Top Banner
SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED EFFECTS OF TOKEN-PRODUCTION AND EXCHANGE-SCHEDULE MANIPULATIONS By CHRISTOPHER BULLOCK A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2003
35

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

Mar 11, 2020

Download

Documents

dariahiddleston
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED EFFECTS OF TOKEN-PRODUCTION AND EXCHANGE-SCHEDULE

MANIPULATIONS

By

CHRISTOPHER BULLOCK

A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

2003

Page 2: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

Copyright 2003

by

Christopher Bullock

Page 3: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank my parents for their support and encouragement throughout my graduate

studies. I would also like to thank my mentor, Timothy Hackenberg, for his

encouragement and guidance in the research and writing of this research project.

iii

Page 4: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ................................................................................................. iii

LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................................................v

ABSTRACT....................................................................................................................... vi

CHAPTER

1 INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................1

2 METHOD .....................................................................................................................5

Subjects.........................................................................................................................5 Apparatus......................................................................................................................5 Procedure ......................................................................................................................6

3 RESULTS...................................................................................................................11

4 DISCUSSION.............................................................................................................18

LIST OF REFERENCES...................................................................................................26

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .............................................................................................28

iv

Page 5: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure page 2-1. Sample of an FR 2 (FR 50) token-reinforcement schedule .......................................10

3-1. Mean responses per minute and standard deviations for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of small, medium, and large token-production ratios, and as a function of exchange ratio..........................13

3-2. Mean responses per minute plotted as a function of token-production segment for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions..........................................14

3-3. Mean latency plotted as a function of token-production segment for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions. .............................................................15

3-4. Mean total responses for each pigeon under unconstrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of unit price.............................................................................16

3-5. Mean consumption (total seconds access to food) for each pigeon under unconstrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of unit price. .............17

4-1. Mean consumption rate as a function of unit price for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions plotted on log-log coordinates. .........................................24

4-2. Mean reciprocal responses per minute and modified unit price for each pigeon plotted as a function of unit price and exchange schedule. ......................................25

v

Page 6: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School

of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED EFFECTS OF TOKEN-PRODUCTION AND EXCHANGE-SCHEDULE

MANIPULATIONS

By

Christopher Bullock

May 2003

Chair: Timothy D. Hackenberg Major Department: Psychology

Four pigeons were exposed to second-order schedules of token reinforcement,

with stimulus lights serving as token reinforcers. Tokens were earned according to a

fixed-ratio (token-production) schedule and were exchanged for 2.5 s access to food

according to a fixed-ratio (token-exchange) schedule. The token-production and token-

exchange schedules were manipulated systematically across conditions. Response rates

varied inversely with the token-production schedule for a given token-exchange schedule

value. Response rates also varied inversely with the token-exchange schedule for a given

token-production value, particularly at the higher token-production ratios. Further, under

higher token-production and exchange-schedule values, response rates tended to increase

in token-production segments closer to exchange periods and food. Several probe

conditions were also studied in a closed economy that permitted unconstrained food

consumption. Under these conditions, response rates were less sensitive to token-

production schedule manipulations than under standard (constrained consumption)

vi

Page 7: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

conditions in which sessions were limited to 48 food presentations. Results were

analyzed using a modification of the behavioral economic concept of unit price (a cost-

benefit ratio comprising responses per unit of food delivery).

vii

Page 8: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

A second-order schedule of reinforcement is one in which a pattern of behavior

reinforced according to one schedule is treated as a unitary response reinforced according

to a second schedule (Kelleher, 1966). A token-reinforcement schedule is a second-order

schedule in which responses produce tokens according to one schedule (the token-

production schedule) and opportunities to exchange those tokens for primary

reinforcement according to a second schedule (the exchange schedule) (Kelleher, 1958;

Malagodi, 1967).

Previous research has shown that response rates and patterns maintained under

token-reinforcement schedules vary systematically as a function of both the token-

production schedules (Kelleher, 1958) and the exchange schedules (Foster, Hackenberg

& Vaidya, 2001; Waddell, Leander, Webbe, & Malagodi, 1972; Webbe & Malagodi,

1978). Most research on token-reinforcement schedules has involved fixed-ratio (FR)

components (schedules that require a specified number of responses for reinforcement).

Similar to FR performance reinforced with food, response rates under FR token-

production and exchange schedules vary inversely with the FR value (Foster, et al., 2001;

Kelleher, 1958; Malagodi, 1967; Webbe & Malagodi, 1978;). Kelleher (1958), for

example, found that chimpanzees' rates of lever pressing decreased as the FR token-

production requirement increased from 30 to 100 with the exchange ratio held constant at

FR 60. Foster et al. (2001) found that pigeons’ response rates decreased as the FR

exchange schedule increased from 1 to 8 with the token-production ratio held constant at

1

Page 9: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

2

FR 50. In both studies, response-rate decrements were mainly a function of increased

pausing early in an FR cycle.

With FR token-production and exchange schedules, a potentially relevant variable

emerges: the ratio of responses per unit of reinforcer, closely related to the economic

concept of unit price (DeGrandpre, Bickel, Hughes, Layng, & Badger, 1993; Hursh,

1978). When the exchange ratio is manipulated with token-production ratio held

constant, changes in reinforcer magnitude are proportional to changes in exchange ratio.

As a result, the unit price remains constant across variations in the exchange schedule.

For example, in the Foster et al. (2001) study, with a token-production ratio of 50 and an

exchange ratio of 2, FR 2 [FR 50], to use the standard nomenclature,100 responses

produced 2 tokens (each exchangeable for 2 s access to food), or 25 responses per second

access to food. So too under the FR 8 [FR 50] condition, 400 responses produced 8

tokens (16 s access to food), or 25 responses per second access to food. According to a

literal version of the unit price concept, response output should be roughly constant under

such conditions of equivalent unit price, without regard to the specific values of its cost

and benefit components(Madden, Bickel, & Jacobs, 2000). Contrary to these predictions,

however, response output varied substantially across conditions of equal unit price.

Responding then was governed not by responses per unit of food delivery (unit price) but

by responses per exchange period (exchange ratio).

Another way of examining the viability of the unit price concept with token-

reinforcement procedures is through manipulations of the token-production ratio. Unlike

exchange-ratio manipulations, altering the token-production ratio while holding constant

the exchange ratio changes the unit price. For example, in the Kelleher (1958) study,

Page 10: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

3

with an FR 30 token-production schedule and an FR 60 exchange schedule, 1800

responses produced 60 tokens, a unit price of 30. When the token-production schedule

was increased to 100, while holding constant the exchange schedule ratio at 60, 6000

responses produced 60 tokens, a unit price of 100. In accordance with unit price

predictions, rates of lever pressing decreased as the FR requirement and unit price of the

token-production schedule increased. Thus, unlike exchange-ratio manipulations, in

which unit price is invariant with respect to changes in ratio requirements; with token-

production manipulations, unit price changes with changes in ratio requirements.

The purpose of the present experiment was to clarify how both the token-

production schedule and exchange schedule contribute to maintaining behavior on token-

reinforcement schedules while assessing the applicability of unit price in accounting for

this behavior. Although response rates and patterns in token-reinforcement procedures

have been shown to vary with the values of both FR token-production and exchange

schedules, previous studies have typically manipulated only one of these variables while

holding the other constant. The effects of manipulating one of the schedules in a token-

reinforcement procedure may depend on the others value. Using a preparation similar to

that of Foster et al. (2001), with stimulus lights serving as token reinforcers, FR token-

production and exchange schedules were parametrically manipulated on a within-subject

basis across a wider range of schedule values than examined in prior research, allowing

for a more detailed analysis of their effects. The present experiment included three

token-production values (small, medium, and large) and three exchange values (2, 4, and

8); and was designed such that most of the possible combinations of these two schedules

were experienced by the subjects.

Page 11: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

4

Unit price experiments are typically conducted in a closed economy, where all food

is obtained via interaction with the experimental conditions (Hursh, 1980). A closed

economy is often contrasted with an open economy, where supplemental feeding occurs

outside of experimental sessions. In the Foster et al. (2001) study, birds received all or

nearly all of their daily food intake within sessions (functionally a closed economy).

However, because supplementary feeding was sometimes necessary, it can be argued that

this was not a genuine closed economy, rendering a unit-price analysis inappropriate. In

the present study, therefore, three probe conditions were conducted under a closed

economy where all food was obtained via interaction with experimental contingencies.

Parametric evaluation of token-production and exchange ratios in closed and open

economic contexts allows for a stronger investigation of the unit price concept and for an

extension of previous token-reinforcement studies. Unit price considerations aside, the

study had as its main objective the extension of previous work in the realm of token-

reinforcement procedures. The results should further clarify the contributions of token-

production and exchange schedules to performance on token-reinforcement procedures.

Page 12: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

CHAPTER 2 METHOD

Subjects

Four male White Carneau pigeons (Columba livia) (numbered 702, 732, 774,

1855) served as subjects. Each pigeon had prior experience with token-reinforcement

schedules. Subjects were housed individually under a 16.5 hour / 7.5 hour light dark

cycle and had continuous access to water and health grit in home cages. Pigeons were

maintained at approximately 20g of their 80% free feeding weights for most conditions of

the experiment. In a few conditions (see below), the weight restrictions were removed,

such that weights were free to vary above the 80% level.

Apparatus

Experimental sessions were conducted in an enclosure 36 cm high by 50 cm long

by 36 cm wide. An intelligence panel on a wall of the enclosure was equipped with

three response keys centered vertically 11.5 cm from the ceiling to the key center and 9

cm from each other (center to center). Thirty-four evenly spaced red light emitting

diodes (LEDs) were centered 5 cm above the keys and 1.25 cm apart from each other

(center to center) and protruded 0.3 cm into the enclosure. The LEDs (hereafter referred

to as tokens) were always illuminated left to right, in sequential order. The presentation

and removal of tokens was controlled by an electromechanical stepping switch (Lehigh

Valley Electronics®, model 1427, Lehigh Valley, PA), the operation of which also

provided auditory feedback each time a token was presented or removed. Centered above

the token array was a yellow house light that provided the enclosure with diffuse

5

Page 13: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

6

illumination. When active, side keys were illuminated green, and the center key was

illuminated red. Pecks exceeding approximately 11 to 14 g of force were counted. A

hopper aperture was centered 11.5 cm below the left key. A solenoid-operated food

hopper could be raised into this opening, allowing access to mixed grain. Food

presentation was accompanied by illumination of a yellow light located inside the

opening. A photo-beam mounted across the aperture recorded head entry into the hopper.

Continuous white noise and ventilation fans were active during experimental sessions to

mask extraneous sounds. Experimental contingencies were programmed, and data

collected, using a computer equipped with Med-pc® software, located in a separate room.

Procedure

Because the pigeons had previous experience with token-reinforcement schedules,

no training was necessary. A session began with the illumination of a green side key

(key position counterbalanced across subjects). Responses on this key (designated the

token-production key), were reinforced according to a FR schedule, that is, a fixed

number of responses illuminated one token. A separate exchange schedule was arranged

with respect to the number of tokens needed to produce an exchange period. During this

exchange period, signaled by illumination of the red center key, a single response

darkened the rightmost token and produced 2.5 s access to food (timed from head entry

into hopper). The exchange period continued until all tokens earned during that cycle

were exchanged, followed immediately by the illumination of the green side key and the

beginning of the next cycle. Sessions continued until 48 tokens had been exchanged for

food. (Because Pigeon 774 was consistently overweight, sessions from the last 5

conditions ended after only 32 tokens had been exchanged). Figure 1 is a procedural

schematic of an FR 50 token production FR 2 exchange token-reinforcement schedule.

Page 14: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

7

All pigeons were exposed to a series of conditions in which FR values of both the

token-production and exchange schedules were varied systematically across conditions.

Conditions included FR exchange-schedule values of 2, 4, and 8 and FR token-

production values of 25, 50, and 100. A variable ratio (VR) schedule 12.5 (on which

reinforcement depended with equal probability on either 12 or 13 responses) was added

as the lowest token-production schedule for Pigeon 1855 because response rates could

not be maintained reliably at the highest ratios. Table 1 shows the sequence of conditions

and the number of sessions conducted under each. (When reporting the results of this

experiment, conditions are referred to by the values of the token-production and

exchange schedules. The token exchange-schedule value is listed first, followed by the

value of the token-production schedule listed in brackets. For example, a condition with

an FR 50 token production FR 2 token exchange would be referred to by the abbreviation

FR 2 [FR 50] or simply as 2 [50].) The experiment consisted of two parts. In Part 1

(constrained consumption conditions) pigeons were maintained at approximately 80% of

their free-feeding weights. This was accomplished by holding constant the number of

food deliveries each session at 48, and providing supplementary post-session feedings as

needed. In Part 2 (unconstrained consumption conditions) sessions lasted until 10 min

elapsed without a response. Because pigeons in this phase were usually above 80%

weights, generally no supplementary feeding was needed. For some conditions in this

phase for Pigeons 702 and 1855, not enough food was earned to maintain their respective

80% free feeding weights. Supplementary feedings were therefore provided. In three

instances, when responding had weakened to the point that sessions were not being

completed consistently, or 80% weight not maintained in unconstrained consumption

Page 15: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

8

conditions, conditions were changed arbitrarily in the absence of stability. Data from

these conditions were not included in the analysis. Further, several conditions with no

tokens were also run, but were not included in the present analysis.

In Part 1 conditions, the token-production ratio and the exchange ratio were

systematically varied across conditions. In Part 2 conditions, the token-exchange

schedule was held constant at FR 2, while the token-production schedule was varied

across conditions. During this phase a limited hold of 10 s was placed on hopper cycles,

such that the amount of time the hopper was raised was restricted. The expiration of the

10 s limited hold was treated as a hopper entry, starting the 2.5 s food timer.

Each condition was in place for a minimum of 20 sessions and until performance

was deemed stable via visual inspection of daily response rates. For several conditions,

due to a programming error, the initial-link latencies to respond were sometimes lost for

given exchange cycles. In those instances where greater than 5% of the total initial-link

latencies were lost from the last 5 sessions of a condition, the condition was replicated.

For two of the subjects, Pigeons 774 and 1855, five replications were required (Table 2-

1).

Page 16: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

9

Table 2-1. Order of conditions and number of sessions per condition listed as: exchange schedule [production schedule] (number of sessions)

Pigeon 702 732 774 1855 Part 1 4[25] (48) 4[50] (23) 4[100] (36) 2[100] (21) 4[50] (24) 8[25] (28) 2[50] (27) 2[25] (25) 2[100] (25)

4[25] (29) 4[50] (56) 4[100] (23) 2[100] (53) 8[25] (25) 2[50] (22) 2[25] (22)

4[25] (57) 4[50] (38) 4[100] (37) 2[100] (33) 8[25] (41) 2[50] (19) 2[25] (20)

4[25] (64) 4[50] (33) 4[12.5] (28) 2[50] (84) 2[25] (20) 2[12.5] (20) 8[12.5] (38)

Part 2 2[100] (70)a 2[25] (24) 2[50] (24) 2[100] (25)a

2[100] (30) 2[50] (28) 2[25] (28)

2[100] (40) 2[50] (19) 2[25] (36)

2[50] (23)a 2[25] (28) 2[12.5] (27) 2[50] (24)

Part 1 2[50] (26) 4[100] (20)a

2[50] (33) 2[25] (21)

2[50] (24) 2[100] (24) 2[25] (22)b 4[25] (21)b 4[50] (21)b 4[100] (26)b 8[25] (20)b

2[50] (20) 4[50] (54) 2[50] (23) 2[25] (25) 2[12.5] (27) 4[25] (20) 4[12.5] (21) 8[12.5] (27)

a Condition ended arbitrarily b 32 reinforcers per session

Page 17: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

10

After all tokens are exchanged the cycle repeats.

A peck on the center key darkens one token and produces 2.5-s access to food.

FR1

FR50

FR50

an exchange2 tokens produce an

50 pecks produce1 token

Figure 2-1. Sample of an FR 2 (FR 50) token-reinforcement schedule

Page 18: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

CHAPTER 3 RESULTS

All data analyses are based on the last five sessions from each condition. Figure 3-

1 shows responses per min from Part 1 conditions, plotted as a function of token-

production ratio value (small, medium, and large), and as a function of exchange-ratio

value within a given token-production ratio. Response rates tended to decrease as the

token-production schedule increased. Under higher token-production ratios response rates

also tended to decrease as the exchange schedule increased. For two pigeons, 732 and

774, replications of conditions were consistent with original exposures. For the other two

pigeons, however, some subsequent exposures to conditions failed to replicate the initial

findings. Replications occurred after an intervening history on unconstrained

consumption conditions. In most of these instances, response rates increased when

compared to initial exposures.

Figure 3-2 shows responses per min across successive token-production segments

for Part 1 conditions under all combinations of token-production and exchange schedules.

A token segment is defined as the portion of an exchange cycle that occurs during the

production of a given token. Graphs in the left, center, and right columns show response

rates under FR 2, 4, and 8 exchange schedules, respectively. For all subjects, response

rates were bi-valued, with lower initial-link rates giving way to higher, relatively constant

rates in subsequent links. Under the FR 2 and FR 4 exchange ratios, the initial-link

response rates were generally lowest in conditions with higher token-production ratios

(FR 50 and FR 100). Under the FR 4 exchange ratio, rates under the highest token-

11

Page 19: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

12

production schedules tended to increase more gradually than under the smaller token-

production ratios.

Figure 3-3 shows for each token-production schedule, from Part 1 conditions, the

pre-ratio pause across successive token-production segments under each exchange

schedule. The data in this figure are organized as in Figure 3-2, with the graphs in each

column showing data for a given exchange schedule and graphs in each row specific to

an individual pigeon. For all pigeons pre-ratio pausing was longest in the initial link of

the exchange ratio, and relatively short and undifferentiated thereafter. Within a given

exchange ratio, initial-link pausing was directly related to the requirements of the token-

production schedules. Increases in the exchange-schedule value from 2 to 4 also tended

to produce increased initial-link pauses, but generally only when comparing conditions

under the highest token-production value. For lower token-production FR values,

increases in the exchange schedule either had no effect on initial-link pausing or

produced small increases.

Figures 3-4 and 3-5 show total response output and total consumption,

respectively, for Part 2 conditions plotted as a function of unit price on log coordinates.

For all subjects total response output tended to increase as unit price increased. Total

consumption, however, decreased very little, if at all, with increases in unit price.

Page 20: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

13

702

2[25] 4[25] 8[25] 2[50] 2[50] 4[50] 4[50] 2[100] 2[100] 4[100]

RES

PON

SES

PER

MIN

UTE

0

40

80120

160

200

240

732

2[25] 2[25] 4[25] 8[25] 2[50] 2[50] 4[50] 2[100] 4[100]0

4080

120

160

200240

small

small

small

small

medium

medium

medium

medium

large

large

large

large

1855

CONDITION2[12.5] 2[12.5] 4[12.5] 4[12.5] 8[12.5] 8[12.5] 2[25] 2[25] 4[25] 4[25] 2[50] 2[50] 2[50] 4[50] 4[50]

0

4080

120160

200240

774

2[25] 2[25] 4[25] 4[25] 8[25] 8[25] 2[50] 2[50] 4[50] 4[50] 2[100] 2[100] 4[100] 4[100]0

40

80120

160200

240

Initial exposurereplications

Figure 3-1. Mean responses per minute and standard deviations for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of small, medium, and large token-production ratios, and as a function of exchange ratio.

Page 21: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

14

FR 2 FR 8 FR 4300

702 200

smallmediumlarge

100

0

300 732

200

100

RES

PON

SES

PER

MIN

UTE

0

300 774

200

100

0

400 1855 300 200 100

0 1 2 2 4 1 2 3 4 6 8

SEGMENT Figure 3-2. Mean responses per minute plotted as a function of token-production

segment for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions. Open symbols represent initial exposures to a condition, filled symbols represent replications.

Page 22: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

15

FR 8FR 2 FR 41000

702100 Small

MediumLarge10

1

0.1

1000732

100

10

PRE-

RAT

IO P

AUSE

(SEC

) 1

0.1

1000774

100

10

1

0.1

10001855

100

10

1

0.11 2 1 2 3 4 2 4 6 8

SEGMENT

Figure 3-3. Mean latency plotted as a function of token-production segment for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions. Open symbols represent initial exposures to a condition, filled symbols replications.

Page 23: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

16

10000 10000702 732

1000 1000

TOTA

L R

ESPO

NSE

S

10 40 10 40

10000 10000

774 1855

1000 100010 40 205

UNIT PRICE

Figure 3-4. Mean total responses for each pigeon under unconstrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of unit price.

Page 24: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

17

702

100

10 205

1855

100

10 4010

732

4010

774

4010

100

TOTA

L SE

CO

ND

S FO

OD

AC

CES

S

10

100

10

UNIT PRICE Figure 3-5. Mean consumption (total seconds access to food) for each pigeon under

unconstrained consumption conditions plotted as a function of unit price.

Page 25: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION

The results of the present experiment are consistent with those previously reported

on extended chain and second-order schedules with ratio components. Similar to

Kelleher (1958), increases in the token-production ratio at a given exchange ratio

decreased response rates. Similar to Foster et al. (2001), increases in the exchange

schedule produced lower overall response rates, but generally only under the larger

token-production ratios. In the context of the lowest token-production value, response

rates varied less, if at all, with the exchange ratio. Further, for both token-production and

exchange-schedule manipulations, decreases in response rates were primarily a result of

longer pre-ratio pausing and weak behavior early in the ratio (see Figures 3-2 and 3-3), a

finding also consistent with previous research (Foster et al., 2001; Kelleher, 1958; Webbe

et al., 1978). Also similar to these previous findings, response rates were low, and pauses

high, in early links of all exchange cycles. The present results also correspond to those

reported by with extended chain and token schedules in regard to the gradually increasing

rates seen under combinations of higher token-production and exchange-schedule values

(Foster et al., 2001; and Jwaidah, 1973). The present results then both replicate and

extend previous investigations with token-reinforcement procedures, manipulating the

token-production and exchange schedules across a wider range of values than previously

examined.

The results also have implications for the unit price concept. Researchers have

typically examined unit price with a closed economy, defined as one in which the total

18

Page 26: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

19

consumption of a reinforcer is limited by a subject’s interaction with the contingencies.

By contrast, an open economy is one in which total consumption is controlled by the

experimenter. The majority of conditions in the present experiment involved an open

economy in that the total consumption for a session was kept constant at 48 reinforcers.

However, for 3 of 4 subjects, daily consumption within a session was sufficient to

preclude post-session feedings, a feature generally consistent with closed economies. In

that the majority of subjects’ consumption of food occurred solely via contact with the

experimental contingencies, the economic conditions might be considered a functional

closed economy, suggesting the potential applicability of a unit price analysis.

The main dependent measures in unit price experiments are total consumption and

response output (Hursh et. al, 1988; Hursh, 1980, 1984; Madden, Bickel, and Jacobs,

2000). Madden et. al (2000) noted two predictions of unit price. First, when the unit

price of a reinforcer increases, one can expect decreases consumption of that reinforcer.

Second, as unit price of a reinforcer is increased, overall responding increases to some

peak, with further price increases decreasing responding. Stated differently, the functions

relating total consumption and responding to unit price are negatively accelerated and

bitonic, respectively.

Data from Part 2 conditions were generally in accord with these predictions.

Total response output varied directly with unit price in all cases. As Figures 3-4 and 3-5

show, total responding generally increased, while consumption remained constant or

decreased, as unit price increased. The number of unconstrained consumption conditions

included in this analysis is insufficient for an examination of the shape of the full

function for these two measures.

Page 27: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

20

Because both consumption and responding were restricted in constrained

consumption conditions, analysis of total consumption and responding as a function of

unit price is not feasible. However, as Sumpter, Temple, and Foster (1999) point out, for

contingencies where absolute consumption is restricted, consumption rate is still free to

vary and may also be sensitive to unit price manipulations. In Sumpter et al. (1999),

consumption rate was examined in sessions that ended after either 30 reinforcers had

been delivered or 40 minutes had passed. Consumption rate proved an orderly measure

and was found to vary as a function of unit price, similar to total consumption in other

contexts. Similarly, response rates are also free to vary in conditions with restricted

response output. In light of the successful use of unit price as an account of consumption

rate in Sumpter et al. (1999), consumption rates were used in the analysis of constrained

consumption conditions. Additionally, because total responding was restricted in the

constrained consumption conditions, response rate was investigated as another measure

potentially sensitive to unit price manipulations.

Figure 4-1 shows consumption rate (s access to food per min) from Part 1

conditions plotted on log coordinates as a function unit price. Consumption rate

decreased as unit price increased, a finding consistent with unit price predictions. As in

Sumpter et al. (1999) consumption rate in this case therefore serves as a suitable proxy

for total consumption with regard to unit price predictions. Under all exchange ratios,

consumption rate generally decreased with increases in unit price. However, under

higher exchange ratios, sharper decreases in consumption rate tended to occur with

increases in the token-production schedule. For Part 1 conditions with higher exchange

values, the function relating consumption rate to unit price has a steeply decreasing slope.

Page 28: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

21

However, for conditions with the same unit price, consumption rates sometimes varied

inversely with token exchange-schedule value. This mirrors the variation in response

rates mentioned earlier for these same conditions, and is not in strict accord with unit

price predictions.

As mentioned earlier, a literal version of the unit price concept predicts that two

schedules of reinforcement with the same response-reinforcer ratios should engender

equivalent response output, regardless of the particular response requirements or

reinforcer amounts that comprise the ratio. In the present study, decreases in responses

rates that occurred as a function of increasing the token-production schedule is consistent

with the unit price formulation. Further, the lack of consistent exchange-schedule effects

(where unit price is held constant) at lower token-production values is also in accord with

the unit price equation. The rate decreases which occurred as a function of increases in

the exchange schedule at higher token-production schedule values (where the cost benefit

ratio remains the same), however, are not predicted by unit price.

When attempting to account for departures from unit price predictions in his data

set, Madden et al. (2000) found that a modification of the unit price concept made better

ordinal predictions than a traditional unit price account. The modified unit price equation

is given by

P = (FR + H) / V (4-1)

where P is modified unit price, FR is total number of responses prior to reinforcement, H

is handling costs (in the present study, the number of responses to exchange each token,

equal to the exchange-schedule value), and V is the reinforcer value (Mazur, 1987). The

equation for reinforcer value is written

Page 29: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

22

V = A / 1 + kD (4-2)

where A is the reinforcer amount, D is the reinforcer delay, and k is a scaling constant

(set to 1s-1 for the present analysis). A modified unit price analysis was conducted on

response rates generated in the present experiment, with s of food access used for

reinforcer amount and the average time from the illumination of the token-production key

to exchange used for reinforcer delay.

Figure 4-2 shows modified unit price and the reciprocal of responses per minute for

each condition as a function of unit price. As with the traditional unit price formulation,

response rates are expected to vary inversely with modified unit price. The reciprocal of

responses per minute was thus used to allow ease of inspection, as the reciprocal

measures would be expected to vary directly with modified unit price. Figure 4-2 is

organized similar to Figure 2-1 in that conditions are plotted with respect to increasing

unit price (Figure 2-1 is organized with respect to token-production value) and within

unit price, as a function of increasing exchange-schedule values. The gray bars situated

directly over each condition label represent the modified unit price for that condition.

The black bar directly to the left of a given gray bar represents the reciprocal of responses

per minute for that condition.

The modified unit price equation allows for better ordinal predictions than a

standard unit price account with respect to exchange-schedule manipulations. That is,

within a given unit price, this formulation correctly predicts the direction of the variations

in responding in the majority of cases. A standard unit price account, based on nominal

programmed values, is silent with respect to such variations. Additional research is

Page 30: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

23

needed to determine the full extent to which unit price, and modified unit price, are useful

metrics for token-reinforcement schedules.

In summary, the present research replicated the results of token-production and

exchange-schedule manipulations of previous token-reinforcement studies (Foster et. al.,

2001; Kelleher, 1958) in showing that response rates vary systematically as a function of

FR token-production and exchange schedules. Unit price does a reasonably good job in

accounting for the effects of token-production manipulations in both constrained and Part

2 conditions. The modified unit price formulation, however, provides better ordinal

predictions of the effects of exchange-schedule manipulations than a traditional unit price

formulation.

Page 31: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

24

73270210 10

1 1

SEC

ON

DS

FOO

D A

CC

ESS

PER

MIN

UTE

0 010 40 10 40

774 185510 10

1 1

FR 2 exchangeFR 4 exchangeFR 8 exchange

0 010 40 205

UNIT PRICE

Figure 4-1. Mean consumption rate as a function of unit price for each pigeon under constrained consumption conditions plotted on log-log coordinates. Open symbols represent initial exposures to a condition, filled symbols replications.

Page 32: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

25

702

2 2ce 4 8 2 2 2ce 4 4 2 2 4102

103

104

105

1855

2 22ce4 4 8 8 2 22ce4 4 2 2 22ce4 4

102

103

104

105

106

732

2 2 2ce 4 8 2 2 2ce 4 2 2ce 4

774

EXCHANGE SCHEDULE

2 2 2ce 4 4 8 8 2 2 2ce 4 4 2 2 2ce 4 4

MO

DIF

IED

UN

IT P

RIC

E

102

103

104

105

10 20 40

10

10

2020

20

1040

40

5

702 732

102

103

104

105

0.00

0.02

0.04

0.06

774

0.00

0.02

0.04

0.06

0.08

0.10

1855

0.00

0.02

0.04

0.06

REC

IPRO

CAL

RESPO

NSES PER

MIN

UTE

MODIFIED UNIT PRICE

RECIPROCAL RESPONSES PER MINUTE

.343

Figure 4-2. Mean reciprocal responses per minute and modified unit price for each

pigeon plotted as a function of unit price and exchange schedule. Conditions denoted with the letters ce were run under a closed economy with unconstrained consumption.

Page 33: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

LIST OF REFERENCES

DeGrandpre, R. J., Bickel, W. K., Hughes, J. R., Layng, M. P., & Badger, G. (1993) . Unit price as a useful metric in analyzing effects of reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 641-666.

Foster, T. A., Hackenberg, T. D., & Vaidya, M. (2001). Second-order schedules of token reinforcement with pigeons: Effects of fixed- and variable-ratio exchange schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 159-178.

Gollub, L. R. (1977). Conditioned reinforcement: Schedule effects. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Stadden (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 288-312) Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Hursh, S. R. (1978). The economics of daily consumption controlling food- and water-reinforced responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 475-491.

Hursh, S. R. (1980). Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219-238.

Hursh, S. R. (1984). Behavioral economics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 435-452.

Hursh, S. R., Raslear, T. F., Shurtleff, D., Bauman, R., & Simmons, L. (1988). A cost-benefit analysis of demand for food. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 419-440.

Kelleher, R. T. (1958). Fixed-ratio schedules of conditioned reinforcement with chimpanzees. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 281-289.

Kelleher, R. T. (1966). Conditioned reinforcement in second-order schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 475-485.

Madden, G. J., Bickel, W. K. & Jacobs, E. A. (2000). Three predictions of the economic concept of unit price in a choice context. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 73, 45-64.

Malagodi, E. F. (1967). Fixed-ratio schedules of token reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 8, 469-470.

26

Page 34: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

27

Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55-73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sumpter, C. E., Temple, W., & Foster, T. M. (1999). The effects of differing response type and price manipulations on demand measures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 329-354.

Waddell, T. R., Leander, J. D., Webbe, F. M., & Malagodi, E. F. (1972). Schedule interactions in second-order fixed-interval (fixed-ratio) schedules of token reinforcement. Learning and Motivation, 3, 91-100.

Webbe, F. M., & Malagodi, E. F. (1978). Second-order schedules of token reinforcement: Comparisons of performance under fixed-ratio and variable-ratio exchange schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, 219-224.

Page 35: SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES OF TOKEN REINFORCEMENT: COMBINED ...ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/00/06/54/00001/bullock_c.pdf · second-order schedules of token reinforcement: combined effects

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Christopher Bullock graduated from J. F. Webb High school in the spring of 1994.

He then enrolled at the University of North Carolina at Wilmington (UNCW) in the Fall

of 1994. He graduated from UNCW in the spring of 1999 with a Bachelor of Arts in

psychology with honors. The next fall he enrolled in the Behavior Analysis graduate

studies program in the Department of Psychology at the University of Florida. He is

presently continuing his education and conducting research in Behavior Analysis.

28