, C. G., & Garner, D. M. (1986). The diagnosis of

bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating

Disorders. 5, 403-419.

Fairburn, C. G., & Garner, D. M. (1986). Diagnostic

criteria for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: The

importance of attitudes to shape and weight. In D. M.

Garner & P. E. Garfinkel (Eds.), Diagnostic issues in

anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa (pp. 87-102). New

York: Brunner/Mazel.

Fallon, A. (1990). Culture in the mirror: Sociocultural

determinants of body image. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky

(Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change

(pp. 80-109). New York: Guilford Press.

Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in

perceptions of desirable body shape. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. 94. 102-105.

Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975).

Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and

theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.

43, 522-527.

Fisher, S. (1986). Development and structure of the body

image: Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Fisher, S. (1986). Development and structure of the body

image: Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Page 128: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

121

Fuchs, C. Z., & Zaichkowsky, L. D. (1983). Psychological

characteristics of male and female bodybuilders: The

iceberg profile. Journal of Sport Behavior. 6, 136-145.

Garfinkel, P. E., & Garner, D. M. (1982). Anorexia nervosa:

A multidimensional perspective. New York:

Brunner/Mazel.

Garner, D. M., Garfinkel, P. E., & Olmsted, M. (1983). An

overview of sociocultural factors in the development of

anorexia nervosa. In P. L. Darby, P. E. Garfinkel, D. M.

Garner, & D. V. Coscina (Eds.), Anorexia nervosa: Recent

developments (pp. 65-82). New York: Allan R. Liss.

Glassner, B. (1989). Men and muscles. In M. S. Kimmel and

M. A. Messner (Eds.), Men/s lives (pp. 310-320). New

York: MacMillan Publishing Co.

Gordon, C., & Galtz, C. (1976). Leisure and lives: Personal

expressivity across the lifespan. In R. H. Binstock & E.

Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of aging and social sciences (pp.

310-341). New York: Van Nostrand Reinholdt.

Gross, J., & Rosen, J. C. (1988). Bulimia in adolescents:

Prevalence and psychosocial correlates. International

Journal of Eating Disorders. 7, 51-61.

Helms, J.E. (1990). Black and white racial identity.

Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

Herzog, D. B., Newman, K. L., Yeh, C. J., & Warshaw, M.

(1992). Body image satisfaction in homosexual and

Page 129: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

122

heterosexual women. International Journal of Eating

Disorders. 11, 391-396.

Hill, E. F. (1972). The Holtzman Inkblot Technique. San

Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Inc.

Holtzman, W. H. (1961). Holtzman Inkblot Technique:

Administration and scoring guide. New York: The

Psychological Corporation.

Kahili, S. (1984). Human figure drawing in adults: An

update of the empirical evidence, 1967-1982. Canadian

Psychology. 25. 269-292.

Kaluger, G., & Kaluger, M. F. (1984). Human development:

The span of life. St. Louis, Missouri: Times

Mirror/Mosby College Publishing.

Kiemle, G., Slade, P. D., & Dewey, M. E. (1987). Factors

associated with abnormal eating attitudes and behaviors:

Screening individuals at risk of developing an eating

disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 6,

713-724.

Klein, A. M. (1985). Pumping iron. Society. 22, 68-75.

Klein, A. M. (1987). Fear and self-loathing in Southern

California: Narcissism and fascism in bodybuilding

subculture. Journal of Psychoanalytic Anthropology. 10,

117-137.

Lerner, R. M., & Jovanovic, J. (1990). The role of body

image in psychosocial development across the life span:

A developmental contextual perspective. In T. F. Cash &

Page 130: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

123

T. Pruzinsky (Eds.)/ Body images: Development, deviance,

and chance (pp. 110-130). New York: Guilford Press.

Lerner, R. M., & Korn, S. J. (1972). The development of

body build stereotypes in males. Child Development. 43.

912-920.

Levinson, D. J. (1977). The midlife transition: A period

in adult psychosocial development. Psychiatry. 40. 99-

112.

Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing

of the human figure. Springfield: Charles c. Thomas.

McCrae, R. R. (1991). The five-factor model and its

assessment in clinical settings. Journal of

Personality Assessment. 57. 399-414.

McDonald, K., & Thompson, J. K. (1992). Eating disturbance,

body image dissatisfaction, and reasons for exercising:

Gender differences and correlational findings.

International Journal of Eating Disorders. 11, 289-292.

Mishkind, M. E., Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Striegel-

Moore, R. H. (1986). The embodiment of masculinity.

American Behavioral Scientist. 29, 545-562.

Myers, P. N., & Biocca, F. A. (1992). The elastic body

image: The effect of television advertising and

programming on body image distortions in young women.

Journal of Communication. 42, 108-133.

Nudelman, S., Rosen, J. C., & Leitenberg, H. (1988).

Dissimilarities in eating attitudes, body image

Page 131: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

124

distortion, depression, and self-esteem between high-

intensity male runners and women with bulimia nervosa.

International Journal of Eating Disorders. 7, 625-634.

Oster, G. D., & Gould, P. (1987). Using drawings in

assessment and therapy: A guide for mental health

professionals. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Padin, M. A., Lerner, R. M., & Spiro, A., Ill (1981). The

role of physical education interventions in the stability

of body attitudes and self-esteem in late adolescents.

Adolescence. 16. 371-384.

Phillips, K. A. (1991). Body dysmorphic disorder: The

distress of imagined ugliness. American Journal of

Psychiatry. 148. 1138-1149.

Post, G., & Crowther, J. H. (1987). Restricter-purger

differences in bulimic adolescent females. International

Journal of Eating Disorders. 6, 757-761.

Raphael, F. J., & Lacey, J. H. (1992). Sociocultural

aspects of eating disorders. Annals of Medicine. 24.

293-296.

Riccio, N. (1989, December 29-31). The ache age. USA

Weekend, pp. 4-5.

Rodin, J. (1992, January/February). Body Mania. Psychology

Todayf pp. 56-60.

Rodin, J., & Langer, E. J. (1980). Aging labels: The

decline of control and the fall of self-esteem. Journal

of Social Issues. 36, 12-29.

Page 132: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

125

Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985).

Women and weight: A normative discontent. In T. B.

Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on motivation,

1984: Psychology and gender. Lincoln: University of

Nebraska Press.

Rosen, J. C. (1990). Body-image disturbance in eating

disorders. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body

images: Development, deviance, and change (pp. 190-216).

New York: Guilford Press.

Ross, M. J., Tait, R. C., Grossberg, G. T., Handal, P. J.,

Brandeberry, L., & Nakra, R. (1989). Age differences in

body consciousness. Journal of Gerontology. 44., 23—24.

Ruderman, A. J., & Grace, P. S. (1988). Bulimics and

restrained eaters: A personality comparison. Addictive

Behaviors, 13., 359-367.

Saxton, L. (1990). The individual, marriage, and the family

(7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Co.

Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The Self-

Consciousness Scale: A revised version for use with

general populations. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology. 15, 687-699.

Schwartz, H. (1986). Never satisfied: A cultural history

of diets, fantasies, and fat. New York: Free Press.

Seid, R. P. (1989). Never too thin: Whv women are at war

with their bodies. New York: Prentice Hall Press.

Page 133: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

126

Sharkey, B. (1993, March). You've come a long way, Madison

Avenue. Lear's, pp. 92-101.

Snyder, D. K. (1981). Manual for the Marital Satisfaction

Inventory. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Sroufe, L. A., Cooper, R. G., & DeHart, G. B. (1992). Child

development: Its nature and course (2nd ed.). New York:

McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Striegel-Moore, R. H., McAvay, G., & Rodin, J. (1986).

Psychological and behavioral correlates of feeling fat in

women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 5,

935-947.

Sue, D. W., Sc Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the Culturally

Different. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Thompson, J. K. (1990). Body image disturbance: Assessment

and treatment. Elmsford, New York: Pergamon Press.

Tucker, L. A. (1982). Relationship between perceived

somatotype and body cathexis of college males.

Psychological Reports. 50, 983-989.

Van-Deusen, J., Harlowe, D., & Baker, L. (1989). Body image

perceptions of the community-based elderly. Occupational

Therapy Journal of Research. £, 243-248.

Wiedel, T. C., & Dodd, J. M. (1983). The relationship

between dietary restraint, personality measures and

weight in college students. Journal of Obesity and

Weight Regulation. 2, 88-96.

Page 134: 37? N8/4 Ao. 38

127

Wilson, G. T., & Smith, D. (1989). Assessment of bulimia

nervosa: An evaluation of the Eating Disorders

Examination. International Journal of Eating Disorders.

&, 173-179.

Wiseman, C. v., Gray, J. J., Mosimann, J. E., & Ahrens, A.

(1992). Cultural expectations of thinness in women: An

update. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 11.

85-89.

Wolf, E. M., & Crowther, J. H. (1983). Personality and

eating habit variables as predictors of severity of binge

eating and weight. Addictive Behaviors. 8, 335-344.

Wooley, S., & Wooley, 0. W. (1986, October). Thinness

Mania. American Health, pp. 68-74.

Yates, A. (1991) . Compulsive exercise and the eating

disorders. New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers.

Yates, A., Leehey, K., & Shisslak, C. M. (1983). Running -

an analogue of anorexia? New England Journal of

Medicine. 308. 251-255.

Zakin, D. F. (1989) . Eating disturbance, emotional

separation, and body image. International Journal of

Eating Disorders. 8, 411-416.