Top Banner
Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*, Krystyna A. Golabek*, Peter J. Apps, Geoffrey D. Gilfillan* & John W. McNutt* * Botswana Predator Conservation Trust, Maun, Botswana Wildlife Conservation Research Unit, Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, The Recanati-Kaplan Centre, Tubney, Oxon, UK Paul G. Allen Laboratory for Wildlife Chemistry, Botswana Predator Conservation Trust, Maun, Botswana Correspondence Neil R. Jordan, Botswana Predator Conservation Trust, Private Bag 13, Maun, Botswana. E-mail: [email protected] Received: July 13, 2012 Initial acceptance: October 29, 2012 Final acceptance: May 17, 2013 (V. Janik) doi: 10.1111/eth.12105 Abstract Scent-marking is common in mammals, but where signals are carried by urine and faeces, distinguishing between scent-marking and mere elimi- nation is problematic. To do so, we documented behaviours and context variables associated with urination and defecation in free-ranging endan- gered African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) and tested whether these were related to the responses of other dogs to deposits. We found that distinct postures were almost uniquely associated with deposits by dominant wild dogs, were more common during urination than defecation, and increased the likelihood that these deposits would be investigated by other wild dogs. The likelihood of investigation depended on the sex and dominance status of the depositor, the type of deposit and the substrate. Urine from dominant females was more likely to be investigated by other wild dogs than any other deposits, and deposits placed on vegetation were more likely to be investigated than those on bare ground. The likelihood that a deposit would be overmarked was affected by the deposit type and the sex and dominance status of the last depositor. Collectively, these results sug- gest that dominant wild dog urine is of greatest interest to other dogs. Our results show that some deposits by African wild dogs are not scent-marks and that detailed observations of behaviours and context variables during elimination events can be used to distinguish deposits that are likely to be of communication value. Introduction Scent-marking is almost ubiquitous among mammals (Macdonald 1980). While it may be relatively easy to recognise scent-marking when it involves elaborate behaviours while depositing secretions from specialist glands (e.g. aardwolf, Proteles cristatus, Sliwa & Rich- ardson 1998), the use of urine and faeces as carriers of scent signals, which is particularly common in canids (see Kleiman 1966; Macdonald 1980), presents prob- lems because all animals urinate and defecate, but not all such excretions are necessarily scent-marks. How- ever, distinguishing between scent-marks and mere eliminations is critical in many contexts. Functional interpretations of scent-marking behaviour and strategies require that scent-marks be distinguished from simple excretions that might have no communication function. Kleiman (1966) attempted to make this distinction on the basis of substrate characteristics by proposing that urine or faeces deposited in response to familiar landmarks or novel odours and objects, orientated towards a con- spicuous object, or repeated frequently on the same object, were active scent-marks. In many species, scent-marks are placed in areas that are interpreted as likely to promote signal discovery and longevity (Alberts 1992; Bradbury & Vehrenkamp 1998). Deposits are commonly found on or near conspicu- ous landmarks such as rocks, trees or crossroads (for reviews see Eisenberg & Kleiman 1972; Macdonald 1985). For example, Iberian wolves, Canis lupus sign- atus, preferentially deposit faeces at trail junctions (Barja et al. 2004) and on specific vegetation (Barja 2009). Variation in the conspicuous placement of Ethology © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 1 Ethology
9

Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

Jul 29, 2018

Download

Documents

hoangliem
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour inAfrican Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus)Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna A. Golabek*†, Peter J. Apps‡, Geoffrey D. Gilfillan* & John W. McNutt*

* Botswana Predator Conservation Trust, Maun, Botswana

† Wildlife Conservation Research Unit, Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, The Recanati-Kaplan Centre, Tubney, Oxon, UK

‡ Paul G. Allen Laboratory for Wildlife Chemistry, Botswana Predator Conservation Trust, Maun, Botswana

Correspondence

Neil R. Jordan, Botswana Predator

Conservation Trust, Private Bag 13, Maun,

Botswana.

E-mail: [email protected]

Received: July 13, 2012

Initial acceptance: October 29, 2012

Final acceptance: May 17, 2013

(V. Janik)

doi: 10.1111/eth.12105

Abstract

Scent-marking is common in mammals, but where signals are carried by

urine and faeces, distinguishing between scent-marking and mere elimi-

nation is problematic. To do so, we documented behaviours and context

variables associated with urination and defecation in free-ranging endan-

gered African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) and tested whether these were

related to the responses of other dogs to deposits. We found that distinct

postures were almost uniquely associated with deposits by dominant wild

dogs, were more common during urination than defecation, and increased

the likelihood that these deposits would be investigated by other wild

dogs. The likelihood of investigation depended on the sex and dominance

status of the depositor, the type of deposit and the substrate. Urine from

dominant females was more likely to be investigated by other wild dogs

than any other deposits, and deposits placed on vegetation were more

likely to be investigated than those on bare ground. The likelihood that a

deposit would be overmarked was affected by the deposit type and the sex

and dominance status of the last depositor. Collectively, these results sug-

gest that dominant wild dog urine is of greatest interest to other dogs. Our

results show that some deposits by African wild dogs are not scent-marks

and that detailed observations of behaviours and context variables during

elimination events can be used to distinguish deposits that are likely to be

of communication value.

Introduction

Scent-marking is almost ubiquitous among mammals

(Macdonald 1980). While it may be relatively easy to

recognise scent-marking when it involves elaborate

behaviours while depositing secretions from specialist

glands (e.g. aardwolf, Proteles cristatus, Sliwa & Rich-

ardson 1998), the use of urine and faeces as carriers of

scent signals, which is particularly common in canids

(see Kleiman 1966; Macdonald 1980), presents prob-

lems because all animals urinate and defecate, but not

all such excretions are necessarily scent-marks. How-

ever, distinguishing between scent-marks and mere

eliminations is critical in many contexts.

Functional interpretations of scent-marking

behaviour and strategies require that scent-marks be

distinguished from simple excretions that might have

no communication function. Kleiman (1966)

attempted to make this distinction on the basis of

substrate characteristics by proposing that urine or

faeces deposited in response to familiar landmarks or

novel odours and objects, orientated towards a con-

spicuous object, or repeated frequently on the same

object, were active scent-marks. In many species,

scent-marks are placed in areas that are interpreted

as likely to promote signal discovery and longevity

(Alberts 1992; Bradbury & Vehrenkamp 1998).

Deposits are commonly found on or near conspicu-

ous landmarks such as rocks, trees or crossroads (for

reviews see Eisenberg & Kleiman 1972; Macdonald

1985). For example, Iberian wolves, Canis lupus sign-

atus, preferentially deposit faeces at trail junctions

(Barja et al. 2004) and on specific vegetation (Barja

2009). Variation in the conspicuous placement of

Ethology © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 1

Ethology

Page 2: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

deposits may suggest that some are active scent-

marks, but are all deposits left in such locations

scent-marks? Conversely, are all less conspicuous

deposits simply inactive excretory eliminations? Spe-

cific postures adopted during deposition, and the

token volumes deposited, may provide additional

insights to a deposit’s communication potential. For

example, scent-marking with urine in canids is often

associated with a raised leg posture and high social

status (e.g. grey wolf, Asa et al. 1990; Peters & Mech

1975).

For communication to occur, there must be a recipi-

ent, and subsequent responses such as investigation

and overmarking may provide confirmation that a

deposit constitutes a signal. Overmarking and coun-

termarking, where scent-marks are deposited on top

of, or alongside, an existing mark (Johnston et al.

1994; Rich & Hurst 1999), are nearly ubiquitous

among mammals and, regardless of their ultimate

function (Ferkin & Pierce 2007), such secondary

marking establishes that the original deposit was a sig-

nal and may help to distinguish active scent-marks

from relatively inactive eliminations. Detailed obser-

vational data on both the circumstances of deposition

and the responses of other individuals are therefore

essential for functional interpretations of scent-mark-

ing, particularly in species where overt use of special-

ist glands does not occur.

In this study, we investigate in African wild dogs

whether context variables associated with excretion

events and subsequent investigation by other individ-

uals can identify which deposits are active scent-

marks as confirmed by overmarking. African wild

dogs are cooperative breeders, and packs typically

consist of a dominant breeding pair and their off-

spring (Malcolm & Marten 1982). Dispersal is

delayed, and mature offspring assist their parents to

rear subsequent litters (Malcolm 1979). Although

wide ranging, wild dog packs are territorial (Mills &

Gorman 1997; Parker 2009; Woodroffe 2011). They

deposit urine and faeces throughout their home

ranges (Parker 2009), but which of these deposits are

scent-marks is uncertain. To determine which African

wild dog deposits are scent-marks, we first explore

the frequencies of urination and defecation, and the

sex and dominance status of the depositor. We then

explore the factors affecting the likelihood that a

deposit will be: (1) encountered and investigated and

(2) overmarked by a conspecific, which are both indi-

cators that deposits are active scent-marks. Our find-

ings are discussed in the context of scent-mark

identification and the study of scent-mark function in

general.

Methods

Study Population and Site

Data were collected between May 2011 and Apr. 2012

from 9 packs of free-ranging African wild dogs in Bots-

wana. The study area (approx. 2600 km2; 19°31′S,23°37′E; elevation approx. 950 m) is bordered by the

Okavango Delta and includes the Moremi Game

Reserve and Wildlife Management Areas. Further

details can be found in McNutt (1996). This subpopu-

lation of African wild dogs has been studied since

1989. Immigrant dogs first identified as adults (n = 2)

were aged on the basis of tooth wear and pelage. Each

individual was identified by its unique tricolour pelage

pattern, and distinctive ear notches and tail stripes, all

of which were drawn and photographed on first

encounter and updated as appropriate. Packs were

radiotracked from the air and from a vehicle, with one

to four individuals in each pack fitted with GPS radio-

collars with a VHF tracking pinger (Vectronic

Aerospace GmbH, Berlin, Germany; <280 g) or VHF

radiocollars (Sirtrack, Havelock West, New Zealand;

<180 g) following procedures described elsewhere

(Osofsky et al. 1996).

In this study, a pack was defined as a group contain-

ing at least one adult male and female forming a

potential reproductive unit (cf. Malcolm 1979;

McNutt 1996). Independent of the mutual overmark-

ing behaviour of dominant wild dogs (see Estes 1997),

the dominant pair was easily identified within estab-

lished packs. Only the dominant female bred in each

pack and was closely guarded by the dominant male,

who licked her vulva and lifted her with his shoulders

during her brief receptive period in Mar./Apr. This

male also repelled all other individuals from her side

during this period. Outside the breeding period, the

dominant pair also typically lay together during rest-

ing periods, and they were the focus of social activity

prior to pack movement. Excluding the individual(s)

that made the kill, the dominant pair also fed first at

kills and repelled others from the carcass when pups

were present. No other individuals in the pack dis-

played this combination of behaviours. During the

study, only the dominant female became visibly preg-

nant (heavily swollen abdomen and distended teats).

The mother of each litter was therefore easily identi-

fied, and she was the only female to nurse the pups

(4/4 packs followed through breeding season). Using

these criteria, mothers were known for all pups born

during the current study and, as this study is part of a

project that has run continuously since 1989, the

mothers of 96.8% (60/62) of individuals were known

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH2

Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt

Page 3: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

overall. Paternity was less certain, but likely related-

ness was determined by assuming that the dominant

male in a pack during the dominant female’s oestrous

fathered the entire litter that year, although some

extra-pair paternity is known in this species (Spiering

et al. 2010). Importantly, immigration events leading

to the formation of the packs followed in this study

involved either unknown (and therefore not closely

related) individuals from outside the study population

(1 pack) or individuals of known origin that were not

closely related to the opposite-sex residents of the

pack into which they immigrated (8 packs). On the

basis of these criteria, and known relatedness within

the pack, individuals were divided into three domi-

nance status categories: DB- dominants with intra-

pack breeding opportunities (unrelated to dominant

partner); SB- subdominants with intra-pack breeding

opportunities (unrelated to opposite-sex dominant);

SN- subdominants with no intra-pack breeding oppor-

tunities (offspring, full sibling or parent of opposite-

sex dominant).

Behavioural Observations

Packs were located by radiotracking and observed

from a vehicle while resting (at distances of 3–40 m)

and travelling (at 20–200 m, depending on terrain,

vegetation and visibility). All deposits, investigations

and overmarking were recorded on video or directly

on datasheets by critical incident sampling (Altmann

1974), with overmarking defined as the placement of

a deposit on an existing deposit so that the two were

at least partially overlapping (sensu Johnston et al.

1994). When a dog deposited at a location, its posture,

deposit type (urine or faeces), the substrate (vegeta-

tion or bare ground) and the identity of the individual

that deposited were recorded. Each location was given

a unique identifying number (the ‘Unique site ID’),

and all behaviour by subsequent visitors to each site

was recorded, until the pack moved away from the

site. Individuals ‘investigated’ a deposit when they

sniffed (muzzle directed at, and lingering within

30 cm of, deposit) or licked it (made direct contact

using the tongue). Leg postures included cocked leg

(single hind leg raised/cocked once); raised leg (both

hind legs raised independently at least once); and

handstand (both hind legs raised simultaneously in a

hop). Squats (i.e. no leg posture) involved lowering

the hindquarters with all legs on the ground. The first

deposit at a site was termed the ‘original deposit’, and

the deposit most recently left on a site was termed the

‘top deposit’. To avoid including sites at which the

first deposit had not been observed, sites were

excluded when the first behaviour observed was

investigation.

To investigate relationships between context vari-

ables, we used binomial tests of proportions with con-

tinuity correction and chi-squared tests to compare

the frequencies that different deposit types (urine and

faeces) were deposited by different classes of individu-

als (sex, dominance status) in conspicuous locations

(or not) and with postures (or not).

To investigate the factors affecting whether or not

deposits were investigated by other individuals,

we ran a series of generalised linear mixed

models (GLMMs) with a binomial error distribution

(0 = deposit not investigated by another dog follow-

ing deposition, 1 = deposit subsequently investigated)

and a logit link function. We included the identity of

the individual that deposited (‘depositor identity’) and

‘pack’ identity as random terms to account for multi-

ple data from the same individuals and packs. We

used Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) to select the

most plausible models from a set of credible options

including all combinations of the likely terms (deposi-

tor type, depositor status, depositor sex) and their

two-way interactions. Lower AIC values correspond

with better relative support for each model (Akaike

1974), and models where delta AIC differed by less

than two with fewer parameters were rated equally

(Burnham & Anderson 2004). Akaike weights were

then calculated to show relative importance (Akaike

1974) between these models.

For all investigated deposits, we evaluated the fac-

tors affecting whether or not they were overmarked

by the investigator. We ran another series of GLMMs

with a binomial error distribution (0 = not over-

marked, 1 = overmarked) and a logit link function.

We included ‘pack’, ‘unique site identity’, ‘recipient

identity’, ‘top depositor identity’ and ‘original deposi-

tor identity’ as random terms. We used Akaike’s infor-

mation criterion (AIC) as described previously to

select the most plausible model from a set of credible

options.

Results

General Scent-Marking Behaviour

A total of 1179 deposits were observed at 715 unique

sites by 62 wild dogs from 9 packs. Each of 15 domi-

nants was observed to defecate 4.60 � 5.87 (�x � SD)

times and urinate 45.93 � 61.49 times. Defecation by

51 subdominants was observed 4.27 � 4.43 times per

individual, and urination was observed 3.98 � 5.10

times per individual. At 66.4% of all sites, the first

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 3

N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs

Page 4: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

deposit was urine, while 75.7% of all (1179) deposits

were urine. 29.9% of all sites were overmarked at

least once; these 215 sites received an average of

3.17 � 1.52 (�x � SD) deposits.

Of 676 initial sites where the substrate could be

clearly identified, a single defecation and 12 urinations

were deposited directly into standing water by 13 dif-

ferent subdominants. A significantly greater propor-

tion of urinations (79.5% of 449) than defecations

(58.1% of 227) was placed on vegetation as opposed

to on bare ground (binomial test of proportions with

continuity correction: v2ð1Þ = 33.319, p < 0.0001). A

significantly greater proportion of deposits from

dominants (87.3% of 324) than subdominants (58.5%

of 352) was placed on vegetation (v2ð1Þ = 68.609,

p < 0.0001). While a greater proportion of dominant

urinations placed on vegetation was investigated by

individuals (62.5% of 253) than were dominant urines

placed on bare ground (41.9% of 31) (v2ð1Þ = 4.033,

p = 0.0446), the proportion of subdominant urines

investigated was unrelated to the substrate marked

(16.4% of 61 investigated on bare ground; 11.5% of

104 on conspicuous sites; v2ð1Þ = 0.420, p = 0.517).

Considering all depositions, individuals were more

likely to adopt a leg posture during urination (72.5%

of 892) than during defecation (2.8% of 287) (bino-

mial test of proportions with continuity correction:

v2ð1Þ = 424.968, p < 0.0001). No leg posturing occurred

during defecation by subdominants (n = 218), and

postures were adopted during only 13.1% of 61 domi-

nant defecation events. Leg posturing during urination

by subdominants was also rare (15.3% of n = 203),

but postures were common during dominant urina-

tions (89.4% of 689). Of all (892) urinations, 56.4%

occurred with raised legs, 14.7%with a cocked leg and

1.5%with handstands.

Considering dominant deposits only, males urinated

significantly more often than females (63.4% of 689

dominant urinations; chi-squared test: v2ð1Þ = 49.674,

p < 0.0001). In contrast, faeces deposition did not dif-

fer significantly between dominant males and females

(Χ2(1) = 1.754, p = 0.185; males produced 58% and

females 42% of 69).

In dominants, leg postures were significantly more

likely during urination than defecation (binomial test

of proportions with continuity correction: v2ð1Þ =255.613, p < 0.0001; Fig. 1). In addition, dominant

males were significantly more likely than dominant

females to adopt leg postures during urination (bino-

mial test of proportions with continuity correction:

v2ð1Þ = 54.786, p < 0.0001; Fig. 1), although both did

so often (dominant males during 96.1% of 437

events; females during 77.8% of 252). In 9.4% of all

203 observed subdominant urinations, a dog urinated

directly in its resting spot while lying on its side.

Factors Affecting Investigation

33.2% of 711 sites were investigated by at least one

individual during observation sessions. Dominant

scents deposited with a posture were more likely to

be investigated by others (61.2% of 250) than

(a) (b) (c)

Fig. 1: Predicted effects on the likelihood that an investigated deposit will be overmarked according to: (a) the interaction between the sex of the

investigator and the last individual to deposit at the site (top depositor sex); (b) the interaction between sex of the investigator and investigator domi-

nance status (DB-dominant with intra-pack breeding opportunities, SB-subdominant with intra-pack breeding opportunities, SN-subdominant with no

intra-pack breeding opportunities) and (c) the top deposit type. Effects were estimated from the best fit GLMM model with the fewest explanatory

parameters and lowest AIC. k = parameters, Δi = AICi-AICmin, wi = Akaike weights (model 1, Table 2). Pack identity (n = 9), unique site (n = 579),

investigator identity (n = 49), top depositor identity (n = 39) and original depositor identity (n = 36) were included as random terms.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH4

Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt

Page 5: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

those deposited without a posture (43.2% of 74;

chi-squared test: v2ð1Þ = 6.801, p = 0.009). Original

deposit type had a significant effect on the likelihood

that a deposit was subsequently investigated (Model

1, Table 1), with sites initiated with urine more often

investigated (42.7% of 472) than sites initiated with

faeces (14.2% of 239). In addition, both the domi-

nance status and the sex of the original depositor

affected the likelihood that a deposit would be investi-

gated (Model 1, Table 1). The deposits of dominants

were significantly more likely to be investigated than

deposits of subdominants, and female deposits were

more likely to be investigated than male deposits.

Factors Affecting Overmarking

At 259 unique sites, 60.3% of all 579 investigations

resulted in overmarking. The likelihood of overmark-

ing was affected by an interaction between the sex of

the investigator and the sex of the last individual to

deposit there (Model 1, Table 2; Fig. 1a). Investigators

of both sexes were more likely to overmark scents

where the top deposit was left by an opposite-

sex individual (69.2% overmarked opposite-sex top

deposits [n = 399]; 40.6% overmarked same-sex top

deposits [n = 180]). Although the model showed that

males (45.2% of 104) and females (66.9% of 178)

were similarly likely to overmark sites where the top

deposit was from a male, male investigators (71.0% of

221) were more likely than female investigators

(34.2% of 76) to overmark sites where the top deposit

was from a female.

Overmarking likelihood was also affected by an

interaction between the sex and the status of the

investigator (Model 1, Table 2; Fig. 1b). Dominants

(79.9% of 398) were more likely than subdominants

(17.1% of 181) to overmark a deposit. Within domi-

nants, males (82.8% of 238) and females (75.6% of

160) were similarly likely to overmark, but female

subdominants (25.5% of 94) were more likely to

overmark than subdominant males (8.0% of 87).

Subdominant females with breeding potential in the

pack were also more likely to overmark deposits

(38.1% of 21) than females with no intra-pack breed-

ing opportunities (21.9% of 73).

Finally, overmarking likelihood was affected by the

top deposit type (Model 1, Table 2; Fig. 1c). Sites

where urine was last deposited were more often over-

marked (65.0% of 506) than sites where faeces were

last deposited (27.4% of 73).

Discussion

African wild dogs are one of many species for which

scent signals are assumed to be incorporated with

urine and faeces. Results presented here suggest that

active scent-marks can be distinguished from mere

waste elimination by characteristics of the deposit the

depositor, the nature of the substrate and whether the

deposit was investigated by a conspecific. Addition-

ally, specific overmarking responses were used to con-

firm that certain deposits were scent-marks with

communicatory significance.

Overmarking was conducted predominantly with

urine and occurred on only 27.4% of encountered

sites where faeces were the top deposit compared

with 65.0% of sites with urine on top. That around

two-thirds of scats had no confirmed scent-marking

Table 1: GLMMs with binomial error distribution and logit link function investigating the factors that predict the likelihood of a deposit being investi-

gated (n = 711)

Model Description Estimate AIC k Δi Wi

Basic – 768.6 2 63.4 0.000

1 Deposit type Faeces 0.000 705.2 7 0.0 0.872

Urine 0.775

Depositor dominance status DB 0.000

SB �1.853

SN �2.647

Depositor sex Female 0.000

Male �0.779

2 *�Depositor dominance status 755.2 5 50.0 0.000

3 *�Deposit type 713.1 6 7.95 0.016

4 *�Depositor sex 709.3 6 4.14 0.110

Pack identity (n = 9) and original depositor identity (n = 69) were included as random terms. Model 1 best fits the data with the fewest explanatory

parameters and lowest AIC. k = parameters, Δi = AICi - AICmin, wi = Akaike weights. Dominance status categories: DB- dominants with breeding

intra-pack breeding opportunities; SB- subdominants with intra-pack breeding opportunities; SN- subdominants with no intra-pack breeding opportu-

nities. In models 2–4, ‘*’ refers to the minimal model with additional (+) or removed (�) terms.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 5

N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs

Page 6: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

function was predictable from context variables

when they were deposited; 97.1% were deposited

with no leg posture; 41.9% were deposited on bare

ground rather than vegetation; and faeces were

investigated only one-third as often as urine and

overmarked less than half as often. Only urine was

deposited in token quantities and at elevated fre-

quencies; while rates of defecation were similar for

both sexes and for dominants and subordinates. Uri-

nation accounted for >90% of all deposits by domi-

nants, and dominants urinated an order of

magnitude more frequently than subdominants,

probably because urination by dominants occurred

in smaller volumes. Token urination by dominants is

consistent with other canids (see Macdonald 1985)

and has been associated with scent-marking gener-

ally. During boundary patrols, Ethiopian wolves,

C. simensis, deposited token urinations but rarely def-

ecated, and raised leg urinations were more likely

than scats to be directed at other scent-marks (Sille-

ro-Zubiri & Macdonald 1998). In contrast to previous

work (Parker 2009), these results suggest that the

majority of African wild dog faeces may be of limited

importance as scent-marks.

Scent-marking among canids is typically carried

out by dominant animals, including territory holders

(coyote, C. latrans, Gese & Ruff 1997; wolves, Roth-

man & Mech 1979; Ryon & Brown 1990; domestic

dogs Lisberg & Snowdon 2011; Cafazzo et al. 2012).

Among mammals in general, responses to previous

deposits are commonly related to the relative domi-

nance status (meerkat, Suricatta suricata, Jordan

2007) or competitive ability (house mouse, Gosling

et al. 1996) of depositors and investigators. We found

the same for African wild dogs; a greater proportion

of dominant than subdominant urines was confirmed

as scent-marks by being overmarked, and dominants

were significantly more likely than subordinates to

overmark encountered deposits. As well as urinating

more frequently than subdominants, dominants were

also significantly more likely than subdominants to

adopt postures while urinating, and these deposits

were more likely to be investigated by other dogs

than those deposited with no posture. Of all initial

Table 2: GLMMs with binomial error distribution and logit link function investigating the factors that predict the likelihood of an investigated scent-

mark being overmarked (N = 259)

Model Description Estimate AIC k Δi wi

Basic – 580.4 6 35.7 0.000

1 Investigator sex Male 1.981 544.6 14 0.0 0.591

Female 0.000

+Investigator dominance status DB 0.000

SB �1.786

SN �2.238

+Top deposit type Urine 1.303

Faeces 0.000

+Top depositor sex Male 1.105

Female 0.000

+Investigator sex:Top depositor sex Male Male �1.867

Female 0.000

+Investigator sex:Investigator dominance status Male DB 0.000

SB �2.753

SN �2.037

2 *+Investigator sex:Investigator dominance status:Top depositor sex 546.1 18 1.4 0.289

3 *�Investigator sex: Investigator dominance status 548.7 12 4.1 0.078

4 *�Investigator sex:Top depositor sex 552.8 13 8.2 0.010

5 *�Top depositor sex � Investigator sex:Top depositor sex 551.0 12 6.4 0.024

6 *�Top deposit type 553.6 13 9.0 0.007

7 *�Investigator dominance status � Investigator sex: Investigator

dominance status

562.2 10 17.6 0.000

8 *�Investigator sex � Investigator sex: Top depositor sex � Investigator

sex: Investigator dominance status

556.1 10 11.5 0.002

Pack identity (n = 9), unique site (n = 579), investigator identity (n = 49), top depositor identity (n = 39) and original depositor identity (n = 36) were

included as random terms. Model 1 best fits the data with the fewest explanatory parameters and lowest AIC. k = parameters, Δi = AICi-AICmin,

wi = Akaike weights. Dominance status categories: DB- dominants with breeding intra-pack breeding opportunities; SB- subdominants with intra-pack

breeding opportunities; SN- subdominants with no intra-pack breeding opportunities. In models 2–8, ‘*’ refers to the minimal model with additional

(+) or removed (�) terms.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH6

Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt

Page 7: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

scent samples, urine deposited by dominant females

was the most likely to be investigated and over-

marked. This may be because the dominant pair

spends much of their time in a loose form of consort,

lying together during resting periods and associating

closely during pack movements and hunts (Creel &

Creel 2002), allowing dominants to regularly check

on and overmark their mate. However, work on

spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta) demonstrated that

females were more likely to investigate and anoint

themselves with scent from more dominant females

(Burgener et al. 2009), suggesting that remote dis-

crimination of dominance status from scents could

also occur.

Subdominant deposits were investigated less often

than dominant deposits, suggesting that subdomi-

nants may sometimes deposit in locations unlikely to

be investigated by pack-mates, or that subordinates’

deposits are less detectable, or perhaps less interesting

than dominants’ deposits. Subdominants occasionally

went to great lengths to avoid leaving detectable

deposits, by depositing directly into water. Such

apparently deliberate inconspicuous deposition is rela-

tively rare but has been previously described in aard-

wolves (Kruuk & Sands 1972), which conceal their

deposits by burying them. Additionally, subdominants

occasionally urinated without moving from their rest-

ing spot, or even standing up, while dominants

always roused themselves to distribute urine nearby.

Urinating while lying down is rarely described in any

species, but Smithers (1983) describes similar behav-

iour in a male lion, Panthera leo.

Urine rather than faeces and dominant rather than

subordinate deposits were more often placed on vege-

tation. Placing deposits on vegetation or other promi-

nent spots is common in the scent-marking behaviour

of many species (e.g. Ethiopian wolf, Sillero-Zubiri &

Macdonald 1998; Iberian wolf, Barja et al. 2005;

C. familiaris, Cafazzo et al. 2012) and is presumed to

increase the likelihood of detection by conspecifics.

Our data confirm that dominant urine deposited on

vegetation is more detectable; a greater proportion of

this urine was investigated and overmarked compared

with that deposited on bare ground.

Following investigation of a scent, animals have the

choice of adding to it with a scent of their own. Such

overmarking (sensu Johnston et al. 1994) is common

in many species (Ferkin & Pierce 2007), and it estab-

lishes unequivocally that both the initial deposit and

the overmark are scent-marks. Most overmarking was

undertaken by African wild dogs of the opposite sex

to the previous individual to deposit there, as in other

carnivores (e.g. banded mongoose, Mungos mungo,

Jordan et al. 2011; meerkat, Jordan 2007), which

may suggest a role in pair bond maintenance. The

overmarking responses of African wild dogs being

influenced by the attributes of the top deposit is con-

sistent with data from golden hamsters (Johnston

et al. 1994) and meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus

(Ferkin 1999), where top scent attributes appear to

mask (or reduce the importance of) overmarked

scents (e.g. Johnston et al. 1997). However, in wild

dogs the most parsimonious explanation may be that

each dominant preferentially over-marks its partner’s

scent rather than its own.

Within dominant pairs of wild dogs, each sex was

similarly likely to overmark encountered scents, while

among subdominants, females were more likely to

overmark than males. To our knowledge, such sex dif-

ferences in the response of subdominants to scents

have not been described previously. Interestingly,

subdominant females with breeding potential in the

pack were more likely to overmark than those with-

out breeding opportunities, suggesting a link between

reproductive access and scent-marking investment

(Jordan 2007; Mares et al. 2011). These patterns

might reflect competition for dominance status or the

advertisement of availability or sexual status to poten-

tial mates both within and outside the pack.

Our results show that not all African wild dog

deposits are scent-marks and those that are can be

recognised by the adoption of specific leg postures and

their being deposited on vegetation. The scent-mark-

ing role of dominant urine was confirmed by its being

more likely to be investigated and then overmarked

by other dogs. The data presented here establish that,

even in a species where scent-marking exclusively

involves urine and faeces, detailed observations of

elimination can be used to determine potential scent-

marks. Future work in scent-marking in other species

must distinguish simple eliminations from scent-

marks before assigning functional significance to

deposits.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the Botswana Ministry of Environ-

ment Wildlife and Tourism, Department of Wildlife

and National Parks for permission to conduct research

in Botswana and to various leaseholders for permis-

sion to work in certain areas, in particular the

Sankuyo community. Fieldwork was supported by a

grant from the Paul G. Allen Family Foundation and

Wild Entrust International. Additional data were

collected by Jessica Vitale, Dikatholo Kedikilwe and

Briana Abrahms.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 7

N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs

Page 8: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

Literature Cited

Akaike, H. 1974: New look at statistical-model identifica-

tion. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 19, 716—723.

Alberts, A. C. 1992: Constraints on the design of animal

communication systems in terrestrial vertebrates. Amer.

Nat. 139, S62—S89.

Altmann, J. 1974: Observational study of behavior: sam-

pling methods. Behaviour 69, 227—267.

Asa, C. S., Mech, L. D., Seal, U. S. & Plotka, E. D. 1990:

The influence of social and endocrine factors on urine-

marking by captive wolves (Canis lupus). Hormon.

Behav. 24, 497—509.

Barja, I. 2009: Decision making in plant selection during

the faecal-marking behaviour of wild wolves. Anim.

Behav. 77, 489—493.

Barja, I., deMiguel, F. J. & Barcena, F. 2004: The impor-

tance of crossroads in faecal marking behaviour of the

wolves (Canis lupus). Naturwissenschaften 91, 489—492.

Barja, I., de Miguel, F. J. & Barcena, F. 2005: Faecal mark-

ing behaviour of Iberian wolf in different zones of their

territory. Folia Zool. 54, 21—29.

Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrenkamp, S. L. 1998: Principles of

Animal Communication. Sinauer Associates, Massachu-

setts.

Burgener, N., Dehnhard, M., Hofer, H. & East, M. L. 2009:

Does anal gland scent signal identity in the spotted

hyaena? Anim. Behav. 77, 707—715.

Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. 2004: Multimodel

inference: understanding AIC and BIC in model selec-

tion. Sociol. Methods Res. 33, 261—304.

Cafazzo, S., Natoli, E. & Valsecchi, P. 2012: Scent-marking

behavior in a pack of free-ranging domestic dogs. Ethol-

ogy 118, 955—966.

Creel, S. & Creel, N. M. 2002: The African Wild Dog.

Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton.

Eisenberg, J. F. & Kleiman, D. G. 1972: Olfactory commu-

nication in mammals. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 3, 1—32.

Estes, R. D. 1997: The Behaviour Guide to African Mam-

mals. Univ. of California Press, California.

Ferkin, M. H. 1999: Meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus,

Arvicolidae) over-mark and adjacent-mark the scent

marks of same-sex conspecifics. Ethology 105,

825—839.

Ferkin, M. H. & Pierce, A. A. 2007: Perspectives on

over-marking: is it good to be on top? J. Ethol. 25, 107

—116.

Gese, E. M. & Ruff, R. L. 1997: Scent-marking by coyotes,

Canis latrans: the influence of social and ecological fac-

tors. Anim. Behav. 54, 1155—1166.

Gosling, L. M., Atkinson, N. W., Dunn, S. & Collins, S. A.

1996: The response of subordinate male mice to scent

marks varies in relation to their own competitive ability.

Anim. Behav. 52, 1185—1191.

Johnston, R. E., Chaing, G. & Tung, C. 1994: The informa-

tion in scent over-marks of golden hamsters. Anim.

Behav. 48, 323—330.

Johnston, R. E., Sorokin, E. S. & Ferkin, M. H. 1997:

Female voles discriminate males’ over-marks and prefer

top-scent males. Anim. Behav. 54, 679—690.

Jordan, N. R. 2007: Scent-marking investment is deter-

mined by sex and breeding status in wild meerkats.

Anim. Behav. 74, 531—540.

Jordan, N. R., Manser, M. B., Mwanguhya, F., Kyabulima,

S., R€uedi, P. & Cant, M. A. 2011: Scent marking in wild

banded mongooses I: sex specific scents and over-mark-

ing. Anim. Behav. 81, 31—42.

Kleiman, D. 1966: Scent marking in the Canidae. Symp.

Zool. Soc. Lond. 18, 167—177.

Kruuk, H. & Sands, W. A. 1972: The aardwolf (Proteles

cristatus Sparrman) 1783 as predator of termites. Afr.

J. Ecol. 10, 211—227.

Lisberg, A. E. & Snowdon, C. T. 2011: Effects of sex, social

status and gonadectomy on countermarking by domes-

tic dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim. Behav. 81, 757—764.

Macdonald, D. W. 1980: Patterns of scent marking with

urine and faeces amongst carnivore communities.

Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 45, 106—121.

Macdonald, D. W. 1985: The carnivores: order Carniv-

ora. In: Social Odours in Mammals (Brown, R. E.,

Macdonald, D. W., eds). Clarendon, Oxford, pp. 619—

722.

Malcolm, J. R. 1979: Social organization and communal

rearing in African wild dogs. Ph.D. thesis, Harvard

Univ.

Malcolm, J. R. & Marten, K. 1982: Natural selection

and the communal rearing of pups in African wild

dogs (Lycaon pictus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10, 1—

13.

Mares, R., Young, A. J., Levesque, D. L., Harrison, N. &

Clutton-Brock, T. H. 2011: Responses to intruder scents

in the cooperatively breeding meerkat: sex and social

status differences and temporal variation. Behav. Ecol.

22, 594—600.

McNutt, J. W. 1996: Sex-biased dispersal in African wild

dogs, Lycaon pictus. Anim. Behav. 52, 1067—1077.

Mills, M. G. L. & Gorman, M. L. 1997: Factors affecting the

density and distribution of wild dogs in the Kruger

National Park. Conserv. Biol. 11, 1397—1406.

Osofsky, S. A., McNutt, J. W. & Hirsch, K. J. 1996: Immo-

bilization of Free-Ranging African Wild Dogs (Lycaon

pictus) Using a ketamine/xylazine/Atropine Combina-

tion. J. Zoo. Wildl. Med. 27, 528—532.

Parker, M. N. 2009: Territoriality and scent marking behavior

of African wild dogs in northern Botswana. PhD thesis,

Univ. of Montana, Missoula.

Peters, R. P. & Mech, L. D. 1975: Scent-marking in wolves.

Am. Sci. 63, 628—637.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH8

Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt

Page 9: Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour … · Scent-Mark Identification and Scent-Marking Behaviour in African Wild Dogs (Lycaon pictus) Neil R. Jordan*†, Krystyna

Rich, T. J. & Hurst, J. L. 1999: The competing countermarks

hypothesis: reliable assessment of competitive ability by

potential mates. Anim. Behav. 58, 1027—1037.

Rothman, R. J. & Mech, L. D. 1979: Scent-marking in lone

wolves and newly formed pairs. Anim. Behav. 27, 750

—760.

Ryon, J. &Brown,R. E. 1990: Urinemarking in female

wolves (Canis lupus): an indicator of dominance status and

reproductive state. In: Chemical Signals in Vertebrates V

(Madconald, D.W.,Muller-Schwarze, D. &Natynczuk,

S., eds). OxfordUniv. Press, Oxford, pp. 346—351.

Sillero-Zubiri, C. & Macdonald, D. W. 1998: Scent-mark-

ing and territorial behaviour of Ethiopian wolves Canis

simensis. J. Zool. 245, 351—361.

Sliwa, A. & Richardson, P. R. K. 1998: Responses of

aardwolves, Proteles cristatus, Sparrman 1783, to

translocated scent marks. Anim. Behav. 56,

137—146.

Smithers, R. H. N. 1983: The Mammals of the Southern

African Subregion. Univ. of Pretoria, Pretoria.

Spiering, P. A., Somers, M. J., Maldonado, J. E., Wildt, D.

E. & Gunther, M. S. 2010: Reproductive sharing and

proximate factors mediating cooperative breeding in the

African wild dog (Lycaon pictus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

64, 583—592.

Woodroffe, R. 2011: Ranging behaviour of African wild

dog packs in a human-dominated landscape. J. Zool.

283, 88—97.

Ethology 119 (2013) 1–9 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 9

N. R. Jordan, K. A. Golabek, P. J. Apps, G. D. Gilfillan & J. W. McNutt Scent-Marks in African Wild Dogs