Top Banner

of 24

Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

Jun 04, 2018

Download

Documents

yochaiataria
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    1/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    2/24

    genuinely be considered shamanism. Furthermore, the personality traits thatinfluence individual susceptibility to shamanic-like techniques are unclear. Theaim of the present study was, thus, to investigate experimentally the effect ofshamanic-like techniques and a personality trait referred to as ego boundaries

    on subjective experience including mood disturbance. Forty-three non-shamanswere administered a composite questionnaire consisting of demographic itemsand a measure of ego boundaries (i.e., the Short Boundary Questionnaire; BQ-Sh). Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: listening tomonotonous drumming for 15 minutes coupled with one of two sets of journeyinginstructions; or sitting quietly with eyes closed for 15 minutes. Participantssubjective experience and mood disturbance were retrospectively assessed usingthe Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) and the Profile of MoodStates-Short Form, respectively. The results indicated that there was a statistically

    significant difference between conditions with regard to the PCI majordimensions of visual imagery, attention and rationality, and minor dimensions ofimagery amount and absorption. However, the shamanic-like conditions were notassociated with a major reorganization of the pattern of subjective experiencecompared to the sitting quietly condition, suggesting that what is typicallyreferred to as an altered state of consciousness effect was not evident. Oneshamanic-like condition and the BQ-Sh subscales need for order, childlikeness,and sensitivity were statistically significant predictors of total mood disturbance.

    Implications of the findings for the anthropology of consciousness are alsoconsidered.k e y w o r d s : shamanic, personality, ego boundaries, subjective experience, mood

    &

    i n t r o d u c t i o n

    Shamanism may be referred to as a body of techniques and activities thatsupposedly enable its practitioners to access information that is not ordinarily

    attainable by members of the social group that gave them privileged status(Krippner 2002:962). Noll (1985:45) states that shamanic techniques (e.g., mo-notonous drumming, hallucinogens, sweat lodges) are often used to cultivate

    enhanced visual mental imagery, particularly during journeying (i.e., soulflight). The shamans mental imagery tends to reflect her learned cosmology

    (Walsh 1995): a universe consisting of multiple worlds such as a lower world(land of the dead), upper world (heaven or sky), and middle world or earth

    (Eliade 1964). However, Rock and Krippner (2007) state that it would be anovergeneralization to assert that all shamanic cultures liken the lower world

    to the land of the dead, and the upper world to sky. For instance, Lepp(2004) contends that Moso shamans of Tibet supposed that souls ascend toheaven rather than descend to the lower world.

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 6 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    3/24

    Bittman et al. (2001) suggest that although shamanic techniques have formed

    the basis of indigenous healing ceremonies since antiquity, such practices haveonly recently begun to generate interest as a complementary therapeutic strat-

    egy in the traditional medical and psychological arenas. One particularly

    relevant study in this context was conducted by Harner and Tryon (1995) whofound that the mood disturbance of 40 experienced shamanic practitioners

    (measured by the Profile of Mood States questionnaire; POMS) was signifi-cantly lower after journeying with drumming compared to baseline. Given the

    therapeutic potential of shamanism (Krippner 2002), it may be useful to inves-tigate experimentally the subjective effects of these practices on non-shamans

    (Rock et al. 2007).Not surprisingly then, recent experimental studies (e.g., Rock et al. 2005,

    2006; Rock 2006) have aimed to assess the effect of shamanic-like techniques

    on university students. Rock et al. (2007:80) suggest that, Techniques may beconceptualized as shamanic-like insofar as they bear some relation to sha-

    manic techniques and yet depart from what may properly be calledshamanism. For example, drinkingayahuascain order to descend to the lowerworld and retrieve a tribal members soul may be considered a shamanictechnique, whereas recreationally usingayahuascato produce purported alter-ations in consciousness is merely shamanic-like.

    It is noteworthy, however, that previous research has not investigated whether

    the therapeutic effects (i.e., the reduction in mood disturbance quantified bythe POMS) reported by Harner and Tryon (1995) may be replicated with a non-shaman population. Indeed, it may be edifying to quantitatively measure the

    degree to which a particular shamanic-like technique (i.e., drumming) affectsmood disturbance when used by non-shamans, thus, providing an indication ofthe therapeutic potential of shamanic practices.

    Additionally, it is salient that previous experimental research has tended touse shamanic-like techniques to facilitate journeying by non-shamans to the

    lower world (e.g., Rock et al. 2005, 2006). It may be such that the therapeutic

    potential of journeying to one world may outweigh journeying to anotherworld. For example, Walsh (1990:147) suggests that, broadly speaking, thelower world is a place of tests and challenges where one may be required

    to battle predatory creatures (e.g., jaguars) and traverse treacherous obstacles(e.g., infernal rivers). In contrast, the upper world may be populated with

    celestial beings and akin to a heavenly realm (Kalweit 1988). Consequently, onemight expect that journeying to the lower world may be associated with more

    negative mood (i.e., mood disturbance) compared to journeying to the upperworld. This hypothesis was not tested by Harner and Tryon (1995:92); instead

    participants were merely instructed to lie down and make a journey for thepurpose of raising a specific immune response, rather than travel to a spe-cific cosmological destination.

    6 2 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    4/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    5/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    6/24

    2. Is there a difference between the psygrams for each of the three conditions?3. Do condition and ego boundaries predict total mood disturbance scores?

    &

    m e t h o dParticipants

    A total of 43 undergraduate students enrolled at Charles Sturt University partici-

    pated in the present study (17 males and 26 females). The mean age for males was20.29 (SD5 2.71) and the mean age for females was 22.46 (SD5 6.40).

    Participation in the present study was voluntary. Participants were treated in

    accordance with American Psychological Association ethical standards.

    Materials (or Apparatus)

    Participants were provided with a pencil-and-paper precondition questionnaireand postcondition questionnaire. The precondition questionnaire consisted of

    demographic items (e.g., age, gender) and the short version of the BoundaryQuestionnaire (BQ-Sh). The postcondition questionnaire consisted of the PCI

    and the Profile of Mood States-Short Form (POMS-SF).The Rawlings (20012002) BQ-Sh was used to quantify the thinness of ego

    boundaries. The BQ-Sh is an empirically derived shortened version of the

    145-item Hartmann (1991) Boundary Questionnaire (BQ). The BQ-Sh consists

    of 46 items with a 5-point Likert-type scale, and produces six subscales: unusualexperiences, need for order, trust, perceived competence, childlikeness, andsensitivity. The BQ-Sh has adequate psychometric properties as evidenced by

    Rawlings empirical examination of the scale, and it can, thus, be considered asatisfactory alternative to the BQ. For example, the BQ-Sh strongly correlates

    with the BQ (r5 0.88) and the a coefficients for the BQ-Sh subscales rangefrom 0.69 to 0.80 (Rawlings 20012002).

    The 30-item POMS-SF (McNair et al. 1992) was used to quantify total mooddisturbance. The scale provides a total mood disturbance score plus additional

    scores for six subscales: tension/anxiety, depression/dejection, anger/hostility,vigor/activity, fatigue/inertia, and confusion/bewilderment. The POMS-SF hasrespectable psychometric properties (McNair et al. 1992).

    The 53-item PCI (Pekala 1991) was used to quantify subjective experience.Items cover 26 dimensions including 12 major (e.g., positive affect, altered

    experience, attention, volitional control, arousal) and 14 minor (e.g., joy, alteredbody image, vividness of imagery, absorption) dimensions (Pekala and Levine

    1981, 1982; Pekala and Kumar 1984, 1986; Pekala 1985). Participants wererequired to respond on a 7-point Likert-type scale (Pekala and Wenger 1983;

    Pekala et al. 1985). The PCI possesses adequate psychometric properties. Forexample, Pekala et al. (1986) reported coefficient as between 0.70 and 0.90 for alldimensions, suggesting that the PCI has good internal consistency. In support

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 6 5

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    7/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    8/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    9/24

    the form of supplying participants with a written statement outlining the de-

    tailed purposes of the research, participants being afforded the opportunity toask on-site researchers questions about the research, and participants being re-

    directed to the phone numbers of the Chief Investigator and appropriate Ethics

    Secretariat for further questions.

    &

    r e s u l t s

    Research Question 1 (a) and (b): PCI Major and Minor Dimension DifferencesTo address Research Question 1, and to avoid violating the assumption of mul-ticollinearity, separate multivariate analyses were performed on the PCI majorand minor dimension scores (Woodside et al. 1997). A one-way between-sub-

    jects multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) revealed a statisticallysignificant difference between conditions with regard to the combined PCI

    major dimension variables,F(24, 58) 5 3.98,p 5 .000; Pillais trace 5 1.24; par-tial eta squared (partial Z2) 5 .62. Subsequently, an a5 .01 was used to assess

    the separate univariate effects (Woodside et al. 1997). Separate univariate ana-lyses revealed statistically significant results for visual imagery, F(2, 39) 5 7.32,p5 .002; attention,F(2, 39)5 6.11,p5 .005; and rationality,F(2, 39)5 10.11,p5 .000. As can be seen from Table 1, the U-Group reported significantly lower

    visual imagery intensity ratings compared to both the L-Group and the C-Group; the U-Group reported significantly lower attention intensity ratingscompared to the C-Group; and both the U-Group and the L-Group reported

    significantly lower rationality intensity ratings compared to the C-Group.Pallant (2001:219) asserts that for a reliable multivariate equation the number

    of cases in each cell must equal or exceed the number of dependent variables.Given that the present study examined 14 PCI minor dimension-dependent vari-

    ables with a sample size of 43 (consisting of between 12 to 17 cases per cell), twoseparate multivariate analyses were conducted for the PCI minor dimensions.

    A one-way between-subjects MANOVA revealed a statistically significantdifference between conditions with regard to the combined PCI minor di-mension variables of altered time sense, altered perception, altered meaning,

    imagery amount, vividness, direction and absorption, F (14, 68) 54.33,p5 .000; Pillais trace 5 0.94; partialZ25 .47. Separate univariate analyses re-vealed statistically significant results for imagery amount, F (2, 39) 5 11.01,p5 .000; and absorption,F(2, 39) 5 6.78,p 5 .003. As can be seen from Table1, both the U-Group and the C-Group reported significantly lower imageryamount intensity ratings compared to the L-Group; and the U-Group reported

    significantly lower absorption intensity ratings compared to the C-Group.In contrast, a one-way between-subjects MANOVA revealed no statistically

    significant difference between conditions with regard to the combined PCI

    6 8 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    10/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    11/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    12/24

    there is an increase in altered state, arousal, memory, and vivid imagery; a de-crease in rationality, self-awareness, internal dialogue, volitional control, and

    inward absorbed attention; and no change in positive affect, negative affect, oraltered experience. As regards pattern relationships, the configurations evident

    in Figure 2 are noticeably different from those depicted in Figure 1. Thestrength of the relationship between negative affect and arousal has doubled,

    and the strength of the relationship between memory and self-awareness hasdecreased. Additionally, rationality and memory; altered experience and posi-

    tive affect and self-awareness; and self-awareness and volitional control are allnow strongly coupled. Furthermore, it is evident that many of the pattern rela-tionships present in the C-Group are no longer present in the L-Group.

    fi g u r e 2 . p s y g r a m a s s o c i a t e d w i t h t h e l - g r o u p .

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 7 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    13/24

    Figure 3 depicts the pattern of relationships between pairs of PCI major di-mensions reported by the U-Group. With the exception of self-awareness,

    volitional control, vivid imagery, and internal dialogue (which have increased),and memory and inward absorbed attention (which have decreased), intensity

    ratings across the 12 major PCI dimensions are identical to the L-Group. De-spite this finding, several changes in pattern relationships are evident. Altered

    state is now strongly coupled with altered experience, inward absorbed atten-tion, and arousal. Internal dialogue is now strongly coupled with rationality and

    self-awareness; whereas volitional control is strongly coupled with vivid imageryand negative affect; and altered experience is strongly coupled with inwardabsorbed attention.

    fi g u r e 3 . p s y g r a m a s s o c i a t e d w i t h t h e u - g r o u p .

    7 2 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    14/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    15/24

    table2

    :

    standard

    multipler

    egression

    of

    u-group

    ,l-group,unusual

    ex

    periences,need

    foro

    rder,

    trust,

    perceived

    competenc

    e,childlikeness,and

    sensitivityon

    totalm

    ood

    disturbance

    Var

    iables

    TMD

    (DV)

    U-

    Group

    L

    -Group

    UE

    NFO

    Tr

    PC

    Ch

    Se

    B

    b

    Sr

    2

    (un

    ique)

    U-Group

    0.22

    1.00

    3.41

    .3

    0

    .07

    L-Group

    0.13

    0.42

    1.00

    3.72

    0.2

    4

    UE

    0.1

    5

    0.18

    0.26

    1.00

    0.23

    0.1

    3

    NFO

    0.43

    0.11

    0.10

    0.2

    4

    1.00

    0.22

    0.3

    3

    0.08

    Tr

    0.18

    0.07

    0.06

    0.22

    0.18

    1.00

    0.4

    5

    0.1

    9

    PC

    0.03

    0.02

    0.07

    0.01

    0.27

    0.26

    1.00

    0.28

    0.2

    0

    Ch

    0.11

    0.1

    5

    0.2

    4

    0.47

    0.18

    0.02

    0.02

    1.00

    0.52

    0.3

    6

    0.13

    Se

    0.43

    0.08

    0.09

    0.16

    0.42

    0.01

    0.31

    0.29

    1.00

    0.8

    4

    0.4

    0

    0.16

    Intercept

    5

    14.62

    M

    11.10

    0.33

    0.26

    17.21

    20.33

    10.9

    5

    17.62

    13.31

    4.33

    R2

    5.5

    0

    SD

    11.69

    0.48

    0.4

    5

    7.67

    7.43

    3.49

    5.8

    5

    3.29

    2.07

    Adjusted

    R2

    5.3

    7

    R5.7

    0

    UE5U

    nusua

    lExperience;NFO5

    Nee

    dfor

    Order;Tr5

    Trust;PC5

    Perceive

    dCompet

    ence;

    Ch5

    Childlikeness;

    Se5

    Sensitiv-

    ity;TMD5

    TotalMoo

    dDisturbance.

    po.05

    ;po.0

    1.

    7 4 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    16/24

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    17/24

    decrease during shaman-like stimulus conditions relative to the control condi-

    tion (see Table 1), participants with a high need for order may have found thatshamanic-like techniques facilitated feelings of, for example, confusion or be-

    wilderment that contributed to total mood disturbance.

    Childlikeness was significantly negatively correlated with total mood distur-bance, suggesting that participants who have thicker boundaries in the sense

    of childlikeness reported more total mood disturbance compared to thinnerboundary counterparts. Childlikeness reflects the tendency to hold beliefs

    about the importance of remaining childlike (e.g., function without strictroles; Rawlings 20012002:140). It is arguable that participants who have thin

    boundaries in the sense of childlikeness are more open than their thickerboundary counterparts and, thus, less likely to become distressed by potentiallynovel stimuli (e.g., subjective experiences associated with exposure to

    shamanic-like stimulus conditions).Sensitivity was significantly positively correlated with total mood disturbance,

    indicating that participants who have thin boundaries with regard to sensi-tivity reported more total mood disturbance compared to thick boundary

    counterparts. Sensitivity involves, for example, the tendency to be easilyhurt, or the feeling that one is falling apart (Rawlings 20012002:134). Walsh(1995) suggests that the affect associated with journeying is partly contingent on

    the content of the journeying imagery encountered. Imagery may have

    more impact on highly sensitive participants and, thus, result in higher affectintensity ratings compared to less sensitive participants.The preceding findings suggest that the concept of ego boundaries is a com-

    plex one; apparently the positive correlation between ego boundaries in one sense(e.g., sensitivity) and total mood disturbance is not indicative of a positive corre-lation between ego boundaries in all other senses and total mood disturbance.

    It is also noteworthy that the upper world stimulus condition was positivelycorrelated with mood disturbance in the present study, suggesting that, for

    example, participants in the U-Group reported more mood disturbance than

    their C-Group and L-Group counterparts. This result is broadly inconsistentwith Harner and Tryons (1995) finding that journeying was accompanied by astatistically significant reduction in mood disturbance compared to a baseline

    condition. The disparity may be explained, at least in part, by the fact thatHarner and Tryon (1995) used experienced shamanic practitioners whereas

    the present study used non-shamans. That is to say, shamanic-like techniquesmay be counterproductive if applied in the absence of shamanic training (e.g.,

    learning a cosmology, cultivating a mastery over mental images; Walsh 1995).Indeed, it is possible that some non-shamans found, for instance, the cultivation

    of specific mental imagery sequences rather demanding thereby elicitingfatigue or inertia whereas others may have been overwhelmed by what maybe conceptualized as a novel stimulus condition, and reported confusion, or

    7 6 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    18/24

    bewilderment. This partial explanation is supported by the finding that the

    lower world stimulus condition was also positively correlated with mood dis-turbance in the present study. However, it is arguable that mood disturbance

    may dissipate with intermittent exposure to shamanic technologies (e.g., mo-

    notonous drumming) over an extended period. Consequently, it might beadvantageous to compareFvia retrospective quantitative assessmentFthe

    upper and lower world journeying experiences (including fluctuatingmood states) of shamans versus non-shamans.

    It is perhaps also salient that Harner and Tryon (1995) created a contextualvariable (i.e., instructing participants to embark upon a journey for the purpose

    of raising a specific immune response), whereas our participants were blind tothe studys aims. The contextual variable may have linguistically conditionedparticipants to, for example, report subjective experiences implicitly held to be

    consistent with raising a specific immune response (e.g., increases in positiveaffect). It might be beneficial to partially replicate the present study by com-

    paring non-shamans who are informed with regard to the purpose of thejourney with non-shamans who are blind to the purpose.

    Implications for the Anthropology of ConsciousnessThe results of the present study suggest that Pekalas (1991) methodology is a

    valuable means of investigating the subjective effects of shamanic-like stimulus

    conditions. The PCI has the advantage of allowing one to quantify, diagram,and thus comparatively analyze the intensity and pattern of PCI dimensionsassociated with a variety of stimulus conditions. Consequently, anthropology of

    consciousness scholars may benefit from applying this methodology to otherconsciousness phenomena (e.g., trance, possession, and dissociative states).

    The finding that various BQ-Sh subscales (e.g., need for order) were statisti-

    cally significant predictors of total mood disturbance suggests, in general terms,

    that ones personality traits may influence ones receptivity to changes in mood.This salient result may encourage anthropology of consciousness researchers to

    consider the importance of personality when investigating the subjective expe-rience of what are typically referred to as altered states of consciousness reportedby, for example, shamans, mediums, and mystics.

    Possible Methodological Limitations and Suggestions for Future ResearchOne important caveat in this study is that one cannot control for the length oftime it takes participants to enter the upper and lower world. Consequent-

    ly, some participants may have spent longer descending to the lower world orascending to the upper world and, thus, less time traversing the landscapes

    than others. This extraneous variable may account for some of the within-groupand between-group variability associated with various PCI dimension intensityratings (e.g., imagery amount).

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 7 7

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    19/24

    Furthermore, the present studys sample size (N543; n 5 1317) was toosmall to allow very thin versus very thick boundary comparisons withineach condition with regards to subjective experience. It is arguable that by re-

    moving the second and third quartile of the total boundary scores and, thus,

    retaining only the very thin and very thick boundary participants, themagnitude of the treatment effect with regard to variables quantified by the

    PCI and the POMS-SF would have been larger.Hartmann (1991) suggests that thin and thick boundaries constitute a broad

    personality dimension that encapsulates measures such as absorption, opennessto experience, and schizotypy. It may be edifying to partially replicate the pres-

    ent study to investigate whether the inclusion of personality measuresconceptually related to the boundary construct serves to account for a largeramount of variability in total mood disturbance.

    Traditionally, shamans have used a plethora of techniques to facilitatejourneying experiences (e.g., plant hallucinogens, sweat lodges, pain rituals,

    sensory deprivation) (Jilek 1982; Achterberg 1987; Dobkin de Rios and Winkel-man 1989). It may prove useful to partially replicate the present study by

    incorporating a variety of shamanic technologies rather than merely monoto-nous drumming.

    &

    c o n c l u s i o n s

    Overall, stimulus conditions consisting of sitting quietly with eyes closed andshamanic-like techniques were associated with statistically significant differ-

    ences in subjective experience. Notably, there was a significant differencebetween the U-Group and the L-Group with regard to the PCI major dimen-

    sion of visual imagery and the PCI minor dimension of imagery amount.However, the shamanic-like stimulus conditions were not associated with a

    major reorganization of the pattern of PCI major dimensions compared to thecontrol condition, suggesting that what is typically referred to as an altered stateof consciousness effect was not evident. These results suggest that Pekalas

    (1991) methodology is a valuable means of examining the subjective effects ofshamanic-like stimulus conditions. Finally, condition and various BQ-Sh sub-

    scales were significant predictors of total mood disturbance. Specifically, theU-Group, childlikeness, need for order, and sensitivity made the largest unique

    contribution to explaining the variance in total mood disturbance. This findingsuggests that shamanic-like techniques may be counterproductive if applied in

    the absence of shamanic training. Furthermore, this finding emphasizes theimportance of evaluating participants personality traits when investigating thesubjective effects of shamanic-like techniques.

    7 8 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    20/24

    &

    n o t e s

    1 Monotonous drumming at eight beats per second for 15 minutes was used in the

    present study because Rock et al. (2005) found that that it was associated with a sta-tistically significantly higher number of ostensibly shamanic journeying images

    reported by non-shamans compared to a control condition, whereas, for example,

    four beats per second for 10 or 15 minutes and eight beats per second for 10 minutes

    were not.

    2 The type of control condition used for consciousness studies is a contentious one.

    The primary area of dissension between authors is the issue of sitting or lying, eyes

    open or closed. As regards to sitting versus lying, Harner (1995:258) advocates the

    latter, utilizing a control condition referred to as the resting condition in which

    participants were instructed to, Just lie down and rest for about 20 minutes. Ha-rners (1995:103) rationale for this condition was to allow participants to reach

    baseline levels prior to exposure to the active treatments and to have such a compar-

    ison condition in addition to their off the street baseline measures. Similarly,

    Harner and Tryons (1995) control, or baseline, condition involved lying on the floor

    for 30 minutes resting. Conversely, Pekala (1991) sets a strong precedence for a sitting

    control condition. Pekala (1991) outlines several studies relating to hypnosis, absorp-

    tion, and out-of-body experiences, all utilizing the sitting control condition.

    Correspondingly, Pekala et al. (1985) use sitting quietly with eyes open as their con-

    trol condition, arguing that this is a condition similar to normal wakingconsciousness. Pekala (1991) discussed extensively the issue of whether a control con-

    dition should involve participants having their eyes open or closed. In research

    developing the Dimensions of Attention Questionnaire, Pekala (1991) performed a

    comparative analysis of the conditions of sitting quietly with eyes open and sitting

    quietly with eyes closed. Pekala (1991) argues that sitting with eyes open simulated

    normal waking consciousness, while sitting with eyes closed was akin to daydream-

    ing. Results of this study showed few phenomenological, attentional, or intensity

    differences between these conditions on the PCI, a result Pekala (1991) interpreted as

    suggesting that the PCI was unable to make such subtle discriminations. Though no

    between-group differences were found, Pekala (1991) did find, however, significantdifferences across the same participants between having eyes open and closed. Sub-

    sequent analysis performed by Pekala (1991) identified the conditions of eyes open

    and eyes closed as identity differing (I-D) states in reference to each other. Pekala

    (1991:231) defines identity states (I-states) as those where stimulus conditions have

    the same (nonsignificantly different) intensities and patterns of phenomenological

    experience. I-D states are those with the same pattern structure but varying intensity

    values for some sub-dimensions. At the conclusion of this discussion, Pekala (1991)

    suggests that the issue is in need of further analysis and subsequently has used both

    eyes open and eyes closed on several occasions.

    3 Psygrams are graphical representations of the squared coefficients of the correlation

    matrices, also depicting the dimension intensity scores.

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 7 9

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    21/24

    4 The Jenrich (1970) test is the appropriate statistical procedure to assess pattern differ-

    ences associated with the 12 major dimensions of the PCI (Pekala 1991). However,

    Pekala (1991:235) asserts that the Jenrich test is a large-sample multivariate proce-

    dure requiring a minimum of 60 participants per condition (provided that

    all 12 major dimensions of the PCI are being examined). Given that the presentstudy consisted of between 12 and 20 participants per group, the Jenrich test

    was not appropriate. Consequently, a Box Test comparison was performed (Pekala

    1991).

    5 The Box Test is typically held to be overly sensitive with regard to the detection of

    differences between independent correlation matrices. Consequently, convention

    dictates that the a level associated with the Box Test should be set at po.001

    (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007).

    6 Coakes (2005) states that categorical variables may be entered as predictors in a

    standard multiple regression equation provided that they are recoded asdummy variables.

    r e f e r e n c e s

    Achterberg, Jeanne1987 The Shaman: Master Healer in the Imaginary Realm.InShamanism: AnExpanded View of Reality. Shirley Nicholson, ed. Pp. 103124. Wheaton, Ill: TheTheosophical Publishing House.

    Bittman, B.B. with L. Berk, D. Felten, J. Westengard, C. Simonton and J. Pappas

    2001 Composite Effects of Group Drumming Music Therapy on Modulation ofNeuroendocrine-immune Parameters in Normal Subjects. Alternative Therapiesin Health and Medicine 7:3847.

    Coakes, Sheridan2005 SPSS Version 12 for Windows: Analysis Without Anguish. Milton, QLD: Wiley.

    Dobkin de Rios, Marlene and Michael Winkelman1989 Shamanism and Altered States of Consciousness: An Introduction. Journal ofPsychoactive Drugs 21:17.

    Eliade, Mircea

    1964 Shamanism: Archaic Techniques of Ecstasy. London: Arkana.

    Funkhouser, A. with O. Wurmle, C. Cornu and M. Bahro2001 Boundary Questionnaire Results in the Mentally Healthy Elderly. Dreaming,11(2):8388.

    Harner, Michael1990 The Way of the Shaman 3rd edition. New York: HarperCollins.

    Harner, Sandra.1995 Immune and Affect Response to Shamanic Drumming. Ph.D. dissertation,Department of Psychology, Fordham University.

    Harner, Sandra and Warren Tryon1995 Psychological and Immunological Responses to Shamanic Journeying withDrumming. Shaman 12:8997.

    8 0 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    22/24

    Hartmann, Ernest1991 Boundaries in the Mind. New York: BasicBooks.

    Hartmann, Ernest with R. Harrison and M. Zborowski2001 Boundaries in the Mind: Past Research and Future Directions. North American

    Journal of Psychology 3:347368.Ingerman, Sandra

    2004 Shamanic Journeying: A Beginners Guide. Boulder: Sounds True.

    Jenrich, J1970 Asymptotic Chi-square Test for the Equality of Two Correlation Matrices.Journal of the American Statistical Association 65:904912.

    Jilek, W1982 Altered States of Consciousness in North American Indian Ceremonials. Ethos10(4):326342.

    Kalweit, Holger1988 Dreamtime and Innerspace: The World of the Shaman. Boston: Shambhala.

    Krippner, Stanley2002 Conflicting Perspectives on Shamans and Shamanism: Points andCounterpoints. American Psychologist 57(11):962977.

    Krippner, Stanley with L. Wickramasekera, J. Wickramasekera and C. Winstead1998 The Ramtha Phenomenon: Psychological, Phenomenological, &Geomagnetic data. The Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research92:124.

    Lepp, T.2004 Psychopomp.InShamanism: An Encyclopedia of World Beliefs, Practices, andCulture (Vol. 1). M. Walter and E. Fridman, ed. Pp. 217218. Santa Barbara, CA:

    ABC-CLIO.

    McNair, D. with M. Lorr and L. Droppleman1992 POMS: Profile of Mood States. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

    Noll, Richard1985 Mental Imagery Cultivation as a Cultural Phenomenon: The Role of Visions inShamanism. Current Anthropology 26:443461.

    Pallant, Julie2001 SPSS Survival Manual: A Step by Step Guide to Data Analysis Using SPSS.Crows Nest: Allen & Unwin.

    Pekala, Ronald J.1985 A Psychophenomenological Approach to Mapping and Diagramming States ofConsciousness. Journal of Religion and Psychical Research 8:199214.

    1991 Quantifying Consciousness: An Empirical Approach. New York: Plenum Press.

    Pekala, Ronald J. and V.K. Kumar1984 Predicting Hypnotic Susceptibility by a Self-Report Phenomenological State

    Instrument. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 27:114121.

    1986 The Differential Organization of the Structures of Consciousness DuringHypnosis and a Baseline Condition. Journal of Mind and Behavior 7:515540.

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 8 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    23/24

    Pekala, Ronald J. and R.L. Levine1981 Mapping Consciousness: Development of an Empirical-Phenomenological

    Approach. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality. The Scientific Study ofConsciousness 1:2947.

    1982 Quantifying States of Consciousness via an Empirical-phenomenologicalApproach. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality. The Scientific Study ofConsciousness 2:5171.

    Pekala, Ronald J. with J. Steinberg and C.K. Kumar1986 Measurement of Phenomenological Experience: Phenomenology ofConsciousness Inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills 63:983989.

    Pekala, Ronald J. and C.F. Wenger1983 Retrospective Phenomenological Assessment: Mapping Consciousness inReference to Specific Stimulus Conditions. Journal of Mind and Behavior

    4:247274.Pekala, Ronald J. with C.F. Wenger and R.L. Levine

    1985 Individual Differences in Phenomenological Experience: States ofConsciousness as a Function of Absorption. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology 48:125132.

    Rawlings, Douglas2001-2002 An Exploratory Factor Analysis of Hartmanns Boundary Questionnaireand an Empirically-Derived Short Version. Imagination, Cognition and Personality21:131144.

    Rock, Adam J.2006 Phenomenological Analysis of Experimentally Induced Visual Mental ImageryAssociated with Shamanic Journeying to the Lower World. International Journal ofTranspersonal Studies 25:4555.

    Rock, Adam J. with Gavin R. Abbott, Hatun Childargushi and Melanie L. Kiehne2007. The Effect of Shamanic-like Stimulus Conditions and the Cognitive-perceptual Factor of Schizotypy on Phenomenology. North American Journal ofPsychology 10(1):7998.

    Rock, Adam J. with Peter B. Baynes and Paul J. Casey2005 Experimental Study of Ostensibly Shamanic Journeying Imagery in NaiveParticipants I: Antecedents. Anthropology of Consciousness 16(2):7292.

    Rock, Adam J. with Paul J. Casey and Peter B. Baynes2006 Experimental Study of Ostensibly Shamanic Journeying Imagery in NaiveParticipants II: Phenomenological Mapping and Modified Affect Bridge.

    Anthropology of Consciousness 17(1):6583.

    Rock, Adam J. and Nicolas Kambouropoulos2007 Toward a Phenomenology of Urge to Drink: A Future Prospectfor the Cue-reactivity Paradigm. North American Journal of Psychology 9(2):387406.

    Rock, Adam J. and Stanley Krippner2007 Shamanism and the Confusion of Consciousness with PhenomenologicalContent. North American Journal of Psychology 9(3):485500.

    8 2 a n t h r o p o l o g y o f c o n s c i o u s n e s s 1 9 . 1

  • 8/14/2019 Ego Boundaries, Shamanic-Like

    24/24

    Tabachnick, Barbara G. and Linda S. Fidell2007 Using Multivariate Statistics (5th edition.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Tart, Charles T.1975 States of Consciousness. New York: Dutton.

    Walsh, Roger N.1990 The Spirit of Shamanism. Los Angeles: J.P. Tarcher.

    1995 Phenomenological Mapping: A Method for Describing and Comparing Statesof Consciousness. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology 27(1):2556.

    Woodside, Lisa N. with V.K. Kumar and Ronald J. Pekala1997 Monotonous Percussion Drumming and Trance Postures: A ControlledEvaluation of Phenomenological Effects. Anthropology of Consciousness 8(23):6987.

    a n e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d y 8 3