Top Banner
Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 1 Topic = Cognitive Processes INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING ABILITY AND ADAPTABILITY Brent Morgan 1 , Sidney D’Mello 2 , Robert Abbott 3 , Gabriel Radvansky 2 , Michael Haass 3 , and Andrea Tamplin 2 1 University of Memphis Memphis, TN, USA 2 University of Notre Dame Notre Dame, IN, USA 3 Sandia National Labs Albuquerque, NM, USA Word Count: 4747 Reference Word Count: 1391 Corresponding Author Brent Morgan 202 Psychology Building 400 Innovation Drive University of Memphis Memphis, TN 38152
34

APA Format 6th Edition Template

Dec 21, 2016

Download

Documents

nguyentu
Welcome message from author
This document is posted to help you gain knowledge. Please leave a comment to let me know what you think about it! Share it to your friends and learn new things together.
Transcript
Page 1: APA Format 6th Edition Template

Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 1

Topic = Cognitive Processes

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING ABILITY AND ADAPTABILITY

Brent Morgan1, Sidney D’Mello2, Robert Abbott3, Gabriel Radvansky2, Michael Haass3, and

Andrea Tamplin2

1University of Memphis

Memphis, TN, USA

2University of Notre Dame Notre Dame, IN, USA

3Sandia National Labs

Albuquerque, NM, USA

Word Count: 4747 Reference Word Count: 1391 Corresponding Author Brent Morgan 202 Psychology Building 400 Innovation Drive University of Memphis Memphis, TN 38152

Page 2: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 1

Structured Abstract

Objective: To identify the cognitive factors that predict ability and adaptability during

multitasking with a flight simulator.

Background: Multitasking has become increasingly prevalent as most professions

require individuals to perform multiple tasks simultaneously. Considerable research has

attempted to identify the characteristics of people (i.e., individual differences) that predict

multitasking ability. Although working memory is a reliable predictor of general multitasking

ability (i.e., performance under normal conditions), there is the question of whether different

cognitive faculties are needed in order to rapidly respond to changing task demands

(adaptability).

Method: Participants first completed a battery of cognitive individual differences tests

followed by multitasking sessions with a flight simulator. After a Baseline condition, difficulty

of the flight simulator was incrementally increased via four experimental manipulations, and

performance metrics were collected to assess multitasking ability and adaptability.

Results: Scholastic aptitude and working memory predicted general multitasking ability

(i.e., performance at baseline difficulty), but spatial manipulation (in conjunction with working

memory) was a major predictor of adaptability (performance in difficult conditions after

accounting for Baseline performance).

Conclusion: Multitasking ability and adaptability may be overlapping but separate

constructs that draw on overlapping (but not identical) sets of cognitive abilities.

Application: The results of this study are applicable to practitioners and researchers in

human factors to assess multitasking performance in real-world contexts and under realistic task

constraints. We also present a framework for conceptualizing multitasking adaptability on the

Page 3: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 2

basis of five adaptability profiles derived from performance on tasks with consistent versus

increased difficulty.

Keywords: cognitive abilities; spatial manipulation; working memory; MATB; task

switching

Précis:

Multitasking ability is a popular topic, but relatively little is known about how people adapt to

changing task constraints. We present evidence that ability and adaptability may be separate

constructs governed by overlapping yet not identical cognitive factors. We also present a

framework for conceptualizing adaptability via five adaptivity profiles.

Page 4: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 3

Individual Differences in Multitasking Ability and Adaptability

From pilots to personal assistants, the average 21st century individual is often expected to

attend to two or more tasks simultaneously or multitask1(Bühner, König, Pick, & Krumm, 2006).

Performing multiple tasks simultaneously is more challenging than doing a single task (Monsell,

2003; Sauer, Wastell, & Hockey, 1999). When multitasking, people can become overloaded as

working memory and attentional resources are exhausted. They might also become anxious and

frustrated when task challenges outweigh cognitive resources (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975).

Furthermore, overall performance can be negatively affected when the demands of one task

interfere with those of another (Altmann & Gray, 2008). Thus, with multitasking, the whole does

not always equal the sum of the individual parts.

Furthermore, there is no one-size-fits-all approach to multitasking because performance

varies across individuals (e.g., Watson and Strayer, 2010). Some people even report a preference

for multitasking (e.g., Branscome, Swoboda, & Fatkin, 2007; Slocombe & Bluedorn, 1999;

Poposki, Oswald, & Brou, 2009), although the association between preference and performance

is unclear. There have been some attempts to identify the individual differences associated with

multitasking performance. For example, differences in perceptual speed (Oberlander, Hambrick,

Oswald, & Jones, 2007), motivation (Oswald, Hambrick, & Jones, 2007), and neuroticism

(Oswald, Hambrick, & Jones, 2007; Poposki, Oswald, & Chen, 2008) have been linked to

multitasking effectiveness. That said, once a person decides to engage in multitasking, the

critical predictor of performance appears to be working memory and executive control,

1 For the purposes of this paper, multitasking is defined as performing two or more tasks at once. It also refers to

switching from one task to another over short time spans of a few seconds (time sharing) (Oswald, Hambrick, &

Jones, 2007).

Page 5: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 4

ostensibly because multitasking requires the individual to maintain representations of different

tasks in working memory and strategically deploy attentional resources to effectively switch

between these tasks (e.g., Bühner, König, Pick, & Krumm, 2006; Hambrick, Oswald, Darowski,

Rench, & Brou, 2009; Oberlander, Hambrick, Oswald, & Jones, 2007; Rubinstein, Meyer, &

Evans, 2001).

Although considerable work has focused on predictors of multitasking ability, relatively

little research has addressed multitasking adaptability. Here we operationalize multitasking

ability as a task-dependent metric of performance when the difficulty level of a given set of tasks

is at some baseline. In contrast, adaptability refers to the capacity to adapt to changing task

constraints relative to baseline performance. That is, how is performance affected if the difficulty

of one or more tasks changes or if a new task is introduced or a task is dropped? Therefore,

ability and adaptability, although related, might be separate constructs.

In this vein, Branscome and Grynovicki (2007) assessed adaptability using SynWork

(Elsmore, 1994), a multitasking environment with four tasks (memory, math, auditory, and

monitoring). They also added a military target-identification task of identifying friendly versus

enemy targets. The three conditions in their study were: all four SynWork tasks, three SynWork

tasks plus the target-identification task, and all four SynWork tasks plus the target-identification

task (with counterbalanced ordering). They found that the third (most difficult) condition was

associated with poorer performance compared to the other two. Additionally, Wang, Proctor,

and Pick (2007) found that when certain SynWork tasks were biased via a payout structure (i.e.,

more points awarded for some tasks), some people strategically adapted to this payout bias

whereas others did not. This suggests that some people show a greater propensity for

multitasking adaptability than others.

Page 6: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 5

The individual differences associated with multitasking adaptability were investigated by

Oswald, Hambrick, & Jones (2007). They reported that cognitive ability was positively

associated with multitasking performance at both baseline (ability) and increased difficulty

(adaptability), whereas conscientiousness was negatively correlated with adaptability (i.e.,

excessive attention to detail impaired performance under increased demand, but improved

performance at lower levels of demand). However, Oswald et al. performed a factor analysis on

their cognitive task battery; as they note, this made it impossible to isolate the impact of

individual cognitive abilities. Thus, the specific cognitive factors associated with multitasking

ability and adaptability remains an open question.

To address this question, we assessed the relationship between cognitive factors and

multitasking ability and adaptability in a pilot simulation task. We collected scores on four

standard cognitive measures (working memory, scholastic aptitude, spatial manipulation, and

creativity) and correlated these with performance under baseline difficulty (ability) and changes

in performance when task(s) difficulty increased (adaptability).

We expected that baseline performance (ability) reliably predicts performance under

increased demand (adaptability). For multitasking ability, we hypothesized that working memory

span is positively correlated with performance. With respect to adaptability, there are two

competing hypotheses. First, the cognitive factors involved in multitasking performance (ability)

are also involved in adaptability. Alternatively, it may be that adaptability is predicted by a set of

cognitive measures that are either different from or complement those that predict ability. This

second hypothesis is graphically depicted in Figure 1. The solid lines indicate predicted

relationships between components. For example, based on the prior research cited earlier, higher

Page 7: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 6

working memory span is associated with higher ability. Dashed lines represent the investigated

relationships between components, which are the focus of this paper.

Figure 1. Proposed hypothesis for multitasking ability and adaptability.

Method

Participants

The sample was 32 participants either enrolled in a Midwestern university or volunteers

of various educational backgrounds from a Southern city in the United States. Participants

selected from the university received partial course credit for participation in the study.

Materials

Individual Differences Measures (IDMs). At the start of the experiment, participants

completed a computer-administered battery of cognitive tests including scholastic aptitude,

working memory, creativity, and spatial manipulation.

Scholastic Aptitude. As a measure of scholastic aptitude, participants self-reported SAT

Reasoning Test or American College Test (ACT) scores. ACT scores were converted to

Page 8: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 7

equivalent SAT scores using a concordance table on the ACT website (ACT, 2008). Self-

reported ACT and SAT scores reliably correlate with actual test scores (Cole & Gonyea, 2010).

Working Memory. Working memory was assessed using a comprehension span task

(Waters & Caplan, 1996), which correlates with other memory span measures, such as the

Operation Span task (Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005). On each trial, participants

indicated whether a given sentence made sense. Sentences were presented in sets of 2 to 6, and

set size increased by one after every five sets. After judging the sentences in a set, participants

recalled the last word of each of those sentences. The sum of correctly recalled sets served as a

measure of working memory span, with one point given for each word in a correctly recalled set.

Creativity. As a measure of creativity, a version of the Remote Association Task

(Mednick, 1963; Topolinski & Strack, 2009) was used. Participants were given a triad of words

with the task of determining whether a fourth word was related to all three (Topolinski & Strack,

2009). For example, given the words ‘super’, ‘ship’, and ‘gazer’, the word ‘star’ would be related

to the first three. Responses were scored with one point for each correct answer across 40 trials.

Spatial Manipulation. Spatial manipulation was measured using a variant of the Shepard

and Metzler Mental Rotation Task (Just & Carpenter, 1985; Shepard & Metzler, 1971).

Participants were shown two 3-D images of block figures and had to determine if the figures

were the same if they were rotated. A correct answer was scored as a ‘1’, whereas incorrect

answers received a ‘-1’. They had one minute to judge as many pairs of figures as possible.

Multitasking Environment. The Multi-Attribute Task Battery (MATB; Comstock &

Arnegard, 1992) is a computerized flight simulator that requires users to simultaneously attend to

four separate tasks: System Monitoring, Communications, Resource Management, and

Page 9: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 8

Tracking. Each task had four difficulty levels: automatic (0), low (1), medium (2), and high (3).

The MATB interface is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2. The MATB interface, with the composite score displayed at the bottom.

System Monitoring. In the top-left quadrant of the screen, participants responded to

feedback from lights and gauges. There were two lights at the top of the quadrant, one green and

one red. Participants were instructed to press the F5 key if the green light turned off and to press

the F6 key if the red light came on. Doing so turned on the green light and turned off the red

light, respectively.

Beneath the lights were four gauges, each associated with a corresponding key on the

keyboard. Each gauge had a yellow pointer that typically hovered around the center line.

Page 10: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 9

Participants were asked to press the corresponding key (F1 to F4) if any gauge’s pointer

exceeded one unit in either direction from the center line. When a correct response was made,

feedback was provided via a yellow line that would briefly appear on the respective gauge.

Communications. In the bottom-left quadrant, participants were asked to monitor for a

call sign (NGT504) and follow audio instructions pertaining to this call sign while ignoring

instructions for other call signs. Each message began with a six-character call sign (which was

repeated), followed by a command to change one of four settings (two navigation and two

communication) to a particular frequency, represented by a four-digit number. An example

command would be, “NGT504, NGT504, change navigation two to one-one-zero-point-five.”

The four navigation and communication settings were changed using the up and down arrow

keys, and frequency was changed using the left and right arrow keys.

Resource Management. In the bottom-right quadrant, participants were asked to manage

the fuel levels (represented in green) of two tanks, A and B. The fuel levels decreased constantly

as fuel was used, and the goal was to keep the fuel level of both tanks between the tick marks on

each tank (5/8ths full). This was done by turning various pumps on or off to transfer fuel from

another tank. Tanks C and D had a finite supply, whereas the other two had an unlimited

capacity. To turn a pump on or off, participants pressed the number key corresponding to the

pump flow indicators on the screen (one through eight). At various points, one or more pumps

would fail and become unusable for a period of time.

Tracking. In the top-right quadrant, the task was to keep a moving reticle (crosshairs) as

close as possible to the center crosshairs using a joystick. The reticle would drift and randomly

change directions with varying speed.

Page 11: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 10

Scheduling and Pump Status. The Scheduling and Pump Status zones on the far right

provided supplementary information for the Communications and Resource Management tasks,

respectively. They were not necessary for completing any of the tasks, and so are not discussed

here.

Performance. Performance was calculated as a product of the scores on the four

individual tasks (the composite score), and was displayed on a gauge at the bottom of the screen.

Completely neglecting any one task would yield a composite score of zero. Scores could range

from 0 to 100. Comstock and Arnegard (1992) provide details on how scores for the individual

tasks are computed.

Procedure

Participants first completed the IDMs on a computer for approximately one hour. After a

short break, they completed the MATB task on a separate computer for another hour.

Participants were seated using a keyboard and mouse (IDMs) or joystick (MATB), and speakers

or headphones were used for audio output. The MATB task had five conditions: Practice,

Baseline, Single Difficulty, Paired Difficulty, and Difficulty Ramp-Up. No instructions were

given regarding priority of the individual tasks or how the composite score was calculated.

Practice. The first condition consisted of four separate practice sessions in which

participants completed each task separately for a total of nine minutes. The composite score was

displayed in all practice conditions. Participants did not engage in all four tasks simultaneously

in the Practice condition.

Baseline (BL). The Baseline condition had all four tasks set at the low difficulty level for

five minutes. No feedback was given on the performance scores to encourage participants to

Page 12: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 11

concentrate on doing all four tasks simultaneously. Subsequent conditions provided feedback on

the composite score.

Single Difficulty (SD). The Single Difficulty condition began with a three-minute warm-

up during which all tasks were at Baseline difficulty, after which the difficulty level of one task

was set to hard for one minute, while the others were set at easy. All difficulties were then set to

easy for another minute. This was done for all four tasks in succession, then the process was

repeated, for a total of 16 minutes.

Paired Difficulty (PD). The Paired Difficulty condition raised the difficulty of both the

System Monitoring and Communications tasks to hard for two minutes, while the Resource

Management and Tracking tasks remained at easy. System Monitoring and Communications

were manipulated here because both provide discrete events that immediately impact composite

scores, thereby encouraging them to constantly attend to these tasks.

Difficulty Ramp-Up (Ramp-Up, RU). For the final condition, after a one-minute warm-

up, the difficulty of all four tasks were raised to medium for one minute (RU2), and then to the

hardest difficulty for an additional minute (RU3).

Results

We began by examining the data for potential outliers. A Mahalanobis D2 test indicated

no multivariate outliers among the individual differences measures (p > .01), and a tolerance

analysis indicated no multicollinearity among aptitude, working memory, and spatial

manipulation (VIF < 1.64). We then proceeded by analyzing the MATB performance data.

Multitasking Ability

Table 1 displays the mean scores and standard deviations for each task in each condition,

as well as an average across tasks. As the goal of this paper is to examine multitasking ability

Page 13: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 12

and adaptability, the analyses generally focus on the combined performance of all four tasks, as

opposed to analyzing each task individually. The similarity in average scores across difficulties

indicates learning on the part of the participants in both individual task skills and coping with

multitasking. This was especially evident in tasks which were relatively more intermittent

(communications and resource) rather than continuous (monitoring and tracking). The similarity

in average performance on the difficult conditions compared to the Baseline might also be

attributed to individual differences in performance in the difficult conditions. That is, mean

performance in the difficult conditions would be comparable to Baseline if the performance of

some participants decreased while others improved as task difficulty increased. It is exactly this

source of individual differences in adaptability that are at the focus of this research.

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of MATB scores for each task and condition

Task

Condition Monitoring Comms. Resource Tracking Average

Baseline 93.0 (3.35) 85.0 (16.6) 82.1 (12.3) 84.3 (5.35) 86.1 (5.88)

Single Difficulty

Monitoring 67.6 (9.19) 81.9 (20.9) 82.2 (18.5) 83.4 (8.78) 78.8 (8.71) Communications 94.9 (4.77) 84.1 (17.7) 81.4 (22.8) 85.8 (5.21) 86.6 (8.20) Resource 93.7 (4.05) 81.6 (20.2) 80.3 (20.6) 86.7 (3.95) 85.6 (8.65) Tracking 94.5 (6.25) 78.9 (23.8) 84.0 (21.0) 75.9 (7.18) 83.3 (9.13)

Paired Difficulty 86.0 (7.67) 86.9 (18.8) 85.7 (11.6) 85.5 (4.85) 86.0 (6.72) Ramp-Up 2 91.7 (5.33) 91.8 (12.9) 89.8 (11.9) 78.1 (8.08) 87.8 (5.35) Ramp-Up 3 86.3 (9.89) 86.0 (18.7) 86.7 (16.9) 70.5 (8.40) 82.4 (8.54)

Note. Bolded values represent tasks with increased difficulty. Comms. = Communication

Page 14: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 13

To investigate how performance on the various MATB conditions was interrelated, we

examined correlations of the average scores across conditions (see Table 2). Since the tasks are

identical for each condition (except for difficulty), we expected the relationships between

conditions to be relatively strong. This was largely confirmed. However, there were also some

differences based on difficulty level. For example, Baseline, SD, and PD had a much stronger

correlation with each other than with RU2 and RU3. Also, the correlations for the RU3 condition

increased uniformly with the relative difficulty level of the other conditions.

Table 2. Correlations between IDMs and MATB scores in each condition

Condition Single Difficulty

Paired Difficulty

Ramp Up 2

Ramp Up 3

Baseline **.674 ***.723 **.452 ***.497 Single Difficulty ***.801 ***.548 ***.657 Paired Difficulty ***.612 ***.704 Ramp-Up 2 ***.881 Notes. * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01

We proceeded by analyzing the individual differences measures. The means and

correlations of the IDMs (listed in Table 3) indicate some covariance between working memory,

aptitude, and spatial manipulation. To assess which individual differences measures (if any)

predicted performance on the MATB, we correlated the scores of each IDM with average

performance in each condition (see Table 4). Although we tested the significance of the

correlational coefficients, a power analysis with alpha = 0.05 and power = 0.8 indicated that our

small sample of 32 participants could only detect correlations of 0.45 or higher with a two-tailed

test. Hence, in addition to discussing significant effects we also consider non-significant

Page 15: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 14

correlations of 0.20 or higher to be meaningful because these might be significant with a larger

sample.

Table 3. IDM descriptives and correlations

Correlations

Measure Mean (SD) Aptitude Spatial Man. Creativity

Working Memory 22.5 (17.8) ***.538 **.436 -3.23 Aptitude 1328 (209) - *.365 0.92 Spatial Man. 6.96 (5.79) - - .268 Creativity 20.4 (3.43) - - - Notes. * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01; Spatial Man. = Spatial Manipulation

The results indicated that scholastic aptitude and working memory were associated with

superior performance at Baseline (multitasking ability), while spatial manipulation and creativity

had negligible correlations with Baseline performance. This confirms prior work suggesting that

executive functioning is a positive predictor of multitasking ability.

A somewhat different pattern emerged for the conditions with increased difficulty.

Consistent with the patterns of correlations with Baseline scores, scholastic aptitude and working

memory were positively correlated, and creativity was negatively or not correlated with

performance on the more difficult conditions. However, in contrast to Baseline correlations,

spatial manipulation was positively correlated with performance in all four increased difficulty

conditions. Lastly, although the effects for creativity and spatial manipulation were similar at

Baseline, unlike spatial manipulation, creativity still did not correlate with performance in the

increased difficulty conditions.

Table 4. Correlations between IDMs and MATB scores in each condition

Page 16: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 15

Measure Baseline Single Difficulty

Paired Difficulty

Ramp Up 2

Ramp Up 3

Working Memory *.353 ***.645 *.394 .203 *.356 Aptitude **.436 **.465 ***.503 *.381 *.337 Spatial Man. .068 **.403 **.435 **.443 ***.577 Creativity .001 -.336 -.039 -.131 -.089 Notes. * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01; Spatial Man. = Spatial Manipulation

Multitasking Adaptability

The correlations in Table 4 represent the cognitive abilities associated with multitasking

ability. However, it remains to be seen if these also govern adaptability. To address this, partial

correlations on IDMs and MATB scores were calculated using Baseline performance as a

covariate (Table 5). These correlations indicate which cognitive abilities predict performance

when task difficulty increases (adaptability) after accounting for general multitasking ability

(Baseline scores).

The partial correlations presented in Table 5 reveal that the effects of both scholastic

aptitude and working memory on adaptability were diminished relative to their effects on

multitasking ability (Table 4). The effect of spatial manipulation, however, was stronger for

adaptability (versus ability) across all four experimental conditions. Finally, controlling for

Baseline scores did not significantly adjust the effects for creativity on adaptability.

Table 5. Partial correlations between IDMs and MATB scores after controlling for Baseline

scores

Page 17: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 16

Measure Single Difficulty

Paired Difficulty

Ramp Up 2

Ramp Up 3

Working Memory ***.588 .215 .052 .222 Aptitude .257 .302 .229 .153 Spatial Man. **.485 ***.560 **.464 ***.628 Creativity -.331 -.034 .114 .092 Notes. * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01; Spatial Man. = Spatial Manipulation

There is the important question of whether the correlations between spatial manipulation

and adaptability are greater than the correlations between aptitude, working memory, creativity,

and adaptability. This question was addressed by testing for significant differences among

overlapping correlation coefficients (Meng, Rosenthal, & Rubin, 1992). The results of these

comparisons are presented in Table 6. Creativity was only compared to spatial manipulation

because it was generally not correlated with multitasking ability (Table 4) or adaptability (Table

5).

Table 6. Correlation comparisons between IDMs and MATB delta scores in each condition

Comparison Single Difficulty

Paired Difficulty

Ramp Up 2

Ramp Up 3

Spatial Man. vs. Aptitude * S > A ** S > A * S > A *** S > A Spatial Man. vs. Working Memory S = WM ** S > WM *** S > WM ***S > WM Spatial Man. vs. Creativity *** S > C ** S > C S = C ** S > C Aptitude vs. Working Memory A = WM A = WM A = WM A = WM Notes. * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01; Spatial Man. = Spatial Manipulation

Page 18: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 17

The results indicated that the correlation between spatial manipulation and adaptability

was greater than the correlation between aptitude and adaptability in all of the increased

difficulty conditions (top row of Table 6). A similar finding was obtained for the comparison

between spatial manipulation and working memory, with the exception of the single difficulty

condition (second row of Table 6). Likewise, the correlation between spatial manipulation and

adaptability was greater than the correlation between creativity and adaptability in all of the

increased difficulty conditions except for Ramp-Up 2 (third row of Table 6). Finally, aptitude

and working memory were equivalent in predicting adaptability (bottom row of Table 6). Taken

together, these findings indicate that the cognitive abilities that predicted performance at

Baseline (ability) were somewhat different from the factors that predicted adaptability2.

Given the positive relationship between spatial manipulation and adaptability, there was

the concern that there might have been an inherent bias towards spatial manipulation in the

MATB environment. However, as reported in Table 4, there was no correlation between spatial

manipulation and Baseline performance. We investigated further and correlated spatial

manipulation with performance on each of the four individual tasks in the MATB. Importantly,

none of these correlations were significant (p > .10), indicating that there was no spatial bias

even at the level of the individual task.

It should also be noted that although pre-post analyses such as this are common in

experimental research, there is some debate on the proper data analysis techniques (e.g., Bereiter,

1963). While regression-based analyses (including partial correlations) have pitfalls (e.g., Linn &

2 These analyses were also replicated with hierarchical linear regressions, with Baseline entered as Step 1, and the IDMs together as Step 2. In all four regressions, spatial visualization was a statistically significant predictor, p < .05, and no other IDM was statistically significant, p > .10.

Page 19: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 18

Slinde, 1977), they are generally preferred over other methods (such as analyses on difference

scores; Blaikie, 2003; Cribbie & Jamieson, 2004; Flury, 1989; Huck & McLean, 1975).

An additional concern may be that the lack of relationship between spatial manipulation

and Baseline performance artificially inflated the findings for spatial manipulation and

multitasking adaptability. However, the same pattern did not emerge for creativity, which was

also unrelated to Baseline performance. Similarly, it could be argued that accounting for

Baseline performance would inevitably cause working memory and aptitude to be unrelated to

adaptability (i.e., a spurious effect due to suppression; Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003).

This is not necessarily the case, as working memory was, in fact, strongly correlated with

adaptability in the Single Difficulty condition.

General Discussion

This paper analyzed multitasking ability and adaptability within the context of a flight

simulator. We administered measures of individual differences to identify cognitive abilities

associated with multitasking performance. By manipulating the task constraints, we aimed to

identify whether multitasking ability and adaptability were separate constructs. In this section,

we consider the extent to which our findings support this position, as well as consider a

framework that might be used to categorize different types of adapters.

Ability vs. Adaptability

The results supported a number of conclusions related to multitasking abilities,

adaptability, and individual differences. First, although the correlations between Baseline

performance and the Single and Paired Difficulty conditions were quite high, the correlations

decreased for the more difficult Ramp-Up conditions (see Figure 3). This suggests that the effect

Page 20: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 19

of Baseline performance (multitasking ability) decreased as the task became harder. Second,

scholastic aptitude and working memory predicted Baseline performance, as expected on the

basis of prior work (Bühner, König, Pick, & Krumm, 2006; Hambrick, Oswald, Darowski,

Rench, & Brou, R. 2009; Oberlander, Hambrick, Oswald, & Jones, 2007; Rubinstein, Meyer, &

Evans, 2001). However, the impact of these measures was somewhat reduced (though not

necessarily zero) with regards to performance in the more difficult conditions (adaptability).

Conversely, spatial manipulation was not related to Baseline multitasking ability, but was the

strongest predictor of performance in the difficult conditions and after controlling for Baseline

performance (adaptability). Simply put, one set of cognitive factors best predicted ability

(Baseline performance) while another best predicted adaptability (performance on difficult

conditions after accounting for Baseline performance). Thus, although it may be too strong to

claim that multitasking ability and adaptability are mutually exclusive, the present analyses do

open the possibility that they might be overlapping but separate constructs.

These results offer a distinct contribution from previous studies of adaptability in

multitasking. Whereas a principal finding from Oswald et al. (2007) was that a noncognitive

factor (conscientiousness) emerged to negatively predict performance under emergency

conditions, the current results suggest that an aspect of cognitive functioning (other than working

memory) was a predictor of adaptability. Additionally, Branscome and Grynovicki (2007) did

not account for baseline performance in their analyses, which may have yielded some additional

patterns.

Page 21: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 20

Figure 3. Correlation of Baseline scores with scores in difficult conditions

Although spatial manipulation was the individual difference that predicted adaptability in

the present task (as well as working memory in the SD condition), it is important to note that

these results may be task-specific. That is, although the MATB environment had no initial bias

towards spatial manipulation (both overall and at the level of the individual task), certain aspects

of the environment might draw on spatial abilities. For example, Oksama (2004) noted that tasks

with multiple moving objects (which would include the MATB) may be biased toward people

with high visuospatial working memory. It is important to note, however, that the MATB

environment is also biased toward those with superior executive functioning because strategic

deployment of attentional resources is critical for this task. Indeed, executive functioning was

strongly correlated with general ability, yet it had small or negligible correlations with

adaptability.

The association between adaptability and spatial manipulation when multitasking may lie

in the close tie in working memory between spatial manipulation (i.e., the visuo-spatial

sketchpad) and the central executive (Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001).

0.4

0.6

0.8

SD PD RU1 RU2

Cor

rela

tion

Correlation with Baseline

Page 22: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 21

Thus, although spatial manipulation may not be a critical factor at baseline difficulty, it may play

a role when the central executive is under increased pressure due to task constraints. Miyake et

al. (2001) also note that this link does not extend to the verbal domain, which might explain the

relative lack of impact of the other individual differences measures (each of which contains a

verbal component).

Additionally, spatial ability is highly correlated with fluid intelligence (Gf; Carroll, 1993;

Lohman, 2000). Gf is considered to be the subset of general intelligence (g) which encompasses

reasoning and adaptability to novel situations or domains (e.g., Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, &

Conway, 1999; Gray, Chabris, & Braver, 2003). Although the experimental conditions in the

current study did not constitute a novel domain per se, manipulating the task constraints of a

familiar environment imposes demands on reasoning ability and spurs adaptive shifts in strategy

(Schunn & Reder, 2001).

Nevertheless, limitations of the current findings recommend further research to confirm

the existence of overlapping (but not identical) sets of cognitive abilities required for adapting to

changing task constraints when multitasking. We considered possible restrictions of statistical

power and analytical methods on our findings for working memory/aptitude vs. spatial

manipulation in regards to multitasking adaptability. Given the sample size and possible effects

due to suppression, our claim is not that there is no relationship between adaptability and

working memory/aptitude; rather, our results indicate that spatial manipulation was the strongest

predictor of adaptability. This was supported by a comparison of correlations among the IDMs

and adaptability and by the regression analyses. Furthermore, despite the potential pitfalls,

Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken (2003) state that the ability to assess unique or partial

relationships is a defining feature of multiple regression/correlation. In addition to further

Page 23: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 22

addressing these issues, future examination should also expand to different multitasking

environments to ensure generalizability.

Adaptability Profiles

Future research on multitasking adaptability may also consider adaptability profiles to

better understand how individuals or groups respond to task constraints. A framework to

conceptualize adaptivity may be useful to distinguish between changes in performance (from a

baseline) under increased task difficulty (Increased Difficulty task) versus constant task

difficulty (Consistent Difficulty task). Such a framework is presented in Figure 4. The vertical

axis represents performance on a difficult task(s) relative to a given baseline difficulty, whereas

the horizontal axis represents performance on a task(s) with baseline difficulty. It is important to

note that this framework assumes that each of the tasks has equal priority when all tasks are at

baseline difficulty. The more difficult task is given priority when difficulty increases.

The different regions in the figure represent various adaptability profiles. For example,

individuals whose performance is not compromised when additional difficulty in a task(s) is

encountered are consistent performers (circle around the origin in Figure 4). The top-right

quadrant encompasses the individuals whose performance increases for both the Increased

Difficulty and Consistent Difficulty tasks (good adapters). Conversely, the bottom-left quadrant

represents individuals who show a decline in performance for both Increased Difficulty and

Consistent Difficulty tasks (poor adapters).

There might also be situations when an individual sacrifices performance in one task(s)

for the sake of another. When encountering additional difficulty on a task(s), some individuals

may choose to tackle the difficult task(s) at the expense of the other task(s). These individuals

Page 24: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 23

are called attackers (top-left quadrant). Alternatively, others may neglect the difficult task(s) and

focus on the other tasks which are at baseline difficulty. These are the avoiders (bottom-right

quadrant).

Figure 4. Framework to characterize multitasking adaptability.

There are two final points to note about the proposed adaptability framework. First, the

distribution of individuals to adaptability profiles need not be uniform. For example, there may

be no avoiders for a given multitasking context, whereas everyone might be classified as

avoiders for a different context. In the latter case, the differences among the individuals may still

be useful; since all avoiders are not equivalent, the relative differences among avoiders can be

informative. Second, the five adaptability profiles are sensitive to differences in multitasking

contexts. That is, a poor adapter in context A is not necessarily a poor adapter in context B.

Rather than a rigid dispositional characterization of an individual’s ability to adapt, the primary

Page 25: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 24

purpose of the framework is to relatively organize the relationship between individuals or groups

within a given multitasking context.

These relationships represent a critical area for future research. For example, although

adaptability may be measured at an individual level (as in this paper), it may be useful to

consider a group-level analysis as well. Here, clustering analyses may be used to determine

whether individual performances can be classified into separate groups, which may be

interpreted using the above framework. This would be quite informative to researchers and

practitioners in the field of human factors.

Concluding Remarks

The findings in this paper suggest some critical implications with respect to multitasking

for practitioners and researchers in human factors. First, performance on the individual tasks

should not be the only consideration; rather, it should also include overall performance when

concurrently switching between the multiple tasks relevant to a given occupation (multitasking).

Furthermore, beyond accounting for the cognitive abilities needed for superior multitasking

ability, practitioners and researchers in human factors should also attempt to identify those

abilities needed for adapting to realistic task constraints, as they may be different from those

required for mere performance.

.

Page 26: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 25

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Sandia National Laboratories’ Laboratory-Directed Research and

Development (LDRD) Project 130787. Sandia is a multiprogram laboratory operated by Sandia

Corporation, a Lockheed Martin Company, for the United States Department of Energy under

Contract DE-AC04-94AL85000.

Page 27: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 26

Key Points:

-Working memory and aptitude predicted multitasking ability, and spatial manipulation was a

major predictor of multitasking adaptability

-Adaptability profiles were presented as a method for categorizing different types of adapters

-Multitasking ability and adaptability may be overlapping but different constructs

Page 28: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 27

References

ACT. (2008). ACT-SAT Concordance. Retrieved May 17, 2010, from

http://www.act.org/aap/concordance

Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task

switching. Psychological Review, 115, 602–639.

Arnegard, R.J., & Comstock, R.J. (1991). Multi-Attribute Task Battery: applications in pilot

workload and strategic behavior research. Proceedings of the Sixth International

Symposium on Aviation Psychology (pp. 1118-1123). Columbus, OH: Ohio State

University Press.

Bereiter, C. (1963). Some persisting dilemmas in the measurement of change. In C. W. Harris

(Ed.), Problems in measuring change (pp. 3-20). Madison, WI: The University of

Wisconsin Press.

Blaikie, N. (2003). Analyzing Quantitative Data: From Description to Explanation. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Branscome, T. A., & Grynovicki, J. O. (2007). An investigation of factors affecting multi-task

performance in an immersive environment (ARL Report No. TR-4325). Aberdeen, MD:

U.S. Army Research Laboratory.

Branscome, T. A., Swoboda, J. C., & Fatkin, L. T. (2007). An initial investigation of factors

affecting multi-task performance (ARL Report No. TR-4025). Aberdeen, MD: U.S. Army

Research Laboratory.

Bühner, M., König, C. J., Pick, M., & Krumm, S. (2006). Working memory dimensions as

differential predictors of the speed and error aspect of multitasking performance. Human

Performance, 19, 253−275.

Page 29: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 28

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation

analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cole, J. S., & Gonyea, R. M. (2010). Accuracy of Self-reported SAT and ACT Test Scores:

Implications for Research. Research in Higher Education, 51, 305-319.

Colom, R., Contreras, M. J., Shih, P. C., & Santacreu, J. (2003). The assessment of spatial ability

with a single computerized test. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 19, 92-

100.

Comstock, J. R., Jr., & Amegard, R. J. (1992). The Multi-Attribute Task Battery for human

operator workload and strategic behavior research. National Aeronautics and Space

Administration Technical Memorandum No. 104174.

Cribbie, R. A., & Jamieson, J. (2004). Decreases in posttest variance and the measurement of

change. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 9(1), 37-55.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading.

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

Elsmore, T. F. (1994). SYNWORK 1: A PC-based tool for assessment of performance in a

simulated work environment. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,

26, 421-426.

Page 30: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 29

Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory,

short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309.

Flury, B. W. (1989). Understanding partial statistics and redundancy of variables in regression

and discriminant analysis. American Statistician, 43, 27–31.

Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid

intelligence. Nature neuroscience, 6(3), 316-322.

Hambrick, D. Z., Oswald, F. L., Darowski, E. S., Rench, T. A., & Brou, R. (2010). Predictors of

Multitasking Performance in a Synthetic Work Paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology,

24, 1149-1167.

Huck, S. W., & McLean, R. A. (1975). Using a repeated measures ANOVA to analyze the data

from a pretest-posttest design. A potentially confusing task. Psychological Bulletin, 82,

511-518.

Just, M. A, & Carpenter, P. A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental

rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. Psychological Review, 92, 137-172.

King, R. E., Retzlaff, P. D., Detwiler, C. A., Schroeder, D. J., Broach, D. (2003). Use of

Personality Assessment Measures in the Selection of Air Traffic Control Specialists;

DOT-FAA-AM-03/20; Federal Aviation Administration, Civil Aeromedical Institute:

Oklahoma City, OK, 2003.

Linn, R. L., & Slinde, J. A. (1977). The determination of the significance of change between pre-

and posttesting periods. Review of Educational Research, 47, 121-150.

Lohman, D.F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.),

Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Page 31: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 30

Meng, X., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients.

Psychological Bulletin, 111, 172-175.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are

visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A

latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 621-640.

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 134–140.

Oberlander, E. M., Oswald, F. L., Hambrick, D. Z., & Jones, L. A. (2007). Individual Difference

Variables as Predictors of Error during Multitasking. Technical report for Navy

Personnel Research, Studies, and Technology (NPRST-TN-07-9). Millington, TN.

Oksama, L., & Hyona, J. (2004). Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early

vision mechanism independent of higher-order cognition? An individual difference

approach. Visual Cognition, 11, 631–671.

Oswald, F. L., Hambrick, D. Z., & Jones, L. A. (2007). Keeping all the plates spinning:

Understanding and predicting multitasking performance. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.),

Learning to solve complex scientific problems. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Oswald, F. L., Hambrick, D. Z., Jones, L. A., & Ghumman, S. S. (2007). SYRUS: Understanding

and predicting multitasking performance. Technical report for Navy Personnel Research,

Studies, and Technology (NPRST-TN-07-5). Millington, TN.

Poposki, E. M., Oswald, F. L., & Brou, R. (2009). Development of a new measure of

polychronicity. Technical report for Navy Personnel Research, Studies, and Technology

(NPRST-TN-09-5). Millington, TN.

Page 32: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 31

Poposki, E. M., Oswald, F. L., & Chen, H. T. (2009). Neuroticism negatively affects multitasking

performance through state anxiety. Technical report for Navy Personnel Research,

Studies, and Technology (NPRST-TN-09-3). Millington, TN.

Rubinstein, J., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in

task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 27, 763–797.

Salthouse, T. & Miles, J. (2002). Aging and time sharing aspects of executive control. Memory

& Cognition, 30, 572-582.

Sauer, J., Wastell, D. G., Hockey, G. R. J. (1999). Multiple-Task Performance on a Computer-

Simulated Life Support System During a Space Mission Simulation. Acta Astronautica,

44, 43-52.

Schunn, C. D., & Reder, L. M. (2001). Another source of individual differences: Strategy

adaptivity to changing rates of success. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,

130, 59–76.

Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171,

701-703.

Slocombe, T. E., & Bluedorn, A. C. (1999). Organizational behavior implications of the

congruence between preferred polychronicity and experienced work-unit polychronicity.

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 75–99.

Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009). The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine

intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence, and of grammaticality in artificial

grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 39-63.

Page 33: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 32

Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the

operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498–505.

Wang, D. D., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2007). Acquisition and transfer of attention

allocation strategies in a multiple-task work environment. Human Factors, 49, 995-1004.

Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and

its relation to reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49,

51-74.

Watson, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2010). Supertaskers: Profiles in extraordinary multi-tasking

ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 479-485.

Page 34: APA Format 6th Edition Template

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MULTITASKING 33

Biographies Brent Morgan is a Graduate Researcher and Doctoral Candidate in Experimental Psychology at the University of Memphis, where he received a Master of Science degree in 2010. Sidney D'Mello is an Assistant Professor in the departments of Psychology and Computer Science at the University of Notre Dame. He received his Ph.D. in Computer Science from the University of Memphis in 2009. Robert G. Abbott is a Principal Member of the Technical Staff at Sandia National Laboratories in Albuquerque, NM. He received his Ph.D. in Computer Science from the University of New Mexico in 2007. Gabriel Radvansky is a Professor in the department of Psychology at the University of Notre Dame. He received his Ph.D. in experimental psychology from Michigan State University in 1992. Michael J. Haass is a Research and Development Engineer at Sandia National Laboratories in Albuquerque, NM. He received his Master of Science degree in Physics from the University of Tennessee in 1993. Andrea K. Tamplin is a Doctoral Candidate in the Cognition, Brain, and Behavior program at the University of Notre Dame. She received her Master’s Degree from the University of Notre Dame in 2008.