Top Banner
Beyond STS: A Research-Based Framework for Socioscientific Issues Education DANA L. ZEI DLE R  Department of Secondary Education, College of Education, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33620-5650, USA TROY D. SADLER School of Teaching & Learning, College of Education, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611-7048, USA MICHAEL L. SIMMONS, ELAINE V . HOWES  Department of Secondary Education, College of Education, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33620-5650, USA  Received 17 March 2004; revised 28 July 2004; accepted 14 August 2004  DOI 10 .1002/sce.20048 Published online 23 March 2005 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). ABSTRACT: An important distinction can be made between the science, technology, and society (STS) movement of past years and the domain of socioscientic issues (SSI). STS education as typically practiced does not seem embedded in a coherent developmental or sociological framework that explicitly considers the psychological and epistemological gro wthof the chi ld, northe de ve lop ment of cha rac ter or vir tue . In con tra st, theSSI movemen t foc use s on emp oweri ng stu den ts to con sider how science-based iss ues reect, in par t, mor al principles and elements of virtue that encompass their own lives, as well as the physical and social world around them. The focus of this paper is to describe a research-based framework of current research and practice that identies factors associated with reasoning about socioscientic issues and provide a working model that illustrates theoretical and conceptual links among key psychological, sociological, and developmental factors central to SSI and science education.  C 2005 Wile y Periodicals, Inc.  Sci Ed  89:357–377, 2005 INTRODUCTION As the 21st century unfolds, professional associations (e.g., American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1989, 1993; National Science Education Standards, 1996; CMEC’s Pan-Canadian Science Project, 1997; Queensland School Curriculum Council, 2001) in science recognize the importance of broadly conceptualizing scientic literacy Corres pondence to: Dana L. Zeidler; e-mail: [email protected] C  2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
22

10-Zeidler Et Al. 2005 Beyond STS

Oct 10, 2015

Download

Documents

paulaoquadros
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • Beyond STS: A Research-BasedFramework for SocioscientificIssues Education

    DANA L. ZEIDLERDepartment of Secondary Education, College of Education, University of South Florida,Tampa, FL 33620-5650, USA

    TROY D. SADLERSchool of Teaching & Learning, College of Education, University of Florida, Gainesville,FL 32611-7048, USA

    MICHAEL L. SIMMONS, ELAINE V. HOWESDepartment of Secondary Education, College of Education, University of South Florida,Tampa, FL 33620-5650, USA

    Received 17 March 2004; revised 28 July 2004; accepted 14 August 2004

    DOI 10.1002/sce.20048Published online 23 March 2005 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).

    ABSTRACT: An important distinction can be made between the science, technology, andsociety (STS) movement of past years and the domain of socioscientific issues (SSI). STSeducation as typically practiced does not seem embedded in a coherent developmentalor sociological framework that explicitly considers the psychological and epistemologicalgrowth of the child, nor the development of character or virtue. In contrast, the SSI movementfocuses on empowering students to consider how science-based issues reflect, in part, moralprinciples and elements of virtue that encompass their own lives, as well as the physicaland social world around them. The focus of this paper is to describe a research-basedframework of current research and practice that identifies factors associated with reasoningabout socioscientific issues and provide a working model that illustrates theoretical andconceptual links among key psychological, sociological, and developmental factors centralto SSI and science education. C 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Sci Ed 89:357377, 2005

    INTRODUCTIONAs the 21st century unfolds, professional associations (e.g., American Association for

    the Advancement of Science, 1989, 1993; National Science Education Standards, 1996;CMECs Pan-Canadian Science Project, 1997; Queensland School Curriculum Council,2001) in science recognize the importance of broadly conceptualizing scientific literacy

    Correspondence to: Dana L. Zeidler; e-mail: [email protected]

    C 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

  • 358 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    to include informed decision making; the ability to analyze, synthesize, and evaluate in-formation; dealing sensibly with moral reasoning and ethical issues; and understandingconnections inherent among socioscientific issues (SSI) (Zeidler, 2001). Achieving a prac-tical degree of scientific literacy necessarily entails practice and experience in developinghabits of mind (i.e., acquiring skepticism, maintaining open-mindedness, evoking criticalthinking, recognizing multiple forms of inquiry, accepting ambiguity, searching for data-driven knowledge). Habits of mind may suffice when arriving at individual decisions basedon an informed analysis of available information; however, they may not be sufficient in aworld where collective decision making is evoked through the joint construction of socialknowledge. In the real world of dirty sinks and messy reasoning, arriving at ideal personaldecisions through objective evaluation of neutral evidence is a phantom image.

    It is clear from recent international research in science education that current reform ini-tiatives in our field demand increased emphasis on the nature of science (NOS) and scientificinquiry, as well as development of broad conceptual frameworks encompassing progressivevisions of scientific literacy that entail a commitment to the moral and ethical dimensionsof science educationincluding the social and character development of children (Zeidler& Keefer, 2003). In particular, students are expected to develop an understanding of theepistemology of scientific knowledge as well as the processes/methods used to develop suchknowledge. In addition to other considerations, it is believed that students understanding ofscience as a way of knowing is absolutely necessary if informed decisions are to be maderegarding the scientifically based personal and societal issues that increasingly confrontour students. Such decisions necessarily involve careful evaluation of scientific claims bydiscerning connections among evidence, inferences, and conclusions. Students capable ofsuch decisions display a functional degree of scientific literacy.

    The focus of this paper is to provide a synopsis of current research and practice thatidentifies factors associated with reasoning about SSI and distinguishes it from sciencetechnologysociety (STS) education. While the study of SSI is conceptually related topast research on STS education, it is important to point out that they represent uniqueapproaches. STS education, as typically envisioned and practiced, does not seem to beembedded in a coherent developmental or sociological framework that explicitly considersthe psychological and epistemological growth of the child, nor the development of characteror virtue. The lack of a theoretical framework with respect to STS materials has been notedby others (Hodson, 2003; Jenkins, 2002; Shamos, 1995), suggesting that STS may be anunderdeveloped idea in search of a theory. Because we suggest substantial reconsiderationof the use of the STS approach, it is important to consider limitations identified with STSeducation; therefore, we do so in the following section.

    PROBLEMS WITH STS EDUCATIONBy the late 1970s, many science education researchers became focused on developing a

    theme of study that reflected the combined influences of science, technology, and society.It was agreed that science would become more meaningful to students when placed in thecontext of how it affects technology and how technology, in turn, directs society. In science,technology, and society (STS) education, science teachers use curricula that engage studentsdue to their social dimensions. Aikenhead (1994) summarized STS teaching as follows:

    STS science teaching conveys the image of socially constructed knowledge. Its student-oriented approach . . . emphasizes the basic facts, skills, and concepts of traditional sci-ence . . . but does so by integrating that science content into social and technological contextsmeaningful to students. (p. 59)

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 359

    In 1982, the National Science Teachers Association (NSTA) published a position paperdescribing characteristics of a scientifically literate person as one who would understandand be knowledgeable of the connections and interdependency of science, technology, andsociety (NSTA, 1982). This shift in emphasis was solidified with the publication of theNSTA 1985 Yearbook, which was devoted to STS teaching (Bybee, 1985). Unfortunately,STS education has become relatively diffuse over the course of its tenure, consisting ofapproaches as disparate as isolated courses addressing STS issues or ancillary text boxesin science textbooks (Pedretti & Hodson, 1995). Shamos (1995) noted, quite correctly,that the problem with STS curriculum is that many of the issues under study (e.g., nuclearpower, global warming) are not particularly exciting or relevant to students because they areremoved from their everyday personal experiences. While STS education typically stressesthe impact of decisions in science and technology on society, it does not mandate explicitattention to the ethical issues contained within choices about means and ends, nor does itconsider the moral or character development of students.

    Recently, some science educators have advocated a more issues-driven STS curricu-lum in the form of sciencetechnologysocietyenvironment education (STSE) (Hodson,1994, 2003; Pedretti, 1997). It has become clear, however, that while STSE represents animprovement over STS strategies, it does not directly address the personal and individualmoral and ethical development of students and it is fair to say that most science educatorsdo not see the subtle distinctions between them. Traditional STS(E) education (or perhapsSTS(E) education as currently practiced by and large) only points out ethical dilemmas orcontroversies, but does not necessarily exploit the inherent pedagogical power of discourse,reasoned argumentation, explicit NOS considerations, emotive, developmental, cultural orepistemological connections within the issues themselves. Hence, STS(E) approaches havebecome somewhat marginalized in the curriculum and in practice. What was once describedas a megatrend in science education (Roy, 1984) has been relegated to brief mentions incurrent school science textbooks as well as in science teacher preparation texts (see, e.g.,Chiapetta & Koballa, 2002; Trowbridge, Bybee, & Powell, 2000). This decline in emphasisin STS(E) education is part of a striking paradox, for the commitment to the inextricableconnections between science, technology, society and the environment remains a majortheme in current science reform documents such as the United States National ScienceEducation Standards and Project 2061s Benchmarks for Science Literacy.

    One likely reason for the recent decline in interest in STS(E) may be a lack of focus orwell-developed unifying theoretical basis. Even staunch supporters of STS have acknowl-edged the absence of a coherent and cohesive framework for STS. Consider the followingstatements regarding STS:

    In this essay I have suggested that science, technology, society (STS) consists of severalseemingly competing, if not conflicting perspectives because they relate to different notionsof power, policy, and method. Nevertheless, the perspectives can be combined. Combiningthe perspectives does not mean however that we create a unitary approach of STS. WhatI intend is rather a pluralistic and open approach. To open the doors among the differentperspectives is a major challenge for STS which may also require a thorough deliberationof the different related policy interests. (Fuglsang, 2001, p. 46)

    Ziman, another ardent proponent of STS education, writes, The fundamental purposes ofSTS education are genuinely and properly diverse and incoherent (1994, p. 22). Althoughthese candid admissions of the considerable difficulties in both describing the purposesof STS and melding the myriad STS factions could be interpreted as assertions merely ac-knowledging the complexity of STS, we suggest that the reported pluralism and incoherence

  • 360 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    support the claim that STS is incomplete or underdeveloped as a pedagogical strategy.Instead, STS is a context for a curriculum. (Yager, 1996, p. 13)

    Hughes (2000) has provided a post-modern analysis of reform curriculum and identifiedseveral reasons why three decades of STS education is out of touch with students, leavingthem ill prepared to deal with scientific and technological controversy. Her central argumentis that STS marginalizes socioscientific material and reinforces gender gaps because of howscience is embedded in a masculine hard science perspective at the exclusion of softersocioscience orientations that allow for contextualized examination of issues and valuesimplicit in scientific development.

    . . . when socioscience is the icing on the cake, not an essential basic ingredient, part of agood-quality product but not fundamental to teaching science, dominant discourses of sci-ence as an abstract body of knowledge are not destabilized and implicit gender hierarchicalbinaries are readily reinforced. (Hughes, 2000, p. 347)

    Similarly, Bingle and Gaskell (1994) have further noted that much of STS education, aspracticed, is most closely aligned with Latours (1987) notion of ready-made science thatcarries with it the connotation of positivist knowledge claims at the expense of constitutivevalues that stress science-in-the-making and suggests a social constructivist view of con-textual values for evaluating scientific knowledge claims. These authors stress that whereSSI arise, it is legitimate for individual citizens to acknowledge and evaluate contextualfactors deemed meaningful with respect to the scientific claims under consideration: A so-cial constructivist view of science . . . challenges the scientists position of privilege becauseindividual citizens have just as much access to the standards of evaluating the impact of thesocial context as do scientists themselves, a prospect that would probably be unsettling tomost scientists (Bingle & Gaskell, 1994, p. 198).

    Whereas the overarching purpose of the STS approach is to increase student interestin science by placing science content learning in a societal context, SSI education aimsto stimulate and promote individual intellectual development in morality and ethics aswell as awareness of the interdependence between science and society. SSI therefore doesnot simply serve as a context for learning science, but rather as a pedagogical strategywith clearly defined goals. Certainly, knowledge and understanding of the interconnec-tions among science, technology, society, and the environment are major components ofdeveloping scientific literacy; however, these interconnections do not exist independentlyof students personal beliefs. It is our stance that STS(E) approaches can be remodeled andsubstantially improved by adding an essential missing componentconsideration of eachstudents own moral and ethical development.

    BEYOND STS: PRESUPPOSITIONS OF THE SSI DOMAINWhile STS has been defined as a context for science education (Yager, 1996), the current

    usage of socioscientific issues refers to a distinctly more developed pedagogical strategy.In contrast to STS, the SSI movement focuses specifically on empowering students toconsider how science-based issues and the decisions made concerning them reflect, inpart, the moral principles and qualities of virtue that encompass their own lives, as wellas the physical and social world around them (Driver et al., 1996; Driver, Newton, &Osborne, 2000; Kolst, 2001a; Sadler, 2004). Accordingly, SSI education is equated withthe consideration of ethical issues and construction of moral judgments about scientifictopics via social interaction and discourse. As Zeidler et al. (2002) point out, Socioscientificissues then, is a broader term that subsumes all that STS has to offer, while also considering

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 361

    the ethical dimensions of science, the moral reasoning of the child, and the emotionaldevelopment of the student (p. 344). Further, recent research (Zeidler & Keefer, 2003) inthe area of SSI has provided theoretical and conceptual links among key psychological,sociological, and developmental factors associated with SSI education. We envision SSIin a manner that considers how controversial scientific issues and dilemmas affect theintellectual growth of individuals in both personal and societal domains.

    In order to advance the claim that science educators should attend to SSI related to cul-tivating the morality of our students to achieve a functional view of scientific literacy,a coherent conceptual framework must be developed that is flexible enough to allow formultiple perspectives while enabling educators and curriculum specialists to better under-stand the moral growth of the child. One framework recently proposed because of its utilityin addressing socioscientific discourse in terms of the psychological, social, and emotivegrowth of the child is derived from a cognitive-moral reasoning perspective (Zeidler &Keefer, 2003). This initial model served as an impetus for our article by identifying poten-tial lines of research that might prove promising in the development of an SSI framework.We have now further extended and refined that model with new research from within thescience education community and related research external to science education. It consistsof themes that collectively attend to many of the factors inherently limited by or missingfrom STS education. This framework should be viewed as a tentative conceptual model thatidentifies four areas of pedagogical importance central to the teaching of SSI: (1) nature ofscience issues, (2) classroom discourse issues, (3) cultural issues, and (4) case-based issues.These issues can be thought of as entry points in the science curriculum that can contributeto a students personal intellectual development and in turn, help to inform pedagogy inscience education to promote functional scientific literacy (see Figure 1).

    Figure 1. Socioscientific elements of functional scientific literacy.

  • 362 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    It should be noted that while our view of cognitive and moral development is concep-tualized, in part, from a neo-Kohlbergian perspective, it does not preclude (and in fact,invites) the use of post-modern perspectives of developmental reasoning (e.g., see Hughes,2000). While these issues certainly cannot comprise all the sufficient conditions of scientificliteracy (functional or otherwise), we suggest that these are critical and necessary issuesfor science educators to grapple with in order to promote functional scientific literacy. Forexample, NOS issues become important because they reveal how varied epistemologicalviews influence the way in which students select and evaluate evidence, and are consideredto have bearing on their pre-instructional views of SSI. Discourse issues direct our attentionto how students construct arguments and utilize fallacious reasoning, and compel us toconsider how prior belief convictions help frame emotional responses, principled commit-ments, or stances on moral issues. Cultural issues remind us that discourse is futile withoutmutual respect and tolerance of dissenting views, while underscoring that the decisions stu-dents make are the result of realizing that as moral agents, they are impacted by normativevalues as well as cultural beliefs about the natural world. Case-based issues enable scienceeducators to move beyond STS curriculum and cultivate habits of mind that promote ethicalawareness and commitment to issue resolution and the moral sensitivity to hear dissentingvoices by examining how power and authority are embedded in scientific enterprises.

    Other authors use the term functional when speaking of scientific literacy (Jenkins,1990, 1997; Ryder, 2001; Shamos, 1995); however, it is important to note that they tendto do so from what can be described as a technocratic perspective. For example, Ryder(2001) presents an analysis of some 31 studies ranging in degree of technocratic decisionmaking on controversial topics (in that there is a lack of clear consensus within the scientificcommunity about data related to these topics). These topics range from risk assessment ingenetic counseling situations to managing methane from a waste disposal site. The authorsdo acknowledge that the studies selected for review are not necessarily representative ofthe many contexts of science, and the examples tend to focus on arguments based on utilityin a democratic and technologically sophisticated society. While it cannot be denied thatthese aspects of science are important because of their inherent connections to NOS (e.g.,understanding the role of models in science, assessing the validity and quality of data, anduncertainty in science), we believe that this view is too narrow with regard to promotingfunctional scientific literacy in that it pays scant attention to the role of personal epistemo-logical and intellectual development in the context of varied cultural settings. For Shamos(1995), functional scientific literacy seems to be relegated to those who are the scienceelite having the expert content knowledge to fully appreciate the scientific and technologi-cal intricacies of issues (thereby achieving true scientific literacy) and have more than ageneral sense of Hirschs (1987) notion of cultural literacy (i.e., general scientific termsfamiliar to citizens in western society). An individual who possesses functional scientificliteracy for Shamos, therefore, is one who has command of a science lexicon, [and] alsobe able to converse, read, and write coherently, using such science terms in perhaps a non-technical but nevertheless meaningful context (1995, p. 88). In a comprehensive review ofthe literature, Laugksch (2000) points out that such conceptualizations of functional scien-tific literacy are embedded in a meaning of literate that require[s] the scientifically literateindividual to use science in performing a function (italics added) in society (p. 84). Again,our view of functional scientific literacy affirms what the views above do not; that any viewof functional scientific literacy falls short of the mark if it ignores the fundamental factorsaimed at promoting the personal cognitive and moral development of students.

    Although Kohlberg (1986) provided educators and researchers interested in the area ofmoral reasoning and development with a rich conceptual basis to raise important questionsabout the nature of moral education, new questions have emerged about the adequacy of

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 363

    the assumption that all one has to do to bring about changes in moral behavior is to inducechanges in moral stages or structures. Furthermore, new questions have also emerged aboutthe distinction between reasoning about formal societal constructs (e.g., laws, duty, socialinstitutions) and engaging in the resolution of differences among individuals via argumen-tation and discussion during face-to face interactions. The former type of reasoning dealswith what Rest et al. (1999) term macromorality, while the latter deals with issues ofmicromorality. The difference can be likened to examining societal conventions from atheoretical perspective (e.g., principles of justice, meta-ethics) and understanding a partic-ular praxis of social constructs (e.g., face-to-face negotiations, normative ethics). Once thisdistinction is made, it exposes a more robust conceptualization of the complex relationshipthat exists between moral reasoning and action and has implications for decisions related topedagogy. For example, researchers point out that the role of affect and emotions in moralfunctioning had been overlooked in past research, and that the particular realm of oneslife being considered (e.g., family, school, peers, workplace, intimate relationships) plays anormative role in moral decision making and character formation (Berkowitz, 1997, 1998;Nucci, 1989, 2001; Sadler & Zeidler, 2004; Turiel, 1998; Zeidler & Schafer, 1984, Zeidleret al., 2002).

    THEMATIC AREAS OF RECENT RESEARCH CONNECTED TO SSIAn overview of the four pedagogical issues (nature of science, classroom discourse,

    cultural, and case based) identified above is presented in order to synthesize current linesof research relevant to the exploration of SSI in science education and further articulate aresearch-based model of issues central to moral education in the context of science educa-tion. The purpose is to provide educators and researchers with a thematic understanding ofhow these areas are at once fundamental and interdependent, and when linked through theexploration of the domains of SSI, address morality.

    (1) Nature of Science Issues reveal the emphasis placed on students epistemologicalbeliefs as they pertain to decisions regarding SSI (e.g., Bell, 2004; Bell, Lederman, & Abd-El-Khalick, 2000). Epistemological orientations regarding the nature of science influencehow students attend to evidence in support of, or in conflict with, their pre-instructionalbelief systems regarding social issues. In this context, moral reasoning proper is understoodto be the result of the opportunity for learners to make meaning using empirical and socialcriteria in both formal and informal educational contexts through rational discourse. Abd-El-Khalicks (2001) and Bells (2003) research has suggested that students decisions regardingSSI are analogous to decisions engaged by scientists regarding the justification of scientificknowledge in that both processes require the use of rational discourse and invoke valuejudgments and common sense. These findings highlight the importance of tapping studentsepistemological orientations (including NOS views) in the process of evaluating scientificdata regarding social issues.

    Likewise, Zeidler et al. (2002) have shown that students who harbor nave and relativisticconceptions of science will likely dismiss scientific knowledge as irrelevant to decisionmaking when reasoning about SSI because they tend to distort whatever data, evidence,or knowledge claims are available to them for the purpose of supporting a predeterminedviewpoint with respect to the issue under consideration. Related research informing theissues of socioscientific reasoning and NOS confirms student reliance on personal relevanceover evaluative decisions based on contemplation of presented evidence (Sadler, Chambers,& Zeidler, 2004). In this study, students rated articles according to which had more scientificmerit, but in determining which articles they found to be most convincing, many (40%)

  • 364 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    selected articles that most complemented their own personal beliefs independently of theirscientific merit. This pattern of responses suggested that for some students, scientific merit(e.g., evidence, data) and persuasiveness were not synonymous. In order to fully appreciatethe empirical nature of science, students must understand what constitutes data and howit can be utilized in the process of decision making. However, if that student is confusedby what data is, then assertions or arguments evoked hold little meaning. In analyzingstudents comments about how data were used to support positions on global warming,Sadler, Chambers, and Zeidler (2004) also revealed that nearly half (47%) of the studentslacked adequate conceptions of scientific data. Some of the students comprising this groupwere able to recognize data without the ability to describe its use or significance, whereasothers could not even distinguish among data, unfounded opinions, and predictions.

    Bell and Lederman (2003) have examined the reasoning patterns of university profes-sors representing various fields (including science educators, science philosophers, andresearch scientists) on SSI and found similar reasoning patterns among these groups, in-cluding emphases on personal philosophy and commitments over reasoning based on sci-entific evidence. Although the participants of this study held varied views of the NOS, theirdecision-making strategies and actual decisions on science and technology-based issuesyielded no discernable patterns unique to particular NOS views. While all of these individ-uals showed some degree of superficial evidence-based reasoning, the primary influenceguiding their decisions were personal values, factors related to morality or ethics, and socialconsiderations. The authors suggested in very clear terms that moral development is a factorof interest when assessing decision-making strategies on SSI. Their research findings alsoprovided supporting evidence for earlier work that revealed explicit links between collegestudents levels of moral reasoning and decision making on SSI irrespective of their sciencecontent knowledge level of sophistication (Zeidler & Schafer, 1984).

    Similarly, Walker and Zeidler (2003) found that students reference to empirical evidencesupporting various positions during a debate activity in high school was limited followingonline exploration of SSI with previous instruction regarding NOS and exposure to multiplearticles of evidence used to gather support and frame arguments for their debate position.Yet when other members of the class voted as to which group presented the best argument,the majority (75%) of the students chose the group as being the most convincing that utilizeda wide base of background information, had the input of different people, quoted statisticsand generally were convinced by their arguments even when presented with a positioncontrary to their own. The findings underscore the need for explicit instruction in NOS, sothat careful evaluations of evidence regarding SSI and subsequent decisions can be utilized.

    This line of research has also revealed more direct transfer of NOS considerations whenreasoning about SSI during informal debate or discourse particularly when the SSI issuescentered around genetically modified foods and global warming (Sadler, Chambers, &Zeidler, 2004; Walker and Zeidler, 2003; Zeidler et al. 2002), suggesting that the degree ofpersonal relevance of the issue is associated with increased validation of knowledge claims.It reasonably follows that the degree to which students perceive personal relevance relatedto scientific topics will determine, in part, the seriousness of the issues at-hand and themerit of conflicting or competing claims. If a goal of teaching NOS in science classroomsis to develop students abilities to critically evaluate competing scientific claims, then weshould be guiding them in the process of synthesizing and applying their understanding ofthe nature of science as they evaluate and make decisions regarding socioscientific issues.The significance of this lies not so much in that future generations of students may beable to articulate the meaning of the nature of science and describe its relevant attributes(although that would be a pedagogically notable benchmark), but rather that NOS under-standing can benefit them in evaluating the efficacy many kinds of claimsscientific or

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 365

    otherwisebased upon the merit of supporting evidence in everyday life. This goal of de-veloping transferable reasoning skills is one that is central to promoting the use of SSI inscience curricula.

    (2) Classroom Discourse Issues stress the crucial role discourse plays in peer interac-tions and its impact on reasoning. This research underscores the importance of developingstudents views about science through argumentation in the constructions of shared socialknowledge via discourse about SSI (e.g., Zeidler et al., 2003). While many science educatorsacknowledge the importance of rich and diverse classroom discussions in the promotionof scientific literacy (Aikenhead, 1985, 2000; Driver, Newton, & Osborne, 2000; Vellom,1999; Zeidler, 1984, 1997; Zeidler, Lederman, & Taylor, 1992), those who seek to studyit have difficulty locating substantive argumentation or classroom discussions in school(Newton, Driver, & Osborne,1999), or find the quantity and quality of discussion with ex-plicit focus on science content very low (Levinson, 2003). Perhaps this is because teachersfind it difficult to implement sustained student discourse with confidence because of thecomplex nature of argumentation. This is precisely what Levinson (2003) found in his in-tensive case study of one Science for Public Understanding college class whose focuswas on exploring controversial science issues (i.e., SSI). Despite the fact that the intent ofthe course was to allow students to develop and express an informed personal viewpointon SSI, the teachers who cotaught the class dominated much of the classroom discourse.Levinson further suggested that because of their inherent complexity, attending to moraland ethical issues may be an unrealistic expectation for science teachers without some typeof support from other teachers representing interdisciplinary studies and/or professionaldevelopment to aid in facilitating the dynamics of argumentation and discourse.

    Settelmaier (2003) focused on high school science students exchanges using a dilemmaapproach. While the results revealed that dilemma-based stories were a viable tool forintroducing SSI which challenged students rational, social, and emotional skills, as well asgrounding the practice of critical self-reflection concerning their personal value and beliefssystems, several problematic factors in using SSI in the classroom were identified includinglogistical and planning problems of integrating coverage of content with moral dilemmasand matching the appropriateness of the dilemmas with student interests. Students caneasily go off-task if the topic is not well focused, extends too long, or the outcomes are notclear. It may be the case that students simply do not have occasion to practice toleratingambiguitywhether it be in the form of anomalous data or conflicting points of view.

    Additional considerations entailing fallacious argumentation have been identified andexplored (i.e., validity concerns, nave conceptions of argument structure, effects of corebeliefs on argumentation, inadequate sampling of evidence altering representation of argu-ment and evidence) and serve as a reminder of the complex nature of discourse involvingSSI (Zeidler, 1997; Zeidler et al., 2003). However, the value of argument in the developmentof moral reasoning has been amply demonstrated in the research literature (Berkowitz &Oser, 1985; Berkowitz, Oser, & Althof, 1987; Keefer, 2002; Keefer, Zeitz, & Resnick, 2000)in terms of creating dissonance thereby allowing opportunity for re-examining ones beliefsand thought-processes. Being exposed to and challenged by the arguments of others pro-vides the opportunity to attend to the quality of claims, warrants, evidence, and assumptionsamong competing positions. The idea of creating dissonance through the use of anomalousdata has broad meaning in the context of SSI which not only supports the use of conflictingempirical data, but further taps into potentially conflicting counter positions and argumentsthat arise through dialogic interaction between or among discussants in which logicallysupported position statements may conflict with a persons pre-existing beliefs regarding agiven scientific concept or issue.

  • 366 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    Driver et al. (1996) have demonstrated how students can handle conflicting evidencerelated to SSI. They indicated that through carefully planned science instruction, studentsdraw on past experiences and combine them with new ideas to explain decisions in scien-tific contexts. Dialogic interaction, also referred to as transactive discussions in the moraleducation research literature, has been shown to improve science learning and lead to morerobust ways to conceptualize the physical world: Whereas transaction can foster studentslogical development by focusing on scientific problems and issues, teachers can fosterthe development of social and moral reasoning by focusing on ethical and social issues(Berkowitz & Simmons, 2003, p. 133). It is important to note that transactional discourseoccurs when one student can clearly internalize and articulate the thoughts, arguments, orposition of another student. Their reasoning then becomes transformational in the sensethat one individuals reasoning becomes integrated with that of another. Berkowitz andSimmons reported that students who use more transactive discussions during classroomdiscourse also learn to solve scientific and mathematical problems more effectively, conse-quently enhancing the development of scientific reasoning and problem-solving strategies.

    In a comprehensive review of the literature, Sadler (2004) summarizes trends related toargumentation as a means of expressing informal reasoning and reiterates that the personalexperiences of decision-makers emerged as a consistent normative influence on informalreasoning related to SSI. More specifically, informal reasoning was found to either mediatescientific knowledge or prevent the consideration of scientific knowledge. Sadler cites re-search that suggests conceptual understanding of content improved informal reasoning onSSI, but more work in this area is needed. Kuhn (1993) further points out that SSI will nec-essarily entail the use of informal reasoning inasmuch as they are complex, open ended, andoften consist of contentious problems without predetermined solutions. Informal reasoningis compatible with the kinds of dilemmas that face students confronted with real-world is-sues in that the issues at-hand are more times than not, dynamic (i.e., premises may changeas new information and perspectives arise).

    It would seem, then, that opportunity to engage in informal reasoning through argu-mentation allows for the evaluation of evidence as well as thought, but finding appropriatepedagogical strategies to seamlessly integrate such dynamic social interaction in the scienceclassroom remains a high priority. Teaching science in this context includes attention andsensitivity to students moral commitments, emotions, and moral behavior. The develop-ment of character in children (as seen via the development of moral reasoning) becomes anadditional important pedagogical outcome arising from the intrinsic nature of argumenta-tion as pedagogy.

    (3) Cultural Issues highlight pluralistic and sociological (subsuming gender) aspects ofscience classrooms. By explicitly attending to cultural issues, science educators recog-nize, acknowledge, and maximize opportunities afforded by diverse assemblies of learn-ers. The diversity of modern science classrooms includes learners from various cultures(e.g., Cobern & Loving, 2000; Lemke, 2001), developmental abilities (e.g., Mastropieri &Scruggs, 1992; McGinnis, 2000), and genders (e.g., Brickhouse, 2001; Tsai, 2000). Thiscultural/sociological perspective toward education underscores the necessity to appreciatestudents as moral agents intimately involved with their own cultural, natural, and techno-logical environments. Regarding individual students as moral agents interacting with theirclassroom contexts and experiences emphasizes the moral nature of education in generaland teaching in particular. The essence of good teaching, in this view, must include theethical and moral development of young people (Clark, 2003; Loving, Lowry, & Martin,2003). In fact, ethical and cognitive growth appear to be tightly linked in the developmentof intercultural understanding (Endicott, Bock, & Narvaez, 2002).

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 367

    Cultural issues are particularly significant for educational experiences related to SSI.Whereas moral theorists adopting cognitive-developmental perspectives on morality (e.g.,Kohlberg, 1986; Rest et al., 1999) have assumed the primacy of moral principles, critical the-orists who adopt cultural perspectives on morality (e.g., Belenky et al., 1986; Gilligan, 1987;Noddings, 1984; Partington, 1997; Snarey, 1985) have expanded the scope and challengedthe construction of moral discourse in order to include care, emotion, and contextual fac-tors. The contributions of multicultural (Boyes & Walker, 1988; Snarey, 1985) and feminist(Donenberg & Hoffman, 1988; Hekman; 1995; Gilligan, 1987; Noddings, 1984) researchon morality inspired some science education researchers to look beyond isolated cognitivefactors (viz. situated cognition) contributing to socioscientific decision making in order toperceive the significance of affect.

    Zeidler and Schafer (1984) produced some of the first empirical evidence suggest-ing the importance of emotions in the resolution of SSI. Although a priori notions ofmorality drove their research design consistent with a Kohlbergian perspective, qualita-tive analyses of dyadic interviews led the authors to postulate the significance of emo-tional reactions to controversial environmental issues. Sadler and Zeidler (2004; in press)extended the investigation on the role of emotions in informal reasoning regarding SSI.Both studies focused on student reactions to, perceptions of, argumentation toward, andresolutions of genetic engineering issues. These findings confirmed the significance ofemotion for the resolution of SSI. More specifically, participants actively displayed asense of empathy toward fictitious characters described in scenarios to which they wereresponding as well as to friends, family members, and acquaintances experiencing sit-uations reminiscent of these scenarios. For example, one participants decision makingregarding cloning issues was driven primarily by her feelings toward a very close fam-ily member who desperately wanted to have children but remained infertile even afterconventional fertility treatments. This individual and many others like her in the study ex-hibited a genuine sense of care that ultimately guided her negotiation and resolution ofSSI.

    Howes (2002) also noted the appearance and use of empathy and other relationally basedconcerns in her teacher-research studies with 10th-grade biology students. Three mainaspects of a semester-long human genetics course were examined: (1) a unit focusing onprenatal testing for genetic conditions, (2) a set of imaginary cases posed to individualstudents during interviews, and (3) students writing and classroom talk concerning the roleof science in society. During the instructional unit on the subject of prenatal tests, the girlsin this study considered the comfort and safety of the fetus, and occasionally of the womancarrying the fetus, as primary aspects in making decisions concerning prenatal testing. Thismay have been an artifact of disincentives to discuss abortion in a high-school classroom,especially on the teachers partdeflecting the more deeply emotional and controversialdecisions concerning abortion to those concerning prenatal tests alone. These girls explicitlyutilized the aspects of comfort and safety when arguing for their decisions as to whethertests should be utilized in particular fictional cases, and used stories from direct personalexperience, or related those they had heard from female adults, to illustrate their points.Boys were not studied in the prenatal testing instructional context. In the second researchcontext, boys and girls were presented with similar fictional cases in which they were askedto put themselves in the place of a scientist in charge of ameliorating an urgent situation(e.g., the imminent extinction of an endangered animal, or a town suffering from a choleraoutbreak). The small sample from this study indicated that girls put the safety and comfortof both the suffering parties and the scientist her/himself in the forefront of their decisionmaking more than boys. This finding is complicated in the third portion of the study. Inwriting and discussion throughout the semester, both girls and boys strongly stated that

  • 368 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    the role of science in society was to provide cures for human diseases, to avoid disturbingecosystems, and, in general, to do good for the Earth and for humanity. Their passion inthis regard is also evident in preservice elementary teachers, as indicated by Cobern andLovings (2002) survey study.

    The empathy and care demonstrated in these studies is consistent with notions of emotion-based morality and care originally conceptualized in research with women (Belenky et al.,1986; Gilligan, 1987; Noddings, 1984). In addition, feminist critiques of science haveled to a notion that leaving emotional, political, and social influences out of scientificdecision making is not only impossible, but dangerous. Importantly, however, feministresearchers insist that care and emotion not be assigned solely to women, as this supportsa dichotomy that equates men with rationality and women with emotion (Harding, 1998;Hughes, 2000). Helping students to practice both of these segments of their personalitiesand knowledge-making skills may be where much of the promise of feminist approacheslies (Gilligan, 1987; Tong, 1996).

    Several researchers have corroborated the importance of this perspective in the contextof SSI (Korpan et al., 1997; Sadler & Zeidler, 2004, 2005; Zeidler & Schafer, 1984). For ex-ample, Sadler and Zeidler (2005) not only concluded that individuals may rely on emotionsfor the resolution of SSI, but that informal reasoning based on emotion was often equivalentto strictly cognitive approaches to decision making in terms of logical constructs such as in-ternal consistency and coherence. Whereas the Kohlbergian paradigm suggests that emotivedecision making represents inherently underdeveloped moral reasoning, Sadler and Zeidlerused an evaluative framework derived from argumentation and informal reasoning theoryand research (Kuhn, 1991; Toulmin, 1958) that did not discount any modes of decision mak-ing a priori. Rather, the quality of decision making was assessed based on logical criteriaof informal reasoning. Using this framework, it was concluded that evaluative distinctionsamong emotive and other forms of reasoning in terms of their decision-making adequacywere unfounded.

    These findings draw attention to the importance of cultural issues and individual studentsbeliefs when addressing SSI in science classrooms. Particular situations are, by definition,embedded in particular cultures and thus intertwined with particular relationships (Gruen,1994; Plumwood, 1993). Recommendations that science education be grounded in localcommunity issues (Aikenhead, 1997) and in students interests (Calabrese Barton, 1998a,1998b; Seiler, 2001) draw on the conclusion that individual students create identities throughexperiences and perspectives shaped by a culturally diverse society. Here is where we finda link between feminist and cultural perspectives, as both sets of theorists recognize per-sonal identity as embedded in individual and social circumstances (Kozoll & Osborne,2004). That identity shaped by culture is made clear through the worldview perspectiveas developed by Cobern (1993), who states that a peoples world view provides a specialplausibility structure of ideas, activities, and values that allows one to gauge the plausibil-ity of any assertion [emphasis in original] (p. 57). While students cultural experiences,therefore, will certainly influence their decisions, it is also clear that personal identities arenot fixed, but vary with setting and are fashioned through students social, intellectual, andmoral growth (Hughes, 2000; Kozoll & Osborne, 2004). Kozol and Osborne (2004) offerthe intriguing possibility that science education can support students development when itopens up the horizons from within which science and self are understood and contributestoward the evolution of both (p. 170).

    Explorations of identity, culture, and their relationships to science, while describingpersonal identity as influenced by context (e.g., classroom, family, ethnic group), recognizeidentity as a vehicle through which individuals interact with and explain their world. Giventhis, it follows that we need research in this area to see how we might create culturally

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 369

    relevant or culturally responsive pedagogy (see, e.g., Foster, 1995; Gay, 2000; Hollins &Oliver, 1999; Ladson-Billings, 1994; Quintanar-Sarellana, 1997; Smith, 2004; Tate, 1995)employing SSI contexts. Central to this research should be the results of SSI in teachingscience with all students, as well as further learning concerning students identities andworldviews. In their complexity, SSI may offer multiple connections to varied interests andbelief systems, thus assisting students in creating pathways to science learning. However,we cannot say this with certainty, as the uses of socioscientific issues per se have not beenwidely examined in science classrooms with an eye to cultural and feminist perspectives.Research in this area may provide a richer picture of moral codes and ethical perspectives,thus supporting teachers in engaging their students in socioscientific issues.

    Students possess diverse arrays of cultural experiences that necessarily contribute to themanners in which they approach and resolve controversial issues including SSI. World-views and identities engendering moral choices are brought into any classroom. Therefore,it is imperative for science educators to foster classroom environments that encourage theexpression of diverse perspectives even when those perspectives are not consistent withtraditional notions of science. In recognizing the moral nature of teaching and the factthat students are active moral agents, it is essential for teachers to understand that theirclassrooms cannot be value-free, but they certainly should be value-fair (Loving, Lowy, &Martin, 2003).

    (4) Case-Based Issues reinforce the stance that in order to develop scientifically literatecitizens, the science education community must reach beyond past STS practices whichusually do not pay explicit attention to the moral growth of the child and instead involvestudents with the kinds of issues and problems to ponder that embrace both their intellectand their sense of character. Recent studies involving example cases of genetically modi-fied foods (Walker & Zeidler, 2003), human genetic engineering (Sadler & Zeidler, 2004;Zohar & Nemet, 2002), animal experimentation (Simonneaux, 2001; Zeidler et al., 2002),and environmental dilemmas (Hogan, 2002; Kolst, 2001b) provide strong support for theefficacy of using controversial socioscientific case studies to foster critical thinking skillsand moral and ethical development. These studies strongly suggest that curricula using suchissues provide an environment where students become engaged in discourse and reflectionthat affect cognitive and moral development. The essence of the strategy of using SSI hasbeen described as follows:

    If we hope to stimulate and develop students moral reasoning abilities, then we mustprovide students with rich and varied opportunities to gain and hone such skills. The presentargument rests on the assumption that using controversial SSI as a foundation for individualconsideration and group interaction provides an environment where students can and willincrease their science knowledge while simultaneously developing their critical thinkingand moral reasoning skills (Simmons & Zeidler, 2003, p. 83).

    Similarly, other researchers have developed (or modified existing) protocols that have ad-dressed the implementation of case-based issues in science classrooms. Pedretti (2003) hashad successful experiences with preservice teachers who embraced the idea of incorporatingSSI via STSE in the curriculum. Using Ratcliffes (1997) model as an organizational frame-work has allowed Pedrettis (2003, p. 231) students to develop their own decision-makingmodels for pedagogy outcomes and includes

    1. OptionsIdentify alternative courses of action for an issue;2. CriteriaDevelop suitable criteria for comparing alternative actions;3. InformationClarify general and scientific knowledge/evidence for criteria;

  • 370 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    4. SurveyEvaluate pros/cons of each alternative against criteria selected;5. ChoiceMake a decision based on the analysis undertaken;6. ReviewEvaluate decision-making process identifying feasible improvements.

    Keefer (2003) has reported the results of using case studies with undergraduate andgraduate science students and has noted that the values of the domain of concern for theissue, rather than gender or a general disposition for a particular moral orientation (contraGilligan), accounted for differences in students reasoning. His work examined ethicalcare responses in professional contexts and led to an empirically derived model for deci-sion making in practical contexts using moral case issues. Keefers (2003, p. 253) modelentails the following:

    1. identify the moral issues at stake;2. identify the relevant knowledge and unknown facts in a problem;3. offer a resolution;4. provide a justification;5. consider alternative scenarios that argue for different conclusions;6. identify and evaluate moral consequences;7. offer alternative resolution.

    This moral heuristic has been used successfully in the analysis of engineering ethics casestudies for professionals and is strikingly similar to the six-component model describedabove. Keefer makes it clear in his analysis that ethical instruction is most successfulwhen it is integrated into those authentic contexts that will subsequently be practiced bystudents.

    Another complementary approach to case-based SSI provides a more explicit criticalexamination of students personal interests and values as they provide arguments that eval-uate scientific knowledge claims. Kolst (2000) reports on a consensus project model usedmainly with upper secondary science students which emphasizes that scientific knowledgeis formulated by consensus building via critical discourse among (competent) peers. Themajor premise of the consensus approach is that a general knowledge of the human na-ture and limitations of scientific claims is needed to place scientific statements in adequateterms so consensus decisions regarding SSI can be achieved. Necessarily broad in nature,consensus projects tend to have four key attributes (Kolst, 2000, p. 652):

    1. Presentation and defense of data/conclusions against possible opposition from theteacher and fellow students with a goal toward consensus of issues;

    2. Views of professionals and nonprofessionals are solicited on a particular socioscien-tific issue so balanced recommendations can be formulated and passed on to politi-cians and/or policy-makers;

    3. Students search for a common conclusion on which they can all agree while seekinginput from experts defined as anyone with relevant knowledge exceeding generalknowledge;

    4. Students write a report containing their assessments and conclusions, which is madeavailable to the public and politicians and/or policy-makers.

    Such an approach no doubt places great demands on the teacher whose role is that of acounselor, consultant and critic, as well as an expeditor. Students realize from the onset thattheir positions will be challenged and met with critical appraisal in the process of consensusbuilding.

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 371

    A last approach worth consideration in terms of the professional development of sec-ondary preservice teachers is described by Loving, Lowy, and Martin (2003) where a sci-ence methods course has built-in explicit case studies related to professional codes of ethics.While the focus is on teaching and learning with respect to academic freedom, the cases aresituated in real science classroom contexts. The authors note that preservice science teach-ers require practice in making decisions with ethical repercussions, and having explicitopportunities to engage in moral and ethical discourse is central to the development of theethical teacher. Questions such as What are your immediate moral intuitions and stirringsabout this case? and Are you experiencing any conflicting moral feelings as you thinkabout this case? provided students with the opportunity to frame an issue and access it in amanner consistent with the kind of valid forms of informal reasoning students used to con-strue SSI found by Sadler and Zeidler (in press). While the focus for Sadler and Zeidler wasnot on teacher education, the college students in their study (both science and nonscience ma-jors) reacted to and made decisions about multiple scenarios involving genetic engineeringby displaying rationalistic, emotive, and intuitive informal reasoning patterns. Rationalisticforms of reasoning have typically been fostered and honored in science classrooms. How-ever, the fact that students also use emotive and intuitive forms of reasoning in makingdecisions about SSI suggests that relational perspectives based on empathy and care, andinitial gut level reactions where a sense of conscience is invoked, is a reasonable meansto interpret and negotiate difficult moral dilemmas. As educators, we need to value stu-dents thinking, thereby providing them with opportunities to become personally engaged inissues.

    From these perspectives, socioscientific issues may be equated with the considerationof ethical issues and construction of moral judgments about scientific topics via socialinteraction and discourse. Students will be confronted with multiple perspectives to moralproblems that inherently involve discrepant viewpoints and information, sometimes at oddswith their own closely held beliefs. The joint construction of scientific knowledge that isat once personally relevant and socially shared therefore relies on exposure to, and carefulanalysis of, cases involving considerations of data, evidence, and argumentation that maybe in conflict with ones existing conceptions regarding various socioscientific moral andethical issues.

    CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONSAs the cases described above help to illustrate, the SSI approach represents a reconstruc-

    tion and evolution of the STS model that provides a means to not only address societalimplications of science and technology, but also to tap into students personal philosophiesand belief systems. As constructivist-learning theory suggests, each students knowledge isbuilt as a result of the combination of all influences, be they external or internal. Where STSfails to overtly consider the epistemological foundations, moral and ethical development,and emotional aspects of learning science, SSI specifically targets these essential personalaspects of learning. The STS approach served to convince the educational community thatscience, technology, and society are not isolated from each other, but did not provide afocus that addressed the intrinsically personal nature of knowledge and belief about sci-ence. By addressing the moral, ethical, emotional, and epistemological development of thestudent, the SSI approach provides a nexus that unites the various forces contributing tothe development of scientific knowledge. The introduction of case-based socioscientificissues represents a pedagogical strategy addressing not only the sociological but also thepsychological ramifications of curriculum and classroom discourse. A major strength ofthe SSI approach therefore lies in its unification power, where the major forces resulting in

  • 372 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    the construction of personal knowledge are simultaneously addressed in planned pedagogybased on a sound theoretical framework.

    Neo-Kohlbergian research has led those concerned with moral reasoning to realize thatsimply possessing the reasoning competence to make decisions consistent with availablestructure does not ensure performance at that level across all contexts. Kohlberg cameto realize and accept this toward the end of his research (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987),and other researchers have confirmed the lack of coherence between the ability to formhigher moral judgments and the likelihood of exercising that reasoning in varied contexts(Carbendale & Krebs, 1992; Wark & Krebs, 1996). With the keen interest in recent years ofcouching science in real-world problems, this point becomes increasingly important. SSIby their very nature occur in a world where the ceteris paribus conditions are unlikely tobe met. All things are not equal, and in fact, are a bit messier in most social settings andduring deliberative contexts where competing interests obfuscate principled rules or acts.

    If we are to use socioscientific issues as a basis for science curriculum, we will beplacing scientific knowledge and its uses squarely within our and our students social,political, and cultural lives. We acknowledge science as a human production, and we rec-ognize its role in a society that is less than perfect, but strives toward (at least in some ofits ongoing streams) morality of a sort that values equity and justice for all. As Goodlad(2003) writes, If our moral ecology encompasses equality and social justice, and if wewant that moral ecology to guide our society, then equality and social justice must betaughtcarefully taught (p. 19). Science educators can look to scholarship in intercul-tural education to help us think about the role that socioscientific issues can play in help-ing students develop morally as well as cognitively. According to Endicott, Bock, andNarvaez (2002), encountering multiple frameworks should be an effective way of en-hancing both moral and intercultural schemas, thereby facilitating more advanced ethicaland intercultural problem solving and attitudes (p. 2). Cultural differences may implyethical disagreements, and in our pluralistic society, these are things that our studentsneed to learn about, and practice negotiating, within and without familiar settings andsituations. Socioscientific issues provide engaging and complex contexts for such work.Perhaps an even loftier goal for SSI education is the mobilization of students who willtake action based on their newly acquired understandings of science and its ramifica-tions. In a dramatic call for an intentionally politicized approach to science education,Hodson (2003) promotes an issue-based curriculum that is . . . intended to produce ac-tivists: people who will fight for what is right, good and just; people who will work tore-fashion society along more socially-just lines; people who will work vigorously in thebest interests of the biosphere (Hodson, 2003, p. 645). Whether the development andimplementation of SSI curriculum will result in the production of individuals who willbe sociopolitically active remains to be seen; however, students exposed to such issueswill be more likely to consider the moral, political, and environmental aspects of politicaldecisions.

    Students who are able to carefully consider SSI and make reflective decisions regardingthose issues may be said to have acquired a degree of functional scientific literacy. Ac-cordingly, these individuals may cultivate a positive skepticism concerning the ontologicalstatus of scientific knowledge. Their decisions ought to be tempered by an awareness ofthe cultural factors that guide and generate knowledge. Perhaps most importantly, theirdecisions should not occur in a vacuum. If educators structure the learning environmentproperly, then opportunities for the epistemological growth of knowledge as fostered by theSSI framework will help students recognize that the decisions we all face involve conse-quences for the quality of social discourse and interaction among human beings, and ourstewardship of the physical and biological world. Moreover, if we as science educators

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 373

    wish to cultivate future citizens and leaders who care, serve the community, and provideleadership for new generations, then we have a moral imperative to delve into the realm ofvirtue, character, and moral development.

    REFERENCESAbd-El-Khalick, F. (2001). Embedding nature of science instruction in preservice elementary science courses:

    Abandoning scientism, but. . . . Journal of Science Teacher Education, 12(3), 215233.Aikenhead, G. (1994). What is STS in science teaching? In J. Solomon & G. Aikenhead (Eds.), STS education:

    International perspectives on reform. New York: Teachers College Press.Aikenhead, G. (1997). Toward a first nations cross-cultural science and technology curriculum. Science Education,

    81, 217238.Aikenhead, G. S. (1985). Collective decision making in the social context of science. Science Education, 69,

    453475.Aikenhead, G. S. (2000). Renegotiating the culture of school science. In R. Millar, J. Leach, & J. Osborne (Eds.),

    Improving science education: The contribution of research. Buckingham: Open University Press.American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1989). Science for all Americans. Washington, DC:

    American Association for the Advancement of Science.American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1993). Benchmarks for science literacy. New York:

    Oxford University Press.Belenky, M. F., Clinchy, B. M., Goldberger, N. R., & Tarule, J. M. (1986). Womens ways of knowing: The

    development of self, voice, and mind. New York: Basic Books.Bell, R. L. (2003). Exploring the role of nature of science understanding in decision-making: Pipe dream or

    possibility? In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse inscience education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Bell, R. L. (2004). Perusing Pandoras box: Exploring the what, when, and how of nature of science instruction,In L. Flick & N. Lederman (Eds.), Scientific inquiry and nature of science: Implications for teaching, learning,and teacher education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Bell, R. L., & Lederman, N. G. (2003). Understandings of the nature of science and decision making on scienceand technology based issues. Science Education, 87(3), 352377.

    Bell, R. L., Lederman, N. G., & Abd-El-Khalick, F. (2000). Developing and acting upon ones conception of thenature of science: A follow-up study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 563581.

    Berkowitz, M. W. (1997). The complete moral person: Anatomy and formation. In James M. DuBois, (Ed.),Moral issues in psychology: Personalist contributions to selected problems. New York: University Press ofAmerica.

    Berkowitz, M. W. (1998). Finding common ground to study and implement character education: Integratingstructure and content in moral education. Journal of Research in Education, 8(1), 38.

    Berkowitz, M., & Oser, F. (1985). Moral education: Theory and applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaumand Associates.

    Berkowitz, M. W., Oser, F., & Althof, W. (1987). The development of sociomoral discourse. In W. M. Kurtines &J. L. Gewitz (Eds.), Moral development through social interaction (pp. 337345). New York: Wiley.

    Berkowitz, M. W., & Simmons, P. (2003). Integrating science education and character education: The role of peerdiscussion. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in scienceeducation. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Bingle, W. H., & Gaskell, P. J. (1994). Scientific literacy for decisionmaking and the social construction of scientificknowledge. Science Education, 78(2), 185201.

    Boyes, M. C., & Walker, L. J. (1988). Implications of cultural diversity for the universality claims of Kohlbergstheory of moral reasoning. Human Development, 31, 4459.

    Brickhouse, N. W. (2001). Embodying Science: A feminist perspective on learning. Journal of Research in ScienceTeaching, 38, 282295.

    Bybee, Rodger (Ed.). (1985). ScienceTechnologySociety. In 1985 NSTA yearbook. Washington: NationalScience Teachers Association.

    Calabrese Barton, A. (1998a). Feminist science education. New York: Teachers College Press.Calabrese Barton, A. (1998b). Teaching science with homeless children: Pedagogy, representation, and identity.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35(4), 379394.Carpendale, J. I., & Krebs, D. L. (1992). Situational variation in moral judgment: In a stage or on a stage? Journal

    of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 203224.Chiappetta, E. L., & Koballa, T. R. (2002). Science instruction in the middle and secondary schools (5th ed.).

    Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

  • 374 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    Clark, C. M. (2003). Care of the soul in teacher education. Paper presented at the American Association of Collegesof Teacher Education, New Orleans, LA.

    Cobern, W. W. (1993). Contextual constructivism: The impact of culture on the learning and teaching of science.In K. G. Tobin (Ed.), The practice of constructivism in science education (pp. 5169). Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Associates.

    Cobern, W. W., & Loving, C. C. (2000). Defining science in a multicultural world: Implications for scienceeducation. Science Education, 85, 5067.

    Cobern, W. W., & Loving, C. C. (2002). Investigation of preservice elementary teachers thinking about science.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(10), 10161031.

    Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment. Vol. 2: Standard issue scoring manual.New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Council of Ministers of Education Canada (CMEC) Pan Canadian Science Project. (1997). Common frameworkof science learning outcomes: K-12. Available at URL: http://www.qscc.qld.edu.au/kla.sose.publicatons.html.

    Donenberg, G., & Hoffman, L. (1988). Gender differences in moral development. Sex Roles, 18, 701717.Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., & Scott P. (1996). Young peoples images of science. Bristol, PA: Open University

    Press.Driver, R., Newton, P., & Osborne, J. (2000). Establishing the norms of scientific argumentation in classrooms.

    Science Education, 84(3), 287312.Endicott, L., Bock, T., & Narvaez, D. (2002). Learning processes at the intersection of ethical and intercultural

    education. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, NewOrleans, LA.

    Foster, M. (1995). African American teachers and culturally relevant pedagogy. In J. A. Banks & C. A. McGeeBanks (Eds.), Handbook of research on multicultural education (pp. 570581). New York: Macmillan.

    Fuglsang, L. (2001). Three perspective in STS in the policy context. In S. H. Cutcliffe & C. Mitcham (Eds.),Visions of STS: Counterpoints in science, technology, and science studies. Albany, NY: State University ofNew York Press.

    Gay, G. (2000). Culturally responsive teaching: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Teachers College Press.Gilligan, C. (1987). Moral orientation and moral development. In E. Kittay & D. Meyers (Eds.), Women and moral

    theory (pp. 1933). Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Littlefield.Goodlad, J. (2003). Teaching what we hold sacred. Educational Leadership, 61(4), 1821.Gruen, L. (1994). Toward an ecofeminist moral epistemology. In K. J. Warren (Ed.), Ecological feminism,

    (pp. 120138). New York: Routledge.Harding, S. (1998). Is science multicultural?: Postcolonialisms, feminisms, and epistemologies. Bloomington, IN:

    Indiana University Press.Hekman, S. J. (1995). Moral voices, moral selves: Carol Gilligan and feminist moral theory. University Park, PA:

    The Pennsylvania State University Press.Hirsch, E. D., Jr. (1987). Cultural literacy: What every American needs to know. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Hodson, D. (1994). Seeking directions for change: The personalization and politicization of science education.

    Curriculum Studies, 2, 7198.Hodson, D. (2003). Time for action: Science education for an alternative future. International Journal of Science

    Education, 25(6), 645670.Hogan, K. (2002). Small groups ecological reasoning while making an environmental management decision.

    Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 341368.Hollins, E. R., & Oliver, E. I. (1999). Pathways to school success: Culturally responsive teaching. Mahwah, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Howes, E. V. (2002). Connecting and science: Constructivism, feminism and science education reform. New York:

    Teachers College Press.Huges, G. (2000). Marginalization of socioscientific material in science technologysociety science curricula:

    Some implication for gender inclusivity and curriculum reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(5),426440.

    Jenkins, E. W. (1990). Scientific literacy and school science education. School Science Review, 71(256),4351.

    Jenkins, E. W. (1997). Towards a functional public understanding of science. In R. Levinson & J. Thomas (Eds.),Science today (pp. 137150). London: Routledge.

    Jenkins, E. W. (2002). Linking school science education with action. In W. M. Toth & J. Desautels (Eds.), Scienceeducation as/for sociopolitical action. New York: Peter Lang.

    Keefer, M. W. (2002). Designing reflections on practice: Helping teachers apply cognitive learning princi-ples in an SFTinquiry-based learning program. Interchange: A Quarterly Review of Education, 33(4),395417.

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 375

    Keefer, M. W. (2003). Moral reasoning and case-based approaches to ethical instruction in science. In D. L. Zeidler(Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht:Kluwer Academic Press.

    Keefer, M. W., Zeitz, C. M., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Judging the quality of peer-led student dialogues. Cognitionand Instruction, 18(1), 5583.

    Kohlberg, L. (1986). A current statement on some theoretical issues. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), LawrenceKohlberg: Consensus and controversy (pp. 485546). Philadelphia: The Falmer Press.

    Kolst, S. D. (2000). Consensus projects: Teaching science for citizenship. International Journal of ScienceEducation, 22(6), 645664.

    Kolst, S. D. (2001a). Scientific literacy for citizenship: Tools for dealing with the science dimension of contro-versial socioscientific issues. Science Education, 85(3), 291310.

    Kolst, S. D. (2001b). To trust or not to trust, . . . pupils ways of judging information encountered in a socio-scientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 877901.

    Korpan, C. A., Bisanz, G. L., Bisanz, J., & Henderson, J. M. (1997). Assessing literacy in science: Evaluation ofscientific news briefs. Science Education, 81, 515532.

    Kozoll, R. H., & Osborne, M. D. (2004). Finding meaning in science: Lifeworld, identity, and self. ScienceEducation, 88: 157181.

    Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Kuhn, D. (1993). Science as argument: Implications for teaching and learning scientific thinking. Science Educa-

    tion, 77, 319337.Ladson-Billings, G. (1994). The dreamkeepers: Successful teachers of African American children. San Francisco,

    CA: Jossey-Bass.Latour, B. (1987). Science in action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Laugksch, R. C. (2000). Scientific literacy: A conceptual overview. Science Education, 84, 7194.Lemke, J. L. (2001). Articulating communities: Sociocultural perspectives on science education. Journal of Re-

    search in Science Teaching, 38, 296316.Levinson, R. (2003, March). Teaching bioethics in science: Crossing a bridge too far? Paper presented

    at the Annual Meeting of the National Association of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia,PA.

    Loving, C. C., Lowy, S. W., & Martin, C. (2003). Recognizing and solving ethical dilemmas in diverse scienceclassrooms. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in scienceeducation. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1992). Science for student with disabilities. Review of Educational Research,62, 377412.

    McGinnis, J. R. (2000). Teaching science as inquiry for students with disabilities. In J. Minstrell & E. van Zee(Eds.), Inquiring into inquiry learning and teaching in science (pp. 425433). Washington, DC: AmericanAssociation for the Advancement of Science.

    National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National AcademyPress.

    National Science Teachers Association. (1982). Science technologysociety: Science education for the 1980s.Washington, DC: National Science Teachers Association.

    Newton, P., Driver, R., & Osborne, J. (1999). The place of argumentation in the pedagogy of school science.International Journal of Science Education, 21(5), 553576.

    Noddings, N. (1984). Caring: A feminine approach to ethics and moral education. Berkeley, CA: University ofCalifornia Press.

    Nucci, L. P. (1989). Challenging conventional wisdom about morality: The domain approach to values education.In L. P. Nucci (Ed.), Moral development and character education (pp. 183203). Berkeley, CA: McCutchanPublishing.

    Nucci, L. P. (2001) Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Partington, G. (1997). Cultural invariance and the denial of moral regression: A critique of Piaget and Kohlberg.

    International Journal of Social Education, 11, 106119.Pedretti, E. (1997). Septic tank crisis: A case study of science, technology and society education in an elementary

    school International Journal of Science Education, 19(10), 12111230.Pedretti, E. (2003). Teaching science, technology, society and environment (STSE) education: Preser-

    vice teachers philosophical and pedagogical landscapes. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moralreasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer AcademicPress.

    Pedretti, E., & Hodson, D. (1995). From rhetoric to action: Implementing STS education through action research.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 463485.

  • 376 ZEIDLER ET AL.

    Plumwood, V. (1993). Ethics and the instrumentalising self. In Feminism and the mastery of nature (pp. 141164). New York: Routledge. Queensland School Curriculum Council (QSCC) (2001). Studies of society andenvironment. Available at URL: http://www.cmec.ca/science/framework/index.htm.

    Quintanar-Sarellana, R. (1997). Culturally relevant teacher preparation and teachers perceptions ofthe language and culture of linguistic minority students. In J. E. King, E. R. Hollins, &W. C. Hayman (Eds.), Preparing teachers for cultural diversity. New York: Teachers CollegePress.

    Ratcliffe, M. (1997). Pupil decision-making about socioscientific issues within the science curriculum. Interna-tional Journal of Science Education, 19(2), 167182.

    Rest, J., Narvaez, Bebeau, M. J., & Thoma, S. J. (1999). Postconventional moral thinking: A neo-Kohlbergianapproach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Roy, R. (1984). S-S/T/S Project: Teaching science via science, technology, and society material in the pre-collegeyears. University Park, PA: The Pennsylvania State University.

    Ryder, J. (2001). Identifying science understanding for functional scientific literacy. Studies in Science Education,36, 146.

    Sadler, T. D. (2004). Informal reasoning regarding socioscientific issues: A critical review of research. Journal ofResearch in Science Teaching, 41, 513536.

    Sadler, T. D., Chambers, F. W., & Zeidler, D. L. (2004). Student conceptualizations of the nature of science inresponse to a socioscientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 26, 387409.

    Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (2004). The morality of socioscientific issues construal and resolution of geneticengineering dilemmas. Science Education, 88(1), 427.

    Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (2005). The significance of content knowledge for informal reasoning regardingsocioscientific issues: Applying genetics knowledge to genetic engineering issues. Science Education, 89,7193.

    Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (in press). Patterns of informal reasoning in the context of socioscientific decision-making. Journal of Research in Science Teaching.

    Seiler, G. (2001). Reversing the standard direction: Science emerging from the lives of African Americanstudents. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 10001014.

    Settelmaier, E. (2003, March). Dilemmas with dilemmas: Exploring the suitability of dilemma stories as a wayof addressing ethical issues in science education. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the NationalAssociation of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA.

    Shamos, M. H. (1995). The myth of scientific literacy. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Simmons, M. L., & Zeidler, D. L. (2003). Beliefs in the nature of science and responses to socioscientific issues.

    In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education.Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Simonneaux, L. (2001). Role-play or debate to promote students argumentation and justification on an issue inanimal transgenesis. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 903927.

    Smith, M. (2004 June). Culturally responsive teaching strategies for Native students. Paper presented at theIntegrating Mathematics and Science Education Research into Teaching Conference, University of Maine,Orono, ME.

    Snarey, J. R. (1985). Cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: A critical review of Kohlbergianresearch. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 202232.

    Tate, William F. (1995). Returning to the root: A culturally relevant approach to mathematics pedagogy. Theoryinto Practice, 34(3), 166173.

    Tong, R. (1996). Feminist approaches to bioethics. In S. M. Wolf (Ed.), Feminism and bioethics: Beyond repro-duction. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Toulmin, S. (1958). The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Trowbridge, L. W., Bybee, R. W., & Powell, J. C. (2000). Teaching secondary school science: Strategies for

    developing scientific literacy (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Tsai, C. (2000). The effects of STS-oriented instruction on female tenth graders cognitive structure outcomes

    and the role of student scientific epistemological beliefs. International Journal of Science Education, 22,10991115.

    Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of childpsychology (pp. 863932). New York: Wiley.

    Vellom, P. (1999). Reasoning about data in middle school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(2),179199.

    Walker, K. A., & Zeidler, D. L. (2003, March). Students understanding of the nature of science and their reasoningon socioscientific issues: A Web-based learning inquiry. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the NationalAssociation of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA.

  • MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 377

    Wark, G., & Krebs, D. L. (1996). Gender and dilemma differences in real-life moral judgment. DevelopmentalPsychology, 32, 220230.

    Yager, R. E. (1996). History of science/technology/society as reform in the United States. In R. E. Yager (Ed.),Science/technology/society as reform in science education. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Zeidler, D. L. (1984). Moral issues and social policy in science education: Closing the literacy gap. ScienceEducation, 68, 411419.

    Zeidler, D. L. (1997). The central role of fallacious thinking in science education. Science Education, 81, 483496.Zeidler, D. L. (2001). Participating in program development: Standard F. In D. Siebert & W. McIntosh (Eds.),

    College pathways to the science education standards (pp. 1822). Arlington, VA: National Science TeachersPress.

    Zeidler, D. L., & Keefer, M. (2003). The role of moral reasoning and the status of socioscientific issues in scienceeducation: Philosophical, psychological and pedagogical considerations. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role ofmoral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer AcademicPublishers.

    Zeidler, D. L., Lederman, N. G., & Taylor, S. C. (1992). Fallacies and student discourse: Conceptualizing the roleof critical thinking in science education. Science Education, 76, 437450.

    Zeidler, D. L., Osborne, J., Erduran, S., Simon, S., & Monk, M. (2003). The role of argument and fallacies duringdiscourse about socioscientific issues. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientificissues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Zeidler, D. L., & Schafer, L. E. (1984). Identifying mediating factors of moral reasoning in science education.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 21(1), 115.

    Zeidler, D. L., Walker, K. A., Ackett, W. A., & Simmons, M. L. (2002). Tangled up in views: Beliefs in the natureof science and responses to socioscientific dilemmas. Science Education, 86(3), 343367.

    Ziman, J. (1994). The rationale of STS education is in the approach. In J. Solomon & G. Aikenhead (Eds.), STSeducation: International perspectives on reform. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Zohar, A., & Nemet, F. (2002). Fostering students knowledge and argumentation skills through dilemmas inhuman genetics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 3562.